53
EVALUATION OF PREFERENCE FOR REINFORCEMENT OR RESPONSE COST CONDITIONS By CRISTINA MARIA WHITEHOUSE A THESIS PRESENTED TO THE GRADUATE SCHOOL OF THE UNIVERSITY OF FLORIDA IN PARTIAL FULFILLMENT OF THE REQUIREMENTS FOR THE DEGREE OF MASTER OF SCIENCE UNIVERSITY OF FLORIDA 2010 1

To my Dad, Henry Remsen Whitehouse IIufdcimages.uflib.ufl.edu/UF/E0/04/19/91/00001/whitehouse... · 2013. 5. 31. · & Kahan, 1992; Frentz & Kelly, 1986, Miltenbeger, Parish, Rickert,

  • Upload
    others

  • View
    1

  • Download
    0

Embed Size (px)

Citation preview

Page 1: To my Dad, Henry Remsen Whitehouse IIufdcimages.uflib.ufl.edu/UF/E0/04/19/91/00001/whitehouse... · 2013. 5. 31. · & Kahan, 1992; Frentz & Kelly, 1986, Miltenbeger, Parish, Rickert,

EVALUATION OF PREFERENCE FOR REINFORCEMENT OR RESPONSE COST CONDITIONS

By

CRISTINA MARIA WHITEHOUSE

A THESIS PRESENTED TO THE GRADUATE SCHOOL OF THE UNIVERSITY OF FLORIDA IN PARTIAL FULFILLMENT

OF THE REQUIREMENTS FOR THE DEGREE OF MASTER OF SCIENCE

UNIVERSITY OF FLORIDA

2010

1

Page 2: To my Dad, Henry Remsen Whitehouse IIufdcimages.uflib.ufl.edu/UF/E0/04/19/91/00001/whitehouse... · 2013. 5. 31. · & Kahan, 1992; Frentz & Kelly, 1986, Miltenbeger, Parish, Rickert,

© 2010 Cristina Maria Whitehouse

2

Page 3: To my Dad, Henry Remsen Whitehouse IIufdcimages.uflib.ufl.edu/UF/E0/04/19/91/00001/whitehouse... · 2013. 5. 31. · & Kahan, 1992; Frentz & Kelly, 1986, Miltenbeger, Parish, Rickert,

To my Dad, Henry Remsen Whitehouse II

3

Page 4: To my Dad, Henry Remsen Whitehouse IIufdcimages.uflib.ufl.edu/UF/E0/04/19/91/00001/whitehouse... · 2013. 5. 31. · & Kahan, 1992; Frentz & Kelly, 1986, Miltenbeger, Parish, Rickert,

ACKNOWLEDGMENTS

I would like to express my sincere gratitude to my mentor throughout graduate

school, Dr. Timothy Vollmer, for all of his immeasurable support and guidance. I would

also like to thank the members of my committee, Drs. Lise Abrams and Brian Iwata for

their editorial comments and assistance. Additional thanks are given to my

undergraduate research volunteers: Rocio Cuevas, Sean Jones, Stephanie Moir, Dana

Spears, Julia Uthmeier, and Danielle Willis for all their assistance with data collection. I

would also like to thank my mother, Rosalia M. Whitehouse, my sister, Joan M.

Whitehouse, for their unwavering love, support, and encouragement. Finally, to my

husband, Ron, and son, Colin, thank you for making everything worthwhile.

4

Page 5: To my Dad, Henry Remsen Whitehouse IIufdcimages.uflib.ufl.edu/UF/E0/04/19/91/00001/whitehouse... · 2013. 5. 31. · & Kahan, 1992; Frentz & Kelly, 1986, Miltenbeger, Parish, Rickert,

TABLE OF CONTENTS page

ACKNOWLEDGMENTS .................................................................................................. 4

LIST OF TABLES ............................................................................................................ 7

LIST OF FIGURES .......................................................................................................... 8

ABSTRACT ..................................................................................................................... 9

INTRODUCTION ........................................................................................................... 11

GENERAL METHOD ..................................................................................................... 18

Setting and Materials .............................................................................................. 18 Session Sequence .................................................................................................. 18 Dependent Variables .............................................................................................. 20 Data Collection and Interobserver Agreement ........................................................ 20

EXPERIMENT I: RESPONSE COST VS. REINFORCEMENT: COLORED BACKGROUNDS .................................................................................................... 22

Method .................................................................................................................... 22 Subjects ............................................................................................................ 22 Procedure ......................................................................................................... 22

Results and Discussion ........................................................................................... 23

EXPERIMENT II: RESPONSE COST VS. REINFORCEMENT: DIFFERENT SYMBOL SETS ...................................................................................................... 27

Methods .................................................................................................................. 27 Subjects ............................................................................................................ 27 Procedure ......................................................................................................... 27 Design .............................................................................................................. 27

Results and Discussion ........................................................................................... 28

EXPERIMENT III: RESPONSE COST VS. REINFORCEMEN: COLOR AND SYMBOLS CONTROLLED ..................................................................................... 33

Methods .................................................................................................................. 33 Subjects ............................................................................................................ 33 Procedure ......................................................................................................... 33 Design .............................................................................................................. 33

Results and Discussions ......................................................................................... 34

5

Page 6: To my Dad, Henry Remsen Whitehouse IIufdcimages.uflib.ufl.edu/UF/E0/04/19/91/00001/whitehouse... · 2013. 5. 31. · & Kahan, 1992; Frentz & Kelly, 1986, Miltenbeger, Parish, Rickert,

EXPERIMENT IV: RESPONSE COST VS REINFORCEMENT: NUMBERING TRIALS ................................................................................................................... 43

Methods .................................................................................................................. 43 Subjects ............................................................................................................ 43 Procedure ......................................................................................................... 43 Design .............................................................................................................. 43

Results and Discussion ........................................................................................... 43

GENERAL DISCUSSION .............................................................................................. 46

LIST OF REFERENCES ............................................................................................... 50

BIOGRAPHICAL SKETCH ............................................................................................ 53

6

Page 7: To my Dad, Henry Remsen Whitehouse IIufdcimages.uflib.ufl.edu/UF/E0/04/19/91/00001/whitehouse... · 2013. 5. 31. · & Kahan, 1992; Frentz & Kelly, 1986, Miltenbeger, Parish, Rickert,

LIST OF TABLES

Table page 3-1. Summary of latencies, point comparisons, and side effects. ................................. 26

4-1. Average latencies for Elena and Allen. .................................................................. 31

4-2. Points at first choice point for Elena and Allen. ...................................................... 32

5-1. Latency summary for all participants. ..................................................................... 40

5-2. Point totals at the first choice point in each phase. ................................................ 41

7

Page 8: To my Dad, Henry Remsen Whitehouse IIufdcimages.uflib.ufl.edu/UF/E0/04/19/91/00001/whitehouse... · 2013. 5. 31. · & Kahan, 1992; Frentz & Kelly, 1986, Miltenbeger, Parish, Rickert,

LIST OF FIGURES

Figure page 3-1. Proportion of choice selections per session for Nicole and Maribel. ...................... 25

4-1. Proportion of choice selections per session for Elena and Allen. ........................... 30

5-1. Results for participants showing a preference for reinforcement. .......................... 37

5-2. Results for participants showing indifference. ........................................................ 38

5-3. Results for participant showing a preference for response cost. ............................ 39

6-1. Results for Cole, Kylie, Monica, and Amelia. ......................................................... 45

8

Page 9: To my Dad, Henry Remsen Whitehouse IIufdcimages.uflib.ufl.edu/UF/E0/04/19/91/00001/whitehouse... · 2013. 5. 31. · & Kahan, 1992; Frentz & Kelly, 1986, Miltenbeger, Parish, Rickert,

Abstract of Thesis Presented to the Graduate School of the University of Florida in Partial Fulfillment of the Requirements for the Degree of Master of Science

EVALUATION OF PREFERENCE FOR REINFORCEMENT OR RESPONSE COST

CONDITIONS

By

Cristina Maria Whitehouse

August 2010

Chair: Timothy R. Vollmer Major: Psychology

The use of response cost and reinforcement-based interventions (e.g., token

economies, group level systems, grading with points) is common in academic settings.

Despite the ubiquity of these interventions, few studies have evaluated child preference

for response cost versus reinforcement. Furthermore, the studies have yielded mixed

results. The present study involved assessments of child preference for reinforcement

or response cost in a series of 4 experiments. In each experiment, typically developing

children were repeatedly presented with a computerized matching to sample task under

both reinforcement and response cost conditions. Following exposure to each condition,

children were asked to select their subsequent working conditions. Child selections

were the primary dependent measure of choice. This preparation was repeated using

different stimuli, to assess if preference results could be reproduced. Additionally, this

preparation was repeated using math problems appropriate for the child’s grade level.

