30
INTRODUCTION to COMMUNICATION SCIENCES AND DISORDERS The Scientific Basis of Clinical Practice Gary Weismer, PhD David K. Brown, PhD

IntroductIon to communIcatIon ScIenceS and dISorderS€¦ · Introduction to Communication Sciences and Disorders:viii The Scientific Basis of Clinical Practice 9 Language Disorders

  • Upload
    others

  • View
    9

  • Download
    0

Embed Size (px)

Citation preview

Page 1: IntroductIon to communIcatIon ScIenceS and dISorderS€¦ · Introduction to Communication Sciences and Disorders:viii The Scientific Basis of Clinical Practice 9 Language Disorders

IntroductIon to

communIcatIon ScIenceS and dISorderS

The Scientific Basis of Clinical Practice

Gary Weismer, PhDDavid K. Brown, PhD

Page 2: IntroductIon to communIcatIon ScIenceS and dISorderS€¦ · Introduction to Communication Sciences and Disorders:viii The Scientific Basis of Clinical Practice 9 Language Disorders

5521 Ruffin RoadSan Diego, CA 92123

e-mail: [email protected]: https://www.pluralpublishing.com

Copyright © 2021 by Plural Publishing, Inc.

Typeset in 10/12 Palatino by Flanagan’s Publishing Services, Inc.Printed in Canada by Friesens Corporation

All rights, including that of translation, reserved. No part of this publication may be reproduced, stored in a retrieval system, or transmitted in any form or by any means, electronic, mechanical, recording, or otherwise, including photocopying, recording, taping, Web distribution, or information storage and retrieval systems without the prior written consent of the publisher.

For permission to use material from this text, contact us byTelephone: (866) 758-7251Fax: (888) 758-7255e-mail: [email protected]

Every attempt has been made to contact the copyright holders for material originally printed in another source. If any have been inadvertently overlooked, the publishers will gladly make the necessary arrangements at the first opportunity.

Library of Congress Cataloging-in-Publication Data

Names: Weismer, Gary, (Professor Emeritus), author. | Brown, David K. (Professor of audiology), author. Title: Introduction to communication sciences and disorders : the scientific basis of clinical practice / Gary Weismer, David K. Brown. Description: San Diego, CA : Plural Publishing, Inc., [2021] | Includes bibliographical references and index. Identifiers: LCCN 2019029827 | ISBN 9781597562973 (paperback) | ISBN 1597562971 (paperback) Subjects: MESH: Communication Disorders | Voice Disorders | Hearing Disorders | Language Development | Speech — physiology | Hearing — physiology Classification: LCC RC423 | NLM WL 340.2 | DDC 616.85/5 — dc23 LC record available at https://lccn.loc.gov/2019029827

Page 3: IntroductIon to communIcatIon ScIenceS and dISorderS€¦ · Introduction to Communication Sciences and Disorders:viii The Scientific Basis of Clinical Practice 9 Language Disorders

v

ContentsPreface xvAcknowledgments xviiReviewers xix

1 Introduction to Communication Sciences 1 and DisordersIntroduction: Communication Sciences and Disorders as a Discipline 1Communication Sciences and Disorders: The Whole Is Greater 2

Than the Sum of Its PartsAn Interdisciplinary Field 3Translational Research 4Does the Basic Science Work? Does the Clinic Work? 6Evidence-Based Practice 7A Typical Undergraduate Curriculum 10

Who Are the Professionals in Communication Sciences and Disorders? 10Preparation for, and the Profession of, Speech-Language Pathology 10Preparation for, and the Profession of, Audiology 12

Order of Chapters in the Text 13Chapter Summary 14References 14

2 The Nervous System: Language, Speech, and 17 Hearing Structures and ProcessesIntroduction 17Central and Peripheral Nervous Systems 17

The Neuron 18The Synapse 21

Tour of Gross Neuroanatomy 21Frontal Lobe 22Occipital Lobe 23Temporal Lobe 23Parietal Lobe 24Hidden Cortex 24Subcortical Nuclei 24Brainstem, Cerebellum, and Spinal Cord 26The Auditory Pathways 27

Page 4: IntroductIon to communIcatIon ScIenceS and dISorderS€¦ · Introduction to Communication Sciences and Disorders:viii The Scientific Basis of Clinical Practice 9 Language Disorders

Introduction to Communication Sciences and Disorders: The Scientific Basis of Clinical Practice vi

The Dominant Hemisphere and the Perisylvian Language Areas 28Arcuate Fasciculus (Dorsal Stream) and Ventral Stream 30

Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging and Speech and Language 31 Brain Activity

Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging 31Diffusion Tensor Imaging 32

Chapter Summary 33References 34

3 Language Science 35Introduction 35What Is Language? 35

Language: A Conventional System 36Language: A Dynamic System 36Language Is Generative 37Language Uses Mental Representations 37Language Is Localized in the Brain 38

Components of Language 38Form 38Social Use of Language (Pragmatics) 42

Language and Cognitive Processes 42Why 43How 43When 43

Chapter Summary 44References 44

4 Communication in a Multicultural Society 45Introduction 45Why It Matters 47

Difference Versus Disorder 47Standardized Testing and Language Difference Versus Disorder 48Accent, Dialect, and Culture 50

Accent 50Dialect 51Code Switching 52Foreign Accent 53

Bilingualism and Multilingualism 54Chapter Summary 55References 55

5 Preverbal Foundations of Speech and 57 Language DevelopmentIntroduction 57Preparatory Notes on Developmental Chronologies 58

0 to 3 Months: Expression (Production) 580 to 3 Months: Perception and Comprehension 603 to 8 Months: Production 613 to 8 Months: Perception and Comprehension 63

Page 5: IntroductIon to communIcatIon ScIenceS and dISorderS€¦ · Introduction to Communication Sciences and Disorders:viii The Scientific Basis of Clinical Practice 9 Language Disorders

viiContents

8 to 12 Months: Production 658 to 12 Months: Perception and Comprehension 65

Gesture and Preverbal Language Development 66Chapter Summary 66References 67

6 Typical Language Development 69Introduction 69

12 to 18 Months 7118 to 24 Months 71Three Years (36 Months) 72

Multiword Utterances, Grammatical Morphology 72Expanding Utterance Length: A Measure of Linguistic Sophistication 74Grammatical Morphology 76

Typical Language Development in School Years 77Metalinguistic Skills 77Pragmatic Skill: Discourse 78Complex Sentences 81

Sample Transcript 81Chapter Summary 83References 83

7 Pediatric Language Disorders I 85Introduction 85Specific Language Impairment/Developmental Language Disorder 85

Language Characteristics of Children with SLI/DLD 86Summary of the Language Disorder in SLI/DLD 88What Is The Cause of SLI/DLD? 88

The Role of Genetics in SLI/DLD 88Language Delay and Autism Spectrum Disorder 89

Language Characteristics in ASD 89Language Delay and Hearing Impairment 92

Epidemiology of Hearing Loss 92Language Characteristics in Hearing Impairment 92Speech and Language Development and Hearing Impairment 93

Chapter Summary 94References 95

8 Pediatric Language Disorders II 99Introduction 99Criteria for a Diagnosis of ID 99

Down Syndrome (DS): General Characteristics 100Epidemiology and the DS Phenotype 101Language Characteristics in DS 102Fragile X Syndrome: General Characteristics 104Epidemiology of FXS 106Language Characteristics in FXS 106

Chapter Summary 109References 109

Page 6: IntroductIon to communIcatIon ScIenceS and dISorderS€¦ · Introduction to Communication Sciences and Disorders:viii The Scientific Basis of Clinical Practice 9 Language Disorders

Introduction to Communication Sciences and Disorders: The Scientific Basis of Clinical Practice viii

9 Language Disorders in Adults 111Introduction 111Review of Concepts for the Role of The Nervous System In Speech, 111

Language, and HearingCerebral Hemispheres 111Lateralization of Speech and Language Functions 112Language Expression and Comprehension Are Represented in 112

Different Cortical Regions of the Left HemisphereConnections Between Different Regions of the Brain 112Perisylvian Speech and Language Areas of the Brain 112

Adult Language Disorders: Aphasia 114Classification of Aphasia 114Aphasia Due to Stroke: A Summary 122Traumatic Brain Injury and Aphasia 124Nature of Brain Injury in TBI 124Language Impairment in TBI 125

