82
1 Outline 1. What is intelligence? 2. Can it be measured? 3. Differences in measured intelligence a. Within group a.Heritability b.Twin studies c.Manipulating environments to increase IQ b. Between groups 4. Are intelligence tests culturally biased? 5. Are there multiple intelligences? 6. Spearman’s g 7. Intelligence in daily life 8. Stanford-Binet

Intelligence

Embed Size (px)

DESCRIPTION

Intelligence & IQ

Citation preview

Page 1: Intelligence

1

Outline

1. What is intelligence?2. Can it be measured?3. Differences in measured intelligence

a. Within groupa. Heritabilityb. Twin studiesc. Manipulating environments to increase IQ

b. Between groups4. Are intelligence tests culturally biased?5. Are there multiple intelligences?6. Spearman’s g7. Intelligence in daily life8. Stanford-Binet

Page 2: Intelligence

2

Outline

1. What is intelligence?2. Can it be measured?3. Differences in measured intelligence

a. Within groupa. Heritabilityb. Twin studiesc. Manipulating environments to increase IQ

b. Between groups4. Are there multiple intelligences?5. Spearman’s g6. Intelligence in daily life7. Stanford-Binet8. Wechsler tests

Page 3: Intelligence

3

What is intelligence?

“Intelligence is a very general mental capability that, among other things, involves the ability to reason, plan, solve problems, think abstractly, comprehend complex ideas, learn quickly and learn from experience. It is not merely book learning, a narrow academic skill, or test-taking smarts. Rather, it reflects a broader and deeper capability for comprehending our surroundings – “catching on,” “making sense” of things, or “figuring out” what to do.” (Gottfredson, 1994)

Page 4: Intelligence

4

Can intelligence be measured?

Yes. And intelligence tests measure it well.

Page 5: Intelligence

5

Can intelligence be measured?

Psychologists define intelligence as “what IQ tests measure”

This makes sense. Intelligence tests

have been carefully created, revised, and improved for 100 years precisely to do that job.

Page 6: Intelligence

6

Can intelligence be measured?

Intelligence tests are accurate (they are reliable and valid)

Intelligence tests do not measure creativity, character, personality, or other individual differences

Page 7: Intelligence

7

Can intelligence be measured?

IQ scores from tests such as the Stanford-Binet and the WAIS are the best predictors we have of a variety of important outcomes

This includes life outcomes relating to health, career, personal relationships, and crime

Page 8: Intelligence

8

Differences in measured intelligence

a. Within groupsa. Heritability

b. Twin studies

c. Manipulating environments to increase IQ

b. Between groups

Page 9: Intelligence

9

Within group differences

Phenotypic variation

Total variation in the appearance of members of a species

Includes things that can be “made visible” (such as blood types)

Page 10: Intelligence

10

Within-group differences

Heritability the proportion of phenotypic variation in a population that is due to genetic variation among individuals in that population

Page 11: Intelligence

11

Within-group differences

Heritability If all environments were to become exactly equal for everyone, heritability would be 1.0 because all remaining differences in IQ would have to be biological

Page 12: Intelligence

12

Within-group differences

Heritability If every person were a clone (so that everyone had exactly the same genetic material), all remaining differences in IQ would have to be environmental (heritability would be 0)

Page 13: Intelligence

13

Within-group differences

Members of the same family tend to differ in IQ by about 12 points on average.

Siblings have different environments and also share only half their genes (on average)

Page 14: Intelligence

14

Within-group differences

Adopted children resemble their birth mothers more than their adoptive mothers in intelligence, even if they have never met their birth mothers

Plomin et al. (1997): no relation at all after early childhood between the IQs of adoptive parents and of the children they adopted

Page 15: Intelligence

15

Within-group differences

McGue et al. (1993): found an average correlation of zero for adoptive siblings tested as adults

Loehlin et al. (1997) on Texas Adoption Project: shared family environment influences IQ only for very young children; genetic effects increase with age

Page 16: Intelligence

16

Twin studies

1. Are identical twins more similar in IQ than fraternal twins?

2. Are fraternal twins more similar in IQ than pairs of non-twin siblings?

3. Are non-twin siblings more similar in IQ than unrelated children raised in the same home?

Page 17: Intelligence

17

Are identical twins more similar in IQ than fraternal twins?

