13
Engagement in family activities: a quantitative, comparative study of children with profound intellectual and multiple disabilities and children with typical development A. K. Axelsson,*† M. Granlund*† and J. Wilder* *School of Health Sciences, Jönköping University, Jönköping, Sweden †Swedish Institute of Disability Research, Jönköping, Sweden Accepted for publication 18 January 2013 Keywords child disability, comparison, engagement, family activities, participation, profound intellectual and multiple disabilities Correspondence: Anna Karin Axelsson, School of Health Sciences, Jönköping University, Box 1026, 551 11 Jönköping, Sweden E-mail: anna.karin.axelsson@ hhj.hj.se Abstract Background Participation is known to be of great importance for children’s development and emotional well-being as well as for their families. In the International Classification of Functioning, Disability and Health – Children and Youth version participation is defined as a person’s ‘involvement in a life situation’.Engagement is closely related to involvement and can be seen as expressions of involvement or degree of involvement within a situation.This study focuses on children’s engagement in family activities; one group of families with a child with profound intellectual and multiple disabilities (PIMD) and one group of families with children with typical development (TD) were compared. Methods A descriptive study using questionnaires. Analyses were mainly performed by using Mann–Whitney U-test and Spearman’s rank correlation test. Results Engagement in family activities differed in the two groups of children. The children with PIMD had a lower level of engagement in most family activities even though the activities that engaged the children to a higher or lesser extent were the same in both groups. Child engagement was found to correlate with family characteristics mostly in the children with TD and in the children with PIMD only negative correlations occurred. In the children with PIMD child engagement correlated with cognition in a high number of listed family activities and the children had a low engagement in routines in spite of these being frequently occurring activities. Conclusions Level of engagement in family activities in the group of children with PIMD was lower compared with that in the group of children with TD. Families with a child with PIMD spend much time and effort to adapt family living patterns to the child’s functioning. Introduction Participation is known to be of great importance for all chil- dren’s development and emotional well-being. Children with profound intellectual and multiple disabilities (PIMD) consti- tute a heterogeneous group in terms of the patterns of impair- ments as well as functioning and behaviour. In the definition of PIMD, learning disabilities are combined with profound physi- cal disabilities, sensory impairments and most often medical complications (Nakken & Vlaskamp 2002, 2007). In our earlier research on child participation in family activities, occurrence of family activities (seen as opportunities for participation) and Child: care, health and development Original Article doi:10.1111/cch.12044 © 2013 John Wiley & Sons Ltd 523

ContentServer (5)

Embed Size (px)

DESCRIPTION

s

Citation preview

  • Engagement in family activities: a quantitative,comparative study of children with profoundintellectual and multiple disabilities and childrenwith typical development

    A. K. Axelsson,* M. Granlund* and J. Wilder*

    *School of Health Sciences, Jnkping University, Jnkping, SwedenSwedish Institute of Disability Research, Jnkping, Sweden

    Accepted for publication 18 January 2013

    Keywordschild disability,comparison,engagement, familyactivities, participation,profound intellectual andmultiple disabilities

    Correspondence:Anna Karin Axelsson,School of HealthSciences, JnkpingUniversity, Box 1026, 55111 Jnkping, SwedenE-mail:[email protected]

    AbstractBackground Participation is known to be of great importance for childrens development and

    emotional well-being as well as for their families. In the International Classification of Functioning,

    Disability and Health Children and Youth version participation is defined as a persons

    involvement in a life situation. Engagement is closely related to involvement and can be seen as

    expressions of involvement or degree of involvement within a situation. This study focuses on

    childrens engagement in family activities; one group of families with a child with profound

    intellectual and multiple disabilities (PIMD) and one group of families with children with typical

    development (TD) were compared.

    Methods A descriptive study using questionnaires. Analyses were mainly performed by using

    MannWhitney U-test and Spearmans rank correlation test.

    Results Engagement in family activities differed in the two groups of children. The children with

    PIMD had a lower level of engagement in most family activities even though the activities that

    engaged the children to a higher or lesser extent were the same in both groups. Child engagement

    was found to correlate with family characteristics mostly in the children with TD and in the children

    with PIMD only negative correlations occurred. In the children with PIMD child engagement

    correlated with cognition in a high number of listed family activities and the children had a low

    engagement in routines in spite of these being frequently occurring activities.

    Conclusions Level of engagement in family activities in the group of children with PIMD was lower

    compared with that in the group of children with TD. Families with a child with PIMD spend much

    time and effort to adapt family living patterns to the childs functioning.

    Introduction

    Participation is known to be of great importance for all chil-

    drens development and emotional well-being. Children with

    profound intellectual and multiple disabilities (PIMD) consti-

    tute a heterogeneous group in terms of the patterns of impair-

    ments as well as functioning and behaviour. In the definition of

    PIMD, learning disabilities are combined with profound physi-

    cal disabilities, sensory impairments and most often medical

    complications (Nakken & Vlaskamp 2002, 2007). In our earlier

    research on child participation in family activities, occurrence

    of family activities (seen as opportunities for participation) and

    bs_bs_banner Child: care, health and developmentOriginal Article doi:10.1111/cch.12044

    2013 John Wiley & Sons Ltd 523

  • the childs presence in these activities for children with PIMD

    was studied. In comparison with a group of families with chil-

    dren with typical development (TD) it was found that the

    majority of listed family activities occurred less often in the

    families with a child with PIMD. Moreover, the children with

    PIMD were commonly present less often in the activities

    (Axelsson & Wilder 2013). However, taking part in an activity

    does not mean that the person is engaged in the activity in a

    meaningful way or with co-participants (Perlman 2007). By also

    investigating the childs engagement in family activities this

    study was designed to increase the knowledge about participa-

    tion in family activities of children with PIMD.

