15
Home Reading Environment and Brain Activation in Preschool Children Listening to Stories John S. Hutton, MD a , Tzipi Horowitz-Kraus, PhD a,b,c , Alan L. Mendelsohn, MD e , Tom DeWitt, MD a , Scott K. Holland, PhD b,c,d , the C-MIND Authorship Consortium abstract BACKGROUND AND OBJECTIVES: Parent-child reading is widely advocated to promote cognitive development, including in recommendations from the American Academy of Pediatrics to begin this practice at birth. Although parent-child reading has been shown in behavioral studies to improve oral language and print concepts, quantiable effects on the brain have not been previously studied. Our study used blood oxygen leveldependent functional magnetic resonance imaging to examine the relationship between home reading environment and brain activity during a story listening task in a sample of preschool-age children. We hypothesized that while listening to stories, children with greater home reading exposure would exhibit higher activation of left-sided brain regions involved with semantic processing (extraction of meaning). METHODS: Nineteen 3- to 5-year-old children were selected from a longitudinal study of normal brain development. All completed blood oxygen leveldependent functional magnetic resonance imaging using an age-appropriate story listening task, where narrative alternated with tones. We performed a series of whole-brain regression analyses applying composite, subscale, and individual reading-related items from the validated StimQ-P measure of home cognitive environment as explanatory variables for neural activation. RESULTS: Higher reading exposure (StimQ-P Reading subscale score) was positively correlated (P , .05, corrected) with neural activation in the left-sided parietal-temporal-occipital association cortex, a hubregion supporting semantic language processing, controlling for household income. CONCLUSIONS: In preschool children listening to stories, greater home reading exposure is positively associated with activation of brain areas supporting mental imagery and narrative comprehension, controlling for household income. These neural biomarkers may help inform eco-bio-developmental models of emergent literacy. WHATS KNOWN ON THIS SUBJECT: The American Academy of Pediatrics recommends parent-child reading from infancy through at least kindergarten, the span of maximal brain growth. Home literacy environment, including reading behaviors and access to books, has been shown to promote oral language and print concepts. WHAT THIS STUDY ADDS: Home reading environment is positively associated with activation of brain areas supporting narrative comprehension and mental imagery in preschool children. This offers novel insight into the neurobiological foundations of emergent literacy and potential effect of shared reading during early childhood. a Division of General and Community Pediatrics, b Pediatric Neuroimaging Research Consortium, c Communication Sciences Research Center, and d Division of Radiology, Cincinnati Childrens Hospital Medical Center, Cincinnati, Ohio; and e Department of Pediatrics, New York University School of Medicine and Bellevue Hospital Center, New York, New York Dr Hutton conceptualized and designed the study, performed all data analysis, and drafted the initial manuscript and subsequent revisions; Dr Horowitz-Kraus provided guidance on study design and analysis, assisted with coordination of data collection, and reviewed and revised the manuscript; Dr Mendelsohn served as national outside facilitator and mentor for this project, provided advice on the use of the StimQ measure, and reviewed and revised the manuscript; Dr DeWitt provided guidance on the study design and reviewed and revised the manuscript; Dr Holland designed and secured funding for the parent neuroimaging study and acquired imaging and behavioral data in all participants; he was also responsible for image and behavioral data quality control, database archiving and rst-level analysis of fMRI data and participated in the study design for addition of reading measures and supervised collection of the StimQ questionnaire; and all authors approved the nal manuscript as submitted. ARTICLE PEDIATRICS Volume 136, number 3, September 2015 by guest on June 8, 2018 www.aappublications.org/news Downloaded from

Home Reading Environment and Brain Activation in Preschool Children Listening …pediatrics.aappublications.org/content/pediatrics/early/... · Home Reading Environment and Brain

Embed Size (px)

Citation preview

Page 1: Home Reading Environment and Brain Activation in Preschool Children Listening …pediatrics.aappublications.org/content/pediatrics/early/... · Home Reading Environment and Brain

Home Reading Environment and BrainActivation in Preschool ChildrenListening to StoriesJohn S. Hutton, MDa, Tzipi Horowitz-Kraus, PhDa,b,c, Alan L. Mendelsohn, MDe, Tom DeWitt, MDa, Scott K. Holland, PhDb,c,d,the C-MIND Authorship Consortium

abstract BACKGROUND AND OBJECTIVES: Parent-child reading is widely advocated to promote cognitivedevelopment, including in recommendations from the American Academy of Pediatrics to beginthis practice at birth. Although parent-child reading has been shown in behavioral studies toimprove oral language and print concepts, quantifiable effects on the brain have not beenpreviously studied. Our study used blood oxygen level–dependent functional magneticresonance imaging to examine the relationship between home reading environment and brainactivity during a story listening task in a sample of preschool-age children. We hypothesized thatwhile listening to stories, children with greater home reading exposure would exhibit higheractivation of left-sided brain regions involved with semantic processing (extraction of meaning).

METHODS: Nineteen 3- to 5-year-old children were selected from a longitudinal study of normalbrain development. All completed blood oxygen level–dependent functional magneticresonance imaging using an age-appropriate story listening task, where narrative alternatedwith tones. We performed a series of whole-brain regression analyses applying composite,subscale, and individual reading-related items from the validated StimQ-P measure of homecognitive environment as explanatory variables for neural activation.

RESULTS: Higher reading exposure (StimQ-P Reading subscale score) was positively correlated (P ,

.05, corrected) with neural activation in the left-sided parietal-temporal-occipital associationcortex, a “hub” region supporting semantic language processing, controlling for household income.

CONCLUSIONS: In preschool children listening to stories, greater home reading exposure ispositively associated with activation of brain areas supporting mental imagery and narrativecomprehension, controlling for household income. These neural biomarkers may help informeco-bio-developmental models of emergent literacy.

WHAT’S KNOWN ON THIS SUBJECT: The AmericanAcademy of Pediatrics recommends parent-childreading from infancy through at leastkindergarten, the span of maximal brain growth.Home literacy environment, including readingbehaviors and access to books, has been shownto promote oral language and print concepts.

WHAT THIS STUDY ADDS: Home readingenvironment is positively associated with activationof brain areas supporting narrative comprehensionand mental imagery in preschool children. Thisoffers novel insight into the neurobiologicalfoundations of emergent literacy and potentialeffect of shared reading during early childhood.

aDivision of General and Community Pediatrics, bPediatric Neuroimaging Research Consortium, cCommunicationSciences Research Center, and dDivision of Radiology, Cincinnati Children’s Hospital Medical Center, Cincinnati,Ohio; and eDepartment of Pediatrics, New York University School of Medicine and Bellevue Hospital Center, NewYork, New York

Dr Hutton conceptualized and designed the study, performed all data analysis, and drafted theinitial manuscript and subsequent revisions; Dr Horowitz-Kraus provided guidance on study designand analysis, assisted with coordination of data collection, and reviewed and revised themanuscript; Dr Mendelsohn served as national outside facilitator and mentor for this project,provided advice on the use of the StimQ measure, and reviewed and revised the manuscript;Dr DeWitt provided guidance on the study design and reviewed and revised the manuscript;Dr Holland designed and secured funding for the parent neuroimaging study and acquired imagingand behavioral data in all participants; he was also responsible for image and behavioral dataquality control, database archiving and first-level analysis of fMRI data and participated in the studydesign for addition of reading measures and supervised collection of the StimQ questionnaire; andall authors approved the final manuscript as submitted.

