195
Florida State University Libraries Electronic Theses, Treatises and Dissertations The Graduate School 2009 Effects of Process-Oriented and Product- Oriented Worked Examples and Prior Knowledge on Learner Problem Solving and Attitude: A Study in the Domain of Microeconomics Christopher Darren Brooks Follow this and additional works at the FSU Digital Library. For more information, please contact [email protected]

Florida State University Libraries254119/datastream/PDF/download/...one of three instructional strategies (process-oriented, product-oriented, or conventional problem solving) developed

  • Upload
    others

  • View
    3

  • Download
    0

Embed Size (px)

Citation preview

Page 1: Florida State University Libraries254119/datastream/PDF/download/...one of three instructional strategies (process-oriented, product-oriented, or conventional problem solving) developed

Florida State University Libraries

Electronic Theses, Treatises and Dissertations The Graduate School

2009

Effects of Process-Oriented and Product-Oriented Worked Examples and PriorKnowledge on Learner Problem Solvingand Attitude: A Study in the Domain ofMicroeconomicsChristopher Darren Brooks

Follow this and additional works at the FSU Digital Library. For more information, please contact [email protected]

Page 2: Florida State University Libraries254119/datastream/PDF/download/...one of three instructional strategies (process-oriented, product-oriented, or conventional problem solving) developed

THE FLORIDA STATE UNIVERSITY

COLLEGE OF EDUCATION

EFFECTS OF PROCESS-ORIENTED AND PRODUCT-ORIENTED

WORKED EXAMPLES AND PRIOR KNOWLEDGE ON LEARNER

PROBLEM SOLVING AND ATTITUDE:

A STUDY IN THE DOMAIN OF MICROECONOMICS

By

CHRISTOPHER DARREN BROOKS

A Dissertation submitted to the Department of Educational Psychology and Learning Systems

in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of

Doctor of Philosophy

Semester Approved: Fall Semester, 2009

Page 3: Florida State University Libraries254119/datastream/PDF/download/...one of three instructional strategies (process-oriented, product-oriented, or conventional problem solving) developed

ii

The members of the committee approve the dissertation of Christopher Darren

Brooks defended on September 11, 2009.

__________________________ A. Aubteen Darabi Professor Directing Dissertation

__________________________ Gerald R. Ferris University Representative __________________________ Linda B. Schrader Committee Member __________________________ Robert A. Reiser Committee Member

Approved: __________________________________________ Betsy Becker, Chair, Department of Educational Psychology and Learning Systems

The Graduate School has verified and approved the above-named committee

members.

Page 4: Florida State University Libraries254119/datastream/PDF/download/...one of three instructional strategies (process-oriented, product-oriented, or conventional problem solving) developed

iii

To my wife, Marianne, and my three children Thomas, Elizabeth, and Jack

who show me each day that the key to life is having an eagerness to love and

learn. Your sacrifice, support and encouragement made fulfilling my dream a

reality. To my parents, Ann R. Brooks and John F. Brooks, for inspiring me to

reach beyond what I thought possible.

Page 5: Florida State University Libraries254119/datastream/PDF/download/...one of three instructional strategies (process-oriented, product-oriented, or conventional problem solving) developed

iv

ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS

I would like to acknowledge several people who have provided extraordinary

guidance and encouragement throughout the dissertation process. A special recognition

goes to Dr. Aubteen Darabi, my committee chair, for his mentorship and friendship

throughout my graduate experience at Florida State University. I would also like to thank

my other committee members Dr. Robert Reiser, Dr. Linda Schrader, and Dr. Gerald

Ferris for their invaluable contributions to this project and my educational experience. To

my entire committee I would like to acknowledge how fortunate I have been to have each

of you serve on my committee. The professionalism and passion you demonstrate for

your work are an inspiration to me to seek excellence in all that I pursue.

In addition, I would like to recognize certain individuals who have provided

support and assistance throughout the dissertation project. In particular, Dr. Joseph

Calhoun, for his friendship, patience and support over the past two-years. His

commitment to this project and me personally has served as a reminder that true success

comes through service to others. I would also like to thank Mary Kate Mckee, Program

Coordinator for the Instructional Systems program, whose indelible enthusiasm touches

everyone she comes in contact with. Dr. Alan Jeong for his continuous support and

friendship. My former supervisors, Lou Bonaiuto, Steve Churchill, Dr. Jeff Ling, Melissa

Jaacks, and Ramin Kouzehkanani who supported me throughout my graduate education

with the flexibility to balance my academic, work, and family priorities. Many of my

colleagues such as Vicki Barnhardt, Deborah Gibson, Megan Caplice, Janie Dixon,

Caryn Salamy and Lori Short provided invaluable support by providing feedback on draft

documents. York Shuler provided exceptional technical expertise in developing a master

data file from multiple data sources that enhanced the data analysis process. I would also

like to thank Dr. David Nelson for his thoughtful insight and suggestions throughout this

project. Lastly, I would like to thank Dr. James C. Smith who inspired me as an

undergraduate research assistant to pursue my doctorate.

Finally, thank you to all my colleagues and friends who have supported me

throughout my educational journey. This degree is a testament to all of you who have

shown me that persistence and an unwavering spirit can accomplish anything.

Page 6: Florida State University Libraries254119/datastream/PDF/download/...one of three instructional strategies (process-oriented, product-oriented, or conventional problem solving) developed

v

TABLE OF CONTENTS

List of Tables ................................................................................................ vii List of Figures ................................................................................................ ix Abstract ................................................................................................ x CHAPTER 1 INTRODUCTION ........................................................................ 1 Statement of the Problem ........................................................................... 1 Context of the Problem .............................................................................. 4 Significance of the Study ........................................................................... 8 Purpose of the Study ................................................................................... 10 Research Questions .................................................................................... 10 Hypotheses ................................................................................................ 11 Summary ................................................................................................ 16 CHAPTER 2 REVIEW OF THE LITERATURE ............................................. 20 Cognitive Load Theory .............................................................................. 20 Cognitive Load Theory and Conventional Problem Solving Strategies .... 22 Learning from Worked Examples ............................................................... 24 Worked Example Strategies: Product-Oriented and Process-Oriented ...... 29 Worked Example Efficacy and Learner Prior Knowledge ......................... 31 Learner Attitude ......................................................................................... 32 Summary ................................................................................................ 34 CHAPTER 3 METHOD ..................................................................................... 36 Participants ................................................................................................ 36 Instructional Environment ......................................................................... 37 Instructional Materials ............................................................................... 39 Variables ................................................................................................ 42 Instruments ................................................................................................ 47 Procedure ................................................................................................ 49 Instructional Strategy ................................................................................. 50 Research Design and Data Analysis .......................................................... 54 CHAPTER 4 RESULTS ..................................................................................... 55 Preliminary Analysis ................................................................................... 55 Primary Data Analysis ................................................................................ 60 Description of Groups ................................................................................. 60 Tests of Hypothesis ..................................................................................... 64 Secondary Analysis ..................................................................................... 76

Page 7: Florida State University Libraries254119/datastream/PDF/download/...one of three instructional strategies (process-oriented, product-oriented, or conventional problem solving) developed

vi

CHAPTER 5 DISCUSSION ................................................................................ 84 Summary of Findings in Context ................................................................ 84 Interpretations of Results ............................................................................ 85 Incidental Findings...................................................................................... 92 Limitations of the Study.............................................................................. 94 Implications for Instructional Design Practice ........................................... 98 Future Research Directions ......................................................................... 101 Conclusion ................................................................................................ 103 APPENDIX A. HUMAN SUBJECTS COMMITTEE APPROVAL MEMORANDUM AND PARTICIPANT CONSENT FORM 104 APPENDIX B. REQUEST FOR STUDENT DEMOGRAPHIC DATA ....... 106

APPENDIX C. CLASSROOM OBSERVATION FORM ............................. 107

APPENDIX D. CONVENTIONAL PROBLEM SOLVING INSTRUCTIONAL ACTIVITY MATERIALS ..................................................... 109 APPENDIX E. PROCESS-ORIENTED INSTRUCTIONAL ACTIVITY MATERIALS ......................................................................... 128 APPENDIX F. PRODUCT-ORIENTED INSTRUCTIONAL ACTIVITY MATERIALS ......................................................................... 144 APPENDIX G. MENTAL EFFORT SURVEY .............................................. 159

APPENDIX H. SUBJECT-SPECIFIC PRIOR KNOWLEDGE ASSESSMENT ITEMS.................................................................................... 160

APPENDIX I. INSTRUCTIONAL MATERIALS MOTIVATION SURVEY 163

APPENDIX J. PERFORMANCE ASSESSMENT ITEMS FOR ACHIEVEMENT AND TRANSFER TESTS ....................... 165 APPENDIX K. ACTIVITY LEADER GUIDE .............................................. 168

APPENDIX L. ACTIVITY INSTRUCTIONS ............................................... 170

APPENDIX M. EVALUATION OF PREVIOUS ECONOMIC EXPERIENCE 172

REFERENCES ................................................................................................ 173

BIOGRAPHICAL SKETCH .............................................................................. 180

Page 8: Florida State University Libraries254119/datastream/PDF/download/...one of three instructional strategies (process-oriented, product-oriented, or conventional problem solving) developed

vii

LIST OF TABLES Table 3.1: Descriptive Statistics on Instructional Strategy Conditions ............... 37 Table 3.2: Differences Between Instructional Strategies ..................................... 42 Table 3.3: Research Design of Instructional Strategy and Prior Knowledge ...... 54 Table 4.1: Tests of Normality of Instructional Strategy Conditions .................... 56 Table 4.2: Tests of Normality of Instructional Strategy and Prior Knowledge .......................................................................................... 57 Table 4.3: Test of Homogeneity of Variance....................................................... 58 Table 4.4: Performance Assessment Statistics for Individual Test Items ............ 59 Table 4.5: Descriptive Statistics of Instructional Strategy on Dependent Measures ............................................................................................. 61 Table 4.6: Descriptive Statistics for Prior Knowledge Assessment Scores ......... 61 Table 4.7: Prior Knowledge Category Frequencies by Instructional Strategy .... 62 Table 4.8: Descriptive Statistics of Prior Knowledge Levels on Dependent Measures ............................................................................................. 62 Table 4.9: Frequency of Participants who Completed High School Economics . 63 Table 4.10: Frequency of Participants who Completed College Introductory Economics ......................................................................................... 64 Table 4.11: Frequency of Participants who Completed College-Level Economics with Course Instructor .................................................... 64 Table 4.12: One-Way Analysis of Variance for Achievement Test Scores ........ 65 Table 4.13: Two-Way Analysis of Variance for Instructional Strategy and Prior Knowledge Level on Achievement Test Scores .............................. 66 Table 4.14: Effect of Prior Knowledge Level on Transfer Test Scores within Instructional Strategy Groups .......................................................... 67 Table 4.15: Two-Way Analysis of Variance on Transfer Test Scores ................ 68

Page 9: Florida State University Libraries254119/datastream/PDF/download/...one of three instructional strategies (process-oriented, product-oriented, or conventional problem solving) developed

viii

Table 4.16: Effect of Prior Knowledge Level on Transfer Test Scores within Instructional Strategy Groups .......................................................... 69 Table 4.17: Descriptive Statistics for Instructional Strategy Mental Effort Ratings 70 Table 4.18: One-Way Analysis of Variance for Mental Effort Ratings .............. 70 Table 4.19: Descriptive Statistics for Learner Attitude by Instructional Strategy 72 Table 4.20: Univariate Analysis of Variance for Instructional Strategy on Attitude 72 Table 4.21: Univariate Analysis of Variance for Instructional Strategy and Prior Knowledge Level on Learner Attitude .................................... 74 Table 4.22: Descriptive Statistics for Confidence by Instructional Strategy and Prior Knowledge Level ..................................................................... 74 Table 4.23: Descriptive Statistics for Attention by Instructional Strategy and Prior Knowledge Level ..................................................................... 75 Table 4.24: Descriptive Statistics for Learner Attitude by Instructional Strategy and Prior Knowledge Level .............................................................. 75 Table 4.25: Descriptive Statistics for Learning Efficiency by Instructional Strategy ............................................................................................. 79 Table 4.26: Descriptive Statistics for Achievement and Mental Effort Z-scores 81 by Instructional Strategy Table 4.27: Descriptive Statistics for Learning Efficiency Z-scores by Instructional Strategy and Prior Knowledge Level ........................... 81 Table 4.28: Summary of Result Findings ............................................................ 82

Page 10: Florida State University Libraries254119/datastream/PDF/download/...one of three instructional strategies (process-oriented, product-oriented, or conventional problem solving) developed

ix

LIST OF FIGURES Figure 1.1: Graphical Display of Worked Example versus Conventional Problem Solving in Promoting Learning ......................................... 3 Figure 3.1: Display of a Process-Oriented Worked Example Incorporating both Procedural and Strategic Information ....................................... 44 Figure 3.2: Display of a Product-Oriented Worked Example with Procedural Steps ............................................................................................... 45 Figure 3.3: Illustration of Participant Categorization into Instructional Strategy and Prior Knowledge Groups............................................................ 50 Figure 3.4: Overview of the Procedures .............................................................. 53 Figure 4.1: Efficiency Graph of Mean Achievement Z-Scores for Learning by Instructional Strategy ........................................................................ 80

Page 11: Florida State University Libraries254119/datastream/PDF/download/...one of three instructional strategies (process-oriented, product-oriented, or conventional problem solving) developed

x

ABSTRACT

The purpose of this study was to investigate the effectiveness of process-oriented

and product-oriented worked example strategies and the mediating effect of prior

knowledge (high versus low) on problem solving and learner attitude in the domain of

microeconomics. In addition, the effect of these variables on learning efficiency as well

as the influence of learner attitude on mental effort was explored as part of a secondary

analysis.

Four-hundred fifteen undergraduate students enrolled in an introductory

microeconomics course participated in the study. Participants were randomly assigned to

one of three instructional strategies (process-oriented, product-oriented, or conventional

problem solving) developed for this study. In addition, participants’ were classified

during the analysis phase as either low prior knowledge or high prior knowledge groups

based on scores from a prior knowledge assessment. In the process-oriented strategy

condition, participants were exposed to a fully worked out example that presented both

procedural “how” and strategic (principle-based) “why” information underlying the

solution. Participants in the product-oriented strategy condition were presented a worked

out example with procedural information showing the steps necessary to solve a problem.

In both worked example strategy conditions, participants completed a series of four

process-oriented worked examples and three practice problems. Participants assigned to

the conventional problem solving condition were asked to complete seven practice-

problems.

Regardless of the instructional condition, all participants received immediate

feedback (i.e., correct answer) after the completion of a practice problem. The study

included two instructional components: (1) a common lecture, and (2) completion of a

print-based instructional activity. The twenty-minute lecture provided a conceptual

overview of the impact of taxes on market activity. Two days later participants were

assigned to one of the three instructional conditions and given fifty-minutes to complete

the instructional activity. A performance assessment was administered four-days later to

measure learning and transfer.

Results suggested that instructional strategy had a minimal affect on participant

learning and transfer performance, mental effort, and attitude toward the instruction.

Page 12: Florida State University Libraries254119/datastream/PDF/download/...one of three instructional strategies (process-oriented, product-oriented, or conventional problem solving) developed

xi

However, mean scores on the achievement test assessing student learning were found to

be slightly higher for participants in either one of the worked example instructional

strategies when compared to conventional problem solving. For mental effort, the

findings indicated that participants, regardless of instructional strategy, invested a low

amount of mental effort during the instructional tasks. Similarly, participants reported a

relatively high (i.e., positive) attitude toward the instruction. In a post hoc analysis,

participants in both the product-oriented worked example and the conventional problem

solving groups that self-reported a higher level of confidence toward the instruction also

invested a higher degree of mental effort during the instructional task. The study did

support the mediating effect of prior knowledge on each dependent measure.

The results of the present study have implications for the design of example-based

instruction and for further research exploration of instructional task sequencing. Based on

the findings of the present study, it is recommended that instructional designers consider

participants’ level of knowledge when designing learning strategies to teach complex

problem solving skills. Furthermore, developing instructional methods or systems that

adapt to a learner’s cognitive and motivational differences has implications for the

measurement of mental efficiency and the design of instructional and feedback protocols.

Consequently, the implications for the design and utilization of process-oriented worked

examples as a component of a sequenced instructional strategy are discussed further.

Page 13: Florida State University Libraries254119/datastream/PDF/download/...one of three instructional strategies (process-oriented, product-oriented, or conventional problem solving) developed

1

CHAPTER 1

INTRODUCTION

This chapter presents the general problem of designing and developing instruction

utilizing supportive worked example strategies to improve learning and transfer of

problem solving skills in the domain of microeconomics. The use of process-oriented and

product-oriented worked example instructional strategies is the specific design feature of

interest. In addition, the mediating effects of participant levels of prior knowledge on

learning, transfer, mental effort, and attitude will also be examined. In Chapter 1, the

problem is stated, the context of the problem is described, and the significance of the

study is presented. To conclude, the specific purpose of the study is presented along with

statements of the research questions and the hypotheses guiding the study.

Statement of the Problem

The purpose of instruction is to prepare learners to acquire and transfer

knowledge and skills to different environments (Nelson, 2006). However, learners are

frequently unable to transfer learned knowledge and skills to novel situations or novel

problems (van Gog, Paas, & van Merrienboer, 2004). The rationale behind transfer

difficulty is that novice learners tend to form problem solutions in a series of linear steps

or tasks (Catrambone, 1996) as opposed to developing an understanding of the principles

underlying the problem (van Gog, Pass, & van Merrienboer, 2004). To overcome transfer

difficulties, instruction should aim to enable the learner to draw inferences about the

structure of the problem and the steps necessary to solve it based on an understanding of

the rationale underlying the problem (Gerjets, Scheiter, & Schuh, 2008).

A compounding problem in educational and training settings is the limited

instructional time afforded to learners. Consequently, Cognitive Load research has sought

to examine instructional strategies that are more effective than more traditional or

conventional problem solving strategies in terms of improving the problem-solving skill

acquisition and transfer performance by optimizing the allocation of cognitive resources.

This study uses two types of worked example instructional strategies to improve the

learning and transfer performance of problem solving skills in the domain of

Page 14: Florida State University Libraries254119/datastream/PDF/download/...one of three instructional strategies (process-oriented, product-oriented, or conventional problem solving) developed

2

microeconomics. Specifically, this study will investigate the effects of two types of

worked example strategies (i.e., process-oriented or product-oriented) and prior

knowledge on various learning outcomes.

Since the 1960s, conventional problem solving strategies, based on discovery

learning principles, have been widely applied as the prevailing method for problem

solving in educational settings (Tuovinen, 1997). Many educators use conventional

problem-solving strategies, based on the belief that providing learners the opportunity to

solve a large number of whole-task problems will lead to effective domain-specific

knowledge acquisition and transfer (Darabi, Sikorski, Nelson, & Palanki, 2006; van

Merrienboer, 1997).

However, researchers examining the influence of the cognitive load of instruction

on learner performance have criticized the use of a conventional problem-solving strategy

(Darabi et al., 2006) as an ineffective instructional strategy. According to Cognitive Load

Theory (Sweller, 1988), a learner’s working memory has a limited capacity (Clarke,

Ayres, & Sweller, 2005) that can easily become overloaded during instructional

activities, especially during the initial stages of learning. Conventional problem-solving

strategies provide little instructional support (Tuovinen & Sweller, 1999), prompting

novice learners to employ weak goal-directed problem solving strategies (i.e., means-end

analysis) that can hinder the learning process by overwhelming working memory.

Contrary to conventional problem solving, the use of worked examples has been shown

to improve transfer performance for novice learners by optimizing cognitive capacity

(Cooper and Sweller, 1987; Paas, 1992; Kalyuga, Chandler, Tuovinen, and Sweller,

2001).

An instructional strategy that consists mainly of worked examples or worked

examples with practice problems (known as worked example-problem pairs) provides

learners with a structured approach to problem solving. Structurally, worked examples

present learners with both the problem state and the goal state as conventional problems

do. However, contrary to conventional problems, worked examples also provide the

solution steps necessary to attain the goal state (van Gog et al., 2006). This eliminates

learners’ unnecessary problem search activity by focusing their attention on tasks or

activities relevant to solving the problem. By focusing the learners’ attention on the

Page 15: Florida State University Libraries254119/datastream/PDF/download/...one of three instructional strategies (process-oriented, product-oriented, or conventional problem solving) developed

3

structure of the problem and the steps necessary to solve it, worked examples actively

promote schema construction (van Gog et al., 2004; Sweller, 1988; Sweller, van

Merrienboer, & Paas, 1998). Figure 1.1 illustrates how worked examples, compared to

conventional problem solving, manage cognitive load by optimizing working memory

capacity and focusing on schema development as opposed to solving the problem.

