9
1 3 Resting state EEG correlates of memory consolidation 4 5 6 Kate Brokaw, Ward Tishler, Stephanie Manceor, Kelly Hamilton, Andrew Gaulden, Elaine Parr, 7 Erin J. Wamsley 8 Furman University, Department of Psychology, United States 9 10 12 article info 13 Article history: 14 Received 5 November 2015 15 Revised 11 January 2016 16 Accepted 16 January 2016 17 Available online xxxx 18 Keywords: 19 Memory consolidation 20 Sleep 21 Resting state 22 Mindwandering 23 Daydreaming 24 EEG 25 Slow oscillation 26 Alpha 27 Verbal memory 28 29 abstract 30 Numerous studies demonstrate that post-training sleep benefits human memory. At the same time, 31 emerging data suggest that other resting states may similarly facilitate consolidation. In order to identify 32 the conditions under which non-sleep resting states benefit memory, we conducted an EEG (electroen- 33 cephalographic) study of verbal memory retention across 15 min of eyes-closed rest. Participants 34 (n = 26) listened to a short story and then either rested with their eyes closed, or else completed a dis- 35 tractor task for 15 min. A delayed recall test was administered immediately following the rest period. 36 We found, first, that quiet rest enhanced memory for the short story. Improved memory was associated 37 with a particular EEG signature of increased slow oscillatory activity (<1 Hz), in concert with reduced 38 alpha (8–12 Hz) activity. Mindwandering during the retention interval was also associated with 39 improved memory. These observations suggest that a short period of quiet rest can facilitate memory, 40 and that this may occur via an active process of consolidation supported by slow oscillatory EEG activity 41 and characterized by decreased attention to the external environment. Slow oscillatory EEG rhythms are 42 proposed to facilitate memory consolidation during sleep by promoting hippocampal–cortical communi- 43 cation. Our findings suggest that EEG slow oscillations could play a significant role in memory consolida- 44 tion during other resting states as well. 45 Ó 2016 Published by Elsevier Inc. 46 47 48 49 1. Introduction 50 A growing literature confirms that memory is better retained 51 when participants sleep after learning, as opposed to staying 52 awake. It is widely proposed that this effect is due to an active pro- 53 cess of memory consolidation during sleep (Diekelmann & Born, 54 2010; Stickgold, 2005). This hypothesis is supported by studies 55 demonstrating that improved memory is associated with specific 56 features of the sleep EEG linked to consolidation, including slow 57 waves (Alger, Lau, & Fishbein, 2012; Diekelmann, Biggel, Rasch, & 58 Born, 2012), slow oscillations (Huber, Ghilardi, Massimini, & 59 Tononi, 2004; Marshall, Helgadóttir, Mölle, & Born, 2006), and 60 sleep spindles (Cox, Hofman, & Talamini, 2012; Mednick et al., 61 2013; Schabus et al., 2004). 62 Yet it is increasingly clear that a full night of sleep is not 63 required to boost memory. Even a partial night of sleep or a short 64 nap can facilitate memory, with effect sizes comparable to those 65 following a full night (Mednick, Nakayama, & Stickgold, 2003; 66 Plihal & Born, 1997; Tucker & Fishbein, 2009; Tucker et al., 67 2006). Furthermore, the duration of nap sleep is often unrelated 68 to its memory effect, with even very short naps providing the same 69 memory benefit as longer sleep periods (Payne et al., 2015; Tucker 70 et al., 2006; Wamsley, Tucker, Payne, & Stickgold, 2010), although 71 see (Alger et al., 2012; Mednick et al., 2003). Even a nap as short as 72 6 min has been reported to lead to a memory-enhancing effect 73 (Lahl et al., 2008). What enables such short periods of sleep to 74 enhance memory performance? One possibility is the presence of 75 fast-acting offline consolidation mechanisms that do not require 76 the completion of a full sleep cycle. Moreover, some propose that 77 consolidation can occur during any state of sleep or alertness, 78 when the encoding of new information is sufficiently reduced dur- 79 ing the consolidation period (Mednick, Cai, Shuman, Anagnostaras, 80 & Wixted, 2011). 81 Might short periods of quiet wakefulness impact memory, even 82 in the absence of sleep? Most studies investigating the effect of 83 sleep on memory have done so in comparison to waking control 84 conditions in which participants watch videos (Lau, Tucker, & 85 Fishbein, 2010; Tucker et al., 2006), listen to music (Elizabeth & 86 McDevitt, 2014; Mednick, Makovski, Cai, & Jiang, 2009), or leave 87 the laboratory to go about their daily activities (Ellenbogen, 88 Hulbert, Stickgold, Dinges, & Thompson-Schill, 2006; Payne et al., 89 2012). These studies have clearly established that sleep benefits http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.nlm.2016.01.008 1074-7427/Ó 2016 Published by Elsevier Inc. Corresponding author at: Furman University, Johns Hall 206K, United States. Fax: +1 864 294 2206. E-mail address: [email protected] (E.J. Wamsley). Neurobiology of Learning and Memory xxx (2016) xxx–xxx Contents lists available at ScienceDirect Neurobiology of Learning and Memory journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/ynlme YNLME 6377 No. of Pages 9, Model 5G 21 January 2016 Please cite this article in press as: Brokaw, K., et al. Resting state EEG correlates of memory consolidation. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory (2016), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.nlm.2016.01.008

Neurobiology of Learning and Memory - WordPress.com · 90 memory relative to an equivalent duration of active wakefulness, 91 during which participants encode new sensory information

  • Upload
    others

  • View
    2

  • Download
    0

Embed Size (px)

Citation preview

1

3

4

5

6

7

8

910

1 2

1314151617

1819202122232425262728

2 9

48

49

50

51

52

53

54

55

56

57

58

59

60

61

62

63

64

65

66

Neurobiology of Learning and Memory xxx (2016) xxx–xxx

YNLME 6377 No. of Pages 9, Model 5G

21 January 2016

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Neurobiology of Learning and Memory

journal homepage: www.elsevier .com/ locate/ynlme

Resting state EEG correlates of memory consolidation

http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.nlm.2016.01.0081074-7427/� 2016 Published by Elsevier Inc.

⇑ Corresponding author at: Furman University, Johns Hall 206K, United States.Fax: +1 864 294 2206.

E-mail address: [email protected] (E.J. Wamsley).

Please cite this article in press as: Brokaw, K., et al. Resting state EEG correlates of memory consolidation. Neurobiology of Learning and Memoryhttp://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.nlm.2016.01.008

Kate Brokaw, Ward Tishler, Stephanie Manceor, Kelly Hamilton, Andrew Gaulden, Elaine Parr,Erin J. Wamsley ⇑Furman University, Department of Psychology, United States

a r t i c l e i n f o a b s t r a c t

303132333435363738394041424344

Article history:Received 5 November 2015Revised 11 January 2016Accepted 16 January 2016Available online xxxx

Keywords:Memory consolidationSleepResting stateMindwanderingDaydreamingEEGSlow oscillationAlphaVerbal memory

4546

Numerous studies demonstrate that post-training sleep benefits human memory. At the same time,emerging data suggest that other resting states may similarly facilitate consolidation. In order to identifythe conditions under which non-sleep resting states benefit memory, we conducted an EEG (electroen-cephalographic) study of verbal memory retention across 15 min of eyes-closed rest. Participants(n = 26) listened to a short story and then either rested with their eyes closed, or else completed a dis-tractor task for 15 min. A delayed recall test was administered immediately following the rest period.We found, first, that quiet rest enhanced memory for the short story. Improved memory was associatedwith a particular EEG signature of increased slow oscillatory activity (<1 Hz), in concert with reducedalpha (8–12 Hz) activity. Mindwandering during the retention interval was also associated withimproved memory. These observations suggest that a short period of quiet rest can facilitate memory,and that this may occur via an active process of consolidation supported by slow oscillatory EEG activityand characterized by decreased attention to the external environment. Slow oscillatory EEG rhythms areproposed to facilitate memory consolidation during sleep by promoting hippocampal–cortical communi-cation. Our findings suggest that EEG slow oscillations could play a significant role in memory consolida-tion during other resting states as well.