Plomin, DeFries, McClearn, & McGuffin (2001) summarized results of studies of more than 10,000 pairs of twins

The average correlation of IQ scores for identical twins: .86

For the fraternal twins: .60

Page 18: Intelligence

18

Are fraternal twins more similar in IQ than pairs of non-twin siblings?

Fraternal twins are no more genetically similar to each other than any pair of non-twin siblings

But fraternal twins are more similar to each other in IQ than non-twin siblings.

Why? Jensen (1998): Prenatal factors

such as mother’s age, nutrition, health

Blood antigen incompatibilities

Obstetrical procedures

Page 19: Intelligence

19Are non-twin siblings more similar in IQ than unrelated children raised in the same home?

Jensen (1998) analyzed 27,000 sibling pairs

average IQ for ordinary siblings reared together was .49

correlation of .25 for IQs of unrelated persons raised together when IQ was measured during childhood

this correlation dropped to 0 when the children were tested as adults

Page 20: Intelligence

20

Does this mean intelligence is hereditary?

Intelligence is partially inherited

About 50% of the variability in IQ scores can be traced to genetic influences

Differences in environment quality are more important for younger children than for adolescents

Page 21: Intelligence

21

Does this mean intelligence is hereditary?

The nature of nurture effect – part of the effect of the environment may be genetic

Differences in environments may be produced by genetic differences

E.g., an artist and an engineer may create very different environments for their children

Page 22: Intelligence

22

Within-group differences

Can we manipulate environments to increase IQ?

U.S. Government’s Head Start program

Milwaukee Project Carolina

Abecedarian project

Page 23: Intelligence

23

Head Start program

Modest gains Smallest gains for

children most at risk

Gains do not survive long after child leaves the program

Neisser et al. (1996): by end of elementary school, no differences between those in vs. not in program

Page 24: Intelligence

24

Milwaukee project

Modest gains Effects do not last

long after exit from program

Gains only on tests requiring skills taught in the program – effects do not generalize to new tasks

Page 25: Intelligence

25

Carolina Abecedarian project

Intensive intervention provided daily for the first 5 years of the child’s life

IQ scores for intervention group 5 points higher than for control group at age 12 (7 years after exit from program)

Positive effect on failure & drop-out rates

Page 26: Intelligence

26

Are intelligence tests culturally biased?

No. Tests of widely varying kinds (e.g., verbal abilities, spatial abilities), including those considered most “fair,” give the same results.

Page 27: Intelligence

27

Are intelligence tests culturally biased?

Group differences just as large on Ravens Progressive Matrices as on WAIS

IQ scores have same utility for prediction regardless of race or socio-economic status.

Page 28: Intelligence

28

Bias vs. Fairness

It’s important to distinguish between these two concepts:

Bias Unfairness

Page 29: Intelligence

29

Bias vs. Fairness

A test is biased if it gives a systematically wrong result when used to predict something.

So, an intelligence test would be biased if, for example, it underestimated one group’s probability of success in a given endeavor.

Page 30: Intelligence

30

Bias vs. Fairness

Use of a test is unfair if it treats people differently

E.g., if a verbal test probes for knowledge acquired from schooling, use of that test with people who have not had such schooling would be unfair

Note that the test itself is not implicitly unfair – but use of the test may be unfair

Page 31: Intelligence

31

Bias vs. Fairness

When you use an unfair test, the result need not be biased. The result may still have good predictive value.

E.g., if you test non-native speakers of English with the SAT, that use of the test is unfair, but not biased

Results will predict academic success in English-speaking countries.

Page 32: Intelligence

32

Are group differences in IQ real?

Yes. Members of all ethnic/racial groups are found at all levels of IQ.

But groups vary in where their scores cluster (that is, in the means).

Page 33: Intelligence

It’s important to note that the group differences are in “central tendency” (mean) – there is lots of overlap, and all groups are represented at low, medium, and high levels of IQ

IQ

Page 34: Intelligence

34

Are group differences in IQ real?

Highest IQ scores are for Ashkenazi Jews

Cochran et al. (2006): medieval social environment for European Jews selected for verbal & math intelligence (but not spatial)

Some relation to disease genes?

Page 35: Intelligence

35

Are group differences in IQ real?