    Participation and engagement

    In the World Health Organizations (WHOs) framework for

    measuring health and disability in children, the International

    Classification of Functioning, Disability and Health Children

    and Youth version (ICF-CY), participation is defined as a

    persons involvement in a life situation (World Health

    Organization 2007). However, due to its actual multidimen-

    sional meaning the definition of participation made by WHO

    has been considered to be quite vague. Hoogsteen and

    Woodgate (2010) stated that in order to participate, a child with

    disabilities must take part in something or with someone, they

    must have a sense of inclusion, control over what they are taking

    part in, and be working toward obtaining a goal or enhanced

    quality of life. Likewise according to Maxwell and Granlund

    (2011), participation includes a psychological perspective that

    places more emphasis on the subjective experience of engage-

    ment. Granlund and colleagues (2012) now indicate that for

    measuring participation ICF might need a third qualifier to

    describe the subjective experience of involvement, in addition

    to the qualifiers capacity and performance. Empirical evidence

    for the need of this third qualifier has been found (Maxwell

    et al. 2012). Engagement seems closely related to involvement

    and can be seen as expressions of involvement or degree of

    involvement within a situation (Granlund et al. 2012). Engage-

    ment, as an aspect in how activities are performed, has

    been defined and operationalized in Childrens Engagement

    Questionnaire (McWilliam 1991). In addition, Hammel and

    colleagues (2008) found active and meaningful engagement/

    being a part of to be one of the values included in participation

    when capturing the insider perspective of participation of

    people with disabilities.

    A range of instruments has, in different ways, tried to capture

    participation of children. A commonly used measure is Chil-

    drens Assessment of Participation and Enjoyment (CAPE)

    measuring childrens participation by using questions about

    diversity, intensity (frequency), where, with whom and enjoy-

    ment. In association there is the Preferences for Activity of

    Children (PAC), which measures activity preference on the

    same items as included in the CAPE (King et al. 2005). In a

    comparison of school-aged children with and without disabili-

    ties, King and colleagues (2009b) have concluded that the enjoy-

    ment in the activity is determined by actual engagement, seeing

    enjoyment and preference as distinct constructs. Coster and

    colleagues (2011), in their recently developed instrument the

    Participation and Environment Measure for Children and

    Youth (PEM-CY), use questions about how often the child par-

    ticipates, how involved the child is and whether the caregiver

    would like to see changes. There are also instruments measuring

    participation in school situations only, such as School Function

    Assessment (SFA) (Coster et al. 1999; Hwang et al. 2002). No

    available instruments provide in-depth information on partici-

    pation in family activities explicitly. To conduct this study on

    participation in family activities it was found to be necessary to

    develop the instrument Child Participation in Family Activities

    (Child-PFA) (Axelsson & Wilder 2013).

    Participation in family activities and aspect associated tochild engagement

    Influences on development can be expected to be greatest when

    the child participates in everyday activities on a regular basis

    and over an extended period of time (Bronfenbrenner 1995).

    The childs participation in family activities is therefore of great

    importance (Gallimore et al. 1989; Dunst et al. 2002). Family

    activities include routines as well as other activities such as play

    and family gatherings. Situation-specific experiences within

    family activities, activity settings, offer contexts for the child to

    learn about his or her own abilities and capabilities and about

    others (Dunst et al. 2001). When incorporating characteristics

    of the individuals in these physical settings they could be

    defined as a niches (Wachs 2000) wherein proximal processes,

    which denotes the interaction, take place. Such proximal proc-

    esses, including presence and engagement, promote the childs

    development and well-being (Bronfenbrenner 1999).

    Both family characteristics and child characteristics influence

    child participation. The childs closest network, the family, has

    the task to construct routines that uphold consistent and satis-

    fying daily activities for the child to participate and engage in.

    This in turn is partly influenced by sociodemographic charac-

    teristics such as family income and parents educational level

    (Gallimore et al. 1989). The level of a persons participation also

    varies as a function of the severity of the disability (Simeonsson

    524 A.K. Axelsson et al.

    2013 John Wiley & Sons Ltd, Child: care, health and development, 39, 4, 523534

  • et al. 2001; King et al. 2009a; Orlin et al. 2010). Moreover, a

    frequently occurring low bio-behavioural state (half wake/low

    alertness) has been suggested as one explanation for interaction

    difficulties in children with PIMD (Munde et al. 2009;

    Granlund et al. 2013). Besides, frequency of occurrence of

    family activities and the childs presence in the activities have

    been found to be lower in a group of families with a child with

    PIMD compared with families with children with TD (Axelsson

    & Wilder 2013). However, when using the CAPE, Majnemer and

    colleagues (2008) have found that impairments and activity

    limitations had little influence on enjoyment. Though, in their

    study on children, most with mild motor dysfunction, greater

    enjoyment in informal activities was found to be related to, e.g.

    lower IQ and younger age. In a study by Keogh and colleagues

    (2000), a child-driven model and a transactional model on chil-

    dren with developmental delays and their families were com-

    pared. The child-driven model was described as when child

    characteristics predict family accommodation activities and the

    transactional model as when family accommodations activities

    and child-characteristics influence each other mutually. It was

    found that the impact of child characteristics on the family

    system was strong. The question now is what engagement in

    family activities looks like for children with PIMD and how

    family and child characteristics are related to this.

    Objectives

    The aim of the study was to compare child engagement in listed

    family activities in two groups of families: one group of families

    with a child with PIMD and one group of families with typically

    developing (TD) children.

    How does child engagement in family activities differ between

    the two groups of children?

    Are there any relationships between child engagement and

    family income, mothers/fathers educational level and the

    earlier studied frequency of occurrence of family activities in

    the two family groups?

    What are the relationships between child engagement and

    child characteristics in the group of children with PIMD?

    Method

    A descriptive and comparative study of two groups of children

    was performed. The study was approved by the regional ethics

    committee, Linkping, Dnr 2010/324-31. The instrument, par-

    ticipants and procedure were the same as in our study on fre-

    quency of occurrence of family activities and child presence

    (Axelsson & Wilder 2013). However, in this conducted study the

    engagement of the children was in focus (see Fig. 1).

