46
Linguistics / Communication Disorders Thomas Roeper Barbara Zurer Pearson Margaret Grace University of Massachusetts Amherst [email protected] BUCLD November 2010 Boston University Quantifier Spreading Is Not Distributive

Linguistics / Communication Disorders Thomas Roeper Barbara Zurer Pearson Margaret Grace University of Massachusetts Amherst [email protected]

  • View
    219

  • Download
    0

Embed Size (px)

Citation preview

Page 1: Linguistics / Communication Disorders Thomas Roeper Barbara Zurer Pearson Margaret Grace University of Massachusetts Amherst bpearson@research.umass.edu

Linguistics / Communication Disorders

Thomas RoeperBarbara Zurer Pearson

Margaret Grace

University of Massachusetts [email protected]

BUCLD November 2010Boston University

Quantifier Spreading Is Not Distributive

Page 2: Linguistics / Communication Disorders Thomas Roeper Barbara Zurer Pearson Margaret Grace University of Massachusetts Amherst bpearson@research.umass.edu

2Linguistics/ Communication Disorders

Plan of the talk

Brief definitions of spreading and distributive

Why spreading has been construed as distributive.

Our evidence that exhaustivity, not distributivity

is at the root of children’s spreading with every,

and even with each.• Adult survey (baseline)• Child survey

Our interpretation

Your questions and suggestions

Page 3: Linguistics / Communication Disorders Thomas Roeper Barbara Zurer Pearson Margaret Grace University of Massachusetts Amherst bpearson@research.umass.edu

3Linguistics/ Communication Disorders

“Classic Spreading”

Quantifiers apply to both nouns:

Is every girl riding a bike?

= every girl rides (every) bike

= and every bike is ridden by a girl

Page 4: Linguistics / Communication Disorders Thomas Roeper Barbara Zurer Pearson Margaret Grace University of Massachusetts Amherst bpearson@research.umass.edu

4Linguistics/ Communication Disorders

Example (from DSLT*): “Is every girl riding a bike?”

No, not this bike.

Copyright 2000 TPC

Dialect Sensitive Language Test (Seymour, Roeper & de Villiers, 2000)

Page 5: Linguistics / Communication Disorders Thomas Roeper Barbara Zurer Pearson Margaret Grace University of Massachusetts Amherst bpearson@research.umass.edu

5Linguistics/ Communication Disorders

Find spreading with other quantifiers:

Applies to all, some, and most

Example some of the circles are red =>

some of the circles have (some) red(Matthei & Roeper, 1975; Philip, 1995)

Also work by Drozd, Crain, Stickney, others

Is it syntactic or semantic or both?

Page 6: Linguistics / Communication Disorders Thomas Roeper Barbara Zurer Pearson Margaret Grace University of Massachusetts Amherst bpearson@research.umass.edu

6Linguistics/ Communication Disorders

Is spreading distributive or exhaustive?

Does the quantifier really float?

The experiment confounds exhaustivity and distributivity

Exhaustivity = all bikes and all girls

Distributivity = one bike for each girl

Can we pull these apart?

Page 7: Linguistics / Communication Disorders Thomas Roeper Barbara Zurer Pearson Margaret Grace University of Massachusetts Amherst bpearson@research.umass.edu

7Linguistics/ Communication Disorders

Lexical properties of quantifiers:

all = collective => all the water

every = collective or distributive• Everyone surrounded the house = collective• *every person surrounded the house

each = distributive and specific

(presupposed set)• Each elephant has two trunks [picture with two trunks]• Does every elephant have two trunks?

Each => defined set in situation Every => possibly generic

Page 8: Linguistics / Communication Disorders Thomas Roeper Barbara Zurer Pearson Margaret Grace University of Massachusetts Amherst bpearson@research.umass.edu

8Linguistics/ Communication Disorders

Tunstall classic example:

[ waiter lifts tray of glasses]

=> he lifted every glass

=> *he lifted each glass

Page 9: Linguistics / Communication Disorders Thomas Roeper Barbara Zurer Pearson Margaret Grace University of Massachusetts Amherst bpearson@research.umass.edu

9Linguistics/ Communication Disorders

Acquisition challenge:

1. Children begin with early collective readings: “allgone milk”

2. Child must learn both exhaustive and distributive meaning

Evidence of cognitive ability early with plurals (Avrutin & Thornton, 1994)

3. Child must associate distributivity with each.

Page 10: Linguistics / Communication Disorders Thomas Roeper Barbara Zurer Pearson Margaret Grace University of Massachusetts Amherst bpearson@research.umass.edu