Dementia 126Brain Pathology in Dementia 126Language Disorders in Dementia 127

Chapter Summary 128References 128

10 Speech Science I 131Introduction 131The Speech Mechanism: A Three-Component Description 131

Respiratory System Component (Power Supply for Speech) 131The Respiratory System and Vegetative Breathing 133Speech Breathing 134Clinical Applications: An Example 137

The Larynx (Sound Source for Speech) 138Laryngeal Cartilages 138Laryngeal Muscles and Membranes 139Phonation 141Characteristics of Phonation 142Clinical Applications: An Example 145

Upper Airway (Consonants and Vowels) 145Muscles of the Vocal Tract 146Vocal Tract Shape and Vocalic Production 146Velopharyngeal Mechanism 147Valving in the Vocal Tract and the Formation of Speech Sounds 149Coarticulation 149Clinical Applications: An Example 149

Chapter Summary 150References 151

11 Speech Science II 153Introduction 153The Theory of Speech Acoustics 154

The Sound Source 154

Page 7: IntroductIon to communIcatIon ScIenceS and dISorderS€¦ · Introduction to Communication Sciences and Disorders:viii The Scientific Basis of Clinical Practice 9 Language Disorders

ixContents

The Sound Filter 154Vowel Sounds Result From the Combination of Source and 155

Filter AcousticsResonant Frequencies of Vowels Are Called Formants: Spectrograms 156The Tube Model of Human Vocal Tract Makes Interesting Predictions 158

and Suggests Interesting ProblemsA Spectrogram Shows Formant Frequencies and Much More 158

Speech Synthesis 159Speech Recognition 159Speech Acoustics and Assistive Listening Devices 159Speech Perception 160

The Perception of Speech: Special Mechanisms? 160The Perception of Speech: Auditory Theories 162Motor Theory and Auditory Theory: A Summary 163Top-Down Influences: It Is Not All About Speech Sounds 163Speech Intelligibility 165

Chapter Summary 166References 166

12 Phonetics 169Introduction 169International Phonetic Alphabet 170

Vowels and Their Phonetic Symbols 170Consonants and Their Phonetic Symbols 174Clinical Implications of Phonetic Transcription 176

Chapter Summary 177References 178

13 Typical Phonological Development 179Introduction 179

Phonetic and Phonological Development: General Considerations 180Phonetic and Phonological Development 180

Phonetic Development 181Phonological Development 181

Typical Speech Sound Development 181Determination of Speech Sound Mastery in Typically Developing Children

183Possible Explanations for the Typical Sequence of Speech Sound Mastery 183Phonological Processes and Speech Sound Development 186Phonological Development and Word Learning 188

Chapter Summary 188References 188

14 Motor Speech Disorders in Adults 191Introduction 191Classification of Motor Speech Disorders 191

Dysarthria 193Subtypes of Dysarthria 193The Mayo Clinic Classification System for Motor Speech Disorders 193

Page 8: IntroductIon to communIcatIon ScIenceS and dISorderS€¦ · Introduction to Communication Sciences and Disorders:viii The Scientific Basis of Clinical Practice 9 Language Disorders

Introduction to Communication Sciences and Disorders: The Scientific Basis of Clinical Practice x

The Dysarthrias: A Summary 201Apraxia of Speech 203

Chapter Summary 203References 204

15 Pediatric Speech Disorders I 205Introduction 205Speech Delay 206

Diagnosis of Speech Delay 207Quantitative Measures of Speech Delay and Speech Intelligibility 208Speech Delay: Phonetic, Phonological, or Both? 209Additional Considerations in Speech Delay and Residual and 210

Persistent Speech Sound ErrorsSpeech Delay and Genetics 211

Childhood Apraxia of Speech 211CAS Compared With Adult Apraxia of Speech (AAS) 212CAS: Prevalence and General Characteristics 214CAS: Speech Characteristics 214CAS and Overlap With Other Developmental Delays 215CAS and Genetics 215

Chapter Summary 216References 217

16 Pediatric Speech Disorders II 219Introduction 219Childhood Motor Speech Disorders: Cerebral Palsy 220

Subtypes of Cerebral Palsy 220Dysarthria in Cerebral Palsy 221

Childhood Motor Speech Disorders: Traumatic Brain Injury 224 and Tumors

Traumatic Brain Injury 224Brain Tumors 225Treatment Options and Considerations 226

Chapter Summary 227References 227

17 Fluency Disorders 229Introduction 229Incidence and Prevalence of Stuttering 229Genetic Studies 231Diagnosis of Developmental Stuttering 231The Natural History of Developmental Stuttering 231

Stage I: Typical Dysfluencies 232Stage II: Borderline Stuttering 232Stage III: Beginning Stuttering 233Stage IV: Intermediate Stuttering 233Stage V: Advanced Stuttering 233Recovery of Fluency 234

Possible Causes of Stuttering 234

Page 9: IntroductIon to communIcatIon ScIenceS and dISorderS€¦ · Introduction to Communication Sciences and Disorders:viii The Scientific Basis of Clinical Practice 9 Language Disorders

xiContents

Psychogenic Theories 234Learning Theories 235Biological Theories 235

Acquired (Neurogenic) Stuttering 238Symptoms of Neurogenic Stuttering Compared With 238

Developmental StutteringTreatment Considerations 239Chapter Summary 239References 240

18 Voice Disorders 243Introduction 243Epidemiology of Voice Disorders 243Initial Steps in the Diagnosis of Voice Disorders 244

Case History 244Perceptual Evaluation of the Voice 244Viewing the Vocal Folds 244Measurement of Basic Voice Parameters 245

Classification/Types of Voice Disorders 247The Hypo-Hyperfunctional Continuum 248Phonotrauma 249Organic Voice Disorders 252Functional Voice Disorders 252Neurological Voice Disorders 254

Pediatric Voice Disorders 257Prevalence of Childhood Voice Disorders 257Types of Childhood Voice Disorders 257Treatment of Childhood Voice Disorders 257

Chapter Summary 258References 258

19 Craniofacial Anomalies 261Introduction 261Definition and Origins of Craniofacial Anomalies 261

Embryological Development of the Upper Lip and Associated 261 Structures

Embryological Errors and Clefting: Clefts of the Lip 264Embryological Errors and Clefting: Clefts of the Palate 265Cleft Lip With or Without a Cleft Palate; Cleft Palate Only 266

(Isolated Cleft Palate)Epidemiology of Clefting 267Speech Production in CL/P and CPO 267

Diagnosis of VPI 269VPI and Hypernasality 270VPI, Consonant Articulation, and Speech Intelligibility 270Clefting and Syndromes 272Cleft Palate: Other Considerations 273

Chapter Summary 274References 275

Page 10: IntroductIon to communIcatIon ScIenceS and dISorderS€¦ · Introduction to Communication Sciences and Disorders:viii The Scientific Basis of Clinical Practice 9 Language Disorders

Introduction to Communication Sciences and Disorders: The Scientific Basis of Clinical Practice xii

20 Swallowing 277Introduction 277Anatomy of Swallowing 277

Esophagus 277Stomach 278

The Act of Swallowing 278Oral Preparatory Phase 280Oral Transport Phase 280Pharyngeal Phase 280Esophageal Phase 282Overlap of Phases 282

Breathing and Swallowing 282Nervous System Control of Swallowing 283

Role of the Peripheral Nervous System 283Role of the Central Nervous System 283

Variables That Influence Swallowing 284Bolus Characteristics 284Development 285Age 286

Measurement and Analysis of Swallowing 286Videofluoroscopy 286Endoscopy 287Client Self-Report 287

Health Care Team for Individuals With Swallowing Disorders 288Chapter Summary 289References 290

21 Hearing Science I: Acoustics and Psychoacoustics 293Introduction 293Oscillation 294

Waveform 295Spectrum 295Waveform and Spectrum 295Resonance 297

Psychoacoustics 297Pitch 298Loudness 299Sound Quality 299

Chapter Summary 300References 300

22 Hearing Science II: Anatomy and Physiology 301Introduction 301Temporal Bone 301Peripheral Anatomy of the Ear 303

Outer Ear (Conductive Mechanism) 303Middle Ear 305Inner Ear (Sensorineural Mechanism) 307

Chapter Summary 314References 314

Page 11: IntroductIon to communIcatIon ScIenceS and dISorderS€¦ · Introduction to Communication Sciences and Disorders:viii The Scientific Basis of Clinical Practice 9 Language Disorders

xiiiContents

23 Diseases of the Auditory System and 317 Diagnostic AudiologyIntroduction 317Hearing Evaluation 317

Case History 318Otoscopy 318Immittance 319Tympanometry 320Acoustic Reflex Threshold 323Audiometric Testing 324Physiological Responses 327

