Team based interactions

Embed Size (px)

Citation preview

  • 7/25/2019 Team based interactions

    1/33

    This article was downloaded by: [McMaster University]On: 11 December 2014, At: 12:28Publisher: RoutledgeInforma Ltd Registered in England and Wales Registered Number: 1072954Registered office: Mortimer House, 37-41 Mortimer Street, London W1T 3JH,UK

    European Journal of Work and

    Organizational PsychologyPublication details, including instructions for authors

    and subscription information:

    http://www.tandfonline.com/loi/pewo20

    Improving work motivation and

    performance in brainstorminggroups: The effects of three

    group goal-setting strategiesJrgen Wegge

    a& S. Alexander Haslam

    b

    aUniversity of Munich (LMU) , Germany

    bSchool of Psychology , Exeter, UK

    Published online: 17 Feb 2007.

    To cite this article:Jrgen Wegge & S. Alexander Haslam (2005) Improving workmotivation and performance in brainstorming groups: The effects of three group goal-

    setting strategies, European Journal of Work and Organizational Psychology, 14:4,

    400-430, DOI: 10.1080/13594320500349961

    To link to this article: http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/13594320500349961

    PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE

    Taylor & Francis makes every effort to ensure the accuracy of all theinformation (the Content) contained in the publications on our platform.However, Taylor & Francis, our agents, and our licensors make norepresentations or warranties whatsoever as to the accuracy, completeness, orsuitability for any purpose of the Content. Any opinions and views expressed

    in this publication are the opinions and views of the authors, and are not theviews of or endorsed by Taylor & Francis. The accuracy of the Content shouldnot be relied upon and should be independently verified with primary sourcesof information. Taylor and Francis shall not be liable for any losses, actions,claims, proceedings, demands, costs, expenses, damages, and other liabilitieswhatsoever or howsoever caused arising directly or indirectly in connectionwith, in relation to or arising out of the use of the Content.

    http://www.tandfonline.com/loi/pewo20http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/13594320500349961http://www.tandfonline.com/action/showCitFormats?doi=10.1080/13594320500349961http://www.tandfonline.com/loi/pewo20
  • 7/25/2019 Team based interactions

    2/33

    This article may be used for research, teaching, and private study purposes.Any substantial or systematic reproduction, redistribution, reselling, loan, sub-licensing, systematic supply, or distribution in any form to anyone is expresslyforbidden. Terms & Conditions of access and use can be found at http://www.tandfonline.com/page/terms-and-conditions

    Downlo

    adedby[McMasterUniver

    sity]at12:2811December

    2014

    http://www.tandfonline.com/page/terms-and-conditionshttp://www.tandfonline.com/page/terms-and-conditions
  • 7/25/2019 Team based interactions

    3/33

    Improving work motivation and performance inbrainstorming groups: The effects of three group

    goal-setting strategies

    Ju rgen WeggeUniversity of Munich (LMU), Germany

    S. Alexander HaslamSchool of Psychology, Exeter, UK

    An experiment was conducted with 30 groups (n 120) solving brainstormingtasks under four different group goal conditions: do your best (DYB), directivegroup goal setting (DGGS), participative group goal setting (PGGS), andPGGS in combination with individual goal setting (PGGS IGS). Asexpected, all groups with specific and difficult group goals performed betterthan DYB control groups. It is hypothesized that these positive effects ofgroup goal setting on brainstorming performance arise because group goal

    setting counteracts motivation losses such as social loafing. In addition,group goal setting should promote motivation gains arising from socialcompensation and related cognitive processes, in particular high identificationwith the group. Consistent with this hypothesis, it was found that group goalsetting increased team identification, the readiness to compensate for otherweak group members, the value of group success, and the value of groupfailure. Mediation analysis also indicated that concern to avoid group failurewas partly responsible for performance improvements. Finally, no largedifferences were found between PGGS IGS and PGGS or DGGS. On thisbasis group goal setting can be considered a robust strategy for improvingwork motivation and brainstorming performance in groups.

    During the last few decades working in groups has become increasingly

    popular in organizations. This is consistent not only with common beliefs

    Correspondence should be addressed to Ju rgen Wegge, Ludwig-Maximilians-Universita t

    Mu nchen (LMU), Department of Psychology, Psychology of Excellence, Martiusstrasse 4,

    D-80802 Mu nchen, Germany. Email: [email protected]

    This research was supported by a grant from the University of Dortmund (Kennzahl 81 14

    31). The authors would like to thank Ed Locke, Daan van Knippenberg, Michael West, and

    three anonymous reviewers for detailed comments on an earlier version of this article.

    EUROPEAN JOURNAL OF WORK AND

    ORGANIZATIONAL PSYCHOLOGY

    2005, 14 (4), 400430

    2005 Psychology Press Ltd

    http://www.psypress.com/ejwop DOI: 10.1080/13594320500349961

    Downlo

    adedby[McMasterUniver

    sity]at12:2811December

    2014

  • 7/25/2019 Team based interactions

    4/33

    about the synergy that can emerge when people work in teams but also

    with insights from organizational and social psychology. Compared to

    more traditional forms of work design, collaboration in teams has been

    found to enhance communication, innovation, and the quality of decision

    making (Cohen & Bailey, 1997; Haslam, 2004; Kerr & Tindale, 2004; West,

    2002; West, Borrill, & Unsworth, 1998). Moreover, working in teams or

    groups (terms that we use here as synonyms) satisfies motivations for

    collective self-actualization and meaningful work (Ellemers, de Gilder, &

    Haslam, 2004). However, it is also widely recognized that teamwork is often

    plagued with specific motivation and coordination problems. With respect to

    group brainstorminga method of collective idea generation in groups

    popularized by Osborn (1957)it is consistently found, for example, that

    the inability for more than one group member to talk at a time (productionblocking) is a powerful process that can hamper team performance.

    In addition, social loafing can occur when group members are not

    individually identifiable or accountable for their performance (Karau &

    Williams, 1993). Evaluation apprehensionwhereby group members are

    concerned about the other group members appraisal of their ideasis also

    another prominent motivation loss that can contribute to poor performance

    on brainstorming tasks (see, for recent reviews, Kerr & Tindale, 2004;

    Paulus, 2000; Paulus, Dugosh, Dzindolet, Coskun, & Putman, 2002;

    Thompson, 2003).Based on this knowledge, research and applied literatures offer several

    recommendations for improving traditional group brainstorming. For

    example, prior research has documented the efficacy of using a facilitator

    who is trained to minimize production blocking and evaluation apprehen-

    sion (Oxley, Dzindolet, & Paulus, 1996). Other promising interventions

    involve using electronic brainstorming (Pinsonneault, Barki, Gallupe,

    & Hoppen, 1999) or exchanging written ideas in a sequential manner

    (Paulus & Yang, 2000). The present research seeks to contribute to this

    literature by analysing the efficiency of setting a specific and challenginggroup goal(e.g., trying to find 35 new ideas for solving a problem as a group

    in 5 minutes). More specifically, the study examines the effects of three

    group goal-setting strategies: (a) directive group goal setting by an authority

    in a friendly and convincing tell and sell manner where a rationale for a

    challenging group goal is given (DGGS), (b) participative group goal

    setting by an authority realized within a fair group discussion about the

    appropriate group goal (PGGS), and (c) PGGS in combination with

    individual goal setting (PGGS IGS). All three strategies are based on

    goal-setting theory (Locke & Latham, 1990, see below), and it is

    expected that these techniques will motivate brainstorming groups

    to work better than groups that are instructed simply to do their best

    (DYB-control).

    GROUP GOAL SETTING 401

    Downlo

    adedby[McMasterUniver

    sity]at12:2811December

    2014

  • 7/25/2019 Team based interactions

    5/33

    Almost no prior research has examined if and how different group goal-

    setting interventions enhance performance in brainstorming groups (but for

    preliminary positive tests see Wegge & Haslam, 2003; Wegge & Kleinbeck,

    1996). Garnering empirical evidence on this issue is therefore important for

    practical reasons. Accordingly, the first aim of this study is to assess whether

    group goal setting should be added to the list of interventions that have the

    potential to increase performance in brainstorming groups. From a more

    theoretical perspective, the study also seeks to investigate the motivational

    underpinnings of the expected performance improvements induced by group

    goal setting. It will be argued that the positive effects of group goal setting

    on brainstorming performance arise because group goal settingcounteracts

    the occurrence of motivation losses in teams such as social loafing. In

    addition, this procedure should promote motivation gains such as socialcompensation (e.g., a deliberate decision to compensate for the weaknesses

    of other team members) and related cognitive and emotional processes

    resulting from high identification with the group. These putative links

    between different group goal-setting manipulations and motivation gains or

    motivation losses in teams have rarely been analysed in previous research

    (Ellemers et al., 2004; Hertel, 2000). Thus, the second aim of the study is to

    examine whether there is a fruitful link to be made between goal-setting

    theory and social psychological research into motivational processes in

    teams.

    THEORETICAL BACKGROUND

    There are literally hundreds of studies that demonstrate a reliable impact of

    goals on behaviour (Locke & Latham, 1990, 2002). Based on this evidence, it

    is widely acknowledged that goals (intentions) are an immediate and

    powerful regulator of human action. Goal-setting research has also

    examined which aspects of goals are most important for predicting

    differences in task performance. The current evidence on this question isconclusive. It has consistently been found that specific and difficult

    performance goals lead to better performance than easy goals or unspecific

    goal instructionstypified by invitations to Do your best.