� 2016 Published by Elsevier Inc.

47

67

68

69

70

71

72

73

74

75

76

77

78

79

80

81

82

83

84

85

1. Introduction

A growing literature confirms that memory is better retainedwhen participants sleep after learning, as opposed to stayingawake. It is widely proposed that this effect is due to an active pro-cess of memory consolidation during sleep (Diekelmann & Born,2010; Stickgold, 2005). This hypothesis is supported by studiesdemonstrating that improved memory is associated with specificfeatures of the sleep EEG linked to consolidation, including slowwaves (Alger, Lau, & Fishbein, 2012; Diekelmann, Biggel, Rasch, &Born, 2012), slow oscillations (Huber, Ghilardi, Massimini, &Tononi, 2004; Marshall, Helgadóttir, Mölle, & Born, 2006), andsleep spindles (Cox, Hofman, & Talamini, 2012; Mednick et al.,2013; Schabus et al., 2004).

Yet it is increasingly clear that a full night of sleep is notrequired to boost memory. Even a partial night of sleep or a shortnap can facilitate memory, with effect sizes comparable to thosefollowing a full night (Mednick, Nakayama, & Stickgold, 2003;Plihal & Born, 1997; Tucker & Fishbein, 2009; Tucker et al.,

86

87

88

89

2006). Furthermore, the duration of nap sleep is often unrelatedto its memory effect, with even very short naps providing the samememory benefit as longer sleep periods (Payne et al., 2015; Tuckeret al., 2006; Wamsley, Tucker, Payne, & Stickgold, 2010), althoughsee (Alger et al., 2012; Mednick et al., 2003). Even a nap as short as6 min has been reported to lead to a memory-enhancing effect(Lahl et al., 2008). What enables such short periods of sleep toenhance memory performance? One possibility is the presence offast-acting offline consolidation mechanisms that do not requirethe completion of a full sleep cycle. Moreover, some propose thatconsolidation can occur during any state of sleep or alertness,when the encoding of new information is sufficiently reduced dur-ing the consolidation period (Mednick, Cai, Shuman, Anagnostaras,& Wixted, 2011).

Might short periods of quiet wakefulness impact memory, evenin the absence of sleep? Most studies investigating the effect ofsleep on memory have done so in comparison to waking controlconditions in which participants watch videos (Lau, Tucker, &Fishbein, 2010; Tucker et al., 2006), listen to music (Elizabeth &McDevitt, 2014; Mednick, Makovski, Cai, & Jiang, 2009), or leavethe laboratory to go about their daily activities (Ellenbogen,Hulbert, Stickgold, Dinges, & Thompson-Schill, 2006; Payne et al.,2012). These studies have clearly established that sleep benefits

(2016),

90

91

92

93

94

95

96

97

98

99

100

101

102

103

104

105

106

107

108

109

110

111

112

113

114

115

116

117

118

119

120

121

122

123

124

125

126

127

128

129

130

131

132

133

134

135

136

137

138

139

140

141

142

143

144

145

146

147

148

149

150

151

152

153

154

155

156

157

158

159

160

161

162

163

164

165

166

167

168

169

170

171

172

173

174

175

176

177

178

179

180

181

182

183

184

185

186

187

188

189

190

191

192

193

194

195

196

197

198

Fig. 1. Experimental timeline. Participants learned a short story just prior to a15 min retention interval during which they either rested quietly with eyes closedor completed a distractor task. A recall test was administered both immediately andfollowing the retention interval.

2 K. Brokaw et al. / Neurobiology of Learning and Memory xxx (2016) xxx–xxx

YNLME 6377 No. of Pages 9, Model 5G

21 January 2016

memory relative to an equivalent duration of active wakefulness,during which participants encode new sensory information. In

contrast, the effect of quiet resting wake on memory – in theabsence of cognitive tasks, activities, and sensory stimulation –has not been sufficiently characterized.

The notion that periods of unoccupied rest can retroactivelyfacilitate memory actually dates back to the earliest days of exper-imental psychology, when Müller and Pilzecker first suggested thatretroactive interference occurs even when the interpolated activityis highly dissimilar to the learned material (Müller & Pilzecker,1900). But in more recent years, this question of whether a generalreduction of mental effort during wakefulness (rest) facilitates con-solidation has received little attention. Just in the last several years,emerging new evidence has begun to suggest that quiet wake doesin fact facilitate memory, at least under some conditions (Craig,Dewar, Della Sala, & Wolbers, 2015; Dewar, Alber, Butler, Cowan,& Della Sala, 2012; Dewar, Alber, Cowan, & Della Sala, 2014). Sev-eral recent experiments report that a brief period of resting wakefollowing learning can improve later memory in both elderly(Dewar et al., 2012, 2014) and young participants (Craig et al.,2015; Mercer, 2015). But because these studies have not employedEEG-monitoring, it is uncertain whether participants might haveobtained brief periods of sleep during the retention interval.Beyond this, we have little understanding of the mechanisms bywhich resting wakefulness might enhance memory, nor the condi-tions under which this benefit emerges. Neurophysiological corre-lates of memory changes across sleep have now been extensivelydocumented (Clemens, Fabó, & Halász, 2005, 2006; Holz et al.,2012; Nishida & Walker, 2007; Schabus et al., 2004; van Dongen,Takashima, Barth, & Fernández, 2011), but corresponding studiesof resting wakefulness are lacking.

Quiet rest might facilitate memory via active consolidationmechanisms similar to those operating during sleep. Much of theneurophysiology purported to support consolidation during sleepis also present during resting wake. Like sleep, quiet rest is charac-terized by a dramatic reduction in sensory processing. Freed fromthe demands of stimulus processing, mental experience is focusedinward, as participants engage in ‘‘mindwandering” – thinkingabout the past, imagining the future, and creating fictitious scenar-ios (Andrews-Hanna, 2011; Andrews-Hanna, Reidler, Huang, &Buckner, 2010; Antrobus, Singer, Goldstein, & Fortgang, 1970;Baird et al., 2012). Meanwhile, the ‘‘reactivation” of recent memoryin the hippocampus and cortex that was first observed during slowwave sleep is also expressed during resting wake in rodents (Carr,Jadhav, & Frank, 2011; Davidson, Kloosterman, & Wilson, 2009;Foster & Wilson, 2006; Gupta, van der Meer, Touretzky, & Redish,2010; Karlsson & Frank, 2009). Although this form of memory reac-tivation has not been directly observed in humans, the hippocam-pal ‘‘sharp-wave ripples” associated with reactivation areprevalent during quiet rest in humans (Axmacher, Elger, & Fell,2008; Clemens et al., 2011). Consolidation-promoting neurochem-ical features of sleep are also partially replicated during rest,including decreased acetylcholine levels during quiet restingwakefulness (Marrosu et al., 1995).