    The instrument

    The Child Participation in Family Activities (Child-PFA) ques-

    tionnaire was used. The instructions included a request to try to

    put oneself in the situation of the whole family as well as in the

    childs/youths situation and perspectives. A family activity was

    defined as an activity that a family does together in everyday life,

    when several family members take part. Besides the question

    How engaged/involved is your child/youth? there were also

    questions about how often the activities occurred, who usually

    were present and about personal assistance and technical aids

    (see Fig. 1).

    Figure 1. The structure of Child-PFA.Number of listed family activities, group withPIMD (group with TD). Differences innumber of activities is due to PIMD-specificactivities. Fifty-three of the activities are thesame in both groups. Child-PFA, ChildParticipation in Family Activities; PIMD,profound intellectual and multiple disabilities;TD, typical development.

    Childrens engagement in family activities 525

    2013 John Wiley & Sons Ltd, Child: care, health and development, 39, 4, 523534

  • The items in the Child-PFA had been developed through a

    review of the literature, expert review, and pilot testing with

    families of children with PIMD (Axelsson & Wilder 2013). To

    measure the childs ability level the Abilities Index (Bailey et al.

    1993) was used after some simplifications had been made in

    scaling and phrasing, consistent with what emerged essential in

    the pilot study. For example, in vision and motor skills compre-

    hensive information was requested rather than information

    about the childs left and right side. Concepts such as cognitive

    ability were briefly explained as well. Abilities Index measure

    functional abilities where high ratings indicate high degree of

    difficulties/health problems. Questions about family demo-

    graphics were also asked.

    Participants and procedure

    In the first group 60 families of a child aged 520 years with

    PIMD participated and in the second group 107 families with

    children with TD including a focus child aged 510 years par-

    ticipated. The inclusion criteria of the first group were families

    having a child, initially aged 020 years, with PIMD who made

    use of the right to personal assistance according to the Swedish

    act of Law LSS/LASS (SFS 1993:387; SFS 1993:389). The laws

    make it possible for parents themselves to be personal assistants,

    often besides traditional jobs, as well as to hire non-relatives

    as personal assistants for the child. Recruitment was done by

    contact with three national disability organizations: JAG, RBU,

    FUB in Sweden. These organizations are not disability specific

    but serve families who have children with a range of disorders.

    Three hundred families fulfilled the inclusion criteria and

    received a questionnaire, 65 questionnaires were returned. Of

    these five were excluded (two due to no completion, two due to

    divergent low age and one due to duplication), resulting in 60

    participating families with a child with PIMD aged between 5

    and 20 years. The childrens diagnoses included, among others,

    CP, other syndromes where motor and intellectual disabilities

    were combined as well as residual conditions after encephalitis.

    No analysis of the attrition rate has been done. The inclusion

    criteria in the second group were families of children with TD

    with a focus child aged 510 years. Recruitment was done by

    using convenience and snow ball sampling and 145 question-

    naires were distributed to families in three different counties

    in the southern parts of Sweden. One hundred and seven ques-

    tionnaires were returned. In both family groups the question-

    naire was answered by a parent or another adult that were a

    member of the close family and all answers were provided con-

    fidential (see Table 1).

    The questionnaires distributed to the two groups were essen-

    tially identical concerning the listed family activities; however, the

    questionnaires distributed to the families with children with TD

    included some modifications in order to match a sample without

    diagnoses. In the questionnaire for children with TD the activities

    exercising physical therapy at home,playing in the sandpit,going

    to habilitation centre activities were taken away and the activities

    doing homework and jumping on trampoline were added. This

    resulted in that 53 of the activities were the same in both groups

    and thus a comparison of these activities could be performed

    between the two groups. The overall internal consistency of the

    engagement questions in the Child-PFA was tested by using

    Cronbachs alpha and resulted in 0.830 for the questionnaire

    distributed to families with a child with PIMD and 0.809 for the

    questionnaire distributed to families with children with TD.

    Data analysis

    Data analyses were performed with PASW Statistics 18 (spss).

    Due to the ordinal data, the non-parametric MannWhitney

    U-test was used in order to compare the differences in total

    income and educational level between the two family groups. It

    was also used when comparing child engagement in listed

    family activities of the two groups. The actual levels of engage-

    ment in the family activities as well as the earlier studied fre-

    quency of occurrence of the activities (Axelsson & Wilder 2013)

    were studied descriptively looking for median values. To look

    at associations between these aspects, the median values were

    checked and differences of >1 step were noted. To test relation-

    ships between selected family and child characteristics and child

    engagement, Spearmans rank correlation test was used. The

    P-value was set to P < 0.05.

    Results

    Differences in levels of engagement

    Children in the group with TD were more engaged in listed

    family activities compared with children in the group with

    PIMD. There were no activities with higher engagement in the

    children with PIMD; however, the differences were found to be

    descriptively less in proportion in the domains Organized

    activities and Outdoor activities (see Fig. 2). The same activities

    elicited higher or lower level of engagement, respectively, in

    both groups, for instance the engagement in joking and fooling

    around and going together to childs leisure activity were

    estimated high in both groups, while doing the dishes was

    estimated low (see Table 2).

    526 A.K. Axelsson et al.

    2013 John Wiley & Sons Ltd, Child: care, health and development, 39, 4, 523534

  • Table 1. Participant characteristics

    Group of children with PIMD (N total = 60) Group of children with TD (N total = 107)

    n (%) Mean (SD) n (%)

    AnsweringMother 47 (78) 69 (65)Father 7 (12) 37 (35)Other adult 6 (10)

    Child characteristicsGender

    Male 37 (62) 53 (50)Female 23 (38) 53 (50)

    Age (years)510 16 (27) 107 (100)1120 44 (73)

    Functional mobility (15) 53 (88) 3.60 (1.59)Fine motor skills (15) 57 (95) 3.53 (1.47)Cognition (16) 60 (100) 4.73 (1.29)Communication, self (16) 60 (100) 5.25 (1.19)Communication, understand (16) 59 (98) 4.46 (1.37)Ability to decide (14) 58 (97) 2.88 (0.9)Vision (16) 60 (100) 3.43 (1.82)Hearing (15) 58 (97) 1.47 (1.11)Health (14) 60 (100) 2.15 (0.97)

    Family characteristicsTotal annual income (EUR)*

    90 400 13 (22) 12 (11)

    Education, mother/fatherGrade 19 2 (4)/3 (5) 3 (3)/8 (8)Grade 1012 19 (33)/23 (42) 40 (38)/56 (55)University 35 (61)/28 (51) 61 (58)/37 (36)Other 1 (1)/1 (2) 2 (2)/1 (1)

    *Converted from SEK 2011-02-01.PIMD, profound intellectual and multiple disabilities; TD, typical development.