Figure 1.1. Graphical display of worked examples versus conventional problem solving in promoting learning

Researchers have described two different worked example strategies, product-

oriented and process-oriented that can enhance learning and bring about transfer

performance (Paas & van Merrienboer, 1994; van Gog, Paas, & van Merrienboer, 2004).

A product-oriented strategy provides the learner with the solution steps to solve the

problem but not the rationale behind taking each step. On the other hand, a process-

oriented strategy provides an explanation of why specific steps have been chosen or why

the steps are appropriate for this particular problem. Recently, van Gog et al. (2004)

proposed that adding process information to a worked example would increase a learner’s

understanding of the principled knowledge underlying a domain by providing both the

purpose of the steps and the strategic knowledge used to select the steps in a procedure.

Page 16: Florida State University Libraries254119/datastream/PDF/download/...one of three instructional strategies (process-oriented, product-oriented, or conventional problem solving) developed

4

More importantly, van Gog and her colleagues proposed that by increasing learner

understanding, the transfer of the learned skill to novel problems would be improved.

In an effort to test these assumptions, van Gog et al. (2006) examined the use of

process-oriented worked examples with electrical engineering students on

troubleshooting tasks. The findings supported the “worked example effect” that states the

studying of worked examples leads to better transfer performance than solving

conventional problems. However, adding process information to worked examples did

not result in higher transfer performance over traditional product-oriented examples. One

possible explanation offered was that a learner’s prior knowledge strongly influences the

effectiveness of worked examples resulting in expertise-reversal effect (Kalyuga, Ayres,

Chandler, & Sweller, 2003; van Gog, Paas, & van Merrienboer, 2008). The findings

suggest that adding process information to a worked example may be beneficial to a

novice learner by imposing germane load. However, the same level of process

information would be experienced as redundant information for a learner with high levels

of prior knowledge imposing extraneous load that may impairment of learning.

Consequently, the presentation of additional information, how this should be done, and

the impact it has on learners are important issues in the instructional design field

(Sweller, 2006). The present study will seek to understand the influence of additional

process information has on learning and transfer of economic problem solving skills and

learner attitude by comparing process-oriented and product-oriented worked example

strategies and their interaction with the level of learner prior knowledge.

Context of the Problem

This study will examine a specific area of microeconomics, the impact of taxes,

that requires the use of both declarative and procedural knowledge in the application of

microeconomic principles to solve market analysis problems. The extension of worked

example research into the domain of microeconomics and the examination of a worked

example’s effect on learner motivation are unique to this line of research. As such, the

findings of this study will contribute to the ongoing body of worked example literature

about the effect of differences in worked example strategy on initial learning and transfer

of problem solving skills.

Page 17: Florida State University Libraries254119/datastream/PDF/download/...one of three instructional strategies (process-oriented, product-oriented, or conventional problem solving) developed

5

Economists have stated that the primary goal of undergraduate economic

principles courses is to enable students to think like economists (Becker, 2000). Perhaps a

more realistic goal is that students should be able to acquire a greater understanding of

the economic concepts and principles that govern market activity. Despite the primary

goal of economic education, disagreements among instructors of economic principles

over the most effective instructional approaches have recently dominated discussions in

economic education literature (Becker, 2000; Jensen & Owen, 2003). For example,

economic education researchers have begun to examine the benefit of various

instructional approaches such as experimental games (Gremmen & Potters, 1997), case

methods (Marks & Rukstad, 1996), and collaborative learning (Johnston, James, Lye, &

McDonald, 2000) on improving economic problem solving skills. While disagreement

will likely continue regarding the most effective instructional approach (Becker, 2000),

the encouraging trend is that administrators and faculty are placing a greater emphasis on

how economics is taught.

The English economist, Alfred Marshall, described the field of economics as, “a

study of mankind in the ordinary business of life” (Britannica Online, 2008). As a social

science, economics is a study of objective activities, such as how monetary and fiscal

policies shape a country’s economic viability. However, economics is also a study of

societal behavior and how changes in economic policy or activity affect interactions

among consumers, markets, and nations. In other words, economics is not just the study

of market forces but also an examination of societies through the prism of economic

behavior. Consequently, economics is a complex discipline comprised of conceptual and

technical content.

For learners to handle the complex nature of economics, instructional strategies

must be oriented toward guiding the learner to understand and apply the various facts,

rules, and principles on complex real world economic and social problems. For example,

microeconomic students learning about the impact of taxation on market exchange learn

how to adjust their understanding of the principle of supply and demand to incorporate

the tax incidence or who bears the burden of a tax, the consumer or the producer/seller of

the good or service. In order to solve a problem involving the impact of a tax, a learner

must coordinate multiple informational elements such as the price and quantity

Page 18: Florida State University Libraries254119/datastream/PDF/download/...one of three instructional strategies (process-oriented, product-oriented, or conventional problem solving) developed

6

equilibrium, the elasticity of the good or service in question, and how the introduction of

a tax changes the relationship between the consumer and the producer/seller. To solve a

complex problem of this nature, learners may require the assistance of external

instructional support in order to process the large amount of interacting information

elements. Otherwise, the complexity of the problem can easily overwhelm a learner’s

working memory capacity, especially in novices.

The lecture-practice problem-exam strategy represents a conventional approach to

teaching problem solving skills commonly used in many higher education domains,

including economics. Conventional problem solving approaches to instruction provide

learners with minimal support in organizing the informational elements in a manner that

facilitates learning and transfer. For example, in most introductory courses of

microeconomics, the prevailing instructional strategy is the lecture/exam supplemented

by large amounts of classroom or homework practice problems. Recently, Becker and

Watts (2001) conducted a survey of introductory economics instructors at 50 academic

institutions in the United States. The study found that a majority of the instructors spent

83% of class time in conventional instructional activities (i.e., lecture-practice problem-

exam strategy).

From the perspective of the instructor, a conventional approach may be utilized

on the basis of its perceived instructional efficiency. In other words, instructors are often

constrained by limited instructional time and a large number of students to teach in

auditorium style classrooms. For example, the proposed study was conducted in an

auditorium style classroom with more than 400 students. The instructor was scheduled

for three 50-minute class sessions per week over a thirteen-week semester to cover the

requisite content. Considering the imposed environmental and time constraints, the

instructor chose to use a lecture-based strategy supplemented by online homework

assignments (i.e., conventional practice problems) as the primary instructional strategy to

teach problem solving skills in microeconomics.

Problem solving by nature is a complex process that requires the processing of

numerous information elements to reach a solution. Cognitive Load Theory suggests that

the use of a weak problem-solving strategy such as conventional problem solving for

complex topics requires the learner to exert a considerable amount of mental effort to

Page 19: Florida State University Libraries254119/datastream/PDF/download/...one of three instructional strategies (process-oriented, product-oriented, or conventional problem solving) developed

7

solve practice problems, especially for novices (Sweller, 1988; Paas & van Merrienboer,

1994; Paas, Renkl, & Sweller, 2004; van Gog, Paas, & van Merrienboer, 2004). In other

words, requiring learners to work numerous practice problems imposes a significant

amount cognitive load on working memory that can hinder the development of schemas

and ultimately slow learning.

Research on the use of worked examples offers instructors an alternative approach

to instruction that will promote learning through schema development. Contrary to

conventional problem solving that presents learners with a problem to solve and the

solution, worked examples provide learners with the problem and the solution but go

further by providing the necessary steps to solve the problem. By studying a worked

example, the learner is able to free up more of their cognitive capacity to constructing

domain specific problem solving schema versus employing an inefficient problem-search

strategy (i.e., means-end analysis) to solve the problem.

Numerous studies in the domains of mathematics, science, and engineering have

demonstrated the effectiveness of worked examples over conventional problem solving at

improving transfer performance and reducing a learner’s cognitive load (Paas & van

Merrienboer, 1994; Clark, Nguyen, & Sweller, 2006). For example, Sweller and Cooper

(1985) found that replacing algebra practice problems with worked examples resulted in

students learning equal or more in significantly less time than students just using

practice-problems. In a field trial using Chinese middle school students, Zhu and Simon

(1987) demonstrated that the use of worked examples instruction in place of conventional

problem solving reduced a three-year algebra/geometry curriculum to two years with

equal to higher learning gains. Expanding on prior worked example research, Paas (1992)

developed a subjective measure of mental effort that demonstrated that the use of worked

example practice-pairs to learn statistical concepts resulted in higher scores on both near

and far transfer tests than the use of conventional problem solving with less investment of

mental effort.

Cognitive Load researchers have only recently to examine the effectiveness of

worked example instruction to business and social science domains such as accounting

(Halabi, Tuovinen, & Farley, 2005). This study will examine the effectiveness of worked

example instructional strategies in a new domain – microeconomics. Solving complex

Page 20: Florida State University Libraries254119/datastream/PDF/download/...one of three instructional strategies (process-oriented, product-oriented, or conventional problem solving) developed

8

economic problems requires the application of both economic principles and rules to real

world problems as opposed to a strictly rule-based activity common among more

traditional lines of worked example research such as mathematics. Because of this

complexity, numerous temporal and situational factors can influence the use of certain

economic principles and rules. As a result, only well-defined and well-structured

problems were presented to the learners during both the instructional and performance

segments of the study.

Significance of the Study

This study extends and enhances the extant worked example literature by

examining the effect of process-oriented and product-oriented worked example strategies

on learning and transfer performance and learner attitude in the new domain area of

microeconomics. As such, the study investigated whether worked example instructional

strategies represent a potential alternative to conventional problem solving methods that

have been part of the traditional approach to economics education (Becker & Watts,

2001) in many higher education settings. Previous Cognitive Load research has suggested

that worked example research is of particular relevance for instructional programs aimed

at promoting the learning and transfer of new skills in well-defined and well-structured

domains (Atkinson, Derry, Renkl, & Wortham, 2000). Because economists use complex

mathematical modeling based on well-established laws and principles to explain market

behavior, microeconomics was seen as a domain potentially well-suited to benefit from

the attributes of worked example instruction, especially in introductory courses.

The domain of microeconomics is a blend of laws that govern the behaviors of

economies and complex algorithms used to predict economic trends. Consequently, the

field by nature is complex requiring learners simultaneously process multiple interacting

or converging elements that can easily overwhelm novices during problem solving

activities. Hence, from an instructional design perspective, the capability of worked

examples to reduce unproductive search activities becomes essential as the complexity or

intrinsic load of economic problems increases. Moreover, research has shown that

utilizing worked examples is an effective technique to optimize cognitive capacity during

the performance of a problem-solving task (Atkinson et al., 2000; van Merrienboer, 1997;

Paas & van Gog, 2006). For example, an instructional strategy consisting of

Page 21: Florida State University Libraries254119/datastream/PDF/download/...one of three instructional strategies (process-oriented, product-oriented, or conventional problem solving) developed

9

conventional problem solving, may require learners to employ weak problem-solving

strategies (i.e., means-end analysis) that results in high levels of ineffective cognitive

load and hampers learning. Conversely, a worked example strategy promotes learning by

preventing random search activities thereby freeing cognitive capacity to attend to

schema construction (Sweller, 2006; Sweller, 1998).

Early worked example research focused on reducing the extraneous or ineffective

cognitive load imposed by poor instructional design as a way to manage complexity

(Sweller, 1998) by utilizing worked examples (i.e., product-oriented worked examples)

(van Gog et al., 2004). More recently, Cognitive Load research has shifted away from

strategies aimed at reducing extraneous load toward the development of instructional

strategies that promote higher levels of germane or effective cognitive load. Recently,

van Gog et al., (2004; 2006; 2008) have argued that in order to improve learning and

transfer a learner must understand why they are performing certain steps during the

generation of a problem solution. Process-oriented worked examples represent a specific

instructional strategy aimed at increasing germane load.

Process-oriented examples (also called modeling examples) are a form of worked

example instruction that enhances learner understanding by presenting the underlying

mental processes (i.e., the systematic approaches to problem solving and heuristics used

to reason through problems) an expert goes through to solve a problem (van Merrienboer,

1997). Thus, van Gog et al. (2004; 2006) argued that process-oriented worked examples

increase germane load by facilitating a learner’s understanding of the solution procedure.

Moreover, van Gog and her colleagues have proposed that because of the depth of

understanding gained from process-information, learners would also demonstrate

increased transfer performance. Both of these propositions were examined in this study.

As the domain of interest in this study, introductory microeconomics requires

participants to demonstrate their understanding of the principles of supply and demand,

and elasticity, as well as the ability to determine who bears the monetary burden imposed

on market participants. Hence, the present study compares the instructional effectiveness

of two types of worked example instructional strategies (i.e., process-oriented and

product-oriented). In addition, the study also examined the interaction of learner prior

knowledge (i.e., low versus high) and worked example instructional strategies on learning

Page 22: Florida State University Libraries254119/datastream/PDF/download/...one of three instructional strategies (process-oriented, product-oriented, or conventional problem solving) developed

10

outcomes. Lastly, this study will also investigate how various worked example

instructional strategies affected learner attitude toward the instruction. Ultimately, the

goal of the study is to contribute to the identification of an optimally effective worked

example strategy that, along with other effective strategies, could strengthen an

instructional design approach to improve the development of economic skills and

enhance learner attitude.

Purpose of the Study

The purpose of this study was to examine the effect of two types of worked

example instructional strategies (i.e., process-oriented and product-oriented) on learning

and transfer of problem solving skills in the complex domain of microeconomics.

Additionally, the mediating effect of participant prior knowledge levels on learning

outcomes defined in this study were also examined. Extending the research of van Gog et

al. (2004; 2006; 2008), this study will apply the current understanding of the “worked

example effect” (Sweller & Cooper, 1985; Sweller, 1988) to a new domain in Cognitive

Load research, microeconomics.

Research Questions

Cognitive Load researchers have suggested the need to “compare the effects of

process-oriented [and product-oriented] worked examples for novice and more advanced

learners” (Van Gog et al., 2006, p. 162). Consequently, this research will expand

previous worked example research by examining the effect of process-oriented and

product-oriented worked example strategies and prior knowledge on problem solving

skill acquisition and transfer performance and learner attitude in the previously

unexplored domain of microeconomics. In addition to comparing two types of worked

example strategies in a new domain, this study will also investigate the effect of the two

worked example strategies on learner attitude. To date, only the cognitive benefits of

worked example instruction have been explored. The literature is devoid of research

examining the influence of worked example instruction on learners’ attitude. This is a

significant weakness in the literature and represents a distinctive feature of this study.

Page 23: Florida State University Libraries254119/datastream/PDF/download/...one of three instructional strategies (process-oriented, product-oriented, or conventional problem solving) developed

11

The general research questions guiding the study are:

1. What effect do different worked example strategies (i.e., process-oriented or

product-oriented worked examples) have on (a) initial learning achievement and

(b) transfer performance?

2. How does the interaction of a particular worked example strategy (i.e., process-

oriented or product-oriented worked examples) and prior knowledge level affect

learning and transfer?

3. How does the interaction of a particular worked example strategy (i.e., process-

oriented or product-oriented worked examples) and prior knowledge level affect

mental effort?

4. How does the interaction of a particular worked example strategy (i.e., process-

oriented or product-oriented worked examples) and prior knowledge level affect

learner attitude, in particular attention and confidence?

Hypotheses

This research will examine the effect of two independent variables, worked

example strategy and learner prior knowledge on four dependent variables (i.e., learning,

transfer, mental effort, and learner attitude). The research hypotheses and supporting

rationales are presented in the following section.

Learning

Hypothesis 1. Participants presented with a worked example strategy (i.e.,

process-oriented or product-oriented) will score higher on an achievement test than

participants presented with a conventional problem-solving strategy.

Rationale. Solving complex problems requires the learner to apply an appropriate

cognitive strategy. Accordingly, worked examples provide the learner with the relevant

domain specific knowledge and organize the information in a manner (Chi, Glaser, &

Rees, 1982) that increases the probability of schema development and learner success in

solving the problem. Numerous empirical studies have demonstrated that worked

example instruction, especially on complex content (Halabi, Tuovinen, & Farley, 2005;

Tuovinen & Sweller, 1999; Sweller & Cooper, 1985; Zhu & Simon, 1987) is a more

effective strategy than conventional problem solving.

Page 24: Florida State University Libraries254119/datastream/PDF/download/...one of three instructional strategies (process-oriented, product-oriented, or conventional problem solving) developed

12

Hypothesis 2. Low prior knowledge participants presented a process-oriented

worked example will score higher on an achievement test than low prior knowledge

participants presented either a product-oriented worked example or a conventional

problem solving strategy. In contrast, high prior knowledge participants presented with

process-oriented worked examples will demonstrate no difference in achievement test

scores than high prior knowledge participants presented with product-oriented worked

examples or a conventional instructional strategy.

Rationale. A learner’s level of prior knowledge is a critical factor in determining a

number of learning outcome factors, including performance. Individuals with low levels

of prior knowledge lack internal cognitive mechanisms (i.e., case-based experience and

schemas) to solve complex problems efficiently (van Gog et al., 2006). As a result,

instruction provides the necessary external support mechanism to assist learners in

developing the appropriate domain-specific schemas. Recently, researchers have argued

that process-oriented worked examples provide learners with principled knowledge

(“why”) and strategic information (“how”) similar to what experts use in solving

problems (van Gog et al., 2004; Ohlsson & Rees, 1991). In a study of electrical

troubleshooting, van Gog et al. (in press) showed that secondary physics students

studying process-oriented worked examples had higher performance test scores on initial

tasks than product-oriented worked examples (van Gog et al., in press). According to

Cognitive Load Theory (Sweller, 1988), combining the “why” and “how” information in

a worked example enhances learner understanding of both the domain specific

knowledge and the rationale used to determine the proper operators required to solve a

problem (van Gog et al., 2004; 2006).

Despite the benefit of process-oriented worked examples on learning for low prior

knowledge students, the aforementioned studies of process-oriented worked examples

have also found the presence of expert-reversal effect with high prior knowledge students

(van Gog et al., in press; 2006). This effect would suggest that the addition of process

information to worked examples may produce redundant information that increases

extraneous instructional load that would interfere with a more knowledgeable learner’s

previously developed schemas (Renkl & Atkinson, 2003). Consequently, the additional

information provided by a process-oriented worked example would hamper learning for

Page 25: Florida State University Libraries254119/datastream/PDF/download/...one of three instructional strategies (process-oriented, product-oriented, or conventional problem solving) developed

13

more knowledgeable learners because some of the working memory capacity that could

be allocated to problem solving is being utilized to reconcile redundant information.

Hence, low prior knowledge participants were expected to benefit more from process-

oriented worked examples whereas high prior knowledge participants would show no

significant improvements in learning from process-oriented examples based on their

scores on an achievement test when compared to scores of high prior knowledge

participants in product-oriented and conventional instructional strategy groups.

Transfer Performance

Hypothesis 3. Low prior knowledge participants presented a process-oriented

worked example will score higher on the transfer test than low prior knowledge

participants presented with either a product-oriented worked example or a conventional

problem-solving strategy. In contrast, high prior knowledge participants presented with

the process-oriented worked example demonstrate no difference on transfer test scores

than high prior knowledge participants presented with either product-oriented worked

examples or the conventional problem-solving strategy.

Rationale. Transfer is defined as a learner’s ability to apply what was taught to a

new situation (Mayer & Wittrock, 1996). Previous research has demonstrated that the use

of worked examples is an effective instructional strategy that results in better transfer

performance (Sweller & Cooper, 1985; Zhu & Simon, 1987) through the development of

the appropriate domain-specific schema. Moreover, van Gog et al. (2004) argued that a

process-oriented worked example strategy improves transfer by enhancing the learner’s

understanding. Enhanced understanding is the result of presenting additional process

information (i.e., “how” and “why”) to the learner (van Gog et al., 2006) during the initial

phase of the learning process. Hence, it has been argued that low prior knowledge

learners benefit from the addition of process-oriented worked examples more than high

prior knowledge learners.

Conversely, high prior knowledge learners may experience the additional process

information as redundant, imposing extraneous or unproductive load that may impede

continued schema development (Kalyuga, Ayres, Chandler, & Sweller, 2003). Therefore,

providing high prior knowledge learners with only the basic strategic (“how”)

information found in a product-oriented worked example decreases the amount of

Page 26: Florida State University Libraries254119/datastream/PDF/download/...one of three instructional strategies (process-oriented, product-oriented, or conventional problem solving) developed

14

redundant or extraneous information presented during the instructional process. In a

series of experiments examining learners’ ability to learn complex material, Pollock,

Chandler, and Sweller (2002) demonstrated that learners’ with higher levels of prior

knowledge required less explanation about the interaction of elements of complex

materials during instruction than did novice learners. The findings appear to support the

principle of redundancy.

