11
Journal  o]  Personality  an d  Social Psychology 1976, Vol.  33, No. 2,  199-208 Multidimensional Locus of Control in Sociopolitical Activists of  Conservative and Liberal  Ideologies Hanna  Levenson  and Jim  Miller Texas  A &M  University Three studies were conducted  to  examine  th e  relationship between  a  multi- dimensional  measure  o f  locus  of  control  an d  sociopolitical activism, controlling for  th e  effects  of  political ideology.  In  Study  1, 98  male college students com- pleted a measure of conservatism-liberalism,  Kerpelman's  Activism scale, and locus  of  control scales designed  to  differentiate  between  tw o  types  o f  externals: belief  in  powerful  others versus  belief  in  chance  forces.  In  Studies  2 and 3, female  college students  N s  ~  26 and 40) who  differed  in the extent of their participation in  leftist  political activities or feminist causes responded to the multidimensional locus  of  control scales.  A s  predicted, results  from  analyses  of variance and trend analyses indicate  that  for liberals, increases in  expectancies of  control  b y  powerful  others  a re  positively associated with increases  in ac- tivism, while for conservatives, there is a negative relationship. The importance of  controlling  fo r  ideology  and the  implications  of the  differentiated  view  of externality for  understanding social action  are  discussed. The purpose of this study is  twofold:  ( a ) to  provide  data  relevant  to a  multidimen- sional (yet generalized-expectancy) delinea- tion  o f  locus  o f  control  and (b) to  examine th e  relationship between locus  o f  control  a n d social-political  activism, controlling for the effects  o f  political ideology. Rotter  (1966), with  his Internal-External  (I-E)  Scale,  has at- tempted to measure the extent to which peo- ple perceive  that  events are contingent upon their  o wn  behavior (internally controlled)  o r the degree to which they  feel  that  events are beyond their  o w n  control  and are  determined b y  fate,  chance,  o r  powerful  others (exter- nally  controlled).  Despite the  fact  that  over 1,000 studies have been published dealing with the construct, findings relating locus of control  to involvement in voluntary social action activities have been  conflicting  a n d A  preliminary version  o f  Study  1 was  presented at the meeting of the Southwestern Psychological Association,  E l  Paso, Texas,  M a y  1974.  T he  authors would  like  to  thank Alice  H .  Eagly  a n d  Lester Shine  fo r  their suggestions regarding  th e  analysis of  data. Jim Miller is now at the Department of Social Rehabilitation  a n d  Control, Southern  Pines,  North Carolina 28387. Requests  fo r  reprints should  b e  sent  to  Hanna Levenson, Department  of  Psychology, Texas  A& M University,  College  Station,  Texas  77843. confusing.  For example, according to Rotter's theory, internals should  b e  more likely  to participate  in  social action because they  be - lieve their behavior  would  bring about de- sired  goals. Externals,  on the  other hand, should  not become involved because they perceive little connection between their be- havior  and desired outcomes. Studies con- ducted  during the early 1960s have supported this reasoning. Gore  a n d  Rotter (1963)  an d Strickland  (1965)  found  that  black youths w ho  were willing to participate in or who h a d  actually engaged  in  civil rights activities held  more internal control expectancies than their less active black peers. Some  researchers, however, have  found that  the I-E  scale scores  are not  significantly related to activism (Blanchard & Scarboro, 1972;  Evans  &  Alexander, 1970; Gootnick, 1974).  In  addition, other investigators have found  that  externals  a re  more politically  a c - tive  than  internals.  A  relationship between black activism  a nd  externality  w as  shown  in a  study  b y  Ransford  (1968), and  Sanger  a n d Alker  (1972)  found  that  feminist  activists scored more externally on a dimension of Protestant ethic ideology than  a  control group. Why should people become involved if  they  feel  they have no mastery over the situation? 19 9

Multidimensional Locus of Control Attitude Scale Levenson Miller 1976 JPSP

Embed Size (px)

Citation preview

Page 1: Multidimensional Locus of Control Attitude Scale Levenson Miller 1976 JPSP

8/10/2019 Multidimensional Locus of Control Attitude Scale Levenson Miller 1976 JPSP

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/multidimensional-locus-of-control-attitude-scale-levenson-miller-1976-jpsp 1/10

Journal  o]  Personality  an d  Social Psychology

1976, Vol.  33, No. 2,  199-208

Multidimensional Locus of Control in Sociopolitical Activists

of

  Conservative and Liberal

  Ideologies

Hanna

  Levenson  and Jim

  Miller

Texas  A&M  University

Three studies were conducted

  to

  examine

  th e

  relationship between

  a

  multi-

dimensional  measure  of  locus  of  control  an d  sociopolitical activism, controlling

for

  the  effects  of

  political ideology.

  In

  Study

  1, 98

  male college students com-

pleted a measure of conservatism-liberalism,  Kerpelman's  Activism scale, and

locus  of  control scales designed  to  differentiate  between  tw o  types  of  externals:

belief

  in

  powerful  others versus  belief

  in

  chance  forces.

  In

  Studies

  2 and 3,

female  college students  N s ~  26 and 40) who  differed  in the extent of their

participation in  leftist  political activities or feminist causes responded to the

multidimensional locus  of  control scales.  As  predicted, results  from  analyses  of

variance and trend analyses indicate  that  for liberals, increases in

  expectancies

of

  control

  by  powerful

  others

  are

  positively associated with increases

  in ac-

tivism, while for conse rvatives , there is a negative relationship. The imp ortance

of  controlling

  fo r

  ideology

  and the

  implications

  of the

  differentiated  view

  of

externality for  understanding social action  are  discussed.