    Figure 2. Differences in engagement betweenthe children with PIMD and TD children.PIMD, profound intellectual and multipledisabilities; TD, typical development.

    Childrens engagement in family activities 527

    2013 John Wiley & Sons Ltd, Child: care, health and development, 39, 4, 523534

  • Table 2. Level of engagement and frequency of occurrence of family activities, group with PIMD and group with TD

    Family activity

    Group with PIMD Group with TD

    Child engagement Frequency of occurrence Child engagement Frequency of occurrenceMedian value

    (25:e75:e percentile)*

    Median value

    (25:e75:e percentile)

    Median value

    (25:e75:e percentile)*

    Median value

    (25:e75:e percentile)

    Indoor activitiesWatching a movie 3 (24) 3 (24) 3 (34) 2 (23)Watching TV 3 (23) 4 (34) 3 (34) 4 (34)Joking and fooling around 4 (34) 3,5 (34) 4 (34) 3 (34)Playing computer games 3 (24) 2 (13) 4 (34) 3 (23)Surfing the internet 2 (14) 1 (13) 3 (24) 2 (13)Doing handicraft 2 (23) 2 (13) 4 (34) 3 (24)Playing board games 2 (13) 1 (12) 4 (34) 2 (22)Playing with you or other adult 3 (24) 4 (34) 4 (34) 4 (34)Playing with children (friends/siblings) 3 (24) 3 (14) 4 (3.54) 3 (34)Playing with pets 2 (23) 2 (14) 3 (24) 1 (14)Story reading 3 (23.75) 3 (13) 4 (34) 3 (34)Singing 3 (24) 3 (14) 3 (34) 3 (23)Playing instruments 3 (23) 1 (13) 2 (1.253) 1 (12)Listening to music 3 (24) 4 (34) 3 (34) 3 (34)Dancing 2 (24) 1 (12) 3 (24) 2 (13)Exercising physical therapy at home 3 (23) 3 (14)

    MealsBeing together in the kitchen 2 (23) 2.5 (23.75) 3 (23) 3 (23)Cooking/baking 2 (23) 2 (12) 3 (34) 2 (23)Doing the dishes 1 (12) 1 (13) 1 (12) 1 (12)Laying the table/cleaning away 2 (13) 2,5 (14) 2 (23) 3 (24)Having tea or coffee together 3 (24) 3 (24) 3 (34) 3 (24)Having breakfast together 3 (24) 3 (24) 3 (24) 3 (34)Having dinner together 3 (24) 4 (34) 3 (34) 4 (44)

    RoutinesCleaning the house 1 (12) 1 (12) 2 (22) 2 (23)Doing morning routines 2 (23) 4 (44) 3 (23) 4 (44)Doing evening routines 2 (23) 4 (34) 3 (24) 4 (44)Packing school bag 1 (12) 3 (14) 2 (23) 3 (24)Picking up after playing 1 (12) 3 (14) 2 (23) 2 (23)Doing homework 3 (34) 3 (34)Lying down for rest 3 (23.75) 2,5 (13.75) 2 (13) 1 (12)Going by car to and from school 3 (23) 3 (14) 3 (23) 3 (14)Going by car at other occasions 3 (23.25) 3 (23) 3 (23) 3 (33)

    Outdoor activitiesShopping for groceries 2 (23) 2 (13) 3 (23) 2 (23)Gardening 1 (13) 1 (12) 2 (23) 2 (12)Playing outside with other children 3 (23.75) 1,5 (12.75) 4 (34) 3 (33)Playing outside with you or other adult 3 (24) 2 (13) 4 (34) 2 (23)Going on a swing 3 (23) 1 (12) 3 (24) 1 (12)Bicycling 3 (24) 2 (12.75) 4 (34) 3 (23)Going for a walk 3 (23.5) 3 (24) 3 (23) 3 (23)Jumping on trampoline 4 (34) 2 (13)Playing in the sandpit 2 (13) 1 (11)Playing ball games 3 (24) 1 (12) 3 (34) 2 (23)

    Organized activitiesGoing together to childs leisure activity 4 (34) 4 (24) 4 (44) 4 (24)Going together to siblings leisure activity 1.5 (13) 1 (12) 3 (24) 2 (14)Going together to parents leisure activity 2 (13.75) 1 (11) 3 (24) 1 (13)Going to church 3 (14) 1 (11) 2 (23) 1 (12)Going to habilitation centre activities 3 (23) 2 (13)

    OutingsGoing to the playground 3 (24) 1 (13) 4 (34) 2 (13)Going shopping 3 (23) 4 (34) 3 (23) 4 (34)Going to the library 3 (24) 1 (12) 3 (34) 2 (13)Going to theatre/cinema/concerts 3 (2.754) 2 (12) 4 (34) 2 (12)Visiting friends who have children 3 (23) 2 (1.753) 4 (34) 4 (34)Visiting friends who do not have children 3 (23.5) 2 (12) 3 (24) 1 (12.25)Visiting relatives 3 (24) 3 (24) 4 (34) 4 (34)Going to parties 3 (24) 2 (22) 4 (34) 3 (24)Going out in the nature 3 (24) 1.5 (14) 3 (34) 3 (24)

    Vacation and holiday cottageGoing on vacation 4 (2.54) 2 (13) 4 (44) 3 (23)Going to holiday cottage 3 (34) 1 (14) 4 (44) 1 (14)

    *14, high ratings indicate high level of engagement.14, high ratings indicate high frequency of occurrence.Marked grey when there are differences >1 between the median values of engagement and frequency of occurrence, darker grey means low level of engagement in combination with a highoccurrence.PIMD, profound intellectual and multiple disabilities; TD, typical development.