    The effects of goal setting, however, have typically been analysed in

    relation to individual performance (Wagner, 1994). As a consequence, far

    fewer studies have examined the impact ofgroup goals (an intention shared

    by a group) on group performance. Nevertheless, there are several recent

    studies indicating that group goal setting improves team performance

    (DeShon, Kozlowski, Schmidt, Milner, & Wiechmann, 2004; Durham,

    Locke, Poon, & McLeod, 2000; Wegge, 2000; Wegge & Haslam, 2003) even

    though some failures have also been reported (e.g., see Sagie, 1996; Wegge &

    Haslam, 2004). In a meta-analysis covering 26 effect sizes derived from

    402 WEGGE AND HASLAM

    Downlo

    adedby[McMasterUniver

    sity]at12:2811December

    2014

  • 7/25/2019 Team based interactions

    6/33

    10 studies conducted between 1978 and 1991 and comprising data from 163

    groups and 1684 individuals, OLeary-Kelly, Martocchio, and Frink (1994)

    found that performance of groups striving for a specific difficult group goal

    is almost one standard deviation higher (d .92) than the performance of

    groups that do not have clear goals (DYB-instructions). This effect is

    referred to here as the GGS-effect and building on this work we propose that

    it will also be observed in brainstorming groups:

    Hypothesis 1: Compared to control instructions (DYB), group goal-

    setting techniques (DGGS, PGGS, PGGS IGS) will increase brain-

    storming performance.

    CURRENT EXPLANATIONS FOR GROUPGOAL-SETTING EFFECTS

    What mediating mechanisms contribute to the overall GGS effect? At least

    two distinct propositions have been developed by previous researchers.

    Often it is suggested that the mediating processes of GGS effects areidentical

    to those mechanismsincreased effort, high persistence, task focusing,

    development and use of appropriate task strategiesthat are responsible for

    goal-setting effects in individual performance situations (see DeShon et al.,

    2004; Durham et al., 2000; Locke & Latham, 1990, 2002). This propositionis plausible since individual performance is usually the basis for team

    performance. Moreover, team members sometimes set individual goals in

    accordance with group goals (Widmeyer & Ducharme, 1997) so that

    individual goals and corresponding mechanisms might mediate the effect of

    group goals.

    However, the individual goals of group members are sometimes in

    conflict with either (a) the individual goals of other group members, (b) self-

    set group goals, or (c) group goals suggested by group leaders (Crown &

    Rosse, 1995; Haslam, 2004; Hinsz, 1995; Mitchell & Silver, 1990; Peterson,1999; Tjosvold, 1998). In the same situation, for example, some group

    members might be guided by their individual goals (e.g., to work slowly and

    reduce ones effort) while other group members strive for the group goal

    (e.g., to work fast and achieve a high group standard). Various types of goal

    conflicts can be found in groups (see Wegge, 2004, for a recent review) and

    these goal conflicts serve to complicate predictions regarding the impact of

    goal-setting manipulations by an (external) authority.

    In the same vein, it is recognized that group work typically requires

    additional processes such as communication and planning within the whole

    team that are not necessarily required in individual performance situations.

    Weldon and Weingart (1993) developed a model describing three specific

    group-level mechanisms: (a) group planning (e.g., talking about who should

    GROUP GOAL SETTING 403

    Downlo

    adedby[McMasterUniver

    sity]at12:2811December

    2014

  • 7/25/2019 Team based interactions

    7/33

    do what, when, and where in the team), (b) cooperation (e.g., listening to the

    ideas of others, helping team mates performing their work), and (c) morale-

    building communication (e.g., statements that build a sense of collective

    efficacy or that stimulate supportive emotions). In this model, it is assumed

    that high values of these three processes increase the quality of group plans

    and the expectancy of successelements that should serve to facilitate team

    performance. To date, there is some evidence showing that the GGS effect is

    mediated by task-specific group planning (e.g., Durham et al., 2000;

    Weingart, 1992; Weldon, Jehn, & Pradhan, 1991). However, empirical

    evidence for the two other group mechanisms is weak. Consistent with the

    assumption of Weldon and Weingart (1993), in the present study it is argued

    that goal-setting theory has to beextendedif it is applied at the group level.

    More specifically, it is proposed that further empirical efforts to identifypossible group-specific mechanisms underlying group goal effects should

    consider those motivational processes that are usually discussed under the

    heading of motivationlosses and motivationgains in social psychology

    (Haslam, 2004; Hertel, 2000; Karau & Williams, 1993; Stroebe, Diehl, &

    Abakoumkin, 1996).

    GROUP GOAL SETTING AND MOTIVATIONLOSSES IN GROUPS

    The basic phenomenon of motivation losses in groups is well documented in

    social psychology (Karau & Williams, 1993). When individuals work in a

    group they sometimes exert less effort than they do when working

    individually on the same task. This basic phenomenon is called motivation

    loss and it takes on several different forms. In particular, research has

    provided evidence of (a) social loafing effects (unintentional reduction of

    work motivation and effort when working collectively; Karau & Williams,

    1993), (b) free-riding effects (deliberate reduction of effort if a persons

    contribution is seen to be unnecessary for the group to succeed; see Kerr &Bruun, 1983), and (c) sucker effects (the deliberate withdrawal of effort that

    occurs if a person realizes that a capable team partner is free-riding; Kerr,

    1983; Kerr & Bruun, 1983).

    To our knowledge, only three previous studies have empirically examined

    possible links between motivation losses and group goal setting. Matsui,

    Kakuyama, and Onglatco (1987) argued that providing both individual and

    group goals and corresponding performance feedback increases the

    identifiability of individual performance in teams, thereby counteracting

    social loafing. In addition, group goals and group feedback should prevent

    the occurrence of sucker effects. This is because having a specific, difficult

    group goal should make people feel that their own efforts are indispensable

    even if other team members are free-riding. Matsui et al.s empirical work

    404 WEGGE AND HASLAM

    Downlo

    adedby[McMasterUniver

    sity]at12:2811December

    2014

  • 7/25/2019 Team based interactions

    8/33

    provides support for both propositions. Similarly, Erez and Somech (1996)

    found that group performance loss (measured by the difference between

    performance scores of individuals working alone and their scores when

    working in groups) is less likely to occur when specific, difficult goals

    (individual or group) are present. Finally, van Leeuwen and van

    Knippenberg (2002) showed that group goal setting can improve group

    performance because it affects social matching processes that might also lead

    to motivation losses in groups (e.g., where individual standards regarding

    ones own contribution to the group product are shifteddownwardsto match

    low performance or the standards of weaker group members; see also Paulus

    et al., 2002).

    Even though these ideas and results are intuitively plausible, it is

    important to note that there is no empirical evidence corroborating linksbetween different types of group goals and motivation losses. Moreover,

    previous research has focused on task performance as a dependent variable.

    Therefore, it is not clear whether different goal-setting strategies system-

    atically change the expectations and judgements of group members that

    underlie the occurrence of motivation losses in teams. A sucker effect arises

    if group members realize or expect that other members of their team are

    engaging in free-riding behaviour (Kerr & Bruun, 1983) and, therefore, this

    phenomenon should not occur ifnofree-riding behaviour is expected. With

    respect to social loafing, findings from previous research show that this canbe encouraged by lack of concern for group success or group failure (Hertel,

    Konradt, & Orlikowski, 2004; Karau & Williams, 1993). Thus, placing value

    on group success or on the prevention of group failure should counteract the

    phenomenon of social loafing. The differentiation between the value placed

    on success and failure is also introduced here in light of findings reported by

    Higgins (1997), which show that the same behaviour might be motivated by

    a promotion focus in self-regulation (i.e., to achieve positive states) or by a

    prevention focus (to avoid negative states). Taken together, the following

    hypothesis can thus be derived:

    Hypothesis 2: Compared to control instructions (DYB), group goal-setting

    techniques (DGGS, PGGS, PGGS IGS) enhance (a) the expectation that

    other team members will not free-ride and (b) the subjective importance

    of team success and team failure.

    GROUP GOAL SETTING AND MOTIVATION GAINSIN GROUPS

    With the exception of social facilitation induced by the mere presence of

    (coacting) others (Bond & Titus, 1983), the possibility that groups can

    contribute to motivation gains has, until recently, been largely overlooked

    GROUP GOAL SETTING 405

    Downlo

    adedby[McMasterUniver

    sity]at12:2811December

    2014

  • 7/25/2019 Team based interactions

    9/33

    (Stroebe et al., 1996). Nevertheless, an emerging body of research suggests

    that group contexts provide specific stimuli that can motivate individuals to

    exert more effort in groups than they do when working on the same task

    individually. This basic phenomenon is called motivation gain. Whereas

    the existence of such phenomena is now becoming widely accepted, there is

    still debate about how many different motivation gains can and should be

    distinguished (for recent reviews see Haslam, 2004; Hertel, 2000; Wegge,

    2004).