Mental Effort

Hypothesis 4. Participants presented a worked example strategy (i.e., process or

product-oriented) will report lower mental effort ratings on the 9-point mental effort

rating scale developed by Paas (1992) than participants presented a conventional

problem-solving strategy.

Hypothesis 5. Participants presented a process-oriented worked example strategy

will report higher mental effort ratings on the 9-point mental effort rating scale developed

by Paas (1992) than participants presented a product-oriented worked example strategy.

Rationale. Early Cognitive Load research demonstrated that by using well

developed product-oriented worked examples instead of conventional problem solving

strategies learners could improve test and transfer performance with lower investments in

time and mental effort by reducing extraneous cognitive load on learners (Paas & van

Merrienboer, 1994; Paas, 1992). In accordance with Cognitive Load research findings,

several studies have shown the effectiveness of worked examples to teach operational

rules in fields such as accounting (Bonner & Walker, 1994) and economics (Marks &

Rukstad, 1996) by increasing learning while simultaneously reducing the amount of

mental effort exerted during the learning process. More recently, research has begun to

focus on ways to modifying traditional product-oriented worked examples by adding

process information in order to improve learning performance and increase germane load.

This is done by providing the learner with the knowledge to understand the rationale

underlying the selection and utilization of a particular operator to solve a problem (Van

Gog et al., 2006) based on how an expert would approach the problem. Process-oriented

worked examples reduce extraneous load the same as product-oriented examples but

simultaneously increases the germane or relevant cognitive load. Despite increasing

Page 27: Florida State University Libraries254119/datastream/PDF/download/...one of three instructional strategies (process-oriented, product-oriented, or conventional problem solving) developed

15

germane load, the addition of text to a worked example is likely to result in an overall

increase in mental effort in learners.

However, as previously discussed, a learner’s level of prior knowledge is a critical

factor in determining the efficacy of a particular instructional strategy. For example,

when a learner has already acquired the necessary problem-solving schema to perform

the task, providing a worked example with equal or greater amounts of information can

be counter productive. Process information added to a worked example strategy may be

appropriate to support a novice during the initial stages of learning. Despite this benefit to

a novice, a learner with higher levels of prior knowledge and therefore, a more developed

schema may experience the addition of process information as redundant, thereby

increasing extraneous load and interfere with the learning process (Kalyuga et al., 2001;

van Gog et al., 2004; Sweller; 2006). Consequently, an instructional strategy can increase

the cognitive load experienced by learners in different ways based on differences in their

prior knowledge characteristic.

As such, this study proposes that process-oriented examples will present

additional germane information that will result in an increase in cognitive load for

learners with low prior knowledge. Similarly, the process-oriented examples will result in

an increase in cognitive load in learners with high prior knowledge. However, because of

advanced learners have previously developed domain-specific schemas, the additional

information will be both extraneous and redundant potentially impeding the learning

process.

Learner Attitude

Hypothesis 6. Participants receiving a worked example strategy (i.e., process or

product-oriented) will report more positive attitude scores toward the instruction relative

to attention and confidence than participants presented with a conventional problem

solving strategy.

Rationale. Worked example research has primarily concentrated on the cognitive

aspects of the learning process. However, questions about the motivational effect worked

example strategies have on learner attitude have been noticeably absent from the

cognitive load literature (Stark, Mandl, Gruber, & Renkl, 2002). Paas and van

Merrienboer (1994) argued that instructional strategies designed to improve learning

Page 28: Florida State University Libraries254119/datastream/PDF/download/...one of three instructional strategies (process-oriented, product-oriented, or conventional problem solving) developed

16

based on cognitive load principles have little effect if the learner lacks the motivation or

attitude to invest mental effort during the learning process. Cognitive load researchers

have acknowledged the lack of research in motivation is due in part because most studies

have been short in duration and conducted in control laboratory settings (Paas, Tuovinen,

van Merrienboer, & Darabi, 2005). Under these conditions, learners may be more

inclined to invest mental effort due to non-motivational factors (e.g., researcher presence)

that are not common in a natural classroom situation. Despite the acknowledgement that

more research is needed to understand the effects of learner attitude on mental effort

investment and performance, there continues to be a deficiency in cognitive load

literature directly examining questions relating to motivation (Paas & van Merrienboer,

1994).

Motivation has been considered one of the more challenging and important

elements in teaching and in the design of instruction (Keller, 2006). In terms of a

learner’s attitude, several important elements are considered contributors to a learner’s

overall attitude toward a specific learning situation. Specifically, learner attention and

confidence will be analyzed in this study. Attention deals with stimulating and sustaining

attention (i.e., focus or engagement) to the concepts, rules, skills, or facts necessary to

learn a new domain topic (Keller, 2006). Confidence draws on the concepts of locus of

control (Rotter, 1966) and self-efficacy (Bandura, 1982). These two concepts emphasize

the importance of a person’s belief that they have control over their cognitive behavior

and that they can successfully learn a new skill.

In this study, it is expected that a worked example strategy will engage and

sustain a learner’s attention during the learning task by organizing the instructional

material in a manner that optimizes cognitive resources and aids in the development of

the appropriate problem-solving schema. Furthermore, this study is expected to

demonstrate that a learner’s confidence will be increased when given an instructional

strategy, in particular worked examples, that provides direct instructional support (Choi

and Hannafin, 1997) during the problem solving process.

Summary

More than two decades of Cognitive Load research has demonstrated the

effectiveness of worked examples over conventional problem solving in improving

Page 29: Florida State University Libraries254119/datastream/PDF/download/...one of three instructional strategies (process-oriented, product-oriented, or conventional problem solving) developed

17

learning and transfer performance and reducing a learner’s extraneous cognitive load in

well-structured domains such algebra, geometry, and physics (Paas & van Merrienboer,

1994; Clark, Nguyen, & Sweller, 2006). Recently, interest has shifted toward examining

instructional techniques, such as process-oriented worked examples, that increase

germane load while simultaneously reducing extraneous load. This study will extend the

research of van Gog and colleagues (2004; 2006; 2008) on the comparative effectiveness

of process-oriented and product-oriented worked examples on learning and transfer in the

new domain of microeconomics. Furthermore, the inherent complexity or intrinsic load

of a task has been shown to be influenced by prior knowledge (Kalyuga et al., 2003;

Pollock et al., 2002). As a result, the effectiveness of process-oriented or product-oriented

worked example strategy to affect the learning outcomes examined in this study is

expected to be mediated by the learners’ levels of prior knowledge. The present study

will compare the effects of a process-oriented and product-oriented worked example

strategy mediated by learner prior knowledge on learning, transfer performance, mental

effort, and learner attitude. An additional distinctive feature of this study is the

examination of the secondary effect of worked example strategies on learner attitude. The

results of this study are intended to extend the knowledge of worked example design in

order to better inform designers and instructors of how to use worked example strategies

to create an effective instructional environment.

Page 30: Florida State University Libraries254119/datastream/PDF/download/...one of three instructional strategies (process-oriented, product-oriented, or conventional problem solving) developed

18

Definition of Key Terms

Worked Examples

Worked examples are a step-by-step model of how to perform a particular skill or

task (Clark, Nguyen, & Sweller, 2006, p. 190). Typically, worked examples used in

instruction consist of three elements: (1) a problem statement, (2) steps to solve the

problem, and (3) the final solution to the problem (Renkl & Atkinson, 2003; Kalyuga,

Chandler, Tuovinen, & Sweller, 2001; Renkl, 1997).

Product-Oriented Worked Example

Product-oriented worked examples show the learner how to solve a problem (the

product) by providing the learner with the problem statement, the solution steps to solve

the problem, and the final solution (van Gog et al., 2006).

Process-Oriented Worked Example

Process-oriented worked examples show the learner how to solve the problem but

also explain why certain steps are chosen (i.e., strategic knowledge) and/or why the steps

are appropriate (i.e., principled knowledge) in an effort to improve a learner’s

understanding of the solution procedure (van Gog et al., 2006).

Conventional Problem Solving

Conventional problem solving provides the learner with practice problems to

solve without any support or guidance (Paas, 1992) during the practice activity. In this

study, conventional problems were given to learners after a common lecture provided to

all participants.

Learning

Participants’ learning will be measured from their total scores on eleven

achievement test questions developed jointly by the researcher and instructor.

Page 31: Florida State University Libraries254119/datastream/PDF/download/...one of three instructional strategies (process-oriented, product-oriented, or conventional problem solving) developed

19

Transfer

Applying what was learned during the instruction to a novel situation (Mayer &

Wittrock, 1996). Transfer will be measured from transfer test scores developed jointly by

the researcher and instructor.

Prior Knowledge

Existing domain-specific knowledge of the learner (Hannafin & Hooper, 1993).

Low Prior Knowledge

Low prior knowledge learners are those who have a limited level of knowledge or

experience in a specific domain. In this study, low prior knowledge participants were

categorized based on a pre-test score equal to or below 11 out of a possible score of 15.

High Prior Knowledge

High prior knowledge learners are those who have substantial previously acquired

knowledge in a specific domain. In this study, high prior knowledge participants were

categorized based on a pre-test score equal to or higher than 13 out of a possible score of

15.

Mental Effort

Investment of mental effort is a reflection of the actual cognitive load allocated to

task performance. In this study, participant mental effort ratings were taken after the

administration of the instructional condition using a 9-point scale self-reporting rating

scale developed by Paas (1992).

Learner Attitude

Learner attitude is defined as the attitude of the participants toward the

instructional material. In this study, attitude will be measured in terms of attention and

confidence scores on Keller’s Instructional Materials Motivation Survey (IMMS). The

survey will contain 21 items, twelve of the questions related to attention and nine of the

questions related to confidence.

Page 32: Florida State University Libraries254119/datastream/PDF/download/...one of three instructional strategies (process-oriented, product-oriented, or conventional problem solving) developed

20

CHAPTER 2

REVIEW OF THE LITERATURE

Cognitive Load Theory

Cognitive load theory (Sweller, 1988) argues that learning a complex cognitive

skill, such as problem solving, is often constrained by a learner’s limited processing

capacity. Therefore, developing instruction that optimizes the allocation of cognitive

resources is essential for learning to occur through the development of domain-specific

schema. Cognitive load theory is based on the underlying assumptions that learners can

only process a few pieces of information at one time (Miller, 1956; Moreno, 2006) and

that long-term memory has an extensive cognitive capacity comprised of vast,

hierarchically organized knowledge structures (Kalyuga, Ayres, Chandler, & Sweller,

2003). Hence, working memory is viewed as a transitory cognitive structure where the

conscious processing of information occurs.

Information enters working memory through either the sensory memory or is

retrieved from long-term memory (Sweller, 2004). Once in working memory, the

information is structured and organized in a manner that allows the learner to store the

information in long-term memory making the information available for retrieval from its

long-term memory in the future. Thus, understanding the structural capacity limitations

of working memory and the vast knowledge stores of long-term memory are essential to

developing effective instruction. Instructional strategies that require learners to engage in

learning activities that place excessive demands on working memory and are not effective

in organizing information in manner that enhances schema development are likely to

produce ineffective results (Kirschner, Sweller, & Clark, 2006).

Cognitive demands placed on learners can be evaluated as excessive based on the

level of element interactivity associated with a particular skill or task to be learned.

Cognitive load researchers have proposed that the cognitive load associated with learning

is related to the degree to which elements interact with each other (Sweller & Chandler,

1994). For example, consider a student learning the definition of a vocabulary word. If

the focus is entirely on the vocabulary word and not the context the word is used then the

Page 33: Florida State University Libraries254119/datastream/PDF/download/...one of three instructional strategies (process-oriented, product-oriented, or conventional problem solving) developed

21

word can be learned without reference to other words. This represents low element

interactivity and consequently low cognitive load.

On the contrary, learning complex tasks, such as economic problem solving,

represents high element interactivity and therefore results in higher levels of cognitive

load (Halford, Maybery, & Bain, 1986; Maybery, Bain, & Halford, 1986). For example,

learners studying microeconomics must learn how to calculate tradeoff ratios by first

determining various cost and gain measures based on analyzing an economy’s production

possibilities for a particular pair of goods or services. In this example, Sweller and

Chandler (1994) argue that in order to generate a problem solution, each step of the

analysis is an element that must interact with other elements in order to arrive at a

solution to the problem. Consequently, the analysis steps cannot be learned in isolation

and must be learned simultaneously. Due to the high degree of element interactivity

required in problem solving, the level of cognitive load will also be high resulting in

some learners having difficulty in learning the material (Sweller & Chandler, 1994).

Cognitive load theory (Sweller, 1988) distinguishes cognitive load according to

three distinct categories: (1) intrinsic, (2) extraneous, and (3) germane (Sweller, van

Merrienboer, Paas, 1998). Intrinsic load is the cognitive load due to the complex nature

or the inherent difficulty of instructional material. Complex tasks such as problem

solving contain a high number of interacting elements that impose a high demand on

working memory to process the information. One non-instructional variable that

influences intrinsic load is a learner’s prior knowledge. Despite the high number of

interacting elements in complex tasks, learners with higher levels of prior knowledge can

combine complex informational elements with existing schemata and treat the expanded

schemata as one element in working memory (van Gog, Paas, & van Merrienboer, 2007).

Therefore, the greater a learner’s prior knowledge the less intrinsic load the learner

experiences during a complex task.

Extraneous load or instructionally ineffective load (van Gog, Paas, & van

Merrienboer, 2004) is caused by poorly designed instruction that fail to recognize the

limits of working memory and the need to facilitate schema development and automation

(Sweller, 2005). A conventional learning approach to problem solving is through means-

ends analysis, often employed by novice learners (Sweller, van Merrienboer, Paas, 1998).

Page 34: Florida State University Libraries254119/datastream/PDF/download/...one of three instructional strategies (process-oriented, product-oriented, or conventional problem solving) developed

22

According to Sweller et al., (1998), means-ends analysis is a “slow and sub-optimal”

approach to learning. Specifically, means-ends analysis places a high demand on working

memory resources in order to solve the problem while simultaneously reducing the

cognitive resources available for learning (i.e., schema development and automation).

Worked examples have been shown to reduce extraneous load by focusing cognitive

resources on studying the solutions and constructing and automating problem solving

schema (van Gog, Paas, & van Merrienboer, 2007).

Lastly, germane load or effective instructional load (Paas & van Merrienboer,

1994) is concerned with capturing the relevant elements of the learning process through

effective instructional design. Providing a learner an example of how to solve a complex

problem may increase cognitive load but the increase in germane load facilitates schema

development (van Gog, Paas, & van Merrienbeor, 2004, 2006; Sweller, 2005) by

focusing the learner’s attention on the salient steps or operators necessary for solving the

particular problem. By engaging in this type of focused learning activity, learners are

better able to develop the appropriate problem-solving schemas necessary for transfer and

retention of information.

Cognitive Load Theory and Conventional Problem-Solving Strategies

In introductory economic principle courses, a conventional problem-solving

approach is generally manifest in the form of a traditional lecture-based instructional

strategy. In this approach, the standard instructional practice is to introduce the problem

to the learner through a lecture and the presentation of a few examples, followed by

extensive independent problem solving practice (Owen & Sweller, 1985; Sweller, 1988),

either during class or as homework. Despite recent studies demonstrating the

effectiveness of alternative approaches to economic instruction (Jensen & Owen, 2003;

Marks and Rukstad, 1996), Becker and Watts (2001) found that the majority of

introductory economics instructors spend 83 percent of the class time lecturing.

Considering the complexity of economics, most instructors using a traditional lecture-

based instructional strategy still assume that learners will acquire the appropriate domain

specific problem solving skills and then correctly apply them during problem solving

transfer activities (i.e., test) with limited instructional guidance. Cognitive load

Page 35: Florida State University Libraries254119/datastream/PDF/download/...one of three instructional strategies (process-oriented, product-oriented, or conventional problem solving) developed

23

researchers view this approach as ineffective for learning (Darabi et al., 2006) due to the

instructional strategy’s lack of attention focused on schema development.

Instructional strategies requiring learners to engage in conventional problem-

solving strategy, such as means-end analysis, requires the novice learner to carry out a

series of “content-free” (Bhaskar & Simon, 1977) if-then action sequences (Anderson,

1982) designed to identify, and then limit possible paths to a solution (Zook & Di Vesta,

1989; Chi, Glaser, & Rees, 1982). Despite the fact that a means-end analysis approach

can facilitate the attainment of a problem goal, numerous studies have demonstrated that

a means-end analysis search has a detrimental effect on learning during problem solving

activities (Cooper & Sweller, 1987; Owen & Sweller, 1985; Sweller & Cooper, 1985;

Mawer & Sweller, 1982) because the approach does not focus on developing appropriate

domain-specific schema. Furthermore, means-end analysis places a high extraneous load

on a learner’s limited cognitive resources (van Gog, Paas, & van Merrienboer, 2006)

thereby reducing the available resources necessary for learning. Sweller (2006) argues

that learning occurs when long-term memory structures are transformed. Therefore,

according to cognitive load theory, only through the development of schema or the

modification of existing schema can learning actually occur.

Research in cognitive load theory suggests that a critical factor in developing

problem solving skills is the “acquisition of domain-specific knowledge schemas”

(Sweller & Cooper, 1985, p. 61). Domain specific schemas allow learners to recognize a

particular problem as belonging to a category based on the salient structural features of

the problem. In contrast to the schema-based approach, novices employing means-end

search strategies tend to direct their attention toward reducing the discrepancies between

the problem state and the goal state. The disadvantage of this approach to learning is the

strategy attempts to reduce differences between problem and goal states without guiding

the novice learner in recognizing the problem-state patterns and the associated steps to

solve the problem (Sweller & Cooper, 1985; Sweller, 1988). Problem solving in this

manner is inefficient often resulting in the random generation of solution possibilities

(Sweller, 2006) that may overwhelm a learner’s working memory capacity. However, an

instructional strategy, such as studying worked examples has been shown to reduce

Page 36: Florida State University Libraries254119/datastream/PDF/download/...one of three instructional strategies (process-oriented, product-oriented, or conventional problem solving) developed

24

extraneous cognitive load and increase learning by fostering the development of problem

solving schema (van Gog, Paas, & van Merrienboer, 2006).

Learning from Worked Examples

Learning from worked examples is an alternative strategy to conventional

problem solving that has shown to be effective in initial skill acquisition for novice

learners (Renkl, 2002) in well-structured environments such as mathematics, computer

programming, accounting, and physical science (Renkl & Atkinson, 2003). Because

novice learners lack the appropriate problem-solving schema in long-term memory,

learners often struggle to organize information in a manner that accurately represents the

problem to be solved. A worked example instructional strategy is a more effective means

of instruction for novice learners because it prevents the learner from having to rely on an

unspecific problem-solving heuristic, such as means-end analysis, for the solution (Renkl,

2005). This is accomplished by directing the learner’s attention to essential relationships

between the problem-solving steps (Kirschner et al., 2006). As a result, learners develop

the appropriate problem schema required to solve a problem (van Gog, Paas, & van

Merrienboer, 2006; Kirschner et al., 2006).

Worked examples are a step-by-step model of how to perform a particular skill or

task (Clark, Nguyen, & Sweller, 2006, p. 190). Typically, worked examples used in

instruction consist of three elements: (1) a problem statement, (2) steps to solve the

problem, and (3) the final solution to the problem (Renkl & Atkinson, 2003; Kalyuga,

Chandler, Tuovinen, & Sweller, 2001; Renkl, 1997). Thus, the presentation of the

solution steps to the problem prior to their engagement of the problem-solving task

differentiates a worked example strategy from a conventional problem solving strategy.

Consequently, the purpose of using a worked example strategy is to prepare learners for

“productive problem solving” by promoting a deep understanding of a domain skill

during the initial stages of cognitive skill acquisition (Renkl, 2005).

The “worked example effect” can be described as the positive performance

outcome of novices who study worked examples that make efficient use of limited

cognitive resources. Moreover, by studying worked examples versus engaging in

conventional problem solving to solve a problem, a novice learner is more able to filter

out extraneous information, thereby optimizing the available cognitive resources to

Page 37: Florida State University Libraries254119/datastream/PDF/download/...one of three instructional strategies (process-oriented, product-oriented, or conventional problem solving) developed

25

induct problem-solving schemas (Moreno, 2006) in long-term memory. The “worked

example effect” was originally demonstrated with novice learners studying algebra by

Sweller and Cooper (1985) and Cooper and Sweller (1987). Since those early studies, the

“worked example effect” has been replicated in numerous experimental domains (for

overviews see Adkinson, Derry, Renkl, & Wortham, 2000; Sweller, van Merrienboer, &

Paas, 1998).