Results of Experiments I and II showed that preference was influenced by variables

other than the contingencies presented. In Experiment III, 8 of the 12 participants

showed a preference for reinforcement. However, preliminary results of Experiment IV

indicated that numbering the trials during the session may attenuate any overall

9

Page 10: To my Dad, Henry Remsen Whitehouse IIufdcimages.uflib.ufl.edu/UF/E0/04/19/91/00001/whitehouse... · 2013. 5. 31. · & Kahan, 1992; Frentz & Kelly, 1986, Miltenbeger, Parish, Rickert,

10

preference for reinforcement. These data have implications for assessing client

treatment preference and the acceptability of response cost procedures.

Page 11: To my Dad, Henry Remsen Whitehouse IIufdcimages.uflib.ufl.edu/UF/E0/04/19/91/00001/whitehouse... · 2013. 5. 31. · & Kahan, 1992; Frentz & Kelly, 1986, Miltenbeger, Parish, Rickert,

CHAPTER 1 INTRODUCTION

When given options, practitioners tend to choose interventions that are effective,

least restrictive, and likely to be implemented by the client or caregivers. Variables that

influence treatment selection decisions include treatment practicality, efficacy, social

acceptability, and client preference (Hanley, Piazza, Fisher, Contrucci, & Maglieri,

1997). Although providing clients with opportunities to choose is an important part of a

client’s right to effective treatment (Bannerman, Sheldon, Sherman, & Harchik, 1990),

research in the area of client treatment preference is sparse.

Recently, however, there have been some investigations that have primarily

focused on client treatment preference. Hanley et al., (1997), for example, examined

preference for two commonly recommended and equally effective interventions,

functional communication training (FCT) and noncontingent reinforcement (NCR), in the

reduction of aggression with two participants. The authors utilized a concurrent-chain

procedure, commonly used in choice experiments, to identify preference. The primary

dependent variable was the number of switch button presses among concurrently

available buttons. Each button was correlated with access to different treatments.

Results for both participants indicated a preference for the FCT training. The authors

suggested that response-dependent schedules may be preferred to response-

independent schedules because participants’ behavior controls the rate of

reinforcement. Importantly, the study established a method for assessing client

treatment preference for children with limited verbal skills.

Hanley, Piazza, Fisher, and Maglieri (2005) applied a similar concurrent-chain

procedure to investigate treatment preference for FCT with and without punishment.

11

Page 12: To my Dad, Henry Remsen Whitehouse IIufdcimages.uflib.ufl.edu/UF/E0/04/19/91/00001/whitehouse... · 2013. 5. 31. · & Kahan, 1992; Frentz & Kelly, 1986, Miltenbeger, Parish, Rickert,

Results from the two participants indicated preferences for FCT with punishment. The

punishment component was a 30-s hands-down procedure, and in addition, for one

participant, a 30-s visual block. The authors concluded that if the interventions were

selected based solely on structure or name, FCT with punishment may not have been

chosen because of the punishment component. Furthermore, the authors suggested

that recent trends toward eschewing punishment procedures based on name or

structure would have eliminated a treatment that was both effective and preferred by

these two individuals. The study raises important ethical implications of examining

treatment preference when considering interventions that involve punishment.

The decision to use or not use punishment procedures is a topic of long standing

controversy in behavior analysis. This issue is often debated in the context of

punishment procedures that are highly restrictive or aversive, such as shock. However,

punishment is defined technically as any stimulus change occurring after a response

that decreases the future probability of that response (Michael, 1993). Thus,

interventions such as response cost, which involves contingent loss of reinforcers, are

punishment procedures if they result in reductions in behavior.

Response cost has been shown to be an effective behavior reduction treatment on

a variety of topographies from skin picking (Allen & Harris, 1966) to sleep problems in

children (Piazza & Fisher, 1991). Response cost also is commonly used in academic

environments, such as when points are lost contingent on incorrect answers, and has

been shown to be effective (Phillips, Phillips, Fixsen & Wolf, 1971). Additionally, several

investigations have used academic tasks to compare reinforcement procedures to

response cost procedures (Brent & Routh, 1978; Broughton & Lahey, 1978; Erickson,

12

Page 13: To my Dad, Henry Remsen Whitehouse IIufdcimages.uflib.ufl.edu/UF/E0/04/19/91/00001/whitehouse... · 2013. 5. 31. · & Kahan, 1992; Frentz & Kelly, 1986, Miltenbeger, Parish, Rickert,

Wyne, Routh, 1973; Holt & Hobbs,1979; Panek, 1970). In these studies, the dependent

measure typically was the number of correct and incorrect responses to a particular

academic task. Overall, results of these studies were mixed as to which procedure is

more effective.

Although response cost procedures have been shown to be effective, they are

often avoided by practitioners in favor of reinforcement-based procedures for various

reasons including the presumed increased potential for negative side effects that are

often associated with punishment procedures (Azrin & Holz, 1966). However, few

studies have actually reported side effects associated with response cost. In fact,

Sprute, Williams, and McLaughlin (1990) did not observe any negative side effects

when tokens were removed contingent on problem behavior, nor did a questionnaire

given to the children indicate any difference in the ratings between reinforcement and

response cost. However, Boren and Colman (1970) observed an increase in escape

behavior, such as skipping meetings, among delinquent soldiers during response cost

conditions compared to reinforcement conditions. The authors concluded that some

counter control may have been observed given the overall restrictiveness of the

environment. Sattler, Betz, and Zellner (1978) suggested that negative side effects may

be more frequent when earned points are removed, rather than when points earned

noncontingently are removed. Overall, however, the number of studies reporting

negative side effects associated with response cost is few.

Another reason practitioners may avoid the use of response cost is because it is

viewed as a more intrusive, restrictive procedure. However, research on treatment

acceptability has shown that parents repeatedly rate response cost favorably (Blampied

13

Page 14: To my Dad, Henry Remsen Whitehouse IIufdcimages.uflib.ufl.edu/UF/E0/04/19/91/00001/whitehouse... · 2013. 5. 31. · & Kahan, 1992; Frentz & Kelly, 1986, Miltenbeger, Parish, Rickert,

& Kahan, 1992; Frentz & Kelly, 1986, Miltenbeger, Parish, Rickert, & Kohr, 1989).

Assumptions about the superiority of positive reinforcement seem to stem from

structural features of the contingencies, such as delivering reinforcers rather than

removing reinforcers. Additionally, response cost is often listed as an intrusive

procedure and regulations are placed on its use without reference to context (e.g.,

Florida Administrative Code, Title XXIX, Chapter 393, 2009). Further, influential authors

in behavior analysis have recommended against the use of response cost (e.g.,

Sidman, 1989). In some such cases, examples are given to distinguish reinforcement

and response cost in ways that are nothing more than semantic, such as losing the

privilege of driving the family car versus earning the right to drive the family car (e.g.,

Latham, 1995), wherein the former is discouraged but the latter encouraged as a

parenting practices.

With regard to preference, few studies have compared preference for

reinforcement or response cost. Two studies specifically compared performance and

preference for reinforcement or response cost conditions. Iwata and Bailey (1974)

divided a special education math class into two groups. The reinforcement group had

tokens placed into a cup for the absence of any classroom rules violations. The

response cost group started the math period with 10 tokens, and lost tokens for

instances of classroom rule violations. The 40-min math class was divided into intervals

ranging from 3-5 min in duration, and, following the sessions, children with at least 6

tokens were allowed to have a snack. After a return to baseline, the groups were

reversed: the group previously exposed to the reinforcement condition switched to the

response cost condition, and the previous response cost group was exposed to the

14

Page 15: To my Dad, Henry Remsen Whitehouse IIufdcimages.uflib.ufl.edu/UF/E0/04/19/91/00001/whitehouse... · 2013. 5. 31. · & Kahan, 1992; Frentz & Kelly, 1986, Miltenbeger, Parish, Rickert,

reinforcement condition. Following 10 sessions, each child was given a daily choice

between the two contingencies. Children’s choices were measured across three days.

Results from the choice phase showed mixed preference; four students consistently

selected reinforcement, five students consistently chose response cost, and six

students switched their choice at least once. Additionally, side effects such as

inappropriate vocalizations were not observed when students were working under the

response cost conditions.

Conversely, Sattler et al. (1978) reported preference for reinforcement over

response cost. In this study, children participated in a switch press task in a laboratory.

Eighty-six children were divided into 2 groups: a reinforcement group and a response

cost group. Each child completed the switch-press task for three sessions. During the

first two sessions, the child was exposed to 44 trials of either reinforcement or response

cost. Under reinforcement, the child started with zero pennies and earned one for every

correct response. Under response cost, the child started with forty pennies and lost one

for every incorrect response. The first 4 trials in each session were practice trials, which

exposed the child to two losses and two wins. In the third session, the child was asked

to select his or her next working condition and completed 20 experimental trials in that

condition. Overall results showed a preference towards reinforcement, with 62 of the 80

children selecting reinforcement. Additionally, the authors reported a negative side

effect associated with response cost. Response times under response cost were

reported as statistically longer than the response times under reinforcement. However,

the mean response time was 2.48 seconds for response cost and 2.14 seconds for

reinforcement, a minimally discernable difference.

