26
http://pom.sagepub.com/ Psychology of Music http://pom.sagepub.com/content/42/4/599 The online version of this article can be found at: DOI: 10.1177/0305735613484548 2014 42: 599 originally published online 22 August 2013 Psychology of Music Patrik N. Juslin, László Harmat and Tuomas Eerola What makes music emotionally significant? Exploring the underlying mechanisms Published by: http://www.sagepublications.com On behalf of: Society for Education, Music and Psychology Research can be found at: Psychology of Music Additional services and information for http://pom.sagepub.com/cgi/alerts Email Alerts: http://pom.sagepub.com/subscriptions Subscriptions: http://www.sagepub.com/journalsReprints.nav Reprints: http://www.sagepub.com/journalsPermissions.nav Permissions: What is This? - Aug 22, 2013 OnlineFirst Version of Record - Jun 19, 2014 Version of Record >> at UNIV FEDERAL DO PARANA on September 30, 2014 pom.sagepub.com Downloaded from at UNIV FEDERAL DO PARANA on September 30, 2014 pom.sagepub.com Downloaded from

What makes music emotionally significant

Embed Size (px)

DESCRIPTION

2014 - Juslin Eerola hatmat - What makes music emotionally significant

Citation preview

  • http://pom.sagepub.com/Psychology of Music

    http://pom.sagepub.com/content/42/4/599The online version of this article can be found at:

    DOI: 10.1177/0305735613484548 2014 42: 599 originally published online 22 August 2013Psychology of Music

    Patrik N. Juslin, Lszl Harmat and Tuomas EerolaWhat makes music emotionally significant? Exploring the underlying mechanisms

    Published by:

    http://www.sagepublications.com

    On behalf of:

    Society for Education, Music and Psychology Research

    can be found at:Psychology of MusicAdditional services and information for

    http://pom.sagepub.com/cgi/alertsEmail Alerts:

    http://pom.sagepub.com/subscriptionsSubscriptions:

    http://www.sagepub.com/journalsReprints.navReprints:

    http://www.sagepub.com/journalsPermissions.navPermissions:

    What is This?

    - Aug 22, 2013OnlineFirst Version of Record

    - Jun 19, 2014Version of Record >>

    at UNIV FEDERAL DO PARANA on September 30, 2014pom.sagepub.comDownloaded from at UNIV FEDERAL DO PARANA on September 30, 2014pom.sagepub.comDownloaded from

  • Psychology of Music2014, Vol. 42(4) 599 623

    The Author(s) 2013Reprints and permissions:

    sagepub.co.uk/journalsPermissions.navDOI: 10.1177/0305735613484548

    pom.sagepub.com

    What makes music emotionally significant? Exploring the underlying mechanisms

    Patrik N. JuslinUppsala University, Sweden

    Lszl HarmatSemmelweis University, Hungary

    Tuomas EerolaUniversity of Jyvskyl, Finland

    AbstractA common approach to study emotional reactions to music is to attempt to obtain direct links between musical surface features such as tempo and a listeners response. However, such an analysis ultimately fails to explain why emotions are aroused in the listener. In this article, we propose an alternative approach, which seeks to explain musical emotions in terms of a set of underlying mechanisms that are activated by different types of information in musical events. We illustrate this approach by reporting a listening experiment, which manipulated a piece of music to activate four mechanisms: brain stem reflex; emotional contagion; episodic memory; and musical expectancy. The musical excerpts were played to 20 listeners, who were asked to rate their felt emotions on 12 scales. Pulse rate, skin conductance, and facial expressions were also measured. Results indicated that target mechanisms were activated and aroused emotions largely as predicted by a multi-mechanism framework.

    Keywordsaffect, expectancy, listening, psychophysiology, theory

    Music moves us. Indeed, it may evoke anything from mere arousal and basic emotions such as happiness and sadness to complex emotions such as nostalgia. Such emotional responses add personal significance to the processes of music perception and cognition, and constitute one of the main reasons for engaging with music (Juslin, 2011).

    Corresponding author:Patrik N. Juslin, Department of Psychology, Uppsala University, Box 1225, SE 751 42 Uppsala, Sweden.Email: [email protected]

    484548 POM42410.1177/0305735613484548Psychology of MusicJuslin et al.2013

    Article

    at UNIV FEDERAL DO PARANA on September 30, 2014pom.sagepub.comDownloaded from

  • 600 Psychology of Music 42(4)

    Evidence of musical emotions comes from many strands of research1 (for a review, see Juslin & Sloboda, 2013). In accordance with the multi-component view of emotions (Oatley, Keltner, & Jenkins, 2006), music may influence feelings (Pike, 1972), expressions (Witvliet & Vrana, 2007), psychophysiological reactions (Krumhansl, 1997), brain activation (Brown, Martinez, & Parsons, 2004), action tendencies (Fried & Berkowitz, 1979), as well as various indirect mea-sures (Vstfjll, 2010). Moreover, there is some evidence of a synchronization of the various components (Lundqvist, Carlsson, Hilmersson, & Juslin, 2009). However, the question of why music arouses emotions has largely remained a mystery.

    Explaining musical emotions

    In the past, the most common approach to understand what causes an emotion to music has been to map factors (in the music, the listener, and the situation) that somehow influence emotions. Such research may provide valuable information about the conditions under which musical emotions occur. Yet producing lists of factors that affect emotions does not actually constitute an explanation of why they occur; this lesson was learned long time ago in general research on emotions, where investigators soon discovered that it is difficult to find objective situation predictors that will invariably influence different people in the same way: different people tend to react in different ways to the same stimulus. This realization forms the basis of theories of emotion causation (for a review, see Moors, 2009). To explain why an emotion occurs, we have to understand how the emotion induction process works that is, the kind of information-processing that leads to the arousal of a specific emotion. A theory of emotions must be able to explain both why a given stimulus arouses an emotion (elicitation) and why the aroused emotion is of a particular kind (differentiation). The fundamental psychological process by which this is achieved is referred to as the underlying mechanism.