Finally, several EEG oscillations proposed to support consolida-tion during sleep also have analogs during quiet rest. Although thepredominant frequencies are different, in comparison to moreactive states of wakefulness EEG slowing characterizes both sleepand eyes-closed quiet rest. In wakefulness, candidate oscillationsthat we hypothesized might relate to memory processing are theEEG alpha oscillation (8–12 Hz) and the slower theta (4–7 Hz)and slow/delta oscillations (0.5–2 Hz). Alpha is the primary EEGsignature of eyes-closed waking rest that distinguishes this statefrom active wakefulness, and is one of the main EEG correlates ofthe fMRI-defined ‘‘default-mode” resting state network, which

Please cite this article in press as: Brokaw, K., et al. Resting state EEG correlathttp://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.nlm.2016.01.008

includes a number of memory-related brain regions including thehippocampus, parahippocamal cortex, and medial frontal cortex(Jann et al., 2009; Knyazev, Slobodskoj-Plusnin, Bocharov, &Pylkova, 2011). On the phenomenological level, alpha rhythmsare associated with a decreased focus on external stimuli andincreased attention to internal states, including memories of thepast (Foulkes & Fleisher, 1975). Alpha has recently been studiedas a mediator of effective memory encoding and retrieval(Klimesch, 1997; Klimesch, Schimke, & Schwaiger, 1994; Vogt,Klimesch, & Doppelmayr, 1998; Williams, Ramaswamy, & Oulhaj,2006). But slower EEG frequencies are also present during quietrest. In sleep, slow oscillations (�1 Hz) and slow waves (up to2 Hz) are thought to be major contributors to systems-level mem-ory consolidation, synchronizing hippocampal sharp-wave rippleswith cortical activity (Clemens et al., 2007, 2011; Mölle,Eschenko, Gais, Sara, & Born, 2009) and thus promoting hippocam-pal–cortical communication and synaptic plasticity (Rosanova &Ulrich, 2005). �1 Hz rhythms are present during quiet rest as well,and these may be relatively attenuated during the execution ofdirected cognitive tasks (Alper et al., 2006; Demanuele, Sonuga-Barke, & James, 2010). Thus, a number of mechanisms proposedto account for the effects of sleep on memory are also present dur-ing quiet wake, which suggests the hypothesis that the memorybenefits of rest and sleep could arise from overlapping active con-solidation mechanisms.

The aims of the current study were to (1) confirm that a periodof EEG-verified quiet rest benefits memory, in the absence of anysleep, (2) isolate EEG correlates of this memory effect, and (3)describe the mental activity associated with this memory effect.We examined memory retention for a short story across a 15-min interval with continuous EEG monitoring. We hypothesizedthat 15 min of quiet rest would lead to improved memory at a sub-sequent test, and expected to find that this effect was related toboth EEG slowing and increased ‘‘mindwandering” (Andrews-Hanna et al., 2010; Baird et al., 2012; Mason et al., 2007) duringthe rest period, both potential signatures of a sleep-like offlinestate conducive to memory consolidation.

2. Methods

2.1. Participants

29 college students (19 female) age 19–22 (M = 20 yrs (±0.8 SD))were recruited by email, advertisement, or word-of-mouth, andpaid $10/h for their participation. By self-report using a 3-day sleeplog, participants stated that they slept an average of 7.4 h (±1.1 SD)per night on the 3 nights prior to the study.

es of memory consolidation. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory (2016),

199

200

201

202

203

204

205

206

207

208

209

210

211

212

213

214

215

216

217

218

219

220

221

222

223

224

225

226

227

228

229

230

231

232

233

234

235

236

237

238

239

240

241

242

243

244

245

246

247

248

249

250

251

252

253

254

255

256

257

258

259

260

261

262

263

264

265

266

267

268

269

270

271

272

273

274

275

276

277

278

279

280

281

282

283

284

285

286

287

288

289

290

291

292

293

294

295

296

297

298

299

300

301

302

303

304

305

306

307

308

309

310

311

312

313

314

315

316

317

318

319

320

321

322

323

324

325

K. Brokaw et al. / Neurobiology of Learning and Memory xxx (2016) xxx–xxx 3

YNLME 6377 No. of Pages 9, Model 5G

21 January 2016

2.2. Procedure

The experimental timeline is illustrated in Fig. 1. Participantsarrived at the laboratory, signed informed consent, and filled outinitial paperwork including demographic surveys, the Epworthsleepiness scale (a measure of trait sleepiness (Johns, 1991)), andthe Stanford Sleepiness Scale (a measure of state sleepiness(Hoddes, Zarcone, Smythe, Phillips, & Dement, 1973)). Participantswere then prepared for EEG (electroencephalographic) recordingby attaching electrodes to the scalp (C3, C4, O1, O2, F3, F4), withbilateral mastoid references, as well as chin leads for assessingmuscle tone, and eye leads for recording EOG (electrooculography,right and left outer canthus).

All participants completed 2 conditions in counterbalancedorder: quiet rest and distractor task. There was a 10 min break inbetween conditions. In each condition, participants first listenedto a short story, and were instructed to remember as much ofthe story as possible (details below). Immediately after listeningto the story, participants were asked to freely recall as much ofthe story as possible. Participants then completed either the quietrest or distractor task condition. In the quiet rest condition, partic-ipants sat in a comfortable chair with their eyes closed for 15 min.Participants were instructed to keep their eyes closed for the entire15 min and to keep movement to a minimum. No instruction wasgiven regarding what participants should be doing mentally duringthis time. In the distractor task condition, participants played thecomputer game ‘‘Snood” for 15 min (see below). EEG, EOG, andEMG were digitally recorded at 400 Hz during the 15 min interval.Immediately afterward, a delayed recall test was administered inwhich participants were instructed to again type as much of thestory as they could remember.

Two self-report measures assessed mental activity during the15 min retention interval. Following each condition, participantscompleted a questionnaire on which they recorded any thoughtsor imagery they could recall from the preceding interval, and ratedthe proportion of the 15 min interval that they had spent engagedin 14 predefined categories of mental activity, including ‘‘thinkingabout the past” (something else earlier today/yesterday to a weekago/past year or several years ago), ‘‘imagining the future” (latertoday/tomorrow to next week/next year or several years), ‘‘think-ing about the short story”, ‘‘thinking about staying still”, ‘‘countingthe time”, ‘‘mind was blank”, ‘‘meditating”, ‘‘sleeping”, ‘‘thinkingabout something else”, and ‘‘other”. Following the methods ofAndrews-Hanna et al. (2010), participants recorded their responsesby dividing a blank circle to reflect the proportional amount oftime they had spent thinking about each topic. Following each con-dition, participants also completed a rehearsal questionnaire onwhich they used a 5-point scale to respond to the questions‘‘how often did you think about the story?”, ‘‘how often did youimagine the story?” and ‘‘how often did you try to remember thestory?” during the 15 min interval.

2.3. Materials

A short story recall task (adapted from the Wechsler MemoryScale (Wechsler, 1987)) was used to assess declarative memory(following Dewar et al., 2012). Participants listened to a digitalrecording of a short story, approximately 30 s long, and were thenasked to freely recall as much of this story as they could, and asaccurately as possible, by typing everything that they rememberedinto an electronic form. They were given as much time as neededto complete their responses. 15 min later, a delayed test was givenin which they were again asked to freely recall as much of the storyas they could. There were two equivalent versions of the story used(version A and version B), one for each experimental condition.Assignment of story version to experimental condition was

Please cite this article in press as: Brokaw, K., et al. Resting state EEG correlathttp://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.nlm.2016.01.008

counterbalanced across subjects, and immediate recall perfor-mance was equivalent between versions (p > .4).

The computer game Snood was used as a distractor task (http://snoodworld.com). Snood is a simple puzzle game in which the par-ticipants eliminate blocks of colors (‘‘snoods”) by creating groupsof 3 or more of the same color. This visuospatial task was chosenin order to provide an engaging distractor activity that would haveminimal overlap in specific content with the purely verbal shortstory learning task. Participants were instructed how to play thegame by pointing and clicking the computer mouse to aim andrelease snoods. Once a game was lost or won, participants wereinstructed to click ‘‘restart” and continue playing until told to stopafter 15 min had passed. Game difficulty level was set to ‘‘easy”.

2.4. Analysis

Free recall responses were scored by 2 raters blind to experi-mental condition. Correctly recalled elements were scored accord-ing to the methods described in the Wechsler Memory ScaleManual (Wechsler, 1987). In short, one point was awarded for eachpiece of correctly reported information, for a total of 25 points perstory. All reports were scored by both raters, and the final score foreach report was calculated as the average score of the two raters.Responses were also scored for the number of falsely recalleditems, defined as story elements which were mentioned in the freerecall response, but described incorrectly. For example, if a partic-ipant provided the protagonists’ first name (Anna), but incorrectlystated what the name was (Elsa, Annie, Astrid, etc.), this would bescored as a falsely recalled element. Inter-rater reliability for cor-rect recall was r = .94, and for false recall was r = .66. For both mea-sures, the primary dependent variable was change in performanceacross the 15-min memory retention interval. To assess fluctua-tions in the total amount of information provided, a total recallscore was also calculated at both the immediate and delayed timepoints (correct recall + false recall).

