5
A Review of 2 Journal Articles on Psychological Factors Influencing Capital Punishment Attitudes By Oleg Nekrassovski Capital punishment is a hot issue in the U.S., whereas in, far less criminal, Canada, it sometimes causes private debates because majority of Canadians supposedly do not believe in sentencing anyone, no matter how evil, to death. Since there are many potential psychological factors which may be responsible for individual views on the subject, I decided to investigate the recent psychological literature regarding such factors, and found two interesting articles which are summarized below. Bloechl et al.’s (2006) study involved a search for correlations between individual attitudes toward the insanity defense and the previously identified factors influencing such attitudes, such as individual demographic characteristics and individual attitudes toward death penalty (Bloechl et al., 2006, p. 155). A sample of 578 undergraduate students (395 females and 183 males) majoring in a variety of subjects and being eligible for jury duty, with a mean age of 19.90 years, constituted the participants of the study. 91.1% of the participants were Euro-Americans. 198 described themselves as Democrats, 193 as Republicans, 159 as non-affiliated, and 28 as “other” (Bloechl et al., 2006, p. 155). The participants of the study completed two tests with self-explanatory names, the Insanity Defense Attitude Scale Revised (IDAS-R) and the Attitudes Toward the Death Penalty Scale (ATDP), as well as the demographic questionnaire. The resulting IDAS-R scores indicated to the authors that the participants, on average, did not support the idea of defense on the grounds of insanity. On the other hand, the scores on the ATDP test indicated to the authors that the participants varied significantly in their attitudes toward capital punishment. The results of the demographic questionnaire presented each participant’s gender, ethnicity, political affiliation, and religious beliefs. While the answers to other, relevant, questions of the questionnaire showed that the participants greatly overestimated the extent of the use of insanity defense and its success rate (Bloechl et al., 2006, pp. 155-156). The influence of individual demographic characteristics and views on capital punishment, on insanity defense attitudes, as revealed by the IDAS-R, was analyzed with exploratory statistics (Bloechl et al., 2006, p. 156). One statistical analysis used in the study demonstrated that the participant’ s gender was clearly an unreliable predictor of his/her attitude towards the insanity defense. The participant’s ethnicity turned out to be equally unreliable (Bloechl et al., 2006, p. 156). Further statistical analysis of the data overturned Tygart’s (1982, 1992) hypothesis by demonstrating that the religious beliefs of the participants were not a reliable indicator of their

Review of 2 Journal Articles on Psychological Factors Influencing Capital Punishment Attitudes

Embed Size (px)

Citation preview

Page 1: Review of 2 Journal Articles on Psychological Factors Influencing Capital Punishment Attitudes

A Review of 2 Journal Articles on Psychological Factors Influencing Capital

Punishment Attitudes

By Oleg Nekrassovski

Capital punishment is a hot issue in the U.S., whereas in, far less criminal, Canada, it

sometimes causes private debates because majority of Canadians supposedly do not believe in

sentencing anyone, no matter how evil, to death. Since there are many potential psychological

factors which may be responsible for individual views on the subject, I decided to investigate

the recent psychological literature regarding such factors, and found two interesting articles

which are summarized below.

Bloechl et al.’s (2006) study involved a search for correlations between individual

attitudes toward the insanity defense and the previously identified factors influencing such

attitudes, such as individual demographic characteristics and individual attitudes toward death

penalty (Bloechl et al., 2006, p. 155).

A sample of 578 undergraduate students (395 females and 183 males) majoring in a

variety of subjects and being eligible for jury duty, with a mean age of 19.90 years, constituted

the participants of the study. 91.1% of the participants were Euro-Americans. 198 described

themselves as Democrats, 193 as Republicans, 159 as non-affiliated, and 28 as “other” (Bloechl

et al., 2006, p. 155).

The participants of the study completed two tests with self-explanatory names, the

Insanity Defense Attitude Scale – Revised (IDAS-R) and the Attitudes Toward the Death Penalty

Scale (ATDP), as well as the demographic questionnaire. The resulting IDAS-R scores indicated

to the authors that the participants, on average, did not support the idea of defense on the

grounds of insanity. On the other hand, the scores on the ATDP test indicated to the authors

that the participants varied significantly in their attitudes toward capital punishment. The

results of the demographic questionnaire presented each participant’s gender, ethnicity,

political affiliation, and religious beliefs. While the answers to other, relevant, questions of the

questionnaire showed that the participants greatly overestimated the extent of the use of

insanity defense and its success rate (Bloechl et al., 2006, pp. 155-156).

The influence of individual demographic characteristics and views on capital

punishment, on insanity defense attitudes, as revealed by the IDAS-R, was analyzed with

exploratory statistics (Bloechl et al., 2006, p. 156).