    The most commonly discussed mechanism previously is cognitive appraisal (Scherer, 1999). This notion refers to a process whereby an emotion is aroused in a person because an event is interpreted as having implications for the persons goals in life (e.g., in terms of goal congruence, coping potential, or compatibility with social norms). The problem is that music does not usually have implications for life goals.2 In fact, results so far suggest that cognitive appraisal is rarely the cause of musical emotions (e.g., Juslin, Liljestrm, Vstfjll, Barradas, & Silva, 2008). Thus it seems necessary to consider alternative mechanisms which are more relevant in the case of music. Much research has proceeded as if there were only one causal route to emotions, but emotions can be aroused in a number of ways (Izard, 1993).

    Thus a better approach to explain musical emotions than to produce lists of influencing factors might be to develop theories of the underlying mechanisms. Meyer (1956), one of the pioneers in the music and emotion field, recognized early on the importance of psychological theory: Given no theory as to the relation of musical stimuli to affective responses, observed behavior can provide little information as to either the nature of the stimulus, the signifi-cance of the response, or the relation between them (p. 10). Meyer himself chose to concen-trate on the role of musical expectancy, eloquently elaborating his theory in what is probably the most cited book in the field. Still, as pointed out by Huron (2006), music can also evoke emotions through many other means apart from whether the sounds are expected or not (p. 365). And ironically, though Meyer was clear about the importance of psychological the-ory in explaining emotions, his ideas would eventually lead to a neglect of such theorizing:

    at UNIV FEDERAL DO PARANA on September 30, 2014pom.sagepub.comDownloaded from

  • Juslin et al. 601

    given a competent listener, the study of emotions could be comfortably reduced to the study of musical structure.

    Unfortunately, emotional responses to music can never be explained merely in terms of the musical structure what matters is how psychological mechanisms of specific listeners in spe-cific contexts engage with selected aspects of the musical structure. Yet a recent search of the literature indicated that few articles have proposed or tested any theory about mechanisms (Juslin & Vstfjll, 2008). In general, emotions to music have been studied without respect to how they were aroused. Researchers have tried to obtain direct links between surface features and aroused emotions. This approach has prevented us from explaining individual differences (e.g., that the same piece of music can evoke different emotions in different listeners), and has led to overly simple conclusions (e.g., that fast tempo evokes positive emotions; Gomez & Danuser, 2007, p. 380). The solution to this dilemma, we argue, is a theory-based approach to musical emotions that goes beyond mere surface features.

    A unified theoretical framework: BRECVEMA

    Although mechanisms have been mostly neglected in previous studies, several scholars have discussed possible mechanisms, typically focusing on a few possibilities (e.g., Berlyne, 1971; Dowling & Harwood, 1986; Meyer, 1956; Sloboda, 1998). Scherer and Zentner (2001) offered a more extensive overview of mechanisms and moderators of emotion induc-tion that might be involved, but the most comprehensive attempt to outline a set of mecha-nisms is the BRECVEMA framework (Juslin & Vstfjll, 2008; Juslin, Liljestrm, Vstfjll, & Lundqvist, 2010), which currently (Juslin, in press) features eight mechanisms (besides appraisal):3

    (1) Brain stem reflex: a hard-wired attention response to simple acoustic features such as extreme or increasing loudness or speed (Simons, 1996);

    (2) Rhythmic entrainment: a gradual adjustment of an internal body rhythm (e.g., heart rate) towards an external rhythm in the music (Harrer & Harrer, 1977);

    (3) Evaluative conditioning: a regular pairing of a piece of music and other positive or nega-tive stimuli leading to a conditioned association (Blair & Shimp, 1992);

    (4) Contagion: an internal mimicry of the perceived voice-like emotional expression of the music (Juslin, 2001);

    (5) Visual imagery: inner images of an emotional character conjured up by the listener through a metaphorical mapping of the musical structure (Osborne, 1980);

    (6) Episodic memory: a conscious recollection of a particular event from the listeners past triggered by the music (Baumgartner, 1992);

    (7) Musical expectancy: a reaction to the gradual unfolding of the musical structure and its expected or unexpected continuation (Meyer, 1956); and

    (8) Aesthetic judgment: a subjective evaluation of the aesthetic value of the music based on an individual set of weighted criteria (Juslin, in press).

    By synthesizing theory and data from many domains mostly outside music, Juslin and Vstfjll (2008) were able to develop the first set of hypotheses that may help researchers to distinguish among the mechanisms. The hypotheses concern such aspects as the information focus, key brain regions, representations, and extent of cultural impact. (For an update of the hypotheses, see Juslin, in press.) One important implication is that it may not be sufficient to

    at UNIV FEDERAL DO PARANA on September 30, 2014pom.sagepub.comDownloaded from

  • 602 Psychology of Music 42(4)

    study musical emotions in general. In order for data to contribute in a cumulative fashion to our knowledge, we need to specify, as far as possible, the mechanism involved in each case.

    Empirical studies of mechanisms

    As noted earlier, few studies so far have investigated specific mechanisms empirically. Steinbeis, Koelsch, and Sloboda (2006) tested Meyers (1956) theory of musical expectancy, and Baumgartner (1992) and Janata, Tomic, and Rakowski (2007) surveyed memories linked to music. A broader range of psychological mechanisms were surveyed by Juslin et al. (2008) and Dingle, Savill, Fraser, and Vieth (2011). However, self-reports of mechanisms from field studies must be interpreted with caution. The listeners may sometimes be unaware of the true cause of their emotions (Fox, 2008, p. 36). Thus mechanisms that are more implicit in nature (e.g., con-ditioning, expectancy) might be underreported relative to mechanisms that are more salient in conscious experience (e.g., episodic memory). Most importantly, field data do not enable research-ers to draw strong conclusions with regard to causal relationships because of insufficient exper-imental control. Thus it is necessary to conduct experiments in a laboratory setting where specific mechanisms can be manipulated so as to produce immediate effects on behavioral measures.