15

Page 16: To my Dad, Henry Remsen Whitehouse IIufdcimages.uflib.ufl.edu/UF/E0/04/19/91/00001/whitehouse... · 2013. 5. 31. · & Kahan, 1992; Frentz & Kelly, 1986, Miltenbeger, Parish, Rickert,

There is reason to view the Sattler et al (1978) results cautiously. Each condition

in the study was correlated with a sound when points were removed or delivered. In all

cases, the response cost conditions included a buzzing sound if tokens were removed,

and the reinforcement conditions included a bell sound when tokens were earned. It is

not clear what effects these correlated sounds had, if any, because a reversal was not

included in this study. For example, to test whether the bell and buzzer sounds had any

effects on the children’s preference, the sound associated in each condition should

have been switched. Thus, earning points under reinforcement would result in a buzzer

sound and errors in response cost would have resulted in a bell sound. Including such a

comparison would have allowed for an examination of whether the correlated sounds

influenced child preference.

Given the limited and mixed results with regard to preference for reinforcement or

response cost, the purpose of this study is to evaluate preference and child behavior

under reinforcement and response cost conditions. Procedural variations in

Experiments I through IV attempted to evaluate preference with an acquisition task. This

type of task was selected for this investigation because of the ubiquity of reinforcement

and response cost procedures in classrooms and because Iwata and Bailey (1974) had

already shown ambiguity of preference during behavior reduction procedures.

Experiment I evaluated child preference for reinforcement or response cost using

a computerized matching to sample task. In this study, background colors were

correlated with each contingency to make the changes in the contingencies more

salient. Experiment II removed the background colors, to control for any potential color

bias and introduced two different but similar tasks for the reinforcement and response

16

Page 17: To my Dad, Henry Remsen Whitehouse IIufdcimages.uflib.ufl.edu/UF/E0/04/19/91/00001/whitehouse... · 2013. 5. 31. · & Kahan, 1992; Frentz & Kelly, 1986, Miltenbeger, Parish, Rickert,

17

cost conditions. In Experiment III, preference was evaluated using the same exact task,

to eliminate any bias towards any small differences in the tasks. Experiment IV

evaluated whether using numbered trials would influence contingency preference.

Page 18: To my Dad, Henry Remsen Whitehouse IIufdcimages.uflib.ufl.edu/UF/E0/04/19/91/00001/whitehouse... · 2013. 5. 31. · & Kahan, 1992; Frentz & Kelly, 1986, Miltenbeger, Parish, Rickert,

CHAPTER 2 GENERAL METHOD

Setting and Materials

Sessions for most participants were conducted in an elementary school cafeteria

during an afterschool program’s homework time, which provided a relatively quiet and

isolated work area. Sessions for three participants (Nicole, Maribel, and Elena) were

conducted in their home.

A laptop computer loaded with a program written in C++® specifically for the

purpose of this study was used during all experiments.

Session Sequence

Each session included 4 components, 10 trials per component, and three choice

selections. The first two components were forced exposures to both reinforcement and

response cost. In the first session, the first condition presented was randomly selected.

For subsequent sessions, the order of the two conditions presented was

counterbalanced across sessions. During the reinforcement condition, a number line

appeared with a red dot on “0.” For every correct response, the screen flashed “correct,”

and the red dot advanced along the number line indicating a point gain. For every

incorrect response, the screen flashed “incorrect,” and the dot did not advance. During

the response cost conditions, the top of the screen showed the same number line but

the red dot started on 10. For every correct response, the screen flashed “correct,” and

the red dot did not move. For every incorrect response, the screen flashed “incorrect,”

and the dot moved down the number line indicating a point loss. In both conditions, the

screen also featured a pause button that allowed the child to take breaks if necessary.

18

Page 19: To my Dad, Henry Remsen Whitehouse IIufdcimages.uflib.ufl.edu/UF/E0/04/19/91/00001/whitehouse... · 2013. 5. 31. · & Kahan, 1992; Frentz & Kelly, 1986, Miltenbeger, Parish, Rickert,

At the end of each component, a summary screen appeared that displayed the number

lines and the number of points earned under each completed component.

Following the forced exposures to reinforcement and response cost (components

1 and 2), the first choice opportunity appeared. The child was given an opportunity to

select the next condition he or she would complete. The choice screen displayed the

two number lines, one with the dot on “0” and the other with the dot on “10.” The first

condition to which the child had been exposed previously appeared on the left of the

screen, and the second condition appeared on the right of the screen. Following the

child’s selection, he or she completed the third component of 10 trials in the selected

condition. Next, the second choice screen appeared. This time the child was asked to

select his or her favorite condition. Again, the number lines appeared in the same

manner as the previous choice screen. Following the child’s selection, the third and final

choice screen appeared. This time the number line placements were reversed from the

previous two choice screens. The child was then asked to select one more working

condition and then completed the fourth component of the session, 10 trials under the

selected condition.

Prior to beginning a session, the children were briefly instructed on how to use the

program and were given a few practice trials with symbols not used during the sessions.

None of the children needed more than 2-3 practice trials before reporting they were

ready to begin a session. The children completed up to two sessions per day.

At the end of each session, children were allowed to exchange points earned for

items from a bin filled with various bite-size candies and chocolates, school supplies,

such as pencils and erasers, and various toys such as a small deck of cards. One-12

19

Page 20: To my Dad, Henry Remsen Whitehouse IIufdcimages.uflib.ufl.edu/UF/E0/04/19/91/00001/whitehouse... · 2013. 5. 31. · & Kahan, 1992; Frentz & Kelly, 1986, Miltenbeger, Parish, Rickert,

points were exchangeable for 1 item from the bin, 13-23 points were exchangeable for 2

items, 24-35 points were exchangeable for 3 items, and 36-40 points were

exchangeable for 4 items. Additionally, there was a second bin exclusively for

exchanging 36-40 points. This bin included items such as arts and craft items, small

notebooks, larger sized candies and chocolates, and packs of stickers.

Dependent Variables

The primary dependent measure was the children’s responses at each of the three

choice points during each session. Additional measures included the number of correct

and incorrect responses, the latency to complete each trial, and number and duration of

any pauses during the session. Participant behavior throughout the sessions also was

observed to capture possible side effects. Measures of interest included vocalizations in

response to correct and incorrect responses, such as sighing or saying “yeah!,” and off-

task behavior such as putting head on table, looking away for longer than 15 seconds,

tapping on the computer mouse, or swinging feet.

Data Collection and Interobserver Agreement

Dependent measures were recorded via the computer interface. The accuracy of

the data collection by the computer program was verified each session by having

observers record the dependent measures using paper and pencil. No errors in

recording were found. Program debuggers were run approximately every 15 sessions to

check for any errors in the program. No errors were reported by the debugger.

Side effects data were recorded using paper and pencil by two independent

observers in 42% of the sessions. Agreement was scored by comparing the number of

observed responses scored by each observer in each component. Interobserver

agreement was calculated by adding the number of agreements per component and

20

Page 21: To my Dad, Henry Remsen Whitehouse IIufdcimages.uflib.ufl.edu/UF/E0/04/19/91/00001/whitehouse... · 2013. 5. 31. · & Kahan, 1992; Frentz & Kelly, 1986, Miltenbeger, Parish, Rickert,

21

dividing that by the total number of agreements and disagreements per component.

Average agreement was 99%, with a range from 97%-100%.

Page 22: To my Dad, Henry Remsen Whitehouse IIufdcimages.uflib.ufl.edu/UF/E0/04/19/91/00001/whitehouse... · 2013. 5. 31. · & Kahan, 1992; Frentz & Kelly, 1986, Miltenbeger, Parish, Rickert,

CHAPTER 3 EXPERIMENT I: RESPONSE COST VS. REINFORCEMENT: COLORED

BACKGROUNDS

Method

Subjects

Two typically developing elementary school students, Nicole, age 7, and Maribel,

age 11, participated in this experiment.

Procedure

The task used in this study was a matching-to-sample task with six random

symbols arranged in 3 random pairs. The same set of symbols was used in the

reinforcement and response cost conditions. To make changes in the contingencies

more salient, the reinforcement condition was correlated with a blue background and

the response cost condition was correlated with a green background. The colored

backgrounds were intended to serve as schedule-correlated stimuli only. All choice

selection screens throughout the session were set against a gray background.

Each participant completed a total of four phases. In the first phase, participants

completed at least 4 sessions. In an attempt to replicate preference results, a second

phase was implemented. In this phase, the participant completed another 2-3 sessions

in the same comparison but with a new symbol set. It should be noted that the first two

conditions presented were counterbalanced throughout the phases. For example, if

reinforcement was the first component of session 1 in the first phase, then response

cost would have been the first component of session 1 in the second phase.