ARTICLE PEDIATRICS Volume 136, number 3, September 2015 by guest on June 8, 2018www.aappublications.org/newsDownloaded from

Page 2: Home Reading Environment and Brain Activation in Preschool Children Listening …pediatrics.aappublications.org/content/pediatrics/early/... · Home Reading Environment and Brain

Emergent literacy is defined as theskills, knowledge, and attitudessupporting reading and writing thataccrue from infancy.1 Although organicreading disability (dyslexia) affects anestimated 5% to 12% of US children,2

the majority of illiteracy is preventable,attributable to inadequate resources,motivation, and/or stimulationrequired to learn to read.3 As parentsare “a child’s first and most importantteachers,”4 the quality of cognitivestimulation in the home, especiallybefore school entry, strongly influencesachievement and health outcomes.5–8

Children’s books are catalysts forparent-child engagement duringsensitive developmental stages whenbrain growth and plasticity aremaximal.9,10 They provide broader,more grammatically correctvocabulary and range of subject matterthan everyday conversation, especiallyin low-socioeconomic status (SES)households.11,12 Given these factors,the American Academy of Pediatrics(AAP) recommends shared readingbeginning at birth, citing direct, lastingbenefits for the developing brain,13

a claim echoed by many advocacygroups.14

While behavioral evidence affirmsmoderate to large benefits of sharedreading on a subset of emergentliteracy skills (oral language and printconcepts) through kindergarten,5,15

quantifiable effects on the brain havenot been previously studied. Similarly,interventions improving homeliteracy environment, a variablydefined measure of reading behaviorsand access to books, have been shownto improve oral language and schoolreadiness,16–20 althoughneurobiological mechanisms have yetto be described. Neuroimaging offersa means to address these knowledgegaps, informing an eco-bio-developmental model of emergentliteracy incorporating genetic,environmental, and neurobiologicalfactors.21–25 Such models have beenadvocated by the AAP and NationalInstitutes of Health25 and are

especially valuable for youngchildren, where behavioral measurescan underestimate the effects oflearning and experience on thedeveloping brain.10,26 Neuroimaginghas been extensively applied indyslexia research (albeit in olderchildren and adults), identifyingactivation patterns associated withreading difficulty and response tointervention,2,27–29 as well as helpingdefine the mature readingnetwork.30,31 Only recently has high-resolution neuroimaging been appliedin younger, preliterate children,32

most often in the context of normallanguage development.33,34 Howlanguage networks become “ready”for reading and to what extent theyare influenced by home literacyenvironment or interventions duringthe critical pre-kindergarten period,however, are unclear.

For our study, a sample of 3- to5-year-old children underwent bloodoxygen level dependent functionalmagnetic resonance imaging (BOLDfMRI) using a story listening task.35,36

This task requires the application ofemergent literacy skills supportingsemantic processing (extraction ofmeaning), including vocabulary andlistening comprehension.37–40 Givenbehavioral evidence,1,5 wehypothesized that children with morestimulating home environments,particularly shared reading exposure,would show more robust activation inbrain areas supporting these skills.The semantic network includes left-sided inferior frontal, middletemporal, inferior parietal, and lateraloccipital lobes.35,39,41 We predictedthat differential activation within thisnetwork would remain significantafter controlling for householdincome, a common confounder instudies of cognitive ability.42–44

METHODS

Participants

All participants in this analysis wereenrolled in a longitudinal study of

normal brain development at ourinstitution (Cincinnati MR Imaging ofNeuroDevelopment; C-MIND).45

Inclusion criteria for C-MIND are asfollows: full-term gestation, healthy,right-handed, native English speakers,and no standard contraindications toMRI. By design, the C-MIND cohort isdemographically diverse (38%nonwhite, 55% female, medianhousehold income $42 500), intendedto reflect the US population. At thetime of our study, 23 childrenbetween 3 and 5 years of age hadcompleted BOLD fMRI whileperforming a story listening task, inaccordance with the C-MIND protocol.Of these, we were able to contact 19families (82.6%) for enrollment andsurvey administration. Despitemultiple attempts, we were unable tocontact the other 4 families, whowere excluded. Informed consent wasobtained from each child’s custodialparent, families were compensatedfor time and travel, and our study wasapproved by the Cincinnati Children’sHospital Medical Center InstitutionalReview Board.

Behavioral Measures

Cognitive stimulation in the homewas assessed using the preschoolversion of the StimQ (StimQ-P),46

which was administered toa custodial parent via telephone orduring C-MIND follow-up visits bya trained clinical researchcoordinator. Time elapsed betweenfMRI scan and StimQ administrationranged from 0 to 20 months (10 68.8). The StimQ-P is validated for ages36 to 72 months47 and involvesmostly “yes/no” questions. Threesubscales were used: (1) Reading,reflecting access to books, frequencyof shared reading, and variety ofbooks read; (2) Parental Involvementin Developmental Advance (PIDA),reflecting the teaching of specificconcepts such as letters; and (3)Parental Verbal Responsivity (PVR),reflecting verbal interaction. Parentswere also asked to report the age of

PEDIATRICS Volume 136, number 3, September 2015 467 by guest on June 8, 2018www.aappublications.org/newsDownloaded from

Page 3: Home Reading Environment and Brain Activation in Preschool Children Listening …pediatrics.aappublications.org/content/pediatrics/early/... · Home Reading Environment and Brain

initiation of reading to their child,which is not included in the StimQ-P.

fMRI Acquisition Specifications andPreliminary Analyses

Details of techniques used toacclimatize children to the MRIacquisition process are described byVannest et al.32 Details of BOLD MRIacquisition specifications used in theC-MIND study are described inSchmithorst et al.35,48 Details of datapreprocessing for the C-MIND studyare described in Sroka et al.49 Allchildren were awake and nonsedatedduring MRI scans. Voxel size used foracquisition and analysis was 3 3 3 34 mm. We used the FEAT (fMRI ExpertAnalysis Tool) modality of FSL (fMRI-Brain Software Library, Oxford, UnitedKingdom) for all group mean andhigher-level regression analyses.50

fMRI Story Listening Task and GroupMean Analysis

The story listening task consists of 10alternating blocks of active andcontrol conditions (5 each) of 64seconds’ duration. During the “active”condition, a series of 5 recordedstories of 9 to 10 sentences each readin a female voice was presented viaheadphones. The stories weredesigned by a speech pathologist withconsistent vocabulary and syntaxappropriate for young children(download: https://www.irc.cchmc.org/software/pedaudio.php). Thecontrol condition consisted ofnonspeech tones in a range offrequencies simulating human speechto control for baseline acousticprocessing. Subjects closed their eyesor saw a blank screen during scanning.

During task presentation, the MRIscanner continuously acquired BOLD-weighted scans, covering the entirebrain with 24 slices at 4-secondintervals. Image time series data wereentered into a general linear modelfor “first-level,” voxelwise analysis,using the story and tone intervals asthe regressor of interest. Contrastmaps (stories . tones activation)were converted to z score maps with

statistical threshold of P , .05,applying a false discovery rate (FDR)correction for multiple voxelcomparisons across the brain. Usingthese, we obtained a whole-brain,group mean activation map for our 19subjects, representing mean neuralactivation listening to stories, minusactivation listening to tones (ie,activation attributable to the storytask, excluding general acousticprocessing).