    528 A.K. Axelsson et al.

    2013 John Wiley & Sons Ltd, Child: care, health and development, 39, 4, 523534

  • Engagement and family characteristics

    Total income in the families with a child with PIMD was sig-

    nificantly higher (P = 0.033) compared with the families withchildren with TD while parents educational level did not differ

    significantly between the two family groups. These family char-

    acteristics in relation to child engagement showed different pat-

    terns in the two groups of children. In the children with PIMD

    there were no positive correlations between the tested family

    characteristics and child engagement in listed family activities.

    This is to be compared with positive correlations in six activities

    in the children with TD where, e.g. both fathers education and

    mothers education were correlated to child engagement in

    playing instruments and going to holiday cottage. Negative

    correlations were found in seven activities in the children with

    PIMD and the children with TD respectively, where only doing

    handicraft and doing morning routines were found in both

    groups however not in combination with the same family

    characteristics (see Table 3).

    To compare child engagement and the earlier studied fre-

    quency of occurrence of the different family activities (Axelsson

    & Wilder 2013), the median values of these aspects were ana-

    lysed descriptively and a difference of >1 step of the ordinal

    scales was looked for. In the families with a child with PIMD

    there were nine activities with low occurrence of the activity and

    at the same time a high child engagement while there were 10

    activities with the same relationship in the families with chil-

    dren with TD. Two of these activities, going to the playground

    and going to holiday cottage, were the same in both groups. The

    opposite relationship, with a low level of engagement in com-

    bination with high frequency of occurrence, occurred only in

    the children with PIMD in the domain Routines (see Table 2).

    Engagement and child characteristics

    Child characteristics were only tested for in the children with

    PIMD. Of these, a positive correlation between engagement and

    cognition were found in the highest number of listed activities.

    Also health, communication, ability to choose and mobility

    were found to be positively related to a number of activities. A

    negative correlation was found between vision and engagement

    (Table 4).

    When exploring the different age groups in this study it was

    found that in children with PIMD there was a significant dif-

    ference in engagement in family activities in six of the 56 listed

    activities depending on age. In younger children there was a

    higher engagement in playing in the sandpit compared with

    the older ones. Older children (aged 1120 years) had a higher

    engagement in dancing, cleaning the house, picking up after

    Table 3. Correlations: family characteristics and engagement in family activities, group with PIMD and group with TD

    Family activity

    Group with PIMD Group with TD

    Familyincome

    Education,father

    Education,mother

    Familyincome

    Education,father

    Education,mother

    rs rs rs rs rs rs

    Positive correlationsPlaying instruments 0.267* 0.435**Dancing 0.248*Going to the playground 0.243*Going to theatre/cinema/concerts 0.274*Going to holiday cottage 0.375** 0.400*

    Negative correlationsDoing handicraft -0.334* -0.561** -0.268**Playing board games -0.340*Playing with pets -0.268*Being together in the kitchen -0.232*Laying the table/cleaning away -0.353*Doing morning routines -0.303* -0.229*Shopping for groceries -0.435**Playing outside with you or other adult -0.348*Bicycling -0.205*Visiting friends who do not have children -0.268*Going out in the nature -0.227*

    *P < 0.05.**P < 0.01.PIMD, profound intellectual and multiple disabilities; TD, typical development.

    Childrens engagement in family activities 529

    2013 John Wiley & Sons Ltd, Child: care, health and development, 39, 4, 523534

  • playing, lying down for rest and in going together to childs

    leisure activity.

    Discussion

    Engagement in family activities differed between the two groups

    of children. The children with PIMD had a lower level of engage-

    ment in most family activities compared with the children with

    TD, but the activities that engaged the children to a higher or

    lesser extent were the same in both groups. In addition, child

    engagement was found to correlate with family characteristics in

    different patterns in the two groups. The children with PIMD had

    a low engagement in routine activities although these activities

    frequently occurred. In the children with PIMD child engagement

    in listed family activities primarily correlated with cognition.

    Differences in level of engagement

    The lower level of engagement of the children with PIMD in

    most family activities probably has several explanations. King

    and colleagues (2009b) used CAPE to compare the enjoyment

    (seen as a mechanism responsible for psychological engage-

    ment) in formal and informal recreation and leisure activities in

    school-aged children with and without disabilities. No signifi-

    cant difference of enjoyment in informal activities between the

    two groups was found. It has to be noted though that in their

    study the children with disabilities did not have the same level

    of difficulties and challenges as for children with PIMD. Also,

    Majnemer and colleagues (2008) used CAPE in their study

    including children with CP, although most with mild motor

    dysfunction. They found that impairments and activity limita-

    Table 4. Correlations: Child characteristics and engagement in family activities, child with PIMD

    Family activityHealth Cognition Communication Vision Decisiveness Mobility

    rs rs rs rs rs rs

    Positive correlationsWatching movie 0.439** 0.364**Watching TV 0.348** 0.432** 0.334* 0.361** 0.397**Playing computer games 0.504** 0.520** 0.357* 0.533** 0.317*Surfing the internet 0.590** 0.548** 0.383* 0.386*Doing handicraft 0.447**Playing with pets 0.540** 0.596** 0.397*Story reading 0.318* 0.335*Being together in the kitchen 0.303*Doing the dishes 0.684**Laying the table/cleaning away 0.501**Having tea or coffee together 0.356*Having dinner together 0.341**Cleaning the house 0.402*Packing school bag 0.483**Picking up after playing 0.425**Gardening 0.452* 0.413* 0.483*Playing outside with other children 0.372*Playing outside with you or other adult 0.435**Going to the playground 0.617** 0.650** 0.620** 0.629**Going to the library 0.424*Going to theatre/cinema/concerts 0.409* 0.407*Going on vacation 0.521**

    Negative correlationsPlaying with you or other adult -0.303*Exercising physical therapy at home -0.404*Cooking/baking -0.296*Going by car to and from school -0.362*Playing outside with you or other adult -0.322*Going for a walk -0.403**Going together to childs leisure activities -0.485**Going to habilitation centre activities -0.510**Visiting friends who have children -0.410*

    *P < 0.05.**P < 0.01.PIMD, profound intellectual and multiple disabilities.