Phase 3 was a color reversal in which the background colors from the previous

phases were reversed. Now, reinforcement screens were set against a green

22

Page 23: To my Dad, Henry Remsen Whitehouse IIufdcimages.uflib.ufl.edu/UF/E0/04/19/91/00001/whitehouse... · 2013. 5. 31. · & Kahan, 1992; Frentz & Kelly, 1986, Miltenbeger, Parish, Rickert,

background and response cost screens against a blue background. The first condition in

this phase was the same as the first condition in phase 1.

In Phase 4, the original background colors were presented again. Specifically, the

reinforcement condition was correlated with a blue background and the response cost

condition was correlated with a green background. The first condition presented was the

opposite of the first condition presented in Phase 3.

Results and Discussion

Figure 3-1 shows the primary dependent measure for both participants. The

proportion was calculated by adding the number of choice selections for reinforcement

and response cost and dividing each total by 3. In the first phase, Nicole showed a

preference for reinforcement and Maribel showed a preference for response cost.

Similar preference results were obtained in the second phase. However, during the

background color reversal phase, both participants’ preference switched. Nicole’s

preference switched to response cost and Maribel’s preference switched to

reinforcement. Preference for both participants switched again when the original

background colors were presented again.

Comments by the participants throughout the sessions, such as “Blue is my

favorite color” and “I’ll pick green because I like that color better” suggested that color

may have been more of an influencing variable than the reinforcement and response

cost contingencies. Thus, the stimuli added to the experiment to signal changes in the

contingencies seemed to have overpowered any preference for reinforcement or

response cost. These results suggested that small procedural components added to

reinforcement and response cost preference studies, such as the bell and buzzer

sounds presented in the Sattler et al. (1976) study also may have influenced child

23

Page 24: To my Dad, Henry Remsen Whitehouse IIufdcimages.uflib.ufl.edu/UF/E0/04/19/91/00001/whitehouse... · 2013. 5. 31. · & Kahan, 1992; Frentz & Kelly, 1986, Miltenbeger, Parish, Rickert,

preference. If there was any preference at all for reinforcement over response cost (or

vice versa), that preference was outweighed by a seemingly modest difference between

the two conditions: color. No discernable differences were seen between response cost

and reinforcement for mean latency. Side effects such as sighing were observed more

under reinforcement. However, reinforcement was the first component presented for

each participant. For both participants, all side effects were observed after incorrect

responses in the first component of the first session. Side effects were not scored any of

the other sessions. Thus, the difference does not appear to be due to the reinforcement

contingency. Additionally, both participants acquired the task quickly, scoring 100%

correct within 3 sessions.

To evaluate whether the number of points earned under each condition influenced

preference, the number of points earned prior to the first choice point was examined.

Nicole had earned 4 points under reinforcement and 2 points under response cost and

she selected reinforcement at the first choice point. Recall that Nicole’s overall

preference was for reinforcement. Maribel had earned 5 points in response cost and 3

points in reinforcement and she selected response cost at the first choice point. Her

overall preference was for response cost. These data suggested that, in addition to

color, the child’s first selection may have also been influenced by the condition that

produced the larger amount of reinforcement (points).

24

Page 25: To my Dad, Henry Remsen Whitehouse IIufdcimages.uflib.ufl.edu/UF/E0/04/19/91/00001/whitehouse... · 2013. 5. 31. · & Kahan, 1992; Frentz & Kelly, 1986, Miltenbeger, Parish, Rickert,

Nicole

0.00

0.33

0.67

1.00

0 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13

Sessions

Response Cost

ReinforcementProp

ortio

n of

Cho

ice

Con

ditio

ns

Same symbolsR=Blue

RC=Green

R=GreenRC=Blue

New symbols R=Blue

RC=Green

R=BlueRC=Green

Maribel

0.00

0.33

0.67

1.00

0 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11Sessions

Reinforcement

Response Cost

Prop

ortio

n of

Cho

ice

Con

ditio

ns

Same symbolsR=Blue

RC=Green

R= GreenRC=Blue

R=BlueRC=Green

New symbolsR=Blue

RC= Green

Figure 3-1. Proportion of choice selections per session for Nicole and Maribel.

25

Page 26: To my Dad, Henry Remsen Whitehouse IIufdcimages.uflib.ufl.edu/UF/E0/04/19/91/00001/whitehouse... · 2013. 5. 31. · & Kahan, 1992; Frentz & Kelly, 1986, Miltenbeger, Parish, Rickert,

26

Table 3-1. Summary of latencies, point comparisons, and side effects. Shaded region = RC

Experiment I Average Latency (sec)

Points at 1st Choice Point in Phase 1

Side Effects: Total Instances of Inapp. Vocal.

Nicole 6.90 4 5 7.09 2 0

Maribel 5.91 3 3 5.66 5 0

Page 27: To my Dad, Henry Remsen Whitehouse IIufdcimages.uflib.ufl.edu/UF/E0/04/19/91/00001/whitehouse... · 2013. 5. 31. · & Kahan, 1992; Frentz & Kelly, 1986, Miltenbeger, Parish, Rickert,

CHAPTER 4 EXPERIMENT II: RESPONSE COST VS. REINFORCEMENT: DIFFERENT SYMBOL

SETS

Methods

Subjects

Two typically developing elementary school students, Andy, age 11, and Elena,

age 10, participated in this experiment.

Procedure

To eliminate the possibility of a color bias, the background colors were removed.

All the screens used throughout the sessions were set against a grey background.

Instead, two different symbol sets were used in this experiment to distinguish the

contingencies. Thus, the reinforcement condition had a symbol set and the response

cost condition had a different symbol set. Additionally, to make the changes in the

contingencies more salient, an arrow was correlated with each condition. In the

reinforcement condition, a right facing arrow appeared below the number line, and in the

response cost condition a left facing arrow appeared under the number line. These

arrows also appeared along with the number lines during the choice selections. All other

features of the experiment were the same as Experiment I.

Design

The first two phases of this experiment were identical to Experiment I. The third

phase of the experiment used the symbols sets from the child’s overall favorite

contingency in the two prior phases. For both Allen and Elena, the symbols used in

phase 3 were the symbols from the response cost condition from phase 1 and the

reinforcement symbols from phase 2, respectively. In the fourth phase, the symbols sets

were reversed from phase 3.

27

Page 28: To my Dad, Henry Remsen Whitehouse IIufdcimages.uflib.ufl.edu/UF/E0/04/19/91/00001/whitehouse... · 2013. 5. 31. · & Kahan, 1992; Frentz & Kelly, 1986, Miltenbeger, Parish, Rickert,

Results and Discussion

Session preference data for Allen and Elena are shown in figure 4-1. Both

participants showed a preference for response cost in the first phase. However, when

new symbols sets were introduced in phase 2, overall preference results were not

replicated with either participant. This finding differed from the findings from Experiment

I. However, results from phases 3 and 4 suggested that the participants’ choices were

influence by the symbol sets. Both participants switched their choices as the symbols

changed rather than according to reinforcement or response cost as exemplified in brief

reversals of symbol sets. Similar to Experiment 1, comments made by the participants

during the experiment indicated a preference for a symbol set rather than the

contingencies.

Minimal side effects were noted in the first component of the first session in phase

1 for Allen. Side effects observed including sighing when he responded incorrectly. No

side effects were observed throughout the experiment for Elena. Allen’s mean latency

was 3.76 sec in reinforcement and 3.48 sec in response cost. Elena’s mean latency was

4.84 sec in reinforcement and 4.39 sec in response cost. Though the mean latencies for

both participants were slightly shorter in response cost, a closer inspection of the data

indicated that the mean latencies were shorter in the preferred condition (see Table

4.2). Overall, the results from Experiment I and II suggested that differences in the

latencies do not seem to be influenced by reinforcement versus response cost.

Recall that results from Experiment I showed that preference may point have also

been influenced by the condition that initially produced the larger amount of reinforcers

(points). However, these findings were not replicated in Experiment II. Table 4-1 shows

the point totals at the first choice selection in each phase of the experiment. Allen’s

28

Page 29: To my Dad, Henry Remsen Whitehouse IIufdcimages.uflib.ufl.edu/UF/E0/04/19/91/00001/whitehouse... · 2013. 5. 31. · & Kahan, 1992; Frentz & Kelly, 1986, Miltenbeger, Parish, Rickert,

point totals at the first choice point in phase 1 were the same in each condition and all

subsequent phases of the experiment, thus eliminating any potential influence of point

totals. Elena earned 7 points under response cost and 8 points under reinforcement in

the first two components in phase 1, earning one fewer point in her favored condition.

Additionally, her point totals were the same in the first two components of phases 3 and

4. Elena’s point totals were slightly higher in her overall favorite condition component

(reinforcement) in phase 2. Overall, however, the influence of the points earned in the

first two components of each phase appears to be minimal.