    There are at least two complementary experimental strategies in exploring mechanisms. First, one can attempt to find existing pieces of music that feature characteristics relevant to a particular mechanism. The use of real music makes it easier to arouse an intense emotion in listeners. However, the internal validity may be limited, due to a lack of experimental control. Thus a second approach is to directly manipulate features of the music (as well as the listener and the situation) to activate a specific mechanism, using highly controlled synthesized (or re-synthesized) pieces. Although such manipulations could suffer from low ecological validity, they allow stronger conclusions regarding causal relationships. In the present experiment, we explored a manipulative approach.4

    To separate the effects of different mechanisms, one must be able to activate, as well as sup-press, specific mechanisms in each case. This may be done in three ways (Juslin, in press). First, one can select or manipulate pieces of music in such a manner as to provide or withhold infor-mation required for a certain mechanism to be activated, while leaving or removing other infor-mation (the principle of information selection). Second, one can design the test procedure in such a way that it will prevent the type of information-processing required for a mechanism to be activated (the principle of interference). Third, one can manipulate listeners, by creating specific memories during the experimental procedure prior to presenting the target stimulus (the prin-ciple of procedural history), in order to investigate mechanisms such as conditioning. In the pres-ent experiment, we focused on the first of these principles.

    Rationale for the present study

    The main aim of this experiment was to make a first attempt to manipulate some of the mecha-nisms underlying musical emotions. Specifically, we intended to test whether it would be pos-sible to selectively activate the four mechanisms brain stem reflex, contagion, episodic memory, and musical expectancy, respectively, such that we could predict to some extent the emotions (e.g., happiness) that would be aroused. The selection of mechanisms was based on practical considerations: given the complexity of the manipulations, it would not be possible to test all mechanisms at the same time, and some mechanisms (e.g., visual imagery) seemed more diffi-cult to manipulate than others (e.g., expectancy). All music excerpts were based on an original

    at UNIV FEDERAL DO PARANA on September 30, 2014pom.sagepub.comDownloaded from

  • Juslin et al. 603

    piece which was manipulated in different ways to activate each target mechanism. Before describing the experiment, we summarize the four mechanisms:

    1. Brain stem reflex refers to a process whereby an emotion is evoked by music because some fundamental acoustic characteristic of the music is taken by the brain stem to indicate a potentially important and urgent event that needs attention. In music, this may involve sounds that are sudden, loud, dissonant, or feature a fast or rapidly increasing temporal pattern. Brain stem reflexes are quick, automatic, and unlearned. A response to an audi-tory event suggesting danger can be emitted as early as at the level of the inferior col-liculus of the brain stem (e.g., Brandao, Melo, & Cardoso, 1993). As a consequence, the brain stem reflex can quickly evoke arousal so that attention can be selectively directed at sensory stimuli of potential importance. In the present study, a brain stem reflex was aroused by inserting an extreme sound event into the original piece (for details, see Method section). We expected this version to arouse mainly surprise in listeners (Simons, 1996).

    2. Emotional contagion refers to a process whereby the emotion is evoked because the listener perceives the emotional expression of the music, and then mimics or mirrors this expression internally (Juslin, 2001). Contagion has primarily been studied in regard to facial expression (e.g., Hatfield, Cacioppo, & Rapson, 1994), although Neumann and Strack (2000) found evidence of contagion from emotion in speech. Because music often includes acoustic patterns that are similar to those that occur in emotional speech (e.g., Juslin & Laukka, 2003), it has been theorized that we get aroused by voice-like aspects of musical expression through a process in which a neural module responds quickly and automatically to specific stimulus features that lead us to mimic the perceived emotion internally (Juslin, 2001, p. 329). In the present study, a contagion reaction was produced by featuring an expressive, voice-like cello timbre within a sad emotional expression (Juslin & Laukka, 2003, pp. 792995). We expected this version to arouse mainly sadness in listeners.

    3. Episodic memory refers to a process whereby an emotion is aroused in a listener when the music evokes a memory of a specific event in life (Baumgartner, 1992; Janata et al., 2007). When the memory is evoked so is the emotion linked to this memory. Such emo-tions can be intense, maybe because the psychophysiological pattern to the original event is stored along with the memory trace (e.g., Lang, 1979). Listeners use music to remind them of valued past events, indicating that music often serves an important nostalgic function in everyday-life contexts (e.g., Sloboda, ONeill, & Ivaldi, 2001). One might expect episodic memories evoked by music to be particularly emotionally vivid for music from adolescence and young adulthood. (For empirical support, see Schulkind, Hennis, & Rubin, 1999.) In this study, episodic memories were evoked by inserting a short musical quote from the soundtrack of a well-known and appreciated movie series (which came out when the participants were in their adolescence/young adulthood) into the original piece. We expected this version to arouse mainly nostalgia and happiness in listeners.