    In the present study, we focus on a motivation gain referred to associal

    compensation. Williams and Karau (1991, p. 571) proposed that under some

    conditions people may work harder in a collective setting than in a coactive

    setting in order to compensate for others in their group. In support of this

    idea, Karau and Williams (1997) and Williams and Karau (1991) foundevidence that group members sometimes compensate for the deficiencies of

    other group members on a collective task (e.g., group brainstorming) in

    order to ensure group success. Thus, contrary to the sucker effect, work

    motivation is enhanced when a group member realizes or anticipates that

    other group members are performing poorly (e.g., because of low work

    motivation or low ability). Based on previous research, individuals

    readiness to socially compensate for other group member should be

    increased if the group task is perceived as meaningful (e.g., the task itself

    is interesting and valuable) and if the person is concerned about how thegroups performance is evaluated (e.g., by other co-workers or by a

    supervisor or other external agencies). Moreover, Williams and Karau

    (1991, p. 580) suggest that social compensation should also be more

    pronounced if the group is relatively small, when it appears that the

    individual must continue to remain in the group, and at earlier stages of

    the collective process. In addition, social compensation in teams should be

    more likely if group cohesion is high (Karau & Hart, 1998) and if group

    members identify with other members of the group so that the group is

    perceived as a salient part of ones own social identity (Haslam, 2004;Haslam & Ellemers, 2005; van Knippenberg & Ellemers, 2003).

    To our knowledge, there is almost no research that has examined possible

    links between group goal setting and social compensation or other related

    processes (e.g., social identification, task interest) that may contribute to this

    relationship. Indeed, aside from a few studies showing that PGGS can

    enhance group cohesion (Wegge & Kleinbeck, 1996; Widmeyer &

    Ducharme, 1997), we have no indication that GGS has an impact on these

    variables. However, having a clear performance goal and corresponding

    feedback often makes tasks more valuable and intrinsically motivating

    (Fried & Ferris, 1987; Mento, Locke, & Klein, 1992). Moreover, at the level

    of individual performance and individual goal setting there is some evidence

    showing that self-set goals increase intrinsic motivation, especially when

    406 WEGGE AND HASLAM

    Downlo

    adedby[McMasterUniver

    sity]at12:2811December

    2014

  • 7/25/2019 Team based interactions

    10/33

    these goals are challenging (Harackiewicz, Barron, Tauer, Carter, & Elliot,

    2000; Latham, Erez, & Locke, 1988). In addition, thinking or talking as a

    group about a common group goal inherently promotes a team focus

    (Haslam et al., in press; Wegge & Haslam, 2003). Hence, it would be

    expected that group goal setting has the power to promote the emergence of

    social compensation along with feelings and judgements that bolster this

    phenomenon. This leads to:

    Hypothesis 3: Compared to control instructions (DYB), group goal-

    setting techniques (DGGS, PGGS, PGGS IGS) enhance (a) readiness

    to engage in social compensation, (b) group cohesion, (c) group

    identification, and (d) intrinsic work motivation in groups.

    A final objective of the study is to assess the relative strengths and

    weaknesses of the three different group goal-setting strategies. Is it effective

    to combine group goal setting and individual goal setting in teams

    (PGGS IGS)? As successful work in groups (e.g., flying a plane) often

    requires specific group members to do a specific job, adding individual goal-

    setting procedures related to the subtasks of group members (e.g., flying

    safely, being polite to passengers) seems a logical supplement to general

    group goal setting (e.g., to produce high customer satisfaction). However, to

    date, this type of goal-setting strategy has received little empirical attention(Crown & Rosse, 1995; Matsui et al., 1987; Mitchell & Silver, 1990) and

    results are mixed (e.g., Mitchell and Silver, 1990, found no beneficial effect

    of adding individual goals to group goals, whereas Matsui et al., 1987,

    reported performance enhancements for the same manipulation). Therefore,

    it is not clear if, and under which circumstances, this strategy might be

    effectivein particular, because studies also differ with respect to the tasks

    employed. Matsui et al., for example, used an additive task with low task

    interdependence, whereas Mitchell and Silver employed a task in which

    task interdependence was very high and competition (due to negative goalinterdependence introduced by an individual goal) was detrimental to

    group performance. Accordingly, the present study seeks to collect more

    evidence pertaining to this issue. In the same vein, the study explores

    potential differences between participative and directive group goal-setting

    strategies. Are the processes through which these interventions improve

    group performance similar or different? We know from previous work (e.g.,

    Erez, 1995; Latham et al., 1988) that assigning goals in a directive tell and

    sell style can be as effective as participative goal setting. However, very few

    similar comparisons have been made at the group level (but see Kerr &

    Tindale, 2004, pp. 22.722.8; Wegge, 2000) and, as a result, firm conclusions

    about the impact of different strategies at this level are hard to draw. It

    might be the case, for example, that participation is more important at

    GROUP GOAL SETTING 407

    Downlo

    adedby[McMasterUniver

    sity]at12:2811December

    2014

  • 7/25/2019 Team based interactions

    11/33

    the group level because the group context intensifies the desire to have a

    voice in decisions (Haslam, Eggins, & Reynolds, 2003) or because directive

    supervisor behaviour has less influence in front of a group, in particular

    when the going gets tough (Wegge & Haslam, 2003, 2004).

    In sum, to our knowledge, previous research has not examined whether

    different strategies of group goal setting (e.g., DGGS, PGGS) do have the

    power to prevent motivation losses in teams such as social loafing and/or

    whether they promote the occurrence of motivation gains in teams such as

    social compensation. Moreover, it is unclear if the most common group

    goal-setting strategies have similar impact with respect to their effectiveness

    in improving work motivation and performance in teams. Given the lack of

    previous work on these issues, the present study is largely exploratory with

    respect to the comparative efficacy of the three group goal-settinginterventions whose impact it investigated.

    METHOD

    Participants

    The sample consisted of 60 male and 60 female students (mean age 24.46

    years, range 19 to 37 years, SD 1.48) from a large German University

    with different majors (psychology excluded). Participants were recruited bymeans of advertisements on notice boards in the university. They worked

    together in 30 four-person (2 male, 2 female) groups. Each person received

    7.50 Euros for participating in the study. No further financial incentives

    (e.g., for goal attainment) were provided.

    General procedure

    On arrival, participants were informed that the purpose of the study was to

    analyse the effectiveness of brainstorming in groups. Group members werefirst requested to answer a personality questionnaire. This questionnaire

    asked participants to provide biographic and demographic details (e.g., age,

    sex). Next, students had to work together on a brainstorming task. All

    groups were asked to solve three different trials (see below). After Trial 2

    was finished, the experimenter determined in which condition the group was

    placed. The experimenter was a trained student assistant who was blind to

    the hypotheses that were tested in this study. After this, he enacted the

    corresponding group goal manipulation (e.g., PGGS or DGGS). Immedi-

    ately after this manipulation, andbeforeTrial 3 of the brainstorming task, a

    second questionnaire was distributed that assessed several key variables

    (e.g., perceived participation, group goal commitment; see the measure

    section below). After completion of Trial 3, intrinsic motivation and desire

    408 WEGGE AND HASLAM

    Downlo

    adedby[McMasterUniver

    sity]at12:2811December

    2014

  • 7/25/2019 Team based interactions

    12/33

    for further team cooperation was assessed by means of a third ques-

    tionnaire. Finally, participants were paid, debriefed, and asked not to

    discuss the experiment with others.

    Task and experimental design

    All groups were asked to solve three trials of a brainstorming task. Each trial

    lasted 3 minutes and presented a new problem. As a group, participants had

    to find and write down individually as many different uses for common

    objects (e.g., a pocket lamp) as they could. Group members were informed

    that brainstorming typically seeks to produce as many different solutions

    to a problem as possible and that, therefore, the number of unique uses

    generated is the appropriate measure of team performance. After each trial,all group members were asked to read their ideas out aloud. The experi-

    menter counted the number of unique uses (e.g., so that lighting a dark

    room and lighting a cellar were counted as one idea) generated by all

    group members and always verified his judgement with the group in a short

    discussion.1

    Trial 1 was a practice trial. In Trial 2 of the brainstorming task, all groups

    were instructed to do their best (DYB) to establish a baseline for group

    performance. In Trial 3, goal instructions varied according to the selected

    group goal-setting strategy (see below). It should be noted that theexperimenter considered several points when deciding before Trial 3 in

    which condition a group should be placed. First, it was important to have

    similar performance values across the four conditions in Trial 2 because

    otherwise it would be difficult to compare group performance. As the number

    of groups was relatively small, the probability of fulfilling this desirable

    condition by chance alone is low. Therefore, the experimenter followed the

    principles of a matched-groups design: Groups were assigned as quad-

    ruplets to conditions after performance in the baseline was known. As soon

    as a match in baseline performance was found, the next available conditionwas selected randomly. Of course, to determine which group goals can be

    assigned to DGGS groups, it is necessary to test some PGGS groups before.

    Therefore, this type of group was tested most often. It was also decided that

    six control groups would be sufficient for the purpose of this study.