Despite efforts to reduce any possible bias by removing the colored backgrounds,

both participants showed a preference for the symbols sets rather than the

contingencies. Similar to the findings from Experiment I, these results suggested that

variables such as differences in stimuli influenced preference more than the

reinforcement or response cost contingencies. Collectively, the results in the first two

experiments suggest that preference for reinforcement over response cost (or vice

versa) may be tenuous at best.

29

Page 30: To my Dad, Henry Remsen Whitehouse IIufdcimages.uflib.ufl.edu/UF/E0/04/19/91/00001/whitehouse... · 2013. 5. 31. · & Kahan, 1992; Frentz & Kelly, 1986, Miltenbeger, Parish, Rickert,

30

Figure 4-1. Proportion of choice selections per session for Elena and Allen.

Allen

0.00

0.33

0.67

1.00

0 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14

Sessions

Reinforcement

Response Cost

Prop

ortio

n of

Cho

ice

Con

ditio

ns

RC p v R pR v RC Different symbols,grey background

R V RC, 6 new, different symbolsgrey background 1 2

Elena

0.00

0.33

0.67

1.00

0 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12

Sessions

Reinforcement

Response Cost

Prop

ortio

n of

Cho

ice

Con

ditio

ns

R v RC Different symbols Grey backgrounds

R v RC 6 Differnet symbols, grey backgrounds 2

RC pref R pref

1

Page 31: To my Dad, Henry Remsen Whitehouse IIufdcimages.uflib.ufl.edu/UF/E0/04/19/91/00001/whitehouse... · 2013. 5. 31. · & Kahan, 1992; Frentz & Kelly, 1986, Miltenbeger, Parish, Rickert,

Table 4-1. Average latencies for Elena and Allen. Shaded region = RC Overall AVG Symbols #1 Symbols #2

Elena 4.84 6.17 4.60 4.39 4.11 5.70

Allen 3.76 5.07 3.66 3.48 3.66 4.43

31

Page 32: To my Dad, Henry Remsen Whitehouse IIufdcimages.uflib.ufl.edu/UF/E0/04/19/91/00001/whitehouse... · 2013. 5. 31. · & Kahan, 1992; Frentz & Kelly, 1986, Miltenbeger, Parish, Rickert,

Table 4-2. Points at first choice point for Elena and Allen. Shaded region = RC

Symbols #1 Symbols #2 RC symbols v

R symbols Symbols Reversal

Elena 7 9 10 10 8 7 10 10

Allen 6 8 10 10 6 8 10 10

32

Page 33: To my Dad, Henry Remsen Whitehouse IIufdcimages.uflib.ufl.edu/UF/E0/04/19/91/00001/whitehouse... · 2013. 5. 31. · & Kahan, 1992; Frentz & Kelly, 1986, Miltenbeger, Parish, Rickert,

CHAPTER 5 EXPERIMENT III: RESPONSE COST VS. REINFORCEMENT: COLOR AND

SYMBOLS CONTROLLED

Methods

Subjects

Eleven typically developing children enrolled in afterschool care at a local

elementary school participated in Experiment III. Both participants from Experiment II

also participated in this experiment. Ages of the children ranged from 5 to 11 years-old.

Procedure

To address the possible bias towards color and symbols found in Experiments I

and II, the symbol sets remained the same for both conditions (thus, rate of acquisition

was moot). Additionally, a grade-level math task was added. All other procedures were

identical to Experiment II.

Design

Each participant completed four phases. The first phase was the matching to

sample symbol task (with novel symbols) used in Experiments I and II. Sessions were

carried out for at least 5 sessions or until stability in preference was obtained by visual

inspection. In phase 2, a new set of symbols was introduced, and the participant

completed at least 5 sessions, or until stability in preference was obtained. The task

was changed to grade level math in phase 3.The same ten math problems were used

for both response cost and reinforcement. In phase 4, a new set of 10 math problems

was introduced. The same math problems were used for both response cost and

reinforcement.

33

Page 34: To my Dad, Henry Remsen Whitehouse IIufdcimages.uflib.ufl.edu/UF/E0/04/19/91/00001/whitehouse... · 2013. 5. 31. · & Kahan, 1992; Frentz & Kelly, 1986, Miltenbeger, Parish, Rickert,

Results and Discussions

Overall, three patterns of preference were found; a preference for reinforcement, a

preference for response cost, and indifference. Preference was classified as indifferent

if the pattern of choice responding was variable based on visual inspection of the data

and if the overall proportion of choice conditions per phase was close to .50. Eight

participants showed a preference for reinforcement (Figure 5-1.), 2 showed indifference

(Figure 5-2), and one participant showed a preference for response cost (Figure 5-3).

For all participants, the pattern of preference indentified in the first phase was replicated

throughout the four phases. There was a two month summer break between sessions

for Abby in phase 4. This break did not influence preference.

Similar to Experiments I and II, side effects were not observed. Additionally,

overall latencies were shorter in the participant’s preferred condition (see Table 5-1).

For some participants (e.g., Leo), a somewhat larger difference in the latencies in the

math phases was observed. This may be explained by more trials completed under the

preferred condition. Specifically, because the same 10 math problems were used

across the 40 trials, and the response latencies decreased throughout the session, the

preferred conditions would have had more trials with shorter latencies.

Latency results from the two participants labeled indifferent showed an overall

shorter latency under reinforcement for Jackie but an overall shorter latency under

response cost for Maggie. Additionally, a closer examination of the latencies per phase

shows that the conditions with shorter latencies varied throughout the phases.

For the participants who showed a preference for reinforcement, an examination

of point totals at the first choice point showed that point totals under response cost were

higher for 4 of the 8 participants (see Table 5-2). Along with the results from Experiment

34

Page 35: To my Dad, Henry Remsen Whitehouse IIufdcimages.uflib.ufl.edu/UF/E0/04/19/91/00001/whitehouse... · 2013. 5. 31. · & Kahan, 1992; Frentz & Kelly, 1986, Miltenbeger, Parish, Rickert,

II; this further demonstrated that point totals during the first exposure to reinforcement or

response cost did not seem to influence overall preference.

An additional measure of interest was the correspondence between the

participant’s reported favorite condition and the two selected working conditions.

Correspondence was calculated as a conditional probability; specifically, the probability

of selecting reinforcement as the working conditions when reinforcement was the

reported favorite condition, and the probability of selecting response cost as the working

conditions when response cost was the reported favorite condition. Overall, when there

was a clear preference, the reported favorite condition was a fairly good indicator of

child working condition selections. For the two participants classified as indifferent, the

conditional probabilities were high for Maggie but low for Jackie (Figure 5-4). Thus for

Jackie, the reported favorite condition was not a good predictor of her working condition

selections.

Although the data indicate an overall preference for reinforcement, verbal reports

by the participants suggested that the preference for reinforcement may have been

influenced by yet another subtle procedural nuance. That is, the participants reported

that they “liked” being able to “keep track” of how many trials were left. The trials in each

condition were not numbered in this experiment. Thus, under the reinforcement

condition, it would have been easier to keep track of trials with the red dot on the

number line moving towards 10. Under response cost, the red dot started and remained

at 10 if all trials were answered correctly. Despite efforts to reduce any possible bias by

removing the colored backgrounds and keeping the symbol sets consistent between

35

Page 36: To my Dad, Henry Remsen Whitehouse IIufdcimages.uflib.ufl.edu/UF/E0/04/19/91/00001/whitehouse... · 2013. 5. 31. · & Kahan, 1992; Frentz & Kelly, 1986, Miltenbeger, Parish, Rickert,

36

conditions, these verbal reports suggested that another procedural nuance may have

influenced preference among the two contingencies.

Page 37: To my Dad, Henry Remsen Whitehouse IIufdcimages.uflib.ufl.edu/UF/E0/04/19/91/00001/whitehouse... · 2013. 5. 31. · & Kahan, 1992; Frentz & Kelly, 1986, Miltenbeger, Parish, Rickert,

Ethan

0.00

0.33

0.67

1.00

0 5 10 15 20 25

Sessions

Prop

ortio

n of

Cho

ice

Cond

ition

s

Symbols #1 Symbols #2 Math #1 Math #2

Figure 5-1. Results for participants showing a preference for reinforcement.