    4. Musical expectancy refers to a process whereby an emotion is evoked when a specific fea-ture of the music violates, delays, or confirms a listeners schematic expectations about the continuation of the music, as famously theorized by Leonard Meyer (1956). The expectations are based on the listeners previous experiences of the same style. Although Meyers theory is highly regarded, it has not stimulated much research with regard to emotions. A seminal study by Steinbeis et al. (2006) demonstrated, however, that viola-tions of musical expectancies may also evoke emotions in listeners. In the present study,

    at UNIV FEDERAL DO PARANA on September 30, 2014pom.sagepub.comDownloaded from

  • 604 Psychology of Music 42(4)

    the musical expectancy mechanism was activated by inserting violations of melodic and harmonic expectations into the original piece. Due to the repeated transgressions of musical expectations, we expected this version to arouse mainly anxiety (Meyer, 1956, p. 27) and irritation (Huron, 2006, p. 348) in listeners.

    In summary, we manipulated a piece of music to selectively activate four mechanisms (brain stem reflex, contagion, episodic memory and musical expectancy). The resulting four musical excerpts were played to 20 listeners, who reported aroused emotions. In accordance with a multi-component view of emotions (e.g., Scherer & Zentner, 2001), we used multiple measures (verbal self-reports, facial expressions, and autonomic activity) to enhance the validity of our conclusions about aroused emotions.

    Method

    Participants

    Twenty listeners (10 males and 10 females, aged 20 61 years, M = 28) took part in the experi-ment. They were either paid or given course credits for their anonymous and voluntary partici-pation. Most of the participants were students who were recruited by means of posters throughout Uppsala University. Sixty percent of them played at least one musical instrument. Forty percent of these had received music education; the rest were self-taught.

    Design

    The experiment used a within-subjects design, with target mechanism as the independent variable (four levels: contagion, brain stem reflex, episodic memory, and musical expectancy) and self-reported feeling (15 scales), facial expression (zygomaticus and corrugator muscles), and autonomic activity (skin conductance and heart rate) as dependent variables.

    Musical material

    The experiment featured four music excerpts which were synthesized using the Vienna Symphonic Library samples to obtain highly realistic performances of music. These excerpts were all based on a short piece by Ernest Bloch (18801959) titled Prayer, from Jewish Life No. 1, composed for cello and piano, with expression marked as andante moderato (circa 80 bpm). This is a slow, lyrical, and expressive piece which has been recorded a few times but is not gener-ally well known. In the present study, we used an excerpt of Prayer consisting of the A1A2B1D1D2 sections of the piece (the original structure is: A1A2B1CD1D2A3A4Coda). The excerpt is roughly 120 seconds in duration.5

    Contagion. The starting point was the contagion version, which served as the template for all the other versions. The contagion mechanism is believed to be activated by a particularly mov-ing emotional expression in the music, and it is assumed that the effect is strengthened by a voice-like lead part, either a real voice or a musical instrument reminiscent of a human voice. It has often been argued that the cello is the closest-sounding instrument to the human voice,6 and previous data suggest that sad performances are perceived as particularly expressive (Jus-lin, 1997, p. 245). Thus a high-quality cello performance with a sad expression was considered

    at UNIV FEDERAL DO PARANA on September 30, 2014pom.sagepub.comDownloaded from

  • Juslin et al. 605

    ideal for this condition. To meet this demand, we used a digital rendition of Prayer created by Jay Bacal (available online at http://www.vsl.co.at/en/67/245/255.vsl). This particular version of the work has been made to resemble a real human performance and includes a number of expressive performance variations in terms of dynamics, micro-timing and articulation styles. (The performance variations were crafted using Vienna Symphonic Library, Solo Strings I). Based on their review of 145 studies of music and speech, Juslin and Laukka (2003, pp. 792795) described patterns of acoustic cues associated with basic emotions, and the present ver-sion of Prayer is consistent with an expression of sadness because it features slow tempo, low sound level, legato articulation, and slow tone attacks. Moreover, vibrato contributes to a con-tagion reaction through its relationship to vocal expression of emotion (e.g., Juslin & Scherer, 2005). Consistent with a mimicry response to the voice-like emotional expression of the music, we expected the contagion version to arouse mainly sadness in listeners.

    Brain stem reflex. The brain stem reflex mechanism is believed to be activated by extreme acoustic features such as high sound level, quick attack, and sharp timbre (Juslin et al., 2010). This mechanism was therefore targeted by inserting a novel sound event into the existing piece. The contagion version was taken as the starting point. However, in this and all other non-con-tagion versions, the voice-like cello playing the melody was replaced by piano samples (PMI Bosendorfer 290 by Vienna Symphonic Library). More importantly, a sudden, loud chord with broad spectrum and quite sharp attack was inserted at the beginning of the 10th bar of the piece. The goal was to mimic naturally occurring brain stem reflex events such as the well-known drum strokes in Joseph Haydns Symphony No. 94 (Surprise) or Gustav Mahlers Sym-phony No. 10, Finale. Special care was taken to calibrate the sound level of the event, although pre-testing indicated that the peak sound level did not quite have to reach the level used in research on the acoustic startle response (Levenson, 2007, p. 163) to produce a reliable effect on the listener. Thus a peak sound level of 72 dB(a) was considered sufficient. Because brain stem reflexes involve local events, we used a shorter excerpt for this mechanism (1 min 30 s) to reduce the time lag from the critical event to the self-report of feelings. We expected the brain stem reflex version to arouse mainly surprise in listeners, consistent with an early response that occurs before any elaborate classification of the sound event has taken place (Simons, 1996).

    Episodic memory. The episodic memory mechanism is thought to be activated by salient melodic themes, which are associated with emotionally charged events that the listener remembers. To evoke music-associated episodic memories, without having to encode them during this experi-ment, we inserted a musical quote from the soundtrack of an extremely well-known movie series, Star Wars. The melodic theme, featured in John Williams original movie soundtrack (1977), was expected to be familiar to many people who grew up with the Star Wars movies over the last three decades. The theme, referred to as Binary sunset, was inserted at the position of the second repetition of the initial theme in Prayer, beginning at bar 5. It should be noted that the tempo and harmony of the theme are very similar to those of Prayer and that the melody has a similarly wistful and sad character to it. However, due to memories associated with the Star Wars movies, we expected this version to arouse mainly nostalgia and happiness in listeners.