The purpose of this study is  twofold:  (a)

to

  provide  data  relevant  to a  mult idimen-

sional (yet generalized-expectancy) delinea-

tion  of  locus  of  control  and (b) to  examine

th e

  relationship between locus

  of

  control

  and

social-political  activism, con trollin g for the

effects

  of

  political ideology. Rotter

  (1966) ,

with his Internal-External

 (I-E)

  Scale, has at-

tempted to measure the extent to which peo-

ple perceive  that  events are contingent upon

their

  ow n  behavior (internally controlled)  or

the degree to which they  feel

  that

  events are

beyond their

  own

  control

  and are

  determined

by  fate,  chance,  or

  powerful

  others (exter-

nally  controlled).  Despite the  fact  that  over

1,000 studies have been published dealing

with the co nstruct, findings relating locus of

control  to involvement in voluntary social

action activities have been

  conflicting

  and

A

  preliminary version

  of

  Study

  1 was

  presented

at the meeting of the Southwestern Psychological

Association,

  El  Paso, Texas,  M ay  1974.  The  authors

would  like  to  thank Alice  H.  Eagly  and  Lester

Shine  fo r  their suggestions regarding  th e  analysis

of

  data.

Jim Miller is now at the Department of Social

Rehabilitation

  and

  Control , Southern  Pines,  N o r t h

Carolina 28387.

Requests

  fo r

  reprints should

  be

  sent

  to

  Hanna

Levenson, Department  of  Psychology, Texas  A&M

University,

  College

  Station,  Texas  77843.

confusing.  For example, according to Rotter's

theory, internals should  be  more likely  to

participate  in  social action because they  be-

lieve their behavior  would  bring about de-

sired

  goals. Externals,

  on the

  other hand,

should  no t becom e involved because they

perceive little connection between their be-

havior  and desired outcomes. Studies con-

ducted  d ur in g the early 1960s have suppo rted

this reasoning. Gore

  and

  Rotter (1963)

  and

Strickland  (1965)  found

  that

  black youths

who   were willing to participate in or who

had  actually engaged  in  civil rights activities

held

  more internal control expectancies than

their less active black peers.

Some

  researchers, however, have

  found

that  the I-E  scale scores are not sign ificantly

related to activism (B lanc har d & Scarboro,

1972;  Evans  &  Alexander, 1970; Gootnick,

1974).

  In

  addition, other investigators have

found  that  externals

  are

  more politically

  ac-

tive  than  internals.  A  relationship between

black activism

  and

  externality

  was

  shown

  in

a

  study

  by

  Ransford

  (1968), and

  Sanger

  and

Alker

  (1972)  found  that  feminist  activists

scored m ore externally on a dimension of

Protestant ethic ideology than  a  control

group. Why should people become involved

if

  they  feel  they have no mastery over the

situation?

199

Page 2: Multidimensional Locus of Control Attitude Scale Levenson Miller 1976 JPSP

8/10/2019 Multidimensional Locus of Control Attitude Scale Levenson Miller 1976 JPSP

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/multidimensional-locus-of-control-attitude-scale-levenson-miller-1976-jpsp 2/10

 

HANNA LEVENSON  AND JIM  MILLER

Changing  Times

  and  Confounding Variables

There appear

  to be two

  related explana-

tions

  of the

  failure

  of the

  Rotter

  I-E

  control

scale  to  predict social activism;  one is  based

on the

  change

  in the

  nature

  of

 social activism

in  this country  and in the  expectancies • of

people who do become  involved, and the

 other

is  based  on the  confounding  of  variables  on

the I-E  scale (Hersch  &  Scheibe, 1967; Joe,

1971;

  Lefcourt ,  1972) .

When

  the I-E  scale  was  developed,  em-

pirical  data  supported  th e  unidimensional

nature

  of the

  items. However, results

  of re-

cent

  factor

  analytic studies (Collins, 1974 ;

Kleiber,

  Veldman,

 &

  Menaker, Note

  1)

  have

indicated  the  presence  of  several factors  on

the

  I-E

  scale. Since there

  is a

  10-year time

span between

  the

  development

  of the I-E

scale  and  recent evidence

  that

  indicates  its

multidimensionali ty,

  one cannot overlook the

possibility  that  th e  seeming discrepancy  is a

function  of the  changing times.  The  social

action activism

  of the

  early  1960s  might have

been  an  outgrowth  of a  belief

  that

  a person

could

  make

  meaningful

  changes in society

through

  information

  and

  increased  effort

(high  belief  in  personal

  control) .

  However,

any changes usually occurred gradually, and

many students began  to  realize  the  power of

th e  system to   control outcomes (high  be-

lief  in system control) .  Thus,  it is not sur-

prising  that  those

  w ho

  participated

  in the

more  violent protests of the  late 1960s  could

have rejected  the  view  of  internal control.

In  addition  to the  changing times explana-

tion, another reason  w hy  researchers have

not  found  consistent relationships between

scores  on the I-E  scale  and  activism may lie

in

  th e

  format

  and  conceptualization  of the

scale. Unfortunately, because  of the  forced-

choice format  of the I-E  scale,  rejection  of

the internal items results

  in a

  high external

score,  which

  is

  denned

  as a

  belief  that  events

are   controlled  by  fate,  chance,  or  powerful

others. Frequently, how ever, the expectancy

of

  control

  by

  powerful

  others

  is not

  taken

into

  consideration in interpreting the results.

When activists score in an external direction,

they are

  often

  interpreted as alienated indi-

viduals

  whose rioting and protesting behav-

iors are seen as noninstrumental expressions

of

  hostili ty (e.g. , Rotter,

  1971).  The

  global

definition of

  externals might obscure

  th e

importance

  of

  perceptions

  of

  powerful  others

(system control)  for  understanding  the in-

s t rumental i ty  of

  protest behavior.

It was  reasoned  that the  multidimensional

scales developed

  by

  Levenson

  (1974)

  would

be

  useful

  in analyzing the relationship be-

tween

  locus

  of

  control

  an d

  activism. Leven-

son constructed the Internal,  Powerful  Oth-

ers, and Chance scales in order to measure

belief  in

  chance

  or

  fate  expectancies

  as

  sepa-

rate  from

  a  powerful

  others orientation.

  The

rationale behind

  differentiating

  tw o  types  of

externals stemmed  from  the reasoning  that

people who

  believe

  that  the

  world

  is un-

ordered would behave

  and

  think

  differently

from  people who believe

  that

  the world is

ordered but  that

  powerful

  others are in con-

trol.

  In the

  latter  case

  a

  potential

  fo r

  control

exists.