Reinforcement

Response Cost

Leo

0.00

0.33

0.67

1.00

0 5 10 15 20

Sessions

Reinforcement

Response Cost

Prop

ortio

n of

Cho

ice

Cond

ition

s

Symbols #1 Symbols #2 Math #1 Math #2

Dan

0.00

0.33

0.67

1.00

0 5 10 15 20 25Sessions

Reinforcement

Response Cos t

Prop

ortio

n of

Cho

ice

Con

ditio

ns

Symbols # 1 Symbols #2 Math #1 Math #2 Callie

0.00

0.33

0.67

1.00

0 5 10 15 20 25Sessions

Reinforcement

Response Cost

Prop

ortio

n of

Cho

ice

Cond

ition

s

Symbols #1 Symbols #2 Math #1 Math #2

Abby

0.00

0.33

0.67

1.00

0 5 10 15 20 25 30 35Sessions

Reinforcement

Response Cos t

Prop

ortio

n of

Cho

ice

Con

ditio

ns

Symbols #1 Symbols #2 Math #1 Math 2

Cate

0.00

0.33

0.67

1.00

0 5 10 15 20 25

Sessions

Nina

0.00

0.33

0.67

1.00

0 5 10 15 20 25

Sessions

Math #2Symbols #1 Symbols #2 Math #1

Reinforcement

Response Cost

Prop

ortio

n of

Cho

ice

Con

ditio

ns

Re info rcement

Res po ns e Co s t

Prop

ortio

n of

Cho

ice

Con

ditio

ns

Symbols #1 Symbols #2 Math #1 Math #2

Allen

0.00

0.33

0.67

1.00

0 5 10 15 20 25

Sessions

Prop

ortio

n of

Cho

ice

Con

ditio

ns

Symbols #1 Symbols #2 Math #1 Math #2

37

Page 38: To my Dad, Henry Remsen Whitehouse IIufdcimages.uflib.ufl.edu/UF/E0/04/19/91/00001/whitehouse... · 2013. 5. 31. · & Kahan, 1992; Frentz & Kelly, 1986, Miltenbeger, Parish, Rickert,

Jackie

8/188/15

12/30

8/1510/187/15

18/30

7/15

0.00

0.33

0.67

1.00

Symbols #1 Symbols #2 Math #1 Math #2

Prop

ortio

n of

Cho

ice

Con

ditio

ns

Reinforcement

Response Cost

Jackie

0.00

0.33

0.67

1.00

0 5 10 15 20 25 30

Sessions

Figure 5-2. Results for participants showing indifference.

Reinforcement

Response Cos t

Prop

ortio

n of

Cho

ice

Con

ditio

ns

Symbols #1 Symbols #2 Math #1 Math #2

Maggie

0.00

0.33

0.67

1.00

0 5 10 15 20 25 30 35 40 45 50 55

Sessions Reinforcement

Maggie

18/39

30/5419/39

13/21

21/39

24/5420/39

8/21

0.00

0.33

0.67

1.00

Symbols #1 Symbols #2 Math #1 Math #2

Prop

ortio

n of

Cho

ice

Con

ditio

ns

ReinforcementSymbols #2 Math #1 Math #2

ortio

n of

Cho

ice

Cond

ition

s

Symbols #1 Response Cost

Prop

Response Cost

38

Page 39: To my Dad, Henry Remsen Whitehouse IIufdcimages.uflib.ufl.edu/UF/E0/04/19/91/00001/whitehouse... · 2013. 5. 31. · & Kahan, 1992; Frentz & Kelly, 1986, Miltenbeger, Parish, Rickert,

Elena

0.00

0.33

0.67

1.00

0 5

Sessions

10

Reinforcement

Response Cost

Prop

ortio

n of

Cho

ice

Cond

ition

s Symbols #1 Symbols #2

Figure 5-3. Results for participant showing a preference for response cost.

39

Page 40: To my Dad, Henry Remsen Whitehouse IIufdcimages.uflib.ufl.edu/UF/E0/04/19/91/00001/whitehouse... · 2013. 5. 31. · & Kahan, 1992; Frentz & Kelly, 1986, Miltenbeger, Parish, Rickert,

Table 5-1. Latency summary for all participants. Shaded region = RC

Overall AVG Symbols #1 Symbols #2 Math #1 Math #2

Dan 7.73 4.59 3.43 6.27 8.67 7.96 4.20 3.77 8.22 11.74 Callie 4.84 4.73 3.52 5.40 5.37 5.86 4.58 4.08 7.18 5.74

Preference Leo 9.93 3.35 3.36 13.17 14.92 for 13.78 3.56 3.98 21.38 17.40

Reinforcement Allen 4.30 2.05 2.62 6.57 4.17 5.71 2.49 2.68 9.90 5.63 Ethan 7.56 5.10 6.29 11.41 7.30 9.04 5.02 5.22 11.84 8.38 Abby 6.04 4.93 6.30 6.09 7.23 7.35 6.58 7.88 7.29 7.50 Cate 9.36 7.15 6.15 12.31 11.82 10.085 8.21 6.59 13.51 12.03 Nina 5.22 4.37 3.67 6.56 6.28 5.71 5.01 3.89 7.01 6.91 Jackie 5.72 3.55 3.35 6.64 8.95

Indifferent 6.19 3.79 2.91 7.23 8.15 Mary 6.48 3.84 3.28 8.87 11.45 Anne 6.38 3.54 3.65 9.20 9.35 Preference for Elena 4.35 5.04 3.66 Response Cost 3.37 3.34 3.41

40

Page 41: To my Dad, Henry Remsen Whitehouse IIufdcimages.uflib.ufl.edu/UF/E0/04/19/91/00001/whitehouse... · 2013. 5. 31. · & Kahan, 1992; Frentz & Kelly, 1986, Miltenbeger, Parish, Rickert,

Table 5-2. Point totals at the first choice point in each phase. Shaded region = RC Symbols #1 Symbols #2 Math #1 Math #2 Dan 6 2 10 10 9 8 10 8 Callie 6 6 10 10 8 6 9 10

Preference Leo 8 7 10 9 for 10 8 10 10

Reinforcement Allen 7 5 10 10 9 7 9 10 Ethan 8 6 10 10 8 9 10 10 Abby 7 4 9 9 7 3 9 9 Cate 2 4 10 9 5 3 9 10 Nina 10 6 10 10 10 10 9 10 Jackie 2 7 9 8

Indifferent 6 10 10 7 Mary 6 9 10 10 Anne 9 9 10 10

Preference for Elena 4 7 Response Cost 6 7

41

Page 42: To my Dad, Henry Remsen Whitehouse IIufdcimages.uflib.ufl.edu/UF/E0/04/19/91/00001/whitehouse... · 2013. 5. 31. · & Kahan, 1992; Frentz & Kelly, 1986, Miltenbeger, Parish, Rickert,

JackieCorrespondence

2/8

2/42/4 2/4

2/6

5/8

3/65/12

0.00

0.25

0.50

0.75

1.00

R v RC R v RC #2 R v RC Math R v RC Math #2

Prob

abili

ty

.Selecting R when R was 'fav.'conditionSelecting RC when RC was'f'av.' condition

M aggieCorre s ponde nce

12/12

16/24

6/6 10/1015/1613/14

12/12

5/6

0.00

0.25

0.50

0.75

1.00

R v RC R v RC #2 R v RC M ath R v RC M ath #2

Prob

abili

ty

.

Figure 5-4. Correspondence data for Jackie and Maggie.

42

Page 43: To my Dad, Henry Remsen Whitehouse IIufdcimages.uflib.ufl.edu/UF/E0/04/19/91/00001/whitehouse... · 2013. 5. 31. · & Kahan, 1992; Frentz & Kelly, 1986, Miltenbeger, Parish, Rickert,

CHAPTER 6 EXPERIMENT IV: RESPONSE COST VS REINFORCEMENT: NUMBERING TRIALS

Methods

Subjects

Four typically developing children enrolled in afterschool care at a local elementary

school participated in this study. Ages of the children ranged from 7 to 10 years-old.

None of the children had participated in any of the previous experiments.

Procedure

In this experiment, the 10 trials in each component were numbered. Trial numbers

appeared below the number line and to the left of the symbols and math problems.

Additionally, for Amelia, a fifth phase was implemented. This phase was a reversal to

the math problems used in phase 3. All other procedures were identical to Experiment

III.

Design

The design was identical to Experiment III.

Results and Discussion

Figure 6-1 shows the results for the four participants. One participant (Cole,

upper left panel) showed a preference for reinforcement, and one participant (Monica,

lower left panel) initially showed a preference towards reinforcement but that preference

switched to indifference in the last two phases of the experiment. The third participant

(Amelia, lower right panel) showed a preference towards reinforcement but switched to

indifference in the fourth phase. The final participant (Kylie, upper right panel) showed a

pattern of responding not observed in any of the previous experiments. Her preference

43

Page 44: To my Dad, Henry Remsen Whitehouse IIufdcimages.uflib.ufl.edu/UF/E0/04/19/91/00001/whitehouse... · 2013. 5. 31. · & Kahan, 1992; Frentz & Kelly, 1986, Miltenbeger, Parish, Rickert,

44

was categorized as inconclusive based on the changes in her preference across the

phases of the experiment.

To address Amelia’s change in preference at during the last 4 sessions of phase

4 of the experiment, a fifth phase was added. The purpose of the phase was to reverse

the math problems used in phase 3 to examine whether the set of math problems was

influencing preference. Amelia continued to show indifference in the reversal to phase 3

math problems.