    Musical expectancy. The musical expectancy mechanism is believed to be activated by unex-pected melodic, harmonic, or rhythmic sequences (Meyer, 1956). Hence, in order to activate this mechanism, we altered the piece to violate melodic and harmonic expectations (while keep-ing the overall structure and performance nuances intact). Each musical phrase was subjected

    at UNIV FEDERAL DO PARANA on September 30, 2014pom.sagepub.comDownloaded from

  • 606 Psychology of Music 42(4)

    to 13 random transpositions of 6 semitones. The transpositions were carried out by hand, using a logic by which the shape of a melodic phrase was preserved, but its harmonies were rendered more or less unconventional. (The manipulations were evaluated by the authors, in terms of musical plausibility, and slightly resembled the harmonic choices characteristic of Stravinskys serial period.) The result of these manipulations was a decrease in tonal stability. Thus in the normal version, for instance, 65% of the 5-second segments were above the typi-cal key correlation in classical music (r = .66, based on a sample of classical music and using an audio-based key finding algorithm created by Gomez, 2006), whereas in the expectancy version, only 27% of the segments were above the typical correlation. Though Meyer (1956) is often interpreted as saying that expectancy violations mainly induce undifferentiated arousal, Meyer did consider more specific emotions as well. For example, he argued that that uncer-tainty (in music and in real life) might arouse anxiety and apprehension (pp. 2729). Similarly, Huron (2006) argued that the contrarian aesthetic of modernist composers such as Stravin-sky will evoke irritation or unease in most listeners (p. 350). Therefore we expected the musical expectancy version to arouse mainly anxiety and irritation in listeners.

    Acoustic measures

    General acoustic characteristics of all four conditions, extracted using the music information retrieval (MIR) toolbox (Lartillot, Toiviainen, & Eerola, 2008), are presented in Figure 1, along with reference levels based on a large-scale analysis of 482 examples of classical music.

    Experiential measures

    We measured the subjective feeling component of the aroused emotions in listeners by means of a 12-item adjective scale, which was developed at Uppsala University specifically for the mea-surement of emotions to music (see Appendix 1). The scale represents a kind of compromise among the response formats currently used in the music-emotion field (Zentner & Eerola, 2010) since the selected terms include basic emotions characteristic of discrete emotion theo-ries (Izard, 1977), cover all four quadrants of a circumplex model in terms of valence and arousal (Russell, 1980), and feature possibly more music-related terms such as nostalgia, expec-tancy, and awe (Juslin & Laukka, 2004). (The selected terms roughly cover the nine factors of the Geneva Emotional Music Scale [GEMS]-9, proposed by Zentner, Grandjean, & Scherer [2008], but since there exists no validated version of GEMS-9 in Swedish, and the scale lacks terms that were needed in this study [e.g., surprise], we decided to use a customized scale.) The list features the emotions reported most commonly in prevalence studies. In addition to the 12 emotions, listeners also rated their liking and familiarity for each version and whether they experienced any chills (defined as piloerection; gshud in Swedish everyday terminology). All ratings were made on a scale from 0 (not at all) to 4 (a lot), except for chills, which were reported in a dichotomous fashion (see Appendix 1).

    In addition to reporting their feelings, the participants also filled out a second response sheet (MecScale) for each musical excerpt (see Appendix 2). This sheet purported to capture the mech-anisms that had occurred and consisted of eight simple questions, each targeting one of the mechanisms in the BRECVEM framework (Juslin et al., 2010) plus appraisal. The idea was that, although some of the mechanisms are implicit in nature, they might co-occur with subjective impressions that can be reported by listeners. For example, a listener who becomes aroused through the expectancy mechanism might find the music difficult to predict, whereas a listener who becomes aroused through the episodic memory mechanism could report having a

    at UNIV FEDERAL DO PARANA on September 30, 2014pom.sagepub.comDownloaded from

  • Juslin et al. 607

    conscious recollection of a previous event. Though self-reports of this type cannot be taken as veridical, we submit that they can at least complement other indices. After pilot testing and refinement, eight items were included here for exploratory purposes, indexing: (1) brain stem reflex; (2) rhythmic entrainment; (3) episodic memory; (4) conditioning; (5) visual imagery; (6) contagion; (7) musical expectancy; and (8) cognitive appraisal.

    Psychophysiology: Facial expression and autonomic activity

    To enhance the validity of the measurement of emotion, we also measured physiological indi-ces. The goal was to obtain evidence of an emotional response in order to distinguish felt

    Figure 1. Acoustic characteristics of the experimental conditions.Note: RMS SD = variability in the sound amplitude, in terms of the standard deviation of the root mean square energy of the signal; Attack time = the time from the initial note onset to its peak amplitude; Tempo = the speed of the music, expressed in beats per minute; Pulse clarity = an index of how prominent or stable the beat is; Roughness = relates to timbre and describes the sensory dissonance based on a psychoacoustic model that computes the interference within the critical bandwidths; High-frequency energy = a description of timbre that is often interpreted as the brightness or darkness of the sound, calculated as the ratio between high and low frequencies in the spectrum. The reference values were based on an analysis of 482 examples of classical music obtained from the last.fm online database.

    at UNIV FEDERAL DO PARANA on September 30, 2014pom.sagepub.comDownloaded from

  • 608 Psychology of Music 42(4)

    emotions from mere perception of emotions. In the former case, we would expect to discover some changes in physiological indices (as part of an emotional reaction), whereas in the latter case there would be no reason to expect such changes. Furthermore, the goal was to be able to distinguish emotions by locating them in one of the four quadrants of the circumplex model of affect (Russell, 1980). For instance, if listeners report happiness, their psychophysiological responses should suggest high arousal and positive valence. These emotion dimensions should be evident from measures of autonomic activity and facial expressions respectively, as argued in the previous literature (Andreassi, 2007, pp. 248251).