Regression With StimQ-P and OtherPredictors

For each subject, the z score maprepresenting the contrast of (stories. tones) was used as the dependentvariable in a series of “higher-level”regression analyses, applying StimQ-Pscores (Reading, PIDA, PVR,Composite) or responses to individualquestions as the explanatory variable.Income category (low/not low) wasapplied as a binary covariate whensignificant neural activation wasfound, with household income,200% of the 2015 Federal PovertyGuidelines,51 adjusted for householdsize, defined as low income (seeTable 1).52 Subject age and genderwere considered as covariates butexcluded because no significantcorrelation was found between neuralactivation and either variable.Regression maps of neural activation(stories . tones), along withsummary statistics for size, intensity,and location of activation clusters,were reported for all significantresults, using a threshold forstatistical significance of P , .05applying FDR correction. TheFSLView50 package was used toidentify brain areas corresponding toactive clusters in normalized, three-dimensional, Montreal NeurologicInstitute (MNI) coordinate space,53

using the Harvard-Oxford CorticalStructural Atlas (2-mm scale).

RESULTS

Demographic characteristics for oursample are described in Table 1.

StimQ-P and Other BehavioralPredictors

A summary of StimQ-P subscale andcomposite scores, and reported age ofinitiation of shared reading aredescribed in Table 2 and Fig 1.

Group Mean Activation for theNarrative Comprehension Task

Group mean activation for thenarrative condition compared withbaseline tones (all voxels P , .05,FDR correction) involved bilateral,left-lateralized cortical andsubcortical regions involved withacoustic, phonological, and semanticlanguage processing (see Fig 2), asdescribed by Karunanayaka et al.54

Regression of Neural Activation WithStimQ-P Scores and Other Predictors

Applying linear regression, StimQ-PReading subscale scores werepositively correlated with higheractivation in a confluent region of left-sided, posterior cortex involving theoccipital fusiform, lateral occipital,

TABLE 1 Demographic Characteristics ofC-MIND Sample Subjects

Characteristic n %

Sample 19 100Age (y)3+ 10 524+ 6 325+ 3 16

GenderMale 8 42Female 11 58

Annual household income (US$),5000 0 05000–10 000 1 510–15 000 1 515 000–25 000 2 1125 000–35 000 1 535 000–50 000 2 1150 000–75 000 4 2175 000–100 000 4 21100 000–150 000 2 11.150 000 2 11

Household income levelBelow 200% poverty (low) 7 37Above 200% poverty 12 63

Children in the household1 3 162–3 12 634–5 3 166 1 5

468 HUTTON et al by guest on June 8, 2018www.aappublications.org/newsDownloaded from

Page 4: Home Reading Environment and Brain Activation in Preschool Children Listening …pediatrics.aappublications.org/content/pediatrics/early/... · Home Reading Environment and Brain

posterior inferior temporal, posteriormiddle temporal, posterior cingulate,and angular gyri, and left precuneus,as illustrated in Fig 3 (all voxels P ,

.05, FDR correction). Collectively,these areas reside within the parietal-temporal-occipital (PTO) associationcortex, which supports multimodalsemantic processing, especially forlanguage.41,55 An exception is theposterior cingulate gyrus, which plays

a role in semantic processing andother functions, including memoryencoding41 and visual attention.56

The correlation between StimQ-PReading subscale score and neuralactivation within the left PTO cortexremained consistent and significantwhen expanding the statistical modelto control for household income asa binary covariate (low/not low).Activation clusters were of similar

intensity, with slight to moderatedecreases in size, as shown in Fig 4(all voxels P , .05, FDR correction).The largest decreases were inposterior cingulate, inferior temporal,occipital fusiform, and the mostsuperior lateral occipital areas. Fig 5displays orthogonal sagittal, coronal,and axial slice views (origin x = –34y = –66, z = 14, MNI coordinate space)to more clearly illustrate the anatomicextent of this PTO activation cluster.

No significant correlation was foundbetween brain activation during thestory listening task and other StimQ-Psubscales, StimQ-P composite, age ofinitiation of reading, or months ofreading exposure (initiation to scan).

DISCUSSION

“Biological embedding” describes thelong-term impact on braindevelopment resulting from thequality of cognitive stimulation andnurturing during early childhood.6,57

Learning to read involves theintegration of a formidable array ofskills sequentially and efficiently,5

supported by language, visual, andassociation brain networks, thegrowth and plasticity of which peakin the first few years of life.58,59

During this critical prekindergartenperiod, children are highly vulnerableto disparities in cognitive stimulation,especially spoken language, as well astoys and books promotingconstructive parent-childengagement.12,58,60 Many childrenarrive at school at a significantdisadvantage in reading readiness,and it is clear that those who are poorreaders in first grade61 are unlikely tocatch up with peers, at great societalcost.62 This underscores the need foreffective interventions applied asearly as possible, when brainnetworks are most amenable tochange.10,58,62

Our findings support our hypothesisthat while listening to stories, youngchildren from more stimulating homereading environments more robustly

TABLE 2 StimQ-P and Specific Reading-Related Item Scores

StimQ-P Possible Mean SD Min Max

Reading 19 17.8 2.0 13 19PIDA 15 11.6 2.2 8 15PVR 7 5.8 1.2 3 7Composite 41 35.2 3.7 27 41

Specific ItemsAge (mo) initiation of reading n/a 5.0 5.5 0 24Months of reading exposure n/a 43.0 9.5 30 63Children’s books in the home n/a 162.0 112.8 10 400Reading nights/wk 7 6.0 1.9 2 7

Summary of StimQ-P subscale, Composite, and reading-related item scores. Total possible score (where applicable),mean, SD, minimum (Min), and maximum (Max) are presented. Individual questions other than age of initiation of readingare part of the StimQ-P Reading subscale.

FIGURE 1StimQ-P Subscale and Composite scores. Histograms and density curves for StimQ-P scores. Meanand SD are provided, with a dashed vertical line for each mean. The Reading subscale reflectsparent-child reading materials and behaviors (maximum score 19); PIDA measures parental in-volvement teaching specific skills (maximum 15); PVR indicates parent-child verbal interaction(maximum 7).