The preference variability among 3 of the 4 participants suggests that numbering

the trials may temper the apparent preference for reinforcement obtained in Experiment

III. Recall that numbering the trials was added to the current experiment to eliminate

another procedural nuance that appeared to influence preference for either contingency.

However, these results should be viewed cautiously, as only 4 participants have

completed this experiment to date. More participants are necessary in order to make

further comparisons to Experiment III.

Similar to the previous experiments, side effects and differences in latencies

were not obtained. Additionally, point totals at the first choice point did not appear to

influence preference. :

Page 45: To my Dad, Henry Remsen Whitehouse IIufdcimages.uflib.ufl.edu/UF/E0/04/19/91/00001/whitehouse... · 2013. 5. 31. · & Kahan, 1992; Frentz & Kelly, 1986, Miltenbeger, Parish, Rickert,

Figure 6-1. Results for Cole, Kylie, Monica, and Amelia

Monica

0.00

0.33

0.67

1.00

0 5 10 15 20 25 30 35

SessionsR einfo rcement

R es po ns e C o s t

Prop

ortio

n of

Cho

ice

Con

ditio

ns

Symbols #1Numbered trials

Symbols #2Numbered Trials

Math #1Numbered Trials

Math #2Numbered T rials

Cole

0.00

0.33

0.67

1.00

0 5 10 15 20 25

Sessions

Reinfo rcement

Res po ns e Co s t

Prop

ortio

n of

Cho

ice

Con

ditio

ns

Symbols #2Numbered Trials

Symbols #1 Numbered trials

Math #1Numbered Trials

Math #2Numbered Trials

Amelia

0.00

0.33

0.67

1.00

0 5 10 15 20 25

Sessions

Symbols #1

Reinfo rcement

Res po ns e Co s tPr

opor

tion

of C

hoic

e C

ondi

tions

Symbols #1Numbered T rials

Symbols #2Numbered Trials

Math #1Numbered T rials

Math #2Numbered T rials

Reversal toMath #1 problems

Kylie

0.00

0.33

0.67

1.00

0 5 10 15 20 25

Sessions

R einfo rcement

R es po ns e Co s t

Prop

ortio

n of

Cho

ice

Con

ditio

ns

umbered trialssymbols #2 Math #1 Math #2

N Numbered Trials Numbered trials Numbered trials

45

Page 46: To my Dad, Henry Remsen Whitehouse IIufdcimages.uflib.ufl.edu/UF/E0/04/19/91/00001/whitehouse... · 2013. 5. 31. · & Kahan, 1992; Frentz & Kelly, 1986, Miltenbeger, Parish, Rickert,

CHAPTER 7 GENERAL DISCUSSION

The present experiments evaluated preference for reinforcement or response cost

using simulated academic task and actual academic tasks. Results of Experiments I

and II suggested that any preference for reinforcement or response cost must have

been so slight (if present at all) that it appeared to be outweighed by idiosyncratic

features of the experiment. Thus, all four participants in the first two experiments can be

viewed as relatively indifferent to reinforcement versus response cost. In Experiment III,

there appeared to be a general preference for reinforcement over response cost. In

Experiment IV, when the trials were numbered, the participants showed mixed,

indifferent, and inconclusive preferences. Despite the results from Experiment III, the

overall results collectively suggest that a preference for reinforcement seems marginal

and situational at best. Preferences apparently were controlled by very minor procedural

nuances that were incidental to the experimental arrangement. In Experiments I and II

the participants stated that they preferred certain colors or symbols, respectively, and,

the data supported those statements. In Experiment III, children reported they preferred

reinforcement because it was easier to “keep track” or see how many trials were left to

complete in each component. The data from Experiment IV, though best viewed as

preliminary, suggest that strong overall preference for reinforcement is mitigated when

the participants can easily keep track of the trials in either condition. Altogether, the

findings of these experiments also suggest that the bell and buzzer sounds used in the

Sattler et al. (1976) study, although innocuous at first glance, may have exerted control

over participants’ preferences

46

Page 47: To my Dad, Henry Remsen Whitehouse IIufdcimages.uflib.ufl.edu/UF/E0/04/19/91/00001/whitehouse... · 2013. 5. 31. · & Kahan, 1992; Frentz & Kelly, 1986, Miltenbeger, Parish, Rickert,

Side effects were not observed differentially in response cost conditions but were

most often seen in the first component of the first session, regardless of condition.

Furthermore, longer latencies to respond were not associated with response cost as

found in Sattler et al (1976). In all experiments, the mean latencies were minimally

shorter in the child’s preferred condition, which may be due to the child completing more

trials in that condition. For example, if a child’s overall preference was for reinforcement

in a session, 30 trial latencies would be averaged for reinforcement, and 10 trial

latencies from the one forced exposure to response cost condition would be averaged.

Given that the latencies decreased as sessions progressed, it makes sense that the

mean latencies would be lower in the condition with more exposure. In fact, a

retrospective review of data in the forced choice components (thus, controlling for

exposure) showed no discernable difference in mean latency.

Some potential limitations of the current experiments should be noted. First, this

investigation only examined preference for reinforcement or response cost in the

context of an acquisition task. Similar experiments could be arranged to evaluate

problem behavior targeted for reduction. Second, because the same symbols sets were

used in Experiments III and IV, no comparison can be made of accuracy during

reinforcement versus response cost. Additionally, the participants were only given the

option to select either reinforcement or response cost during choice conditions. The

participants did not have an opportunity to select other options such as “I don’t care.” An

evaluation of adding such an option to the experiment is currently ongoing.

There are a number of other potentially interesting future manipulations. Sattler et

al., (1976) suggested that removing points the child had earned may influence

47

Page 48: To my Dad, Henry Remsen Whitehouse IIufdcimages.uflib.ufl.edu/UF/E0/04/19/91/00001/whitehouse... · 2013. 5. 31. · & Kahan, 1992; Frentz & Kelly, 1986, Miltenbeger, Parish, Rickert,

preference and increase the probability of observing side effects. Previous response

cost evaluations have utilized removing points earned non-contingently (e.g., Kazdin,

1973; Pace & Foreman, 1982) and points earned contingently (e.g., Phillips, 1968;

Phillips et al., 1971). Though both have been shown to be effective, none of these

studies evaluated child preferences. It is also unclear whether the investigators

observed any side effects, as no discussion of side effects was presented. Thus, an

extension of the current study would be to investigate whether removing earned points

versus noncontingent points influences preference or side effects. Another future

direction would be to manipulate different parameters, such as how many points are

earned or lost (e.g., Kazdin, 1971), and assessing preference. Additionally,

reinforcement and response cost are rarely implemented in isolation. Thus, future

studies should include a combined reinforcement and response cost condition.

Response cost procedures are ubiquitous (e.g., McSweeny, 1978); speeding, if

caught, leads to a fine; failing to pay an electric bill may result in the electricity being

discontinued; and paying a credit card bill late may result in an increase in the annual

percentage rate. Response cost procedures are commonly found in typical classrooms

too. Thus, it is important for researchers to continue to examine such punishment

procedures and to evaluate any preferences and side effects in the context of naturally

occurring instances of reinforcement or response cost.

The issue of client treatment preference becomes especially important if behavior

analysts are to consider such information when making treatment decisions. Outside of

behavior analysis, most studies in the area of client treatment preference assess it

through the use of questionnaires or verbal reports (Dwight-Johnson, Sherbourne, Liao,

48

Page 49: To my Dad, Henry Remsen Whitehouse IIufdcimages.uflib.ufl.edu/UF/E0/04/19/91/00001/whitehouse... · 2013. 5. 31. · & Kahan, 1992; Frentz & Kelly, 1986, Miltenbeger, Parish, Rickert,

49

& Wells, 2000). Although Hanley et al. (1997) demonstrated a method to identify

treatment preference with consumers with limited vocal verbal behavior, the use of

client treatment preference assessments remains largely unexplored. Thus, it is

important for behavior analysts to continue to examine the best way to accurately and

efficiently measure preference for consumers with both limited and extensive verbal

behavior. For example, can verbal reports of preference be verified by setting up a

simple concurrent operant treatment selection? Subsequently, how many sessions or

choice points must be completed to identify an overall preference? Presumably,

preference is not static, and a number of known factors (e.g., delay to reinforcement,

amount of reinforcement, effort, severity of the behavior problem) influence choice. For

now, at least in the context of the types of tasks used in the current experiments, it can

be said that any preference for reinforcement over response cost is at best marginal.

Page 50: To my Dad, Henry Remsen Whitehouse IIufdcimages.uflib.ufl.edu/UF/E0/04/19/91/00001/whitehouse... · 2013. 5. 31. · & Kahan, 1992; Frentz & Kelly, 1986, Miltenbeger, Parish, Rickert,

LIST OF REFERENCES

Allen, K., & Harris, F. (1966). Elimination of a child's excessive scratching by training the mother in reinforcement procedures. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 4(2), 79-84. doi:10.1016/0005-7967(66)90046-5.