Vestibular Assessment 332Audiometric Results 334

Type, Degree, and Configuration of Loss 335Hearing and Balance Disorders 337

Patient Examples 337Chapter Summary 341References 341

24 Assistive Listening Devices 343Introduction 343Hearing Aids 343

Steps in Selecting and Fitting a Hearing Aid 343Types of Hearing Aids 345Hearing Aid Components 348

Auditory Implantable Devices 349Bone-Anchored Implant 350Middle Ear Implant 351Cochlear Implant 354

Chapter Summary 355Hearing Aids 355Auditory Implantable Devices 356

References 356

25 Aural Habilitation and Rehabilitation 359Introduction 359Aural Habilitation 360

Assessment of Communication Needs in Children 360Pediatric Intervention 362Components of a

Family-Centered Intervention 364Auditory Training in Aural Habilitation 365Communication Options 365Outcome Measures for Children 367

Aural Rehabilitation 367Assessment of Communication Needs in Adults 367Adult Intervention 368Auditory Training in Aural Rehabilitation 368Communication Strategies 369Speechreading 369

Page 12: IntroductIon to communIcatIon ScIenceS and dISorderS€¦ · Introduction to Communication Sciences and Disorders:viii The Scientific Basis of Clinical Practice 9 Language Disorders

Introduction to Communication Sciences and Disorders: The Scientific Basis of Clinical Practice xiv

Outcome Measures for Adults 370Group Aural Rehabilitation 371

Chapter Summary 372Aural Habilitation 372Aural Rehabilitation 372

References 373

Index 375

Page 13: IntroductIon to communIcatIon ScIenceS and dISorderS€¦ · Introduction to Communication Sciences and Disorders:viii The Scientific Basis of Clinical Practice 9 Language Disorders

xv

PrefaceIntroduction to Communication Sciences and Disorders: The Scientific Basis of Clinical Practice is a textbook designed and written for undergraduate students who enroll in a course that lays out the scientific foundations for the clinical disciplines of speech-language pathology and audiol-ogy. The great majority of departments in our field that offer an undergraduate major have a regularly taught introductory course among their course offerings. Introductory courses in any field, whether in psychology, anthropology, linguistics, or communication sciences and disorders (hereafter, CS&D), are survey courses in which nearly all aspects of a field are pre-sented. For academic disciplines that have many aspects — and most do — breadth of coverage takes precedence over depth of coverage. Simplification of complicated material is inevitable, and long-standing, ongoing debates in a field cannot be described in detail. An introductory course in CS&D is subject to these characteristics, and these constraints. That being said, we have attempted to provide a carefully measured depth in each chapter, in the hope of con-veying the sense of excitement in the continuing expansion of the scientific basis of clinical practice in CS&D.

This textbook is organized with a general plan of matching individual chapters to indi-vidual lectures, or perhaps to one-and-one-half lectures. The textbook is written to give the instructor the option of not including selected chapters in the classroom lectures, or not assigning them as required reading material, if that is desired. For example, there are two chapters that present information on pediatric language disorders, and two chapters that pres-ent information on pediatric speech sound disorders. For each pair of chapters, one chapter presents information on two or three disorders, and the other presents information on two or three other disorders. An instructor who decides to present examples of a particular pediatric language or speech sound disorder can surely choose one chapter for a lecture and assign (or not) the other chapter for reading. The same can be said of several other chapters in the textbook. In this sense, we believe the textbook is a flexible instructional companion for both instructors and students.

The graduate training of speech-language pathologists (SLPs) and audiologists (AuDs) is a significant mission of CS&D departments. Communication Sciences and Disorders is, at its core, a clinical discipline. But if a clinical endeavor is to be disciplined, the core must include material that supports and motivates clinical practice with knowledge that has emerged from the research laboratory. This text is primarily concerned with the scientific basis of clinical practice, the former being a first step to qualify for the latter professional skill.

Clinical information is not ignored in the textbook. In fact, all chapters that present the nature of language, speech, and hearing disorders include some information on diagnosis and treatment of communication disorders. In some chapters, this information is integrated with the presentation of the main material, in others a brief section describes clinical issues relevant to the communication disorder(s) under discussion. A fixed formula is not used for the inclusion of clinical information in various chapters of the textbook; rather, in each chapter that presents information on communication disorders, the clinical information is placed in the location that seemed (in our opinion) to make the most sense.

Page 14: IntroductIon to communIcatIon ScIenceS and dISorderS€¦ · Introduction to Communication Sciences and Disorders:viii The Scientific Basis of Clinical Practice 9 Language Disorders

Introduction to Communication Sciences and Disorders: The Scientific Basis of Clinical Practice xvi

Curricula in departments of CS&D are structured to include classes on typical and dis-ordered language, on typical and disordered speech, and on typical and disordered hearing. This is to say that language, speech, and hearing occupy three different categories of course-work. The categories are organized more for the structure of a curriculum, rather than a belief that language, speech, and hearing processes are separate. They are not. The integrated nature of language, speech, and hearing processes, whether typical (normal) or disordered, is known by all clinicians and scientists concerned with communication sciences and disorders. For example, a child who is seen in the clinic for a delay in the mastery of speech sounds often has delays in language acquisition as well, and is at risk for reading delays. Similarly, an Ameri-can child who is born deaf may have delays in oral language development but have typical language development in American Sign Language (ASL).

This textbook follows the approach of separating language, speech, and hearing chapters. But we ask students to keep in mind that this is a teaching decision (much like the organiza-tion of courses, as stated earlier), not a statement that the areas are separate. Language chap-ters are presented first, followed by speech chapters and then hearing chapters; this sequence is arbitrary. One of us (GW) taught the introductory course in the University of Wisconsin–Madison CS&D department for 20 years, changing the order of the language, speech, and hearing categories several times to see if one sequence was more effective than others; the order did not seem to make a difference.

The textbook covers a lot of information; this is a necessary feature of a text designed to be the primary reading material for a survey course in communication sciences and disorders. Some areas of the field may be mentioned only briefly, which does not mean we believe they do not merit careful discussion. Decisions were made to limit discussion of certain areas to a minimum to accommodate the goal of a compact textbook.

Two final comments are in order. First, the use of pronouns is an efficient and straight-forward way to construct sentences in a textbook with frequent references to people. In cases (which constitutes most of the uses) we have chosen to limit pronouns to “he” and “she,” and to alternate between the use of the two when the reference is to a person who is (for example), a clinician or person seeking services. Second, the pattern and extent of citations vary across chapters. Every effort has been made to provide interested students and instructors with up-to-date references, and with review papers that provide overviews of the current state of both the research and clinical aspects of a topic under study.

We hope the textbook and the course are effective in creating an enhanced understanding of the importance of successful communication, and of the need to understand the impact of a communication disorder on every aspect of an individual’s life.

Happy learning!

Page 15: IntroductIon to communIcatIon ScIenceS and dISorderS€¦ · Introduction to Communication Sciences and Disorders:viii The Scientific Basis of Clinical Practice 9 Language Disorders

xvii

AcknowledgmentsKalie Koscielak, Valerie Johns, and Angie Singh, we are indebted to you for years of support and encouragement.

Susan Ellis Weismer had a profound influence on the shaping of Chapters 3, 5, 6, 7, and 8. Professor Ellis Weismer read and reread successive drafts of these chapters, each time making spot-on suggestions for revision. We cannot thank her enough.

Once again, as it is with previous textbooks, Maury Aaseng’s beautiful artwork is a defining feature of this textbook. Thanks, Maury.

Thanks to Professor Susan Thibeault, and Eileen Peterson, for their gracious offer and prepa-ration of images for Chapter 18.

Thanks to Denny and Shelley Weismer for the photo of Friday, their African gray.

Thanks to Professor Jenny Hoit for her enormous and generous influence on several parts of this textbook.

Anna Ollinger read drafts of several chapters and made excellent suggestions for clarification of concepts and organization.

Thanks to Professor Steven Kramer for his influence on the audiology portions of this textbook.

The people named are not responsible for any errors that may exist in the textbook; whatever errors exist are solely our responsibility.