    Psychophysiological indices were obtained using the BIOPAC MP 150 System (Biopac Systems, Santa Barbara, CA) and the AcqKnowledge version 4.1 software. Skin conductance level (SCL) was measured using the GSR100C Electrodermal Activity Amplifier module and EL507 disposable snap electrodes that were placed on the palmar surface of the non-dominant hand, at the thenar and the hypothenar eminences (Fowles et al., 1981). Skin conductance was recorded in microSiemens (mho).

    Pulse rate (PR) was measured based on the arterial pulse pressure, using the PPG 100C Pulse Plethysmogram Amplifier and the TSD200 (Biopac Systems, Santa Barbara, CA) photoplethys-mogram transducer attached to the index finger on the non-dominant hand. The TSD200 uses a matched infrared emitter and photodiode detector that transmits changes in infrared reflec-tance which results from varying blood flow. Band-pass filter was used to remove frequencies below 0.05 Hz and above 10 Hz. Pulse rate was recorded in beats per minute (bpm).

    Bipolar facial electromyography (EMG) recordings were made from the left corrugator and zygomatic muscle regions in accordance with Fridlund and Cacioppos (1986) guidelines. Before attaching the 4 mm miniature surface Ag/AgCl electrodes, filled with EMG gel (GEL 100, Biopac Systems), we cleansed the participants skin to reduce interelectrode impedance. All impedance was reduced to less than 10 k (Fridlund & Cacioppo, 1986). The electrodes were connected to the EMG100C amplifier module with low- and high-pass filters set at 500 Hz and 10 Hz, respectively, and notch filters set at 50 Hz were used to diminish interference with the electric mains. The sampling rate was set at 1.000 Hz. Facial EMG was measured in microvolts (V) and analyzed using the root mean square (RMS).

    Mean values for PR, SCL, and facial EMG (zygomaticus, corrugator) were calculated for base-line and the experimental conditions. Baseline recordings were obtained prior to the listening test during relaxation under silent conditions. During the listening test there was a break between musical excerpts to allow levels to return to baseline before the next stimulus.

    Procedure

    When participants arrived at the laboratory, they were seated in a comfortable armchair and received the following instructions (translated from Swedish):

    Welcome to the music laboratory. You will soon listen to a selection of short pieces of music. After each piece we want you to describe your experience of the music. This should be done in two ways: first, we want you to describe your feelings during the music on a response sheet. This sheet consists of 12 emo-tions. Your task is to rate how much of each emotion you felt on a scale from 0 (not at all) to 4 (a lot). You also report whether you experienced chills, as well as how much you liked the music and how familiar you were with it. Then we want you to fill out a second response sheet featuring eight questions con-cerning other aspects of your music experience. You will also be fitted with some electrodes so that we can conduct physiological measurements. These electrodes are completely harmless and do not emit strong radiation or electricity. However, in order to obtain as accurate measurements as possible, it is

    at UNIV FEDERAL DO PARANA on September 30, 2014pom.sagepub.comDownloaded from

  • Juslin et al. 609

    important that you dont touch any of the electrodes during the experiment. Watches and rings have to be removed and your cell phone must be switched off. First, you will be asked to relax for a while during silence. Then the actual listening test begins. When the playback of a piece of music ends, there will be a brief intermission before the next piece begins, to give you time to fill out the two response sheets. Then you will relax again for a while before the next piece begins. Note that any emotion you may experience during listening need not correspond to the musics emotional expression. That is, you should rate your own emotions, not what the music expresses. After the experiment you will be asked to respond to some background questions.

    Participants were tested individually in a soundproofed room, and listened to the music through a pair of high-quality loudspeakers (Dali Ikon 6 MK2). Sound level was pre-set to a comfortable level and was held constant across participants. Stimulus order was randomized for each participant, whereas the order of rating scales was kept constant across participants. After the listening test, the participants filled out a short questionnaire with regard to various background variables (e.g., age, gender, music education). They were also interviewed about the experiment. However, the participants were not fully de-briefed about the purpose of the experiment until all had been tested, to prevent confounding effects (Neale & Liebert, 1986). An experimental session lasted about 50 minutes.

    Results

    Self-reports: Ratings

    To evaluate the effects of target mechanism on listeners self-reports, we conducted an analysis of variance (ANOVA) with mechanism as the within-subjects factor (four levels) on each rating scale. Table 1 shows the results. As can be seen, mechanism yielded significant effects on all rating scales, except interest-expectancy, disgust-contempt, and pride-confidence. The right-most column presents effect sizes in terms of eta-squared. Beginning with the emotions, the largest effects occurred for the scales sadness-melancholy, anxiety-nervousness, surprise-astonishment, admira-tion-awe, happiness-elation, and nostalgia-longing (p2 .306). Table 1 (lower part) also shows the results from the ANOVAs on the two additional scales liking and familiarity. As seen, mechanism produced significant effects on both scales, though the effect was larger on the liking scale.

    Figure 2 shows means and standard errors for listeners ratings on all scales that showed significant effects of the experimental manipulation in the ANOVAs (see preceding paragraph). We begin by looking at the six emotions related to our predictions, and then look at the addi-tional emotions featured in the response sheet. For the six emotions involved in our predictions, we conducted planned comparisons (t-tests) between the target mechanism and the other three mechanisms to examine whether the target mechanism received the highest ratings. For the remaining six emotions and the ratings of familiarity and liking, we conducted post-hoc tests in the form of Tukeys HSD to explore further contrasts.