One statistical analysis used in the study demonstrated that the participant’s gender

was clearly an unreliable predictor of his/her attitude towards the insanity defense. The

participant’s ethnicity turned out to be equally unreliable (Bloechl et al., 2006, p. 156).

Further statistical analysis of the data overturned Tygart’s (1982, 1992) hypothesis by

demonstrating that the religious beliefs of the participants were not a reliable indicator of their

Page 2: Review of 2 Journal Articles on Psychological Factors Influencing Capital Punishment Attitudes

attitudes toward the insanity defense. Another of Tygart’s (1982, 1992) hypotheses, namely

that political affiliation is a reliable indicator of the insanity defense attitudes, was well

supported by the statistical analysis of the data in this study. Also, the analysis of data revealed

that opposition to the insanity defense strongly correlates with the overestimation of the

extent to which it is used, as well as its success rate (Bloechl et al., 2006, p. 157).

The final statistical analysis of the study analyzed the relationship between the scores

on ATDP and IDAS-R, demonstrating that there was a significant correlation between the

participants’ views on capital punishment and on defense on the grounds of insanity. In

particular, it was revealed that those participants who were the strongest supporters of capital

punishment were most strongly opposed to allowing the use of mental illness diagnosis as a

legal defense (Bloechl et al., 2006, pp. 157-158).

In spite of the seeming success of the study, the authors readily pointed out its many

limitations. The generally young age of the participants could potentially mean that the results

of the study are unique to this age group. Also, the ethnic diversity of the participants was very

different from the general American population, which may as a result have different attitudes

on the subject. Finally, this research was conducted in a non-death penalty state, a variable

which may have significantly biased the views of the participants and made the results

unrepresentative of the general American population (Bloechl et al., 2006, p. 159).

The focus of Edens et al.’s (2005) study was on testing whether (1) labeling the

defendant, by an expert witness, in a capital murder case as being psychopathic or psychotic

made the potential jurors more likely to rank him as possessing greater future danger to society

and sentence him to death, than when the same defendant was judged to be free of any

mental illness; (2) the extent of influence of expert testimony, regarding the defendant’s

mental health, on those potential jurors who already consider the defendant to be a future

danger to society, determined the extent of their support for a death sentence; (3) the extent

to which the jurors perceive the defendant in a capital murder case as being psychopathic or

psychotic, before hearing expert testimony on the subject, determined the extent of their

support for a death sentence; (4) labeling the defendant, by an expert witness, as being at a low

as compared to a high risk to engage in future violence caused a significant decrease in the

amount of future danger attributed to him by the potential jurors; (5) the more future danger is

attributed, for whatever reason, to the defendant by a potential juror, the more it is likely that

he will be sentenced to death by that same juror (Edens et al., 2005, pp. 608-609).

231 American university students taking psychology courses, constituted the

participants of the study. The mean age of the participants was 20.0 years. 64.5 % of them were

female. 93.5% were unmarried. 71.4% were Caucasian; 14.3% African American; and 10.8%

Hispanic (Edens et al., 2005, pp. 609-610).

The participants of the study were presented with a list of key facts about an actual

capital murder case, which concluded with the statement that the defendant was found guilty.

Page 3: Review of 2 Journal Articles on Psychological Factors Influencing Capital Punishment Attitudes

Next, the participants were randomly split into six groups, and presented with six different

testimonies of psychological experts regarding the defendant’s psychiatric condition. The

expert testimonies presented the defendant to the participants, depending on the group to

which they were assigned, as being either mentally healthy, psychotic, or psychopathic and

either at high or at low risk to engage in violence in the future (Edens et al., 2005, pp. 610, 612-

613).

The next step in the study asked the participants to complete a questionnaire, which

was taken from the work of Edens et al. (2004) and was aimed at identifying those participants

whose views of capital punishment or the defendant were too biased to allow them to give him

a fair sentence. As a result, all assessments of twenty five, out of the original 231 participants,

were excluded from the final data used for analysis. This group of twenty five consisted of

twenty who indicated their inability to impose a death sentence on anyone; three, who

indicated that they would sentence to death every convicted capital offender; and two, who

believed that the defendant is not guilty, even though the summary of the case, that was

presented to them, clearly stated that the defendant has confessed to the two homicides he

was accused of and that several witnesses identified him as the perpetrator of the two crimes

(Edens et al., 2005, pp. 610, 611, 613).