Curves for some Asians are somewhat higher than for Whites; curves for Blacks, Hispanics somewhat lower than for Whites

We don’t know why these effects are found, but there is much debate on this question

Page 36: Intelligence

36

Sources of between-group differences

Next two slides have statements from leading scholars in the field on what we know (and don’t know) about the sources of between-group differences in measured intelligence

Gottfredson (1997), Intelligence Neisser et al. (1996), American

Psychologist

Page 37: Intelligence

37

Sources of between-group differences

“There is no definitive answer to why IQ bell curves differ across racial-ethnic groups. The reasons for these IQ differences may be markedly different from the reasons for why individuals differ among themselves within any particular group… Most experts believe that environment is important in pushing the bell curves apart, but that genetics could be involved, too.” Statement of the 52 experts, Intelligence, 1997, p.15.

Page 38: Intelligence

38

Sources of between-group differences

“It is clear that genes make a substantial contribution to individual differences in intelligence test scores, at least in the White population. The fact is, however, that the high heritability of a trait within a given group has no necessary implications for the source of a difference between groups…Thus the issue ultimately comes down to a personal judgment: How different are the relevant life experiences of Whites and Blacks in the United States today? At present, this question has no scientific answer.” Neisser et al., (1996), p.95

Page 39: Intelligence

39

Sources of between-group differences

Gene-based temperamental factors?

Family size (now decreasing in N.A.)?

SES?but differences

present when SES controlled

Caste?Lacking “effort

optimism” (Ogbu, 1978)

Page 40: Intelligence

40

Sources of between-group differences

Priming effects Bargh, Chen, &

Burrows (1996) IV: elderly

stereotype DV: Walking speed Primed subjects

walked more slowly

Dijksterhuis & van Knippenberg (1998) IV: intelligence/

stupidity traits primed DV: performance on

a knowledge test Trait primes affected

performance

Page 41: Intelligence

41

Sources of between-group differences

Relevance of the priming studies

Stereotypes may influence those who are stereotyped

Direct effect on test performance?

Indirect effect on effort optimism?

Both?

Page 42: Intelligence

42

Sources of between-group differences

Culture (Boykin, 1996) Assessment itself

alienates? American

schooling conflicts with deep structure of African-American culture?

Members of ethnic groups might answer some items differently but still correctly – some non-standard responses given by (e.g.) minority children may be standard in their sub-culture.

Page 43: Intelligence

43

More reading (1): People who argue group differences are real:

Neisser, U. et al. (1996), Intelligence: Knowns and Unknowns. Am. Psychologist, 51(2),77-101

Buckhalt, J.A. (2002). Learning and Individual Differences, 13, 101-114.

Gottfredson, L.S. (1997). Intelligence, 24 (1), 13-23.Gottfredson, L.S. (2000) Psychology, Public Policy,

& Law Special Issue, 6(1), 129 – 143.Jensen, A.R. (2000). Psychology, Public Policy,

and Law Special Issue, 6 (1), 121-127.

Page 44: Intelligence

44

More reading (2): People who argue group differences are artifacts:

Chan, D., et al. (1997). J. Applied Psychology, 82 (2), 300-310.

Hale, J.B., et al. (2001). School Psychologist, Fall,113-118.

Helms, J.E. (1997). In D.P. Flanagan, J.L. Genshaft, & P.L. Harrison (Eds.), Contemporary intellectual assessment: theories, tests, and issues (517-53).

Steele, C.M. (1997 & 1998). American Psychologist, 52 (6) 613-629 and 53 (6) 797-811

Page 45: Intelligence

45

Are there “multiple intelligences?”

No. Intelligence is

multi-dimensional, but all intelligence tests, whatever their form, measure the same ability.

This is true whether tests emphasize verbal or non-verbal skills, and whether they require specific cultural knowledge (such as vocabulary).

Page 46: Intelligence

46

Are there “multiple intelligences?”

Howard Gardner’s idea: IQ tests tap only one of many different kinds of intelligence (e.g., bodily-kinesthetic, musical, inter-personal, etc.)

No evidence supports this view. Gardner’s tests appears to measure interest, motivation, other things.

Most cannot be scored objectively.

Page 47: Intelligence

47

Are there “multiple intelligences?”

Sternberg’s Triarchic Theory is just as bad as Gardner’s.

No useful tests of his “practical intelligence” exist.