    1The correlations between quantity and quality of ideas (unique uses) was very high in this

    study (e.g.,r .88,p5 .01 in Trial 2 andr .85,p5 .01 in Trial 3) so that only the number of

    unique ideas is considered below as an indicator of group performance. This is not only

    warranted in view of the high correlations but also because instructions emphasized the qualityof ideas and group goals were also formulated with respect to this performance indicator. It

    should be also noted that brainstorming tasks often include additional features (building upon

    other group members ideas) that were not present in our study.

    GROUP GOAL SETTING 409

    Downlo

    adedby[McMasterUniver

    sity]at12:2811December

    2014

  • 7/25/2019 Team based interactions

    13/33

    With the help of this procedure it was possible to hold both baseline

    performance and goal difficulty constant across conditions even though

    group assignment was still almost random.

    For control groups (DYB, n 6), the DYB-instruction was simply

    repeated in Trial 3. Groups in the participative group-goal condition

    (PGGS,n 10) were asked to determine a specific group goal through group

    discussion for Trail 3. For this purpose, each group member first made an

    individual suggestion for the group goal. In order to facilitate a GGS effect,

    the experimenter attempted to influence the suggestions of group members

    by emphasizing that the group should agree on achallenginggroup goal. He

    stated that, based on data from pilot studies, good groups achieve a

    performance improvement of 40% from baseline. Thus, 40% more ideas

    would constitute a challenging group goal. Next, individual suggestionswere collected and announced by the experimenter. The mean value of these

    suggestions was computed and fed back to the group. As expected on the

    basis of pilot testing, this value was usually below 40%. Next, the

    experimenter asked the group to discuss these suggestions and to come up

    with a more challenging group goal. The experimenter accepted every group

    goal that represented an increase in difficulty (the mean of these goals is

    between 34% and 38% in the three conditions with specific group goals, see

    Table 3 below).

    In the second participation condition (PGGS IGS, n 6), all groupmembers had to determine individual goals in combination with group goals

    for Trial 3. After group goals had been established in the same manner as

    PGGS, the experimenter explained that individual goal setting usually

    assists group goal setting. Therefore, each group member was told to

    formulate individual performance goals (e.g., finding 8 ideas) to support the

    group goal (e.g., finding 32 ideas as a group in the last trial). The

    experimenter then asked group members to write down these suggestions

    and asked the group to discuss them so that the sum of the goals would be

    consistent with the previously established group goal. The range of theseindividual goals was between 6 and 17 ideas (M 10.4 for the 24 relevant

    participants) and the mean within-group variance of individual goals was

    2.47 indicating that individual goals within groups were quite different.2

    However, discussions were not difficult as all groups found it easy to agree

    on a set of individual goals that was consistent with the group goal.

    Finally, following the rules of a matched-group design, 8 of 10 group

    goals that were set participatively in PGGS conditions were later assigned in

    2Difficulty of individual performance goals is correlated both with individual performance in

    baseline trials (r .74,p5 .01) and with difficulty of the first individual suggestion for a group

    goal (r .41, p5 .05).

    410 WEGGE AND HASLAM

    Downlo

    adedby[McMasterUniver

    sity]at12:2811December

    2014

  • 7/25/2019 Team based interactions

    14/33

    a tell and sell style by the experimenter in the directive group goal

    condition (DGGS, n 8). He referred to results from pilot studies and

    stated, for example, that 34% performance improvements from baseline

    constitute a challenging but reachable group goal. The experimenter

    then encouraged the group to strive for this goal and calculated the number

    of ideas that needed to be generated in order to achieve this goal in the

    last trial.

    Measures

    Constructs were assessed by observation on the part of the experimenter and

    with the help of three questionnaires distributed during the experiment.

    Unless stated otherwise below, all self-report items used in Questionnaires13 were composed and developed in a series of experiments summarized by

    Wegge (for details see Wegge, 2004).

    Observation by the experimenter

    For each group, the experimenter noted on a special sheet several aspects of

    the group process for each trial. These variables were: (a) individual

    performance (the number of different uses in one trial), (b) group

    performance (the total number of unique uses found in one trial by allgroup members, always verified with the group in a short discussion, see

    above), (c) individual suggestions for a group goal, (d) individual suggestion

    for an individual goal, and (e) chosen individual and group goals.

    Questionnaire 1

    To collect demographic data (e.g., with respect to age, gender, major),

    several questions were asked before Trial 1. Gender was balanced across

    conditions. As the age of group members also did not vary systematicallyacross the four experimental conditions (F .06,p5 .98), and because data

    in this study is analysed at the group level (see below), these variables are not

    considered further.

    Questionnaire 2

    This questionnaire was administered before Trial 2 and assessed the

    following eight variables:

    Perceived participation. As a manipulation check, participants with

    specific group goals responded to the following two items using 4-point

    scales ranging from low (1) to high (4): I had a real voice in determining

    GROUP GOAL SETTING 411

    Downlo

    adedby[McMasterUniver

    sity]at12:2811December

    2014

  • 7/25/2019 Team based interactions

    15/33

    which group goal we selected to strive for and In comparison to

    the experimenter, my impact on the difficulty of group goal was low;

    reverse coded).

    Group goal commitment. This was measured by means of nine items

    using 4-point response scales ranging from low (1) to high (4). These items

    assessed different antecedents of commitment to goals that have been

    identified as important in previous studies (see Klein, Wesson, Hollenbeck,

    & Alge, 1999): the value of goal attainment (e.g., If the group reaches its

    goal, this will have pleasant consequences for me), expectancy of group

    goal attainment (e.g., It is highly likely that we are a little bit better than

    the group goal requires), and volitional strength during goal attainment (I

    stick to the group goal even when I realize that my feelings divert me fromthis goal).

    Group cohesion. This was assessed using six items, three indexing liking

    of other group members (e.g., Quite frankly, I have to admit that I like the

    other group members) and three items addressing pride in being a member

    of the group (e.g., If you are a member of this group, you can feel proud).

    Individuals responded to these items on 4-point response scales ranging

    from low (1) to high (4).

    Group identification. This was assessed with four items using 4-point

    response scales ranging from low (1) to high (4) selected from Luhtanen and

    Crocker (1992). Typical items were: My membership in this group has

    much to do with how I feel as a person; The group I belong to is an

    important reflection of who I am).

    Social compensation. This was assessed by means of three items using 4-

    point scales ranging from low (1) to high (4). A typical statement with which

    participants stated their agreement was If other group members fail tosolve the group task because they do not have enough ability, I will work

    especially hard to reach the group goal.

    Value of group success andvalue of group failure. These were measured

    with following two items: Please mark on the following scale how valuable

    the success or failure of the group would be for you3 . . .. The value of

    3

    As success and failure are defined somehow differently in DYB groups (e.g., belonging tothe best groups is a success) and in groups with specific group goals (e.g., achieving the group

    goal is a success), corresponding explanations were added in the specific questionnaires used in

    these conditions.

    412 WEGGE AND HASLAM

    Downlo

    adedby[McMasterUniver

    sity]at12:2811December

    2014

  • 7/25/2019 Team based interactions

    16/33

    success can range from 0 without value to 5 of very high value;

    the value of failure can range from 0 without meaning to 5 very

    meaningful.

    Expect no free-riding. This was measured with three items using 4-point

    scales ranging from low (1) to high (4). An illustrative item was, In my

    group we have a free-rider who is taking advantage of other group

    members (reverse coded).

    Questionnaire 3

    After Trial 3 of the brainstorming task, a final questionnaire was

    administered. This incorporated measures of the following two constructs:

    Intrinsic motivation. This was assessed before performance feedback by

    means of following three items: Even if we were not successful in reaching

    the group goal . . . (a) working on the group task was really fun, (d) the

    group task was interesting, and (c) during the group task time was passing

    very quickly. Individuals responded to these items on a 5-point scale

    ranging from low (1) to high (5).

    Desire for further team cooperation. This was measured after performancefeedback from the experimenter as an index of future work motivation.

    For this measure, individuals responded to three items (e.g., I can imagine

    solving more tasks with this group) once again using 4-point answering scales

    ranging from low (1) to high (4).

    RESULTS

    Measurement reliability, level of analysis, and descriptive data

    Based on calculated alpha reliabilities, the measurement reliability of all

    scales was good (see Table 1). Thus, all constructs are measured in a

    consistent way.4 In this study, performance of whole intact groups is

    examined. Even though individual performance data is available, for

    theoretical reasons the level of analysis is the group (for a discussion of

    4This is also true for the variable social compensation because this scale was comprised of

    only three items. Here, an alpha of .60 still indicates a satisfactory measurement consistency

    (Cortina, 1993). This is because the alpha statistic strongly depends on the number of items. Analpha of .50 would indicate, for example, a mean item correlation of .25 for a scale with 3 items

    and this would be equal to an alpha of .77 for a scale with 10 items having the same average item

    correlation.