After the evaluation of participants’ views on death penalty and of the defendant, the

participants completed a brief questionnaire, which assessed their desire to either sentence the

defendant to death, because in their view he is clearly a future threat to society, or to life

imprisonment, because there is little reason to believe that he is prone to future acts of

violence. Also, a four-item scale from a previous study (Edens et al., 2004) was used to examine

the participants’ perception of defendant’s future dangerousness, based on the expert

testimony provided to them. Moreover, in order to assess whether the participants might have

sentenced the defendant in the way they did out of their own perceptions of his psychopathic

or psychotic inclinations, irrespective of the testimony provided, the participants were asked to

decide whether in their view the defendant was likely to exhibit the twenty specific traits from

the Hare Psychopathy Checklist – Revised (Hare, 1991, 2003) and five specific traits of

schizophrenics (Edens et al., 2005, pp. 611-612).

Statistical analysis of the collected data revealed that, on average, those participants

that were in the psychopathic- or psychotic-defendant-groups, consistently ranked him as

possessing greater future danger to society, than those participants who were in the mental-

illness-free-defendant-groups (Edens et al., 2005, p. 613). However, 60% of the participants in

the psychopathic-defendant-groups and only 30% of those in the psychotic-defendant-groups

voted for a death penalty; while among those who were in the mental-illness-free-defendant-

groups, 38% sentenced him to death (Edens et al., 2005, p. 614). On the other hand, among

those participants who already ranked the defendant as possessing considerable future danger

to society, 69% of those asked to judge a psychopathic defendant, sentenced him to death; as

Page 4: Review of 2 Journal Articles on Psychological Factors Influencing Capital Punishment Attitudes

did 57% of those dealing with a non mentally disordered defendant; and 38% of those who

dealt with a psychotic defendant (Edens et al., 2005, pp. 615-616). Also, the greater was the

extent to which the participants perceived the defendant as being psychopathic, before

considering expert testimony on the subject, the greater was the likelihood that they later

sentenced him to death (Edens et al., 2005, pp. 616, 618). It was also revealed that labeling the

defendant, through expert testimony, as being at a low as compared to a high risk to engage in

future violence caused only a slight decrease in the amount of future danger attributed to him

by the participants (Edens et al., 2005, pp. 613-614). However, the more future danger was

attributed, for whatever reason, to the defendant by a participant, the more likely he was to be

sentenced to death by that same participant (Edens et al., 2005, p. 615).

In spite of the informative nature of the results, the authors acknowledged several

limitations of their study. They pointed out that in this study they did not consider many other

factors (such as the evidence regarding defendant’s future dangerousness, the type of

information used in assessing the defendant’s mental health, or cross-examination testimony)

that could influence the decisions of the jurors exposed to the expert testimony on the

defendant’s mental health. Also, the authors pointed out that unlike real jury members, the

participants of the study were not asked to deliberate about their sentencing decisions; hence

the results of this research cover only individual decisions of would-be jurors rather than the

combined, final, decisions of whole juries (Edens et al., 2005, pp. 619-620).

The above two studies are consistent with my initial view that there are many

psychological factors which do or may influence individual attitudes toward capital punishment.

Also, the above two studies made it clear to me that these factors can only be uncovered

through controlled experiments and rigorous statistical analysis of experimental data, rather

than through conversations of opportunity.

Page 5: Review of 2 Journal Articles on Psychological Factors Influencing Capital Punishment Attitudes

References

Bloechl, A. L., Vitacco, M. J., Neumann, C. S., & Erickson, S. E. (2006). An empirical investigation

of insanity defense attitudes: exploring factors related to bias. International Journal of

Law and Psychiatry, 30, 153-161.

Edens, J.F., Colwell, L. H., Desforges, D. M., & Fernandez, K. (2005). The impact of mental health

evidence on support for capital punishment: Are defendants labeled psychopathic

considered more deserving of death? Behavioral Sciences and the Law, 23, 603-625.

Edens, J.F., Desforges, D. M., Fernandez, K. & Palac, C. A. (2004). Effects of psychopathy and

violence risk testimony on mock juror perceptions of dangerousness in a capital murder

trial. Psychology, Crime and Law, 393-412.

Hare, R. D. (1991). The Hare Psychopathy Checklist – Revised manual. Toronto: Multi-Health

Systems.

Hare, R. D. (2003). The Hare Psychopathy Checklist – Revised technical manual (2nd ed.).

Toronto: Multi-Health Systems.

Tygart, C. E. (1982). Effects of religiosity on public opinion about legal responsibility for mentally

retarded felons. American Journal of Mental Deficiency, 86, 457-464.

Tygart, C. E. (1992). Public acceptance/rejection of insanity mental illness legal defenses for

defendants in criminal homicide cases. Journal of Psychiatry and Law, 20, 375-389.