According to Cognitive Load Theory (Sweller, 1988; Sweller, van Merrienboer,

& Paas, 1998), a worked example strategy is based on the premise that worked examples

optimize the learner’s processing capacity by reducing or eliminating random problem

solving actions commonly associated with means-end analysis. Research in the field of

expert-novice differences has shown that individuals with well-developed domain-

specific problem solving schema (i.e., experts) have more efficient representations of

their domain than novices do. Consequently, expert representations focus on the deep

structure of a problem and ignore irrelevant surface details that tend to distract novices

(Pretz, Naples, & Sternberg, 2003). Whereas the application of more conventional

problem solving strategies require the learner to work backwards from a randomly

generated goal followed by tests for effectiveness (Sweller & Sweller, 2006), a worked

example requires the learner to study problem solutions prior to attempting to solve

similar problems. Worked examples can organize and structure information in a manner

that reduces extraneous load while simultaneously increasing germane load and facilitates

the development of schema in long-term memory. Therefore, in order to develop

effective instructional programs, it is critical to understand the interplay between working

memory and long-term memory (Sweller et al., 1998).

Despite the benefits of utilizing worked examples or a combination of worked

examples with practice problems versus conventional problem solving, there are some

instances where the use of worked examples is detrimental to learning. Poorly designed

worked examples can add extraneous load and thereby eliminate the benefit of using a

worked example. Consequently, worked examples should be designed in a format that

minimizes extraneous load by managing three potential design concerns: (1) split-

attention effect, (2) modality effect, and (3) redundancy effect.

Page 38: Florida State University Libraries254119/datastream/PDF/download/...one of three instructional strategies (process-oriented, product-oriented, or conventional problem solving) developed

26

Split-attention effect (Chandler & Sweller, 1991) refers to the extraneous load

imposed when a learner must integrate two or more sources of visual information that are

physically or spatially separated (Clark et al., 2006, p.351). In domains such as

economics, a learner is often required to simultaneously integrate textual information and

a related diagram. Split-attention commonly occurs when a visual is explained by text

that is distant from the visual such as a geometry problem with the step-by-step

procedures required to solve it provided below the problem. In this scenario, in order to

understand the solution to the problem the learner must read a particular step and search

for the relevant section of the visual. The search process followed by the mental

integration of the informational elements increases extraneous load on the learner and

hampers learning. In order to integrate complex information, worked examples should be

designed to focus the learner’s attention on the relevant content necessary to solve the

problem. One way to do this is by integrating explanatory text close to the related visuals.

Several studies have demonstrated the split-attention effect in worked examples.

Tarmizi and Sweller (1988) ran a series of experiments on high school geometry students

comparing a worked example that integrated text within the visual with a conventional

problem solving approach. The results supported their hypothesis that the worked

example that integrated text within the visual was more instructionally effective at

improving learner performance while reducing working memory load. In a similar study,

Ward and Sweller (1990) replicated the findings of Tarmizi and Sweller (1988) with high

school physics students providing further support that the use of an integrated worked

example design was crucial to enhance learning and transfer. Research on split-attention

effect suggests that worked example strategies that lead to split attention are quite

common in instructional materials. To improve learning, designers should develop

examples that limit split-attention effect and perform the integration for the learner by

providing the text explanation of a visual in close proximity to the visual and using cues

and identifiers such as arrows to focus the learner’s attention to the relevant part of the

visual. In addition, where possible present all relevant text and visuals on the same page

or screen (Clark et al., 2006).

The examination of split-attention effect, especially in multimedia environments,

led to the discovery of the modality effect. The modality effect occurs when complex

Page 39: Florida State University Libraries254119/datastream/PDF/download/...one of three instructional strategies (process-oriented, product-oriented, or conventional problem solving) developed

27

visuals are more easily understood if the explanation is presented in an audio modality

than if presented in text modality (Clark et al., 2006, p. 348). The influence of the

modality effect and the split-attention effect on student performance was demonstrated in

a study of trade school apprentices learning how to conduct electrical tests on appliances

using one of three print-based instructional modules: (1) integrated text/visual, (2)

separated text/visual with text presented underneath the visual, and (3) audio narration of

the visual (Tindall-Ford, Chandler, and Sweller, 1997). The researchers found that

learners studying the integrated text/visual module or the audio narration module scored

twice as high on a performance test as learners in the separated text/visual module. In a

similar study of multimedia learning, Moreno and Mayer (1990) compared the efficacy of

integrated, separated, and audio worked example strategies in an animated multimedia

lesson on the formation of lightning. The findings suggested that learners viewing the

narrated version had more significant learning gains than learners studying either the

integrated or the separated strategies.

Like the modality effect, the redundancy effect was discovered while researching

the effect of split-attention on learning. The redundancy effect (Chandler & Sweller,

1991) occurs when the instruction provides more expressions of content than is necessary

for a learner to gain understanding. For example, if a visual is self-explanatory because it

provides all the information necessary for a learner to understand the content adding

additional text may actually overload working memory capacity and hamper learning.

The issue of redundancy becomes more critical when dealing with complex content that

places high demands on a learner’s working memory (Clark et al., 2006). In a study of

the effectiveness of worked examples that integrated text and visuals with physics

students, Ward and Sweller (1990) found that while integration has a positive effect on

learning and transfer, providing more explanatory information than was necessary was

redundant and therefore counterproductive to learning. In other words, including

redundant information placed greater demands on working memory capacity that can

depress learning. Hence, removing redundant information is a necessary design

consideration for the development of effective instruction.

Chandler and Sweller (1991) further examined the effect of redundant information

and split-attention on instructional time and student learning. Students studying a biology

Page 40: Florida State University Libraries254119/datastream/PDF/download/...one of three instructional strategies (process-oriented, product-oriented, or conventional problem solving) developed

28

lesson on blood flow through the heart were presented with three versions of the

instruction: (1) diagram only, (2) diagram plus embedded text, and (3) diagram plus

separate text. Results showed that test scores were lower for students in the diagram plus

separate text lesson than those in either the diagram plus embedded text or the diagram

only lessons. Moreover, instructional time was longer for students in the diagram plus

separate text lesson than the other two instructional lessons. Of particular interest is that

students in the diagram only lesson significantly out performed students in the diagram

plus embedded text lesson after less instructional time. The findings of this study suggest

that by adding additional text instructional time was increased and student learning was

reduced.

In addition, a series of experiments conducted by Sweller and Chandler (1994)

and Chandler and Sweller (1996) extended our understanding of the redundancy effect by

comparing different approaches to the instruction (i.e., conventional-manual, modified-

manual only, and modified-manual plus a computer) of a computer program and the

interaction of various approaches and content complexity. The researchers found that for

content low in complexity there were no significant performance differences in the

instructional approaches. However, as the complexity of the content increased, learners

exposed to the modified-manual only approach showed higher performance scores, lower

cognitive load, and spent less time evaluating the instruction than the other two

instructional approaches.

In summary, over two decades of research has demonstrated that worked

examples can effectively increase the likelihood of deeper cognitive processing (i.e.,

schema development) while simultaneously limiting the cognitive load imposed on a

learners’ working memory. Consequently, it can be argued that an effectively designed

worked example increases cognitive capacity by effectively managing working memory

load in order to promote schema development and automation (Darabi, Nelson, &

Palanki, 2007; Sweller, van Merrienboer, & Paas, 1998, Zhu & Simon, 1987). For

example, contrary to novices, experts use previously acquired schemas to classify the

problem, and apply the most effective solution to the problem (Sweller & Cooper, 1985).

The contrast between novice and expert problem solving approaches demonstrates that a

properly designed worked example should focus on assisting learners to develop domain-

Page 41: Florida State University Libraries254119/datastream/PDF/download/...one of three instructional strategies (process-oriented, product-oriented, or conventional problem solving) developed

29

specific schemas during the learning process, especially during the initial stages of skill

acquisition.

Worked Example Strategies: Product-Oriented and Process-Oriented

Recently, two types of worked example strategies have been discussed in the

literature: “product-oriented” and “process-oriented” (van Merrienboer, Kirschner, &

Kester, 2003; Van Gog, Paas, & van Merrienboer, 2004, 2006). Product-oriented worked

examples, also referred to as conventional worked examples, provide the learner with a

problem solution step-by-step from the given state to goal state. This type of worked

example presents the learner with the “product” an expert has produced at each stage of

the problem-solving process (van Gog et al., 2004). A limitation of this form of worked

example is that it fails to provide learners with the principled knowledge (domain specific

knowledge) necessary for learners to appropriately represent the problem and make the

correct selection and application of steps and operators to solve the problem. In other

words, product-oriented worked examples do not explain “why” certain steps are taken

during the problem solving process (van Gog et al., 2006). Since knowledge of a domain

consists of principled knowledge and its teleology, students need to understand the

principles underlying the solution, and why these steps are taken in a particular order

(van Gog et al., 2004; Ohlsson & Rees, 1991).

Variations of worked examples have been tested as a means to compensate for the

lack of principled and strategic knowledge incorporated in more traditional product-

oriented examples. Specifically, worked examples with self-explanation prompts and

instructional explanations that provide the rationale for solution steps are two worked

example strategies that have been advocated as ways to provide principled information

and enhance transfer performance (van Gog et al., 2006; Renkl, 2002). Despite the fact

that learners who self-explain tend to demonstrate a greater level of understanding than

learners who do not, research findings suggest that early in the learning process many

learners are unable to generate the appropriate self-explanations independently (Chi,

Bassok, Lewis, Reimann, & Glaser, 1989; Renkl, 1997).

Similarly, worked examples with instructional explanations have been examined

as an alternative to worked examples with self-explanation prompts. According to van

Gog et al. (in press; 2006), in this type of worked example, the information provided was

Page 42: Florida State University Libraries254119/datastream/PDF/download/...one of three instructional strategies (process-oriented, product-oriented, or conventional problem solving) developed

30

designed to assist the learner in developing the underlying principle behind each solution

step but lack strategic information (heuristics and systematic approaches to solving

problems). A worked example with instructional explanations is a more guided approach

to example instruction than requiring learners to “self-generate” information. However,

several studies have failed to find consistent positive effects from instructional

explanations on learning and transfer performance (Gerjects, Scheiter, & Catrambone,

2006; Grobe & Renkl, 2006; Renkl, 2002).

Recently, van Gog et al. (2004; 2006) proposed that developing worked examples

comprised of solution steps, and both strategic and principled (i.e., declarative)

information would enhance learners’ understanding and transfer performance. This type

of worked example has been described as a “process-oriented” worked example because

it expands beyond the presentation of solution steps providing both the how (strategic)

and why (principled) behind the problem steps. Several studies have examined the effect

of worked examples designed with principled information to enhance learner

understanding. Gerjets, Schieter, and Catrambone (2003) investigated the effect of

worked examples and learner prior knowledge on transfer performance. The study found

that transfer performance was improved with less study time when learners were

provided with a worked example that combined the procedural steps necessary to solve

the problem and the explanation underlying each step. A study by Singley and Anderson

(1989) examined the role of declarative (i.e., principled) knowledge in the domain of

calculus and logic and found that the use of declarative knowledge provides the basis for

transfer performance. In addition, while the findings supported the use of principled

knowledge the researchers noted that the benefit of using principled knowledge may be

diminished as a learner’s knowledge level increases.

Despite the stated benefits of using process-oriented worked examples, the results

of a recent study on troubleshooting revealed that the use of process-oriented worked

examples had a limited positive effect on student learning (van Gog et al., 2006). Several

factors are likely to have influenced the effectiveness of process-oriented worked

examples, such as example design, timing of information presentation, learner attention

to the information, and information integration. One of the most critical factors in

determining the effectiveness of any instructional material is a learner’s prior knowledge.

Page 43: Florida State University Libraries254119/datastream/PDF/download/...one of three instructional strategies (process-oriented, product-oriented, or conventional problem solving) developed

31

The next section will provide an overview of the influence of prior knowledge on learner

performance.

Worked Example Efficacy and Learner Prior Knowledge

The advantage of worked examples during the initial acquisition stage of learning

for novice learners has been well documented in the Cognitive Load literature. Therefore,

instructional designers should consider a learner’s level of prior knowledge when

developing any instruction, particularly if the material has a high degree of complexity.

Several empirical studies have provided strong evidence that novices learn differently

than more advanced learners (Lim, 2006). Moreover, research has demonstrated that

advanced learners organize their knowledge more effectively and access information

more efficiently from long-term memory than novices (Chi, Glaser, & Farr, 1988). In

other words, advanced learners have more developed schemas that allow them to access

greater amounts of information while using less working memory capacity than novices.

As a result, advanced learners require less external support to solve a problem or task.

Kalyuga, Ayres, Chandler, & Sweller, (2003) described this phenomenon as the “expert-

reversal effect”. The principle underlying the expert-reversal effect stipulates that

learners with higher levels of prior knowledge may not benefit from the same level of

instructional support required by novice learners.

Research on expertise-reversal effect has shown that a learner’s prior knowledge

determines the effectiveness of the type of worked examples (Van Gog et al., 2006). For

example, in a study of mechanical trade apprentices, Kalyuga, Chandler, Tuovinen, and

Sweller (2001) found that apprentices’ learning about relay circuits benefited from

worked examples in the initial stages of acquisition but the benefit diminished over time

as the apprentices gained experience in the domain. In other words, an instructional

approach that aided novice learners (i.e., low prior knowledge) in acquiring a skill

yielded the opposite effect for learners with an already developed schema (i.e., high prior

knowledge). Cognitive Load Theory (Sweller, 1988) explains this reversal by the

redundancy effect (Renkl & Atkinson, 2003).

The redundancy effect occurs when non-essential information is presented to a

learner that increases extraneous load that interferes with learning (Sweller, 2005). For

example, in a study comparing the effectiveness of worked examples to problem-solving

Page 44: Florida State University Libraries254119/datastream/PDF/download/...one of three instructional strategies (process-oriented, product-oriented, or conventional problem solving) developed

32

instructional strategies in accounting Halabi, Tuovinen, and Farley (2005) found that low

prior knowledge participants using worked examples scored significantly higher than low

prior knowledge participants completing the problem-solving strategy on test

performance and learning efficiency measures. In contrast, high prior knowledge

participants showed little difference on test performance and learning efficiency between

the two instructional strategies. (i.e., worked examples and problem-solving). It can be

concluded that the efficacy of worked examples is largely effected by the learner’s prior

knowledge. Hence, as expertise or proficiency increases during the learning process,

providing the same level of instructional guidance can be counter-productive to learning

gains. Consequently, extraneous load can be reduced by scaffolding the instructional

support throughout the knowledge acquisition process from more guidance at the initial

learning stage (i.e., process-oriented worked examples) to faded guidance (i.e., product-

oriented worked examples) at later stages. Simultaneously, by reducing the instructional

support in response to the knowledge proficiency of the learner germane load is increased

resulting in a higher level of learning efficiency (van Gog et al, in press; Renkl &

Atkinson, 2003).

More recently, van Merrienboer et al. (2003) argued that the sequencing of

instructional information plays an important role in schema development and activation

in novice learners. Consequently, van Gog et al., (in press; 2006) examined how

variations in the sequencing of worked example conditions can affect learning efficiency.

The study found that process-oriented worked examples initially foster learning, but

subsequently hinder learning as expertise is gained by the learner during the learning

process further supporting the expert-reversal effect. As a result, worked example

research needs to further examine the effect of worked example strategies, particularly a

process-oriented worked example strategy, and learner prior knowledge characteristics on

learning, transfer, mental effort, and attitude toward the instructional material.

Learner Attitude

A learner’s acquisition of problem solving skills is of limited value if the

individual is unmotivated to apply them. Consequently, solving complex problems

requires more than domain knowledge (Zimmerman & Campillo, 2003). Problems of a

complex nature place a heavy demand on a learner’s working memory capacity (Sweller,

Page 45: Florida State University Libraries254119/datastream/PDF/download/...one of three instructional strategies (process-oriented, product-oriented, or conventional problem solving) developed

33

1988) as well as on their motivational attitude toward the instruction (Paas, Tuovinen,

van Merrienboer, & Darabi, 2005). In such instances, anxiety and fear of failure (Keller,

2006) can overwhelm a learner’s ability to effectively process the necessary solution

steps. In fact, researchers have argued that a positive motivational attitude generates

positive learning outcomes by giving the learner confidence to deal with the cognitive

challenges associated with complex problems (van Merrienboer, Kirschner, & Kester,

2003). Hence, instruction should be developed in a manner that encourages both the

development of the appropriate domain-specific schema and positively affecting the

attitude of the learner.

According to Keller (2006), in order to develop effective instruction a designer

should consider the attitude of the learner and its effect on their performance. However,

in a natural classroom environment that imposes a variety of environmental and

instructional distractions the challenge of addressing learner attitude becomes particularly

problematic. In discussing Cognitive Load Theory and the role of motivation, Paas et al.

(2005) argued that “non-laboratory” experimental settings represent an environment for

which “cognitive load researchers need to determine the [attitudinal] effects of

instructional conditions, and identify strategies that keep student attention on the learning

materials” (p. 27). Consequently, instructional designers should seek ways to gain and

maintain a learner’s attention on the essential processes necessary to solve the problem

while simultaneously encouraging persistence by instilling confidence in their ability to

solve complex problems. Worked examples may provide an instructional strategy that

can effectively attend to both student attention and confidence.

Traditionally, worked example research has concentrated on the cognitive aspects

of the learning process without considering the influence learner attitude has on learning

(Paas et al., 2005). However, questions about the motivational effect worked example

strategies have on learner attitude have been noticeably absent from the cognitive load

literature (Stark, Mandl, Gruber, & Renkl, 2002). Paas and van Merrienboer (1994)

argued that instructional strategies designed to improve learning based on cognitive load

principles have little effect if the learner lacks the motivation or attitude to invest mental

effort during the learning process. Cognitive load researchers acknowledge the lack of

research in motivation is due in part because most studies have been short in duration and

Page 46: Florida State University Libraries254119/datastream/PDF/download/...one of three instructional strategies (process-oriented, product-oriented, or conventional problem solving) developed

34

conducted in control laboratory settings (Paas, Tuovinen, van Merrienboer, & Darabi,

2005). Under these conditions, learners may be more inclined to invest mental effort due

to non-motivational factors (e.g., researcher presence) that are not common in a natural

classroom situation. Recently, many CLT researchers have expanded their inquiry to

include the mediating influence of prior knowledge and motivational attitude (Paas et al.,

2005; Moreno, 2006; Paas & van Merrienboer, 1994). This study intends to “advance our

understanding of who benefits from different worked example designs” (Moreno, 2006,

p. 179) and the effect worked examples have on learner attitude.

Summary

Research on the worked example effect has provided instructional designers with

a valuable source of information by advancing our understanding of human cognition

(Sweller, 2006). Cognitive Load Theory (Sweller, 1988; Sweller et al., 1998; van Gog et

al., 2006) clarifies the relationship between learning and effectively developed

instructional material. Accordingly, Cognitive Load Theory argues that instructional

materials pull on a learner’s cognitive resources based on the intrinsic, extraneous, and

germane load the materials impose. Hence, the way a worked example is developed will

influence the level of cognitive load experienced by the learner. Therefore, a designer

should be cognizant of the impact cognitive load has on learning and develop materials

that optimize a learner’s working memory capacity by providing the necessary

instructional support throughout the learning process.

Worked example research has recently shifted its focus away from independently

examining extraneous load toward investigating how variations in example strategy can

increase germane load on learners. Most cognitive load effects from worked example

instruction do not vary germane load directly. Instead, germane load is increased by

reducing extraneous load that in turn frees up working memory capacity (Sweller, 2006).

As a result, van Gog et al. (2004) proposed that providing learners process-information

(i.e., principled and strategic information) relevant to understanding the will improve

understanding and transfer because they will be “challenged to invest germane effort in

studying the why and how information” (van Gog et al., 2004, p. 96).

However, contrary to expected findings van Gog et al. (2006) reported that

learners in conditions with process-oriented worked examples had lower near and far

Page 47: Florida State University Libraries254119/datastream/PDF/download/...one of three instructional strategies (process-oriented, product-oriented, or conventional problem solving) developed

35

transfer scores, took longer to complete tasks during training and on transfer tests, and

exerted more mental effort than those in conditions with product-oriented worked

examples. A possible explanation for the contradictory findings was the level of learner

prior knowledge and the amount of instructional support provided by the worked

example. In a follow up study, van Gog and her colleagues (in press) explored the

interaction of prior knowledge and worked example sequencing. The researchers found

evidence that process-oriented worked examples can initially improve learning in novice

learners but the additional process-information may become redundant and hamper

learning for more proficient learners. The purpose of the proposed study is to extend this

line of research by examining the effect different worked example strategies and prior

knowledge have on learning and transfer of problem solving skills and learner attitude in

the domain of microeconomics.