    GROUP GOAL SETTING 413

    Downlo

    adedby[McMasterUniver

    sity]at12:2811December

    2014

  • 7/25/2019 Team based interactions

    17/33

    TAB

    LE1

    AlphaR

    eliabilities(inparenthesis),A

    verageWithin-Group-Variances(V),AverageWithin-Group

    Agreement(Rwg),GroupMe

    ans(M),

    StandardDeviations(SD)andCorrelationsofVariablesforallG

    roups

    Variable

    V

    Rwg

    M

    SD

    1

    2

    3

    4

    5

    6

    7

    8

    9

    1.group

    goalcommitment1

    0.25

    0.91

    2.8

    0.2

    (.79)

    2.group

    cohesion

    0.32

    0.92

    2.4

    0.2

    .47*

    (.85)

    3.group

    identification

    0.26

    0.88

    1.3

    0.3

    7.02

    .23

    (.83)

    4.socialcompensation

    0.34

    0.82

    3.1

    0.3

    .53*

    *

    .25

    .37*

    (.60)

    5.intrinsicmotivation

    0.57

    0.84

    3.8

    0.4

    .38

    .51**

    .25

    .49**

    (.70)

    6.expect

    nofree-riding

    0.28

    0.89

    1.6

    0.3

    .24

    .35*

    .03

    .40*

    .18

    (.74)

    7.valueofgroupsuccess

    1.20

    0.60

    2.2

    0.8

    .65*

    *

    .23

    7.04

    .41*

    .41*

    7.13

    ()

    8.valueofgroupfailure

    1.01

    0.50

    1.3

    0.5

    .47*

    .32

    .15

    .38*

    .34

    7.18

    .74**

    ()

    9.furtherteamcooperation

    0.41

    0.82

    2.9

    0.2

    .33

    .53**

    .11

    .23

    .52**

    .14

    .33

    .49**

    (.74)

    10.performanceimprovement

    10.1

    5.6

    .29

    .17

    .23

    .18

    .40*

    .05

    .20

    .32

    .31

    Notes:

    1n

    24fourpersongroups(otherwisen

    30fourpersongroups);*

    p5

    .05,**p5

    .01.

    414

    Downlo

    adedby[McMasterUniver

    sity]at12:2811December

    2014

  • 7/25/2019 Team based interactions

    18/33

    these issues see Klein & Kozlowski, 2000). Thus, with the exception of

    alpha reliabilities, statistical analysis is based on group means. In order to

    check whether aggregation at the group level is appropriate, within-

    group variance (V) of variables was calculated. This measure is easily

    interpretable and recommended5 by Schmidt and Hunter (1989). As

    documented in Table 1, the average within-group variance is below the

    critical value of 1.00 for almost all variables indicating that analysing the

    data at the group level is justified. A more fine-grained examination of this

    index reveals that only 7 out of 204 group values are larger than 1.00 for

    variables measured with scales. For the two one-item measures (value of

    group success and group failure) the within-group variance is higher.

    However, only 9 out of 60 groups have values greater 1.50 so for these

    variables there is moderate within-group consistency. As most priorresearchers have used the Rwg-index developed by James, Demaree, and

    Wolf (1984) to estimate within-group agreement, we also calculated this

    index (assuming random measurement error). Similar conclusions can be

    drawn with respect to this data. Mean values of the Rwg-index for all

    constructs that were measured using scales are higher than .70, a value

    typically considered to indicate substantial within-group agreement. For

    the two one-item measures, average within-group agreement is lower.

    Accordingly, taken together, this analysis indicates that aggregation to the

    group level is justified as there is moderate to very high within-groupconsistency.

    In view of the observed correlations, the following points are worth

    noting. First, all correlations are meaningful. For example, high group goal

    commitment correlates positively with high group cohesion and with

    readiness to engage in social compensation. Second, in most cases the

    correlations are of low to moderate strength. Thus, it can be concluded that

    different scales (e.g., group cohesion and group identification) do indeed

    measure different constructs. Third, the correlations with respect to

    improvement in group performance (ideas in Trial 3 minus ideas in Trial 2)are in line with various theories of work motivation. Here we obtain

    positive correlations between performance improvement and group goal

    5This measure is similar to the typically computed Rwg-index but less dependent on scale

    features. For a detailed discussion about differences see Schmidt and Hunter (1989) as well as

    James, Demaree, and Wolf (1993). Some researchers argue that ICC values should be computed

    for this purpose, too. These values determinebetweengroup inconsistency. However, we follow

    the argument of George and James (1993) who explained why documenting between group

    inconsistency is not required for justifying aggregation at the group level if consistency withingroups is assumed. According to the terms defined by Klein and Kozlowski (2000), our model

    for aggregation of the data to the group level is a consensus model. Thus, we do not analyse the

    variancebetween groups but only the agreement within the group.

    GROUP GOAL SETTING 415

    Downlo

    adedby[McMasterUniver

    sity]at12:2811December

    2014

  • 7/25/2019 Team based interactions

    19/33

    commitment, group cohesion, group identification, and value of group

    success. These correlations are not significant in this sample. Nevertheless,

    they are of similar strength to effects observed in recent meta-analyses that

    have examined the impact of these variables (e.g., r .23 for the goal

    commitmentperformance relationship; see Klein et al., 1999). Finally, there

    is also one significant correlation with respect to performance improvement

    in this study. Performance enhancement is correlated positively with

    intrinsic task motivation, r .40, p5 .05. As this variable was measured

    aftertask completion, the correlation probably reflects not only the effects of

    motivation on performance but also the effects of group performance on

    motivation.

    Manipulation checks

    Table 2 presents data relevant to examination of the effectiveness of the

    experimental manipulations. First, with respect to the degree of perceived

    participation it was found, as expected, that group members in

    participative groups (PGGS and PGGS IGS) reported having more

    input in determining the group goal than group members with assigned

    group goals, t73.46, p5 .01. There were no differences between PGGS

    and PGGS IGS and both values are significantly higher (p5 .05) than

    values from DGGS groups. Thus, in both conditions group membersperceived themselves to have more voice than in DGGS groups. Second, as

    expected, it was also found that group goal commitment was high in all

    these conditions. Thus, goal setting was effective in establishing commit-

    ment to group goals. Third,baseline performance (ideas in Trial 2) did not

    differ significantly across the four experimental conditions even though the

    number of groups in each condition is rather small. Thus, experimental

    procedures to assign groups to conditions were effective. Finally, a further

    potential confound in this study is group goal difficulty as a direct

    comparison between directive and participative goals is only justified if thegoal difficulty is similar. Furthermore, a GGS effect is only expected for

    teams striving for challenging group goals. Again, the data with respect to

    both issues (see Table 2) indicates that all manipulations were successful.

    Compared to baseline performance (ideas in Trial 2), all specific group

    goals can be considered to have been difficult. On average, groups

    intended (or were asked by the experimenter when goals were assigned)

    to improve their performance by more than 30% (DGGS38%;

    PGGS38%; PGGSIGS 34%). This is almost twice as much as

    the average performance improvement usually observed in goal-setting

    studies. Thus, group goal difficulty was high and comparable across

    conditions with specific group goals and, therefore, a GGS effect should be

    observed.

    416 WEGGE AND HASLAM

    Downlo

    adedby[McMasterUniver

    sity]at12:2811December

    2014

  • 7/25/2019 Team based interactions

    20/33

    TAB

    LE2

    Means(M)andstandar

    ddeviations(SD)ofselected

    variablesandcorresponding

    resultsfrom

    MANOVAs

    DYB(n

    6)

    DGGS(n

    8)

    PGGS(n

    10)

    PGGS

    IGS

    (n

    6)

    C-A

    D-P

    P1-P2

    M

    SD

    M

    SD

    M

    SD

    M

    SD

    Perceivedparticipation

    2.3

    0.7

    3.0

    0.3

    3.1

    0.3

    73.46**

    Groupgoalcommitment

    2.9

    0.2

    2.8

    0.2

    2.7

    0.2

    Ideasintr

    ial2

    27.0

    5.5

    28.4

    6.5

    28.3

    9.8

    26.8

    2.6

    Groupgoalsfortrial3

    38.3

    8.9

    37.6

    12.3

    34.2

    3.9

    Ideasintr

    ial3

    32.8

    8.4

    40.0

    5.0

    40.1

    12.9

    36.5

    4.6

    Performan

    ceimprovement

    5.8

    a

    6.3

    11.6

    b

    5.5

    11.8

    b

    5.6

    9.7

    b

    3.8

    C-A(trial23)72.12*

    Notes:DY

    B

    doyourbest,DGGS

    direc

    tivegroupgoalsetting,PGGS

    participativegroupgoalsetting,

    PGGS

    IGS

    PGGSincombinationwith

    individual

    goalsetting,C-A

    HelmertcontrastDYB-controlvs.allotherconditions,D-P

    Helmertcon

    trastdirectivevs.participative

    conditions,

    P1-P2H

    elmertcontrastPGGSvs.PGGS

    IGS;*p5

    .05,**p5

    .01,onlysignificantt-valuesarepresented.

    a,

    b

    indicatehomogeneoussub

    -groupings

    thatdiffer

    accordingtoadditionallycondu

    ctedposthoctestswithp5

    .10

    .

    417

    Downlo

    adedby[McMasterUniver

    sity]at12:2811December

    2014

  • 7/25/2019 Team based interactions

    21/33

    Statistical group comparisons

    To test mean differences across the four experimental conditions,

    MANOVAs were used in which data from all groups is analysed

    simultaneously. Here the use of Helmert contrasts is most appropriate

    because this form of contrast compares (a) the first group (DYB) to the

    remaining groups (C A, this reveals overall GGS effects), (b) the second

    group (DGGS) to the remaining groups (D P, this reveals differences

    due to participation) and (c) the third group (PGGS) to the last

    group (P1P2, this shows differences due to adding IGS). It should be

    noted that this form of contrast does not constitute a post hoc com-

    parison of groups as our corresponding hypotheses require testing exactly

    these differences. In addition, results from post hoc tests (Duncan)are reported below to describe other significant differences between

    groups in order to provide a more fine-grained analysis of motivational

    variables.