Previous work with

  th e

  Internal,  Powerful

Others,

  and

  Chance scales

  has led to a  fuller

unders tanding

  of the  phenomenological vari-

ables involved in perceived parental upbring-

ing,

  psychopathological diagnosis, clinical im-

provement, membership in antipollution

groups,

  imprisonment,

  and

  academic

  perform-

ance (Levenson, 1973a, 1973b, 1974,

 in

  press;

Prociuk  &  Breen,  197S).  The  authors  hy-

pothesize that the  powerful  others dimension

of

  the

  tripartite  differentiation

  of

  locus

  of

control  will  be  meaningfully  related to ac-

tivism, especially  in  view  of  past  research,

which can be interpreted as relating perceived

discrimination against

  oneself

  (system con-

trol)

  and

  activism.

Political

  Ideology

In  addition  to the  changing times  and the

conceptual weaknesses within

  the I-E

  scale,

another consideration  that

  may

  explain con-

flicting

  data relating locus

  of

  control

  to

activism

  is the

  confounding

  of

  political

  in-

volvement

  and

  political ideology. Kerpelman

(1969) and

  Lewis  and  Kraut

  (1972)

  have

pointed

  out

  that

  many conclusions relating

personality  differences

  to

  political activism

are based on sudies in which either liberal

activists

  were compared  to  liberal  an d  con-

servative nonactivists or very active liberals

were  compared to slightly active conserva-

Page 3: Multidimensional Locus of Control Attitude Scale Levenson Miller 1976 JPSP

8/10/2019 Multidimensional Locus of Control Attitude Scale Levenson Miller 1976 JPSP

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/multidimensional-locus-of-control-attitude-scale-levenson-miller-1976-jpsp 3/10

LOCUS

  OF  CONTROL IN  SOCIOPOLITICAL ACTIVISTS

201

tives.  Most  of the  studies relating locus  of

control

  to

  activism  fall  into

  th e  former

  cate-

gory, with

  no

  attempt made

  to

  control

  fo r

political ideology.

  In

  fact,  there

  has

  been

a

  tendency

  in

  psychological research

  in

  gen-

eral to equate activism with involvement in

leftist  causes. How ever, since there are pro -

test  groups  of  conservative ideologies, activ-

ism   should  be denned  independently  of po-

litical  philosophy.  Neglect  of ideological dif-

ferences

  is a  serious shortcoming  for  locus of

control research, since  it has  been shown

 that

internal items  on the

  I E

  scale  are  more

appealing  to

  conservatives (Thomas,  L970)

an d  to  those supporting  a Protestant  ethic

philosophy (Mirels

  &

  Garret t ,

  1971).

Abramowitz  (1973 )  has  been  one of the

few  researchers to attempt to analyze scores

on  the I-E  scale  as a

  function

  of  activism

and  political ideology

  (left ,

  middle , r ight) .

He  found  that  left  activists had higher ex-

ternal scores than right activists. However,

interpretation of

  these data

  is

  complicated

by the

  fact  that

  A bram ow itz was not able to

control for activism level across the political

ideologies; in his study

  left

  activists were

significantly more  politically  active  than  the

right activists (mean Activism scale score of

4 1 . 2   versus  25.S) .

From   the  foregoing  review  of  relevant stud-

ies,  it

  appears  that

  the

  relationship

  between

activism

  and  locus  of  control might  be  clari-

fied  if  locus  of  control scores  of  groups  that

were  equated  fo r  activism  and  political ide-

ology were compared  and if

  differentiated

measures

  of

  externality were used.

  The

  pur-

pose of this study is to examine the relation-

ship between Internal,  Powerful  Others,

  and

Chance scale scores

  and  social-political  ac-

tivism, controlling for political ideology.

Often-cited  investigations on activism and

apathy

  in adolescents (e.g., Block,

  Haan,

  &

Smith, 1968; Flacks, 1967; Keniston, 1970)

have

  emphasized

  the

  importance

  of  percep-

tions  of  powerful  others. Conservatives  are

seen as those who accept traditional Ameri-

can

  values

  and

  authority, while

  the

  liberal

activists

  are

  described

  as

  those

  who

  reject

policies and institutions

  that

  conflict  with

their

  egalitarian view

  of

  political participa-

tion.

It  appears  that  most  of the  work done on

activism   and  political ideology indicates  that

atti tudes toward authority  are of  crucial  im -

portance.  It was  reasoned  that  the  locus of

control dimension  of  expectancy  for  control

by

  powerful

  others would

  be

  differentially

related to activism depending upon one's

political ideology.  It was  expected  that  the

more  liberals perceived  that  powerful  others

play

  a

  major role

  in

  controlling their lives,

the  more activist they would become,  be-

cause the power would be perceived as in-

hibiting realization

  of

  their potentials.

  On

th e

  other hand,  it was  predicted  that  con-

servatives would  see power  as legitimate  and,

therefore,  increases

  in

  expectancies

  of

  control

by

  powerful

  others would  be  associated with

decreases

  in

  activism levels. Specifically,

  it

was hypothesized  that  activist liberals would

have significantly higher

  Powerful

  Others

scale scores than

  the

  activist  conservatives.

One  main study and two corollary investiga-

tions  were  conducted.

STUDY   1

Method

Subjects  and

  Procedure

Although  scores

  on

  self-report  measures  were  used

to  divide  subjects  into groups according  to  political

ideology and  activism,  an  attempt  was  made  to

include

  students

  who were  highly  active in conserva-

tive and

  liberal  causes

  in the

  testing

  procedure.  In

order to accomplish  this,  th e  presidents of the senior

class  and/or  representatives of the  student

  senate

  at

four  large  state  universities  in the  Southwest  pro-

vided  the  present

  authors

  with  the  names  of  stu-

dents

  who  were  most

  involved

  in

  l e f t

  an d  right

political activities. These  informants were  told  the

nature

  of the

  research

  in very

  general

  terms,

  an d

they  were assured  that  th e  students ' names  would

be

  kept  confidential.

  Since

  very

  few

  females  were

identified  by  these  student leaders, data  were  gath-

ered only  for  males.  By  obtaining  the  names  of

activist

  subjects

  in  this

  manner

  n

 =

 5l),  we hoped

to  test  a  number  of  conservative  activists  who  were

just  as  active  as  their  liberal  counterparts, since  the

inability  to  equate  activism  levels  between  the two

ideologies has been a serious shortcoming in most

studies  (Kerpelman,  1969).