    530 A.K. Axelsson et al.

    2013 John Wiley & Sons Ltd, Child: care, health and development, 39, 4, 523534

  • tions had little influence on enjoyment. The different pattern of

    engagement found in our study might be due to the focus on

    children with PIMD. Children with PIMD are reported to have

    difficulties in regulating sleep-wakefulness and spend more

    time in less alert states (Roberts et al. 2005). Moreover, the

    complex interactions of impairments that children with PIMD

    experience may lead to a narrower repertoire of behaviours than

    for children with TD. Thus, parents may have to assign meaning

    to other kinds of expressions of children with PIMD. If a child

    with PIMD lacks one or more of the common expressions for

    complex feelings, such as curiosity, it may cause parents prob-

    lems in how to interpret the childs reaction who thus receives

    less feedback on being engaged (Iacono et al. 1998).

    In our study, children seem to be more or less engaged in the

    same activities regardless of disabilities or not. For example, in

    both groups of children the level of engagement when joking

    and fooling around was rated highest possible versus doing the

    dishes lowest possible, indicating that child-focused activities

    are more involving than routines. King and colleagues (2009b)

    also found differences similar in children with and without

    disabilities. Further, the results indicate that the types of activi-

    ties that children prefer are impacted by family factors. Thus,

    family activity patterns, i.e. naturally occurring learning situa-

    tions, and childrens interests are important to assess in plan-

    ning family-based interventions (Dunst et al. 2001).

    Engagement and family characteristics

    Only a limited number of family and child characteristics in

    relation to child engagement were studied. Regarding the family

    characteristics educational level and income, only in families

    with a child with PIMD no activities with positive correlations

    between these aspects and engagement were found, meaning

    that for children with PIMD a higher family income or parent

    educational level was not associated to higher child engagement

    in family activities. It might be that family everyday life are so

    affected by having a child with PIMD that the family activity

    patterns usually related to socio-economic status are suppressed

    and the system becomes more child-driven (Keogh et al. 2000).

    Gallimore et al. (1989) argue that family constraints have to be

    considered in relation to the goals and purposes of the family,

    which might be related to socio-economic status. The goals and

    purposes might also change as a consequence of child impair-

    ments. For example, family life might become more focused on

    making within home family activities available for the child.

    This can explain why a negative correlation was found between

    parental educational level and child engagement in home-based

    family activities.

    In the study by Majnemer and colleagues (2008) on children

    with CP, a moderate positive relation between enjoyment in

    informal activities and frequency and numbers of activities was

    found. Considering this, it is interesting to note that in our

    study it was found that a low level of engagement in children

    with PIMD was seen in the frequently occurring routines. This

    might be interpreted as in such routines the child is allowed to

    have a more passive role, maybe in order to be given some kind

    of rest. An alternative interpretation is that the parents are so

    focused on doing the routine that they have difficulties to pay

    attention to the relatively weak expressions of engagement by

    many children with PIMD (Iacono et al. 1998).

    Engagement and child characteristics

    Concerning child characteristics, cognition was related to

    engagement in a high number of family activities. The relation

    between cognitive level and engagement in children with PIMD

    may be dependent on the previously mentioned states of alert-

    ness of the child (Granlund et al. 2013). Cognition and engage-

    ment were primarily correlated in unstructured child driven

    activities. Also the activities where there were correlations

    between engagement and health, ability to choose, and mobility

    could be seen as child driven. In contrast, the negative correla-

    tions between engagement and vision were primarily seen in

    activities that seem to be less child-driven. Instead, they include

    an engaged adult, for example a parent who verbally described

    the environment when going for a walk.

    Child-PFA

    Child-PFA was developed to get an insight in childrens lives in

    their family micro setting. Frequency of occurrence of the

    activities and the childs presence (Axelsson & Wilder 2013)

    were not assumed to generate enough information about the

    childs participation and thus questions about engagement were

    included as well. The results of the children with PIMD were

    compared with the children with TD to explore any possible

    restriction in natural learning opportunities. The collected

    information about who usually was present in the activities

    could form the basis for future, deeper studies about participa-

    tion in family activities. However, to really understand what

    participation in family activities looks like observation studies

    with interaction analyses are needed (Vlaskamp 2005; Wilder

    2008; Munde et al. 2011). Regarding the outcome of interaction

    childrens level of engagement is probably possible to observe

    (Granlund et al. 2012). Though, ethical issues should be con-

    sidered carefully since observations might interfere with family

    life and personal integrity.

    Childrens engagement in family activities 531

    2013 John Wiley & Sons Ltd, Child: care, health and development, 39, 4, 523534

  • Limitations of the study

    The childs subjective experiences of engagement were in both

    groups proxy estimated even though the child ideally should be

    enabled to rate the perceived level of engagement himself or

    herself (Simeonsson et al. 2003). Children with TD are not

    expected to be able to report about their engagement until

    around age 8 or 9 years old (Young et al. 1995) and for children

    with PIMD proxy estimation is considered the only alternative.

    In consequence we asked parents to be proxies and thus we had

    to rely on that the parents knew their children very well and

    were able to interpret the childrens expressions. Of all distrib-

    uted Child-PFA questionnaires to the families with a child with

    PIMD, just 20% were returned and included. A somewhat

    higher attrition rate was reported by King and colleagues

    (2009b) and in their study it was mainly due to no response or

    not willing to participate. These high attrition rates may indi-

    cate lack of time and energy in families to be involved in

    surveys.

    The two groups of children in the present study diverge con-

    cerning age, level of needs and challenges. However, for children

    with PIMD an increasing discrepancy between the developmen-

    tal and chronological age is developed over time (Granlund

    et al. 2013). In the studied group of children with PIMD, the age

    differences were found to only partly affect child engagement in

    activities, where an increased engagement was primarily seen in

    the older group of children (aged 1120 years). Given that chil-

    dren with TD normally become less engaged by age in family

    activities, this result could in fact possibly mean that if only

    younger children with PIMD had been included the result

    would have diverged even more between the two groups. In

    addition, it should be noted that a large number of statistical

    tests were performed, increasing the risk of type I errors.