PEDIATRICS Volume 136, number 3, September 2015 469 by guest on June 8, 2018www.aappublications.org/newsDownloaded from

Page 5: Home Reading Environment and Brain Activation in Preschool Children Listening …pediatrics.aappublications.org/content/pediatrics/early/... · Home Reading Environment and Brain

engage neural circuitry supportingnarrative comprehension,a foundational component ofemergent literacy.63 Specifically,children in our study with higherStimQ-P Reading scores showedgreater activation in the left parietal-temporal-occipital (PTO) associationcortex, a “hub” region facilitatingsemantic processing.41,64 OutboundPTO connections include limbic areasinvolved with long-term memory (eg,hippocampus) and assigning

emotional value to experiences, andprefrontal executive function areas,each integral for learning.65

“Recycling” their role in orallanguage, areas within the PTO arerecruited for reading, facilitatingefficient assignment of meaning toletters and words.41,66,67 The angulargyrus (located in the inferior parietallobe) at the core of the PTO isparticularly noteworthy and plays anintegral role in this process.23,41,68

Although not observed in our

subjects, hypoactivation of theangular gyrus during reading taskshas been cited as a biomarker fordyslexia, with potential applicationfor early identification andremediation.27,69

Importantly, PTO activation in oursubjects associated with homereading environment reflectsrecruitment of oral language skillssupporting context andcomprehension (semantics), notword-level decoding. This is

FIGURE 2Group mean activation map for the story listening task. Group mean BOLD fMRI activation map (stories . tones) in 3- to 5-year-old children (N = 19). Allvoxels significant at P , .05 (FDR corrected), slice thickness 5 mm for contiguous slices. Slices range from z = –28 to z = 74 in MNI coordinate space.Color scale ranges from t = 1.25 (cooler) to 4 (hotter). Radiologic orientation, left = right, right = left.

470 HUTTON et al by guest on June 8, 2018www.aappublications.org/newsDownloaded from

Page 6: Home Reading Environment and Brain Activation in Preschool Children Listening …pediatrics.aappublications.org/content/pediatrics/early/... · Home Reading Environment and Brain

consistent with behavioral evidencefor the influence of parent-childreading exclusively on “outside-in”oral language skills (understandingoutside of the word itself) describedby Whitehurst et al.1,5 Vocabulary isamong the most important of theseskills,70 shown to be influenced byhome reading environment17 andrecently found to be positivelyassociated with left angular gyrusactivation during our story listening

task in young children.49 Thus, PTOactivation may offer potential asa biomarker of oral language ability(the outside-in domain of emergentliteracy), although further studies areneeded to clarify how the PTO isintegrated into the reading network.That home reading environment wasnot associated with activation ofbrain areas supporting phonologicalprocessing (“inside-out” decodingskills) in our study reinforces

behavioral evidence5 that these skillsseem largely dependent on explicitinstruction.15 Additional research inthis area is also needed.

Higher StimQ Reading scores wereassociated with particularly robustactivation in occipital areas within thePTO cortex, notably lateral occipitalgyrus and precuneus. Schmithorstet al attributed activation in theseareas during the story listening task(when no visual stimulus is

FIGURE 3Regression map (stories . tones activation) with StimQ-P Reading subscale score as explanatory variable. Regression map for the story listening task(stories . tones) in 3- to 5-year-old children (N = 19), with StimQ-P Reading score as explanatory variable. Cluster size 4087 voxels significant at P , .05(FDR corrected), z score local maxima 3.25 to 3.44. Five-millimeter slices from z = –28 to z = 74 in MNI coordinate space. Color scale from t = 1.25 (cooler)to 4 (hotter). Radiologic orientation, left = right, right = left.

PEDIATRICS Volume 136, number 3, September 2015 471 by guest on June 8, 2018www.aappublications.org/newsDownloaded from

Page 7: Home Reading Environment and Brain Activation in Preschool Children Listening …pediatrics.aappublications.org/content/pediatrics/early/... · Home Reading Environment and Brain

presented) to mental imagery.35 Theability to “see” what is being heardhas been extensively shown inbehavioral studies to improvenarrative comprehension and recall.71

This was affirmed in a recent,imaging-based study that foundpositive association between greateractivation of lateral occipital cortexduring the story listening task in 5- to7-year-old children and higherreading scores at age 11.40

Recruitment of left-sided PTO areasduring high-imagery tasks has alsobeen described in adults.72 Thus, ourresults provide a neurobiologicalcorrelate to the enchantment oftenseen at preschool story time,especially in children with greaterpractice at home: activation of PTOcircuits to visualize and understandwhat is happening. It is intriguing toinfer that children better able torecruit these circuits and apply

mental imagery may better managethe transition from picture- to text-based books as they advance inschool. Conversely, those with lesspractice seeing and understanding,with consequently underdevelopedvisual-semantic neural infrastructure,may be more likely to struggle.

Surprisingly, we did not findsignificant association betweenneural activation and PIDA, PVR, orComposite StimQ-P scores. We view

FIGURE 4Regression map (stories . tones activation) with StimQ-P Reading subscale score as explanatory variable, controlling for household income. Regressionmap for the story listening task (stories. tones) in 3- to 5-year-old children (N = 19), with StimQ-P Reading score as explanatory variable, controlling forhousehold income. Cluster size 2467 voxels significant at P , .05 (FDR corrected), z score local maxima 3.15 to 3.38. Five-millimeter slices from z = –28 toz = 74 in MNI coordinate space. Color scale from t = 1.25 (cooler) to 4 (hotter). Radiologic orientation, left = right, right = left.

472 HUTTON et al by guest on June 8, 2018www.aappublications.org/newsDownloaded from

Page 8: Home Reading Environment and Brain Activation in Preschool Children Listening …pediatrics.aappublications.org/content/pediatrics/early/... · Home Reading Environment and Brain

FIGURE 5Triplanar view of neural activation (stories . tones) with StimQ-P Reading subscale score as explanatory variable, controlling for household income.Orthogonal triplanar view (origin x = –34, y = –66, z = 14, MNI coordinate space) of activation for the story listening task (stories . tones), with StimQ-PReading score as explanatory variable, controlling for household income. Cluster size 2467 voxels significant at P , .05 (FDR corrected). Color scaleranges from t = 1.25 (cooler) to 4 (hotter). All views in radiologic orientation, left = right, right = left, with sagittal plane viewed from the right.

PEDIATRICS Volume 136, number 3, September 2015 473 by guest on June 8, 2018www.aappublications.org/newsDownloaded from

Page 9: Home Reading Environment and Brain Activation in Preschool Children Listening …pediatrics.aappublications.org/content/pediatrics/early/... · Home Reading Environment and Brain

this as likely a byproduct of subscalethemes. The StimQ-P Reading subscalemeasures reading-specific practices,assessing frequency, access to books,and variety of subject matter. As theseopportunities and experiences aredirectly related to story listening,small variations, even with scoresskewed toward the maximum, seemadequate to differentiate neuralactivation in subjects performing thistask. By contrast, PIDA measures theteaching of specific cognitive skills andPVR assesses parent-childconversation, each possibly moreapplicable to abilities other thannarrative comprehension. Anycomposite effect was likely diluted byPIDA and PVR scores.

Contrary to our hypothesis, age ofinitiation of shared reading andmonths of reading exposure were notassociated with neural activation,although behavioral studies haveassociated these with home literacyorientation.9,73 This may beattributable to responses skewed bysocial desirability and/or recall biasor, more likely, greater predictivepower of the validated StimQ-Pmeasure. The Reading subscalecaptures 3 aspects of home readingenvironment: frequency (4 points,including for days/week), access tobooks (5 points, including for numberof books in the home), and variety ofcontent (10 points, for different typesof books, eg concepts, beliefs,relationships). The relative influenceof each of these factors on neuralactivation supporting narrativeprocessing is complex, likelyinvolving behaviors and proclivitiesthat are more difficult to capture, andmerits further study. For example,greater variety may reflectdifferences in how books are shared,in addition to how many and howoften. This qualitative aspect ofreading aloud (notably, dialogicreading in which the child activelyparticipates) has been shown toprovide a disproportionate share of itsbenefits, behaviorally74,75 and possiblyin terms of neurobiological effect.