Page 48: Florida State University Libraries254119/datastream/PDF/download/...one of three instructional strategies (process-oriented, product-oriented, or conventional problem solving) developed

36

CHAPTER 3

METHOD

The present study investigated the effect of process-oriented and product-oriented

worked example instructional strategies on learning and transfer performance in

microeconomics. Specifically, participants were instructed on how to apply the

microeconomic principles of supply and demand and elasticity to solve complex

problems posed by the burden of taxes on the market behavior of buyers and sellers. In

addition, the mediating effect of prior knowledge on learning and transfer was also

investigated. This chapter describes the approach used to compare the effects of worked

example strategies, process-oriented and product-oriented, and conventional problem-

solving on participant learning and transfer performance, mental effort, and attitude based

on factors attributable to the characteristics of the instructional strategies being examined.

Participants

The participants in this study included 415 undergraduate students enrolled in an

introductory microeconomics course of a large southeastern United States public

university. The study took place over the course of three regularly scheduled class

periods in a large lecture hall located in a shared teaching facility in the center of campus.

All participants involved in the study did so voluntarily, in full compliance with

guidelines established by the University Human Subjects Committee. Each participant

granted permission by signing an informed consent form (see Appendix A).

Available participant demographic data including gender, race, age, year in

school, and academic major was obtained from the university registrar’s office through a

formal request (see Appendix B). The average age of participants was 19.5 years

(SD=1.803). Freshman and sophomores comprised 80.9% of the participants with 113

freshman, 188 sophomores, 55 juniors, and 16 seniors. Participant gender composition

was 48.4% (n=198) female and 51.6% (n=211) male. The race/ethnicity of participants

was mixed with the majority 76.9% (n=286) being white/Caucasian and 12.4% (n=46) of

Hispanic descent. Participant academic majors were also mixed. The majority of

Page 49: Florida State University Libraries254119/datastream/PDF/download/...one of three instructional strategies (process-oriented, product-oriented, or conventional problem solving) developed

37

participants had identified a major as either business 49.5% (n=184), social science

12.9% (n=48) or merchandising 8.4% (n=35).

Three instructional conditions were created for the present study based on the type

of strategy (i.e., process-oriented, product-oriented, and conventional problem solving).

Participants were randomly assigned to one of the three conditions as they entered the

lecture hall. Table 3.1 shows the number of participants assigned to each instructional

condition. The number of participants assigned to each instructional condition was

relatively equal.

Table 3.1. Descriptive Statistics on Instructional Strategy Conditions

Instructional strategy Frequency Percent

Process 143 34.5 Product 146 35.2

Conventional 126 30.4 Total 415 100.0

Instructional Environment

The design and arrangement of the instructional environment can have significant

affect on both student and instructor behavior. Educational psychologists have contented

that the structure and organization of the environment in which instruction occurs can

facilitate or inhibit learning (Johnson, Johnson, & Holubec, 1994). In many university

settings, introductory courses are taught in lecture halls hosting large numbers of students

that support a more traditional passive, instructor-centered learning environment. To

evaluate both the environment factors and instructional approach of the course, three

separate classroom observations were conducted over a four-week period prior to the

study.

To guide the collection of data a classroom observation checklist was developed

(see Appendix C) to collect information on four distinct elements: (1) variety and pacing

of instruction, (2) organization and presentation of information, (3) instructional clarity,

and (4) instructor-student interaction. In addition, environmental data was collected, such

as number of students attending each class session, and potential extraneous factors such

as distracting loud noises or activities during class time.

Page 50: Florida State University Libraries254119/datastream/PDF/download/...one of three instructional strategies (process-oriented, product-oriented, or conventional problem solving) developed

38

The present study was conducted in a lecture hall with a maximum seating

capacity of 500 students. The layout of the room provided for student seating in a

quadrant arrangement with seating sections in both the lower tier and the upper tier of the

room. The instructor’s lectern was located at the front of the lecture hall. The lectern

consisted of a large table equipped with multi-media devices such as a personal

computer, audio and video devices, and touch screen command controls. Behind the

instructor’s podium were two large projector screens for presentation of instructional

material. The room was also equipped for wireless data transmission devices. The

technology supported two essential pedagogical aspects of the course. First, it provided

the instructor the ability to use a mobile notepad to control the instructional presentation

and activities as well as the ability to roam freely around the room in an attempt to

connect with the students. Second, all students enrolled in the course were required to use

a PRS device (personal response system) to respond to questions, quizzes, and activities

presented during the instruction.

In each of the pre-study classroom observations, the course instructor used a

variety of methods such as knowledge check questions, demonstrations, and visuals to

engage the students to verify their level of understanding of the material covered during

the class period. For example, at the conclusion of a particular topic, the instructor would

present students with a series of multiple choice questions using the large projector

screen. Students would be given approximately two minutes to respond to the question

using their electronic transmitting device (PRS) to record their response. Responses were

then aggregated by the instructional software program and displayed as a bar graph on the

screen. This mobile technology enabled the instructor to quickly assess the students level

of understanding allowing for real-time instructional adjustments.

In addition to presenting lecture content, the course instructor used a variety of

multimedia and other classroom activities to gain and maintain student engagement

throughout the course period. It is common for music to be playing at the beginning of

the course as students are entering the lecture hall. Throughout the lecture a video clips

are use to elaborate or exemplify a particular point or concept.

In addition, student participation is actively solicited for periodic demonstrations.

For example, during one class observation the instructor asked for 10 volunteers to come

Page 51: Florida State University Libraries254119/datastream/PDF/download/...one of three instructional strategies (process-oriented, product-oriented, or conventional problem solving) developed

39

to the front of the lecture hall with the instructor to demonstrate the principle of

diminishing returns using two large buckets and twenty tennis balls. The demonstration

showed that production, the movement of balls from one bucket to the other, increased as

students were added to the exercise until the number of students reached 6 students. At

this point, production significantly declined demonstrating that there is an optimal level

of resources needed to obtain maximum output.

Throughout the class period, the instructor routinely used a variety of instructional

techniques, tools, and activities to enhance the learning process. Moreover, classroom

observations revealed that the instructor attempted to construct a learning environment

that engaged students throughout the course while conveying the appropriate amount of

content.

Instructional Materials

Participants in this study were expected to learn how to apply the microeconomic

concepts of supply and demand, tax burden, tax incidences, price and quantity

equilibrium, and elasticity to solve complex economic analysis problems. To apply these

concepts, a participant’s cognitive ability to determine who bears the actual burden of tax

increase, the buyer or the seller, was predicated on their understanding of how to

calculate and interpret shifts in the supply and demand curve and the overall impact this

movement has on the price and quantity equilibrium. In analyzing the impact of elasticity

and taxes on market activity, the rules provide the necessary guidance to calculate gains

or losses in the price and quantity of a product or service, whereas the principles provide

the foundation for interpretation and application of the calculations.

The materials developed for this study consisted of a subject-specific prior

knowledge assessment, three instructional packets, and a performance assessment. The

fifteen-item subject-specific prior knowledge assessment was developed to assess

participants’ level of knowledge of the basic principles of supply and demand and

product elasticity. Similarly, a performance assessment was developed comprising eleven

questions to assess participant learning and four questions to assess transfer of knowledge

to novel domains. Both the prior knowledge and performance assessments were in a

multiple choice format requiring participants to select the correct answer from four to

five possible options provided. All participants included in the study were required to

Page 52: Florida State University Libraries254119/datastream/PDF/download/...one of three instructional strategies (process-oriented, product-oriented, or conventional problem solving) developed

40

complete both assessments. A detailed description of the subject-specific prior

knowledge and performance assessments are provided in a subsequent section titled

“Instruments”.

The instructional phase of the study was separated into two distinct segments. The

first segment was an introductory lecture covering basic terms, concepts, and general

examples of analyzing the impact of taxes on market activity. The materials used during

the lecture were the instructor’s existing powerpoint slides and activity questions. The

second instructional segment required participants to complete a print-based instructional

packet developed specifically for this study. The packet was developed in order to

provide participants the opportunity to apply the knowledge gained from the lecture.

Each participant received one of the three instructional strategies, either the conventional

problem solving, process-oriented worked example, or product-oriented worked example

strategy.

The lecture was a 30-minute instructional presentation that included an overview

of basic concepts and a general demonstration of how to analyze the impact of a tax on

market activity. The instructor reviewed the basic concepts and computational

underpinnings of a market equilibrium problem. Elements of the lecture included topics

such as the (1) laws of supply and demand, (2) major determinants of supply and

demand, (3) the law of elasticity and (4) how to determine the effect of taxes on the price

and quantity of goods and services in a competitive market. The instructor concluded the

lecture with a series of conceptual examples demonstrating how to represent tax problems

graphically by plotting the changes in the supply and demand curve.

The instructional strategy conditions were presented to participants two days after

the classroom lecture. Table 3.2 provides an overview of the structural differences

between the three instructional strategies, (1) conventional problem solving, (2) process-

oriented worked example-practice pair, and (3) product-oriented worked example-

practice pair.

The self-paced instructional materials varied between 20 to 24 typed pages based

on the type of instructional strategy. Specifically, the instructional strategy determined

the type of information presented and how. For example, a conventional problem was

comprised of a problem statement (i.e., question) and a problem goal (i.e., answer). The

Page 53: Florida State University Libraries254119/datastream/PDF/download/...one of three instructional strategies (process-oriented, product-oriented, or conventional problem solving) developed

41

participant was required to decide the most effective way to generate a solution to the

problem by recalling the appropriate concepts, calculations, and steps and processing this

information in the correct way. By design, this strategy provided minimal instructional

support requiring the participant to work out the solution steps independently.

Participants were provided space to work out the problem. Feedback on the correctness of

the solution steps was provided only in terms of the correct answer. Participants were not

prohibited from viewing the solution while working out the problem.

In contrast, the worked example strategies presented participants with the same

information as in the conventional problem strategies a problem statement, and a problem

goal. However, the worked example strategies added the solution steps necessary to solve

the problem. The degree of information provided differed among the worked example

strategies. In each worked example strategy, the example was followed by an opportunity

for the participant to complete a practice problem with no instructional support provided.

The example-practice approach was designed to allow the participant to study an example

of how to work through a problem correctly before attempting a similar problem

independently. As described in Table 3.2, the difference between the two work example

strategies was the inclusion of the underlying principles supporting each step in the

process-oriented example strategy. All problem scenarios, regardless of the problem type

were the same. The only difference was the amount and type of instructional support

provided in the scenarios. All practice problems were the same across all instructional

strategy conditions.

Page 54: Florida State University Libraries254119/datastream/PDF/download/...one of three instructional strategies (process-oriented, product-oriented, or conventional problem solving) developed

42

Table 3.2. Differences Between Instructional Strategies

Conventional

Process-Oriented

Product-Oriented

Strategy

Conventional practice problems

Process-oriented worked examples; emphasizing both procedural steps and underlying principles of the problem (strategic knowledge)

Product-oriented worked examples emphasize the procedural steps of solving the problem

Rule/Principle Principle and example problem with solution presented during the instructor-led lecture

Principle(s) provided with procedural steps and equations, definition of the concepts involved and how the steps are related to one another

Procedures provided with each required step in order to find the correct solution to the problem

Example No worked examples were provided during the treatment condition activity

4 examples provided to learners covering the burden of a tax and the effects of elasticity on supply and demand

4 examples provided to learners covering the burden of a tax and the effects of elasticity on supply and demand

Practice 7 conventional practice problems

3 practice problems

3 practice problems

Practice Feedback

Final answer provided Final answer provided Final answer provided

Variables

Independent Variables

The two independent variables used in the present study were the type of

instructional strategy and learner prior knowledge. The instructional strategy type used in

this study had three levels: (1) the conventional practice of solving problems, (2) the

process-oriented worked example strategy, and (3) the product-oriented worked example

strategy. The second independent variable, learner prior knowledge, comprised two

levels: (1) low prior knowledge, and (2) high prior knowledge.

Page 55: Florida State University Libraries254119/datastream/PDF/download/...one of three instructional strategies (process-oriented, product-oriented, or conventional problem solving) developed

43

Instructional strategy. The conventional problem solving instructional strategy

consisted of seven conventional practice problems requiring a completion time of

approximately 30-minutes (see Appendix D for the complete packet). The participants

receiving conventional practice received only the correct answer to the problem. No

additional feedback or instructional support was provided to assist participates in this

condition to generate a problem solution. For example, participants completing a

conventional problem would be required to determine the amount of tax a producer and

seller would bear based on a scenario such as the following:

The participant was required to use the information provided in the scenario and draw on

their knowledge from the lecture provided two days earlier in order to work out the

solution to the problem. Participants were asked to show their work. For each of the

seven practice problems, ample space was provided to work out the problem, including

graphing the problem as it was demonstrated during the lecture. The answer to the

problem was provided to the participant on the next page. For example, participants

working out the problem listed above were given the following answer:

The process-oriented worked example instructional strategy presented the learner

with four worked examples and three practice problems requiring a completion time of

approximately 30-minutes (see Appendix E for the complete packet). The worked

example provided the participant with the necessary procedural steps required to perform

Scenario: The seller of used cars is interested in selling a used car for any price over $6,000. However, a recent $1,000 tax has been placed on the sale of used cars. The seller would prefer to pass the tax increase on to buyers but recognizes that the higher price may result in fewer car sales. As a result, the seller and the buyer have to share the burden of the new tax. Assume the before-tax price of a used car is $7,000 resulting in the sale of 750 units. Used car prices increase by $400 which reduces demand by 250 units.

Answer: In this scenario, the government passed a $1,000 tax on the seller of used cars. Despite the imposed tax on the seller, both the seller and buyer share the actual tax burden. Each $1,000 of tax revenue paid to the government imposes a burden of $400 on the buyer in the form of higher used-car prices and a $600 burden on the seller in the form of lower net revenue received from the sale of a used car.

Page 56: Florida State University Libraries254119/datastream/PDF/download/...one of three instructional strategies (process-oriented, product-oriented, or conventional problem solving) developed

44

the analysis process as well as the underlying principle or the “why” behind the step or

task being performed. Figure 3.1 illustrates the procedural and strategic or principle-

based information provided in a process-oriented worked example.

Figure 3.1. Display of a process-oriented worked example incorporating both procedural and strategic information.

The practice problems were the same problems as those provided in the other

instructional conditions. These problems required the participant to work through a

solution without external instructional support. The feedback provided for all practice

problems consisted of the correct answer to the problem.

The product-oriented worked example instructional strategy also presented the

learner with four worked examples and three practice problems requiring a completion

time of approximately 30-minutes (see Appendix F for the complete packet). However,

this worked example strategy provided participants with only the necessary procedural

steps to solve the problem. Using the same scenario as in the previous examples, Figure

3.2 illustrates the procedural steps provided to a participant in a product-oriented worked

example.

Page 57: Florida State University Libraries254119/datastream/PDF/download/...one of three instructional strategies (process-oriented, product-oriented, or conventional problem solving) developed

45

Figure 3.2. Display of a product-oriented worked example with procedural steps only.

The practice and feedback strategy of the product-oriented worked examples is

the same as the approach used in the process-oriented strategy and conventional problem-

solving strategy.

Prior Knowledge. The variable learner prior knowledge was comprised of two

levels, (1) low and (2) high. A fifteen-question multiple choice subject-specific prior

knowledge assessment was used to assess participants level of prior knowledge. Items

were designed to assess participants knowledge proficiency in the laws governing supply

and demand, and elasticity. For each question, participants were asked to select the

correct answer from one of four or five possible answers. Participants recorded their

responses on a scantran form for scoring. The creation of prior knowledge categories was

based on the participant’s number of correct responses.

Dependent Measures

Learning. Two days after completing the two instructional segments (i.e., lecture

and instructional conditions), participants were asked to complete an achievement test as

Page 58: Florida State University Libraries254119/datastream/PDF/download/...one of three instructional strategies (process-oriented, product-oriented, or conventional problem solving) developed

46

part of an overall performance assessment to measure learning. Learning was measured

by a participant’s score on a print-based multiple choice test consisting of eleven

knowledge assessment items that were developed by the instructor with input from the

researcher. Overall learning achievement was measured based on the number of correct

responses to each assessment question (e.g., 7 out of 11 correct). Responses were

captured on a scantran form.

Transfer. Transfer performance was measured based on the results from four item

multiple choice transfer test developed by the instructor with input from the researcher.

The assessment examined whether or not participants were able to apply the relevant

schema (i.e., tax impact analysis) acquired during the instruction to a novel domain (i.e.,

labor markets and health care). Transfer was measured based on the number of correct

responses selected. Responses were captured on a scantran form.

Mental effort. Mental effort was measured using the subjective rating scale based

on a 9-point scale developed by Paas (1992), ranging from 1 “very, very low effort” to 9

“very, very high effort” (see Appendix G). Participants completed the mental effort rating

by indicating the amount of mental effort they exerted after completing the instruction

(i.e., worked examples or conventional lecture). Responses were recorded on the

instructional packet.

Learner attitude. Learner attitude was measured in terms of attention and

confidence scores on Keller’s Instructional Materials Motivation Survey (IMMS). The

survey contained 21 items, eleven of the questions relate to attention and ten of the

questions relate to confidence. Responses to the items were in the form of a five-point

Likert-type scale. The IMMS was administered at the end of the instructional lesson and

responses were captured on the instructional packet. Participants required between 5 and

10 minutes to complete the survey based on individual differences.

Pre-Treatment Condition Classroom Observations

In addition to the aforementioned quantitative measures, the researcher captured

field notes from three classroom observations in order to gain a deeper understanding of

the contextual and/or environmental factors that affect participants. The purpose of the

observations was two-fold: (1) to identify pertinent behavioral patterns of participants

Page 59: Florida State University Libraries254119/datastream/PDF/download/...one of three instructional strategies (process-oriented, product-oriented, or conventional problem solving) developed

47

while engaged in an instructional activity, and (2) to identify any events during the

instruction that may be problematic to the study outcomes.

Instruments

The primary instruments used to collect data in the present study were the prior

knowledge assessment, the mental effort rating scale, the learner attitude survey, and the

performance assessment that is the combination of an achievement test and transfer test.

Each of the instruments is described in the following sub-sections.

Subject-Specific Prior Knowledge Assessment

The levels of prior knowledge were assessed by participant responses to specific

questions from the first exam of the course. The prior knowledge assessment comprised

fifteen multiple-choice items specifically developed to determine classification as either a

low or high prior knowledge participant. The questions assessed the participants’ level of

knowledge proficiency on the relationship between product and resource markets, price

controls, supply and demand, and elasticity. Participants were asked to select the correct

answer from among either four or five choices. The assessment score was based on the

number of correct answers selected.

The prior knowledge assessment was developed by the instructor with input from

the researcher. The items selected for the assessment were generated on the basis of their

applicability to the content being examined by the study. After items are developed by

the instructor they were reviewed by the researcher for analysis and suggested revision.

The resulting items were revised on the basis of appropriateness and clarity. The final

revised assessment questions are presented in Appendix H.

Mental Effort Rating Scale

After completing the instructional activity, participants were instructed to indicate

how much mental effort they invested after completing the instructional packet on a 9-

point rating scale (Paas, 1992), ranging from 1 “very, very low effort” to 9 “very, very

high effort”.

Learning Attitude Survey

The attitude of the participants toward the instruction was measured by a 21-item

five-point Likert-type questionnaire (see Appendix I) adapted from the Instructional

Material Motivational Survey (IMMS) to measure attitude, attention, and confidence

Page 60: Florida State University Libraries254119/datastream/PDF/download/...one of three instructional strategies (process-oriented, product-oriented, or conventional problem solving) developed

48

(Keller, 1993). The IMMS instrument was developed to provide a situational measure of

participants’ motivational response to instructional materials and included in the

instructional packet that was provided to participants. The survey was developed on the

basis of Keller’s ARCS model (1987a; 1987b) and required participants to record their

degree of agreement with a variety of statements concerning their attention (e.g., “the

variety of reading passages, exercises, and illustrations helped keep my attention on the

lesson”), and confidence (e.g., “as I worked on the lesson, I was confident that I could

learn the content”).

Participants responded to these statements on a five-point Likert scale (1 = not

true, 5 = very true). Of those 21 items, eleven of the items pertained to attention and ten

pertained to confidence. Based on Cronbach’s alpha coefficient, the overall learner

attitude questionnaire had a reliability of .80. Moreover, questions relating to attention

had a reliability measure of .73 and those relating to confidence had a reliability measure

of .74.

Performance Assessment

The performance assessment comprised fifteen multiple-choice items on a print-

based assessment. The assessment comprised two parts an achievement test and a transfer

test (see Appendix J). The achievement test was developed to assess recall of general

knowledge relating to the content area. The transfer test was used to assess a participant’s

ability to apply acquired knowledge to a new domain problem.