He is accused of misrepresenting data, using unclear definitions, obscuring his methods, not reporting results fully, using unrepresentative samples. (Gottfredson, 2003)

Page 48: Intelligence

48

Are there “multiple intelligences?”

All intelligence tests measure the same ability

The most important aspect of intelligence is g –general ability

Page 49: Intelligence

49

Spearman’s g

Spearman (1904): When a large group of people are given a variety of ability tests, the correlations among the test results are almost always positive.

Page 50: Intelligence

50

Spearman’s g

Spearman (1904): That correlation is called the positive manifold

It is the basis for the idea that there is a general mental ability (called g).

Page 51: Intelligence

51

Spearman’s g

g is not the same as IQ

to find g, you have to do factor analysis

to find IQ, you use an IQ test

Page 52: Intelligence

IQ

gS

V

Test Score

FactorAnalysis

Measurement error

Page 53: Intelligence

53

Spearman’s g

g is essentially a biological variable

all tests involving cognitive ability measure g to some extent

none measure only g: any cognitive test has some variance due to causes unique to that test.

Page 54: Intelligence

54

Spearman’s g

IQ scores reflect individual differences in underlying constructs (e.g., g and group factors), unique abilities, and measurement error.

In contrast, factors, such as g, are derived from correlations and reflect individual differences in underlying constructs. Factor scores provide best estimates of g.

Page 55: Intelligence

55

Does g matter?

Yes. g is a better predictor of educational and work performance than any other measure we have.

g is the most important determiner of scores on every test of cognitive ability (in people who can be tested).

Page 56: Intelligence

56

Is intelligence more than just g?

Yes. More than 70 different “group factors” have been identified.

Hierarchical theory: g at top (most general)

At Level II, 8 broad cognitive abilities (e.g., fluid intelligence)

At Level I, narrow abilities (memory span, inductive reasoning, etc.)

Page 57: Intelligence

57

Is intelligence important in daily life?

Yes. (See Slide 58.)

IQ is more strongly related to important educational, occupational, economic, and social outcomes than any other single variable.

Relation is strong in education, military training

Moderate in social competence

Modest in law-abidingness

Page 58: Intelligence

58

Is intelligence important in daily life?

IQ increases in importance as life gets more complex – in novel, ambiguous, changing, unpredictable, or multi-dimensional situations.

IQ is important in professions, management

Less important where only routine decision-making, simple problem-solving are required

Page 59: Intelligence

59

Is intelligence important in daily life?

Some personality characteristics, talents, physical capabilities, etc. are also important.

But intelligence transfers across tasks and settings; those other characteristics do so less or not at all.

Page 60: Intelligence

High Uphill Keeping Out Yours toRisk Battle Up Ahead Lose

IQ: <70 71-90 90-110 110-130 > 130% populn 5 20 50 20 5

LF (M) 22 19 15 14 10Job (M) 12 10 7 7 2Divorce 21 22 23 15 9Illegit (F) 32 17 8 4 2Poverty 30 16 6 3 2Prison (M) 7 7 3 1 0Welfare (F) 31 17 8 2 0Dropout 55 35 6 0.4 0

LF = Out of labor force > 1 month in the last year; Job = Unemployed > 1 month/year; Welfare = Chronic welfare recipient

Page 61: Intelligence

61

Individual Intelligence Tests

administration requires advanced training tests cover wide range of age and ability examiner-subject rapport is important immediate scoring of items usually requires about one hour allows opportunity for observation

Page 62: Intelligence

62

2 Important Tests

Binet asked to identify

intellectually limited children so they could be removed from the regular classroom and put in special education

Wechsler Responded to

perceived shortcomings of the Binet test thirty years later

Page 63: Intelligence

63

Binet’s 1905 test

No intelligence tests existed to guide Binet and colleague Simon

Like Spearman, thought of intelligence as a general mental ability

Wanted tasks to measure judgment, attention, and reasoning.

Two major concepts: Age differentiation General mental

ability.

Page 64: Intelligence

64

Binet’s principles of test construction:

Age differentiation: Binet searched for tasks that could be completed by 2/3 to ¾ of the children in a particular age group & were completed by fewer younger children and more older children.

Page 65: Intelligence

65

Binet’s principles of test construction:

General mental ability:

Measured only the total output on the various tasks.

Judged value of task in terms of its correlation with the combined result of all other tasks.