    Conclusion

    Participation has been recognized as an important overall

    outcome for children and their families. In an earlier study

    using the same sample the frequency of occurrence of family

    activities and the childs presence were found to be lower in

    families with a child with PIMD compared with families with

    children with TD. In this study also a lower level of engagement

    was found when comparing the two groups of children

    however the pattern of engagement dependent on type of activ-

    ity was the same. In this study it was found that the childs

    cognitive level affected the level of engagement positively where

    the opposite has been found in other studies, mostly including

    children with less severe disabilities. In families with a child with

    PIMD the family system appears more child-driven and the

    effects of socio-economic status on family activity patterns seen

    in families with children with TD are less pronounced. This in

    turn might be due to the time and effort it takes in accommo-

    dating everyday family life when having a child with PIMD.

    When seeing the differences in both performance and engage-

    ment between the two groups of children, the need for an

    increased knowledge about how to improve participation in

    children with PIMD emerges. Future research should focus on

    finding facilitating factors for participation in family activities

    for children with PIMD.

    Key messages

    Participation is known to be of great importance for all

    childrens development and emotional well-being.

    Engagement in family activities of children with profound

    intellectual and multiple disabilities (PIMD) has not

    explicitly been studied earlier in research.

    This study found that engagement in family activities was

    lower in children with PIMD compared to children with

    typical development.

    The children with PIMD had a low engagement in routine

    activities although they frequently occurred.

    It was also found that childs cognition affected the level of

    engagement positively.

    Acknowledgement

    We are grateful to all the families who participated in this study

    and for the cooperation with the disability organizations JAG,

    RBU and FUB.

    The study was financed by the Swedish Inheritance Fund.

    References

    Axelsson, A. K. & Wilder, J. (2013) Frequency of occurrence and

    child presence in family activities: a quantitative, comparative

    study of children with profound intellectual and multiple

    disabilities and children with typical development. International

    Journal of Developmental Disabilities (in press) doi: 10.1179/

    2047387712Y.0000000008.

    Bailey, D. B., Simeonsson, R. J., Buysse, V. & Smith, T. (1993)

    Reliability of an index of child characteristics. Developmental

    Medicine and Child Neurology, 35, 806815.

    Bronfenbrenner, U. (1995) Developmental ecology through space and

    time: a future perspective. In: Examining Lives in Context:

    532 A.K. Axelsson et al.

    2013 John Wiley & Sons Ltd, Child: care, health and development, 39, 4, 523534

  • Perspectives on the Ecology of Human Development, 1st edn, APA

    science volumes (eds P. Moen, G. H. Elder, K. Lscher &

    U. Bronfenbrenner), pp. 619647. American Psychological

    Association, Washington, DC, USA.

    Bronfenbrenner, U. (1999) Environments in developmental

    perspective: theoretical and operational models. In: Measuring

    Environment across the Life Span: Emerging Methods and

    Concepts, 1st edn (eds S. L. Friedman & T. D. Wachs),

    pp. 328. American Psychological Association, Washington, DC,

    USA.

    Coster, W., Bedell, G., Law, M., Khetani, M. A., Teplicky, R.,

    Liljenquist, K., Gleason, K. & Kao, Y. C. (2011) Psychometric

    evaluation of the participation and environment measure for

    children and youth. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology,

    53, 10301037.

    Coster, W. J., Mancini, M. C. & Ludlow, L. H. (1999) Factor structure

    of the school function assessment. Educational and Psychological

    Measurement, 59, 665677.

    Dunst, C. J., Bruder, M. B., Trivette, C. M., Hamby, D., Raab, M. &

    Mclean, M. (2001) Characteristics and consequences of everyday

    natural learning opportunities. Topics in Early Childhood Special

    Education, 21, 6892.

    Dunst, C. J., Hamby, D., Trivette, C. M., Raab, M. & Bruder, M. B.

    (2002) Young childrens participation in everyday family and

    community activity. Psychological Reports, 91 (3 Pt 1), 875897.

    Gallimore, R., Weisner, T. S., Kaufman, S. Z. & Bernheimer, L. P.

    (1989) The social construction of ecocultural niches family

    accommodation of developmentally delayed children. American

    Journal of Mental Retardation, 94, 216230.

    Granlund, M., Arvidsson, P., Niia, A., Bjrck-kesson, E.,

    Simeonsson, R., Maxwell, G., Adolfsson, M., Eriksson-Augustine, L.

    & Pless, M. (2012) Differentiating activity and participation of

    children and youth with disability in Sweden: a third qualifier in

    the International Classification of Functioning, Disability, and

    Health for Children and Youth? American Journal of Physical

    Medicine and Rehabilitation, 91, S84.

    Granlund, M., Wilder, J. & Almqvist, L. (2013) Positive psychology

    and severe multiple disabilities. In: Handbook of Positive Psychology

    and Disability (ed. M. Wehmeyer). Oxford University Press, New

    York, NY, USA (in press).

    Hammel, J., Magasi, S., Heinemann, A., Whiteneck, G., Bogner, J. &

    Rodriguez, E. (2008) What does participation mean? An insider

    perspective from people with disabilities. Disability and

    Rehabilitation, 30, 14451460.

    Hoogsteen, L. & Woodgate, R. L. (2010) Can I play? A concept

    analysis of participation in children with disabilities. Physical &

    occupational therapy in pediatrics, 30, 325339.

    Hwang, J. L., Davies, P. L., Taylor, M. P. & Gavin, W. J. (2002)

    Validation of school function assessment with elementary

    school children. OTJR: Occupation, Participation and Health,

    22, 4858.

    Iacono, T., Carter, M. & Hook, J. (1998) Identification of intentional

    communication in students with severe and multiple disabilities.

    Augmentative and Alternative Communication, 14, 102114.