Achievement Test. Learning achievement, defined as the ability of the participant

to apply the learning from the instruction on the performance assessment questions. The

achievement portion of the performance assessment consisted of eleven-items designed to

assess participants’ application of their knowledge of (1) supply and demand curve shifts,

(2) the impact of the tax burden on market activity, and (3) the influence of product

elasticity on the allocation of the tax burden among buyers and sellers. Participants

received one point for each correct answer selected.

Transfer Test. Transfer, defined as the ability of the participants to apply the

knowledge learned from the instruction to a novel problem. The transfer test consisted of

four-items that differed in the domain of focus. Participants received one point for each

Page 61: Florida State University Libraries254119/datastream/PDF/download/...one of three instructional strategies (process-oriented, product-oriented, or conventional problem solving) developed

49

correct answer selected. The transfer test was developed by the instructor with input from

the researcher.

Procedure

The procedure for the present study involved completion of three phases: (1) a

prior knowledge assessment to determine participants level of prior knowledge, (2) a

common lecture presented two days prior to the administration of the instructional

conditions, the administration of instructional strategy conditions via a print-based

instructional packet, and (3) a performance assessment developed to assess participant

knowledge recall and transfer ability. These phases of the procedure are described below.

Phase One: Prior Knowledge Assessment

In this phase of the study, participants completed a 25-question exam as part of

their normal course requirement. Fifteen questions of the exam were specifically

developed to assess a participant’s prior knowledge level (i.e., low or high) for the

present study. In this study, a participant’s prior knowledge level was determined by

calculating the number of questions answered correctly. Figure 3.3 illustrates how

participants were categorized by prior knowledge.

Phase Two: Instructional Segments

Lecture. One week after the prior knowledge assessment was administered,

participants were presented a lecture titled, “Supply and Demand: Application and

Extensions”. The 30-minute lecture introduced participants to (1) the relationship

between resource and product markets, (2) analyzing the impact of government policy on

market activity, and (3) the effects of tax incident and elasticity on market activity

between buyers and sellers. The presentation covered general concepts and principles

associated with the lecture topics.

The instructor used supportive instructional techniques to improve participant

understanding of the content. Conceptual examples were shown as part of the lecture to

demonstrate how to plot shifts in the supply and demand curve resulting from the

imposition of taxes on goods and services. Due to the relationship among three critical

elements prices, taxes, and elasticity, three examples were presented showing the impact

of a tax increase on the cost of a product and based on the elasticity of the product (i.e.,

non-elastic, inelastic, and elastic). To encourage student participation in the instructional

Page 62: Florida State University Libraries254119/datastream/PDF/download/...one of three instructional strategies (process-oriented, product-oriented, or conventional problem solving) developed

50

phase of the study, prior to the end of class the course instructor announced that

participants attending class and completing the instructional activity during the next class

would receive double class participation points for that day.

Figure 3.3. Illustration of participant categorization into instructional strategy and prior knowledge groups

Instructional Strategy. Two days after the lecture, students who agreed to

participate in the instructional condition phase of the study were asked to complete one of

the three possible versions of a self-paced instructional packet (i.e., instructional

condition). To ensure random assignment of the instructional conditions, three graduate

students and the researcher were each stationed at the one of the four entry points to the

classroom with a pre-sorted mixture of instructional packets. As participants entered the

lecture hall, they were randomly handed one of three instructional packets (i.e.,

conventional problem solving, process-oriented, and product-oriented).

After the materials were distributed, the researcher spent the first 10-minutes of

the course period to provide instructions for the activity. The researcher stated that the

activity was not a test and explained that the purpose of the activity was to evaluate the

effectiveness of different instructional strategies (see Activity Leaders Guide in Appendix

K). Participants were also informed that since data was being collected, if they chose to

Page 63: Florida State University Libraries254119/datastream/PDF/download/...one of three instructional strategies (process-oriented, product-oriented, or conventional problem solving) developed

51

participate, they would need to read and sign the Research Study Participant Consent

form included in the instructional packet.

Instructions described the tasks participants were required to complete during the

activity (i.e., answer all questions, complete the mental effort rating and the instructional

materials survey). Specific attention was given to describing the particular sections of the

instructional packet. Participants were informed that the activity was comprised of two

sections, one instructional and one evaluation. The instructional section contained either a

series of conventional practice-problems or a series of worked out examples and practice

problem-pairs to solve. The difference in the strategy was the amount of instructional

assistance provided to participants as they worked through the problems. The

distinguishing features of each instructional strategy are described below.

Participants receiving the process-oriented worked example strategy received four

worked examples and three practice problems to complete. Participants were provided

with a worked out example followed immediately by a practice problem similar to the

worked example. The process-oriented example strategy included information that

emphasized both the procedural steps necessary to solve the problem as well as the

underlying principles of the problem.

Participants receiving the product-oriented worked example instructional strategy

also received four worked examples and three practice problems to complete. The

worked out example was followed by a practice- problem similar to the worked example

previously reviewed. The product-oriented example strategy differed from the process-

oriented strategy in that it only provided the necessary procedural steps required to solve

the problem. No principle information was presented in these examples.

In both instructional strategies, each worked example was followed by a practice-

problem to solve. Practice-problems were presented without any instructional support.

Feedback (i.e., the correct answer) to the practice-problem was available on the next

page.

Participants receiving the conventional problem solving instructional strategy

were presented with seven practice-problems to solve. The practice-problems were

presented one at a time and required the participants to work out the problem in the space

provided. As in the worked example strategies, the feedback (i.e., the correct answer) to

Page 64: Florida State University Libraries254119/datastream/PDF/download/...one of three instructional strategies (process-oriented, product-oriented, or conventional problem solving) developed

52

the practice-problem was available on the next page. No additional elaboration or

informational cues about how to solve the problem was provided. Participants were

instructed they would have 40-minutes to complete both sections of the activity. Figure

3.4 provides an overview of the procedures used in the study.

The evaluation section required participants to (1) rate the amount of mental effort

they exerted while completing the problems and (2) evaluate the motivational or

attitudinal influence of the instructional materials used in the activity. In order to ensure

the same information covered during the instructions was provided to participants

arriving late an overhead with the activity instructions was displayed during the session

(see Appendix L for Overhead Instructions).

Phase Three: Performance Assessment

The performance assessment was conducted four days after the administration of

the instructional conditions. Four days after participating in the instructional phase of the

study, participants were asked to complete a fifteen-item multiple-choice assessment. The

performance assessment comprised two parts, (1) an eleven-item achievement test and (2)

a four-item transfer test. The performance assessment procedures were identical for all

participants in the present study. Participants were given a maximum of 50-minutes to

complete the assessment. Responses to the assessment were captured on a scantran form.

Page 65: Florida State University Libraries254119/datastream/PDF/download/...one of three instructional strategies (process-oriented, product-oriented, or conventional problem solving) developed

53

Phase I

Prior Knowledge Assessment 50 Minutes

One Week Before Instructional Conditions

Fifteen multiple choice questions

Phase II Instructional Segment One: Lecture 20 Minute

Two Days Before Instructional Conditions

Lecture Topic: Application of Supply and Demand: The Impact of a Tax o How does a tax affect the market? o What determines the distribution of the tax burden between buyers and

sellers?

Instructional Segment Two: Instructional Strategy Conditions, Mental Effort Rating, and Attitude Survey 50 Minutes

Process-Oriented Example

Process-oriented worked examples emphasize both procedural steps and underlying principles of the problem (strategic knowledge)

Four worked examples, three practice problems

Product-Oriented Example

Product-oriented worked examples emphasize the procedural steps of solving the problem

Four worked examples, three practice problems

Conventional Problem-Solving

Seven conventional practice problems presented to the learners to solve

Feedback consists of answers to practice problems provided to the learners

1. Rating of Mental Effort on a 9-Item Scale 2. 21-question Attitude Survey measuring attention and confidence

Phase III Performance Assessment: Achievement Test and Transfer Test 50 minutes

Four Days after Instructional Conditions

Eleven Achievement test questions

Four Transfer test questions

Figure 3.4. Overview of the Procedures.

Page 66: Florida State University Libraries254119/datastream/PDF/download/...one of three instructional strategies (process-oriented, product-oriented, or conventional problem solving) developed

54

Research Design and Data Analysis

The research design for the present study was quasi-experimental in nature due to

the assignment of intact groups to treatment conditions and a three (instructional strategy)

x two (level of learner prior knowledge) factorial design. Table 3.3 provides an overview

of the study’s research design.

Table 3.3. Research Design of Instructional Strategy and Prior Knowledge

Learner Prior Knowledge

Instructional

Strategy

Low Prior Knowledge (LPK)

High Prior Knowledge (HPK)

Process-Oriented LPK/ Process-Oriented HPK/ Process-Oriented

Product-Oriented LPK/ Product-Oriented HPK/ Product-Oriented

Conventional LPK/ Conventional HPK/ Conventional

To test the proposed hypotheses (see Chapter 2), quantitative data was collected

on each of the following outcome measures: (1) learning, (2) transfer, (3) mental effort,

and (4) learner attitude. A detailed description of the data analyses that were conducted

for the present study appears in the next chapter.

Page 67: Florida State University Libraries254119/datastream/PDF/download/...one of three instructional strategies (process-oriented, product-oriented, or conventional problem solving) developed

55

CHAPTER 4

RESULTS

The results of this study are presented in three sections: (1) preliminary data

analyses, (2) primary data analyses, and a (3) secondary data analysis. The first section of

this chapter reports the tests of assumptions that were required for the statistical tests used

to test hypotheses and an item analysis of the achievement and transfer test items used in

the study. The second section presents an analysis of results for each of the hypotheses

tested. The final section presents the results of secondary analyses inspired by the

findings of the primary analysis.

Preliminary Analysis

Prior to conducting the primary analysis of the hypotheses a preliminary analysis

was performed on the tests of assumptions associated with the statistical tests used for the

analysis. In addition, an item analysis was conducted on the performance assessment. In

general, tests of assumptions conducted for analysis of variance (ANOVA) with

instructional conditions indicated that the assumptions were not violated beyond the

degree to which the statistical tests were sufficiently robust to handle minor to moderate

variations.

Tests of Assumptions

Preliminary data analyses were conducted to test the assumptions associated with

the various parametric statistical tests that were performed. These tests of assumptions

included: (1) normal distribution assumption, (2) independence assumption, (3) interval

scale assumption, and (4) homogeneity of variance assumption. The test results for each

assumption are discussed next.

Normal distribution assumption. In parametric tests it is assumed that the data

from the population are normally distributed. To detect violations of this assumption

visual inspection of graphical representations of the data (i.e., histogram and Q-Q plot)

and an examination of formal statistical analyses were conducted for each dependent

variable and instructional condition. The Kolmogorov-Smirnov normality test was used

to analyze the distribution of the dependent variable measures. Results indicated that

Page 68: Florida State University Libraries254119/datastream/PDF/download/...one of three instructional strategies (process-oriented, product-oriented, or conventional problem solving) developed

56

overall scores showed a non-normal distribution of the instructional conditions for all the

dependent variables with the exception of Learner Attitude and the attitude sub-scale of

attention. The results likely reflect the negative skewness found on achievement and

transfer scores as well as the positively skewed scores found on mental effort and learner

attitude scores. For example, examination of the learner attitude results indicated a

normal distribution D(393) =.033, p ≥.200 (see Table 4.1). Moreover, the attention sub-

scale of the learner attitude scores also indicated a normal distribution for each

instructional condition. The Kolmogorov-Smirnov test for attention scores for the

process-oriented strategy was D(135)=.069, p≥.200, the product-oriented strategy

D(140)=.064, p≥.200, and for the conventional strategy D(118)=.068, p≥.200. The

confidence sub-scale of Learner Attitude scores for the process-oriented strategy was also

not significant indicating a normal distribution, D(135)=.073, p≥.074.

Table 4.1. Tests of Normality of Instructional Strategy Conditions

Dependent Variable Instructional Strategy Kolmogorov-Smirnov

Statistic df Sig.

Achievement Test Scores Process .182 135 .000

Product .122 140 .000

Conventional .144 118 .000

Mental Effort Rating Process .131 135 .000

Product .126 140 .000

Conventional .140 118 .000

Transfer Test Scores Process .199 135 .000

Product .250 140 .000

Conventional .247 118 .000

Learner Attitude Process .057 135 .200(*)

Product .065 140 .200(*)

Conventional .062 118 .200(*)

* This is a lower bound of the true significance.

Table 4.2 shows similar results from examining normality based on instructional

conditions and segregated by participant prior knowledge. Despite the findings that some

of the dependent measures were not normally distributed, the normal distribution

Page 69: Florida State University Libraries254119/datastream/PDF/download/...one of three instructional strategies (process-oriented, product-oriented, or conventional problem solving) developed

57

assumption was regarded to be satisfied since ANOVA, the primary statistical technique

utilized to analyze the data in the present study, is robust enough to withstand minor to

moderate violations of this assumption.

Table 4.2. Tests of Normality of Instructional Strategy and Prior Knowledge

Prior Knowledge Category

Instructional Strategy

Kolmogorov-Smirnov

Statistic df

Sig. Low Achievement Test Scores Process .157 56 .002

Product .141 36 .067

Conventional .182 49 .000

Mental Effort Rating Process .130 56 .019

Product .183 36 .004

Conventional .133 49 .029

Transfer Test Scores Process .221 56 .000

Product .327 36 .000

Conventional .232 49 .000

Learner Attitude Process .101 56 .200(*)

Product .060 36 .200(*)

Conventional .081 49 .200(*)

High Achievement Test Scores Process .217 57 .000

Product .168 68 .000

Conventional .171 43 .003

Mental Effort Rating Process .150 57 .003

Product .148 68 .001

Conventional .183 43 .001

Transfer Test Scores Process .218 57 .000

Product .242 68 .000

Conventional .244 43 .000

Learner Attitude Process .101 57 .200(*)

Product .101 68 .084

Conventional .074 43 .200(*)

* This is a lower bound of the true significance.

Independence assumption. This assumption requires that the observation or score

of a participant must not be influenced by the observation or score of another participant

Page 70: Florida State University Libraries254119/datastream/PDF/download/...one of three instructional strategies (process-oriented, product-oriented, or conventional problem solving) developed

58

(Field, 2005). To satisfy the assumption of independence, a logical study design was

conducted to ensure that all observations and scores were independent. All attempts were

made to control the interactions among participants. Moreover, individual learning tasks

were assigned to each participant particular to their instructional strategy. Consequently,

the assumption of independence was satisfied.

Interval-scale assumption. The dependent variables in the present study were

measured at the interval level using a continuous scale. As a result, the interval-scale

assumption was satisfied.

Homogeneity of variance assumption. The Levene’s test was conducted to test the

assumption of homogeneity of variance for each dependent variable to determine whether

or not the variances in the groups were equal. The results of the test indicated that the

assumption of homogeneity for each dependent variable was not violated (see Table 4.3).

Table 4.3.Test of Homogeneity of Variance

Dependent Variable Levene Statistic df1 df2 Sig.

Achievement Test Scores 2.508 2 306 .083

Mental Effort Rating 2.433 2 306 .089

Transfer Test Scores 1.526 2 306 .219

Learner Attitude .024 2 306 .976

Item Analysis

An item analysis was performed on both the achievement and transfer portions of

the performance assessment. Results are presented in Table 4.4. The difficulty index for

the assessment ranged from .67 to .74. The items with the lowest ratings on the difficulty

index were transfer question number two and four with ratings of .67 and .69

respectively. The data reflected on the difficulty index suggests that most of the

participants had little difficulty correctly answering the assessment questions. The narrow

range of index percentages seems to indicate that the knowledge of participants’

Page 71: Florida State University Libraries254119/datastream/PDF/download/...one of three instructional strategies (process-oriented, product-oriented, or conventional problem solving) developed

59

exceeded the complexity of the performance assessment items. In addition, the limited

number of achievement and transfer items that comprised the performance assessment are

suggested to have influenced the Cronbach alpha coefficients (Fields, 2005; Cortina,

1993) that are presented in this section. The next chapter will discuss in more detail the

impact of these issues on the study result.

In addition to analyzing item difficulty, reliability measures were produced to

assess the internal consistency of the performance assessment items. The internal

consistency of the fifteen-item assessment was calculated from the performance

assessment scores as a Cronbach’s alpha coefficient of .416. A separate analysis was

conducted for each subscale (i.e., achievement test and transfer test). The internal

consistency for the eleven-item achievement test scores had a Cronbach’s alpha

coefficient of .340. The reliability analysis for the four-item transfer test had a

Cronbach’s alpha coefficient of .023.

Table 4.4. Performance Assessment statistics for Individual Test Items

Test Item

Difficulty Index

Scale Mean if

Item Deleted

Scale Variance if

Item Deleted

Corrected Item-Total Correlation

Squared Multiple

Correlation

Cronbach's Alpha if Item

Deleted

Achievement 1 .72 9.94 4.514 .175 .066 .389

Achievement 2 .70 9.96 4.703 .070 .049 .418

Achievement 3 .70 9.96 4.581 .131 .051 .401

Achievement 4 .74 9.92 4.590 .142 .064 .398

Achievement 5 .72 9.94 4.566 .148 .073 .396

Achievement 6 .70 9.96 4.571 .138 .037 .399

Achievement 7 .73 9.93 4.548 .161 .062 .393

Achievement 8 .72 9.94 4.518 .172 .051 .389

Achievement 9 .72 9.94 4.651 .102 .034 .409

Achievement 10 .71 9.95 4.572 .141 .035 .398

Achievement 11 .72 9.94 4.561 .151 .057 .395

Transfer 1 .70 9.96 4.569 .138 .047 .399

Transfer 2 .67 9.99 4.548 .141 .076 .398

Transfer 3 .71 9.95 4.638 .106 .041 .408

Transfer 4 .69 9.97 4.620 .109 .051 .407

Page 72: Florida State University Libraries254119/datastream/PDF/download/...one of three instructional strategies (process-oriented, product-oriented, or conventional problem solving) developed

60

In addition to the achievement and transfer test item analysis, two subscale

analyses were conducted. The focus of the performance assessment was to identify

general knowledge and transfer ability on student understanding of elasticity and the

impact of taxes on market behavior. Subscale items examining elasticity were

achievement items 1-4 and transfer items 6 and 7. The internal consistency for the six-

item subscale had a Cronbach’s alpha coefficient of .182. The impact of taxes on market

activity was examined by achievement items 6, 8-11 and transfer items 3 and 4. The

Cronbach’s alpha coefficient for the seven-item subscale was .242.

Primary Data Analysis

Primary data analyses were conducted to test the hypotheses derived from the four

research questions. For the present study, analysis of variance (ANOVA) was used to

examine the effect of (1) instructional strategy on learning and transfer performance, (2)

the interaction between instructional strategy and prior knowledge level on learning and

transfer, (3) the interaction of instructional strategy and prior knowledge level on mental

effort, and (4) the interaction of a instructional strategy and prior knowledge on learner

attitude. A significance level of .05 was used for the analyses reported in the present

study. The results are presented below under each corresponding hypotheses.

Descriptions of Groups

The means and standard deviations for the instructional strategies on the

dependent measures of learning, transfer, mental effort, and attitude are presented below

in Table 4.5. As discussed in the previous chapter, the present study examined the effect

of prior knowledge on the dependent measures. Hence, prior knowledge categories (i.e.,

low prior knowledge and high prior knowledge) were created based on the number of

items correctly answered by a participant on the fifteen-item prior knowledge assessment.

The mean score of the assessment was 12.01 (SD=1.928). Based on work by Mahan

(2007), the original research design called for the prior knowledge assessment mean

score to be used as the cut point to classify participant prior knowledge. However, due to

the clustered nature of prior knowledge scores in the upper range of scores, it was

determined prior to running any outcome analyses to use scores falling in the 25th and

75th percentile range as cut points for determining a participant’s prior knowledge. Table

4.6 shows the prior knowledge scores and the associated quartile.

Page 73: Florida State University Libraries254119/datastream/PDF/download/...one of three instructional strategies (process-oriented, product-oriented, or conventional problem solving) developed

61

Table 4.5. Descriptive Statistics of Instructional Strategy on Dependent Measures

Dependent Measures

Achievement Test Scores Transfer Scores Mental Effort Rating Learner Attitude

Instructional Strategy M SD N M SD N M SD N M SD N

Process 7.74 1.74 142 2.70 0.95 142 3.78 1.95 143 3.03 .50 136

Product 8.03 1.87 143 2.81 0.92 143 4.13 1.92 146 3.01 .50 143

Conventional 7.8 2.04 123 2.78 0.95 123 4.06 2.10 126 3.06 .51 119

Total 7.86 1.88 408 2.76 0.94 408 3.99 1.99 415 3.03 .51 398

Note: Ranges for scores on dependent measures: Achievement test score 1 to 11; Transfer test score 1 to 4;

Mental effort rating 1 to 9; Learner attitude ratings 1 to 5.