Page 16: IntroductIon to communIcatIon ScIenceS and dISorderS€¦ · Introduction to Communication Sciences and Disorders:viii The Scientific Basis of Clinical Practice 9 Language Disorders
Page 17: IntroductIon to communIcatIon ScIenceS and dISorderS€¦ · Introduction to Communication Sciences and Disorders:viii The Scientific Basis of Clinical Practice 9 Language Disorders

xix

ReviewersPlural Publishing, Inc., and the authors would like to thank the following reviewers for taking the time to provide their valuable feedback during the development process:

Gretchen Bennett, MA, CCC-SLPNYS Licensed Speech-Language PathologistCoordinator of Speech-Language Clinical

ServicesClinical Associate Professor/SupervisorSUNY at Buffalo Speech-Language and

Hearing Clinic

Kate Bunton, PhD, CCC-SLPAssociate ProfessorSpeech, Language, and Hearing SciencesUniversity of Arizona

Jaime Fatás-Cabeza, MMAAssociate ProfessorDirector of Translation and InterpretationDepartment of Spanish and PortugueseUniversity of Arizona

Vicki L. Hammen, PhD, CCC-SLPProfessor and Program DirectorCommunication DisordersIndiana State University

Jennifer M. Hatfield, MHS, CCC-SLPSpeech-Language PathologistClinical Assistant ProfessorIndiana University, South Bend

Rachel Kasthurirathne, MA, CCC-SLPIndiana University, Bloomington

Breanna Krueger, PhD, CCC-SLPUniversity of Wyoming

Florence Lim-Hardjono, MA, PhD (ABD), CCC-SLPMount Vernon Nazarene University

Avinash Mishra, PhD, CCC-SLPUniversity of Connecticut

Elisabeth A. Mlawski, PhD, CCC-SLPAssistant ProfessorMonmouth University

Nikki Murphy, MS, CCC-SLPUniversity of Nevada, Reno

Kelly S. Teegardin, MS, CCC-SLP, LSLS Cert AVTInstructor ICommunication Sciences and DisordersUniversity of South Florida

Angela Van Sickle, PhD, CCC-SLPTexas Tech University Health Sciences

Center

Jason A. Whitfield, PhD, CCC-SLPBowling Green State University

Page 18: IntroductIon to communIcatIon ScIenceS and dISorderS€¦ · Introduction to Communication Sciences and Disorders:viii The Scientific Basis of Clinical Practice 9 Language Disorders
Page 19: IntroductIon to communIcatIon ScIenceS and dISorderS€¦ · Introduction to Communication Sciences and Disorders:viii The Scientific Basis of Clinical Practice 9 Language Disorders

For Susan

For Dianne

Page 20: IntroductIon to communIcatIon ScIenceS and dISorderS€¦ · Introduction to Communication Sciences and Disorders:viii The Scientific Basis of Clinical Practice 9 Language Disorders
Page 21: IntroductIon to communIcatIon ScIenceS and dISorderS€¦ · Introduction to Communication Sciences and Disorders:viii The Scientific Basis of Clinical Practice 9 Language Disorders

1

1

Introduction to Communication Sciences and Disorders

We would build a profession independent of medicine or psychology or speech, based in colleges and public schools.

— Van Riper, 1981

INTRoDuCTIoN: CoMMuNICATIoN SCIeNCeS AND DISoRDeRS AS A DISCIPLINe

This is how Charles Van Riper, one of the pioneers of the field of Communication Sciences and Disorders, remembered the early 20th-century beginnings of the discipline. From the time he began to speak as a child, Van Riper had a severe stuttering problem. In young adulthood, he continued to stutter and desperately sought a “scientific” explanation for his problem. He reasoned that if an explanation could be identified through a program of systematic discovery — a pro-gram of scientific research — treatment methods would follow from the explanations, perhaps leading to a cure for stuttering.

Van Riper interacted with a small group of indi-viduals, several of whom were also people who stut-tered; jointly they decided to break away from the

domination of medical and Freudian perspectives on speech disorders. In 1925, approximately 25 individuals established an independent society called the American Academy of Speech Correction. This society was intended as a research organization. One of the charter members of this organization was Dr. Sara Mae Stinchfield, who was the first person in the United States to be awarded a PhD (from the University of Wisconsin) in the field of Speech Pathology. In 1929, the organization changed its name to the American Society for the Study of Disorders of Speech. The word “Study” in the organization’s new name highlighted the scientific goals of the group. This contrasted with the more practical but (in the opinion of some of the founding members of that society) less lofty goal of treating Communication Sciences and Dis-orders. “Speech teachers,” or people who attempted to help individuals with problems such as stuttering, articulation disorders, language delay, speech and lan-guage problems associated with neurological disease, or unintelligible speech resulting from absence or loss of hearing, were well known in society but certainly not professional mainstays in schools and hospitals.

The newly minted American Society for the Study of Disorders of Speech struggled a bit because of small membership and some disagreements among mem-bers. As recounted by Van Riper (1981), several of the

Page 22: IntroductIon to communIcatIon ScIenceS and dISorderS€¦ · Introduction to Communication Sciences and Disorders:viii The Scientific Basis of Clinical Practice 9 Language Disorders

Introduction to Communication Sciences and Disorders: The Scientific Basis of Clinical Practice 2

influential members wanted the group to focus on sci-entific investigation of stuttering, but others saw the world of Communication Sciences and Disorders more broadly. Pauline Camp, who was serving as the head of speech correction in the State of Wisconsin, proposed that the field could grow by establishing speech cor-rection clinics in universities. These clinics would train future “speech correctionists” as well as scientists inter-ested in the nature and cause of speech disorders. As trained clinicians found employment in public schools and demonstrated their ability to help children with speech problems, the need for additional trained pro-fessionals would increase, and the American Society for the Study of Disorders of Speech would grow.

Camp’s proposed strategy for growing the pro-fession was right on target. University programs were developed, with the training of “service providers” (clinicians) and scientists conducted in the same envi-ronment. The guiding principle of this training concept was the presence of clinicians and scientists in a com-mon environment, teaching each other and enhancing their respective knowledge and performance. Scien-tists formulated more specific and worthy research questions by obtaining information about the clinical details of communication problems in actual patients, and clinicians sharpened their diagnostic procedures and practice techniques by learning from the research. This training model has persisted until the present day, and has been successful.

In 1934, the young speech organization, much larger than it was in 1930, was reconstituted under a third name: the American Speech Correction Association. This name stuck until 1947, when the association was renamed the American Speech and Hearing Association, or ASHA. In 1978, the group was renamed the Ameri-can Speech-Language-Hearing Association, to recognize the equivalent importance of language function (as compared to the act of producing speech, or the ability to hear) in the understanding of normal and disorder- ed communication function. The association has retained this name to this day but is still referred to as “ASHA.”

As of 2018, ASHA reported a membership (including student members) of 203,945 individuals (https://www .asha.org/uploadedFiles/2018-Member-Counts .pdf).

Among the members of ASHA are 12,480 who have their primary training in Audiology and practice as Clinical Audiologists. Many of these professionals are also members of the American Academy of Audiol-ogy (AAA), an organization whose mission is to define the training and practice guidelines for professionals who work as clinical audiologists (https://www.audi-ology.org/about-us/academy-information). AAA was

founded in 1988, in recognition of the need for an orga-nization whose primary purpose would be serving the profession of Clinical Audiology. Many of the 12,000+ Audiologists who are members of ASHA are also mem-bers of the American Academy of Audiology.

There is a difference between the perspectives of ASHA and AAA on the right to practice Clinical Audi-ology. ASHA currently argues that a Clinical Audiolo-gist must have a Certificate of Clinical Competence in Audiology (CCC-A), issued by ASHA, as the proper credential for the practice of audiology. AAA’s posi-tion is that the CCC-A is not necessary for the practice of audiology; what is required is that students-in-training in audiology have a sequence of courses that is recognized as the foundation for training profes-sional audiologists, and that a year of professional work (much like an internship) follows the completion of the coursework training. In the view of AAA, this training prepares the student for state licensure as a Clinical Audiologist, which when obtained provides the “legal” right to practice clinical audiology. The different perspectives on the credentials needed by trainees to practice clinical audiology are complicated; readers are encouraged to visit https://www.audiology .org/publications-resources/document-library/audi-ology-licensure-vs-certification. There is a concerted effort among several different associations, including ASHA and AAA, to resolve these different perspec-tives (https://www.asha.org/uploadedFiles/Aligned-Sense-of-Purpose-for-the-Audiology-Profession.pdf).

