17
Report Number 12/02 How linear features alter predator movement and the functional response by Hannah W. McKenzie1, Evelyn H. Merrill, Raymond J. Spiteri and Mark A. Lewis Oxford Centre for Collaborative Applied Mathematics Mathematical Institute 24 - 29 St Giles’ Oxford OX1 3LB England

ReportNumber12/02 · 2012-12-11 · ReportNumber12/02 Howlinear features alter predatormovementandthe functional response by Hannah W. McKenzie1, EvelynH. Merrill, RaymondJ. Spiteri

  • Upload
    others

  • View
    3

  • Download
    0

Embed Size (px)

Citation preview

Page 1: ReportNumber12/02 · 2012-12-11 · ReportNumber12/02 Howlinear features alter predatormovementandthe functional response by Hannah W. McKenzie1, EvelynH. Merrill, RaymondJ. Spiteri

Report Number 12/02

How linear features alter predator movement and the functional

response

by

Hannah W. McKenzie1, Evelyn H. Merrill, Raymond J. Spiteri

and Mark A. Lewis

Oxford Centre for Collaborative Applied Mathematics

Mathematical Institute

24 - 29 St Giles’

Oxford

OX1 3LB

England

Page 2: ReportNumber12/02 · 2012-12-11 · ReportNumber12/02 Howlinear features alter predatormovementandthe functional response by Hannah W. McKenzie1, EvelynH. Merrill, RaymondJ. Spiteri
Page 3: ReportNumber12/02 · 2012-12-11 · ReportNumber12/02 Howlinear features alter predatormovementandthe functional response by Hannah W. McKenzie1, EvelynH. Merrill, RaymondJ. Spiteri

How linear features alter predatormovement and the functional response

Hannah W. McKenzie1, Evelyn H. Merrill2, Raymond J. Spiteri3

and Mark A. Lewis1,2,*

1Centre for Mathematical Biology, Department of Mathematical and Statistical Sciences,632 CAB, University of Alberta, Edmonton, Alberta, Canada T6G 2G1

2Department of Biological Sciences, CW 405, Biological Sciences Building, Universityof Alberta, Edmonton, Alberta, Canada T6G 2E9

3Department of Computer Science, 176 Thorvaldson Building, University of Saskatchewan,110 Science Place, Saskatoon, Saskatchewan, Canada S7N 5C9

In areas of oil and gas exploration, seismic lines have been reported to alter the movement pat-terns of wolves (Canis lupus). We developed a mechanistic first passage time model, based onan anisotropic elliptic partial differential equation, and used this to explore how wolf movementresponses to seismic lines influence the encounter rate of the wolves with their prey. The modelwas parametrized using 5 min GPS location data. These data showed that wolves travelledfaster on seismic lines and had a higher probability of staying on a seismic line once theywere on it. We simulated wolf movement on a range of seismic line densities and drew impli-cations for the rate of predator–prey interactions as described by the functional response.The functional response exhibited a more than linear increase with respect to prey density(type III) as well as interactions with seismic line density. Encounter rates were significantlyhigher in landscapes with high seismic line density and were most pronounced at low prey den-sities. This suggests that prey at low population densities are at higher risk in environmentswith a high seismic line density unless they learn to avoid them.

Keywords: encounter rate; mean first passage time; seismic lines;spatial heterogeneity; wolf movement

1. INTRODUCTION

One of the most common functional response models isthe Holling disc equation [1]. In the disc equation, theencounter rate is assumed to be linearly related toprey density by the ‘instantaneous search rate’ [2] or‘area of discovery’ [1]. This leads to the type I functionalresponse, or the type II functional response once hand-lingQ1 time is included. Previous theoretical work suggeststhat this assumption of linearity may not hold if pred-ator movement is partially random, i.e. the new areasearched by the predator per unit time is not constant[3]. The results suggest that, in one dimension andunder certain assumptions regarding prey availability,the encounter rate of predators undergoing a randomwalk has a quadratic relationship with prey density.This quadratic relationship arises because during arandom walk, predators may repeatedly return toregions previously searched. When substituted intothe disc equation, McKenzie et al. [3] showed that aquadratic encounter rate leads to a type III functional

response, thereby demonstrating a link between preda-tor movement modes and the shape of the functionalresponse. McKenzie et al. [3] worked in a simplifiedtheoretical framework, but it is known that predatorssearching for prey in spatially heterogeneous habitatshave both directed and random components to theirmovement. In this case, the encounter rate is expectedto be a more complex function of prey density, perhapsinvolving both linear and quadratic terms, and there-fore possibly leading to some combination of thefamiliar type II and type III functional responses.

Owing to the challenges of reproducing the theoreti-cal arguments used by McKenzie et al. [3] underassumptions of more realistic predator movement inheterogeneous landscapes, here we take a differentapproach to investigate the link between predatormovement and the functional response. We first derivemore realistic predator movement models based onpredator movement data. Then, using these movementmodels, we simulate prey encounter rates over a range ofprey densities and fit the simulated encounter rate datato the encounter rate models. Finally, we substitute theencounter rate function into the disc equation to deter-mine the shape of the resulting functional response.

For the source of spatial heterogeneity, we focus onseismic lines, which are narrow, linear stretches of

*Author for correspondence ([email protected]).

Electronic supplementary material is available at http://dx.doi.org/10.1098/rsfs.2011.0086 or via http://rsfs.royalsocietypublishing.org.

One contribution to a Theme Issue ‘Mathematical and theroreticalecology’.

Interface Focus (2012) 00, 1–12

doi:10.1098/rsfs.2011.0086

Published online 00 Month 0000

Received 30 September 2011Accepted 19 December 2011 1 This journal is q 2012 The Royal Society

1

2

3

4

5

6

7

8

9

10

11

12

13

14

15

16

17

18

19

20

21

22

23

24

25

26

27

28

29

30

31

32

33

34

35

36

37

38

39

40

41

42

43

44

45

46

47

48

49

50

51

52

53

54

55

56

57

58

59

60

61

62

63

64

65

66

67

68

69

70

71

72

73

74

75

76

77

78

79

80

81

82

83

84

85

86

87

88

89

90

91

92

93

94

95

96

97

98

99

100

101

102

103

104

105

106

107

108

109

110

111

112

113

114

115

116

117

118

119

120

121

122

123

124

125

126

ARTICLE IN PRESS

rsfs20110086—23/12/11—21:27–Copy Edited by: Mahalakshmi S.

Page 4: ReportNumber12/02 · 2012-12-11 · ReportNumber12/02 Howlinear features alter predatormovementandthe functional response by Hannah W. McKenzie1, EvelynH. Merrill, RaymondJ. Spiteri

forest, cleared for energy exploration ([4] and electronicsupplementary material, appendix figure S1a). Wolves(Canis lupus) have been found to both avoid and uselinear features, depending on the linear feature densityand level of human use [5–8]. Because studies haveshown that wolves moved up to 2.8 times faster onlinear features than in the forest [6], it has been hypoth-esized that seismic lines may benefit wolves if increasedtravel rates result in higher encounter rates with prey[9–11]. Consequently, in landscapes with high densi-ties of seismic lines, species such as caribou (Rangifertarandus) and elk (Cervus elaphus) may be at higherpredation risk owing to higher encounter rates [12–14].

In this paper, we use this example of wolf movementin response to seismic lines to demonstrate the linkbetween animal response to spatial heterogeneity andfunctional response models. We follow the mean firstpassage time approach described McKenzie et al. [3]extended to two dimensions. In the context of preda-tion, we define the mean first passage time as theaverage time required for a moving predator to locatea first stationary prey, given a specific prey density[3]. Therefore, the inverse of mean first passage timeis the encounter rate at that prey density. Using this fra-mework, it is possible to formulate first passage timemodels that reflect the effect of landscape features onpredator movement. After evaluating the influence ofdifferent movement responses of wolves on seismiclines, we use model outputs to assess a set of a priorimodels relating encounter rate to seismic line andprey density, both independently and together. Thefunctional forms of the candidate models are chosento reflect different underlying predator movementmechanisms. Using the best-fit model, we investigatethe implications for wolf functional responses owing toincreased predator mobility in the presence of seismiclines, and discuss how these ideas could be extendedto include other sources of landscape heterogeneity.