Our study has several importantstrengths. Our sample of 3- to 5-year-old children is considerably youngerthan most neuroimaging-basedstudies of emergent literacy,27 withample sample size76 drawn froma diverse cohort, applying anestablished fMRI paradigm andvalidated measure of home cognitiveenvironment. Our findings areconsistent with current models oflanguage and reading brainnetworks,23 complementary withbehavioral models of emergentliteracy,15 and robust in controllingfor household income, a commonconfounder in studies of cognitivedevelopment.77 Using an innovativeapproach, our results also informclinical practice during a foundationalstage of development in which“preventative medicine” may offermaximal benefit. For example,because there is evidence that theReach Out and Read interventionadvocated in AAPrecommendations13 improves homereading environment,13,18 and wehave found that home readingenvironment is positively associatedwith activation of brain circuitssupporting semantic processing,logical inference leads us to speculatethat early home literacy interventionsuch as Reach Out and Read,consistently applied, has the potentialto enhance the development of thesebrain circuits.

Our study also has several limitations.Although it used existing imaging andbehavioral data, the StimQ-P wasretrospectively administered, witha variable time from fMRI acquisition.Thus, recall and social desirabilitybias are possible, with parentsoverreporting reading practices. Thatsaid, household reading behaviorshave been shown to be stable duringthe preschool period, tempering suchrecall effects.78 Families agreeing toparticipate in our study may be morelikely to constructively engage intheir child’s development(participation bias); although C-MINDis not advertised in the context of

reading, its demographic mix isdiverse by design, and all subjectswho were able to be contacted agreedto participate, minimizing theprospect of self-selection. Theexclusion of four low-SES familieswas a consequence of unreliablecontact information (ie, phone out ofservice), shifting our demographicprofile toward higher SES, although37% of our sample was low-income.Our high reported StimQ-P subscalescores suggest potential ceilingeffects, although the Reading subscaleprovided sensitivity ideal for our task.Finally, whereas our results showrobust association between homereading environment and neuralactivation, our cross-sectional designcannot establish causation.Longitudinal studies are needed todiscern the influence of sharedreading on emergent literacy skillsbeginning in infancy, especially inlow-SES populations. Such studiesmay help us better understand howthe developing brain responds tovarious platforms, styles (notablydialogic reading), and interventions atdifferent developmental stages, aswell as identify children at-risk asearly as possible to ensure the bestpossible outcome for all.

CONCLUSIONS

Our study used fMRI to for the firsttime demonstrate an associationbetween home reading environmentand activation of specific brainregions supporting emergent literacyduring the prekindergarten period.While listening to stories, childrenwith greater home reading exposureshowed significantly higher activationin areas within the left-sided,multimodal association cortex, whichfacilitates mental imagery andextraction of meaning (semanticprocessing). Critical for oral language,this region is later integrated into thereading network,35,54 withhypoactivation, a biomarker ofreading disability.27 This studysuggests a novel, neurobiological

474 HUTTON et al by guest on June 8, 2018www.aappublications.org/newsDownloaded from

Page 10: Home Reading Environment and Brain Activation in Preschool Children Listening …pediatrics.aappublications.org/content/pediatrics/early/... · Home Reading Environment and Brain

correlate to oral language skillsfostered by parent-child reading inearly childhood, offering insight intohow this practice may shape thedeveloping brain, and informing aneco-bio-developmental model ofemergent literacy and its promotion.

ACKNOWLEDGMENTS

The authors thank Claire Sroka andSarah Finucane for their invaluablehelp conducting the StimQ surveysand organizing behavioral data. Wealso thank Molly Grainger for herassistance with the FSL fMRI analysispackage.

The C-MIND database used for thisstudy can be accessed free of chargeat https://research.cchmc.org/c-mind. The authors thank the C-MINDAuthorship Consortium:

Scott K. Holland, PhD, PediatricNeuroimaging Research Consortium,Department of Radiology, Departmentof Otolaryngology, CommunicationSciences Research Center, CincinnatiChildren’s Hospital Medical Center,University of Cincinnati, Cincinnati,Ohio; Jennifer Vannest, PhD, PediatricNeuroimaging Research Consortiumand Division of Neurology,Department of Pediatrics, CincinnatiChildren’s Hospital Medical Center,University of Cincinnati, Cincinnati,Ohio; Vincent J. Schmithorst, PhD,Pediatric Neuroimaging ResearchConsortium, Cincinnati Children’sHospital Medical Center, University ofCincinnati, Cincinnati, Ohio, andPediatric Imaging Research Center,Department of Radiology, Children’sHospital of Pittsburgh of UPMC,Pittsburgh, Pennsylvania; MekibibAltaye, PhD, Pediatric NeuroimagingResearch Consortium and Division ofBiostatistics and Epidemiology,Department of Pediatrics, CincinnatiChildren’s Hospital Medical Center,University of Cincinnati, Cincinnati,Ohio; Gregory Lee, PhD, PediatricNeuroimaging Research Consortiumand Department of Radiology,

Cincinnati Children’s Hospital MedicalCenter, University of Cincinnati,Cincinnati, Ohio; Luis Hernandez-Garcia, PhD, Functional MRILaboratory, Department ofBiomedical Engineering, University ofMichigan, Ann Arbor, Michigan;Michael Wagner, PhD, PediatricNeuroimaging Research Consortiumand Division of BiomedicalInformatics, Deparment of Pediatrics,Cincinnati Children’s Hospital MedicalCenter, University of Cincinnati,Cincinnati, Ohio; Arthur Toga, PhD,Laboratory of Neuroimaging andDepartments of Ophthalmology,Neurology, Psychiatry, and theBehavioral Sciences, Radiology andEngineering, Keck School of Medicineof USC, Los Angeles, California;Jennifer Levitt, MD, Psychiatry andBiobehavioral Sciences, UCLA, LosAngeles, California; Anna W. Byars,PhD, Pediatric NeuroimagingResearch Consortium and Division ofNeurology, Department of Pediatrics,Cincinnati Children’s Hospital MedicalCenter, University of Cincinnati,Cincinnati, Ohio; Andrew Dimitrijevic,PhD, Department of Otolaryngologyand Communication SciencesResearch Center, Cincinnati Children’sHospital Medical Center, University ofCincinnati, Cincinnati, Ohio; NicolasFelicelli, Division of BiomedicalInformatics, Department of Pediatrics,Cincinnati Children’s Hospital MedicalCenter, University of Cincinnati,Cincinnati, Ohio; Darren Kadis, PhD,Pediatric Neuroimaging ResearchConsortium and Division ofNeurology, Department of Pediatrics,Cincinnati Children’s Hospital MedicalCenter, University of Cincinnati,Cincinnati, Ohio; James Leach, MD,Department of Radiology, CincinnatiChildren’s Hospital Medical Center,University of Cincinnati, Cincinnati,Ohio; Katrina Peariso, MD, PhD,Division of Neurology, Department ofPediatrics, Cincinnati Children’sHospital Medical Center, University ofCincinnati, Cincinnati, Ohio; ElenaPlante, PhD, Deptartment of Speech,