CoMMuNICATIoN SCIeNCeS AND DISoRDeRS: THe WHoLe IS GReATeR THAN THe SuM oF ITS PARTS

When Van Riper remembered the early vision of a dis-cipline “independent of medicine or psychology or speech,” he was not thinking of abandoning the con-tent of these other fields of study. Rather, he imagined an academic and clinical field with a separate identity, forged from the concepts and facts of medicine, psy-chology, and other disciplines, but clearly something different and new — a field with its own identity, able to stand on its own merits. It is comically ironic (to this author, at least) that over the past 10 to 15 years, two buzzwords on college campuses have been “inter-disciplinary research” and “translational research.” The field of Communication Sciences and Disorders embraced these two activities — in fact, defined itself by an interdisciplinary and translation mentality — long before they became fashionable and fundable claims in university settings.

Page 23: IntroductIon to communIcatIon ScIenceS and dISorderS€¦ · Introduction to Communication Sciences and Disorders:viii The Scientific Basis of Clinical Practice 9 Language Disorders

1 Introduction to Communication Sciences and Disorders 3

An Interdisciplinary Field

Communication Sciences and Disorders is a field prac-ticed and studied by individuals with expertise in a variety of academic and clinical disciplines. It is truly interdisciplinary, the product (but not merely the sum) of many different areas of knowledge. Speech is pro-duced by moving structures of the respiratory system, larynx, and vocal tract (the latter sometimes referred to as the “upper articulators,” including the tongue, lips, and jaw). Scientists and clinicians who are inter-ested in communication disorders must understand the anatomy (structure) and physiology (function) of these body parts. When a person speaks, air pressures and flows are generated throughout the speech mech-anism, and an acoustic signal (what you hear when someone talks) is emitted from the lips and/or nose. An understanding of these aerodynamic and acoustic phenomena of speech requires at least a foundation of knowledge of basic physics.

When the acoustic signal emerges from the talk-er’s mouth (or nose), it is metaphorically “aimed” at another person who receives it through his or her audi-tory mechanism. This makes it clear that the anatomy and physiology of the auditory system must be mas-tered by the person specializing in Communication Sciences and Disorders. As with the process of speech production, hearing and comprehending acoustic sig-nals involve complex mechanisms understood prop-erly only with a decent amount of knowledge in the areas of anatomy, physiology, and physics (and other areas as well).

Of course, when talkers produce speech, they want to communicate a message. The nature and structure of the message — what is being communicated, and the form it takes when it is spoken — is determined by linguistic-cognitive processes. For example, linguistic-cognitive processes are set into motion by the simple act of asking someone to have coffee. An idea must be developed and structured in linguistic terms according to the intent and wishes of the person doing the ask-ing. The idea is something like, “I want to spend time with this person and suggesting we have coffee at a comfortable café seems like a good approach,” but the manner in which this “want” is structured as a message can vary wildly, depending on many factors. “Would you like to have coffee?” “Hey, how ’bout we grab some coffee?” “I’m really sleepy, let’s stop at Completely Wired and get some coffee.” “I’d really like to talk to you over coffee.” “Let’s have a no-obligation date over coffee.” “Coffee?” These different ways to convey the same message reflect variation in underlying cognitive processes and linguistic structure, both of which are

critical to language usage. The clinician and scientist in Communication Sciences and Disorders deal with disorders of language structure and usage, and must therefore have expertise in the broad areas of hearing, cognition, and linguistics.

The term “cognitive-linguistic” refers to psychological processes applied to the use of language forms. “Cogni-

tion” refers to several psychological processes, including memory; executive function (e.g., planning behavior, connecting current behavior with future consequences); the development, refinement, and stabilization of mental represen-tations; brain computation speeds; and transfer of information from one type of memory (e.g., short-term memory) to another (e.g., long-term memory). These various aspects of cognition are listed here as separate processes but in fact may overlap and in some cases be different reflections of a single psychological process. “Linguistic” refers to any aspect of language form — sounds, words, sentences, tone of voice, and so forth. The term “cognitive-linguistic” is used here to indi-cate that the psychological processes previously listed (among others) are applied to language forms and therefore to communication. The same cognitive processes are applied to other forms of knowledge, as well (such as spatial reasoning or mathematics).

We are not done. Because speech and language develop throughout infancy and childhood and may change throughout the lifetime and especially in old age, expertise in Communication Sciences and Disor-ders requires a solid knowledge of child development and aging. Most obvious, perhaps, is the need to have a broad and deep expertise concerning the many dis-eases and conditions associated with speech, hearing, and language disorders. Extensive medical knowledge is absolutely necessary to function as an effective spe-cialist in Communication Sciences and Disorders. This knowledge ranges from how surgeries on structures of (for example) the brain, tongue, and ear affect speech, hearing, and language function, to how pharmaceutical interventions (such as drugs for Parkinson’s disease, or schizophrenia, or even chronic arthritis) may change a patient’s ability to communicate.

Finally, legal and technical issues are relevant to the profession of Communication Sciences and Disorders.

Page 24: IntroductIon to communIcatIon ScIenceS and dISorderS€¦ · Introduction to Communication Sciences and Disorders:viii The Scientific Basis of Clinical Practice 9 Language Disorders

Introduction to Communication Sciences and Disorders: The Scientific Basis of Clinical Practice 4

These issues concern a person’s right to receive the proper services when he or she has a speech, hearing, or language disorder, as well as the requirements for professional accreditation as someone who can pro-vide services or train people to provide services, or the requirement of extensive training in research to mentor students who intend to devote their careers to research. Our field has been fortunate to have professional lead-ers who can lay claim to both clinical and research expertise.

Table 1–1 provides a partial summary of the areas of knowledge and, in many cases, expertise, required of the professional in Communication Sciences and Disorders. This list includes the areas previously men-tioned and adds a few more for good measure. There are (at least) two ways to react to this list. One is to feel intimidated by the need to know so much about so many areas. The other is to look at the combination of these different types of knowledge as something spe-cial, as an opportunity to be informed about many dif-ferent areas of study and, most importantly, to employ an integrated and synthesized fund of this information in an understanding of the most human of behaviors, communication. Of course, a single individual is not likely to be an accomplished expert in each of these areas, but a commitment to learn the basic principles of each of the disciplines listed in Table 1–1, to use this knowledge when providing clinical services to a per-son with a communication disorder, to function as an effective member of a clinical or research team, or to develop an answer to a research question, is genuinely exciting. Communication Sciences and Disorders is the original, lifelong learning discipline.1

Translational Research

Researchers and clinicians are often trained in the same department and yet do not interact professionally to a significant degree. This has been a concern in various branches of medicine, as well as in departments such as Psychology, and Communication Sciences and Dis-orders. Many scientists in these professions are trained to do something they understand as “basic science.” In basic science, research questions are asked for the sake of improving the knowledge base in a field, or to address purely theoretical questions. An assumption of this approach to research has been that basic science, if done well, will eventually have an effect on clinical practice. In this way of thinking, “basic science” does

not need to be motivated or prompted by immediate clinical concerns; any improvement in knowledge of the world must have implications for the betterment of humankind.

TABLe 1–1. Some Areas of Knowledge Required for People to be Effective Professionals in the Field of Communication Sciences and Disorders

Neuroscience

Brain anatomy (structure)

Brain physiology (function)

Neuropharmacology (chemicals and their role in brain function)

Motor control (how brain controls movement)

Sensory function (how brain processes sensation)

Anatomy and Physiology of the Speech Mechanism (muscles, ligaments, membranes, cartilages, etc., associated with the respiratory, laryngeal, and upper airway system, which collectively are called the “speech mechanism”)

Anatomy and Physiology of the Hearing Mechanism (bones, membranes, ligaments, special structures of the ear)

Child Development

Aging

Diseases of the Head, Neck, Respiratory System, Auditory System, and Brain

Syndromes

Physics

Aerodynamics

Acoustics

Movement

Cognition

Memory and Processing

Planning

Manipulation and Use of Symbols

Linguistics

Phonetics and Phonology

Morphology

Syntax

Semantics

Pragmatics

1 As a university professor in Communication Sciences and Disorders, I more than once told students that it was hard to believe someone was willing to pay me to come to my office every day, learn new things in many different areas, and use this information in my research, in the classroom, and in mentoring teaching (one-on-one instruction, as with graduate students training to be researchers).