These  identified  activist  students  were  either  ap -

proached individually  in the universities' student

government

  o f f i c e s

  or

  they

  were telephoned

  an d

asked to  meet  the  researcher  to  discuss  possible

participation  in a research  project.  All

  students

  who

were

  able to be  contacted  (« = 48) agreed to par-

ticipate.

  No

  mention

  was

  made

  of

  political

 ideology

or of  student activism  in  recruiting  the  subjects.

They  were  instructed

  to

  complete

 a

  number

  of  self-

Page 4: Multidimensional Locus of Control Attitude Scale Levenson Miller 1976 JPSP

8/10/2019 Multidimensional Locus of Control Attitude Scale Levenson Miller 1976 JPSP

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/multidimensional-locus-of-control-attitude-scale-levenson-miller-1976-jpsp 4/10

202

H A N N A

  LEVENSON  AND JIM  MILLER

report measures  in  private  and to  return them  to

th e

  second

  au thor ,  a

  male graduate s tudent ,

  who

projected the appearance of neither a right-wing

reactionary nor a left-wing radical.

In   order  to  obtain subjects  n  =

  SO )

  w ho  would

probably report less involvement on the self-report

activism measure, several classes

  at the  four

  uni-

versities used above were tested

  by the

  same male

researcher. The average age for the identified  activ-

ists was 23 years;

  that

  of the remaining sample

was 22 years. To avoid the possibility of

  confound-

ing race with political ideology, all subjects

  n —

98)

  were

  white.  All  data  were collected  in the

spring  of  1973.

Instruments

Locus

  of

  control.

  The  self-report measures  for

assessing locus of control were Levenson's (1974)

Internal,  Powerfu l  Others,

  an d

  Chance scales, each

of  which

  is

  comprised

  of

  eight items

  in a

  Likert

fo rma t  (possible range  on  each scale =  0-48).  T he

three scales were derived

  on an a

  priori basis

  from

several items adapted  from  Rotter 's  I-E  scale

  and

a set of statements written  specifically  for the new

scales. While  previous  studies  have found  slight  to

moderate correlations between the

  Powerfu l

  Others

and

  Chance scales (.23

  to .59), the

  internal, power-

fu l  others, and chance orientations were the first

three clusters

  to

  emerge

  from factor

  analyses

  on

data  from  normal (Levenson, 1974) and psychiatric

samples (Levenson, 1973a). These

  factors

  seem

  to

be

  conceptually pure

  in

  that  only items  from

  th e

appropriate scale  load  on  that  one  factor. Table  1

contains

  th e

  items

  that

  were presented

  to the

  sub-

jects as a

  unified

  attitude  scale of 24 items. The

statements attempt

  to

  measure

  th e

  degree

  to

  which

a subject perceives the events in his own  life  as

being  a  consequence  of his own acts,  under  the

control of  powerfu l  others, or determined by chance

forces.

T he

  three

  new

  scales  differ

  from

  Rotter's

  I- E

scale  in

  four

  important  ways:  (a)  Instead  of a

forced-choice

  format,

  a

  Likert 6-point scale

  is

  used,

so

  that  th e  three scales  are  statistically indepen dent

of  one ano ther , (b) The statements on the scales

are

  worded  in the first  person (personal control)

rather  than  mixing  first-person  with third-person

(system control) phrasing. This distinction  w as

made based on the factor analyses of Gurin, Gurin,

Lao,  and

  Beattie (1969)

  and

  Mirels (1970).

  (c )

The

  scales have

  a

  high degree

  of

  parallelism

  in

  con-

tent among each

  triad,

  (d) The  scales  are not  cor-

related  with  a  measure  of  social desirability.  For

th e

  present sample,

  Cronbach's

  alpha

  is .77 for the

Internal scale, .71 for the Powerful Others scale,

and .73 for the Chance scale.

Political  activity.  Social-political involvement

  was

measured  by  Kerpelman's (1969) Actual Activism

subscale of the Political Activity scale. The Actual

Activism subscale is comprised of 12

  Likert-type

i tems  that  ask how  much time  has  been spent  in

var ious

  activist pursuits (range = 12-60).  In the

T A B L E  1

I N T E R N A L , P O W E R F U L

  O T H E R S ,  A N D  C H A N C E  Locus

O F   C O N T R O L   S C A L E   ITEMS

Internal scale

1.

  Whether

 or not I get

  to

 be a

  leader depends mostly

on   my ability.

4. Whether  or not I get  into  a car  accident depends

mostly

 on how

 good

  a

  driver

  I am.

5.  When  I  make plans,  I am  almost  certain  to  make

them work.

9. How  many  friends  I  have depends  on how  nice  a

person I am.

18. I can

  pretty much determine what

  will

  happen

  in

my

  life.

19 .

  I am

  usually able

  to

 protect

  m y

 personal  interests.

21 .

  When

  I get

  what

  I

  want, it's usually because

  I

worked  hard for it.

23. My  life  is determined by my own actions.

Powerful

  others scale

3. I  feel

  like

  what happens in my  life  is mostly deter-

mined by  powerful  people.

8.

  Although

  I

  might have good

  ability,  I  will  not be

given leadership responsibility without appealing

to

  those

  in

 positions

  of

 power.

11. My  life  is

  chiefly

  controlled b y

  powerful

  others.

13 .

  People like

  myself

  have very little chance

  of

  pro-

tecting

  ou r

 personal interests when

 they

  conflict

with  those  of  strong pressure groups.

15. Getting what

  I

  w ant requir es pleasing those people

above

  me.

17.  If important people were to decide they didn't like

m e,

  I  probably wouldn't make many

  friends.

20.

  Whether

  or not I get

  into

  a car

  accident depends

mostly on the other driver.

22 .  In  order  to  have  m y  plans work,  I  make sure  that

they fit in with the desires of people who have

power  over

 m e.

Chance scale

2.  To a great extent my

 life

 is controlled by

 accidental

happenings.

6.

  Often there

  is no

 chance

 of

 protecting

  my

 personal

interest  from  bad luck happenings.