2. METHODS

2.1. Modelling

Encounter rate is the rate at which predators encounterprey in the landscape (unit: time21). Here, we considera mechanistic model for encounter rate that includes theeffect of predator movement. We modelled encounterrate using mean first passage time. Mean first passagetime, T(x) (unit: time), is the average time requiredfor a predator starting at location x to encounter anynumber of stationary prey, given a specified prey distri-bution and landscape. Although it would be possible toapproximate T(x) from averaging repeated randomwalk simulations from each point x of interest, analternative, computationally efficient approach uses apartial differential equation to describe the surfaceT(x). This approach is based on the Fokker–Planckapproximation for animal movement patterns that isdescribed in depth by Turchin [15] and in particularfor this model by McKenzie et al. [3]. To derive a partialdifferential equation for T, McKenzie et al. [3] begin byencoding aspects of wolf movement believed to beimportant in determining space use, in this case

focusing on wolf response to seismic lines, into a set ofprobabilistic movement rules for individual wolves.These are then translated, using mathematical approxi-mations, into the following partial differential equationfor T.

dxxðxÞ@2T

@x2þ 2dxyðxÞ

@2T

@x@yþ dyyðxÞ

@2T

@y2

þ cðxÞ � rT þ 1 ¼ 0; x [ V;

ð2:1Þ

where the domain V is the landscape of interest. In thismathematical formulation, as is the case in the Hollingdisc equation, the prey are assumed to be stationary andare represented by interior Dirichlet (absorbing disc)boundary conditions. This means that if the predatorstarts within perception radius r of a prey item, thetime required to locate a prey item, T, equals zero.Defining prey in this way allows us to study the behav-iour of the first passage time model for any finitenumber of prey, with spatial locations of our choosing.The particular prey scenarios we considered are furtherdescribed in the §2.3. The coefficients of the partialderivatives are derived mechanistically based on theunderlying predator movement behaviour (electronicsupplementary material, appendix). Equation (2.1)includes diffusive movement in the first three terms(dxx , dxy and dyy), and advective movement in thefourth term (c), which together approximate animalmovement. The dependence of the diffusion andadvection terms on the location x indicates that move-ment terms can vary from one location to other.Directionality in the movement terms can arise eitherfrom the advection term (c), which indicates a direc-tional bias in the movement, or from anisotropicdiffusion (unequal values for dxx , dxy and dyy), whichindicates different levels of random movement in differ-ent directions. For example, in the absence of otherinfluences, far from the landscape features predatorsmay move in a random fashion. However, their move-ment may become more directed as they interact withlandscape features.

The solution to equation (2.1) is a two-dimensionalsurface T(x), where the value of the surface at eachpoint in space is the mean first passage time of a predatorlocated at x. Therefore, the value of the surface at eachpoint in space indicates how long, on average, a predatorstarting x would need to search before locating a preyitem. This surface provides a picture of how the meanfirst passage time varies in space, and can be summarizedby the spatially averaged mean first passage time,

�T ¼

ðV

1

ATðxÞ dx; ð2:2Þ

where A is the area of V. The average mean first passagetime is the mean first passage time assuming that thepredator initially is randomly distributed in the land-scape. For a given landscape and prey density, theaverage mean first passage time can then be related tothe encounter rate by

E ¼1�T :

ð2:3Þ

2 Linear features and predator movement H. W. McKenzie et al.

Interface Focus (2012)

127

128

129

130

131

132

133

134

135

136

137

138

139

140

141

142

143

144

145

146

147

148

149

150

151

152

153

154

155

156

157

158

159

160

161

162

163

164

165

166

167

168

169

170

171

172

173

174

175

176

177

178

179

180

181

182

183

184

185

186

187

188

189

190

191

192

193

194

195

196

197

198

199

200

201

202

203

204

205

206

207

208

209

210

211

212

213

214

215

216

217

218

219

220

221

222

223

224

225

226

227

228

229

230

231

232

233

234

235

236

237

238

239

240

241

242

243

244

245

246

247

248

249

250

251

252

ARTICLE IN PRESS

rsfs20110086—23/12/11—21:27–Copy Edited by: Mahalakshmi S.

Page 5: ReportNumber12/02 · 2012-12-11 · ReportNumber12/02 Howlinear features alter predatormovementandthe functional response by Hannah W. McKenzie1, EvelynH. Merrill, RaymondJ. Spiteri

It would be possible to account for a non-random initialdistribution of the predator by replacing the uniformweight 1/A in equation (2.2) with a more general prob-ability density function for the initial location of thepredator u(x). For example, the distribution u(x) couldbe a statistical home range model, such as that ofKernohan et al. [16], or a mechanistic home rangemodel, such as that of Moorcroft & Lewis [17].

Based on previous studies of wolf movement in land-scapes with linear features [6,7], wolves are likely toshow varied responses to these features. In the simplestmodel (no response), wolves do not alter their move-ment in response to seismic lines. This model isanalogous to wolf movement in a landscape withoutseismic lines and corresponds to a random walk every-where in the landscape. Although this model may bebiologically unreasonable [5,8], it provides a baselinefor comparison in understanding the effect of increasingprey density on encounter rate in the absence of seismiclines. In the second model (anisotropic diffusion),wolves move faster on seismic lines and are morelikely to continue along them in either direction onceon them. Mechanistically, we considered the anisotropyas arising from higher movement speed in either direc-tion along the seismic lines as well as possiblecorrelations in the random walk when moving alongthe seismic lines. Both these mechanisms individuallywould lead to an enhanced diffusion coefficient [18]but only in the direction of the seismic line. This wasincorporated mathematically by allowing anisotropicdiffusion on seismic lines, where diffusion was increasedalong the seismic line and decreased across it. This

model corresponds to a random walk away from seismiclines and a random walk with anisotropic diffusion onseismic lines. The final model (anisotropic diffusion þbias) was an extension of the previous model where,in addition to being more likely to continue along seis-mic lines when on them, wolves also biased theirmovement towards the seismic line when near them.This model corresponds to a random walk far from seis-mic lines, a biased random walk near seismic lines, anda random walk with anisotropic diffusion on seismiclines. In addition to the terms previously discussed,the equation for this model also includes an advectionterm in regions near seismic lines, pointing in the direc-tion towards the nearest seismic line. Each of the threewolf movement models leads to different forms ofequation (2.1) as summarized in table 1. The detailsunderlying the calculation of the coefficients are givenin the electronic supplementary material, appendix.

2.2. Wolf movements

We studied the movements of four GPS-collared wolvesin the central east slopes of the Rocky Mountains,Alberta, Canada where average daily mean temperatureswere 27.58C in winter (January/February) and 18C inspring (March/April) and total snowfall was 54 cmin winter and 24 cm in spring. This area supportsprey populations of moose (Alces alces), mule andwhite-tailed deer (Odocoilues hemionus and Odocoilesvirginianus), and elk (Cervus canadensis), as well astheir main predator, wolves. Wolf densities ranged from9.7 to 22.3 wolves per 1000 km2 [19]. Seismic line density

Table 1. Summary of the proposed wolf movement models and the corresponding form of the mean first passage time equationfor each. Exact formulae for the coefficients are given in the electronic supplementary material, appendix.

model explanation and form of equation (2.1)

no response wolves do not alter their movement in response to seismic lineswolves move according to a random walk everywhere in the landscape

d @2T@x2

þ d @2T@y2

þ 1 ¼ 0; x [ V

anisotropic diffusion far from seismic lines wolves move randomlywolves move faster on seismic lines and are more likely to continuealong them in either direction once on them

d@2T

@x2þ d

@2T

@y2þ 1 ¼ 0; x off seismic lines

dxxðxÞ@2T

@x2þ 2dxyðxÞ

@2T

@x@yþ dyyðxÞ

@2T

@y2þ 1 ¼ 0; x on seismic lines

anisotropic diffusion þ bias far from seismic lines wolves move randomlywolves bias their movement towards seismic lines when near themwolves move faster on seismic lines and are more likely to continuealong them in either direction once on them

d@2T

@x2þ d

@2T

@y2þ 1 ¼ 0; x off seismic lines

dxxðxÞ@2T

@x2þ 2dxyðxÞ

@2T

@x@yþ dyyðxÞ

@2T

@y2

þcðxÞ � rT þ 1 ¼ 0; x near seismic lines

dxxðxÞ@2T

@x2þ 2dxyðxÞ

@2T

@x@yþ dyyðxÞ

@2T

@y2þ 1 ¼ 0; x on seismic lines

Linear features and predator movement H. W. McKenzie et al. 3

Interface Focus (2012)

253

254

255

256

257

258

259

260

261

262

263

264

265

266

267

268

269

270

271

272

273

274

275

276

277

278

279

280

281

282

283

284

285

286

287

288

289

290

291

292

293

294

295

296

297

298

299

300

301

302

303

304

305

306

307

308

309

310

311

312

313

314

315

316

317

318

319

320

321

322

323

324

325

326

327

328

329

330

331

332

333

334

335

336

337

338

339

340

341

342

343

344

345

346

347

348

349

350

351

352

353

354

355

356

357

358

359

360

361

362

363

364

365

366

367

368

369

370

371

372

373

374

375

376

377

378

ARTICLE IN PRESS

rsfs20110086—23/12/11—21:27–Copy Edited by: Mahalakshmi S.

Page 6: ReportNumber12/02 · 2012-12-11 · ReportNumber12/02 Howlinear features alter predatormovementandthe functional response by Hannah W. McKenzie1, EvelynH. Merrill, RaymondJ. Spiteri

varied from 0.18 km km22 near the Western border to4.4 km km22 near Rocky Mountain House, with amean of 1.8 km km22. It is likely that seismic linesexperienced a range of human use year-round for hunt-ing, trapping, snowmobiling, off-roading and hiking.