The amount of information contained in free recall responseswas highly variable between immediate and delayed testing insome individuals, which might have been caused by lack of moti-vation to or fatigue with repeatedly supplying a lengthy writtenresponse. Thus, six performance outliers were thus excluded fromfurther analysis due to scores on the performance change depen-dent measures lying >1.5 interquartile ranges above the 75th per-centile or below the 25th percentile. The primary results of thestudy were robust to alternate methods of excluding outliers –Using the same method of outlier exclusion, but applied only toencoding performance on the Story Recall Task resulted in theexclusion of fewer outlying points (n = 2), but the same primaryfinding that memory performance is relatively preserved across aperiod of quiet wakefulness, in comparison to a significant declineacross active wakefulness (see below).

Five additional participants were excluded from all or someanalyses because they failed to comply with instructions to keeptheir eyes closed during the quiet rest period (n = 1), had pervasiveartifact in the EEG recording that prevented accurate spectral anal-ysis (n = 1), or had corrupted or missing recall data (n = 3).

Sleep stage was determined according the standard criteriaestablished by the American Academy of Sleep Medicine (Iber,Ancoli-Israel, Chesson, & Quan, 2007). Prior to analysis, EEG artifact(due to movement, muscle activity, eye movement, and othersources) was manually rejected via visual inspection. Spectral anal-ysis via fast Fourier transform (Brain Products BrainVision Analyzerv2.0.2) was then applied to all artifact-free 4 s intervals (50% seg-ment overlap, Hanning window) to assess mean power spectraldensity (lV2/Hz) in the frequency bands of interest, includingalpha (8–12 Hz), beta (13–25 Hz), theta (4–7 Hz), delta (1–4 Hz),and slow oscillation (0.3–1 Hz). To compensate for individual

es of memory consolidation. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory (2016),

326

327

328

329

330

331

332

333

334

335

336

337

338

339

340

341

342

343

344

345

346

347

348

349

350

351

352

353

354

355

356

357

358

359

360

361

362

363

364

365

366

367

368

369

370

371

372

373

374

375

376

377

378

379

380

381

382

383

384

385

386

387

388

389

390

391

392

393

Table 1Story recall across quiet rest and distractor task conditions.

DRecall acrossquiet rest

DRecall acrossdistractor

p

Mean ±SD Mean ±SD

Correct recall �.61 1.30 �1.28 1.29 0.018False recall .13 .76 .73 1.32 0.09Total recall (Correct + False) �.56 1.12 �.75 1.25 0.71

p-values derived from paired-samples t-tests.

4 K. Brokaw et al. / Neurobiology of Learning and Memory xxx (2016) xxx–xxx

YNLME 6377 No. of Pages 9, Model 5G

21 January 2016

differences in EEG amplitude, relative power values were used(normalized such that total power across all frequency bands isrendered equivalent in all analyzed segments). Eye movementswere automatically detected by marking peaks wherein amplitudeof the right outer canthus recording exceeded an absolute thresh-old of 30 lV.

Effect of experimental condition (quiet rest vs. distractor task)on memory retention was assessed using paired-samples t-tests,and the association between memory retention and resting-stateEEG was assessed using Pearson’s correlations. In cases where sep-arate correlations were run for multiple electrodes, Type I errorwas controlled by using a Bonferroni-corrected significance thresh-old of a = .0083 (a = .05/6 electrodes).

3. Results

3.1. Effect of quiet rest on memory

As hypothesized, quiet rest led to improved memory for thestory at 15 min, relative to the distractor task (change in correctrecall: t18 = 2.60, p = 0.018; Fig. 2). In contrast, quiet rest did notsignificantly affect false recall scores (change in false recall:t18 = 1.82, p = 0.09; Fig. 2). At immediate recall, performance wasequivalent between conditions for both correct (p > .3) and false(p > .8) recall. There was no effect of condition on the total amountof information reported (total recall score: p > .7). These perfor-mance data are reported in Table 1.

3.2. EEG correlates of memory retention

Memory retention during quiet rest was associated with an EEGsignature of proportionally increased slow oscillation power(0.3–1 Hz), in concert with decreased power in the alpha frequencyband (8–12 Hz). Slow oscillation power was strongly associatedwith improved recall following quiet rest (mean across allelectrodes: r18 = 0.63, p = .005; Fig. 3 Left). The magnitude of thiseffect was strongest frontally, and survived Bonferroni correctionfor multiple comparisons at F3, F4 and C3 recording sites (Table 2;Fig. 3 Right; complete spectral analysis correlation results reportedin Supplementary Table S1).

As anticipated, compared with the distractor task, quiet restwas characterized by an increased proportion of EEG spectralpower in the alpha frequency band (8–12 Hz; t20 = 6.68,

Fig. 2. Effect of condition on recall. Quiet rest significantly enhanced recall at15 min. Error bars ± SEM.

Please cite this article in press as: Brokaw, K., et al. Resting state EEG correlathttp://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.nlm.2016.01.008

p = 0.000002). Yet within quiet rest, the amount of alpha activitythat participants expressed was negatively associated with correctrecall (Table 2).

Slow oscillation power during quiet rest was unrelated to mem-ory following the distractor task (Table 2). In contrast, resting alphamarginally predicted memory both following the distractor condi-tion and following quiet rest. There were no other significant asso-ciations between EEG power and correct recall across quiet wake.There were no significant associations between EEG measuresand false recall.

Supplemental analyses ruled out the possibility that residualeye movement artifact might have contributed to slow EEG fre-quencies during quiet rest. First, in contrast to the strong associa-tion with frontal EEG signals, story recall scores were notsignificantly correlated with slow oscillatory activity in the eyemovement channels (p > .1). Second, the number of automaticallydetected eye movements during quiet rest (see Section 2) was cor-related neither with story recall (p > .7), nor slow oscillation powerin the EEG channels (p > .7).

394

395

396

397

398

399

400

401

402

403

3.3. Mental activity correlates of memory retention

Improved memory was associated with decreased attention tothe external environment. First, during quiet rest, participantsspent much less time thinking about what they were currentlydoing, as compared to during the distractor task (t21 = 6.02,p = 0.000006; Fig. 4). Instead, they spent more time thinking aboutthe past and imagining the future. This included increased timeduring quiet rest thinking about the past week (t21 = 2.87,p = 0.009), the past years (t21 = 2.05, p = .05), the rest of that day(t21 = 2.04, p = 0.05), as well as the future year (t21 = 2.6, p = 0.02).10 participants also reported meditating during quiet rest, whichoccurred more commonly than during the distractor task(t21 = 2.4, p = 0.02).

Second, within the distractor task condition, participantsshowed superior memory retention when spending less time think-ing about the Snood game itself (r20 = �0.48, p = 0.03), and moretime thinking about other things, including the past week(r20 = .56, p = 0.01), the rest of the day (r20 = 0.46, p = 0.04), tomor-row (r20 = 0.51, p = 0.02), and meditating (r20 = 0.51, p = 0.02).There was no correlation between mental activity during quiet restand memory performance.

404

405

406

407

408

409

410

411

412

413

3.4. Rehearsal effects

Condition did not significantly affect the extent to which partic-ipants reported effortfully trying to remember the story (t21 = 1.80,p = 0.09) or imagine the story (t21 = 1.50, p = 0.15). Participants didreport ‘‘thinking” about the story significantly more in the quietrest condition than during the distractor task (t21 = 3.46,p = 0.002). However, thinking about the story was not correlatedwith the improved recall seen during quiet rest (p > .3), nor didany other rehearsal questionniare responses correlate with mem-ory change during either quiet rest or the distractor task.

es of memory consolidation. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory (2016),

414

415

416

417

418

419

420

421

422

423

424

425

426

427

428

429

430

431

432

Fig. 3. Slow oscillation power predicts memory benefit of quiet rest. Left: The amount of slow oscillatory EEG power (0.3–1 Hz) was correlated strongly with change in recallscore across the quiet resting period. Right: This association was strongest at frontal electrodes.