7.  When  I get what  I  want, it's usually because  I'm

lucky.

10 .

  I  have  often found  that  what  is  going  to  happen

will  happen.

12.

  Whether

  or not I get

  into

  a car

  accident

  is

 mostly

a  matter  of luck.

14 .  It's  no t  always wise  for me to  plan  too far  ahead

because m any things tur n out to b e a m atte r of

good or bad  fortune.

16 .

  Whether  or not I get to be a  leader depends  on

whether

  I'm

  lucky enough

  to be in the

  right

place

  at the

  right time.

24. It's

  chiefly  a

  matter

  of fate

  whether

  or not I

  have

a few

 freiends

  or many

  friends.

Note.  Al l

  24  locus  of  control i tems  (8 for  each scale)  ar e

included

  in the table. The n um berin g represents the orde r in

which  th e

  items appeared

  to

  part ic ipants

  in the

  study.

present investigation,

  th e

  behaviorally

  identified

student activists scored more than one standard

deviation higher on the Activism subscale than the

classroom-tested  subjects  p <  .001),  even though

several  self-identified  activists were  in the  latter

Page 5: Multidimensional Locus of Control Attitude Scale Levenson Miller 1976 JPSP

8/10/2019 Multidimensional Locus of Control Attitude Scale Levenson Miller 1976 JPSP

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/multidimensional-locus-of-control-attitude-scale-levenson-miller-1976-jpsp 5/10

LOCUS

  OF

  CONTROL

  IN

  SOCIOPOLITICAL ACTIVISTS

  3

TABLE

  2

MEANS,

  S T A N D A R D D E V I A T IO N S ,

  AND CELL «s

  F O R

  THE

  A C T I V I S M

  X

  I D E O L O G Y  CELLS  S T U D Y

  1)

Measure

Activism

Conservatives

Liberals

Conservatism-liberalism

Conservatives

Liberals

Cell ns

Conservatives

Liberals

Lo w

M

18.06

1 9 . 7 5

98.29

53.38

17

8

Activis

Modera te

SD

2.70

1.75

19.60

12.50

M

25.54

25.43

87.92

53.50

13

14

SD

1.81

1.91

1 6 . 2 7

9.76

m

  level

High

M

32.40

33.00

98.67

48.50

9

14

SD

3.24

3.11

18.40

9.80

Very

  high

M

43.67

4 7 . 2 1

97.22

44.07

SD

4.58

5 . 2 7

24.32

10.48

9

14

group.

  For the

  present sample,

  th e

  alpha  coefficient

for

  th e  Actual Activism subscale  is

  .91.

  Scores  on

th e

  Activism subscale provided

  th e

  basis

  fo r

  assign-

ing subjects to activism groups.

Political ideology.  The m easu re of political ideol-

ogy was

  constructed

  specifically  fo r

  this study, since

it was  felt

  that

  little

  informat ion

  on criterion valid-

ity was available for existing ideology scales and

that many items contained in older scales were not

appropriate

  in

  1973.

  The

  Conservatism-Liberalism

scale is comprised of  25  Likert-type items, and its

alpha reliability  is .92  (Levenson  &  Miller, 1974).

A

  high score indicates a conservative view on topics

such as drugs, national

  defense,

  censorship, and

welfare  (range =

  25-175).

  In the  present study  the

behaviorally identified conservative activists scored

almost

  two

  standard deviations higher than their

behaviorally

  identified

  liberal activist peers  p <

.001).

  Scores

  on the

  Conservatism-Liberalism scale

formed  the basis for dich otom izing subjects into

either conservative

  or

  liberal ideology groups.

In the  present study,  the  Conservatism-Liberal-

ism scale, the locus of control scales, and the Actu al

Activism scale were combined into

  a

  booklet

  an d

presented  to the  subject  in  that  order, preceded  by

a cover sheet requesting certain demographic

  infor-

mation

  and

  containing

  the

 statement,

  If you

  would

like  to

  obtain

  a

  copy

  of the

  results

  at the

  com-

pletion of  this  project, please give me your name

after

  you are finished and I  will  be  glad  to  send

you  a  copy.

Results

Design

It was

  decided

  to use a

  two-way factorial

design  to  test  the  presence  of the  predicted

interaction

  effect

  between activism and ide-

ology in predicting locus of control scores.

Subjects  who  scored above  the  median (68)

on

  the  Con servatism-Liberalism scale consti-

tuted  the  conservative group  (« = 49) and

those

  who

  scored below

  the

  median repre-

sented  the  liberal group

  (w

 =  49).  Subjects

were

  also

  divided into quartiles

  based

  on

their Activism

  scale

  scores. Since

  past

  re-

search  has  indicated  that  activism  is  posi-

tively related  to a  liberal  political  ideology,

it was  considered extremely important  to

make sure

  that

  the

  various levels

  of

  activism

between conservative

  and

  liberal students

were

  equivalent and to control for ideology

across  the  activism levels  before  analyzing

the locus of control scores.  Table  2 presents

the

  mean scores

  and

  standard deviations

  on

the  Conservatism-Liberalism  and the  Activ-

ism

  scales

  for

  each

  of the

  cells

  in the 2 X 4

grouping.

Two

  2 X 4  analyses

  of

  variance  indicated

a

  significant interaction

  on the

  scores

  on the

Activism  scale  and on the  Conservatism-Lib-

eralism

  scale.