Q2 Movement data were obtained from GPS locations ofcollared wolves from four individuals in three packsduring January–April 2005, occupying territoriesacross a gradient of seismic line densities from 1.73 to

Q3 3.60 km km22 [20]. Collars were programmed to collectlocations at 5 min intervals and successfully recordedlocations on 90 per cent of fixed attempts. Data weredownloaded upon retrieval via a remote release mechan-ism (three wolves) or recapture (one wolf). Wolves wereconsidered to be independent units because they wereeither from different packs or the data were collectedduring different time periods.

We described movements via vectors joining the cur-rent and next consecutive wolf location. Each vectorwas characterized by step length and movement direc-tion (with direction North having 08). To investigatethe appropriateness of a diffusion-type model for move-ment, we calculated the mean-squared displacement foreach wolf for time intervals of length 5, 15, 30, 60 and120 min. A simple diffusion model with no correlations

Q2 predicts a linear increase in the MSD as a function oftime interval. We also calculated correlations in succes-sive movement directions using the circular correlationcoefficient (raa)s [21].

Our movement model characterized wolf movementvia step length (r. 0) and relative move direction.Relative move direction (21808 � j � 1808 ) is theangle between the ‘beeline’ move direction of theanimal and the direction of a straight line pointingfrom the current location towards the nearest seismicline (electronic supplementary material, appendixfigure S1). A relative move direction of j+ 908 rep-resents moves along the seismic line j ¼ 08 representsmoves towards the seismic line, and j+ 1808 representsmoves away from the seismic line. Because GPSmeasurement error may result in incorrect inference ofmove direction between locations that are less than 5s.d. of the GPS error kernel apart [22], we consideredonly those relative move directions with correspondingmove distances greater than 55 m [23]. However, weused all of the distances when calculating step lengthsso as to avoid introducing a bias towards longer moves.

To understand how the distance of a wolf to a seismicline affected the step lengths and relative move direc-tions of the wolves, we classified wolf locations intothree groups: on, near and off seismic lines. Seismiclines were assumed to have an average width of 5 mand were buffered by an additional 24.5 m on eachside to account for GPS measurement error in wolflocations [23]. Locations within the GPS error bufferwere classified as ‘on’. Locations between the GPSerror buffer and the distance at which we assumedwolves perceived seismic lines were classified as ‘near’.We arbitrarily chose a distance of 50 m to representthe distance at which seismic lines might be visible towolves. Locations beyond the perceptual range wereclassified as ‘off’. We quantified and compared thedistributions of each near, far and off seismic lines.

Step lengths of canids often follow the exponentialdistribution

f ðrÞ ¼ a exp½�ar�; ð2:4Þ

where a is the mean step length [17]. The maximum-likelihood estimate a is the sample mean, �r. Wecompared the mean step lengths on, near and off seis-mic lines using 90% confidence intervals obtained bynon-parametric bootstrapping [24].

The von Mises distribution is commonly usedto describe animal movement directions ([17] andelectronic supplementary material, appendix). Weassumed that the distribution of relative move direc-tions of wolves on seismic lines followed the bivariatevon Mises distribution

KðjÞ ¼1

4pI0ðkÞexp½k cosðjþ 90WÞ�

þ1

4pI0ðkÞexp½k cosðj� 90WÞ�; ð2:5Þ

where the movement directions are oriented in thedirection of the seismic line. To determine whether ani-sotropic movement was present, we tested the nullhypothesis H0 : k ¼ 0 (no tendency to continue alongthe seismic line in the next move) against the alterna-tive hypothesis Ha : k . 0 (tendency to continue alongthe seismic line in the next move) using the parametricbootstrap likelihood-ratio (PBLR) test [25,26]. Formoves near seismic lines, we assumed that the dis-tribution of relative move directions was univariatevon Mises,

KðjÞ ¼1

2pI0ðkÞexp½k cosðjÞ�: ð2:6Þ

where the movement directions are oriented towards theseismic line. To determine whether bias towardsthe lines was present, we tested the null hypothesisH0 : k ¼ 0 (no tendency to move towards the seismicline in the next move) against the alternative hypoth-esis H0 : k. 0 (tendency to move towards the seismicline in the next move) using the PBLR test [25,26]. Ineach case, the maximum-likelihood estimate k wasfound by numerical maximization of the likelihoodfunction and non-parametric bootstrapped 90%confidence intervals constructed [24].

To investigate the effect that possible correlations insuccessive movement directions would have on themovement patterns, we simulated 1000 wolf movementpaths based on choosing the turning angle and steplength randomly from the measured data values.These simulations were repeated for each individualwolf for situations on and off linear features and themean-squared displacement per day was calculated asa summary statistic.

2.3. Model scenarios

To study the effect of seismic line density and prey densityon encounter rate for wolves, we solved equation (2.1)numerically for various scenarios using COMSOLMultiphysics (COMSOL, Inc. Stockholm, Sweden). Wechose the domain V to be a 25 km � 25 km landscape,

4 Linear features and predator movement H. W. McKenzie et al.

Interface Focus (2012)

379

380

381

382

383

384

385

386

387

388

389

390

391

392

393

394

395

396

397

398

399

400

401

402

403

404

405

406

407

408

409

410

411

412

413

414

415

416

417

418

419

420

421

422

423

424

425

426

427

428

429

430

431

432

433

434

435

436

437

438

439

440

441

442

443

444

445

446

447

448

449

450

451

452

453

454

455

456

457

458

459

460

461

462

463

464

465

466

467

468

469

470

471

472

473

474

475

476

477

478

479

480

481

482

483

484

485

486

487

488

489

490

491

492

493

494

495

496

497

498

499

500

501

502

503

504

ARTICLE IN PRESS

rsfs20110086—23/12/11—21:27–Copy Edited by: Mahalakshmi S.

Page 7: ReportNumber12/02 · 2012-12-11 · ReportNumber12/02 Howlinear features alter predatormovementandthe functional response by Hannah W. McKenzie1, EvelynH. Merrill, RaymondJ. Spiteri

similar in area to the average home range size of thewolvesin the study area [19]. The edges of the domain were sub-ject to Neumann (reflecting) boundary conditions. Thiscorresponds to the wolf remaining within its homerange. We then computed the encounter rate for eachscenario using equation (2.3). Because wolves remain ata kill site for several hours or longer and typically donot hunt immediately after consuming prey, it is reason-able to assume that between hunting bouts, the preyspecies have time to move. Therefore, the assumption incomputing E from �T that the predator initially is ran-domly distributed with respect to the prey is notunreasonable. Although the movement data of threeout of the four wolves showed evidence of anisotropicdiffusion, only wolf 233 showed evidence of both aniso-tropic diffusion and bias towards seismic lines (see §3).Therefore, in order to compare the effects of the threedifferent movement models on encounter rate, weestimated the coefficients for all the three models (noresponse, anisotropic diffusion and anisotropicdiffusionþ bias) from the wolf movement data for wolf233 using the methods described in the electronicsupplementary material, appendix.

We generated simulated landscapes with varyingseismic line densities based on seismic line layers ofwest central Alberta mapped at a resolution of 5 musing Indian Remote sensing satellite imagery [27].A baseline seismic line density of 4.46 km km22 wasused to create landscapes with seismic line densi-ties ranging from approximately 2 km km22 to 9 kmkm22 (electronic supplementary material, appendixfigure S2). Prey were randomly placed in these landscapesat densities of 0.16, 0.5, 1, 1.5, 2, 2.5 and 3 prey km22.We chose to include the density of 0.16 as it is similarto the lower range of density of common prey species inour study area [19]. We assumed wolves encountered aprey when they came within radius r ¼ 100 m of theprey. In the model, this corresponds to a disc inthe domain with radius r ¼ 100 m, centred on the prey,with Dirichlet (T ¼ 0) boundary conditions.

We evaluated the effect of wolf movement responsesto seismic lines based on the first passage time solutionsfrom two sets of simulations. First, for a fixed prey

density of 0.16 prey km22, we simulated the effects ofthe three different movement models outlined intable 1. Simulations were iterated 10 times at each seis-mic line density using a different distribution ofrandomly located prey. Encounter rates were plottedagainst seismic lines densities and visually inspectedto assess the effects of movement responses to seismicline on encounter rates across a range of seismic linedensities. Second, we assumed wolves moved accordingto the anisotropic diffusion model (table 1) and solvedequation (2.1) for 10 replications of each prey densityand seismic line density (i.e. six prey densities � 10replications � 4 seismic line densities ¼ 240 model sol-utions). We chose the anisotropic diffusion movementmodel because we found that it reflected the greatesteffect of seismic lines on encounters with prey for ourinvestigation. In each case, we used T(x) to computethe encounter rate using equations (2.2) and (2.3). Weused the outputs of these simulations to evaluate a setof a priori candidate models (table 2) relating encoun-ter rates to seismic line or prey densities or both usingthe nonlinear regression analysis package nls in R andcomparing the fit of the models based on AIC [28] Q2.