    Tests of Hypothesis 1

    As shown in Table 2, consistent with goal-setting theory and in support of

    Hypothesis 1, striving for specific, difficult group goals (DGGS, PGGS, and

    PGGS IGS) improved group performance more than striving for DYBgoals. A MANOVA with the factor group (the four group goal conditions)

    and the factor trial (Trial 2 vs. Trial 3) as a repeated-measures factor was

    conducted in order to examine this effect more closely. In this analysis, two

    effects were significant: the main effect for trial, F(1, 26) 95.14, p5 .01,

    indicating that all groups improved their performance over time, and the

    theoretically relevant contrast of the interaction (DYB vs. all other groups

    for performance differences across trials),t(26)72.12,p5 .05, indicating

    that all groups with specific group goals showed improved performance

    relative to DYB control groups. As can be seen from Table 2, there wasevidence of a substantial GGS effect as groups with challenging,

    specific group goals improved performance about twice as much as control

    groups.

    To estimate the effect size of this interaction,6 an additional regression

    analyses was conducted in which a dummy variable was computed that

    recoded the four conditions in such a way that DYB groups had the value 0

    and all other groups the value 1. As expected, a regression of this dummy

    6Unfortunately, the SPSS (11.0) output for the MANOVA procedure does not report effect

    sizes for Helmert contrasts (solely t-values are given), so additional computations are necessary

    to get this information.

    418 WEGGE AND HASLAM

    Downlo

    adedby[McMasterUniver

    sity]at12:2811December

    2014

  • 7/25/2019 Team based interactions

    22/33

    variable (representing the corresponding Helmert contrast C A) on

    performance improvements from Trial 2 to Trial 3 yielded a significant

    effect, standardized .39, R2 .15, F(1, 29) 5.1, p5 .03. Thus, about

    15% of variance in group performance improvement was based on the use

    of group goal-setting interventions.

    Tests of Hypothesis 2

    Table 3 presents data with respect to those motivational variables that

    were expected to underlie the GGS effect. Do group goal-setting

    techniques decrease the probability of a sucker effect by enhancing the

    expectation that other team members showno free-riding behaviour? This

    Hypothesis (2a) finds only weak support in the data as the correspondingHelmert contrast (C A) is not significant. However, the results of post

    hoc testing indicate that the combination of both group and individual

    goals (PGGS IGS) enhances this expectation compared to control

    groups. In line with expectations, the results also show that the value of

    group success, t(26)72.15, p5 .05, and the value of group failure,

    t(26)72.62, p5 .02, is indeed much higher in groups with specific

    group goals than in DYB groups (Hypothesis 2b). As social loafing in

    groups is typically considered to be a motivation loss that is linked with

    low values for group success and low value of group failure, socialloafing should therefore be less likely in groups striving for challenging

    group goals.

    Tests of Hypothesis 3

    With respect to potential motivation gains four variables are relevant. As

    expected, groups striving for specific, difficult group goals reported a higher

    readiness to engage in social compensation than DYB groups. However,

    results from post hoc tests and the significant Helmert contrast comparingDGGS to the two participative conditions (DP), indicate that this finding

    was only obtained for DGGS groups. Thus, only directive group goal-

    setting procedures increased group members readiness to compensate for

    other weak group members, t(26) 2.71, p5 .01. A parallel result was

    found with respect to the variable intrinsic motivation. Once again, DGGS

    was the best strategy for enhancing this motivational state, t(26) 2.17,

    p5 .05. With respect to group cohesion there were no significant differences.

    However, for the group identification measure we found the expected

    pattern such that groups with specific, challenging group goals identified

    more with their group than DYB groups, t(26)72.23, p5 .04. Finally,

    the desire for further team activity was generally quite high and did not vary

    across conditions.

    GROUP GOAL SETTING 419

    Downlo

    adedby[McMasterUniver

    sity]at12:2811December

    2014

  • 7/25/2019 Team based interactions

    23/33

    TAB

    LE3

    Means

    (M)andstandarddeviations

    (SD)ofvariablesacrossfour

    groupgoalconditionsandco

    rrespondingresultsfrom

    MA

    NOVAs

    DYB(n

    6)

    DGGS(n

    8)

    PGGS(n

    10)

    PGGSIG

    S

    (n

    6)

    C-A

    D-P

    P1-P2

    M

    SD

    M

    SD

    M

    SD

    M

    S

    D

    Expectno

    free-riding

    1.5

    a

    0.4

    1.7

    ab

    0.2

    1.6

    ab

    0.3

    1.8

    b

    0

    .1

    Valueofg

    roupsuccess

    1.6

    a

    1.0

    2.5

    b

    0.6

    2.3

    b

    0.5

    2.1

    ab

    0

    .9

    72.15*

    Valueofg

    roupfailure

    0.9

    a

    0.6

    1.6

    b

    0.5

    1.5

    b

    0.3

    1.2

    ab

    0

    .5

    72.62**

    Socialcom

    pensation

    2.9

    a

    0.3

    3.3

    b

    0.2

    3.0

    a

    0.2

    3.1

    a

    0

    .1

    2.71**

    Groupcohesion

    2.5

    0.2

    2.4

    0.3

    2.4

    0.2

    2.4

    0

    .2

    Groupide

    ntification

    1.1

    a

    0.1

    1.4

    b

    0.3

    1.4

    b

    0.2

    1.3

    ab

    0

    .1

    72.23*

    Intrinsicm

    otivation

    3.6

    a

    0.2

    4.1

    b

    0.4

    3.8

    ab

    0.5

    3.7

    a

    0

    .4

    2.17*

    Furtherte

    amcooperation

    3.0

    0.2

    3.1

    0.4

    3.0

    0.2

    2.9

    0

    .1

    Notes:DY

    B

    doyourbest,DGGS

    direc

    tivegroupgoalsetting,PGGS

    participativegroupgoalsetting,

    PGGS

    IGS

    PGGSincombinationwith

    individual

    goalsetting,C-A

    Helmertcon

    trastDYB-controlvs.allotherc

    onditions,D-P

    Helmertcontra

    stdirectivevs.participativeconditions,P1-

    P2

    Helm

    ertcontrastPGGSvs.PGGS

    IGS;*p5

    .05,**p5

    .01;onlysignificantt-valuesarepresented;a

    ,b

    indicatehomogeneoussub-groupingsthat

    differacco

    rdingtoadditionallyconducted

    posthoctestswithp5

    .10.

    420

    Downlo

    adedby[McMasterUniver

    sity]at12:2811December

    2014

  • 7/25/2019 Team based interactions

    24/33

    Mediational analysis

    We intimated above that the prevention of motivation losses and the

    promotion of motivation gains in groups could be a causal processes

    responsible for performance improvements arising from group goal setting.

    To explore this possibility, exploratory hierarchical regression analyses

    were conducted. First, a variable was computed that recoded the four

    conditions such that DYB groups had the value 0 and all other groups the

    value 1. As already described above, a regression of this variable on

    performance improvements from Trial 2 to Trial 3 yielded a significant

    effect, standardized .39, R2 .15, F(1, 29) 5.1, p5 .03. Next, we

    tested whether this effect could be substantially reduced by incorporating

    the three potential mediator variables7 in the regression before the dummyvariable is entered. This was not the case for value of success, .37,

    p5 .06, or group identification, .43, p5 .03. However, when the value

    of group failure is entered as a potential mediating variable, the effect

    was reduced, .31, p5 .12, R2 .11, reduction in R2 .04. This finding

    suggests that the value placed on group failure plays an important role in

    explaining group goal-setting effects on performance in brainstorming

    groups. That is, teams striving for difficult group goals seem to be

    concerned to avoid failure and this contributes to good brainstorming

    performance.

    DISCUSSION

    The results of this study are important in several aspects. First, we found the

    expected group goal-setting effect in groups performing group brainstorm-

    ing tasks. This effect was substantial in magnitude with groups striving for

    challenging group goals generating approximately twice as many additional

    ideas (11 ideas) as DYB control groups (6 ideas). On this basis, it can be

    concluded that group goal-setting techniques do indeed have the capacity toimprove brainstorming performance in groups. As performance improve-

    ments were similar for all three of the group goal-setting strategies (DGGS,

    PGGS, and PGGS IGS) examined in this study, we can draw the

    additional conclusion that this effect is not only substantial but also quite

    robust: It can be achieved in several ways. In addition, if we consider that

    performance improvements in DYB groups probably reflect group learning

    7

    Following the basic logic of Baron and Kenny (1986), there are three potential mediatingvariables that might underlie the interaction found for group performance because similar

    interactions (CA) were found for these variables (Table 3). A further test in which all three

    variables were considered did not lead to different results.

    GROUP GOAL SETTING 421

    Downlo

    adedby[McMasterUniver

    sity]at12:2811December

    2014

  • 7/25/2019 Team based interactions

    25/33

    processes, it can further be concluded that this effect is found even when

    these learning processes are controlled.