10Linguistics/ Communication Disorders

Syntactic proposal (Roeper Strauss & Pearson, 2006) Every => syntactic Operator

Spreading = floated quantifier

Note: quantifier as higher operator argued for Hungarian (see Kang,1999; Brody, 1990)

Page 11: Linguistics / Communication Disorders Thomas Roeper Barbara Zurer Pearson Margaret Grace University of Massachusetts Amherst bpearson@research.umass.edu

11Linguistics/ Communication Disorders

Floating is lexically specific:

All the children are here The children are all here

Each of the children are here The children are each here

Every boy is here *the boys are every here Note: possible with jeder in German

Page 12: Linguistics / Communication Disorders Thomas Roeper Barbara Zurer Pearson Margaret Grace University of Massachusetts Amherst bpearson@research.umass.edu

12Linguistics/ Communication Disorders

Semantic alternative

Many Scandinavians won the Nobel Prize => many Nobel Prize winners are Scandinavian

(Drozd, 2001)

Semantic account: Strong quantifiers (every, each, most) obey

conservativity: Q applies only to NP and requires truth of VP

=> No syntactic effects predicted

Page 13: Linguistics / Communication Disorders Thomas Roeper Barbara Zurer Pearson Margaret Grace University of Massachusetts Amherst bpearson@research.umass.edu

13Linguistics/ Communication Disorders

(semantic alternative – cont.

Weak Quantifiers (many): involve Context variable (C) and a formula:

A = set of Scandinavians, B = set of nobel prizes and C =

set of contextually relevant Scandinaviansmany => Union of A,B where A,B > C = B

Conclusion: pragmatically conditioned

Page 14: Linguistics / Communication Disorders Thomas Roeper Barbara Zurer Pearson Margaret Grace University of Massachusetts Amherst bpearson@research.umass.edu

14Linguistics/ Communication Disorders

Smits (2010), (see Stickney for most):

many parrots are wearing hats True for: 4/5 parrots have hats

6/30 monkeys have hats

Result: acquired later but with clear pragmatic conditioning =/= spreading

Page 15: Linguistics / Communication Disorders Thomas Roeper Barbara Zurer Pearson Margaret Grace University of Massachusetts Amherst bpearson@research.umass.edu

15Linguistics/ Communication Disorders

Hypothesis

Children do not identify lexical properties correctly

Maybe All Q’s = exhaustive or distributive

Generalization: each = every = exhaustive• every = each = distributiveChild could go in either direction

Page 16: Linguistics / Communication Disorders Thomas Roeper Barbara Zurer Pearson Margaret Grace University of Massachusetts Amherst bpearson@research.umass.edu

16Linguistics/ Communication Disorders

Is spreading syntactic? Can we see the Q float?

Data from the DSLT (Seymour Roeper & de Villiers, 2000)

Pilot version of the DELV tests (Seymour, Roeper & de Villiers, 2003, 2005).

Piloted with 1458 children, African American English speakers, general American English speakers, typically developing, language impaired, ages 4 to 12.

The following graph is from 333 typically-developing general American English speakers

Page 17: Linguistics / Communication Disorders Thomas Roeper Barbara Zurer Pearson Margaret Grace University of Massachusetts Amherst bpearson@research.umass.edu

17Linguistics/ Communication Disorders

Long trajectory -increases before it decreases

4 5 6 7-8 9-10 11-12

0

10

20

30

40

50

60

70

spreading responsestarget

age in years

% o

f ch

ild

ren

giv

ing

re

pso

nse

ty

pe

And doesn’t go awayFrom Roeper et al. 2006

Page 18: Linguistics / Communication Disorders Thomas Roeper Barbara Zurer Pearson Margaret Grace University of Massachusetts Amherst bpearson@research.umass.edu

18Linguistics/ Communication Disorders

Looks like children are either---

Trying to distribute the girls to the bikes, and can’t if they don’t have enough girls—so they say “no, what about this bike?”

Or they may just be thinking the “every” is telling them to attend to everything in the picture (and in the sentence)—to be exhaustive and so they say “no.”

Every girl is riding a bike.

Page 19: Linguistics / Communication Disorders Thomas Roeper Barbara Zurer Pearson Margaret Grace University of Massachusetts Amherst bpearson@research.umass.edu

19Linguistics/ Communication Disorders

To tease apart explanations

Gave child 3 options for EVERY• 1-1 Distributive/ NOT exhaustive

• Exhaustive/ not 1-1 distributive

• Collective/ NOT exhaustive

Tried to push toward distributivity, by giving an opportunity with EACH (lexically, strongly distributive).