Page 25: IntroductIon to communIcatIon ScIenceS and dISorderS€¦ · Introduction to Communication Sciences and Disorders:viii The Scientific Basis of Clinical Practice 9 Language Disorders

1 Introduction to Communication Sciences and Disorders 5

Let’s consider an example of a possible link between basic science and clinical application. A fair number of scientists have investigated birdsong and its relation-ship to the evolution of human language (reviews can be found in Fitch, 2000, 2006, and Deacon, 1998). Much of this work has been funded by a federal agency, the National Institutes of Health (NIH), whose primary mission is to sponsor research that ultimately improves health care in the United States. The research on bird-song (and vocalizations produced by other, nonhuman species) has been “sold” to the federal agency by claim-ing potential links between, on the one hand, an under-standing of why and how birds sing, and on the other hand, a better understanding of speech and language capabilities in humans. The link between birdsong and human communication is evolutionary, in which bird-song is a “step” along the evolutionary path to human vocalization for purposes of communication. The rea-soning is extended by arguing that a better understand-ing of the basic “mechanisms” of vocal communication, which can be studied in birds using techniques that cannot be used in humans,2 should eventually lead to a better understanding of the partial or complete failure of similar mechanisms in humans. A better understand-ing of disease-related problems in human vocalization should, this reasoning concludes, result in better ways to diagnose and treat human vocalization disorders.

Basic science such as work on birdsong has been criticized for occupying federal funds that might be used to fund “applied” research. “Applied science” is research with more immediate clinical consequences, research with less distance between the results of a study and its potential use in clinical settings. For example, funding could be provided for a research program in which participants with healthy voices are enrolled in a vocal exercise regime (like the kind of warm-up exercises used by many professional singers) and compared to a group of participants who do not engage in this exercise (a “control group”). The applied research question is, do nonspeech vocal exercises gen-eralize, or translate, to the use of the voice in everyday speech? Perhaps the effect of the vocal exercise could be evaluated by having listeners judge the quality of participants’ voices, with the critical comparison being the “goodness” (pleasing quality?) of voices pre- versus postexercise. This is basic, nonclinical research — non-clinical because the participants do not have voice dis-orders — but a positive result, where exercise produces a more pleasing voice, points more directly to a specific clinical application in patients with voice problems.

The relatively new buzzword for applied sci-ence is “translational research,” or research in which the results of basic science can be translated relatively quickly to clinical application. The hypothetical vocal exercise study is one example of translational research; many others have been proposed (see Ludlow et al., 2008; Raymer et al., 2008). The National Institutes of Health (NIH), the federal agency having the mission of funding and setting priorities for health-care-related research activities in the United States, published in 2008 the following text on its website concerning trans-lational research:

To improve human health, scientific discoveries must be translated into practical applications. Such dis-coveries typically begin at “the bench” with basic research — in which scientists study disease at a molec-ular or cellular level — then progress to the clinical level, or the patient’s “bedside.”

Scientists are increasingly aware that this bench-to-bedside approach to translational research is a two-way street. Basic scientists provide clinicians with new tools for use in patients and for assessment of their impact, and clinical researchers make novel observa-tions about the nature and progression of disease that often stimulate basic science. See https://nexus.od.nih .gov/all/2016/03/25/nihs-commitment-to-basic-sci ence/ for a summary of the benefits of funding both kinds of research.

The National Institute on Deafness and Other Com-munication Disorders (NIDCD), the NIH institute that is the primary funder of research in Communication Sciences and Disorders, has a specific funding program for translational research (as of 2017). This funding mechanism is called the Research Grants for Translating Basic Research into Clinical Tools. The stated objective and requirements of these grants are as follows:

[T]o provide support for research studies that trans-late basic research findings into better clinical tools for human health. The application should seek to trans-late basic behavioral or biological research findings, which are known to be directly connected to a human clinical condition, to a practical clinical impact. Tools or technologies advanced through this FOA [Funding Opportunity Announcement] must overcome existing obstacles and should provide improvements in the diagnosis, treatment or prevention of a disease process. For the purposes of this FOA, the basic science advance-

2 Such as creating a small area of brain damage to see how it affects the development of birdsong, or depriving a newborn bird of exposure to his or her species’ song to determine if, as the baby bird matures, the song develops in the same way as in birds who are exposed to their song from birth.

Page 26: IntroductIon to communIcatIon ScIenceS and dISorderS€¦ · Introduction to Communication Sciences and Disorders:viii The Scientific Basis of Clinical Practice 9 Language Disorders

Introduction to Communication Sciences and Disorders: The Scientific Basis of Clinical Practice 6

ment must have previously demonstrated potential for clinical impact and the connection to a human clinical condition must be clearly established. The research must be focused on a disease/disorder within one or more of the NIDCD scientific mission areas: hearing, balance, smell, taste, voice, speech, or language.

Research conducted under this FOA is expected to include human subjects. Preclinical studies in animal models are allowed only for a candidate therapeutic that has previously demonstrated potential for the treatment of communication disorders. The scope of this FOA allows for a range of activities encouraging the translation of basic research findings to practical impact on the diagnosis, treatment, and prevention of deafness and other communication disorders. [https://grants .nih.gov/grants/guide/pa-files/PAR-17-184.html]

The first statement presents the issue of “trans-lational research” with molecular or cellular work as the basic science, but basic science exists at the behav-ioral level of analysis, as well. This is why the NIDCD description mentions a “range of activities” in its mis-sion to fund translational research in Communication Sciences and Disorders.

Both of these NIH statements imply that it is the basic scientist’s obligation to show how laboratory results can be “translated” to clinical settings. This is in contrast to earlier models of the basic science/applied science dichotomy, in which the basic scientist might have said, “I’ll do the bench work (very basic science) and down the road, perhaps way down the road, clini-cians can figure out how to use my findings when they diagnose and treat patients.” In this view, the clinician, not the scientist, has the primary responsibility for translating the basic science to clinical application. The second paragraph of the statement sounds remarkably similar to the concept, described previously, of train-ing “speech correctionists” in university settings where clinical practice informs the direction of research pro-grams, and research findings enhance clinical practice. Pauline Camp suggested this concept in 1934, and our discipline has been guided by the “two-way street” philosophy since that time. As a field, we have under-stood the potential value of “translational research” for a long time.

Does the Basic Science Work? Does the Clinic Work?

It is all well and good to claim that people in the field of Communication Sciences and Disorders understood the value of interdisciplinary work, and practiced translational research well before the concept was so christened and attained the status of an official move-

ment on 21st-century university campuses and in gov-ernment funding agencies. It is quite another thing to claim scientific success as the result of interdisciplinary efforts, or to show that basic science has indeed been translated to clinical application. A major goal of this text is to present introductory information on normal and disordered communication processes in a way that highlights previous, and the latest, scientific findings that have emerged from interdisciplinary thinking. For the time being, the reader is asked to trust the claim that the growth of the scientific basis of normal com-munication processes, and Communication Sciences and Disorders, has been nothing short of spectacular over the last 50 years. None of this would have been possible if speech, language, and hearing scientists had not been open to the influences and thinking of scientists in areas such as linguistics, physiology, neuroscience, and psychology (among others). Most importantly, the openness of these scientists to the experience and knowledge of clinical speech-language pathologists and audiologists has made a huge differ-ence to the growth of the scientific knowledge base in normal and disordered communication.

It is not a goal of this text to present detailed information on therapy (management) techniques for persons with speech, language, and/or hearing disor-ders. Readers will learn a great deal about speech, lan-guage, and hearing disorders, but a full treatment of clinical processes and procedures is a topic for a more advanced course of study, typically in graduate pro-grams (see later in the chapter).

An aspect of the clinical process that is discussed throughout this text is the diagnosis of speech, lan-guage, and/or hearing disorders. Technically, diagno-sis involves the identification and determination of the nature and cause of a disorder. Notice the inclusion of “nature” in this definition. Proper techniques must be employed to describe a disorder and to document the characteristics of a communication disorder that make it different from other communication disorders. A good part of this text is therefore devoted to descrip-tions of how we know a specific speech, language, and/or hearing disorder is “x” and not “y.”

This text does not shy away from controversies in our field about the nature and causes of certain com-munication disorders. As in any health-care-related field, many diagnoses remain unclear and are the subject of ongoing debate. In the best of all worlds (sorry,Voltaire), we would welcome absolute certainty concerning the diagnosis of human diseases and con-ditions. The world-as-is, however, does not allow such certainty, but let’s not regard the gray areas as defeats; they are opportunities. Uncertainty and controversy have always been the engines of scientific advance-ment. Not knowing, or disagreement about what we

Page 27: IntroductIon to communIcatIon ScIenceS and dISorderS€¦ · Introduction to Communication Sciences and Disorders:viii The Scientific Basis of Clinical Practice 9 Language Disorders

1 Introduction to Communication Sciences and Disorders 7

do know, pushes science forward. Diagnosis, then, is a critical part of the scientific underpinnings of a health-care-related discipline such as Communication Sciences and Disorders. In many cases, questions con-cerning clinical diagnosis and the basic science founda-tion of our field are completely intertwined.