Table 2EEG correlates of change in story recall across the 15 min retention interval.

Memory following quiet rest Memory following distractor

r p r p

Quiet rest EEGSlow oscillation lV2/Hz (0.3–1 Hz)C3 .51 .03* .12 .62C4 .69 .002** .20 .41O1 .57 .01* .25 .30O2 .52 .03* .28 .25F3 .61 .008** .15 .54F4 .65 .004** .08 .74

Alpha lV2/Hz (8–12 Hz)C3 �.51 .03* �.42 .08C4 �.54 .02* �.41 .09O1 �.41 .09 �.45 .05O2 �.39 .11 �.45 .05F3 �.60 .009* �.51 .03*

F4 �.53 .02* �.31 .20

Distractor task EEGSlow oscillation lV2/Hz (0.3–1 Hz)C3 �.22 .37 .04 .86C4 .15 .57 �.12 .64O1 �.27 .28 �.39 .10O2 .33 .19 .19 .44F3 .18 .48 .18 .46F4 .09 .71 �.07 .77

Alpha lV2/Hz (8–12 Hz)C3 .07 .78 .01 .96C4 �.16 .54 .01 .96O1 .19 .45 �.06 .86O2 �.07 .78 �.16 .69F3 �.23 .36 �.04 .82F4 �.21 .41 .10 .53

Pearson’s correlations testing the association between EEG during the retention interval and change in story recall following the quietrest and distractor task conditions.

* p < .05.** Survives Bonferroni correction for multiple comparisons at a = .0083.

K. Brokaw et al. / Neurobiology of Learning and Memory xxx (2016) xxx–xxx 5

YNLME 6377 No. of Pages 9, Model 5G

21 January 2016

3.5. Effects of sleep and sleepiness

During quiet rest, 5 participants fell asleep for an average of2.0 min (±2.6 SD). However, falling asleep did not improve recallscores. In fact, those who fell asleep for this brief time actually per-formed numerically worse at delayed recall (change in correctrecall = �1.2 ± 0.6 SD) than those who did not fall asleep (changein correct recall = �0.39 ± 1.4 SD, p > .2). Additionally, when partic-ipants who slept were excluded from analysis, the memory benefitof quiet rest was still apparent (t12 = 2.5, p = .03). Sleep also had no

Please cite this article in press as: Brokaw, K., et al. Resting state EEG correlathttp://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.nlm.2016.01.008

effect on change in false recall scores following quiet rest (p > .4).Neither slow oscillation power nor alpha power showed a detect-able difference between those who fell asleep during the retentioninterval and those who did not (slow oscillation: p = .4; alpha:p = .7).

Self-reported sleepiness was equivalent between conditions atbaseline. However, participants became significantly sleepier fol-lowing quiet rest compared to following the distractor task(t21 = 2.25, p = 0.04). The sleepiness induced by quiet rest did notcorrelate with memory change across rest (p > .7).

es of memory consolidation. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory (2016),

433

434

435

436

437

438

439

440

441

442

443

444

445

446

447

448

449

450

451

452

453

454

455

456

457

458

459

460

461

462

463

464

465

466

467

468

469

470

471

472

473

474

475

476

477

478

479

480

481

482

483

484

485

486

487

488

489

490

491

492

493

494

495

496

497

498

499

500

501

502

503

504

505

506

507

508

509

510

511

512

513

514

515

516

517

518

519

520

Fig. 4. Mental activity during the retention interval. Proportion of the 15 minretention interval that participants spent engaged in various categories of mentalactivity, by self-report.

6 K. Brokaw et al. / Neurobiology of Learning and Memory xxx (2016) xxx–xxx

YNLME 6377 No. of Pages 9, Model 5G

21 January 2016

3.6. Order effects

Order of experimental condition affected memory, but this wasunrelated to the memory benefit of quiet rest. Order of conditionsignificantly affected immediate (F1,18 = 13.9, p = 0.002) anddelayed recall (F1,18 = 16.9, p = 0.001) within the quiet rest condi-tion, such that participants remembered the story better whenquiet rest followed the distractor task condition, as opposed towhen quiet rest preceded the distractor task condition. This couldrepresent a facilitation effect in which the distractor task benefitssubsequent new encoding. But importantly, when we controlledfor this order effect by including counterbalancing order as a factorin the ANOVA model, we found that order did not impact the effectof rest on memory (p > 0.4), and that the main effect of quiet restvs. distractor task was still statistically significant (F1,18 = 6.43,p = 0.02). There was no such order effect for false recall scores.

4. Discussion

Here, we confirm that a short period of eyes-closed rest facili-tates declarative memory, and we identify two novel predictorsof this phenomenon that elucidate the neurophysiological andphenomenological conditions under which the effect occurs. Onthe neurophysiological level, an increase in slow oscillatory EEGrhythms and decrease in alpha rhythms predicted improved mem-

Please cite this article in press as: Brokaw, K., et al. Resting state EEG correlathttp://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.nlm.2016.01.008

ory following rest. On the phenomenological level, improved mem-ory was associated with decreased attention to the externalenvironment and an increase in ‘‘mindwandering”, as participantsturned to thinking of the past and imagining the future. In severalrespects, the resting state in the present study resembles sleep.Taken together, our observations suggest that eyes-closed restcould facilitate memory consolidation via mechanisms similar tothose that operate during sleep.

Although �1 Hz rhythms are not a visually prominent feature ofthe resting EEG, these oscillations are present during rest, and maybe relatively attenuated during the execution of directed cognitivetasks (Alper et al., 2006; Demanuele et al., 2010). During wakeful-ness, EEG oscillations in the slow and delta bands have been asso-ciated with the activation of memory-related structures includingthe parahippocampal gyrus (Chen, Feng, Zhao, Yin, & Wang,2008) and medial prefrontal cortex (Alper et al., 2006). Impor-tantly, these oscillations are also among the neurophysiologicalsignatures most strongly associated with memory consolidationduring sleep. Hippocampal sharp-wave ripple bursts, during whichmemory ‘‘reactivation” is seen in rodents, are temporally synchro-nized with the up-state of the sleep slow oscillation, which isthought to group functionally relevant faster brain rhythms. Assuch, slow oscillations are indirectly linked to the reactivationand consolidation of memory in hippocampal–cortical circuits(Carr et al., 2011; Davidson et al., 2009; Foster & Wilson, 2006;Gupta et al., 2010; Karlsson & Frank, 2009). Because slow oscilla-tory EEG activity during waking rest is at least superficially similarto that seen during sleep, one possibility is that there is similar hip-pocampal–cortical communication occurring during this time. It isunknown whether this frequency of EEG oscillation during wake-fulness is generated by the same mechanisms as the sleep EEGslow oscillation, but recent work demonstrates that slow mem-brane potential oscillations are indeed present during quiet rest(Crochet & Petersen, 2006; Poulet & Petersen, 2008).

Attenuated alpha was also associated with enhanced memoryacross quiet rest. The fact that reduced resting alpha was associ-ated with memory in both experimental conditions suggests thatthe alpha correlation may arise from a trait-like associationbetween alpha generation and memory, rather than a specificeffect of the EEG state immediately following encoding. Of note,the disappearance of alpha from eyes-closed resting EEG is a pri-mary indicator of entry into Stage 1 sleep (Iber et al., 2007). Butin this case, there was no association between decreased restingalpha and visually-identified sleep onset, nor were there any otherindications that sleep might account for the observed memoryeffects. Although several participants did fall asleep for a brief time,this was unrelated to memory performance. We cannot rule outperiods of spatially localized sleep (Hung et al., 2013; Vyazovskiyet al., 2011) or very short ‘‘microsleeps” below the threshold ofvisual detection as contributors to the memory effect and theEEG findings. Future studies using high-density EEG might seekto test the hypothesis that experience-dependent local sleep(Hung et al., 2013; Vyazovskiy et al., 2011) accounts for the effectof quiet rest on memory.