  Analysis

  fo r

  simple

  effects

  indi-

cated  that

  for the

  highest level

  of

  activism,

liberals were significantly more active

  than

conservatives;  these liberals also scored sig-

nificantly  more liberal  than

  two

  other groups

of  liberals.  Therefore,

  th e

  subjects

  in the

highest level  of  activism were excluded

  from

the analysis of locus of control scores. The

remaining  2 X 3  (Ideology X Activism) de -

sign

  was

  considered most appropriate

  to

  test

the  hypotheses  for the  following  reasons:  (a)

In all

  cells ideology

  and

  activism were

equated, thus permitt ing  a  test  of the  pre-

dicted interaction

  on

  locus

  of

  control

  scale

scores,  (b) The  data  from  over 75 of the

Page 6: Multidimensional Locus of Control Attitude Scale Levenson Miller 1976 JPSP

8/10/2019 Multidimensional Locus of Control Attitude Scale Levenson Miller 1976 JPSP

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/multidimensional-locus-of-control-attitude-scale-levenson-miller-1976-jpsp 6/10

Page 7: Multidimensional Locus of Control Attitude Scale Levenson Miller 1976 JPSP

8/10/2019 Multidimensional Locus of Control Attitude Scale Levenson Miller 1976 JPSP

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/multidimensional-locus-of-control-attitude-scale-levenson-miller-1976-jpsp 7/10

Page 8: Multidimensional Locus of Control Attitude Scale Levenson Miller 1976 JPSP

8/10/2019 Multidimensional Locus of Control Attitude Scale Levenson Miller 1976 JPSP

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/multidimensional-locus-of-control-attitude-scale-levenson-miller-1976-jpsp 8/10

206

HANNA LEVENSON  AND JIM  M ILLER

The

  behavioral criterion

  for the  liberal

  student

activists was participation in a small lesbian group

that  was

  quite involved

  in

  trying

  to

  change

  sexist

laws

  an d

  attitudes.

  The

  corresponding campus group

was a  relatively inactive chapter  of a  national wom-

en's liberation group. All

  data

  in Study 3 were

collected

  at a coeducational

  college

  in the Southwest.

One of the two

  undergraduate testers

  in

  Study

  2

went to a

  meeting

  of the

  lesbian group  n

  —  12 )

and the

  other went

  to a

  meeting

  of the  feminist

organization

  (» = 28) .  T he

  testers explained

  that

they were interested  in the  attitudes  of  students  on

campus. No mention was made of political ideology,

feminist

  issues,

  or

  activism.

  All

  students present

agreed

  to

  participate,

  and the

  locus

  of

  control scales

were  administered during

  that

  meeting.

Results

As

  predicted,

  the

  students

  in the

  activist

lesbian group scored extremely high

  on the

Powerful  Others

  scale

  M

  =  39.75)  as

  com-

pared  to the

  feminist

  group members  M

 =

15.89), ^(1,38)  =  81.88,

  p <

  .001.  The dif-

ference

  between groups on the Chance scale

is not  significant  F =  .91),  but the

  differ-

ence

  between means

  on the

  Internal  scale

is also highly

  significant.

  The members of the

activist lesbian group

  felt  they  had  less

  per-

sonal control over their lives

  M — 21.67)

than those  in the  feminist  group

  M =

37.39) ;  F(l, 38) =  98.76, p <  .001.

G E N E R A L   D I S C U S S I O N

The

  results

  indicate  that  expectancies of

control  by  powerful  others  appear  quite rele-

vant

  for

  understanding

  the

  activism

  of

  these

conservative  and  liberal students.  The finding

that

  there  is a  differential  relationship  be-

tween activism and locus of control depend-

ing  upon one's political ideology

  can be

described according  to  attribution theory.

Attribution theory describes  the  processes

which operate

  as if  the

  individual  were  moti-

vated

  to

  attain

  a

  cognitive mastery

  of the

causal

  nature

  of the

  environment (Kelley,

1967).  From

  an

  attributional point

  of

  view,

Rotter's

  external control dimension represents

the

 attribution

  of causality  to  external  forces,

while  internal control represents  the  attribu-

tion

  of

  causality

  to

  personal forces.

By

  examining

  an

  attributional model

  of

achievement motivation, Weiner and his col-

leagues  (Weiner,  Heckhausen, Meyer,  &

Cook, 1972) have delineated

  four

  elements

that are contained within two causal dimen-

sions:  locus of control (internal versus ex-

ternal) and  stability  (fixed  versus variable).

Ability and  effort  are perceived as internal

determinants

  of

  success

  and

  failure,  while

luck

  and  task  difficulty  are external causes.

However,

  as

  Weiner

  et

  al.

  point out, ability

and

  task

  difficulty  are relatively constant

over time, while  effort  and luck are more

variable.

  These

 investigators  feel

  that

  changes

in expectancy of success are due to the  sta-

bility  of the  attributional dimension, rather

than  to the

  locus

  of

  control dimension.

Weiner's reasoning for suggesting a Stabil-

ity X

 Locus

  of

  Control classification scheme

is quite similar to the rationale for  differen-

tiating between two types of externals in the

development of the  Powerful  Others  and

Chance

  scales.

  Both of

  these

  scales measure

orientations  that  are  external  to the  indi-

vidual,

  but

  chance implies great variability,

while

  powerful  others could be seen as rela-

tively

  stable

  or  predictable.

  This

  added  di-

mension  of  predictability becomes quite  im -

portant

  fo r

  understanding

  how

  some students

with  an

  external locus

  of

  control could

  en-

gage  in  instrumental activism.  In  such  a  situ-

ation, the potential for change with increased

effort  exists.

  If one

  incorrectly equated

  ex-

ternality with a  view  of an unstable world,

then  one  would probably interpret  the

  acti-

vism

  of  externals  as

  noninstrumental,

  ex-

pressive releases brought about  by  frustra-

tion.

Pertinent to the consequences of causal

attributions, Weiner et al.

  (1972)

  not only

have evidence that changes in expectancy of

success

  are

  related

  to the

  stability factor,

  but

they

  also have shown

  that

  affective  responses

to an

  outcome

  are

  determined primarily

  by

the

  locus

  of

  control dimension

  of

  causality.

This  emphasis  on  affect  is of  interest  in

delineating  differences  between liberals' and

conservatives' attributions  for  negative condi-

tions.

When faced

  with

  situations  in  which pow-

erful  others are believed to control outcomes,

the activism level of the conservative could

be less because the expectancy for success

would  be

  low.

  For

  liberals, however, attribut-

ing  the  causes  for  such negative conditions  as

poverty, sexism, and racism to other people

instead  of to  one's  own inadequacies should

Page 9: Multidimensional Locus of Control Attitude Scale Levenson Miller 1976 JPSP

8/10/2019 Multidimensional Locus of Control Attitude Scale Levenson Miller 1976 JPSP

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/multidimensional-locus-of-control-attitude-scale-levenson-miller-1976-jpsp 9/10

LOCUS  OF  CONTROL  IN  SOCIOPOLITICAL ACTIVISTS

  7

augment activism

  and

  lessen negative

  affect

because, according

  to

  Weiner's conceptuali-

zation,

  th e  external locus  of  causality reduces

personal responsibility  fo r  present  negative

circumstances. Similar reasoning

  has

  been

used

  by

  Gurin

  et

  al.