    Predicted emotions. Inspection of Figure 2 suggests that the episodic memory version produced the highest mean rating on the happiness-elation scale. Planned comparisons confirmed that the episodic memory version received significantly higher ratings than the brain stem reflex (t = 3.37, p < .01), contagion (t = 5.15, p < .001), and musical expectancy (t = 3.94, p < .001) versions.

    Similarly, on the sadness-melancholy scale (see Figure 2), planned comparisons showed that the contagion version received significantly higher ratings than the brain stem reflex (t = 5.20,

    at UNIV FEDERAL DO PARANA on September 30, 2014pom.sagepub.comDownloaded from

  • 610 Psychology of Music 42(4)

    p < .001), episodic memory (t = 16.66, p < .001), and musical expectancy (t = 13.85, p < .001) versions.

    On the surprise-astonishment scale (see Figure 2), planned comparisons indicated that the brain stem reflex version received higher ratings than the contagion (t = 4.88, p < .001), epi-sodic memory (t = 3.74, p < .01), and musical expectancy (t = 3.38, p < .01) versions.

    Concerning the anger-irritation scale, inspection of Figure 2 suggests that the musical expec-tancy version received the highest mean rating, and planned comparisons revealed that the musical expectancy version received significantly higher ratings of anger-irritation than the epi-sodic memory version (t = 3.86, p < .01). However, the remaining two contrasts were not sig-nificant. Note that the anger-irritation ratings were fairly low overall.

    For nostalgia-longing, planned comparisons revealed that the episodic memory version received significantly higher ratings than the brain stem reflex (t = 3.52, p < .01) and musical expectancy (t = 4.93, p < .001) versions. In contrast, though the episodic memory version did

    Table 1. Analysis of variance for listeners ratings.

    MS F pa eta-squared

    Scale Happiness-elation Mechanism 7.213 8.820 < .0036 .317Sadness-melancholy Mechanism 12.879 15.419 < .0036 .448Surprise-astonishment Mechanism 11.783 9.182 < .0036 .326Calm-contentment Mechanism 6.750 7.657 < .0036 .287Anger-irritation Mechanism 5.513 5.175 < .0036 .214Nostalgia-longing Mechanism 10.100 8.380 < .0036 .306Interest-expectancy Mechanism 0.179 0.174 ns. .009Anxiety-nervousness Mechanism 9.883 9.573 < .0036 .335Love-tenderness Mechanism 8.213 7.598 < .0036 .286Disgust-contempt Mechanism 3.013 4.614 ns. .195Admiration-awe Mechanism 4.983 9.003 < .0036 .322Pride-confidence Mechanism 3.746 4.851 ns. .203Liking Mechanism 12.479 17.429 < .0036 .478Familiarity Mechanism 8.233 6.536 < .0036 .256

    Note: df = mechanism (3), error (57). aBonferroni corrected from = .05 to = .0036.

    at UNIV FEDERAL DO PARANA on September 30, 2014pom.sagepub.comDownloaded from

  • Juslin et al. 611

    Happiness-elation

    0

    1

    2

    3

    4

    Mea

    n ra

    ting

    Sadness-melancholy

    0

    1

    2

    3

    4

    Mea

    n ra

    ting

    Surprise-astonishment

    0

    1

    2

    3

    4

    Mea

    n ra

    ting

    Anger-irritation

    0

    1

    2

    3

    4

    Mea

    n ra

    ting

    Nostalgia-longing

    0

    1

    2

    3

    4

    Mea

    n ra

    ting

    Anxiety-nervousness

    0

    1

    2

    3

    4

    Mea

    n ra

    ting

    Calm-contentment

    0

    1

    2

    3

    4

    Mea

    n ra

    ting

    Love-tenderness

    0

    1

    2

    3

    4

    Mea

    n ra

    ting

    Brain stemMechanism

    ExpectancyMemoryContagion Brain stemMechanism

    ExpectancyMemoryContagion

    Brain stemMechanism

    ExpectancyMemoryContagion Brain stemMechanism

    ExpectancyMemoryContagion

    Brain stemMechanism

    ExpectancyMemoryContagion Brain stemMechanism

    ExpectancyMemoryContagion

    Brain stemMechanism

    ExpectancyMemoryContagion Brain stemMechanism

    ExpectancyMemoryContagion

    (Figure 2. Continued)

    at UNIV FEDERAL DO PARANA on September 30, 2014pom.sagepub.comDownloaded from

  • 612 Psychology of Music 42(4)

    obtain higher mean ratings than the contagion version (see Figure 2), this difference fell short of statistical significance (t = 1.98, p = .06).

    Finally, as regards anxiety-nervousness, planned comparisons revealed that the musical expectancy version produced significantly higher ratings than the episodic memory version (t = 4.24, p < .001). However, the remaining two contrasts were not significant.

    Additional emotions. Post-hoc tests (Tukeys HSD) on the additional emotion scales featured (see Figure 2) revealed significant contrasts for the episodic memory and musical expectancy ver-sions. The episodic memory version produced higher ratings of calm-contentment than the con-tagion (p < .01) and musical expectancy (p < .001) versions. The musical expectancy version, however, received lower ratings of love-tenderness than the brain stem reflex (p < .05), conta-gion (p < .01), and episodic memory (p < .001) versions; and lower ratings of admiration-awe than the brain stem reflex (p < .05), contagion (p < .01), and episodic memory (p < .001) ver-sions (the remaining differences were not statistically significant).

    Familiarity and liking. Figure 2 also presents listeners mean ratings of familiarity and liking as a function of target mechanism. Starting with familiarity, it can be seen that the episodic mem-ory version was rated as familiar by many listeners, whereas the other versions were not.