    Keogh, B. K., Garnier, H. E., Bernheimer, L. P. & Gallimore, R.

    (2000) Models of child-family interactions for children with

    developmental delays: child-driven or transactional? American

    Journal of Mental Retardation, 105, 3246.

    King, G., Law, M., King, S., Hurley, P., Hanna, S., Kertoy, M. &

    Rosenbaum, P. (2005) Measuring childrens participation in

    recreation and leisure activities: construct validation of the

    CAPE and PAC. Child: Care, Health and Development, 33,

    2839.

    King, G., McDougall, J., Dewit, D., Petrenchik, T., Hurley, P. & Law,

    M. (2009a) Predictors of change over time in the activity

    participation of children and youth with physical disabilities.

    Childrens Health Care, 38, 321351.

    King, G., Petrenchik, T., Law, M. & Hurley, P. (2009b) The enjoyment

    of formal and informal recreation and leisure activities: a

    comparison of school-aged children with and without physical

    disabilities. International Journal of Disability, Development and

    Education, 56, 109130.

    Majnemer, A., Shevell, M., Law, M., Birnbaum, R., Chilingaryan, G.,

    Rosenbaum, P. & Poulin, C. (2008) Participation and enjoyment

    of leisure activities in school-aged children with cerebral palsy.

    Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology, 50, 751758.

    Maxwell, G. & Granlund, M. (2011) How are conditions for

    participation expressed in education policy documents? A review

    of documents in Scotland and Sweden. European Journal of Special

    Needs Education, 26, 251272.

    Maxwell, G., Augustine, L. & Granlund, M. (2012) Does thinking

    and doing the same thing amount to involved participation?

    Empirical explorations for finding a measure of intensity for a

    third ICF-CY qualifier. Developmental Neurorehabilitation, 15,

    274283.

    McWilliam, R. A. (1991) Childrens Engagement Questionnaire,

    Chapel Hill, NC: Frank porter Graham Child Development Center,

    University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill.

    Munde, V., Vlaskamp, C., Ruijssenaars, W. & Nakken, H. (2011)

    Determining alertness in individuals with profound intellectual

    and multiple disabilities: the reliability of an observation list.

    Education and Training in Autism and Developmental Disabilities,

    46, 116123.

    Munde, V. S., Vlaskamp, C., Ruijssenaars, A. J. J. M. & Nakken, H.

    (2009) Alertness in individuals with profound intellectual and

    multiple disabilities: a literature review. Research in Developmental

    Disabilities, 30, 462480.

    Nakken, H. & Vlaskamp, C. (2002) Joining forces: supporting

    individuals with profound multiple learning disabilities. Tizard

    Learning Disability Review, 7, 1015.

    Nakken, H. & Vlaskamp, C. (2007) A need for a taxonomy for

    profound intellectual and multiple disabilities. Journal of Policy

    and Practice in Intellectual Disabilities, 4, 8387.

    Orlin, M. N., Palisano, R. J., Chiarello, L. A., Kang, L. I. N. J. U.,

    Polansky, M., Almasri, N. & Maggs, J. (2010) Participation in

    home, extracurricular, and community activities among children

    and young people with cerebral palsy. Developmental Medicine

    and Child Neurology, 52, 160166.

    Childrens engagement in family activities 533

    2013 John Wiley & Sons Ltd, Child: care, health and development, 39, 4, 523534

  • Perlman, D. (2007) The best of times, the worst of times: the place

    of close relationships in psychology and our daily lives. Canadian

    Psychology = Psychologie Canadienne, 48, 718.Roberts, S., Arthur-Kelly, M., Foreman, P. & Pascoe, S. (2005)

    Educational approaches for maximizing arousal in children

    with multiple and severe disability: new directions for research

    and practice in early childhood contexts. Developmental

    Neurorehabilitation, 8, 8891.

    SFS 1993:387. Lag (1993:387) om std och service till vissa

    funktionshindrade (LSS) [The Act Concerning Support and

    Service for Persons with Certain Functional Impairments].

    Stockholm: Socialstyrelsen.

    SFS 1993:389. Lag (1993:389) om assistansersttning (LASS) [The Act

    Concerning Public Funding of Personal Assistance]. Stockholm:

    Socialstyrelsen.

    Simeonsson, R. J., Carlson, D., Huntington, G. S., Mcmillen, J. S. &

    Brent, J. L. (2001) Students with disabilities: a national survey of

    participation in school activities. Disability and Rehabilitation, 23,

    4963.

    Simeonsson, R. J., Leonardi, M., Lollar, D., Bjorck-Akesson, E.,

    Hollenweger, J. & Martinuzzi, A. (2003) Applying the International

    Classification of Functioning, Disability and Health (ICF) to

    measure childhood disability. Disability and Rehabilitation, 25,

    602610.

    Vlaskamp, C. (2005) Assessing people with profound intellectual

    and multiple disabilities. In: Assessing Adults with Intellectual

    Disabilities: A Service Providers Guide (eds J. Hogg & A. Langa),

    pp. 152163. BPS Blackwell, Malden, MA, USA.

    Wachs, T. D. (2000) Necessary but not Sufficient: The Respective

    Roles of Single and Multiple Influences on Individual Development.

    American Psychological Association, Washington, DC, USA.

    Wilder, J. (2008) Proximal processes of children with profound

    multiple disabilities. Doctoral thesis. Stockholm University,

    Stockholm, Sweden.

    World Health Organization (2007) International Classification

    of Functioning, Disability and Health: Children and Youth

    Version: ICF-CY. World Health Organization, Geneva,

    Switzerland.

    Young, N. L., Yoshida, K. K., Williams, J. I., Bombardier, C. &

    Wright, J. G. (1995) The role of children in reporting their

    physical-disability. Archives of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation,

    76, 913918.

    534 A.K. Axelsson et al.

    2013 John Wiley & Sons Ltd, Child: care, health and development, 39, 4, 523534

  • This document is a scanned copy of a printed document. No warranty is given about theaccuracy of the copy. Users should refer to the original published version of the material.