During rest, participants’ thoughts differed substantially fromthose during active wake. While resting, participants reportedthinking about the past and/or the future and most noticeablynot of what they were doing presently. This is different from thedistractor task condition, during which participants were highlyfocused on their current stimulus environment. However, therewere substantial individual differences in the extent to which par-ticipants were focused on the distractor task during the retentioninterval. Memory retention was superior in the distractor task con-dition when participants reported more mindwandering and lesstask-related thought, echoing the mental activity profile of quietrest. In both cases, improved memory followed a period of time

es of memory consolidation. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory (2016),

521

522

523

524

525

526

527

528

529

530

531

532

533

534

535

536

537

538

539

540

541

542

543

544

545

546

547

548

549

550

551

552

553

554

555

556

557

558

559

560

561

562

563

564

565

566

567

568

569

570

571

572

573

574

575

576

577

578

579

580

581

582

583

584

585

586

587

588

589

590

591

592

593

594

595

596

597

598

599

600

601

602

603

604

605

606

607

608609610611612613614615616617618619620621622623624625626627628629630631632633634635636637638639640641642643644645646647648649650651652653654655656657658

K. Brokaw et al. / Neurobiology of Learning and Memory xxx (2016) xxx–xxx 7

YNLME 6377 No. of Pages 9, Model 5G

21 January 2016

during which participants attended more to internal, task-unrelated thoughts, and less to the ongoing task. This observationis consistent with a hypothesized role for mindwandering in mem-ory consolidation, but alternatively, it could be that increasedmindwandering during the distractor task signifies decreasedattention to external stimuli, rather than itself enhancing the con-solidation process in any way. Because this non-verbal distractortask had no content overlap with the short story, stimulus-specific retroactive interference induced by the distractor is anunlikely explanation for the results.

In contrast, interference caused by general mental effortdevoted to stimulus processing is a potential explanation for thefindings. Our data are broadly consistent with theoretical accountsof memory consolidation which propose that any encoding activityduring wakefulness interferes with the initiation of offline consol-idation processes (Dewar, Cowan, & Della Sala, 2007; Mednicket al., 2011; Wixted, 2004). Notably however, quiet rest was notassociated with a lack of mental activity in the present study. Par-ticipants experienced rich and varied mental activity during therest period, which differed from the distractor task primarily inthat this mentation was inwardly focused, rather than directedtoward the current stimulus environment. Thus, our observationsare consistent with the notion that stimulus-oriented mental effortinterferes with consolidation, whereas inwardly-focused mentalactivity does not. In fact, the present observations suggest thatmindwandering may serve as an indicator that the brain hasentered an offline state conducive to consolidation.

During rest, amongst the thought and imagery reported werethoughts specifically about the experimental learning task. How-ever, no participants reported effortfully rehearsing the story,and thinking about the learning task was unrelated to memoryperformance. Thus, in line with the observations of one prior report(Dewar et al., 2014), we conclude that active rehearsal of thelearned material is an unlikely explanation for the memoryoutcome.

Numerous questions remain unanswered. First, the currentstudy does not establish the duration of this memory effect(although prior research suggests a long-lasting impact (Dewaret al., 2012)). Second, rest may be beneficial for some forms ofmemory and not others. The small body of literature to date hasfocused on learning tasks which are presumably hippocampus-dependent (Craig et al., 2015; Dewar et al., 2012, 2014; Mercer,2015). Future research should extend this work to tasks in themotor, procedural, and perceptual domains, which rely on distinctbrain systems but also show a benefit of post-training sleep. Third,numerous forms of offline memory processing have been proposedto exist – insight formation (Wagner, Gais, Haider, Verleger, &Born, 2004), memory integration (Tamminen, Payne, Stickgold,Wamsley, & Gaskell, 2010) and the extraction of ‘‘gist” (Payneet al., 2009), to name only a few (Robert Stickgold & Walker,2013). Waking rest may affect memory in some of these ways,but not others. Finally, the effect of rest may not be due to the sameactive consolidation processes that are proposed to account forsleep’s effect on memory. Although prior research supports ourhypothesis of a causal role for slow oscillations in resting statememory consolidation, our current data cannot directly supportcausation. And although we argue that the mechanisms accountingfor the benefit of rest could overlap with those operating duringsleep, future research will be required to determine the extent towhich rest and sleep benefit memory via distinct vs. overlappingmechanisms.

In summary, these data demonstrate that under the right condi-tions, non-sleep resting states can facilitate declarative memory.Prior research has demonstrated that during human rest, patternsof brain activity associated with recent learning are re-expressed,and that this ‘‘reactivation” of learning-related activity predicts

Please cite this article in press as: Brokaw, K., et al. Resting state EEG correlathttp://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.nlm.2016.01.008

subsequent memory (Deuker et al., 2013; Tambini & Davachi,2013; Tambini, Ketz, & Davachi, 2010). This mechanism mayexplain the behavioral memory effect demonstrated here. Our cur-rent data complement these observations by elucidating the globalbrain state under which this ‘‘reactivation” of memory duringhuman rest may occur. It appears critical that a particular state isentered characterized on the neural level by increased slow oscil-latory and decreased alpha activity, and on the phenomenologicallevel by an increase in task-unrelated mental processing (e.g.‘‘mindwandering”). Together, these features of rest may indicatethat the brain has entered a state of reduced encoding-relatedactivity optimal for offline memory reactivation and consolidation.

Acknowledgments

We thank Yvette Graveline for technical support, and MatthewTucker for comments on the draft manuscript. This research wassupported by intramural funding through the Furman AdvantageResearch Fellowship Program.

Appendix A. Supplementary material

Supplementary data associated with this article can be found, inthe online version, at http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.nlm.2016.01.008.

References

Alger, S. E., Lau, H., & Fishbein, W. (2012). Slow wave sleep during a daytime nap isnecessary for protection from subsequent interference and long-term retention.Neurobiology of Learning and Memory. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.nlm.2012.06.003.

Alper, K. R., John, E. R., Brodie, J., Günther, W., Daruwala, R., & Prichep, L. S. (2006).Correlation of PET and qEEG in normal subjects. Psychiatry Research:Neuroimaging, 146(3), 271–282. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.pscychresns.2005.06.008.

Andrews-Hanna, J. R. (2011). The brain’s default network and its adaptive role ininternal mentation. The Neuroscientist. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/1073858411403316.

Andrews-Hanna, J. R., Reidler, J. S., Huang, C., & Buckner, R. L. (2010). Evidence forthe default network’s role in spontaneous cognition. Journal of Neurophysiology,104(1), 322–335. http://dx.doi.org/10.1152/jn.00830.2009.

Antrobus, J. S., Singer, J. L., Goldstein, S., & Fortgang, M. (1970). Mindwandering andcognitive structure. Transactions of the New York Academy of Sciences, 32(2 SeriesII), 242–252.

Axmacher, N., Elger, C. E., & Fell, J. (2008). Ripples in the medial temporal lobe arerelevant for human memory consolidation. Brain: A Journal of Neurology, 131(Pt7), 1806–1817. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/brain/awn103.

Baird, B., Smallwood, J., Mrazek, M. D., Kam, J. W. Y., Franklin, M. S., & Schooler, J. W.(2012). Inspired by distraction: mind wandering facilitates creative incubation.Psychological Science, 23(10), 1117–1122. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0956797612446024.

Carr, M. F., Jadhav, S. P., & Frank, L. M. (2011). Hippocampal replay in the awakestate: A potential substrate for memory consolidation and retrieval. NatureNeuroscience, 14(2), 147–153. http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/nn.2732.

Chen, A. C. N., Feng, W., Zhao, H., Yin, Y., & Wang, P. (2008). EEG default modenetwork in the human brain: Spectral regional field powers. NeuroImage, 41(2),561–574. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.12.064.

Clemens, Z., Fabó, D., & Halász, P. (2005). Overnight verbal memory retentioncorrelates with the number of sleep spindles. Neuroscience, 132(2), 529–535.http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroscience.2005.01.011.