    A second objective of this study was to explore how group goal setting

    improves work motivation and performance in teams. With respect to this

    goal, several new insights were gained. Previous research has shown that

    group goal setting improves performance in teams for cognitive reasons

    (e.g., by motivating group planning and knowledge exchange; Weldon &

    Weingart, 1993). Here, we proposed that group goal setting (GGS) also

    improves group performance for motivational reasons. It was argued that

    GGS enhances work motivation in teams by preventing motivation losses

    such as social loafing and sucker effects and by promoting motivation gains

    such as social compensation and related phenomena (e.g., group identifica-

    tion). In support of this idea, we found (a) that PGGS IGS increased theexpectation that no-one is free-riding in the group (thereby reducing the

    likelihood of a sucker effect), (b) that GGS increased the value of group

    success and the value of group failure (thereby counteracting social loafing),

    (c) that GGS increased group identification, and (d) that DGGS, in

    particular, increased intrinsic task motivation and the readiness of group

    members to engage in social compensation.

    The finding that the establishment of group goals has an impact on group

    identification is quite novel (Wegge & Haslam, 2003). As group identifica-

    tion is an important variable in explaining other motivation gains (e.g.,social labouring) and motivations losses (e.g., soldiering) in teams, and

    because high group identification can also promote organizational citizen-

    ship behaviour (e.g., helping new colleagues; Haslam, Powell, & Turner,

    2000; Tyler & Blader, 2000) and other forms of positive organizational

    functioning (e.g., see Haslam et al., 2003), this observation deserves further

    attention (see van Knippenberg & Ellemers, 2003; Worchel, Rothgerber,

    Day, Hart, & Butemeyer, 1998). Although the present results suggest that

    this effect is not especially strong, group goal setting did increase group

    identificationsuggesting that this manipulation also has the potential to beuseful in situations where achieving high team identification is a meaningful

    objective in itself.

    Considering these findings together, it indeed seems promising to examine

    the possible links between goal-setting theory and social psychological

    processes pertaining to motivation losses and motivation gains in teams.

    However, it is worth noting that the reduction of motivation losses and the

    promotion of motivation gains that was observed on the basis of

    corresponding attitude measures didnot mediate the performance improve-

    ments produced by the various group goal-setting strategies. Only one

    variable (i.e., the value of group failure) emerged as a significant mediating

    variable in this respect. Thus, even though some attitudes related to

    motivation gains and losses in teams varied significantly as a function of

    422 WEGGE AND HASLAM

    Downlo

    adedby[McMasterUniver

    sity]at12:2811December

    2014

  • 7/25/2019 Team based interactions

    26/33

    condition, there may well have been other processes that were responsible

    for the performance improvements obtained in this study (e.g., investing

    more cognitive effort, differences in search of long term memory, a reduction

    of evaluation apprehension; see Paulus et al., 2002; Wegge, 2001). This is

    clearly an issue that merits investigation in future research.

    Third, this study provided an opportunity to explore potential differences

    in the efficacy of three different group goal-setting strategies. With respect to

    this issue, the findings are rather meagre as the differences between

    conditions were weak. It was not possible, for example, to show that

    striving for a combination of participatively set group goals and partici-

    patively set individual performance goals was superior to striving for group

    goals alone (PGGS or DGGS). Even though previous evidence regarding

    this issue is mixed, we expected a positive effect for PGGS IGS becausecommitment to group goals should be increased. This assumption can also

    be derived from findings in the area of volitional psychology which

    document an increase in goal commitment and goal fulfilment as a result of

    having clear, specific implementation intentions (e.g., Brandsta tter, Leng-

    felder, & Gollwitzer, 2001). Why was there no comparable effect in the

    present study? In our view, the most plausible explanation is that the rather

    simple group task used here was not one that is especially likely to lead to

    improvement in goal commitment and performance due to combining both

    types of goals. That is, group brainstorming was an additive task and therewas no specialization of labour within the group. Therefore, establishing a

    common group goal (to produce 32 ideas as a group) almost inevitably

    makes it clear which goals individual members should strive for (i.e., if the

    group has four members then every group member should produce 8 ideas).

    It might therefore be the case that the effectiveness of this group goal-setting

    strategy would be more pronounced in relation to more complex tasks (i.e.,

    those with high task interdependence or high task complexity). Nevertheless,

    we should not forget that, consistent with the findings from Crown and

    Rosse (1995), the data from the present study also indicate that acombination of both goals might establish a group-supportive performance

    orientation as expectations that other group members willnot free ride were

    only increased in this condition. Hence, this group goal-setting strategy

    could prove to be most appropriate in situations where sucker effects are a

    potential problem.

    In sum, the results of this study clearly demonstrate that goal-setting

    theory has to be extended when moving from the individual to the group

    level. Moving beyond the work of Weldon and Weingart (1993), the study

    has shown that the effects of different group goals on group performance

    rely on the promotion of motivation gains and the prevention of multiple

    motivation losses in groups. Therefore, these motivational processes should

    be considered as important mediating variables in an extended theory of

    GROUP GOAL SETTING 423

    Downlo

    adedby[McMasterUniver

    sity]at12:2811December

    2014

  • 7/25/2019 Team based interactions

    27/33

    group goal setting. However, it is also worth noting that further work

    linking goal-setting research to research traditions within social psychology

    will not only be of benefit to group goal-setting research. It can be expected

    that social psychological theories that deal with motivational phenomena in

    groups (e.g., social compensation, social labouring) can also be substantially

    advanced (see Ellemers et al., 2004, for a similar argument). In particular,

    there seems to be considerable potential for integrating the principles of the

    social identity approach (Haslam, 2004; van Knippenberg & Ellemers, 2003)

    with the tenets of goal-setting theory (Locke & Latham, 2002) in order to

    develop a more complete explanation of a range of motivational phenomena

    in groups.

    LIMITATIONS AND FUTURE DIRECTIONS

    This study has several limitations. In the first instance, caution in

    interpreting the results is warranted due to the rather low statistical power

    at the group level. However, it should be also noted that the strategy we

    developed to cope with some of the problems linked to small samples (e.g.,

    using a matched group design) was successful as several important

    preconditions (e.g., having a similar baseline performance before interven-

    tions are enacted, having constant goal difficulty across conditions) were

    achieved. Nevertheless, further replications with a larger number of groupswould of course be desirable.

    Caution in generalizing these findings is also warranted due to the fact

    that we examined ad hoc groups of students in a laboratory. While

    conducting a study in a laboratory context has some advantages (e.g., in

    allowing a fine-grained analysis of different group goal-setting procedures),

    it also has limitations. We do not know, for example, whether the behaviour

    of university students and their temporary supervisor in a short-term

    setting can be generalized to the behaviour of employees at the workplace

    where career success and promotion are at stake. However, especially in therealm of goal-setting research there is ample evidence that findings from the

    laboratory generalize to more realistic conditions (Locke, 1986; Locke &

    Latham, 2002). Primarily, this is because this generalization is based on

    soundtheorythat is validated in an array of contexts (see Haslam & Reicher,

    in press, for a similar argument). Thus, it seems reasonable to assume that

    corresponding results to those obtained here could also be obtained in

    organizational teams. Nevertheless, as laboratory groups are necessarily

    decontextualized relative to applied settings, and because these groups

    neither have a common history nor a common future, replication studies in

    field settings certainly seem warranted.

    Moreover, we should be also aware of the fact that potentially influential

    factors were not measured (e.g., perceptions regarding indispensability of

    424 WEGGE AND HASLAM

    Downlo

    adedby[McMasterUniver

    sity]at12:2811December

    2014

  • 7/25/2019 Team based interactions

    28/33

    own efforts, evaluation apprehension) and not manipulated in this study.

    Even though the limited evidence we have so far indicates that advantages of

    group goal setting are probably task-specific (Wegge & Kleinbeck, 1996;

    Weldon & Weingart, 1993), task demands were also not systematically

    varied in this study. Future research should address this lacuna by

    examining other group tasks and the impact of task interdependence during

    teamwork. Of course, it also remains to be seen how effective group

    brainstorming might become if the practice of setting challenging group

    goals were combined with other interventions (e.g., use of a trained

    facilitator, brain-writing techniques, or procedures that ask group members

    to build on the ideas of other group members in a round-robin manner).

    Moreover, in this study we focused only on thequantity of ideas (checking

    and correcting for very similar ideas), not their quality. Because priorresearch has found that there is often a strong association between the

    number of ideas and the flexibility and originality of ideas (Thompson, 2003,

    p. 98), we did not attempt a more detailed analysis of our data. However, a

    more sophisticated analysis in terms of originality or flexibility of ideas

    produced by group members could potentially yield additional insights (e.g.,

    see Adarves-Yorno, Postmes, & Haslam, in press). Moreover, this might be

    another avenue for further research as such an analysis might help to

    uncover the cognitive processes that are involved in producing group goal-

    setting effects in brainstorming groups.Two further limitations of this study are linked to the procedures we used

    for group brainstorming. First, the available time for finding ideas was

    rather short (3 minutes). In brainstorming tasks, ideas are generated with

    low effort and difficulty in particular at the beginning of the process (Oxley

    et al., 1996; Paulus et al., 2002). Thus, the impact of motivational forces is

    probably much easier to demonstrate over a longer time period. Future

    studies, therefore, should examine whether group goal setting is even more

    effective over longer time periods (e.g., 15 minutes). That said, it was notable

    that group goal setting significantly improved performance even under therestricted time conditions selected in this studyso in many ways this was a

    stringent test of our arguments.