The functional forms of the candidate models(table 2) were proposed based on the results obtainedearlier [3], which suggested that different underlyingpredator movement mechanisms lead to differentrelationships between encounter rate and prey density.McKenzie et al. [3] found that in one dimension, Q4theencounter rate for predators undergoing only advectivemovement was a linear function of prey density. This isconsistent with the assumption of a constant areasearched per time made by Holling [1]. In Q5contrast,McKenzie et al. [3] found that the encounter rate forpredators undergoing only random movement was aquadratic function of prey density. Depending on thedensity of seismic lines, predators in our model undergosome combination of anisotropic diffusion along seismiclines and random searching off seismic lines via isotropicdiffusion. Although the anisotropic diffusion model dif-fers mathematically from the advective movementmodel, there is a similarity. Anisotropic diffusionbiases movement in relation to both directions along

Table 2. Candidate models for encounter rate (E) of wolves as a function of prey density (N) and seismic line density (S).

model explanation form

single variate models: seismic line density

A1 linear E ¼ b0 þ b1S

A2 quadratic E ¼ b0 þ b1S þ b2S2

A3 exponential E ¼ AebS

single variate models: prey density

B1 directed search E ¼ b1N

B2 random search E ¼ b1N2

B3 combination of directed and random search, using a sum E ¼ b1N þ b2N2

B4 combination of directed and random search using an intermediate power E ¼ ANb

multivariate models

C1 no effect of seismic lines E ¼ ANb

C2 linear interaction between seismic lines and prey density E ¼ ANb þ b1NS

C3 nonlinear interaction between seismic lines and prey density (same power) E ¼ ANb þ b1NbS

C4 nonlinear interaction between seismic lines and prey density (different power) E ¼ ANb1 þ b1Nb2S

Linear features and predator movement H. W. McKenzie et al. 5

Interface Focus (2012)

505

506

507

508

509

510

511

512

513

514

515

516

517

518

519

520

521

522

523

524

525

526

527

528

529

530

531

532

533

534

535

536

537

538

539

540

541

542

543

544

545

546

547

548

549

550

551

552

553

554

555

556

557

558

559

560

561

562

563

564

565

566

567

568

569

570

571

572

573

574

575

576

577

578

579

580

581

582

583

584

585

586

587

588

589

590

591

592

593

594

595

596

597

598

599

600

601

602

603

604

605

606

607

608

609

610

611

612

613

614

615

616

617

618

619

620

621

622

623

624

625

626

627

628

629

630

ARTICLE IN PRESS

rsfs20110086—23/12/11—21:27–Copy Edited by: Mahalakshmi S.

Page 8: ReportNumber12/02 · 2012-12-11 · ReportNumber12/02 Howlinear features alter predatormovementandthe functional response by Hannah W. McKenzie1, EvelynH. Merrill, RaymondJ. Spiteri

the seismic line, whereas advection biases movement be-haviour in relation to a single direction. Based on thissimilarity, we consider the anisotropic diffusion todescribe a particular form of directed motion. There-fore, we proposed several model forms for theindependent and combined effects of prey density andseismic lines that have a mechanistic basis for describ-ing encounter rates when the search is a combinationof directed and random motion by analogy with theearlier results [3].

2.4. The functional response

The functional response f(N) describes the per capitakill rate as a function of prey density N. Here, weinvestigate the potential variation in the functionalresponse owing to increased predator mobility in thepresence of seismic lines. To model the functionalresponse, we use the Holling disc equation,

f ðN Þ ¼EðNÞ

1þ EðNÞTh; ð2:7Þ

where the encounter rate E depends on prey density andTh is the constant handling time. The handling time isdefined by Holling to be the sum of the attack time(including evaluating, pursuing and catching the prey)and the handling time (including processing and con-suming the prey). Traditionally, the encounter rate isassumed to be proportional to the prey density. Instead,we used the best multivariate model for encounter ratefrom table 2. Using the best model for encounter rate,we asked the question: how does the functional responsechange as the proportion of directed and random move-ment changes owing to increasing seismic line density?To answer this question, we compared the functionalresponse in landscapes with seismic line densities of 0,4.46 and 8.91 km km22 by computing the ratio of thefunctional response at each seismic line density tothat when there are no seismic lines present. If theratio is 1, then seismic lines do not alter the kill rate.If the ratio is greater than 1, the presence of seismiclines leads to an increase in the kill rate. The largerthe ratio, the larger the difference between the encoun-ter rates in landscapes with and without seismic lines,and the stronger the effect of seismic lines. To see ifthe magnitude of the effect of seismic lines on the func-tional response depended on the handling time, wecompared results assuming handling times for small-bodied (Th ¼ 10.6 h) and large-bodied (Th ¼ 20.4 h)prey [19].

3. RESULTS

3.1. Wolf movements

Visual inspection of the mean-squared displacements asa function of measurement time interval showed varia-bility between wolves but exhibited approximatelylinear growth in the MSD as a function of time,except at short time intervals (electronic supplemen-tary material, appendix figure S3). When the 5 minmove directions were constrained to include only thosewith step lengths of at least 55 m so as to remove

errors (§2), we found statistically significant positivecorrelations in wolf movement directions with positivecircular correlation coefficients of 0.20, 0.61, 0.33 and0.43 for wolves 230, 232, 233 and 234. Movement pat-terns of wolves in landscapes with seismic lines werenot consistent among individuals. All wolves had alonger mean step length on seismic lines than off seismiclines (figure 1a, p, 0.1). Wolves 230, 232 and 234 hada shorter mean step length off seismic lines than nearseismic lines, while wolf 233 had a shorter mean steplength near seismic lines. On seismic lines wolves 230,233 and 234 had distributions of relative move direc-tions that differed from the uniform distribution, withmoves along seismic lines occurring more often thanmoves in other directions (figure 1b, p, 0.001). Wolf232 moved randomly with respect to seismic lines. Incontrast, near seismic lines, only wolf 233 had a non-uniform distribution of relative move directions(figure 1c, p, 0.001). When off seismic lines, allwolves had uniform distributions of relative movedirections (figure 1d). We chose to use the move-ment parameters from the data for wolf 233 in oursimulations because wolf 233 showed evidence of allthe movement behaviours of interest (electronicsupplementary material, appendix table S1). The maxi-mum-likelihood fit of wolf 233 step lengths to theexponential probability density function yielded mean-squared displacement values of 0.017 km2 5 min21

off-lines and 0.043 km2 5 min21 on-lines (electronic sup-plementary material, appendix table S1) scaling up to4.90 km2 d21 off-lines and 12.4 km2 d21 on-lines. Thesimulations of movement patterns yielded estimates forthe off-line mean-squared displacement as 4.96 km2 d21

and the on-line mean-squared displacement of18.83 km2 d21 for wolf 233, indicating that successivecorrelations in move direction have the effect of increas-ing the on-line mean-squared displacement per unit timeby about 50 per cent. Other simulatedmean-squared dis-placements for wolves were 5.86, 9.88 and 7.92 km2 d21

(off-line) and 10.93, 24.08 and 32.26 km2 d21 (on-line)for wolves 230, 232 and 234, respectively.

3.2. Model scenarios

Examples of the solution to mean first passage timeequation (equation (2.1)) are shown in figure 2. Forany point x ¼ (x,y) in the domain, the value of themean first passage time surface is the average time fora wolf starting at that location to encounter a preyitem. Changes in prey density or seismic line densitycause local differences in the mean first passage timesurfaces. For example, surface height, the presence ofpeaks and the steepness of gradients differ betweenthe example surfaces. These local differences translateinto different average mean first passage time values,�T (see equation (2.2)), which we investigated furtherwith statistical models.

For a fixed prey density, encounter rate was constantwhen wolves did not alter their movement (no responsemodel) with response to seismic lines (figure 3a).Encounter rates increased linearly with seismic line den-sity when wolves followed a movement pattern resultingfrom faster movement on seismic lines and a tendency

6 Linear features and predator movement H. W. McKenzie et al.

Interface Focus (2012)

631

632

633

634

635

636

637

638

639

640

641

642

643

644

645

646

647

648

649

650

651

652

653

654

655

656

657

658

659

660

661

662

663

664

665

666

667

668

669

670

671

672

673

674

675

676

677

678

679

680

681

682

683

684

685

686

687

688

689

690

691

692

693

694

695

696

697

698

699

700

701

702

703

704

705

706

707

708

709

710

711

712

713

714

715

716

717

718

719

720

721

722

723

724

725

726

727

728

729

730

731

732

733

734

735

736

737

738

739

740

741

742

743

744

745

746

747

748

749

750

751

752

753

754

755

756

ARTICLE IN PRESS

rsfs20110086—23/12/11—21:27–Copy Edited by: Mahalakshmi S.