Language, and Hearing Sciences,University of Arizona, Tucson,Arizona; Akila Rajagopal, MS,Pediatric Neuroimaging ResearchConsortium, Cincinnati Children’sHospital Medical Center, University ofCincinnati, Cincinnati, Ohio; AndrewRupert, MS, Division of BiomedicalInformatics, Department of Pediatrics,Cincinnati Children’s Hospital MedicalCenter, University of Cincinnati,Cincinnati, Ohio; Mark Schapiro, MD,Pediatric Neuroimaging ResearchConsortium and Division ofNeurology, Department of Pediatrics,Cincinnati Children’s Hospital MedicalCenter, University of Cincinnati,Cincinnati, Ohio; Karen Crawford,Laboratory of Neuroimaging, KeckSchool of Medicine of USC, LosAngeles, California; Ronald Ly,Psychiatry and BiobehavioralSciences, UCLA, Los Angeles,California; Katherine Narr, PhD,Department of Neurology, UCLA, LosAngeles, California; Petros Petrosyan,Laboratory of Neuroimaging, KeckSchool of Medicine of USC, LosAngeles, California; J.J. Wang, PhD,Department of Neurology, UCLA, LosAngeles, California; and Lisa Freund,PhD, Eunice Kennedy ShriverNational Institute of Child Health andHuman DevelopmentBethesda, Maryland.

ABBREVIATIONS

AAP: American Academy ofPediatrics

BOLD: blood oxygenlevel–dependent

C-MIND: Cincinnati MR Imaging ofNeuroDevelopment

FDR: false discovery ratefMRI: functional magnetic

resonance imagingMNI: Montreal Neurologic InstitutePIDA: Parental Involvement in

Developmental AdvancePVR: Parental Verbal ResponsivityPTO: parietal-temporal-occipitalSES: socioeconomic status

PEDIATRICS Volume 136, number 3, September 2015 475 by guest on June 8, 2018www.aappublications.org/newsDownloaded from

Page 11: Home Reading Environment and Brain Activation in Preschool Children Listening …pediatrics.aappublications.org/content/pediatrics/early/... · Home Reading Environment and Brain

www.pediatrics.org/cgi/doi/10.1542/peds.2015-0359

DOI: 10.1542/peds.2015-0359

Accepted for publication Jun 2, 2015

Address correspondence to John S. Hutton, MD, Division of General and Community Pediatrics, Cincinnati Children’s Hospital Medical Center, 3333 Burnet Ave, MLC

7035, Cincinnati, OH 45229. E-mail: [email protected]

PEDIATRICS (ISSN Numbers: Print, 0031-4005; Online, 1098-4275).

Copyright © 2015 by the American Academy of Pediatrics

FINANCIAL DISCLOSURE: The authors have indicated they have no financial relationships relevant to this article to disclose.

FUNDING: All phases of this study were supported by National Institutes of Health contract HHSN275200900018C to the Pediatric Functional Neuroimaging Research

Network, a Ruth L Kirschstein National Research Service Award, and an Academic Pediatric Association Young Investigator Award (YIA) for Primary Care Strategies

for the Promotion of Early Literacy and School Readiness Supported by Reach Out and Read.

POTENTIAL CONFLICT OF INTEREST: John Hutton received an Academic Pediatric Association Young Investigator Award supported by Reach out and Read in support

of this project, although this award provided no direct financial or salary compensation. Tom DeWitt has served on the board of directors of Reach Out and Read

from 2009-present but receives no financial compensation. Alan Mendelsohn serves as Senior Research Advisor for Child Development and School Readiness for

Reach Out and Read but receives no financial compensation. The other authors have indicated they have no potential conflicts of interest to disclose.

REFERENCES

1. Whitehurst GJ, Lonigan CJ. Childdevelopment and emergent literacy.Child Dev. 1998;69(3):848–872

2. Norton ES, Beach SD, Gabrieli JD.Neurobiology of dyslexia. Curr OpinNeurobiol. 2015;30:73–78

3. Cree A. The Economic and Social Cost ofIlliteracy: A Snapshot of Illiteracy ina Global Context. World LiteracyFoundation; April 2012

4. Ramey CT, Ramey SL. Early interventionand early experience. Am Psychol. 1998;53(2):109–120

5. National Early Literacy Panel. DevelopingEarly Literacy: Report of the NationalEarly Literacy Panel. Washington, DC:National Institute for Literacy; 2008

6. Bradley RH, Corwyn RF. Socioeconomicstatus and child development. Annu RevPsychol. 2002;53:371–399

7. Cates CB, Dreyer BP, Berkule SB, WhiteLJ, Arevalo JA, Mendelsohn AL. Infantcommunication and subsequentlanguage development in children fromlow-income families: the role of earlycognitive stimulation. J Dev BehavPediatr. 2012;33(7):577–585

8. Sanders LM, Federico S, Klass P, AbramsMA, Dreyer B. Literacy and child health:a systematic review. Arch PediatrAdolesc Med. 2009;163(2):131–140

9. Zuckerman B, Augustyn M. Books andreading: evidence-based standard ofcare whose time has come. Acad Pediatr.2011;11(1):11–17

10. Knudsen EI. Sensitive periods in thedevelopment of the brain and behavior. JCogn Neurosci. 2004;16(8):1412–1425

11. Duursma E, Augustyn M, Zuckerman B.Reading aloud to children: the evidence.Arch Dis Child. 2008;93(7):554–557

12. Hart B, Risley T. Meaningful Differencesin the Everyday Experience of YoungAmerican Children. Baltimore, MD: PaulBrookes Publishing Company; 1995

13. High PC, Klass P; Council on EarlyChildhood. Literacy promotion: anessential component of primary carepediatric practice. Pediatrics. 2014;134(2):404–409

14. Too STF. Hillary Rodham Clinton to UnveilEarly Literacy Toolkit for Pediatriciansand Parents at American Academy ofPediatrics National Conference. 2014.Available at: http://toosmall.org/news/press-releases. Accessed February 1,2015

15. Storch SA, Whitehurst GJ. Oral languageand code-related precursors to reading:evidence from a longitudinal structuralmodel. Dev Psychol. 2002;38(6):934–947

16. Golova N, Alario AJ, Vivier PM, RodriguezM, High PC. Literacy promotion forHispanic families in a primary caresetting: a randomized, controlled trial.Pediatrics. 1999;103(5 pt 1):993–997

17. Mendelsohn AL, Mogilner LN, Dreyer BP,et al. The impact of a clinic-basedliteracy intervention on languagedevelopment in inner-city preschoolchildren. Pediatrics. 2001;107(1):130–134

18. Needlman R, Toker KH, Dreyer BP, Klass P,Mendelsohn AL. Effectiveness ofa primary care intervention to supportreading aloud: a multicenter evaluation.Ambul Pediatr. 2005;5(4):209–215

19. Sharif I, Rieber S, Ozuah PO. Exposure toReach Out and Read and vocabularyoutcomes in inner city preschoolers.J Natl Med Assoc. 2002;94(3):171–177

20. Whaley SE, Jiang L, Gomez J, Jenks E.Literacy promotion for familiesparticipating in the women, infants andchildren program. Pediatrics. 2011;127(3):454–461