Table 4.6. Descriptive Statistics for Prior Knowledge Assessment Scores

Participants with a score equal to or less than eleven were classified as low prior

knowledge whereas high prior knowledge participants had scores equal to or greater than

thirteen. Participants with scores equal to twelve (n=87) were excluded from use in

analyses of prior knowledge. Table 4.7 shows the number of participants assigned to a

particular instructional strategy that were categorized as either low or high prior

knowledge.

Finally, the means and standard deviations are presented in Table 4.8 for each

instructional strategy condition grouped by prior knowledge level categories on the

dependent measures of learning, transfer, mental effort, and attitude.

Number of Participants 415 Mean Prior Knowledge Assessment Score 12.01

Standard Error of Mean .095

Standard Deviation 1.98

Minimum Prior Knowledge Score 4

Maximum Prior Knowledge Score 15

Percentiles of Prior Knowledge Scores 25 11.00

50 12.00

75 13.00

Page 74: Florida State University Libraries254119/datastream/PDF/download/...one of three instructional strategies (process-oriented, product-oriented, or conventional problem solving) developed

62

Table 4.7. Prior Knowledge Category Frequencies by Instructional Strategy

Prior Knowledge Process Product Conventional Total

Low (score of ≤ 11) 58 37 51 146

High (score of ≥ 13) 61 72 49 182

Total 119 109 100 328

Excluded Value 24 37 26 87

Total 143 146 126 415

Table 4.8. Descriptive Statistics of Prior Knowledge Levels on Dependent Measures

Dependent Measures

Achievement Test Scores Transfer Scores Mental Effort Rating Learner Attitude

Instructional Strategy/

Prior Knowledge

Level M SD N M SD N M SD N M SD N

Process

Low 7.47 1.79 57 2.49 0.97 57 4.09 2.11 58 2.93 .50 57

High 8.21 1.53 61 2.98 0.87 61 3.69 1.87 61 3.05 .49 57

Product

Low 7.59 1.61 37 2.76 0.6 37 4.49 1.82 37 2.85 .52 36

High 8.16 2.18 70 2.86 0.98 70 3.74 1.72 72 3.13 .46 70

Conventional

Low 7.36 2.07 50 2.54 0.93 50 4.27 2.14 51 2.94 .63 49

High 8.47 2.09 47 2.96 0.96 47 3.92 2.22 49 3.10 .40 44

Total

Low 7.47 1.84 144 2.58 0.87 144 4.25 2.04 146 2.91 .55 142

High 8.26 1.95 178 2.93 0.93 178 3.77 1.90 182 3.10 .45 171

Note: Ranges for scores on dependent measures: Achievement test score 1 to 11; Transfer test score 1 to 4;

Mental effort rating 1 to 9; Learner attitude ratings 1 to 5.

Page 75: Florida State University Libraries254119/datastream/PDF/download/...one of three instructional strategies (process-oriented, product-oriented, or conventional problem solving) developed

63

In addition to traditional demographic information and separate from the subject-

specific prior knowledge assessment, each participant’s previous experience of

economics course work was also collected (see Appendix M for the Economic

Experience Survey). The collection of previous experience was sought to provide

additional explanatory data relating to the possibility of some participants having

partially formed basic schemas (Pollock, Chandler, & Sweller, 2002) of the content prior

to exposure to the instruction. This is an important factor to consider considering the

existence of partial schema may not have been apparent from prior knowledge

assessment scores but could have affected dependent outcomes. To collect this

information, a three-question in-class survey was conducted at the beginning of the term.

Participants were ask to indicate whether or not they had (1) completed a high school

economics course, (2) completed a college level introductory economics course, and (3)

completed a college level economics course with the instructor in the present study.

Three hundred and forty participants responded to the question asking if

participants had previously taken high school economics. Table 4.9 shows that more than

eighty-five percent (n=291) of participants indicated they had previously taken a high

school economics course. Moreover, more than fourteen percent (n=49) indicated they

had not taken economics in high school.

Table 4.9. Frequency of Participants who Completed High School Economics

Frequency Percent

Valid Yes 291 85.6

No 49 14.4

Total 340 100.0

Missing 75

Total 415

Table 4.10 shows that forty-three percent (n=147) of the participants responded that they

had previously taken a college-level introductory economics course whereas fifty-seven

percent (n=195) indicated they had not. Lastly, Table 4.11 shows that of three hundred

Page 76: Florida State University Libraries254119/datastream/PDF/download/...one of three instructional strategies (process-oriented, product-oriented, or conventional problem solving) developed

64

and fifty-one respondents, almost eighteen percent (n=62) indicated they had previously

taken a college-level economics course with the instructor of the course.

Table 4.10. Frequency of Participants who Completed College Introductory Economics

Frequency Percent

Valid Yes 147 43.0

No 195 57.0

Total 342 100.0

Missing 73

Total 415

Table 4.11. Frequency of Participants who Completed a College-Level Economics Course with this Course Instructor

Frequency Percent

Valid Yes 62 17.7

No 288 82.1

Total 351 100.0

Missing 64

Total 415

Tests of Hypotheses

Learning. The first hypothesis predicted that participants completing a worked

example strategy (i.e., process-oriented or product-oriented) would score higher on the

achievement test than participants completing a conventional problem solving strategy.

Results did not support the hypothesis. Mean achievement scores for the instructional

strategy conditions showed that participants in the product-oriented strategy had higher

scores (M=8.03, SD=1.869) than participants in either the process-oriented strategy

(M=7.74, SD=1.737) or the conventional strategy (M=7.8, SD=2.036). As evidenced by

the results, participants completing the two strategies providing the least amount of

instructional support (i.e., product-oriented and conventional problem solving strategies)

had slightly higher scores than participants completing the process-oriented strategy.

Page 77: Florida State University Libraries254119/datastream/PDF/download/...one of three instructional strategies (process-oriented, product-oriented, or conventional problem solving) developed

65

Contrary to expectations, a one-way ANOVA revealed no significant difference, F(2,

405)= .935, p=.393, ώ²=.001 (see Table 4.12) in achievement scores between the three

instructional strategies.

Table 4.12. One-Way Analysis of Variance for Achievement Test Scores

Sum of Squares df Mean Square F Sig.

ώ²

Between Groups 6.589 2 3.294 .935

.393

.001

Within Groups 1427.166 405 3.524

Total

1433.755

407

In addition, a planned contrast was conducted on mean achievement scores to

examine differences between worked example strategies and conventional problem

solving. Results showed no difference, t(405)=.429, p=.668, between worked example

strategies and the conventional problem solving strategy.

The second hypothesis examined the interaction effect of prior knowledge and

instructional strategy on achievement scores. The hypothesis was not supported. The first

part of the hypothesis predicted that low prior knowledge participants completing a

process-oriented worked example strategy would score higher on the achievement test

than low prior knowledge participants completing either a product-oriented worked

example strategy or a conventional problem solving strategy. In contrast, the second part

of the hypothesis predicted that participants with high prior knowledge who were

presented with process-oriented worked examples would demonstrate no significant

difference in achievement test scores than high prior knowledge participants presented

with product-oriented worked examples or conventional problem solving strategy.

To test this hypothesis, achievement test scores were analyzed using a two-way

ANOVA. The interaction effect of instructional strategy and prior knowledge level was

not significant, F(2, 316)=.517, p=.597, ώ²=.003 (see Table 4.13). This was counter to the

expectation that low prior knowledge participants would score higher than low prior

knowledge participants in other strategy conditions (see Table 4.8 for mean scores). In

Page 78: Florida State University Libraries254119/datastream/PDF/download/...one of three instructional strategies (process-oriented, product-oriented, or conventional problem solving) developed

66

addition, the results failed to reject the null hypothesis of no difference in achievement

test scores. Although no difference in achievement scores was revealed across

instructional strategies for high prior knowledge participants, the failure to reject the null

hypothesis does not provide support of the hypothesis. As a result, the second part of the

hypothesis was not supported. However, analysis of the mean scores did reveal that high

prior knowledge participants in the conventional problem solving strategy group

demonstrated slightly higher mean achievement scores, possibly indicating the presence

of expertise reversal effect.

Table 4.13. Two-Way Analysis of Variance for Instructional Strategy and Prior Knowledge level on Achievement Test Scores

Source Sum of Squares df

Mean Square F Sig.

ώ²

Instructional Strategy

.265 2 .133 .036 .964

.01

Prior Knowledge Level

49.846 1 49.846 13.675 .000

.07

Instructional Strategy X Prior Knowledge Level

3.772 2 1.886 .517 .597

.003

Error 1151.853 316 3.645

R Squared = .045 (Adjusted R Squared = .030)

An analysis of the main effect of prior knowledge on achievement scores showed

a significant difference on achievement scores, F(1, 316)=13.675, p<.000, ώ²=.07

(medium association). Consequently, a simple effect analysis was performed to further

examine the effect of prior knowledge level on individual levels of each instructional

strategy condition on achievement scores. The results in Table 4.14 show a significant

difference in mean achievement scores between participants with high-levels and low-

levels of prior knowledge in both the process-oriented strategy, F(1, 318)=4.42, p=.036,

ώ²=.02 and the conventional problem solving strategy, F(1, 318)=8.18, p=.005, ώ²=.04.

Page 79: Florida State University Libraries254119/datastream/PDF/download/...one of three instructional strategies (process-oriented, product-oriented, or conventional problem solving) developed

67

Table 4.14. Effect of Prior Knowledge Level on Transfer Test Scores within Instructional Strategy Groups

Prior Knowledge Level within Specified Instructional Strategy

Sum of Squares

df

Mean Square

F

Sig.

ώ² Process

16.03 1 16.03 4.42 .036 .02

Product

8.20 1 8.20 2.26 .133 .01

Conventional 29.64 1 29.64 8.18 .005 .04

R Squared = .045 (Adjusted R Squared = .030)

Transfer Test. The third hypothesis predicted that low prior knowledge

participants completing a process-oriented worked example strategy would score higher

on transfer test performance than low prior knowledge participants completing either a

product-oriented worked example or a conventional problem-solving strategy. In

contrast, high prior knowledge participants completing a process-oriented worked

example strategy would demonstrate no significant difference on transfer test scores than

high prior knowledge participants completing either product-oriented worked examples

or the conventional problem-solving strategy on a transfer test. Findings did not support

the stated hypothesis.

To test this hypothesis a two-way ANOVA was conducted to analyze the

interaction effect of instructional strategy and prior knowledge on transfer test scores.

Results (see Table 4.15) show that instructional strategy had no significant main effect on

transfer test scores, F(2, 316)=.172, p=.842, ώ²=.001. Moreover, results showed that the

interaction effect of instructional strategy and prior knowledge was not significant,

F(2,316)=1.1341, p=.263, ώ²≤.001. This finding was contrary to the predicted outcome

that low prior knowledge participants receiving the process-oriented strategy would score

higher than participants in either the product-oriented or conventional problem solving

conditions.

Page 80: Florida State University Libraries254119/datastream/PDF/download/...one of three instructional strategies (process-oriented, product-oriented, or conventional problem solving) developed

68

Table 4.15. Two-Way Analysis of Variance of Transfer Test Scores

Source Sum of Squares df

Mean Square F Sig.

ώ²

Instructional Strategy

.284 2 .142 .172 .842

.001

Prior Knowledge Level

8.758 1 8.758 10.606 .001

.01

Instructional Strategy X Prior Knowledge Level

2.215

2

1.108

1.341

.263

.004

Error

260.946 316 .826

R Squared = .044 (Adjusted R Squared = .029)

Similar to the achievement score findings, the results failed to reject the null

hypothesis of no difference in transfer test scores across instructional strategies.

Consequently, the second part of the hypothesis was not supported. However, comparable

to the findings on achievement scores, further analysis of the mean scores did reveal that

high prior knowledge participants in the conventional problem solving strategy group

demonstrated slightly higher mean transfer test scores, possibly indicating the presence of

expertise reversal effect.

ANOVA results indicated that a participant’s level of prior knowledge was a

significant factor in transfer test scores. Consequently, a further examination of the effect

of participant prior knowledge level on transfer scores within each instructional strategy

was conducted using a simple effect analysis. Table 4.16 shows the results of the analysis

of prior knowledge levels within each instructional strategy. Specifically, prior

knowledge showed the strongest affect within the process-oriented strategy

F(1,318)=8.64, p=.004, ώ²=.01 and the conventional problem solving strategy,

F(1,318)=5.16, p=.024, ώ²=.001. Similar to the results for achievement, the findings for

transfer test performance may indicate that participant domain knowledge was at a level

high enough to render the additional support provided in the worked example conditions

nominal. Specifically, mean transfer scores across all treatment groups were virtually

Page 81: Florida State University Libraries254119/datastream/PDF/download/...one of three instructional strategies (process-oriented, product-oriented, or conventional problem solving) developed

69

identical indicating that participants were able to obtain the same level of domain

understanding regardless of the instructional condition.

Table 4.16. Effect of Prior Knowledge Level on Transfer Test Scores within Instructional Strategy Groups

Prior Knowledge Level within Specified Instructional Strategy

Sum of Squares

df

Mean Square

F

Sig.

ώ² Process

7.10 1 7.10 8.64 .004 .01

Product

.42 1 .42 .51 .474 .001

Conventional 4.24 1 4.24 5.16 .024 .01

R Squared = .043 (Adjusted R Squared = .034)

Mental Effort. The fourth hypothesis predicted that participants presented with a

worked example strategy (i.e., process or product-oriented) would report lower mental

effort ratings than participants presented with a conventional problem-solving strategy. In

addition, hypothesis five predicted that participants presented with a process-oriented

worked example strategy would report higher mental effort ratings than participants

presented a product-oriented worked example strategy. Neither hypothesis was support.

Mean scores of mental effort among the three instructional strategy conditions

showed that participants receiving the product-oriented (M=4.13, SD=1.924) strategy

reported exerting higher levels of mental effort during the instructional activity than did

either conventional problem solving (M=4.06, SD=2.097) or process-oriented (M=3.78,

SD=1.954) strategy participants (see Table 4.17). A one-way analysis of variance showed

that instructional strategy had no effect on the degree of mental effort exerted by

participants during the instructional activity, F(2, 412)=1.222, p=.296, ώ²=.002 (see

Table 4.18).

Page 82: Florida State University Libraries254119/datastream/PDF/download/...one of three instructional strategies (process-oriented, product-oriented, or conventional problem solving) developed

70

Table 4.17. Descriptive Statistics for Instructional Strategy Mental Effort Ratings

N Mean Std.

Deviation Std.

Error

95% Confidence Interval for Mean

Min Max Lower Bound

Upper Bound

Process 143 3.78 1.954 .163 3.46 4.11 1 9

Product 146 4.13 1.924 .159 3.82 4.44 1 9

Conventional 126 4.06 2.097 .187 3.69 4.43 1 9

Total 415 3.99 1.989 .098 3.80 4.18 1 9

Planned contrasts were run on the mental effort ratings of participants receiving

one of the worked example strategies (i.e., process-oriented or product-oriented) versus

those with a conventional problem solving strategy, t(412)=.503, p=.615. Results

indicated no significant difference. Furthermore, results from a planned contrast

comparing the mental effort ratings of participants receiving a process-oriented strategy

with participants receiving a product-oriented strategy indicated there was no significant

difference in mental effort rating, t(412)=1.483, p=.139.

Table 4.18. One-way Analysis of Variance for Mental Effort Ratings

Sum of Squares df

Mean Square F Sig.

ώ²

Between Groups

9.662 2 4.831 1.222

.296 .002

Within Groups

1628.299 412 3.952

Total

1637.961 414

Learner Attitude. Learner attitude, specifically the individual subscales of

confidence and attention, towards the instructional materials were examined in the

present study. Attitude was measured based on subjective ratings on a five-point Likert-

type scale ranging from 1 (not true) to 5 (very true) on a twenty-one item questionnaire

adapted from the Instructional Materials Motivation Survey (Keller, 1993). Hypothesis

six predicted that participants receiving a worked example strategy (i.e., process or

product-oriented) would report higher attitude scores on attention and confidence than

Page 83: Florida State University Libraries254119/datastream/PDF/download/...one of three instructional strategies (process-oriented, product-oriented, or conventional problem solving) developed

71

participants presented with conventional problem solving. The hypothesis was not

supported.

To test participant attitude toward the instruction, a one-way MANOVA was

performed to analyze the main effects of instructional strategy on overall learner attitude,

and the attitude sub-scales of confidence and attention. In addition, a follow-up two-

factor MANOVA was conducted to examine the main effect of participant prior

knowledge level on attitude as well as any interaction effects of instructional strategy and

prior knowledge level. Table 4.19 shows the means and standard deviations of learner

attitude, including sub-scales, by instructional condition. The one-way MANOVA

indicated no difference in attitude toward the instructional material based on instructional

strategy, Wilks’ Lambda=.991, F(4, 395)=.869, p=.482, ŋ²=.004. The one-way univariate

ANOVA results, F(2, 395)=.373, p=.689, ŋ²=.002, showed that attitude ratings were

similar between instructional strategies.

The sub-scales of confidence and attention were analyzed using univariate test

statistics. Mean rating scores (see Table 4.19) revealed that the conventional instructional

strategy had higher confidence scores (M=3.52, SD=.67) than either the process strategy

(M=3.42, SD=.62) or the product strategy (M=3.40, SD=.67). Attention mean rating

scores showed that worked example strategies (process-oriented M=2.67, SD=.53;

product-oriented M=2.66, SD=.59) produced slightly higher scores than the conventional

problem solving strategy (M=2.65, SD=.60).

Overall, attitude mean scores, the combination of the two sub-scale scores,

indicated that the conventional problem solving strategy (M=3.06, SD=.54) produced

slightly higher attitude scores than either the process strategy (M=3.03, SD=.50) or the

product strategy (M=3.01, SD=.50). In addition, the sub-scale measures results displayed

in Table 4.20, confidence, F(2, 395)=1.304, p=.273, and attention, F(2, 395)=.038,

p=.962, indicated that none of the instructional strategies utilized in the study produced

differences, as predicted by the hypothesis. Thus, the analyses of attitude scores indicated

that the hypothesis that worked example participants would have higher attitude scores

(i.e., confidence and attention scores) was not supported. In general, attitude scores

suggested that participants had a positive view of the instructional materials. Attention

scores were slightly lower than confidence scores possibly indicating that either the

Page 84: Florida State University Libraries254119/datastream/PDF/download/...one of three instructional strategies (process-oriented, product-oriented, or conventional problem solving) developed

72

material lacked enough variation in the presentation of the content to stimulate learner

inquiry arousal or participants’ prior knowledge level influenced their level of interest or

attentiveness to the activity. Consequently, the interaction effect of instructional strategy

and prior knowledge was examined further.

Table 4.19. Descriptive Statistics for Learner Attitude by Instructional Strategy

Instructional Strategy

Mean Std. Deviation

N

Attitude Process 3.03 .50 136

Product 3.01 .50 143

Conventional 3.06 .54 119

Total 3.03 .51 398

Confidence Process 3.42 .62 136

Product 3.40 .67 143

Conventional 3.52 .67 119

Total 3.44 .65 398

Attention Process 2.67 .53 136

Product 2.66 .59 143

Conventional 2.65 .60 119

Total 2.66 .58 398

Note. The total number of items for the learner attitude was 21and represented the aggregate of subscale scores. The confidence subscale comprised 10 items; the attention subscale comprised 11 items. Participants rated their attitude toward the instruction on a five-point Likert-type scale ranging from 1(not true) to 5 (very true).

Table 4.20. Univariate Analysis of Variance for Instructional Strategy on Attitude

Source Dependent Variable

Type III Sum of Squares df

Mean Square F Sig.

Instructional Strategy

Confidence 110.971 2 55.485 1.304 .273

Attention 3.088 2 1.544 .038 .962

Attitude 86.113 2 43.057 .373 .689

Error Confidence 16811.213 395 42.560

Attention 15881.517 395 40.206

Attitude 45562.721 395 115.349

Page 85: Florida State University Libraries254119/datastream/PDF/download/...one of three instructional strategies (process-oriented, product-oriented, or conventional problem solving) developed

73

An analysis of the main effect of prior knowledge level on learner attitude was

also examined. Tables 4.22, 4.23, and 4.24 show the means and standard deviations of the

confidence and attention sub-scale scores, and the learner attitude scores by instructional

strategy condition and prior knowledge level. The two-way MANOVA reveal that prior

knowledge had a significant effect on attitude scores, Wilks’ Lambda=.936, F(2,

306)=10.441, p<.000, ŋ²=.064. Examination of the interaction effect between

instructional strategy and prior knowledge level failed to show significant findings,

Wilks’ Lambda=.993, F(2, 612)=.574, p=.682, ŋ²=.004.