Page 9: ReportNumber12/02 · 2012-12-11 · ReportNumber12/02 Howlinear features alter predatormovementandthe functional response by Hannah W. McKenzie1, EvelynH. Merrill, RaymondJ. Spiteri

on near off on near off on near off on near off

wolf 230

0

100

200

location location location location

step

len

gth

(m

)

(a)

(b)

(c)

(d)

ac

wolf 232

a

c

wolf 233

a

bc

wolf 234

a

bc

0

0.2

0.4

pro

bab

ilit

ypro

bab

ilit

ypro

bab

ilit

y

0

0.2

0.4

0

0.2

0.4

−180 0 90 180relative move

direction (º)

−180 0 90 180relative move

direction (º)

−180 0 90 180relative move

direction (º)

−180 0 90 180relative move

direction (º)

Figure 1. Mean step lengths and distributions of relative move directions of wolves calculated from 5 min GPS data. (a) Meanstep lengths of wolves on, near and off seismic lines. Bars are 90% non-parametric bootstrapped confidence intervals. Relativemove direction of wolves (b) on, (c) near, and (d) off seismic lines. Solid lines are the maximum-likelihood fit of the bestmodel chosen using the PBLR test.

5 10 15 20 250 5 10 15 20 250

5

10

15

20

25

location, x location, x

loca

tion, y

loca

tion, y

5

10

15

20

25

(a)

(b) (d)

(c)

0

5

10

15

20

25

Figure 2. Example mean first passage time surfaces for the anisotropic diffusion model. (a–c) The prey density is 1.5 prey km22

and the seismic line densities are 0, 4.46 and 8.91 km km22, respectively. As seismic line density increases, the values of the meanfirst passage time surface decrease, leading to a decrease in the average mean first passage time, �T . (d), There are no seismic lines,but prey density is increased to three prey km22. Increasing prey density also leads to a decrease in the mean first passage timeand the average mean first passage time.

Linear features and predator movement H. W. McKenzie et al. 7

Interface Focus (2012)

757

758

759

760

761

762

763

764

765

766

767

768

769

770

771

772

773

774

775

776

777

778

779

780

781

782

783

784

785

786

787

788

789

790

791

792

793

794

795

796

797

798

799

800

801

802

803

804

805

806

807

808

809

810

811

812

813

814

815

816

817

818

819

820

821

822

823

824

825

826

827

828

829

830

831

832

833

834

835

836

837

838

839

840

841

842

843

844

845

846

847

848

849

850

851

852

853

854

855

856

857

858

859

860

861

862

863

864

865

866

867

868

869

870

871

872

873

874

875

876

877

878

879

880

881

882

ARTICLE IN PRESS

rsfs20110086—23/12/11—21:27–Copy Edited by: Mahalakshmi S.

Page 10: ReportNumber12/02 · 2012-12-11 · ReportNumber12/02 Howlinear features alter predatormovementandthe functional response by Hannah W. McKenzie1, EvelynH. Merrill, RaymondJ. Spiteri

to continue along seismic lines once on them (anisotro-pic diffusion model). When an additional bias towardsseismic lines when wolves were near seismic lines wasincluded (anisotropic diffusion þ bias model), encoun-ter rates increased linearly but not as rapidly whencompared with the model without the bias.

Given that seismic lines had the greatest effect onencounter rates under the anisotropic diffusion move-ment model, we examined the joint effects of seismicand prey densities for this movement mode(figure 3b,c). The model providing the best fit for theeffects of seismic lines on encounter rate (electronic sup-plementary material, appendix table S2:A1) wasconsistently the linear model at all prey densities, butthe slope of the relationship increased with prey density(figure 3b) indicating an interaction between seismiclines and prey density under this movement mode. Apower model provided the best fit for relating encounterrate to prey density (electronic supplementary material,appendix, table S2:B4), where the constant scaling expo-nent (b), but variable A coefficient, also suggested anonlinear interaction (electronic supplementary

material, appendix table S3). Indeed, when encounterrate was modelled as a function of both prey and seismicline density, the best-fit model included a significantinteraction term between seismic line and prey density(p, 0.001, electronic supplementary material, appendixtable S3). The positive coefficient for the interactionterm showed that seismic line density enhanced therate at which the predators encountered prey.

3.3. The functional response

The observed increase in encounter rate due to increasingseismic line density translates into a functional responsethat increases more quickly to saturation (figure 3d,e). Inall cases, the ratio of the functional responses in land-scapes with and without seismic lines is greater than 1,meaning that the presence of seismic lines leads to anincrease in kill rates (figure 3f ). For handling times forboth small-bodied and large-bodied prey, the ratio islarger at low prey densities than at higher prey densities.This suggests that the effect of seismic lines on the func-tional response is larger when prey density is low. In

0 2 4 6

0.016

0.018

0.020

0.022

seismic line density (km km–2) seismic line density (km km–2)

enco

unte

r ra

te (

h−

1)

(a)

(d) (e) ( f )

(b) (c)

0 2 4 6 8 10 12

0

0.05

0.10

0.15

0.20

0.25

0.30

0.35 3.0(prey km–2)

2.5

2.0

1.5

1.0

0.5

0 1 2 3 4

0

0.05

0.10

0.15

0.20

0.25

0.30

0.35

prey density (prey km–2)

8.91(km km–2

6.654.462.190

0 2 4 6 8 10

0

0.2

0.4

0.6

0.8

1.0

1.2

1.4

enco

unte

r ra

te (

h−

1)

prey density (km km–2) prey density (km km–2) prey density (km km–2)

0 2 4 6 8 10 0 2 4 6 8 10

0

0.02

0.04

0.06

0.08

funct

ional

res

ponse

(pre

y h

–1)

0.8

0.9

1.0

1.1

1.2

1.3

1.4

rati

o o

f fu

nct

ional

res

ponse

s

Figure 3. Simulated encounter rates (equations (2.1)–(2.3)) and the effect of seismic line density on the encounter rate and func-tional response. (a) Encounter rates for the different movement models as a function of seismic line density when prey density isfixed at 0.16 prey km22 (squares, no response; circles, anisotropic diffusion; asterisks, anisotropic diffusion þ bias). (b) Encounterrate at each prey density for the anisotropic diffusion model. Lines show the fit of the best model for encounter rate, which was thelinear model in each case. (c) Encounter rate at each seismic line density for the anisotropic diffusion model. The dotted lineindicates a density of 0.16 prey km22 (compare with (a)). Lines show the fit of the best model for encounter rate, which wasthe power model in each case. (d) Predicted encounter rate for three seismic line densities. Encounter rate was predicted bythe model C3: E ¼ ANb þ b1 Nb S, where the coefficients are A ¼ 7.43 � 102 2, b ¼ 1.15 and b1 ¼ 2.74 � 1023 (solid lines, noseismic lines; dashed lines 4.46 km km22; dotted lines, 8.91 km km22). (e) The functional response, assuming handling timesfor small-bodied (black lines) and large-bodied (grey lines) prey. ( f ) The ratio of functional responses in landscapes with andwithout seismic lines, assuming handling times for small- and large-bodied prey.

8 Linear features and predator movement H. W. McKenzie et al.

Interface Focus (2012)

883

884

885

886

887

888

889

890

891

892

893

894

895

896

897

898

899

900

901

902

903

904

905

906

907

908

909

910

911

912

913

914

915

916

917

918

919

920

921

922

923

924

925

926

927

928

929

930

931

932

933

934

935

936

937

938

939

940

941

942

943

944

945

946

947

948

949

950

951

952

953

954

955

956

957

958

959

960

961

962

963

964

965

966

967

968

969

970

971

972

973

974

975

976

977

978

979

980

981

982

983

984

985

986

987

988

989

990

991

992

993

994

995

996

997

998

999

1000

1001

1002

1003

1004

1005

1006

1007

1008

ARTICLE IN PRESS

rsfs20110086—23/12/11—21:27–Copy Edited by: Mahalakshmi S.

Page 11: ReportNumber12/02 · 2012-12-11 · ReportNumber12/02 Howlinear features alter predatormovementandthe functional response by Hannah W. McKenzie1, EvelynH. Merrill, RaymondJ. Spiteri

addition, across prey densities, the ratio of functionalresponses in landscapes with and without seismic linesis larger for small-bodied prey than for large-bodiedprey. Therefore, the effect of seismic lines is moreapparent when handling time is shorter.

4. DISCUSSION

All four wolves in this study, as well as wolves in theboreal forests of Alberta [6], have demonstratedincreased movement rates when travelling on seismiclines. Reduced debris and snow crusting on open seismiclines in winter compared with the forest may facilitatemovement along the lines [6,29]. Three of the fourwolves studied also exhibited a higher probability ofcontinuing along seismic lines once on them. The pro-pensity to remain on seismic lines is also reflected inhabitat selection studies, where wolves were found toselect seismic lines more than expected by chance [12].The rapid movement and use of linear features havecontributed to reports that wolves use them as travelroutes [5,7,9,10,12]. The strong directional persistenceof wolves while travelling on the seismic lines that wereport may have been shaped, in part, by the straight-ness of seismic line across the landscape relative toother linear features like roads and trails [30]. Wolf233 showed biased movement towards seismic lineswhen near them, which is the first quantification ofsuch a bias, despite other reports of wolves changingtheir direction in order to move directly to adjacentcompacted trails in nearby montane areas [7].