21. National Scientific Council on theDeveloping Child. Working Paper #5: TheTiming and Quality of Early ExperiencesCombine to Shape Brain Architecture.Cambridge, MA: Harvard UniversityCenter on the Developing Child; 2008

22. Carreiras M, Seghier ML, Baquero S,et al. An anatomical signature forliteracy. Nature. Oct 15 2009;461(7266):983–986

23. Dehaene S. Reading in the Brain: TheNew Science of How We Read. New York,NY: Penguin Books; 2010

24. Shonkoff JP. Building a newbiodevelopmental framework to guidethe future of early childhood policy. ChildDev. 2010;81(1):357–367

25. American Academy of Pediatrics Councilon Early Brain and Child Development.Eco-Bio-Developmental Model of HumanHealth and Disease. 2014. Available at:http://www.aap.org/en-us/advocacy-and-

476 HUTTON et al by guest on June 8, 2018www.aappublications.org/newsDownloaded from

Page 12: Home Reading Environment and Brain Activation in Preschool Children Listening …pediatrics.aappublications.org/content/pediatrics/early/... · Home Reading Environment and Brain

policy/aap-health-initiatives/EBCD/Pages/Eco-Bio-Developmental.aspx. AccessedFebruary 1, 2015

26. Hoeft F, Ueno T, Reiss AL, et al. Predictionof children’s reading skills usingbehavioral, functional, and structuralneuroimaging measures. BehavNeurosci. 2007;121(3):602–613

27. Shaywitz SE, Shaywitz BA. Payingattention to reading: the neurobiology ofreading and dyslexia. Dev Psychopathol.2008;20(4):1329–1349

28. Blau V, Reithler J, van Atteveldt N, et al.Deviant processing of letters and speechsounds as proximate cause of readingfailure: a functional magnetic resonanceimaging study of dyslexic children. Brain.2010;133(pt 3):868–879

29. Maisog JM, Einbinder ER, Flowers DL,Turkeltaub PE, Eden GF. A meta-analysisof functional neuroimaging studies ofdyslexia. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 2008;1145:237–259

30. Dehaene S, Pegado F, Braga LW, et al.How learning to read changes thecortical networks for vision andlanguage. Science. 2010;330(6009):1359–1364

31. Brem S, Bach S, Kucian K, et al. Brainsensitivity to print emerges whenchildren learn letter-speech soundcorrespondences. Proc Natl Acad SciUSA. 2010;107(17):7939–7944

32. Vannest J, Rajagopal A, Cicchino ND,et al; CMIND Authorship Consortium.Factors determining success of awakeand asleep magnetic resonance imagingscans in nonsedated children.Neuropediatrics. 2014;45(6):370–377

33. Szaflarski JP, Schmithorst VJ, Altaye M,et al. A longitudinal functional magneticresonance imaging study of languagedevelopment in children 5 to 11 yearsold. Ann Neurol. 2006;59(5):796–807

34. Kuhl PK. Early Language Learning andLiteracy: Neuroscience Implications forEducation. Mind Brain Educ. 2011;5(3):128–142

35. Schmithorst VJ, Holland SK, Plante E.Cognitive modules utilized for narrativecomprehension in children: a functionalmagnetic resonance imaging study.Neuroimage. 2006;29(1):254–266

36. Vannest JJ, Karunanayaka PR, Altaye M,et al. Comparison of fMRI data frompassive listening and active-response

story processing tasks in children.J Magn Reson Imaging. 2009;29(4):971–976

37. Holland SK, Vannest J, Mecoli M, et al.Functional MRI of language lateralizationduring development in children. Int JAudiol. 2007;46(9):533–551

38. Pugh KR, Landi N, Preston JL, et al. Therelationship between phonological andauditory processing and brainorganization in beginning readers. BrainLang. 2013;125(2):173–183

39. Berl MM, Duke ES, Mayo J, et al.Functional anatomy of listening andreading comprehension duringdevelopment. Brain Lang. 2010;114(2):115–125

40. Horowitz-Kraus T, Vannest JJ, Holland SK.Overlapping neural circuitry fornarrative comprehension and proficientreading in children and adolescents.Neuropsychologia. 2013;51(13):2651–2662

41. Binder JR, Desai RH, Graves WW, ConantLL. Where is the semantic system? Acritical review and meta-analysis of 120functional neuroimaging studies. CerebCortex. 2009;19(12):2767–2796

42. Noble KG, Tottenham N, Casey BJ.Neuroscience perspectives ondisparities in school readiness andcognitive achievement. The Future ofChildren. 2005;15(1):71–89

43. Noble KG, McCandliss BD, Farah MJ.Socioeconomic gradients predictindividual differences in neurocognitiveabilities. Dev Sci. 2007;10(4):464–480

44. Noble KG, McCandliss BD. Readingdevelopment and impairment:behavioral, social, and neurobiologicalfactors. J Dev Behav Pediatr. 2005;26(5):370–378

45. Holland SKVJ, Schmithorst VJ, Wagner M.Overview of the Pediatric FunctionalNeuroimaging Research NetworkProject, Methods, Tools and Database: a.k.a CMIND. Paper presented at NewHorizons in Human Brain Imaging: AFocus on the Neuroimaging of BrainDevelopment; May 5–7, 2014; Turtle Bay,HI

46. The Bellevue Project for Early LanguageL; Education Success (BELLE), STIMQCognitive Home Environment. http://pediatrics.med.nyu.edu/developmental/

research/the-belle-project/stimq-cognitive-home-environment, 2014

47. Dreyer BPMA, Tamis-LeMonda CS.Assessing the child’s cognitive homeenvironment through parental report:reliability and validity. Early Dev Parent.1996;5:271–287

48. Schmithorst VJ, Vannest J, Lee G, et al;CMIND Authorship Consortium. Evidencethat neurovascular coupling underlyingthe BOLD effect increases with ageduring childhood. Hum Brain Mapp.2015;36(1):1–15

49. Sroka MC, Vannest J, Maloney TC,Horowitz-Kraus T, Byars AW, Holland SK;CMIND Authorship Consortium.Relationship between receptivevocabulary and the neural substrates forstory processing in preschoolers. BrainImaging Behav. 2015;9(1):43–55

50. Smith SM, Jenkinson M, Woolrich MW,et al. Advances in functional andstructural MR image analysis andimplementation as FSL. Neuroimage.2004;23(S1):208–219

51. Office of the Secretary. Annual Update ofthe HHS Poverty Guidelines In:Department of Health and HumanServices, ed. Vol 80. Washington, DC:Federal Register; 2015

52. The Henry J. Kaiser Family Foundation.The Kaiser Family Foundation StateHealth Facts. 2015. Available at: http://kff.org/statedata. Accessed June 9, 2015

53. Brett MCK, Cusack R, Lancaster J. Usingthe Talairach Atlas with the MNItemplate. Neuroimage. 2001;13(6):85

54. Karunanayaka PR, Holland SK,Schmithorst VJ, et al. Age-relatedconnectivity changes in fMRI data fromchildren listening to stories.Neuroimage. 2007;34(1):349–360

55. Sabsevitz DS, Medler DA, Seidenberg M,Binder JR. Modulation of the semanticsystem by word imageability.Neuroimage. 2005;27(1):188–200