Clemens, Z., Fabó, D., & Halász, P. (2006). Twenty-four hours retention ofvisuospatial memory correlates with the number of parietal sleep spindles.Neuroscience Letters, 403(1–2), 52–56. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neulet.2006.04.035.

Clemens, Z., Mölle, M., Eross, L., Barsi, P., Halász, P., & Born, J. (2007). Temporalcoupling of parahippocampal ripples, sleep spindles and slow oscillations inhumans. Brain: A Journal of Neurology, 130(Pt 11), 2868–2878. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/brain/awm146.

Clemens, Z., Mölle, M., Eross, L., Jakus, R., Rásonyi, G., Halász, P., & Born, J. (2011).Fine-tuned coupling between human parahippocampal ripples and sleepspindles. The European Journal of Neuroscience, 33(3), 511–520. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1460-9568.2010.07505.x.

Cox, R., Hofman, W. F., & Talamini, L. M. (2012). Involvement of spindles in memoryconsolidation is slow wave sleep-specific. Learning & Memory (Cold SpringHarbor, N.Y.), 19(7), 264–267. http://dx.doi.org/10.1101/lm.026252.112.

Craig, M., Dewar, M., Della Sala, S., & Wolbers, T. (2015). Rest boosts the long-termretention of spatial associative and temporal order information. Hippocampus.http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/hipo.22424.

es of memory consolidation. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory (2016),

659660661662663664665666667668669670671672673674675676677678679680681682683684685686687688689690691692693694695696697698699700701702703704705706707708709710711712713714715716717718719720721722723724725726727728729730731732733734735736737738739740741742743744

745746747748749750751752753754755756757758759760761762763764765766767768769770771772773774775776777778779780781782783784785786787788789790791792793794795796797798799800801802803804805806807808809810811812813814815816817818819820821822823824825826827828829830

8 K. Brokaw et al. / Neurobiology of Learning and Memory xxx (2016) xxx–xxx

YNLME 6377 No. of Pages 9, Model 5G

21 January 2016

Crochet, S., & Petersen, C. C. H. (2006). Correlating whisker behavior withmembrane potential in barrel cortex of awake mice. Nature Neuroscience, 9(5),608–610. http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/nn1690.

Davidson, T. J., Kloosterman, F., & Wilson, M. A. (2009). Hippocampal replay ofextended experience. Neuron, 63(4), 497–507. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2009.07.027.

Demanuele, C., Sonuga-Barke, E. J. S., & James, C. J. (2010). Slow neuronaloscillations in the resting brain vs task execution: A BSS investigation of EEGrecordings. In 2010 Annual International Conference of the IEEE Engineering inMedicine and Biology Society (EMBC) (pp. 1638–1641), doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1109/IEMBS.2010.5626645.

Deuker, L., Olligs, J., Fell, J., Kranz, T. A., Mormann, F., Montag, C., ... Axmacher, N.(2013). Memory consolidation by replay of stimulus-specific neural activity. TheJournal of Neuroscience, 33(49), 19373–19383. http://dx.doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0414-13.2013.

Dewar, M., Alber, J., Butler, C., Cowan, N., & Della Sala, S. (2012). Brief wakefulresting boosts new memories over the long term. Psychological Science. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0956797612441220.

Dewar, M., Alber, J., Cowan, N., & Della Sala, S. (2014). Boosting long-term memoryvia wakeful rest: Intentional rehearsal is not necessary, consolidation issufficient. PLoS ONE, 9(10), e109542. http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0109542.

Dewar, M., Cowan, N., & Della Sala, S. (2007). Forgetting due to retroactiveinterference: A fusion of early insights into everyday forgetting and recentresearch on anterograde amnesia. Cortex; A Journal Devoted to the Study of theNervous System and Behavior, 43(5), 616.

Diekelmann, S., Biggel, S., Rasch, B., & Born, J. (2012). Offline consolidation ofmemory varies with time in slow wave sleep and can be accelerated by cuingmemory reactivations. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory, 98(2), 103–111.http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.nlm.2012.07.002.

Diekelmann, S., & Born, J. (2010). The memory function of sleep. Nature Reviews.Neuroscience, 11(2), 114–126. http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/nrn2762.

Elizabeth, A., & McDevitt, K. M. R. (2014). The benefit of offline sleep and wake fornovel object recognition. Experimental Brain Research. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s00221-014-3830-3.

Ellenbogen, J. M., Hulbert, J. C., Stickgold, R., Dinges, D. F., & Thompson-Schill, S. L.(2006). Interfering with theories of sleep and memory: Sleep, declarativememory, and associative interference. Current Biology, 16(13), 1290–1294.

Foster, D. J., & Wilson, M. A. (2006). Reverse replay of behavioural sequences inhippocampal place cells during the awake state. Nature, 440(7084), 680–683.http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/nature04587.

Foulkes, D., & Fleisher, S. (1975). Mental activity in relaxed wakefulness. Journal ofAbnormal Psychology, 84(1), 66–75. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/h0076164.

Gupta, A. S., van der Meer, M. A. A., Touretzky, D. S., & Redish, A. D. (2010).Hippocampal replay is not a simple function of experience. Neuron, 65(5),695–705. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2010.01.034.

Hoddes, E., Zarcone, V., Smythe, H., Phillips, R., & Dement, W. C. (1973).Quantification of sleepiness: A new approach. Psychophysiology, 10(4),431–436.

Holz, J., Piosczyk, H., Feige, B., Spiegelhalder, K., Baglioni, C., Riemann, D., & Nissen,C. (2012). EEG sigma and slow-wave activity during NREM sleep correlate withovernight declarative and procedural memory consolidation. Journal of SleepResearch. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1365-2869.2012.01017.x.

Huber, R., Ghilardi, M. F., Massimini, M., & Tononi, G. (2004). Local sleep andlearning. Nature, 430(6995), 78–81. http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/nature02663.

Hung, C.-S., Sarasso, S., Ferrarelli, F., Riedner, B., Ghilardi, M. F., Cirelli, C., & Tononi,G. (2013). Local experience-dependent changes in the wake EEG after prolongedwakefulness. Sleep. http://dx.doi.org/10.5665/sleep.2302.

Iber, C., Ancoli-Israel, S., Chesson, A., & Quan, S. (2007). The AASM manual for thescoring of sleep and associated events: Rules, terminology, and technicalspecifications. Westchester, IL: American Academy of Sleep Medicine.

Jann, K., Dierks, T., Boesch, C., Kottlow, M., Strik, W., & Koenig, T. (2009). BOLDcorrelates of EEG alpha phase-locking and the fMRI default mode network.NeuroImage, 45(3), 903–916.

Johns, M. W. (1991). A new method for measuring daytime sleepiness: The Epworthsleepiness scale. Sleep, 14(6), 540–545.

Karlsson, M. P., & Frank, L. M. (2009). Awake replay of remote experiences in thehippocampus. Nature Neuroscience, 12(7), 913–918. http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/nn.2344.

Klimesch, W. (1997). EEG-alpha rhythms and memory processes. InternationalJournal of Psychophysiology: Official Journal of the International Organization ofPsychophysiology, 26(1–3), 319–340.

Klimesch, W., Schimke, H., & Schwaiger, J. (1994). Episodic and semantic memory:An analysis in the EEG theta and alpha band. Electroencephalography and ClinicalNeurophysiology, 91(6), 428–441.

Knyazev, G. G., Slobodskoj-Plusnin, J. Y., Bocharov, A. V., & Pylkova, L. V. (2011). Thedefault mode network and EEG alpha oscillations: An independent componentanalysis. Brain Research, 1402, 67–79. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.brainres.2011.05.052.

Lau, H., Tucker, M. A., & Fishbein, W. (2010). Daytime napping: Effects on humandirect associative and relational memory. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory,93(4), 554–560. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.nlm.2010.02.003.