Incorporating these movement data into an advec-tion–diffusion framework, we assessed the implicationsof varied movement strategies using the first passagetime models on two movement components. We intro-duced anisotropy in the diffusion components of firstpassage models. At the same time, the possibility ofmovement bias towards seismic lines led us to introducea bias term via advection. Both of these movementresponses to seismic lines increased encounter rateswith prey over when seismic lines were absent, consistentwith the results of previous spatially explicit, individual-based models [31]. However, we also expected that thebias towards the seismic line, i.e. wolves being morelikely to get on seismic lines than to leave them, wouldresult in the highest encounter rates. This was not thecase. In fact, although increased mobility was advan-tageous in covering more area to find prey, a movementbias towards seismic lines resulted in wolves findingfewer prey that were positioned away from the seismiclines. Unless prey are attracted to seismic lines or existat very high densities (figure 3c), our results indicatethat it would not be advantageous for predators to biastheir movements towards seismic lines, explaining poss-ibly why the bias was not commonly observed amongthe wolves we studied.

The purpose of fitting statistical encounter ratemodels to the first passage time simulation results wasto assess how well the model parameters would reflectthe underlying movement mechanisms being modelled.Under the anisotropic diffusion model, the statisticalencounter rate models showed that wolf movement

did not follow either a purely directed search (modelB1) or a purely random search (model B2), even whenno effect of seismic lines was included. Instead, thebest model fit was intermediate, reflecting a mixtureof both movement modes. This model exhibited amore than linear increase with respect to prey density,which is consistent with type III functional response.This is consistent with the results of McKenzie et al.[3], who showed that the encounter rate of randomlydiffusing predators in one dimension was related toprey density by a power law.

Encounter rates increased linearly with seismic linedensity, but this increase was contingent on the distri-bution of prey with respect to the seismic lines [32].Simulations were based on real landscapes from thestudy area. For this study area, seismic lines havebeen shown to be distributed randomly across the land-scape, unlike some other linear features such as roads(E. H. Merrill, unpublished data) Q6. Because prey werealso assumed to be randomly distributed, the linearincrease directly reflects that wolves spent more timein directed movement in landscapes with higher seismicline densities than when seismic lines were not pre-sent. If prey were to avoid seismic lines, as James &Stuart-Smith [12] have reported for caribou (Rangifertarandus), encounter rates are expected to decline.However, as seismic line density increases, prey areless able to avoid seismic lines [33].

Our assumption that the ungulate prey did not moveis a simplification that we needed to make to apply firstpassage time analysis to a complex spatial environment.Clearly, prey can move during the search period,although typically at a slower rate than the wolves,and this would have some effect on the results. However,it is unlikely that the wolves were following the prey onthe seismic lines because their rates of movement weresubstantially higher than those of ungulate prey [13].We assumed that all prey encountered were killed andthat prey were encountered when wolves were within100 m of the prey. While the success in killing a preyonce detected can be highly variable, we simplified themodel for the purpose of understanding the influencemovements during search on functional response.

The presence of the interaction between prey densityand seismic line density in the best-fit model indicatesthat the effect of prey density on encounter rate is modi-fied by seismic line density. Therefore, it is not the effectof increasing seismic line density alone that leads toincreased encounter rate, but the interaction betweenseismic line density and prey density. We interpret thepositive coefficient of the interaction term to meanthat increasing seismic line density will have a strongereffect on encounter rate at high prey densities than atlow prey densities. Despite this interaction term leadingto a larger increase owing to seismic lines in the magni-tude of the encounter rate at high prey densities, usingthe Holling disc equation, we show that seismic lineshave the greatest relative influence on potential killrates in environments with low prey densities. This fol-lows because predator mobility constrains searchingsuccess more acutely at low prey densities. As prey den-sity increases, predators shift from being search limitedto being handling time limited, and the benefit of

Linear features and predator movement H. W. McKenzie et al. 9

Interface Focus (2012)

1009

1010

1011

1012

1013

1014

1015

1016

1017

1018

1019

1020

1021

1022

1023

1024

1025

1026

1027

1028

1029

1030

1031

1032

1033

1034

1035

1036

1037

1038

1039

1040

1041

1042

1043

1044

1045

1046

1047

1048

1049

1050

1051

1052

1053

1054

1055

1056

1057

1058

1059

1060

1061

1062

1063

1064

1065

1066

1067

1068

1069

1070

1071

1072

1073

1074

1075

1076

1077

1078

1079

1080

1081

1082

1083

1084

1085

1086

1087

1088

1089

1090

1091

1092

1093

1094

1095

1096

1097

1098

1099

1100

1101

1102

1103

1104

1105

1106

1107

1108

1109

1110

1111

1112

1113

1114

1115

1116

1117

1118

1119

1120

1121

1122

1123

1124

1125

1126

1127

1128

1129

1130

1131

1132

1133

1134

ARTICLE IN PRESS

rsfs20110086—23/12/11—21:27–Copy Edited by: Mahalakshmi S.

Page 12: ReportNumber12/02 · 2012-12-11 · ReportNumber12/02 Howlinear features alter predatormovementandthe functional response by Hannah W. McKenzie1, EvelynH. Merrill, RaymondJ. Spiteri

increased mobility diminishes. However, even at highprey densities increased search efficiency may stillalter predation rates when handling times are short.Although search time theoretically may not be the lim-iting process at high prey densities [1], wolves do notalways invest the time in consuming the full prey and,at the extreme they have demonstrated surplus killing[34], which is consistent with short handling time.Additionally, in multi-prey systems, high search effi-ciency in environments with seismic lines may increaseencounters with rare prey, that if more preferred or vul-nerable, may result in dietary shifts [35], particularly fora coursing predator like the wolf, whose broad scalemovements may homogenize spatial heterogeneity inprey [36]. Altered predation pressures within a preycommunity have implications of apparent competitionand prey persistence ([37–39], see also [40]).

The approximately linear relationship betweenmean-squared displacement and measurement timeinterval (electronic supplementary material, appendixfigure S3) indicates that a diffusion-based model maybe appropriate for modelling the movement of wolves.However, the issue of positive correlations in the set ofmovement directions retained for analysis has impli-cations for both the bootstrapping methods, whichimplicitly assume independence of data, and for the cal-culation of the diffusion coefficient, which assumes nopersistence in movement direction. This is a difficultissue to deal with in a satisfactory manner. Oneapproach for reducing correlations in move directionsis to subsample data by taking it over less frequenttime intervals [17]. However, we chose not to use thissubsampling approach because it would have removedthe important detailed information required to seehow wolves move in relation to roads over short spatialscales. A second approach is to rescale the estimate ofthe diffusion coefficient based on the simulations ofwolf movement on- and off-lines. This would have leftthe off-line diffusion coefficient the same but wouldhave increased the on-line diffusion coefficient byapproximately 50 per cent. An alternative method forcorrecting the diffusion coefficient, which leads to simi-lar results, is based on the work of Patlak (seeappendix C of Moorcroft & Lewis [17]). This attemptsto approximately correct for persistence in move-ment direction by rescaling the diffusion coefficient by1/(1 2 c), where c is the mean cosine of the turningangle, and the turning angle is the difference in consecu-tive movement directions. We measured that thequantity c averaged 0.18 off seismic lines and 0.43 onseismic lines, suggesting that correlations could makethe diffusion coefficient 22 per cent higher off seismiclines and 75 per cent higher on seismic lines. WhilePatlak’s approach can deal with spatial heterogeneity,it has not yet been extended to the case of anisotropy,which is a central element in our model. Regardless,we also chose not use either of these methods to rescalethe diffusion coefficient, noting that the variation calcu-lated between the different wolves is larger thancorrection term that such rescaling would entail. How-ever, had we made such a rescaling, the overall firstpassage times would be somewhat reduced, and theeffect of seismic lines on first passage times would be

enhanced. A third mathematically interesting approachto deal with correlations is to use a non-diffusive model,based a velocity jump process where there is persistencein movement direction. This non-diffusive Q7transportequation framework has only just recently been extendedto deal with anisotropic movement patterns [41] and theconnection with first passage time analysis has not yetbeen developed. Finally, a full analysis of correlationsmust also include autocorrelations over periods longerthan a single time step, which have the potential tofurther increase the mean-squared displacement per unittime (electronic supplementary material, appendixfigure S3), as well as interactions from measured cor-relations with GPS errors. We suggest that these areimportant avenues for future research.

Our understanding of the influence of predatorresponse to spatial heterogeneity on search behavioursand its implications for predator–prey interactions isnow emerging [3,42–45]. Because details on animal move-ments are now readily available with GPS technology [46],it may be possible to quantify distinct modes of movement[47–50] even if an understanding of motivation for themovement behaviours is less clear [51]. In this paper, wehave shown that a new alternative model structure forthe encounter rate component of the functional responseis appropriate when predators alter their movement inresponse to landscape heterogeneity. We illustrate thepoint for wolves in real landscapes where seismic line den-sities alter the directional component of search. In termsof conservation, our results indicate that increasing seis-mic line density or other linear features associated withland development that affects wolf movement will havea relatively large impact on single prey systems whenthe prey is at risk (i.e. low density) and small (i.e. smallhandling times) or on multi-prey systems where there ispreference for the rare species.