56. Leech R, Sharp DJ. The role of theposterior cingulate cortex in cognitionand disease. Brain. 2014;137(pt 1):12–32

57. Hertzman C. The biological embedding ofearly experience and its effects onhealth in adulthood. Ann N Y Acad Sci.1999;896:85–95

58. National Research Council and Instituteof Medicine Committee on Integrating

PEDIATRICS Volume 136, number 3, September 2015 477 by guest on June 8, 2018www.aappublications.org/newsDownloaded from

Page 13: Home Reading Environment and Brain Activation in Preschool Children Listening …pediatrics.aappublications.org/content/pediatrics/early/... · Home Reading Environment and Brain

the Science of Early ChildhoodDevelopment. From Neurons toNeighborhoods: The Science of EarlyChildhood Development. Washington, DC:National Academy Press; 2000

59. Redcay E, Haist F, Courchesne E.Functional neuroimaging of speechperception during a pivotal period inlanguage acquisition. Dev Sci. 2008;11(2):237–252

60. Tomopoulos S, Dreyer BP, Tamis-LeMondaC, et al. Books, toys, parent-childinteraction, and development in youngLatino children. Ambul Pediatr. 2006;6(2):72–78

61. Gabrieli JD. Dyslexia: a new synergybetween education and cognitiveneuroscience. Science. 2009;325(5938):280–283

62. The Annie E. Casey Foundation. DoubleJeopardy: How Third Grade ReadingSkills and Poverty Influence High SchoolGraduation. Baltimore, MD: The Annie E.Casey Foundation; 2012

63. Cornelissen P, Hansen P, Kringelbach M,Pugh K. The Neural Basis of Reading. NewYork, NY: Oxford University Press; 2010

64. Ganis G, Thompson WL, Kosslyn SM. Brainareas underlying visual mental imageryand visual perception: an fMRI study.Brain Res Cogn Brain Res. 2004;20(2):226–241

65. Wright A. Higher Cortical Functions:Association and Executive Processing.Neuroscience Online: An ElectronicTextbook for the Neurosciences. Houston,

TX: Department of Neurobiology andAnatomy at University of Texas MedicalSchool at Houston; 2014

66. Dehaene S. Inside the letterbox: howliteracy transforms the human brain.Cerebrum. 2013;2013:7

67. Shankweiler D, Mencl WE, Braze D, TaborW, Pugh KR, Fulbright RK. Readingdifferences and brain: corticalintegration of speech and print insentence processing varies with readerskill. Dev Neuropsychol. 2008;33(6):745–775

68. Pugh KR, Mencl WE, Shaywitz BA, et al.The angular gyrus in developmentaldyslexia: task-specific differences infunctional connectivity within posteriorcortex. Psychol Sci. 2000;11(1):51–56

69. Hoeft F, Meyler A, Hernandez A, et al.Functional and morphometric braindissociation between dyslexia andreading ability. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA.2007;104(10):4234–4239

70. Duff FJ, Reen G, Plunkett K, Nation K. Doinfant vocabulary skills predict school-age language and literacy outcomes?J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2015; (Jan):4

71. National Reading Panel. Report of theNational Reading Panel: TeachingChildren to Read: An Evidence-BasedAssessment of the Scientific ResearchLiterature on Reading and ItsImplications for Reading Instruction.Washington, DC: Eunice Kennedy ShriverNational Institute of Child Health andHuman Development; 2000

72. Just MA, Newman SD, Keller TA, McEleneyA, Carpenter PA. Imagery in sentencecomprehension: an fMRI study.Neuroimage. 2004;21(1):112–124

73. Needlman R, Silverstein M. Pediatricinterventions to support reading aloud:how good is the evidence? J Dev BehavPediatr. 2004;25(5):352–363

74. Whitehurst GJ. Dialogic reading: aneffective way to read to preschoolers.2013. Available at: http://www.readingrockets.org/article/400.Accessed February 1, 2015

75. Swanson E, Wanzek J, Petscher Y, et al.A synthesis of read-aloud interventionson early reading outcomes amongpreschool through third graders at riskfor reading difficulties. J Learn Disabil.2011;44(3):258–275

76. Desmond JE, Glover GH. Estimatingsample size in functional MRI (fMRI)neuroimaging studies: statistical poweranalyses. J Neurosci Methods. 2002;118(2):115–128

77. Raizada RD, Kishiyama MM. Effects ofsocioeconomic status on braindevelopment, and how cognitiveneuroscience may contribute to levellingthe playing field. Front Hum Neurosci.2010;4:3

78. National Household Education Survey.Children’s School Readiness Skills: 1993and 2007. Washington, DC: US CensusBureau Statistical Abstract of the UnitedStates; 2012

478 HUTTON et al by guest on June 8, 2018www.aappublications.org/newsDownloaded from

Page 14: Home Reading Environment and Brain Activation in Preschool Children Listening …pediatrics.aappublications.org/content/pediatrics/early/... · Home Reading Environment and Brain

originally published online August 10, 2015; Pediatrics Holland and the C-MIND Authorship Consortium

John S. Hutton, Tzipi Horowitz-Kraus, Alan L. Mendelsohn, Tom DeWitt, Scott K.Listening to Stories

Home Reading Environment and Brain Activation in Preschool Children

ServicesUpdated Information &

015-0359http://pediatrics.aappublications.org/content/early/2015/08/05/peds.2including high resolution figures, can be found at:

Subspecialty Collections

_subhttp://www.aappublications.org/cgi/collection/preventative_medicinePreventive Medicinemilestones_subhttp://www.aappublications.org/cgi/collection/growth:development_Growth/Development Milestonesal_issues_subhttp://www.aappublications.org/cgi/collection/development:behaviorDevelopmental/Behavioral Pediatricsfollowing collection(s): This article, along with others on similar topics, appears in the

Permissions & Licensing

http://www.aappublications.org/site/misc/Permissions.xhtmlin its entirety can be found online at: Information about reproducing this article in parts (figures, tables) or

Reprintshttp://www.aappublications.org/site/misc/reprints.xhtmlInformation about ordering reprints can be found online:

by guest on June 8, 2018www.aappublications.org/newsDownloaded from

Page 15: Home Reading Environment and Brain Activation in Preschool Children Listening …pediatrics.aappublications.org/content/pediatrics/early/... · Home Reading Environment and Brain

originally published online August 10, 2015; Pediatrics Holland and the C-MIND Authorship Consortium

John S. Hutton, Tzipi Horowitz-Kraus, Alan L. Mendelsohn, Tom DeWitt, Scott K.Listening to Stories

Home Reading Environment and Brain Activation in Preschool Children

http://pediatrics.aappublications.org/content/early/2015/08/05/peds.2015-0359located on the World Wide Web at:

The online version of this article, along with updated information and services, is

ISSN: 1073-0397. 60007. Copyright © 2015 by the American Academy of Pediatrics. All rights reserved. Print the American Academy of Pediatrics, 141 Northwest Point Boulevard, Elk Grove Village, Illinois,has been published continuously since 1948. Pediatrics is owned, published, and trademarked by Pediatrics is the official journal of the American Academy of Pediatrics. A monthly publication, it

by guest on June 8, 2018www.aappublications.org/newsDownloaded from