The second part of the heading for this section asks, “Does the Clinic Work?” Do speech-language patholo-gists and audiologists make a difference in the lives of people with communication disorders? Although this text does not present detailed information about treat-ment of communication disorders, there is widespread evidence for treatment success.

It is important for the reader to know that many of the services offered by clinicians in our field have been documented as being effective. In the absence of such documentation, the entire enterprise of train-ing clinicians to treat communication disorders could be questioned. Fortunately, our interdisciplinary and translational approach to understanding communica-tion disorders has produced diagnosis and manage-ment techniques that are effective for many patients. A selective sampling of publications in which this clinical success is reviewed includes results for voice therapy (Angadi, Croke, & Stemple, 2017; Desjardins, Halstead, Cooke, & Bonilha, 2017; Ramig &Verdolini, 1998; Ruotsalainen, Sellman, Lehto, Jauhiainen, & Verbeek, 2007), hearing disorders (Ferguson, Kitter-ick, Chong, Edmonson-Jones, Barker, & Hoare, 2017; Kaldo-Sandström, Larsen, & Andersson, 2004; Mendel 2007), stuttering (Baxter et al., 2015; R. Ingham, J.C. Ingham, Bothe, Wang, & Kilgo, 2015; Tasko, McClean, & Runyan, 2007), childhood articulatory disorders (Gierut, 1998; Wren, Harding, Goldbart, & Roulstone, 2018), and childhood language disorders (Law, Garrett, & Nye, 2008; Tyler, Lewis, Haskill, & Tolbert (2003). Students who obtain undergraduate and graduate degrees in our field learn the scientific basis and tech-nical details of these successful clinical strategies. This is not to say that we have conquered all, or even many, of the communication disorders affecting people around the world. Indeed, there is a substantial amount of disagreement concerning precisely what constitutes therapy “success” for people with communication disorders, and a specific therapy technique may work for some patients but not others. But the articles listed previously show a pattern of success for many com-munication disorders; continuing research will add to this list.

evidence-Based Practice

Although this text does not present detailed informa-tion on management (treatment) of communication

disorders, the concept of evidence-based practice (EBP) and its role in speech, language, and/or hearing ther-apy is integral to an understanding of how knowledge of typical and disordered communication is related to treatment of communication disorders.

EBP, a movement with roots in the medical world, takes as its central concept that any treatment approach should be supported by scientifically based evidence of the treatment’s effectiveness (the term “efficacy” is often used to refer to effectiveness of a therapy procedure, but the technical sense of “efficacy” is an experimental demonstration that a particular clinical technique shows promise as an effective management tool; it is like a first step in the determination of a treat-ment’s real-world effectiveness). The need to formalize such a notion may at first glance seem surprising, for should a treatment not be administered in the absence of solid evidence that it works? Again, in the best of all worlds this would be so, but in much of medicine and behavioral sciences, including Communication Sciences and Disorders, the effectiveness of treatments is often unknown or only partially supported by research data.

EBP must be based on proper outcome measures. Evidence for the success of a therapeutic approach requires the measurement of one or more variables after (or sometimes during) the treatment. Outcome measures should have the best possible face validity, meaning that the measures provide good indices of the phenomena they are supposed to represent. An example from basketball helps to understand face validity of outcome measures. If an outcome measure is desired for a player’s in-game shooting accuracy fol-lowing several months of intense practice of nongame, unguarded shooting, the percentage of shots made over 100 attempts has good face validity if the measure is taken during games. The measure has much poorer face validity if the measure is taken over 100 shots attempted during multiple games of HORSE. Shoot-ing percentage during games is a much better outcome measure for “real-world” shooting as compared to shooting percentage during games of HORSE.

An example from health care, closer to the con-cerns of this textbook, is one of drug treatment for epi-lepsy for which there may be the potential for multiple outcome measures with face validities that are only subtly different. The question is, after 6 months of drug treatment, are there fewer seizures as reported by the patient (one potential outcome measure)? As reported by the patient, are there no seizures over the same time period (a second potential outcome variable)? After 6 months of drug treatment, can a seizure be induced in the clinical setting by very bright flashing lights (a third potential outcome variable)? Or, after 6 months, are the blood levels of the drug in the “correct” range based on values reported in the scientific literature (a fourth

Page 28: IntroductIon to communIcatIon ScIenceS and dISorderS€¦ · Introduction to Communication Sciences and Disorders:viii The Scientific Basis of Clinical Practice 9 Language Disorders

Introduction to Communication Sciences and Disorders: The Scientific Basis of Clinical Practice 8

potential outcome variable)? At first glance, the first two outcome measures have the best face validity — the best evidence for reduction of seizures is a report from a patient that seizure episodes have been reduced or eliminated. Some clinicians and scientists, however, may think that patient-reported data are unreliable because they are subject to the notorious uncertainties of memory or even a patient’s misrepresentation of sei-zure history. Measures such as inducement of a seizure by flashing lights or drug blood levels are regarded as more objective (and have a clearly quantitative basis) and therefore may seem more reliable than the patient reports of seizure history. Yet, from the perspective of the patient, inducement of a seizure in a controlled clinical setting or “good” drug blood levels mean very little when he or she is losing consciousness two or three times a week or even having many episodes of preseizure activity.

The choice of a proper outcome measure (or mea-sures) is not straightforward and is often the subject of considerable debate. The debate is lively and even heated when the behaviors of speech, language, and hearing disorders are evaluated for their response to therapy. Readers may want to keep this in mind when considering the concept of EBP.

The concept of EBP has taken on a life of its own as an academic discipline, and there is no end to the debate about precisely what serves as “good” scientific evidence for the efficacy of a treatment. Table 1–2 pre-sents a six-level EBP model of “goodness” of evidence, with the “best” evidence at the top (Level I) and the worst at the bottom (Level VI). This simplified model of EBP serves the purposes of this discussion well and has been presented several times in the Communica-tion Sciences and Disorders literature (Dodd, 2007; Dollaghan, 2004; Moodie, Kothari, Bagatto, Seewald, Miller, & Scollie, 2011).

Levels of Evidence

Level I and II evidence are usually based on large numbers of participants to generate the most reli-able statistical results. In Table 1–2, Level I evidence is summarized as “systematic reviews” or “meta-analyses” of RCTs. An RCT is an experiment in which each individual from an initial, large pool of partici-pants is randomly assigned to one of two (or more) treatments. Ideally, neither the experimenters nor the participants have knowledge about which treatment has been assigned to any participant in the study. The participants (and in many cases, the experimenters) are “blind” to which participants have been assigned to which treatments, and the participants are “blind” to the status of their treatment condition (real treat-ment group, or placebo group). This is an example of a “double-blind” experiment.

In Table 1–2, Level I evidence is summarized as “systematic reviews” or “meta-analyses” of RCTs. A systematic review is the organization and evaluation of data from many different, individual RCTs, and a “meta-analysis” is a quantitative (statistical) analysis of the data from many such studies. A meta-analysis of the results of many different studies can only be done when the data from each study are sufficiently compa-rable — as when the same pretreatment and outcome measures were used in the different studies (such as number of seizures per week), the same blinding con-ditions, the same dosage levels, and so forth.

Level II evidence is the result from a single RCT. Level II evidence is high-level scientific evidence but is not as trustworthy as having many different dem-onstrations, from different laboratories and different scientists, of the same outcome. In other words, when Level II evidence is replicated several times, Level I evi-dence has been produced.

Level I and II evidence in communication Sci-ences and disorders. In Communication Sciences and Disorders, it is relatively difficult to obtain Level I and II evidence. How easy is it to find, for example, 100 people who have a similar stuttering problem, or 100 people who have had a stroke and who have very similar problems with expressing or comprehending speech? How easy is it to find 100 children with autism, who all have the same communication challenges and similar characteristics in noncommunication domains? In each of these cases, the answer is: Not easy at all. In addition, it is unusual for different laboratories that study communication disorders, and even a single communication disorder such as stuttering (as one example), to use the same measure of stuttering fre-

TABLe 1–2. Levels of Evidence Applied to Evidence-Based Practice: A Simplified Model

Level I Systematic reviews and meta-analyses of randomized controlled trials (RCTs)

Level II A single RCT

Level III Nonrandomized, controlled (well-designed) treatment studies

Level IV Nonexperimental studies

Level V Case reports and/or narrative literature reviews

Level VI Expert/authority opinion

Page 29: IntroductIon to communIcatIon ScIenceS and dISorderS€¦ · Introduction to Communication Sciences and Disorders:viii The Scientific Basis of Clinical Practice 9 Language Disorders

1 Introduction to Communication Sciences and Disorders 9

quency (perhaps number of stuttered words per 100 words produced). For these reasons and others, RCTs are unusual in our field.