We wish to acknowledge support from Alberta Ingenuity,NSERC C-GSM, the University of Alberta and the MITACSMobility Fund (H.W.M.), an NSERC CRO-261091-02(E.H.M.), NSERC Discovery and Accelerator Grants and aCanada Research Chair (M.A.L.). This publication was basedon the work supported in part by Award no. KUK-CI013-04made by King Abdullah University of Science andTechnology (KAUST; M.A.L.). We thank N. Webb forproviding helpful feedback, H. Beyer for assisting with theGIS analysis and J. Berger for field expertise.

REFERENCES

1 Holling, C. 1959 The components of predation as revealedby a study of small mammal predation of the Europeanpine sawfly. Can. Entomol. 91, 293–320. (doi:10.4039/Ent91293-5)

2 Hassell, M. 1978 Arthropod predator–prey systems. No. 12in monographs in population biology. PrincetonUniversity Press Q8.

3 Mckenzie, H. W., Lewis, M. A. & Merrill, E. H. 2009 Firstpassage time analysis of animal movement and insightsinto the functional response. Bull. Math. Biol. 71,107–129. (doi:10.1007/s11538-008-9354-x)

4 Timoney, K. & Lee, P. 2001 Environmental managementin resource-rich Alberta, Canada: first world jurisdiction,third world analogue? J. Environ. Manag. 63, 387–405.(doi:10.1006/jema.2001.0487)

10 Linear features and predator movement H. W. McKenzie et al.

Interface Focus (2012)

1135

1136

1137

1138

1139

1140

1141

1142

1143

1144

1145

1146

1147

1148

1149

1150

1151

1152

1153

1154

1155

1156

1157

1158

1159

1160

1161

1162

1163

1164

1165

1166

1167

1168

1169

1170

1171

1172

1173

1174

1175

1176

1177

1178

1179

1180

1181

1182

1183

1184

1185

1186

1187

1188

1189

1190

1191

1192

1193

1194

1195

1196

1197

1198

1199

1200

1201

1202

1203

1204

1205

1206

1207

1208

1209

1210

1211

1212

1213

1214

1215

1216

1217

1218

1219

1220

1221

1222

1223

1224

1225

1226

1227

1228

1229

1230

1231

1232

1233

1234

1235

1236

1237

1238

1239

1240

1241

1242

1243

1244

1245

1246

1247

1248

1249

1250

1251

1252

1253

1254

1255

1256

1257

1258

1259

1260

ARTICLE IN PRESS

rsfs20110086—23/12/11—21:27–Copy Edited by: Mahalakshmi S.

Page 13: ReportNumber12/02 · 2012-12-11 · ReportNumber12/02 Howlinear features alter predatormovementandthe functional response by Hannah W. McKenzie1, EvelynH. Merrill, RaymondJ. Spiteri

5 Thurber, J., Peterson, R., Drummer, T. & Thomasma, S.1994 Gray wolf response to refuge boundaries and roads inAlaska. Wildl. Soc. Bull. 22, 61–68.

6 James, A. 1999 Effects of industrial development on thepredator–prey relationship between wolves and caribouin Northeastern Alberta. Universtiy of Alberta, Canada.

7 Callaghan, C. 2002 The ecology of gray wolf (Canis lupus)habitat use, survival, and persistence in the central rockymountains. University of Guelph, Canada.

8 Whittington, J., Clair St, C. C. & Mercer, G. 2005 Spatialresponses of wolves to roads and trails in mountain valleys.Ecol. Appl. 15, 543–553. (doi:10.1890/03-5317)

9 Edmonds, E. & Bloomfield, M. 1984 A study of wood-

land caribou (Rangifer tarandus caribou) in west central

Alberta, 1979 to 1983. Edmonton, Alberta, Canada: AlbertaEnergy and Natural Resources, Fish and Wildlife Division.

10 Bergerud, A. & Elliot, J. 1986 Dynamics of caribou andwolves in northern British Columbia. Can. J. Zool. 64,1515–1529. (doi:10.1139/z86-226)

11 Jalkotzy, M., Ross, P. & Nasserden, M. 1997 The effectsof linear developments on wildlife: a review of selectedscientific literature. Arc Wildlife Services LtdQ8 .

12 James, A. & Stuart-Smith, A. 2000 Distribution of caribouand wolves in relation to linear corridors. J. Wildl. Manag.

64, 154–159. (doi:10.2307/3802985)13 Frair, J. L., Merrill, E. H., Allen, J. R. & Boyce, M. S. 2007

Know thy enemy: experience affects elk translocationsuccess in risky landscapes. J. Wildl. Manag. 71,541–554. (doi:10.2193/2006-141)

14 McPhee, H. M. 2009 Kill site selection and attack rates of

wolves in heterogeneous environments in West Central

Alberta. University of Alberta.Q815 Turchin, P. 1998 Quantitative analysis of movement.

Sinaur Associates, Inc.Q816 Kernohan, B. J., Gitzen, R. A. & Millspaugh, J. J. 2001

Analysis of animal space-use and movements. In Radio

tracking and animal populations (eds J. J. Millspaugh &J. M. Marzluff), pp. 126–166. Academic Press.Q8

17 Moorcroft, P. & Lewis, M. A. 2006 Mechanistic home

range analysis. Princeton University Press.Q818 Turchin, P. 1991 Translanting foraging movements in het-

erogeneous environments into the spatial distribution offoragers. Ecology 72, 1253–1266. (doi:10.2307/1941099)

19 Webb, N. F. 2009 Density, demography, and functional

response of a harvested wolf population in west-central

Alberta, Canada. University of Alberta.Q820 Webb, N. F., Allen, J. R. & Merrill, E. H. 2011 Demogra-

phy of a harvested population of wolves (Canis lupus) inwest-central Alberta, Canada. Can. J. Zool. 89, 744–752. (doi:10.1139/z11-043)

21 Webb, K. 1975 Statistics for directional data. J. R. Stat.Soc. Ser. B (Methodological) 37, 349–393.

22 Jerde, C. L. & Visscher, D. R. 2005 GPS measurementerror influences on movement model parameterization.Ecol. Appl. 15, 806–810. (doi:10.1890/04-0895)

23 Mckenzie, H. W., Jerde, C. L., Visscher, D. R., Merrill, E.H. & Lewis, M. A. 2009 Inferring linear feature use in thepresence of GPS measurement error. Environ. Ecol. Stat.16, 531–546. (doi:10.1007/s10651-008-0095-7)

24 Efron, B. & Tibshirani, R. 1993 An introduction to the

bootstrap. Chapman and Hall/CRC.Q825 Barnard, G. A. 1963 Discussion of: the spectral analysis of

point processes (Barlett, MS). J. R. Stat. Soc. Ser. B

(Methodological). 25, 294.26 Dennis, B. & Taper, M. 1994 Density-dependence in time-

series observations and natural population: estimation andinference. Ecol. Monogr. 64, 205–224. (doi:10.2307/2937041)

27 Frair, J., Merrill, E., Visscher, D., Fortin, D., Beyer, H. &Morales, J. 2005 Scales of movement by elk (Cervus ela-

phus) in response to heterogeneity in forage resourcesand predation risk. Landscape Ecol. 20, 273–287.(doi:10.1007/s10980-005-2075-8)

28 Burham, K. P. & Anderson, D. R. 1998 Model selection

and multinomial inference, 2nd edn. Springer. Q829 Huggard, D. J. 1993 Effect of snow depth on predation

and scavenging by gray wolves. J. Wildl. Manag. 57,382–388. (doi:10.2307/3809437)

30 Whittington, J., St Clair, C. C. & Mercer, G. 2004 Pathtortuosity and the permeability of roads and trails towolf movement. Ecol. Soc. 9 Q9.

31 McCauley, E., Wilson, W. & De Roos, A. 1993 Dynamicsof age-structured and spatially structured predator–preyinteractions: individual-based models and population-level formulations. Am. Nat. 142, 412–442. (doi:10.1086/285547)

32 Cain, M. 1985 Random search by herbivorous insects: asimulation model. Ecology 66, 876–888. (doi:10.2307/1940550)

33 Frair, J. L., Merrill, E. H., Beyer, H. L. & Morales, J. M.2008 Thresholds in landscape connectivity and mortalityrisks in response to growing road networks. J. Appl.