Azrin, N. H. & Holz, W. C. (1966). Punishment. In W.K. Honig (Ed.), Operant behavior:

areas of research and application. East Norwalk, CT US: Appleton-Century-Crofts. Retrieved from PsycINFO database.

Bannerman, D., Sheldon, J., Sherman, J., & Harchik, A. (1990). Balancing the right to

habilitation with the right to personal liberties: The rights of people with developmental disabilities to eat too many doughnuts and take a nap. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 23(1), 79-89. doi:10.1901/jaba.1990.23-79.

Blampied, N., & Kahan, E. (1992). Acceptability of alternative punishments: A

community survey. Behavior Modification, 16(3), 400-413. doi:10.1177/01454455920163006.

Boren, J., & Colman, A. (1970). Some experiments on reinforcement principles within a

psychiatric ward for delinquent soldiers. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 3(1), 29-37. doi:10.1901/jaba.1970.3-29.

Brent, D., & Routh, D. (1978). Response cost and impulsive word recognition errors in

reading-disabled children. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology: An official publication of the International Society for Research in Child and Adolescent Psychopathology, 6(2), 211-219. doi:10.1007/BF00919126.

Broughton, S., & Lahey, B. (1978). Direct and collateral effects of positive

reinforcement, response cost, and mixed contingencies for academic performance. Journal of School Psychology, 16(2), 126-136. doi:10.1016/0022-4405(78)90051-1.

Carr, J., & Sidener, T. (2002). On the relation between applied behavior analysis and

positive behavioral support. The Behavior Analyst, 25(2), 245-253. Retrieved from PsycINFO database.

Dwight-Johnson, M., Sherbourne, C., Liao, D., & Wells, K. (2000). Treatment

Preferences Among Depressed Primary Care Patients. JGIM: Journal of General Internal Medicine, 15(8), 527-534. doi:10.1046/j.1525-1497.2000.08035.x.

Errickson, E. A., Wyne, M. D., & Routh, D. K. (1973). A response-cost procedure for

reduction of impulsive behavior of academically handicapped children. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 1, 350-357.

50

Page 51: To my Dad, Henry Remsen Whitehouse IIufdcimages.uflib.ufl.edu/UF/E0/04/19/91/00001/whitehouse... · 2013. 5. 31. · & Kahan, 1992; Frentz & Kelly, 1986, Miltenbeger, Parish, Rickert,

Fisher, W., Thompson, R., Piazza, C., Crosland, K., & Gotjen, D. (1997). On the relative reinforcing effects of choice and differential consequences. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 30(3), 423-438. doi:10.1901/jaba.1997.30-423.

Florida Administrative Code, Title XXIX, Chapter 393 (2009). Frentz, C., & Kelley, M. (1986). Parents' acceptance of reductive treatment methods:

The influence of problem severity and perception of child behavior. Behavior Therapy, 17(1), 75-81. doi:10.1016/S0005-7894(86)80116-2.

Hanley, G., Piazza, C., Fisher, W., Contrucci, S., & Maglieri, K. (1997). Evaluation of

client preference for function-based treatment packages. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 30(3), 459-473. doi:10.1901/jaba.1997.30-459.

Hanley, G., Piazza, C., Fisher, W., & Maglieri, K. (2005). On The Effectiveness Of And

Preference For Punishment And Extinction Components Of Function-Based Interventions. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 38(1), 51-65. doi:10.1901/jaba.2005.6-04.

Holt, M., & Hobbs, T. (1979). The effects of token reinforcement, feedback and

response cost on standardized test performance. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 17(1), 81-83. doi:10.1016/0005-7967(79)90054-8.

Iwata, B., & Bailey, J. (1974). Reward versus cost token systems: An analysis of the

effects on students and teacher. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 7(4), 567-576. doi:10.1901/jaba.1974.7-567.

Kazdin, A. (1973). The effect of response cost and aversive stimulation in suppressing

punished and nonpunished speech disfluencies. Behavior Therapy, 4(1), 73-82. doi:10.1016/S0005-7894(73)80075-9.

Kazdin, A. (1971). The effect of response cost in suppressing behavior in a pre-

psychotic retardate. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 2(2), 137-140. doi:10.1016/0005-7916(71)90029-2.

Latham, G. I. (1995). The Power of Positive Parenting: A Wonderful Way to Raise

Children. Logan, UT: P&T ink. McSweeny, A. (1978). Effects of response cost on the behavior of a million persons:

Charging for directory assistance in Cincinnati. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 11(1), 47-51. doi:10.1901/jaba.1978.11-47.

Michael, J. (1993). Concepts and principles of behavior analysis. Kalamazoo: Society

for the advancement of behavior analysis.

51

Page 52: To my Dad, Henry Remsen Whitehouse IIufdcimages.uflib.ufl.edu/UF/E0/04/19/91/00001/whitehouse... · 2013. 5. 31. · & Kahan, 1992; Frentz & Kelly, 1986, Miltenbeger, Parish, Rickert,

52

Miltenberger, R. G., Parrish, J. M., Rickert, V., & Kohr, M. (1989). Assessing treatment acceptability with consumers of outpatient child behavior management services. Child and Family Behavior Therapy, 11, 35-44. doi:10.1300/J019v11n01_03.

Pace, D. M., & Forman, S. G. (1982). Variables related to the effectiveness of response

cost. Psychology in the Schools, 19, 365-370. doi:10.1002/1520-6807(198207)19:3<365::AID-PITS2310190317>3.0.CO;2-T.

Panek, D. M. (1970). Word association learning by chronic schizophrenics on a token

economy ward under conditions of reward and punishment. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 26, 163-167. doi:10.1002/1097-4679(197004)26:2<163::AID-JCLP2270260208>3.0.CO;2-5.

Phillips, E. L. (1968). Achievement place: Token reinforcement procedures in a home-

style rehabilitation setting for "pre-delinquent" boys. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 1, 213-223. doi:10.1901/jaba.1968.1-213.

Phillips, E. L., Phillips, E. A., Fixsen, D. L., & Wolf, M. M. (1971). Achievement place:

Modification of the behaviors of pre-delinquent boys within a token economy. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 4, 45-59. doi:10.1901/jaba.1971.4-45.

Piazza, C. C., & Fisher, W. (1991). A faded bedtime with response cost protocol for

treatment of multiple sleep problems in children. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 24, 129-140. doi:10.1901/jaba.1991.24-129.

Piazza, C. C., Fisher, W. W., Roane, H. S., & Hilker, K. (1999). Predicting and

enhancing the effectiveness of reinforcers and punishers. In A. C. Repp & R. H. Horner (Eds.), Functional analysis of problem behavior (pp. 55-77). Belmont, CA: Wadsworth.

Sattler, H. E., Betz, M. A., & Zellner, R. D. (1978). Children’s preference for response

cost or positive reinforcement as working conditions. The Journal of Psychology, 100, 71-75. Retrieved from PsycINFO database.

Sidman, M. (1989). Coercion and its fallout. Boston, MA: Authors Cooperative, Inc.

Retrieved from PsycINFO database. Sprute, K. A., Williams, R. L., & McLaughlin, T. F. (1990). Effects of a group response

cost contingency procedure on the rate of classroom interruptions with emotionally disturbed secondary students. Child and Family Behavior Therapy, 12, 1-12. doi:10.1300/J019v12n02_01.

Page 53: To my Dad, Henry Remsen Whitehouse IIufdcimages.uflib.ufl.edu/UF/E0/04/19/91/00001/whitehouse... · 2013. 5. 31. · & Kahan, 1992; Frentz & Kelly, 1986, Miltenbeger, Parish, Rickert,

BIOGRAPHICAL SKETCH

Cristina Whitehouse graduated from Rollins College in 1995 with a Bachelor of

.Arts. in Psychology. Cristina became interested in Behavior Analysis while at Rollins

and completed her senior thesis on the Personalized System of Instruction with her

undergraduate advisor, Dr. Maria Ruiz. Upon graduation, Cristina began working at

Threshold Inc. providing early intervention behavioral services for children with autism

and developmental delays. Cristina then spent two years working as a Primary

Therapist providing behavioral services at Quest Kids. These opportunities lead to work

at Community Services for Autistic Adults and Children in Rockville, MD where she was

responsible for in-home behavioral programming and staff training.

After moving back to Florida, Cristina spent 3 years working as a Behavior

Analyst providing behavioral services to foster children and teaching foster parents

behavioral parenting skills under the supervision of Dr. Timothy Vollmer. This

opportunity motivated Cristina’s to pursue a graduate degree in applied behavior

analysis, and she began her graduate studies at the University of Florida in 2004. Since

beginning graduate school, Cristina has had the opportunity to conduct research in the

areas of assessment of preference and large scale program evaluation. Cristina will

continue her graduate studies at the University of Florida to obtain her doctoral degree.

53