  (1969)  and

  Sanger

  and

Alker  (1972)  to  explain  w hy  externality  is

positively correlated with activism  fo r  blacks

and  feminists.

Several

  factors  are  important  fo r  deciding

on

  the  generality  of the findings in

  these

three studies. College-educated liberal  activ-

ists  could have scored higher

  on the

  Power-

ful  Others scale because they tended  to  view

authority with suspicion.  However,  there

  are

probably

  several

  groups

  of

  conservative

  ac-

tivists  w ho  might have  a  similar distrust  of

authorities (e.g., Wallace supporters, John

Birch  Society

  members ) .  In

  addition,

  not all

liberal activists

  feel

  they  are  controlled  by

other people

  per se; for

  many, institutional-

ized  barriers  an d  policies  are  seen  as  more

constraining.  Therefore,  it may be  that  the

findings  from

  these studies

  do not

  generalize

to all  activists,  but  depend  on the  specific

external

 targets

 relevant

  to a

  particular group.

Future work

  on the

  relationship between

  the

Powerful

  Others scale

  and

  other types

  of

predictable, controlling

  forces

  (e.g., racism,

poverty) should

  be

  informative.

 I n

  Studies

 2

and 3,  subjects were tested  as  they appeared

for  a  group meeting. Although  th e  experi-

menter  did not  mention political ideology  or

activism,  there

  is a

  possibility  that  responses

to  the

  self-report measures were partially

  a

reflection of the  salience  of  group member-

ship.  It may be  that  people  who knew they

were  being tested because they were members

of

  a

  radical group responded

  in a

  more

paranoid,

  defensive

  manner.

In

  Studies

  1 and 2,

  differences  between

groups were

  significant

  only

  fo r  Powerful

Others

  scale

 scores.  In  Study  3, however, the

lesbians (highly active liberals) scored higher

on

  perceptions

  of

  control

  by  powerful

  others

than  less  active

  feminists,  as

  predicted,

  but

they

  also

  felt

  that

  they

  had

  significantly less

personal control over their lives.  One  might

speculate

  that

  there

  is an

  interplay between

the

  three locus

  of

  control orientations

  as a

function  of

  success

  or

  failure

  in

  achieving

desired

  goals. Although

 the

  lesbians

 had

  been

striving

  to

  change attitudes

  and

  laws, they

perceived  that

  they

  had had little

  success,

and

  therefore

  their  lo w  Internal scale scores

could  be a  reflection  of

  their estimation

  of

their ability

  and/or

  effort.

  If  such  is the

case,

  their

  defensive externality (Minton,

1972)  does  not  seem  to be  alleviating self-

blame.  The  lower Internal scores could

  also

be  an  internalization  of  society's  negative

view  of homosexuals. Since the Internal, Pow-

erful

  Others,  and  Chance scales  are  empiri-

cally independent,

  it

  might

  be  frui t fu l  in

fu ture

  research  to  make predictions based  on

two or

  three

  of the

  locus

  of

  control scale

scores.

REFERENCE  NOTES

1.

  Kleiber,

  D.,

  Veldman,

  D. J., & Menaker,  S. L.

Th e  multidimensionality  of

  locus

  of  control.

Paper  presented  at the  meeting  of the  Eastern

Psychological  Association,  Washington,

  D.C.,

Apri l

  1973.

2.  Barr,

  A.

 J.,

  &

 Goodnight,

 J. H.  Statistical analy-

si s

  system.  Unpublished manuscript, 1972. (Avail-

able  from  Students' Supply Stores, North Caro-

lina State University,

  Raleigh,

  North Carolina

27607) .

REFERENCES

Abramowitz ,  S. I. The  comparative competence-

adjus tment

  of  student

  left

  social-political activists.

Journal

  of

  Personality

1973,

  41,

  244-260.

Appelbaum,

  M.  I.,

  &

 Cramer,

 E.

  M. Some problems

in the  nonorthogonal analysis of  variance.

  Psycho-

logical

  Bulletin,  1974,  81 ,  33S-343.

Blanchard,  E. B., & Scarboro, M. E.  Locus  of  con-

trol,  political attitudes,  and  voting behavior  in a

college  ag e  population.  Psychological Reports

1972,

  30 ,

  529-530.

Block,

  J.

  H.,

  Haan,

  N., &

 Smith,

  M, B.

  Activism

and  apathy  in contemporary adolescents.  In J. F.

A d a m s

  (Ed.),  Understanding adolescence. Boston:

Allyn & Bacon, 1968.

Carlson,

  J.  E., & Timm, N. H.  Analysis of variance.

Psychological  Bulletin,

  1974,

  81,

  563-570.

Collins,  B. E.  Four  components  of the  Rotter  In -

ternal-External Scale:  Belief  in a  difficult  world,

a  just world,  a  predictable world,  and a  politi-

cally

  responsive world.  Journal

  of

  Personality

  an d

Social  Psychology

1974,

  29 ,  381-391.

Evans,  D.  A .,  &  Alexander,  S.  Some psychological

correlates of  civil rights activity.  Psychological

Reports

1970,

 26 ,

  899-906.

Flacks,  R. The  liberated generation:  A n exploration

of  the  roots  of  student protest.

  Journal

  of

  Social

Issues,

  1967,

 23 3),  52-75.

Fowler,  M .  G.,  Fowler,  R.

  L.,

  & Van de  Riet,  H.

Feminism

  an d

  political radicalism.

  Journal  of

Psychology

1973,

  83 ,  237-242.

Page 10: Multidimensional Locus of Control Attitude Scale Levenson Miller 1976 JPSP

8/10/2019 Multidimensional Locus of Control Attitude Scale Levenson Miller 1976 JPSP

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/multidimensional-locus-of-control-attitude-scale-levenson-miller-1976-jpsp 10/10

208

H A N N A

  LEVENSON  AND JIM MILLER

Gaito,

  J.