Ecol. 45, 1504–1513. (doi:10.1111/j.1365-2664.2008.01526.x)

34 DelGiudice, G. D. 1998 Surplus killing of white-tailed deerby wolves in northcentral Minnesota. J. Mammal. 79,227–235. (doi:10.2307/1382858)

35 Garrott, R. A., Bruggeman, J. E., Becker, M. S., Kali-nowski, S. T. & White, P. J. 2007 Evaluating preyswitching in wolf-ungulate systems. Ecol. Appl. 17,1588–1597. (doi:10.1890/06-1439.1)

36 Holt, R. D. 1984 Spatial heterogeneity, indirect inter-actions, and the coexistence of prey species. Am. Nat.

124, 377–406. (doi:10.1086/284280)37 Holt, R. D. 1977 Predation, apparent competition, and

structure of prey communities. Theor. Popul. Biol. 12,197–229. (doi:10.1016/0040-5809(77)90042-9)

38 Noonburg, E. & Byers, J. 2005 More harm than good:when invader vulnerability to predators enhances impacton native species. Ecology 86, 2555–2560. (doi:10.1890/05-0143)

39 DeCesare, N. J., Hebblewhite, M., Robinson, H. S. &Musiani, M. 2010 Endangered, apparently: the role ofapparent competition in endangered species conservation.Anim. Conserv. 13, 353–362. (doi:10.1111/j.1469-1795.2009.00328.x)

40 McLellan, B. N., Serrouya, R., Wittmer, H. U. & Boutin,S. 2010 Predator-mediated Allee effects in multi-prey sys-tems. Ecology 91, 286–292. (doi:10.1890/09-0286.1)

41 Hillen, T. & Painter, K. 2012 Transport and anisotropicdiffusion models for movement in oriented habitats. InDispersal, individual movement and spatial ecology: a

mathematical perspective (eds M. A. Lewis, P. K. Maini &S. Petrovskii). Springer. Q10

42 Cuddington, K. & Yodzis, P. 2002 Predator–preydynamics and movement in fractal environments. Am.

Nat. 160, 119–134. (doi:10.1086/340611)43 Mols, C., van Oers, K., Witjes, L., Lessells, C., Drent, P. &

Visser, M. 2004 Central assumptions of predator–preymodels fail in a semi-natural experimental system.Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B 271(Suppl. 3), S85–S87. (doi:10.1098/rsbl.2003.0110)

44 Travis, J. & Palmer, S. 2005 Spatial processes can deter-mine the relationship between prey encounter rate andprey density. Biol. Lett. 1, 136–138. (doi:10.1098/rsbl.2004.0293)

Linear features and predator movement H. W. McKenzie et al. 11

Interface Focus (2012)

1261

1262

1263

1264

1265

1266

1267

1268

1269

1270

1271

1272

1273

1274

1275

1276

1277

1278

1279

1280

1281

1282

1283

1284

1285

1286

1287

1288

1289

1290

1291

1292

1293

1294

1295

1296

1297

1298

1299

1300

1301

1302

1303

1304

1305

1306

1307

1308

1309

1310

1311

1312

1313

1314

1315

1316

1317

1318

1319

1320

1321

1322

1323

1324

1325

1326

1327

1328

1329

1330

1331

1332

1333

1334

1335

1336

1337

1338

1339

1340

1341

1342

1343

1344

1345

1346

1347

1348

1349

1350

1351

1352

1353

1354

1355

1356

1357

1358

1359

1360

1361

1362

1363

1364

1365

1366

1367

1368

1369

1370

1371

1372

1373

1374

1375

1376

1377

1378

1379

1380

1381

1382

1383

1384

1385

1386

ARTICLE IN PRESS

rsfs20110086—23/12/11—21:27–Copy Edited by: Mahalakshmi S.

Page 14: ReportNumber12/02 · 2012-12-11 · ReportNumber12/02 Howlinear features alter predatormovementandthe functional response by Hannah W. McKenzie1, EvelynH. Merrill, RaymondJ. Spiteri

45 McPhee, H. M., Webb, N. F. & Merrill, E. H. 2011Time-to-kill: measuring attack rates in a heterogeneouslandscape with multiple prey types. Oikos. (doi:10.1111/j.1600-0706.2011.20203.x)Q11

46 Cagnacci, F., Boitani, L., Powell, R. A. & Boyce, M. S. 2010Theme issue ‘challenges and opportunities of using GPS-based location data in animal ecology’ preface. Phil.

Trans. R. Soc. B 365, 2155. (doi:10.1098/rstb.2010.0098)47 Morales, J., Haydon, D., Frair, J., Holsiner, K. & Fryxell,

J. 2004 Extracting more out of relocation data: buildingmovement models as mixtures of random walks. Ecology85, 2436–2445. (doi:10.1890/03-0269)

48 Fryxell, J. M. et al. 2008 Multiple movement modes bylarge herbivores at multiple spatiotemporal scales. Proc.

Natl Acad. Sci. USA 105, 19 114–19 119. (doi:10.1073/pnas.0801737105)

49 Van Moorter, B., Visscher, D., Benhamou, S., Boerger, L.,Boyce, M. S. & Gaillard, J. M. 2009 Memory keeps youat home: a mechanistic model for home range emergence.Oikos. 118, 641–652. (doi:10.1111/j.1600-0706.2008.17003.x)

50 del Mar Delgado, M., Penteriani, V., Nams, V. O. &Campioni, L. 2009 Changes of movement patterns fromearly dispersal to settlement. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 64,35–43. (doi:10.1007/s00265-009-0815-5)

51 Nathan, R. 2008 An emerging movement ecology para-digm. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 105, 19 050–19 051.(doi:10.1073/pnas.0808918105)

12 Linear features and predator movement H. W. McKenzie et al.

Interface Focus (2012)

1387

1388

1389

1390

1391

1392

1393

1394

1395

1396

1397

1398

1399

1400

1401

1402

1403

1404

1405

1406

1407

1408

1409

1410

1411

1412

1413

1414

1415

1416

1417

1418

1419

1420

1421

1422

1423

1424

1425

1426

1427

1428

1429

1430

1431

1432

1433

1434

1435

1436

1437

1438

1439

1440

1441

1442

1443

1444

1445

1446

1447

1448

1449

1450

1451

1452

1453

1454

1455

1456

1457

1458

1459

1460

1461

1462

1463

1464

1465

1466

1467

1468

1469

1470

1471

1472

1473

1474

1475

1476

1477

1478

1479

1480

1481

1482

1483

1484

1485

1486

1487

1488

1489

1490

1491

1492

1493

1494

1495

1496

1497

1498

1499

1500

1501

1502

1503

1504

1505

1506

1507

1508

1509

1510

1511

1512

ARTICLE IN PRESS

rsfs20110086—23/12/11—21:27–Copy Edited by: Mahalakshmi S.

Page 15: ReportNumber12/02 · 2012-12-11 · ReportNumber12/02 Howlinear features alter predatormovementandthe functional response by Hannah W. McKenzie1, EvelynH. Merrill, RaymondJ. Spiteri
Page 16: ReportNumber12/02 · 2012-12-11 · ReportNumber12/02 Howlinear features alter predatormovementandthe functional response by Hannah W. McKenzie1, EvelynH. Merrill, RaymondJ. Spiteri

RECENT REPORTS52/11 Image Inpainting based on coherence transport with Adapted dis-

tance functionsMarz

53/11 Surface growth kinematics via local curve evolution MoultonGoriely

54/11 A multiple scales approach to evaporation induced Marangoniconvection

HennesseyMunch

55/11 The dynamics of bistable liquid crystal wells LuoMajumdarErban

56/11 Real-Time Fluid Effects on Surfaces using the Closest PointMethod

AuerMacdonaldTreibSchneiderWestermann

57/11 Isolating intrinsic noise sources in a stochastic genetic switch Newby

58/11 Riemann-Cartan Geometry of Nonlinear Dislocation Mechanics YavariGoriely

59/11 Helices through 3 or 4 points? GorielyNeukirchHausrath

60/11 Bayesian data assimilation in shape registration CotterCotterVialard

61/11 Asymptotic solution of a model for bilayer organic diodes andsolar cells

RichardsonPleaseKirkpatrick

62/11 Neural field model of binocular rivalry waves BressloffWebber

63/11 Front propagation in stochastic neural fields BressloffWebber

64/11 Stability estimates for a twisted rod under terminal loads: a three-dimensional study

MajumdarPriorGoriely

65/11 Adaptive Finite Element Method Assisted by Stochastic Simula-tion of Chemical Systems

CotterVejchodskyErban

66/11 On the shape of force–free field lines in the solar corona PriorBerger

67/11 Tear film thickness variations and the role of the tear meniscus PleaseFulfordFulfordCollins

68/11 Comment on “Frequency-dependent dispersion in porous media” Davit

Page 17: ReportNumber12/02 · 2012-12-11 · ReportNumber12/02 Howlinear features alter predatormovementandthe functional response by Hannah W. McKenzie1, EvelynH. Merrill, RaymondJ. Spiteri

69/11 Molecular Tilt on Monolayer-Protected Nanoparticles GiomiBowickMaMajumdar

70/11 The Capillary Interaction Between Two Vertical Cylinders CoorayCicutaVella

71/11 Nonuniqueness in a minimal model for cell motility GallimoreWhiteleyWatersKingOliver

72/11 Symmetry of uniaxial global Landau-de Gennes minimizers in thetheory of nematic liquid crystals

HenaoMajumdar

73/11 Filling of a Poisson trap by a population of random intermittentsearchers

BressloffNewby

01/12 Mechanical growth and morphogenesis of seashells MoultonGorielyChirat

Copies of these, and any other OCCAM reports can be obtainedfrom:

Oxford Centre for Collaborative Applied MathematicsMathematical Institute

24 - 29 St Giles’Oxford

OX1 3LBEngland

www.maths.ox.ac.uk/occam