Page 20: Linguistics / Communication Disorders Thomas Roeper Barbara Zurer Pearson Margaret Grace University of Massachusetts Amherst bpearson@research.umass.edu

20Linguistics/ Communication Disorders

Separating Distributivity and Exhaustivity.

A no distrib not exhaustive

B 1-1 distributivenot exhaustive

C not 1-1 distrib Exhaustive(partial distrib?)

A

B

C

Stimuli from Brooks et al. 2001

Page 21: Linguistics / Communication Disorders Thomas Roeper Barbara Zurer Pearson Margaret Grace University of Massachusetts Amherst bpearson@research.umass.edu

21Linguistics/ Communication Disorders

(aside for “partial distributivity” – see S. Lima, 2010)

A

/ | \

C D E

| | / \

F G H I

Partial distributivity

under E, not one to one

A

/ \

B C

/ \ / | \

D E F G

Partially distributive

because C has empty node(non-exhaustive if any of the nodes are empty)

Page 22: Linguistics / Communication Disorders Thomas Roeper Barbara Zurer Pearson Margaret Grace University of Massachusetts Amherst bpearson@research.umass.edu

22Linguistics/ Communication Disorders

Participants

Adults N = 40

http://www.kwiksurvey etc.

Native English speakers Ages 20 to 71 (20+) Residence UK (6),

Canada (3) and U.S. (31)

Children = 38

Ages 5-9 (most 6-8) Average 7;4 Grade K-3 Middle to lower

middle-class school district in western MA

Page 23: Linguistics / Communication Disorders Thomas Roeper Barbara Zurer Pearson Margaret Grace University of Massachusetts Amherst bpearson@research.umass.edu

23Linguistics/ Communication Disorders

Ask, which pictures does this sentence describe?

(could be 0, 1, any 2 of them, or all three)

Could it be any others? Which is best? Why? and why not?

Every flower is in a vase.

Each flower is in a vase.

A

B

C

Stimuli from Brooks et al. 2001

Page 24: Linguistics / Communication Disorders Thomas Roeper Barbara Zurer Pearson Margaret Grace University of Massachusetts Amherst bpearson@research.umass.edu

24Linguistics/ Communication Disorders

(Add your intuitions)

A version (of the adult survey)—is available athttp://www.kwiksurveys.com/online survey.php?surveyID=OIHKG_7f21b1b7

(be entered in a raffle for a copy of Tom’s or my book)

Page 25: Linguistics / Communication Disorders Thomas Roeper Barbara Zurer Pearson Margaret Grace University of Massachusetts Amherst bpearson@research.umass.edu

25Linguistics/ Communication Disorders

Adult preferences – Every flower is in a vase.

All ok Prefer A Prefer B Prefer C0

0.2

0.4

0.6

0.8

1

Every

Page 26: Linguistics / Communication Disorders Thomas Roeper Barbara Zurer Pearson Margaret Grace University of Massachusetts Amherst bpearson@research.umass.edu

26Linguistics/ Communication Disorders

Adult preferences – Each flower is in a vase.

All OK B Best only B Rejects B0

0.2

0.4

0.6

0.8

1

Each

Page 27: Linguistics / Communication Disorders Thomas Roeper Barbara Zurer Pearson Margaret Grace University of Massachusetts Amherst bpearson@research.umass.edu

27Linguistics/ Communication Disorders

Children’s Every (with adult shaded as reference)

All ok Prefer A Prefer B Prefer C0

0.2

0.4

0.6

0.8

1

AdultChild

Page 28: Linguistics / Communication Disorders Thomas Roeper Barbara Zurer Pearson Margaret Grace University of Massachusetts Amherst bpearson@research.umass.edu

28Linguistics/ Communication Disorders

Children’s Each (with adult shaded as reference)

All OK B Best only B Rejects B

0

0.2

0.4

0.6

0.8

1 AdultChild

Page 29: Linguistics / Communication Disorders Thomas Roeper Barbara Zurer Pearson Margaret Grace University of Massachusetts Amherst bpearson@research.umass.edu

29Linguistics/ Communication Disorders

(only) 4 children focused on flowers for every “All the flowers have vases that they’re in.” (5;4)

“There are empty vases, [clearly a concern] but where there are flowers, they are in a vase.” (8;1)

Page 30: Linguistics / Communication Disorders Thomas Roeper Barbara Zurer Pearson Margaret Grace University of Massachusetts Amherst bpearson@research.umass.edu