    Second, social loafing effects occur mainly in situations where group

    members contributions are not identifiable and where performance feed-

    back is not available (Karau & Williams, 1993; Paulus et al., 2002). As

    group members were asked to read their ideas out loud to the rest of the

    group after each trial, group members performance was identifiable and

    evaluated by other group members and the experimenter. Thus, social

    loafing would not necessarily be expected to have occurred in this context

    and, accordingly, it would be interesting to investigate whether group goal-

    setting effects are stronger in conditions where social loafing is more likely

    (e.g., when ideas generation is anonymous; see Erez & Somech, 1996).

    GROUP GOAL SETTING 425

    Downlo

    adedby[McMasterUniver

    sity]at12:2811December

    2014

  • 7/25/2019 Team based interactions

    29/33

    Again, though, when we consider that social loafing was unlikely to occur in

    this experiment and that DYB instructions also motivated groups to

    increase their brainstorming performance, the demonstration of a group

    goal-setting effect in comparison to DYB groups is nontrivial and all the

    more impressive.

    Finally, it should be also emphasized that improving group performance

    in organizations necessarily involves much more than just using the most

    appropriate leadership (goal-setting) technique. To make the use of group

    work in organizations a real success, we also have to take into consideration

    a range of other factors. These include, for example, effective group task

    design (Ulich & Weber, 1996; van Vijfeijken, Kleingeld, van Tuijl, Algera, &

    Thierry, 2002), relevant group feedback (Deshon et al., 2004; Kluger &

    DeNisi, 1996; Schmidt & Kleinbeck, 1997), group training and groupdevelopment (Kozlowski, Gully, Nasson, & Smith, 1999) and appropriate

    group compensation (DeMatteo, Eby, & Sundstrom, 1998; Hertel et al.,

    2004). Nonetheless, the preliminary signs are that, added to these factors,

    group goal-setting techniques can be a very effective tool in the

    organizational psychologists armoury. On this basis, we encourage

    researchers as well as practitioners to use these strategies in order to

    improve brainstorming and other related forms of performance in teams.

    REFERENCES

    Adarves-Yorno, I., Postmes, T., & Haslam, S. A. (in press). Social identity and the recognition

    of creativity in groups. British Journal of Social Psychology.

    Baron, R. M., & Kenny, D. A. (1986). The moderator mediator variable distinction in social

    psychological research: Conceptual, strategic, and statistical considerations. Journal of

    Personality and Social Psychology, 51, 1173 1182.

    Bond, C. F., & Titus, L. J. (1983). Social facilitation: A meta-analysis of 241 studies.

    Psychological Bulletin, 94, 265 292.

    Brandsta tter, V., Lengfelder, A., & Gollwitzer, P. M. (2001). Implementation intentions and

    efficient action initiation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 81, 946 960.

    Cohen, S. G., & Bailey, D. E. (1997). What makes teams work: Group effectiveness research

    from the shop floor to the executive site. Journal of Management, 23, 239 290.

    Cortina, J. M. (1993). What is coefficient alpha: An examination of theory and applications.

    Journal of Applied Psychology, 78, 98104.

    Crown, D. F., & Rosse, J. G. (1995). Yours, mine and ours: Facilitating group productivity

    through the integration of individual and group goals. Organizational Behavior and Human

    Decision Processes, 64, 138 150.

    DeMatteo, J. S., Eby, L. T., & Sundstrom, E. (1998). Team-based rewards: Current

    empirical evidence and directions for future research. Research in Organizational Behavior,

    20, 141 183.

    DeShon, R. P., Kozlowski, S. W. J., Schmidt, A. M., Milner, K. R., & Wiechmann, D. (2004).

    A multiple-goal, multilevel model of feedback effects on the regulation of individual and

    team performance. Journal of Applied Psychology, 89, 1035 1056.

    426 WEGGE AND HASLAM

    Downlo

    adedby[McMasterUniver

    sity]at12:2811December

    2014

  • 7/25/2019 Team based interactions

    30/33

    Durham, C. C., Locke, E. A., Poon, J. M. L., & McLeod, P. L. (2000). Effects of group goals

    and time pressure on group efficacy, information seeking, strategy and performance. Human

    Performance, 13, 115 138.

    Ellemers, N., de Gilder, D., & Haslam, S. A. (2004). Motivating individuals and groups at work:A social identity perspective on leadership and group performance. Academy of Manage-

    ment Review, 29, 459 478.

    Erez, M. (1995). Prospects of participative management in developing countries: The role of

    socio-cultural environment. In D. M. Saunders & R. N. Kanungo (Eds.), New approaches to

    employee management (Vol. 3, pp. 171 196). Greenwich, CT: JAI Press.

    Erez, M., & Somech, A. (1996). Is group productivity loss the rule or the exception? Effects of

    culture and group based motivation. Academy of Management Journal, 39, 1513 1537.

    Fried, Y., & Ferris, G. R. (1987). The validity of the job characteristics model: A review and

    meta-analysis.Personnel Psychology, 40, 287 322.

    George, J. M., & James, L. R. (1993). Personality, affect, and behavior in groups revisited:

    Comment on aggregation, levels of analysis and a recent application of within and betweenanalysis. Journal of Applied Psychology, 78, 798 804.

    Harackiewicz, J. M., Barron, K. E., Tauer, J. M., Carter, S. M., & Elliot, A. (2000). Short-term

    and long-term consequences of achievement goals: Predicting interest and performance over

    time. Journal of Educational Psychology, 92, 316 330.

    Haslam, S. A. (2004). Psychology in organizations: The social identity approach (2nd ed.).

    London: Sage.

    Haslam, S. A., Eggins, R. A., & Reynolds, K. J. (2003). The ASPIRe model: Actualizing social

    and personal identity resources to enhance organizational outcomes. Journal of Occupa-

    tional and Organizational Psychology, 76, 83113.

    Haslam, S. A., & Ellemers, N. (2005). Social identity in industrial and organizational

    psychology: Concepts, controversies and contributions. In G. P. Hodgkinson & K. Ford(Eds.),International review of industrial and organizational psychology (Vol. 20, pp. 39 118).

    Chichester, UK: Wiley.

    Haslam, S. A., Powell, C., & Turner, J. C. (2000). Social identity, self-categorization and work

    motivation: Rethinking the contribution of the group to positive and sustainable

    organizational outcomes. Applied Psychology: An International Review, 49, 319 339.

    Haslam, S. A., & Reicher, S. D. (in press). Stressing the group: Social identity and the unfolding

    dynamics of responses to stress. Journal of Applied Psychology.

    Haslam, S. A., Ryan, M. K., Postmes, T., Spears, R., Jetten, J., & Webley, P. (in press). Sticking

    to our guns: Social identity as a basis for the maintenance of commitment to faltering

    organizational projects. Journal of Organizational Behavior.

    Hertel, G. (2000). Motivation gains in groups: A brief review of the state of the art. Zeitschrift

    fur Sozialpsychologie, 31, 169 175.

    Hertel, G., Konradt, U., & Orlikowski, B. (2004). Managing distance by interdependence: Goal

    setting, task interdependence and team-based rewards in virtual teams. European Journal of

    Work and Organizational Psychology, 13, 128.

    Higgins, E. T. (1997). Beyond pleasure and pain. American Psychologist, 52, 1280 1300.

    Hinsz, V. B. (1995). Goal setting by groups performing an additive task: A comparison with

    individual goal setting. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 25, 965990.

    James, L. R., Demaree, R. G., & Wolf, G. (1984). Estimating within-group interrater reliability

    with and without response bias. Journal of Applied Psychology, 69, 8598.

    James, L. R., Demaree, R. G., & Wolf, G. (1993). Rwg: An assessment of within-group

    interrater agreement. Journal of Applied Psychology, 78, 306 309.

    Karau, S. J., & Hart, J. W. (1998). Group cohesiveness and social loafing: Effects of a social

    interaction manipulation on individual motivation within groups.Group Dynamics: Theory,

    Research and Practice, 2, 185 191.

    GROUP GOAL SETTING 427

    Downlo

    adedby[McMasterUniver

    sity]at12:2811December

    2014

  • 7/25/2019 Team based interactions

    31/33

    Karau, S. J., & Williams, K. D. (1993). Social loafing: A meta-analytic review and theoretical

    integration. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 65, 681 706.

    Karau, S. J., & Williams, K. D. (1997). The effects of group cohesiveness on social loafing and

    social compensation. Group Dynamics: Theory, Research and Practice, 1, 156 168.Kerr, N. L. (1983). Motivation losses in small groups: A social dilemma analysis. Journal of

    Personality and Social Psychology, 45, 819 828.

    Kerr, N. L., & Bruun, S. E. (1983). Dispensability of members effort and group motivation

    losses: Free-rider effects. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 44, 7894.

    Kerr, N. L., & Tindale, R. S. (2004). Group performance and decision making. Annual Review

    of