Page 66: Intelligence

66

Binet-Simon (1905):

First formal intelligence test

30 items ordered by difficulty

Drawbacks: Output: labeled

children idiot, imbecile, and moron (these were technical terms at that time)

Norms: produced using only 50 children

Validity: no evidence offered

Page 67: Intelligence

67

Binet-Simon (1908):

Grouped items according to age level rather than simply according to increasing difficulty. Introduced concept of mental age to deal with problem of output

Increased norm group to 203 children.

Still produced only one score heavily dependent on verbal, language, and reading abilities

Page 68: Intelligence

68

1916 Stanford Binet scale:

Lewis Terman of Stanford University translated Binet test into English and introduced it to America.

Terman increased size of standardization sample, but included only white native-Californian children.

Page 69: Intelligence

69

1916 Stanford Binet scale:

Introduced intelligence quotient (IQ) concept to show subjects’ rate of mental development.

IQ = (MA/CA) x 100

Maximum mental age was 19.5. Set maximum chronological age at 16.

S-B tests used on millions of U.S. Army recruits starting in 1917, after mobilization for World War I – a rich source of data for post-war research.

Page 70: Intelligence

70

1937 Stanford-Binet Scale

Extended age range down to 2 and up to 22 years, 10 months.

Some performance items added – but 75% of items still verbal

Scoring standards and instructions were improved

Standardization sample improved to include 3184 subjects from 11 states.

Developed alternate forms (Forms L & M) to facilitate research

Page 71: Intelligence

71

Problems with 1937 Stanford-Binet

Reliability higher for older subjects than for younger ones and higher for those in the lower IQ ranges

Scores were most unstable for young children with high IQ

Each age group also had different standard deviations which made interpretation difficult

Page 72: Intelligence

72

1960 Stanford-Binet:

Used Binet’s principles to redo scale

Looked for tasks on which success becomes more likely as age increases

Looked for tasks for which scores correlated with test scores.

Page 73: Intelligence

73

1960 Stanford-Binet:

Introduced the deviation IQ concept. Set mean at 100 with SD of 16.

Could now compare scores of one age level with another.

Deviation IQ: score worked out in terms of number of SDs above or below age mean

Page 74: Intelligence

74

1960 Stanford Binet

Each boundary (---) marks 1 standard deviation

Page 75: Intelligence

75

1986 Stanford-Binet scale

Multi-dimensional, but hierarchical – with g at the top of the structure

4 main factors:

Verbal reasoning Abstract/visual

reasoning Quantitative

reasoning Short-term memory

Page 76: Intelligence

76

1986 Stanford-Binet scale

The individual tests (such as Pattern Analysis or Vocabulary) each give information about one of the factors

The factors in turn give information about g.

g reflects common variability across all the tasks

Page 77: Intelligence

77

2003 Stanford-Binet scale

Now 5 main factors, each tested in verbal & nonverbal domains

Fluid Reasoning Knowledge Quantitative

Reasoning Visual-Spatial

Processing Working Memory

Page 78: Intelligence

78

2003 Stanford-Binet scale

Uses routing tests to estimate subject’s level of ability

Verbal and non-verbal routing tests

Back to original age-scale approach (items with differing content grouped together on basis of difficulty).

Page 79: Intelligence

79

2003 Stanford-Binet scale

The S-B5 was normed on a stratified random sample of 4,800 individuals (2 – 85 years of age) that matches the 2000 U.S. Census.

Bias reviews on all items for sex, ethnic, cultural/religious, regional, and socioeconomic status issues.

Page 80: Intelligence

80

Psychometric properties of 2003 S-B

Internal consistency reliability is .98 for composite and .93-.97 for area scores.

Some individual test scores are lower: .73 for memory for objects is the lowest.

Page 81: Intelligence

81

Psychometric properties of 2003 S-B

Test-retest reliabilities for composite score were .91 and .90 for 5 and 8-year-olds.

Factor analysis supports the structure of the test.

Correlations with other IQ tests are generally in the .70s and .80s

Page 82: Intelligence

82

Psychometric properties of 2003 S-B

Convergent validity assessed with:

Stanford-Binet Intelligence Scale, 4th Edition, the Stanford-Binet Form L-M, the Woodcock-Johnson III, the WAIS-III, the WISC-III, and the WPPSI-R