30Linguistics/ Communication Disorders

Most children focused on vases

[C], “the only one where vases are filled with flowers” (8;0)

“these two vases don’t have flowers” (6;2) “not A or B, no flowers in those two vases” (7;8) “no, two vases empty there” (6;5) “no, the others have empty vases” (6;11) (7;4)

(8;4) “no, because some of the vases are empty” (7;9) “not A, only one filled vase” (8;2)

Page 31: Linguistics / Communication Disorders Thomas Roeper Barbara Zurer Pearson Margaret Grace University of Massachusetts Amherst bpearson@research.umass.edu

31Linguistics/ Communication Disorders

Spontaneously SPREAD the quantifier to vases. “[C], it’s the only one with flowers in every vase.”

(9;4)

“Not B, there’s just one in each [vase]” (6;1)

“No, they don’t have flowers in all vases.” (9)

(for every flower in a vase), “could be 1 flower in each vase” (9)

Page 32: Linguistics / Communication Disorders Thomas Roeper Barbara Zurer Pearson Margaret Grace University of Massachusetts Amherst bpearson@research.umass.edu

32Linguistics/ Communication Disorders

SPREAD a quantifier to vases even with “each” “looks like each flower is in each vase” (8;0) “all vases are full” (8) “flowers in all [vases]” (7;9) “could be C, if there was just one flower in each,

in all the vases” (7;1) “one [flower] in each [vase]” (8;1)

“these two vases don’t have flowers” (6;2) “not A or B, no flowers in those two vases” (7;8)

??? Each flower has its own vase?

Page 33: Linguistics / Communication Disorders Thomas Roeper Barbara Zurer Pearson Margaret Grace University of Massachusetts Amherst bpearson@research.umass.edu

33Linguistics/ Communication Disorders

They did not like empty vases.

Little concern for distributivity…..

Page 34: Linguistics / Communication Disorders Thomas Roeper Barbara Zurer Pearson Margaret Grace University of Massachusetts Amherst bpearson@research.umass.edu

34Linguistics/ Communication Disorders

(only) 4 children described configuration of flowers, even for each

“B is a little better because it’s spread out” (8;1)

“B – each flower has its own vase.” (9;0)

“C has too many flowers; A they’re all in one” (6;5)

“Could be C if there was just one flower in each, in all the vases.” (7;1)

**(one appealed to config for every—”only C, all in same is wrong; 1 in 1 is wrong”) (7;7)

Page 35: Linguistics / Communication Disorders Thomas Roeper Barbara Zurer Pearson Margaret Grace University of Massachusetts Amherst bpearson@research.umass.edu

35Linguistics/ Communication Disorders

Each was clearly not distributive for the children In fact, 14 children did not distinguish

each and every • (either gave same answer, or said “I already

told you” when asked why about the second sentence)

To the extent that it’s confounded with every might be more likely exhaustive as well.

Page 36: Linguistics / Communication Disorders Thomas Roeper Barbara Zurer Pearson Margaret Grace University of Massachusetts Amherst bpearson@research.umass.edu

36Linguistics/ Communication Disorders

Acquisition Theory

Treat quantificational elements as Operators Attach to the Root CP

Negation:

• “I don’t want none’

• Tense:

• “wented” “did lifted”, “had came” etc

• Plural:

• Does a dog have tails => dogs have tails

Page 37: Linguistics / Communication Disorders Thomas Roeper Barbara Zurer Pearson Margaret Grace University of Massachusetts Amherst bpearson@research.umass.edu

37Linguistics/ Communication Disorders

Every, some, most, all Wh- => who bought what

• Schulz (2010)

who gave what to whom

=> 3 quantifiers no harder than 2

Cf. John didn’t buy anything anyhow anywhere

Page 38: Linguistics / Communication Disorders Thomas Roeper Barbara Zurer Pearson Margaret Grace University of Massachusetts Amherst bpearson@research.umass.edu

38Linguistics/ Communication Disorders

How do children eliminate quantifier spreading?