Many RCTs in medical fields contrast an experi-mental group that receives a trial drug for a condition or disease, and a control group that receives a placebo. Both groups take pills on a schedule, but do not know if they are taking the experimental medication or sugar pills. Such experiments in Communication Sciences and Disorders raise an ethical question: how do you withhold treatment from a group of individuals with a communication disorder?

RCTs are unusual in our field. They are difficult to execute because of many factors, not the least of which is assembling an initial participant pool, with the same kind and degree of speech/language/hearing challenges, from which random assignment to different treatment types is possible. Perhaps this explains why some introductory texts in Communication Sciences and Disorders (e.g., Justice & Redle, 2014) choose to talk broadly about EBP from sources external to scien-tific pursuits. These sources include patient values and preferences, and clinician expertise (Sackett, Rosen-berg, Gray, Haynes, & Richardson, 1996). These factors are considered jointly with scientific data as contribu-tions to EBP in the life of a speech-language patholo-gist or audiologist. The absence of solid Level I and II “high-level” evidence in our field places greater weight on the other factors (patient preference, clinical experi-ence) in treatment decisions made by speech-language pathologists and audiologists.

Level III evidence. The description of Level III evi-dence in Table 1–2 is “non-randomized, controlled (well-designed) treatment studies.” As in the case of RCTs, two groups are typically studied and compared, one receiving Treatment X, the other Treatment Y (or no treatment). Level III evidence does not involve ran-domization from a pool of eligible subjects but must be well controlled in other ways.

Studies that produce Level III evidence are rela-tively common in the communication sciences and dis-orders literature. Level III evidence often comes from studies with a relatively small number (e.g., 10 to 20) of participants in each group, certainly smaller than the group numbers in (for example) drug trials. In addi-tion to the absence of randomization of participants to treatment conditions, the relatively small number of participants in Level III studies renders them less pow-erful statistically and, therefore, less “valued” than RCTs.

Level IV evidence. Level IV evidence is produced when a study is performed in the absence of proper

experimental controls. The lack of a control group whose performance can be compared to an experimen-tal group is a common problem in experiments that align with Level IV evidence.

Level IV-type evidence is found in in the speech, language, and hearing literature. Treatments are applied to a group of individuals with communication disorders, in the absence of proper controls. People with communication disorders improve following the treatment, and a conclusion is reached that the specific treatment is to be valued for its positive effect on the communication impairment. In the absence of controls, however, any form of treatment, not the specific treat-ment employed, may have improved the communica-tion skills of a group of persons with a communication impairment.

Levels V and VI evidence. Levels V and VI are types of evidence considered to be poor support for a treat-ment approach in any field. Case reports, which con-sider the outcome of a specific treatment applied to a single patient, or to a series of patients with similar characteristics, lack controls and cannot be generalized to a larger group of patients. The absence of experimen-tal controls and the study of only a single or few indi-viduals contribute heavily to the evaluation of this kind of evidence as “poor quality.” Even so, case reports are common in the health care literature, including the treatment literature in Communication Sciences and Disorders.

An argument can be made that case reports gain value when they are organized and synthesized in a single publication, with conclusions drawn from the careful analysis of results across reports. The problem with this line of thinking is that the primary problem of lack of experimental controls in each case report is not solved by accumulating many case stud-ies. The shared flaw of most case studies, of no experi-mental controls, means that a summary of many cases for the purpose of providing evidence to support a treatment approach is a summary of many flawed experiments.

Another type of Level V evidence is the narrative literature review. Narrative literature reviews are pub-lications in which a large number of research papers, most often those that provide Level III evidence, are organized and evaluated for the purpose of drawing qualitative conclusions about a focused issue. Narrative reviews are popular in Communication Sciences and Disorders and are published in leading journals. Narra-tive reviews have poor evidence quality for the purpose of supporting a treatment approach, because ultimately they are position papers, like editorials, with a primary

Page 30: IntroductIon to communIcatIon ScIenceS and dISorderS€¦ · Introduction to Communication Sciences and Disorders:viii The Scientific Basis of Clinical Practice 9 Language Disorders

Introduction to Communication Sciences and Disorders: The Scientific Basis of Clinical Practice 10

aim of persuading readers that their conclusion(s) is (are) preferable to alternate conclusions.3

The narrative review and its aim to persuade by summaries of existing research findings and theoretical issues, is a more scholarly version of the lowest evi-dence level, that of expert/authority opinion. Anyone can have an opinion that is stated as the likely truth. When “anyone” turns out to be an authority in a disci-pline, and asks that his or her position be accepted not on the basis of published data but on his or her author-ity, the evidence has little or no value.

The concept of EBP is firmly grounded in the inter-action and co-dependency of laboratory experiments and clinical practice. Scientists construct experiments to generate results in support of proper diagnosis and effective clinical management, and clinicians apply the findings to their patients and evaluate their real-world results. On the basis of those clinical results, scientists may adjust their experiments to provide additional and improved data for EBP.

A Typical undergraduate Curriculum

Table 1–3 shows the undergraduate, major curricu-lum in Communication Sciences and Disorders at the University of Wisconsin–Madison. This sequence of courses is more or less representative of curricula in any department in the United States that offers an undergraduate degree in our field (some variation will occur from department to department). The course for which this text was written is shown in parentheses because it is not a requirement in the UW–Madison department for an undergraduate major in the field. Rather, this course is taken each semester by a large number of students to satisfy a breadth requirement in the College of Letters and Science. Many students who choose to major in Communication Sciences and Disor-ders at UW–Madison do take the introductory course, and in many cases, the exposure to the field provided by the class is the reason they choose Communication Sciences and Disorders as their major.

A group of courses in the curriculum (Speech Sci-ence; Hearing Science; Neural Bases of Speech, Hear-ing, and Language; Speech Acoustics and Perception; Language Development in Children and Adolescents; the Phonetic Transcription module of Phonological Development and Disorders) establishes a solid sci-entific foundation for normal (typical) processes of communication. Other courses (the second part of

Phonological Development and Disorders; Voice, Cra-niofacial, and Fluency Disorders; parts of Neural Bases of Speech, Language, and Hearing Disorders; Auditory Rehabilitation; Child Language Disorders: Assessment and Intervention) provide basic information on the classification, causes, and nature of the many diseases and conditions associated with communication disor-ders. Some curricula may have a course called “Pre-clinical Observation,” in which students are introduced to the clinical process by observing clinical sessions, rather than being directly involved in diagnosing or treating communication disorders.

WHo ARe THe PRoFeSSIoNALS IN CoMMuNICATIoN SCIeNCeS AND DISoRDeRS?

Students obtain undergraduate and graduate degrees in preparation for a job. In the field of Communication Sciences and Disorders, this preparation is for employ-ment as a speech-language pathologist or audiologist in an educational or health care setting. Or, a student may prepare for a career as a professor in a college or university setting. At the undergraduate level, training is not differentiated across these different career paths. Nearly everyone who intends to be a professional in Communication Sciences and Disorders learns a com-mon scientific foundation for the field, as summarized in Table 1–3.

Preparation for, and the Profession of, Speech-Language Pathology

The requirements to practice as a speech-language pathologist (SLP) include coursework that furnishes a knowledge base specified by ASHA, completion of a master’s degree, a clinical fellowship, and success-ful performance on a national exam. The information presented here is based on ASHA’s published certifi-cation standards as of 2014, as well as some revisions and amendments to these standards published in 2016. ASHA documents are available at https://www.asha.org

Students finishing an undergraduate major in Communication Sciences and Disorders apply to mas-ter’s degree training programs in the fall semester of their senior year (or later, if they decide to take a year or two off before beginning graduate school). There are currently over 200 such training programs in the

3 The author feels free to point to the evidentiary weakness of narrative reviews because he has published several of them. Conversely, narrative reviews may organize the literature in a way that is useful for clinicians and scientists as they pursue their professional goals.