Marrosu, F., Portas, C., Mascia, M. S., Casu, M. A., Fà, M., Giagheddu, M., ... Gessa, G. L.(1995). Microdialysis measurement of cortical and hippocampal acetylcholinerelease during sleep-wake cycle in freely moving cats. Brain Research, 671(2),329–332.

Please cite this article in press as: Brokaw, K., et al. Resting state EEG correlathttp://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.nlm.2016.01.008

Marshall, L., Helgadóttir, H., Mölle, M., & Born, J. (2006). Boosting slow oscillationsduring sleep potentiates memory. Nature, 444(7119), 610–613. http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/nature05278.

Mason, M. F., Norton, M. I., Van Horn, J. D., Wegner, D. M., Grafton, S. T., & Macrae, C.N. (2007). Wandering minds: The default network and stimulus-independentthought. Science, 315(5810), 393–395. http://dx.doi.org/10.1126/science.1131295.

Mednick, S. C., Cai, D. J., Shuman, T., Anagnostaras, S., & Wixted, J. T. (2011). Anopportunistic theory of cellular and systems consolidation. Trends inNeurosciences. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.tins.2011.06.003.

Mednick, S. C., Makovski, T., Cai, D. J., & Jiang, Y. V. (2009). Sleep and rest facilitateimplicit memory in a visual search task. Vision Research, 49(21), 2557–2565.http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.visres.2009.04.011.

Mednick, S. C., McDevitt, E. A., Walsh, J. K., Wamsley, E., Paulus, M., Kanady, J. C., &Drummond, S. P. A. (2013). The critical role of sleep spindles in hippocampal-dependent memory: A pharmacology study. The Journal of Neuroscience: TheOfficial Journal of the Society for Neuroscience, 33(10), 4494–4504. http://dx.doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3127-12.2013.

Mednick, S. C., Nakayama, K., & Stickgold, R. (2003). Sleep-dependent learning: Anap is as good as a night. Nature Neuroscience, 6(7), 697–698. http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/nn1078.

Mercer, T. (2015). Wakeful rest alleviates interference-based forgetting.Memory, 23(2), 127–137. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/09658211.2013.872279.

Mölle, M., Eschenko, O., Gais, S., Sara, S. J., & Born, J. (2009). The influence of learningon sleep slow oscillations and associated spindles and ripples in humans andrats. European Journal of Neuroscience, 29(5), 1071–1081.

Müller, G., & Pilzecker, A. (1900). Experimentelle Beiträge zur Lehre vomGedächtniss. Zeitschrift Für Psychologie, Ergänzungsband (1), 1–300.

Nishida, M., & Walker, M. P. (2007). Daytime naps, motor memory consolidationand regionally specific sleep spindles. PLoS ONE, 2(4), e341.

Payne, J. D., Kensinger, E. A., Wamsley, E. J., Spreng, R. N., Alger, S. E., Gibler, K., ...Stickgold, R. (2015). Napping and the selective consolidation of negative aspectsof scenes. Emotion (Washington, D.C.), 15(2), 176–186. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0038683.

Payne, J. D., Schacter, D. L., Propper, R. E., Huang, L.-W., Wamsley, E. J., Tucker, M. A.,... Stickgold, R. (2009). The role of sleep in false memory formation. Neurobiologyof Learning and Memory, 92(3), 327–334. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.nlm.2009.03.007.

Payne, J. D., Tucker, M. A., Ellenbogen, J. M., Wamsley, E. J., Walker, M. P., Schacter, D.L., & Stickgold, R. (2012). Memory for semantically related and unrelateddeclarative information: The benefit of sleep, the cost of wake. PLoS ONE, 7(3),e33079. http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0033079.

Plihal, W., & Born, J. (1997). Effects of early and late nocturnal sleep on declarativeand procedural memory. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 9(4), 534–547.

Poulet, J. F. A., & Petersen, C. C. H. (2008). Internal brain state regulates membranepotential synchrony in barrel cortex of behaving mice. Nature, 454(7206),881–885. http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/nature07150.

Rosanova, M., & Ulrich, D. (2005). Pattern-specific associative long-termpotentiation induced by a sleep spindle-related spike train. The Journal ofNeuroscience: The Official Journal of the Society for Neuroscience, 25(41),9398–9405. http://dx.doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2149-05.2005.

Schabus, M., Gruber, G., Parapatics, S., Sauter, C., Klosch, G., Anderer, P., ... Zeitlhofer,J. (2004). Sleep spindles and their significance for declarative memoryconsolidation. Sleep, 27(8), 1479–1485.

Stickgold, R. (2005). Sleep-dependent memory consolidation. Nature London, 7063,1272.

Stickgold, R., & Walker, M. P. (2013). Sleep-dependent memory triage: Evolvinggeneralization through selective processing. Nature Neuroscience, 16(2),139–145. http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/nn.3303.

Tambini, A., & Davachi, L. (2013). Persistence of hippocampal multivoxel patternsinto postencoding rest is related to memory. Proceedings of the NationalAcademy of Sciences, 110(48), 19591–19596. http://dx.doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1308499110.

Tambini, A., Ketz, N., & Davachi, L. (2010). Enhanced brain correlations during restare related to memory for recent experiences. Neuron, 65(2), 280–290. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2010.01.001.

Tamminen, J., Payne, J. D., Stickgold, R., Wamsley, E. J., & Gaskell, M. G. (2010). Sleepspindle activity is associated with the integration of newmemories and existingknowledge. The Journal of Neuroscience: The Official Journal of the Society forNeuroscience, 30(43), 14356–14360. http://dx.doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3028-10.2010.

Tucker, M. A., & Fishbein, W. (2009). The impact of sleep duration and subjectintelligence on declarative and motor memory performance: How much isenough? Journal of Sleep Research, 18(3), 304–312. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1365-2869.2009.00740.x.

Tucker, M. A., Hirota, Y., Wamsley, E. J., Lau, H., Chaklader, A., & Fishbein, W. (2006).A daytime nap containing solely non-REM sleep enhances declarative but notprocedural memory. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory, 86(2), 241–247.

van Dongen, E. V., Takashima, A., Barth, M., & Fernández, G. (2011). Functionalconnectivity during light sleep is correlated with memory performance for face-location associations. NeuroImage, 57(1), 262–270. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.04.019.

Vogt, F., Klimesch, W., & Doppelmayr, M. (1998). High-frequency components in thealpha band and memory performance. Journal of Clinical Neurophysiology:Official Publication of the American Electroencephalographic Society, 15(2),167–172.

es of memory consolidation. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory (2016),

831832833834835836837838

839840841842843844845846

K. Brokaw et al. / Neurobiology of Learning and Memory xxx (2016) xxx–xxx 9

YNLME 6377 No. of Pages 9, Model 5G

21 January 2016

Vyazovskiy, V. V., Olcese, U., Hanlon, E. C., Nir, Y., Cirelli, C., & Tononi, G. (2011).Local sleep in awake rats. Nature, 472(7344), 443–447. http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/nature10009.

Wagner, U., Gais, S., Haider, H., Verleger, R., & Born, J. (2004). Sleep inspires insight.Nature, 427(6972), 352–355. http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/nature02223.

Wamsley, E. J., Tucker, M. A., Payne, J. D., & Stickgold, R. (2010). A brief nap isbeneficial for human route-learning: The role of navigation experience and EEGspectral power. Learning & Memory, 17(7), 332.

847

Please cite this article in press as: Brokaw, K., et al. Resting state EEG correlathttp://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.nlm.2016.01.008

Wechsler, D. (1987). WMS-R: Wechsler memory scale-revised: Manual. HarcourtBrace Jovanovich.

Williams, J., Ramaswamy, D., & Oulhaj, A. (2006). 10 Hz flicker improves recognitionmemory in older people. BMC Neuroscience, 7, 21. http://dx.doi.org/10.1186/1471-2202-7-21.

Wixted, J. T. (2004). The psychology and neuroscience of forgetting. Annual Reviewof Psychology, 55(1), 235–269. http://dx.doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych.55.090902.141555.

es of memory consolidation. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory (2016),