Roeper et al: they experience a second quantifier As in: Every dog has some hats [extra hat] Prediction: children will stop spreading in these cases earlier than in others

Page 39: Linguistics / Communication Disorders Thomas Roeper Barbara Zurer Pearson Margaret Grace University of Massachusetts Amherst bpearson@research.umass.edu

39Linguistics/ Communication Disorders

Conclusion:

1. Children take quantifiers to be exhaustive but not distributive initially

• 2. Each interpreted as exhaustive like every• 3. Verbatim evidence supports the original claims of syntactic account of spreading• 4. Weak quantification is a separate phenomenon

5. Operators are Default syntactic assumptions for children

Page 40: Linguistics / Communication Disorders Thomas Roeper Barbara Zurer Pearson Margaret Grace University of Massachusetts Amherst bpearson@research.umass.edu

40Linguistics/ Communication Disorders

References Brody, M. (1990). Some remarks on the focus field in Hungarian. UCL Working

Papers 2: 201-225.

Brooks, P., Braine, M., Jia, G. & da Graca Dias (2001). Early representations of all, each and their counterparts in Mandarin Chinese and Portuguese. In Bowerman, M. and S. Levinson (eds.) Language Acquisition and Conceptual Development, p. 316-339. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Crain, S., Thornton, R., Boster, C., Conway, L., Lillo-Martin, D. & Woodams, E. (1996). “Quantification without qualification.” Language Acquisition, 5(2): 83-153.

Drozd, K.F. (2001). Children’s weak interpretation of universally quantified sentences. In Bowerman, M. and S. Levinson (eds.) Language Acquisition and Conceptual Development, pp. 340-376. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Geurts, B. (2001). Quantifying kids. Ms., Humboldt University, Berlin and University of Nijmegen, Nijmegen. 

Page 41: Linguistics / Communication Disorders Thomas Roeper Barbara Zurer Pearson Margaret Grace University of Massachusetts Amherst bpearson@research.umass.edu

41Linguistics/ Communication Disorders

References - 2 Kang, H.-K. (1999). Quantifier spreading by English and Korean children.

Ms., University College, London.

Philip, W. (1995). Event quantification in the acquisition of universal quantification, Doctoral dissertation, UMass Amherst.

Roeper, T. & , E. (1974). “On the acquisition of some and all,” Presented at the Sixth Child Language Research Forum, Stanford University, April 1974. Appeared in Papers and reports on child language development (1975), Stanford University, 63-74.

Roeper, T., Strauss, U., & Pearson, B. Z. (2006). The acquisition path of the determiner quantifier every: Two kinds of spreading. In T. Heizmann (Ed.), Papers in Language Acquisition (pp. 97-128), University of Massachusetts Occasional Papers UMOP, 34. Amherst, MA: GLSA.

Schulz, P. (2010). Presentation on wh- and exhaustive pairing. COST meeting, London.

Page 42: Linguistics / Communication Disorders Thomas Roeper Barbara Zurer Pearson Margaret Grace University of Massachusetts Amherst bpearson@research.umass.edu

42Linguistics/ Communication Disorders

References -3

Smits, E-J. (2010) Acquiring quantification:How children use semantics and pragmatics to constrict meaning. Dissertation Groningen, Holland.

Tunstall, S. (1998). The interpretation of quantifiers: Semantics and

processing. Doctoral dissertation, University of Massachusetts Amherst.

Westerstahl, D. (1985). Determiners and context sets In J. van Bentham and A. ter-Meulen (Eds.), Generalized Quantifiers in Natural Language. Dordrecht: Foris Publications.

Page 43: Linguistics / Communication Disorders Thomas Roeper Barbara Zurer Pearson Margaret Grace University of Massachusetts Amherst bpearson@research.umass.edu

43Linguistics/ Communication Disorders

Acknowledgments

Some of the materials in the current experiment were assembled while Pearson was a visiting researcher at the ESRC in Bangor.

We want to thank Margaret Grace, who was helpful in adapting the adult survey for children, and administering it to them with me.

Page 44: Linguistics / Communication Disorders Thomas Roeper Barbara Zurer Pearson Margaret Grace University of Massachusetts Amherst bpearson@research.umass.edu

44Linguistics/ Communication Disorders

Thank you.

Questions?

Page 45: Linguistics / Communication Disorders Thomas Roeper Barbara Zurer Pearson Margaret Grace University of Massachusetts Amherst bpearson@research.umass.edu

45Linguistics/ Communication Disorders

Prizes: Prism of Grammar or RBC (english/spanish)

[email protected]@linguist.umass.edu

Page 46: Linguistics / Communication Disorders Thomas Roeper Barbara Zurer Pearson Margaret Grace University of Massachusetts Amherst bpearson@research.umass.edu

46Linguistics/ Communication Disorders

(Note also that Brooks et al., 2001, working on all and each in English, (and Mandarin, and Portuguese) say they found that

the English learning children didn’t seem to “pay attention to the location of each” until around age 9.)