  Unequal intervals

  and

  unequal

  n  in

  trend

analyses.  Psychological

  Bulletin,

  196S,

  63, 125-121.

Gootnick,  A. T.  Locus  of  control  an d  political par-

ticipation

  of

  college students:

  A

  compar ison

  of

unidimensional

  and

  mult idimens ional approaches .

Journal

  of

  Consulting

  an d

  Clinical Psychology,

1974,

  42,

  54-58.

Gore,

  P. M,, & Rotter ,  J. B. A  personality correlate

of  social

  action.

  Journal

  of

  Personality,  1963,

  31 ,

58-64.

Gurin,  P.,

  Gur in ,

  G .,

  Lao,

  R., &

  Beattie,

  M. In-

ternal-external control  in the  motivational  dy-

namics  of  Negro

  youth.

  Journal of Social Issues,

1969,

  25(3),  29-S3.

Hersch,

  P. D., &

  Scheibe,  K.

  E. On the

  reliability

and va lidity of internal-external contro l as a

personality dimension.  Journal  of  Consulting Psy-

chology,  1967,  31,  609-614.

Joe,

  V. C.

  Review

  of the

  internal-external control

cons truct

  as a

  personality variable.  Psychological

Reports,

  1971,

  28,

  619-640.

Kelley,

  H. H. Attribution

  theory

  in social

  psychol-

ogy.  In D.  Levine

  (Ed.),

  Nebraska Symposium

on  motivation

  (Vol.

  I S ) .

  Lincoln: University

  of

Nebraska

  Press,

  1967.

Keniston,

  K. The

  sources

  of

  student dissent.

  In P.

Mussen,

  J.

  Conger ,

  & J.

  Kagen  (Eds.),  Readings

in child development and personality.  N e w Y o r k :

Harper

  &

  Row, 1970.

Kerpelman,  L. C.

  Student polit ical activism

  and

ideology: Comparative characteristics

  of

  activists

an d

  nonactivists .  Journal

  o f

  Cou nseling Psychol-

ogy,  1969, 16,  8-13.

Lefcourt ,

  H, M.  Recent developments  in the  study

of

  locus of control. In B. A.

  Maher (Ed . ) ,

Progress

  in experimental personality research

(Vol.  4). New

  Y ork: Academ ic Press, 1972.

Levenson,

  H.

  Multidimensional locus

  of

  control

  in

psychiatric

  patients.  Journal of Consulting and

Clinical  Psychology,  1973,

  41,

  397-404.

  (a )

Levenson,

  H.

  Perceived parental antecedents

  of

internal,

  powerfu l

  others,

  and

  chance locus

  of

control orientations.

  Developmental

  Psychology,

1973,

  9,

  260-265.

  (b)

Levenson,  H.  Activism  and  powerful  others: Dis-

tinctions within

  th e

  concept

  of

  internal-external

control.

  Journal  of  Personality Assessment,  1974,

38,  377-383.

Levenson,

  H.

  Multidimensional locus

  of

  control

  in

prison inmates.

  Journal  of  Applied Social Psychol-

ogy,

  1975,

  in press.

Levenson,

  H.,  &

 M iller,

  J.

  Development

  of a

  current

scale

  to

  measure conservatism-liberalism.

  Journal

o f  Psychology,  1974,  88 ,  241-244.

Lewis,

  S.

  H.,

  &

 Kraut ,

  R. F.

  Correlates

  of

  student

political activism

  an d

  ideology.  Journal  of  Social

Issues,  1972,

 28 4),  131-149.

Minton,

  H. L.

  Power

  and

  personality.

  In J. T.

Tedeschi  ( E d . ) ,  The  social

  influence

  process.

Chicago: Aldine, 1972.

Mirels,

  H. L.

  Dimensions

  of

  internal versus external

control.

  Journal

  of

  Consulting

  an d

  Clinical Psy-

chology,  1970,

  34 ,  226-228.

Mirels,  H.,  & G a r r e t t,  J. The  Protestant ethic  as a

personality variable.

  Journal  o f  Consulting  an d

Clinical  Psychology,  1971,

  36 , 40-44.

Overall,

  J.

  E.,

  &  Spiegel,  D. K.  Con cerning least

squares analysis of experimental

  data.

  Psycho-

logical  Bulletin,

  1969, 72, 311-322,

Prociuk,

  T.

  J.,

  &

 Breen,

  L. J.  Defensive

  external i ty

and its  relation  to  academic performance.  Journal

o f  Personality  an d  Social Psychology,  1975,  31 ,

549-556.

Ransford ,

  H. E.  Isolation, powerlessness  and  vio-

lence:  A  study  of

  attitudes

  and

  participation

  in

the Watts

  riot.

  American Journal

  of

  Sociology,

1968,  73,  581-591.

Rotter ,  J. B.  Gene ralized expectancies  for  internal

versus external control  of  reinforcement .  Psycho-

logical  Monographs,

  1966,

  80 1,

  Whole

  N o.

  609) .

Rotter,

  J. B.

  External control

  and

  internal control.

Psychology Today,  Jun e 1971, 37-42;  58-59.

Sanger,

  S. P., &  Alker,  H. A.

  Dimensions

  of in-

ternal-external

  locus

  of

  control

  and the

  women's

l iberat ion movement .  Journal  of  Social Issues,

1972,

  28 4),

  115-129.

Strickland,

  B. R. The prediction of social action

from  a

  dimension

  of  internal-external control,

Journal  of  Social Psychology,  1965,  66 ,  353-358.

Thomas,

  L. E. The

  I-E  scale, ideological bias,

  and

polit ical participation.  Journal  o f  Personality,

1970,  38 ,

  273-286.

Weiner,

  B.,

  Heckhausen,  H.,  Meyer ,  W., &  Cook,

R. E.

  Causal ascriptions

  and

  achievement behav-

ior:

  A

  conceptual analysis

  of  effort  an d

  reanalysis

of

  locus

  of

  control.  Journal

  of

  Personality

  an d

Social Psychology,

  1972,

  21 ,  239-248.

(Received January  29 ,  1975)