    0

    1

    2

    3

    4

    Mea

    n ra

    ting

    Admiration-awe Familiarity

    0

    1

    2

    3

    4

    Mea

    n ra

    ting

    Liking

    0

    1

    2

    3

    4

    Mea

    n ra

    ting

    Brain stemMechanism

    ExpectancyMemoryContagion Brain stemMechanism

    ExpectancyMemoryContagion

    Brain stemMechanism

    ExpectancyMemoryContagion

    Figure 2. Means and standard errors for the listeners ratings on each scale as a function of target mechanism.

    at UNIV FEDERAL DO PARANA on September 30, 2014pom.sagepub.comDownloaded from

  • Juslin et al. 613

    Post-hoc tests (Tukeys HSD) confirmed that the episodic memory version was rated as more familiar than the contagion (p < .01) and musical expectancy (p < .001) versions, but the con-trast with the brain stem reflex version fell just short of statistical significance (p < .06).

    Regarding liking, inspection of Figure 2 indicates that the musical expectancy version was less liked than the other versions particularly the contagion and the episodic memory ver-sions. Post-hoc tests confirmed that the musical expectancy version yielded lower ratings of liking than the contagion (p < .001) and episodic memory (p < .001) versions; and, further, that the brain stem reflex version yielded lower ratings of liking than the contagion (p < .01) and episodic memory (p < .01) versions. (Remaining differences were not significant.) Note that a low mean value for familiarity (M = 0.80) and a fairly high mean value for liking (M = 2.44) suggests that the musical excerpts were mostly unfamiliar to the listeners, but that they were reasonably well liked on the whole.7

    Self-reports: Chills

    The occurrence of chills (i.e., a tingling sensation of piloerection) was reported in a dichoto-mous manner (did/did not occur) by listeners for each musical stimulus. The results indicated that the music evoked chills in 12 of the 80 trials (15%). The contagion version yielded the largest number (7), followed by the brain stem reflex (3), episodic memory (2), and musical expectancy (0) versions. A non-parametric test, in terms of a Friedman ANOVA, confirmed a significant effect of mechanism on the number of self-reported chills (2 = 9.75, df = 3, p < .05, Kendalls W = .163). However, there were large individual differences, such that 50% of the listeners did not experience any chills at all.

    Self-reports: MecScale

    The listeners also responded to eight items which targeted specific mechanisms (see Appendix 2). A primary question is whether the listeners responses may predict the target mechanisms. To address this question, we computed the non-parametric Spearmans rho () correlations between the four target mechanism conditions and the eight mechanism items featured in the MecScale (all variables coded dichotomously). To the extent that MecScale has predictive value, we would expect only four of the 32 (possible) correlations to be statistically significant as well as positive in direction; more specifically, those correlations that involve items corresponding to the four target mechanisms of the experiment. All other correlations should be negative and/or non-significant. The results indicated that only two out of the 32 correlations (circa 6%) deviated from this pat-tern. There were thus significant and positive correlations between target mechanism condition and corresponding MecScale item for the brain stem ( = .51), contagion (.33), episodic memory (.28) and musical expectancy (.25) versions. In addition, however, the imagery item correlated significantly with the contagion version (.28) and the expectancy item correlated positively with the brain stem version (.30; all ps < .05).

    In order to investigate whether the MecScale self-reports would also be predictive of the feel-ings experienced by listeners, we carried out one simultaneous as opposed to stepwise mul-tiple regression on the listeners ratings for each of the six emotion scales included in our predictions. The dependent variable was the rating on each scale (continuously coded) and the independent variables were the scale items corresponding to each mechanism (dichotomously coded). Table 2 presents the results. Note that target mechanism items (set in bold) received significant beta weights () on their expected scales (e.g., contagion sadness-melancholy),

    at UNIV FEDERAL DO PARANA on September 30, 2014pom.sagepub.comDownloaded from

  • 614 Psychology of Music 42(4)

    Tab

    le 2

    . Su

    mm

    ary

    of m

    ultip

    le r

    egre

    ssio

    n an

    alys

    es: P

    redi

    ctio

    n of

    em

    otio

    n ra

    tings

    from

    res

    pons

    es t

    o M

    ecSc

    ale

    ques

    tionn

    aire

    (se

    e A

    ppen

    dix

    2).

    Scal

    eM

    ech

    anis

    m it

    em (

    )

    RB

    rain

    ste

    mEn

    trai

    nm

    ent

    Con

    diti

    onin

    gC

    onta

    gion

    Imag

    ery

    Mem

    ory

    Expe

    ctan

    cyA

    ppra

    isal

    Sadn

    ess-

    mel

    anch

    oly

    .36

    .10

    .0

    1

    .02

    .35*

    .0

    3.0

    4.0

    4.0

    3Su

    rpri

    se-a

    ston

    ish

    men

    t.5

    2*

    .33*

    .12

    .1

    8.0

    2.0

    7

    .01

    .25

    *

    .11

    Nos

    talg

    ia-l

    ongi

    ng

    .62

    *.0

    1.0

    1.0

    3.1

    7.1

    4.41*

    .1

    4

    .07

    Hap

    pin

    ess-

    elat

    ion

    .49

    *

    .01

    .07

    .2

    4.0

    6.1

    9.24*

    .1

    4.1

    6A

    nxi

    ety-

    ner

    vou

    snes

    s.4

    4*

    .2

    6*

    .1

    2.1

    5.1

    8

    .14

    .0

    6.43*

    .11

    An

    ger-

    irri

    tati

    on.2

    8.0

    8

    .06

    .25

    .0

    1

    .14

    .01

    .10

    .0

    9

    Not

    es: R

    = m

    ultip

    le c

    orre

    latio

    ns;

    = b

    eta

    wei

    ghts

    ; df =

    8, 7

    1; *

    p