However, a univariate analysis (see Table 4.21) indicated that prior knowledge

did influence participant perceptions of the instruction in terms of their general attitude

and confidence rating scores. Prior knowledge levels failed to show a significant effect on

participant attention rating scores. Not surprisingly, the results seem to support that prior

knowledge level did affect participants’ self reported levels of confidence as well as their

overall attitude toward the instructional material.

In conclusion, the primary analysis of data was conducted to examine the effect of

process-oriented and product-oriented worked example strategies on (1) learning and

transfer performance, (2) mental effort, and (3) learner attitude. In addition, the study

explored how the effectiveness of a particular instructional strategy was mediated by the

prior knowledge level of participants. Table 4.28 provides a summary of the result

findings. The next chapter will discuss the findings in context of extant research and

provide an interpretation of the study’s results.

Page 86: Florida State University Libraries254119/datastream/PDF/download/...one of three instructional strategies (process-oriented, product-oriented, or conventional problem solving) developed

74

Table 4.21. Univariate Analysis for Instructional Strategy and Prior Knowledge Level on Learner Attitude

Source Dependent Variable

Type III Sum of Squares df

Mean Square F Sig.

Instructional Strategy

Attitude 9.207 2 4.603 .042 .959

Confidence 43.273 2 21.637 .554 .575

Attention 14.447 2 7.224 .180 .836

Prior Knowledge Level

Attitude 1088.749 1 1088.749 9.899 .002

Confidence 778.075 1 778.075 19.932 .000

Attention 26.032 1 26.032 .648 .421

Instructional Strategy X Prior Knowledge

Attitude 151.311 2 75.656 .688 .503

Confidence 69.613 2 34.806 .892 .411

Attention 28.073 2 14.037 .349 .705

Error Attitude 33764.671 307 109.983

Confidence 11984.416 307 39.037

Attention 12333.127 307 40.173

Table 4.22. Descriptive Statistics for Confidence by Instructional Strategy and Prior Knowledge Level

Measure Instructional Strategy

Prior Knowledge Mean

Std. Deviation N

Confidence Process Low 3.27 .64 57

High 3.48 .58 57

Total 3.37 .62 114

Product Low 3.17 .71 36

High 3.61 .55 70

Total 3.46 .64 106

Conventional Low 3.30 .72 49

High 3.62 .58 44

Total 3.45 .67 93

Total Low 3.25 .69 142

High 3.57 .57 171

Total 3.42 .64 313

Note. The confidence subscale was comprised of 10 items. Participants rated their level of confidence of understanding the content after receiving the instruction on a five-point Likert-type scale ranging from 1(not true) to 5 (very true).

Page 87: Florida State University Libraries254119/datastream/PDF/download/...one of three instructional strategies (process-oriented, product-oriented, or conventional problem solving) developed

75

Table 4.23. Descriptive Statistics for Attention by Instructional Strategy and Prior Knowledge Level

Measure Instructional Strategy

Prior Knowledge Mean

Std. Deviation N

Attention Process Low 2.63 .54 57

High 2.67 .54 57

Total 2.65 .54 114

Product Low 2.57 .60 36

High 2.69 .62 70

Total 2.65 .62 106

Conventional Low 2.60 .71 49

High 2.60 .37 44

Total 2.60 .57 93

Total Low 2.60 .54 142

High 2.66 .54 171

Total 2.64 .57 313

Note. The attention subscale was comprised of 11 items. Participants rated the instructional materials effectiveness in gaining and maintaining their attention during the activity on a five-point Likert-type scale ranging from 1(not true) to 5 (very true).

Table 4.24. Descriptive Statistics for Learner Attitude by Instructional Strategy and Prior Knowledge Level

Measure Instructional Strategy

Prior Knowledge Mean

Std. Deviation N

Learner Attitude Process Low 2.93 .50 57

High 3.05 .49 57

Total 2.99 .49 114

Product Low 2.85 .52 36

High 3.13 .46 70

Total 3.04 .50 106

Conventional Low 2.94 .63 49

High 3.08 .39 44

Total 3.01 .53 93

Total Low 2.91 .55 142

High 3.09 .45 171

Total 3.01 .51 313

Note. Twenty-one items adapted from Keller’s IMMS (1993) were used to measure learner attitude. The overall attitude score represents the aggregate scores of both sub-scales.

Page 88: Florida State University Libraries254119/datastream/PDF/download/...one of three instructional strategies (process-oriented, product-oriented, or conventional problem solving) developed

76

Secondary Analysis

Prior to testing the hypotheses, several other analyses were planned to examine

the data for evidence that would further illuminate the findings and were conducted after

testing the hypotheses. First, the effect of learner attitude on the amount of mental effort

participants reported exerting during the instructional activity was examined with a one-

way ANOVA. Second, the effect of each of the attitude sub-scales, confidence and

attention, on mental effort ratings were examined using separate one-way ANOVAs.

Finally, an analysis of the effect of instructional strategy on participant instructional

efficiency was conducted using a two-way ANOVA to examine strategy and prior

knowledge level.

Effect of Learner Attitude on Mental Effort. During the completion of the

instructional activity, participants self-reported ratings on (1) their attitude toward the

instruction, and (2) the amount of mental effort they exerted during the instructional

activity. The analysis results revealed a significant main effect of overall attitude on

mental effort ratings, F(52, 345)=1.756, p=.002, ώ²=.002. The results indicated a

correlation between a participant’s attitude toward the instructional material and the

amount of effort they invest on the learning activity. Further analysis examined whether

the effect of a learner’s attitude on mental effort was influenced by the particular

instructional strategy condition a participant was assigned. Interestingly, only product-

oriented strategy showed significant a significant main effect, F(44, 98)=1.665, p=.019,

ώ²=.005. Both the process-oriented strategy, F(41, 94)=1.042, p=.425, ώ²=.0004 and the

conventional strategy, F(44, 74)=1.361, p=.120, ώ²=.003 showed no significant main

effect on a participant’s attitude affecting their mental effort investment. However, results

indicated that high prior knowledge participants, F(42, 128)=1.497, p=.045, ώ²=.003,

with a more positive attitude toward the instruction were more likely to invest more

mental effort during the instruction than did those with low prior knowledge participants,

F(45, 96)=1.046, p=.418, ώ²=.0003.

Both sub-scales of learner attitude, confidence and attention, were analyzed

separately using a one-way ANOVA to determine if differences existed between the

subscale relationships with a participant’s investment of mental effort during the

instructional activity. The analysis of confidence scores revealed a significant main effect

Page 89: Florida State University Libraries254119/datastream/PDF/download/...one of three instructional strategies (process-oriented, product-oriented, or conventional problem solving) developed

77

suggesting that a participant’s confidence was related the amount of mental effort

invested by a participant during the instructional task, F(30, 367)=3.320, p<.000,

ώ²=.006. Additionally, comparing confidence scores across instructional strategies

showed significant differences with the product-oriented strategy group, F(28,

114)=2.566, p<.000, ώ²=.01, and the conventional strategy group, F(27, 91)=2.337,

p=.001, ώ²=.01, but not for the process-oriented strategy group, F(24, 111)=1.266,

p=.205, ώ²=.002. Furthermore, participant prior knowledge level was shown to be a

significant factor in determining a participants attitude and the amount of mental effort

invested during the instructional activity, regardless of their knowledge category, low

prior knowledge, F(29, 114)=1.920, p=.008, ώ²=.01, and high prior knowledge, F(25,

145)=2.043, p=.005, ώ²=.01. Despite the findings, it must be acknowledged that both

attitude and mental effort can be influenced by various environmental or situational

factors and learner characteristics, such as prior knowledge level. The results suggest

only that a correlational relationship was found between attitude and mental effort with

participants examined in this study. Analysis of the attention subscale revealed no

significant findings.

Instructional Efficiency. Cognitive load theory is fundamentally about learning

efficiency (Clark et al., 2006). However, in order to determine efficiency researchers

needed to develop a way to measure it. Accordingly, Paas and van Merrienboer (1993,

1994) devised a formula that combined performance measures with the mental effort

ratings to examine the efficiency of instructional conditions (Pollock et al., 2002). To

determine instructional efficiency, learner performance and mental effort ratings must be

captured, then converted to standardized z-scores. The objective test scores of a particular

learning task are used to measure learner performance. However, mental effort ratings

necessitate learners to convert the mental effort requirements of a task to a numerical

value. Once this is accomplished the mental effort values are converted to standardized z-

scores. The standardized z-scores are then combined in the following formula:

Instructional Efficiency = (Performance – Mental Effort)

√2

Page 90: Florida State University Libraries254119/datastream/PDF/download/...one of three instructional strategies (process-oriented, product-oriented, or conventional problem solving) developed

78

To interpret efficiency appropriately depends on understanding the relationship between

performance and mental effort. For example, if performance and mental effort are equal

(P=ME) then efficiency would be zero. However, if performance z-scores are higher than

mental effort z-scores (P>ME), then efficiency will be higher suggesting higher

performance scores with lower mental effort invested. Similarly, if mental effort z-scores

are higher than performance z-scores (P<ME), then efficiency will be lower.

In the present study, instructional efficiency was visually represented by an

efficiency graph. To create the graph, mean z-scores for achievement and mental effort

were plotted on a Cartesian graph. The horizontal axis of the graph represents the range

of mental effort scores and the vertical axis represents the range of achievement test

scores. In addition, a theoretical reference line is included that indicates the performance

and mental effort equal zero. An instructional strategy with a reference point that

demonstrates high achievement scores and simultaneously low mental effort will fall into

the upper left quadrant indicating high efficiency. A strategy with a reference point

showing low achievement scores and high mental effort will fall in the lower right

quadrant indicating low efficiency.

This study compared the instructional efficiency of each instructional strategy

through statistical testing and plotting standardized performance and mental effort z-

scores efficiency graph. Table 4.25 shows the mean instructional efficiency scores and

standard deviation for achievement test scores. A one-way analysis of variance showed

no significant difference in mean learning efficiency scores between instructional

strategies, F(2,407)=.261, p=.771, ώ²=.002. Detailed examination of means efficiency

scores shows that the process-oriented instructional strategy had slightly higher overall

efficiency score. Further analysis was conducted by plotting the z-scores for achievement

and mental effort (see Figure 4.1).

The learning efficiency graph showed the results of mean z-score plots based on

instructional strategy. Participants in the process-oriented strategy group showed lower

achievement test scores and mental effort ratings. Product-oriented strategy participants

showed the opposite results with higher achievement test scores but higher levels of

mental effort. The conventional problem-solving strategy fell in the low efficiency

quadrant showing both low performance scores and higher levels of mental effort.

Page 91: Florida State University Libraries254119/datastream/PDF/download/...one of three instructional strategies (process-oriented, product-oriented, or conventional problem solving) developed

79

Table 4.25. Descriptive Statistics for Instructional Efficiency by Instructional Strategy

Instructional Strategy N Mean Std. Deviation Std. Error

Process 142 .0346 .97607 .08191

Product 143 .0259 .97688 .08169

Conventional 123 -.0496 1.14124 .10290

Total 408 .0062 1.02702 .05085

Additionally, a two-way ANOVA was used to analyze the interaction effect of

instructional strategy and prior knowledge on instructional efficiency. Table 4.26 shows

the mean z-scores for achievement and mental effort and Table 4.27 shows the learning

efficiency scores and standard deviation segregated by instructional strategy and prior

knowledge level. No significant interaction effect was found between instructional

strategy and prior knowledge level, F(2, 316)=.213, p=.808, ώ²=.003. However, the main

effect of prior knowledge was shown to influence instructional efficiency, F(1,

316)=18.339, p<.000, ώ²=.03. As expected, participants with higher levels of prior

knowledge tended to operate more efficiently than those of lower prior knowledge.

Page 92: Florida State University Libraries254119/datastream/PDF/download/...one of three instructional strategies (process-oriented, product-oriented, or conventional problem solving) developed

80

-0.15

-0.1

-0.05

0

0.05

0.1

0.15

-0.15 -0.1 -0.05 0 0.05 0.1 0.15

Mental Effort

Per

form

an

ce

Low Efficiency

E = 0

Conventional

Product

Process

High Efficiency

Figure 4.1. Efficiency Graph of Mean Achievement Z-Scores for Learning by Instructional Strategy

Page 93: Florida State University Libraries254119/datastream/PDF/download/...one of three instructional strategies (process-oriented, product-oriented, or conventional problem solving) developed

81

Table 4.26. Descriptive Statistics for Achievement and Mental Effort Z-scores by Instructional Strategy

Instructional Strategy N Mean

Std. Deviation Std. Error

Achievement Z-scores Process 142 -.06 .93 .08

Product 143 .09 .10 .08

Conventional 123 -.03 1.10 .10

Total 408 .00 1.00 .05

Mental Effort Z-scores Process 143 -.10 .98 .08

Product 146 .07 .97 .08

Conventional 126 .04 1.10 .09

Total 415 .00 1.00 .05

Table 4.27. Descriptive Statistics for Learning Efficiency Z-scores by Instructional Strategy and Prior Knowledge Level

Prior Knowledge Category Scores

Instructional Strategy Mean Std. Deviation N

Low Process -.17 .99 57

Product -.28 .78 37

Conventional -.31 1.10 50

Total -.25 .96 144

High Process .24 .88 61

Product .22 1.04 70

Conventional .28 1.30 47

Total .24 1.10 178

Total Process .04 .96 118

Product .05 .98 107

Conventional -.03 1.21 97

Total .03 1.05 322

Page 94: Florida State University Libraries254119/datastream/PDF/download/...one of three instructional strategies (process-oriented, product-oriented, or conventional problem solving) developed

82

Summary of Result Findings

Table 4.28 provides a summary of the data analysis results of the present study.

Table 4.28. Summary of Results

Dependent Measure

Hypotheses

Data Analysis Results

Learning Hypothesis 1: Participants who are presented with a worked example strategy (i.e., process-oriented or product-oriented) will perform better on an achievement test than participants presented with a conventional problem solving strategy.

Hypothesis was not supported; Null hypothesis of no significant difference is retained.

Hypothesis 2: Low prior knowledge participants presented a process-oriented worked example will perform better on an achievement test than low prior knowledge participants presented either a product-oriented worked example or a conventional problem solving strategy. In contrast, high prior knowledge participants presented with process-oriented worked examples will demonstrate no significant difference in achievement test scores than high prior knowledge participants presented with product-oriented worked examples or a conventional instructional strategy.

Hypothesis was not supported. High prior knowledge participants showed no difference on achievement scores across instructional strategy conditions. Contrary to prediction, low prior knowledge participants demonstrated no difference on achievement scores across instructional strategy conditions.

Transfer Hypothesis 3: Low prior knowledge participants presented a process-oriented worked example will perform better on a transfer test performance than low prior knowledge participants presented with either a product-oriented worked example or a conventional problem-solving strategy. In contrast, high prior knowledge participants presented with the process-oriented worked example will demonstrate no significant difference on transfer test performance than high prior knowledge participants presented with either product-oriented worked examples or the conventional problem-solving strategy on a transfer test.

Hypothesis was not supported. High prior knowledge participants showed no differences on transfer test scores across instructional strategy conditions. Contrary to prediction, low prior knowledge participants demonstrated no difference on transfer test scores across instructional strategy conditions.

Page 95: Florida State University Libraries254119/datastream/PDF/download/...one of three instructional strategies (process-oriented, product-oriented, or conventional problem solving) developed

83

Table 4.28. Summary of Results (Continued)

Dependent Measure

Hypotheses

Data Analysis Results

Mental Effort

Hypothesis 4. Participants presented a worked example strategy (i.e., process or product-oriented) will report lower mental effort ratings than participants presented a conventional problem-solving strategy.

Hypothesis was not supported; Null hypothesis of no significant difference is retained.

Hypothesis 5. Participants presented a process-oriented worked example strategy will report higher mental effort ratings than participants presented a product-oriented worked example strategy.

Hypothesis was not supported; Null hypothesis of no significant difference is retained.

Learner Attitude

Hypothesis 6. Participants receiving a worked example strategy (i.e., process or product-oriented) will report higher attitude scores on attention and confidence than participants presented with conventional problem solving.

Hypothesis was not supported; Null hypothesis of no significant difference is retained.

Page 96: Florida State University Libraries254119/datastream/PDF/download/...one of three instructional strategies (process-oriented, product-oriented, or conventional problem solving) developed

84

CHAPTER 5

DISCUSSION

This chapter (a) summarizes the study’s key findings in context of extant

research, and (b) presents an interpretation of the results and examines possible reasons

for the findings. The results of the study are discussed in a sequential manner based on

the order of the hypotheses presented in chapter four. In addition to the primary findings,

other notable analysis results are discussed (e.g., instructional efficiency). The limitations

of the study, implications for instructional design practice, and recommendations for

future research are presented in the concluding sections of the chapter.

Summary of Findings in Context

The effect of process-oriented and product-oriented worked examples strategies

reported in the literature was not observed in the present study. More specifically, the

study showed that the instructional strategy had a minimal affect on participant

performance, mental effort, and attitude toward the instruction. Overall, worked example

participants (M=7.89, SD=1.80) scored higher than conventional problem solving

participants (M=7.80, SD=2.04). Furthermore, for both learning and transfer

performance, a review of the mean scores showed slightly higher scores for the product-

oriented worked example strategy group compared to the process-oriented strategy group.

For mental effort, most participants self-reported a mental effort rating of “rather low” to

“average”. These findings suggest that participants, regardless of the instructional

strategy, invested a low amount of mental effort during the instructional tasks. This may

be due to either a personal choice not to expend effort or because participants prior

knowledge level reduced the complexity of the task, thereby reducing the cognitive load

imposed on the learner. The prior knowledge principle offers a possible explanation for

these findings.

The prior knowledge principle is concerned with the integration of instructional

information held in working memory and information held in long-term memory

(Kalyuga, 2005). High prior knowledge learners have previously acquired information

that can be retrieved from existing domain-specific schemas to solve a problem. Novices

Page 97: Florida State University Libraries254119/datastream/PDF/download/...one of three instructional strategies (process-oriented, product-oriented, or conventional problem solving) developed

85

lack the relevant domain knowledge and therefore benefit from direct instructional

support. Conversely, advanced learners benefited less from additional instructional

support because the additional information presented in worked examples is redundant

information that interferes with efficient processing. As a result, designers and instructors

need to recognize that as the level of learner knowledge changes the relative effectiveness

of a particular instructional strategy may reverse, a principle known as the expert-reversal

effect (Kalyuga et. al., 2003).

In addition to learning and transfer performance, the study investigated whether or

not learner attitude was affected by differences in instructional strategy. Learner attitude

was examined relative to overall attitude and two-subscales, confidence and attention.

Results indicated that most participants had a relatively positive attitude toward the

instruction. More specifically, mean scores showed that participant attitude and

confidence scores in both the conventional problem solving and product-oriented worked

example conditions were slightly more positive than those in the process-oriented group.

Factors such as the novelty of the instructional strategies used for in this study and the

level of learner prior knowledge may have affected learner attitude toward the

instruction. The next section of the chapter will discuss in more detail possible

explanations for the findings of the present study.

Interpretation of Results

The present study examined the effects of worked example strategies (i.e.,

process-oriented and product-oriented) on learning and transfer of problem solving skills

and learner attitude of undergraduate students in the complex domain of microeconomics.

In addition, the study examined the mediating effects of participant levels of prior

knowledge on learning, transfer, mental effort, and attitude. This section discusses the

interpretation of the findings and offers possible explanations for each dependent

outcome measure.

Effect of Instructional Strategy on Learning Performance

The first hypothesis predicted that participants presented with a worked example strategy

(i.e., process-oriented or product-oriented) would score higher on the achievement test

than those completing a conventional strategy. The findings of the study did not support

the stated hypothesis.

Page 98: Florida State University Libraries254119/datastream/PDF/download/...one of three instructional strategies (process-oriented, product-oriented, or conventional problem solving) developed

86

A possible explanation may be due to the lack of congruence between the

cognitive tasks participants were required to complete during the instructional activity

and the tasks they were required to complete during the performance assessment. The

performance assessment comprised fifteen items, eleven of which measured learning (i.e.,

achievement questions) and four items measured transfer of learning to novel problems.

Both the assessment format and the items used were selected by the course instructor.

The assessment items were presented in a multiple-choice format whereas the

instructional activity required the participants to work out a solution to either a series of

practice problems or a series of worked example-practice problem pairs using only the

information presented in a problem scenario. As a result, the performance assessment

examined the participants’ conceptual understanding of the topic rather than their ability

to apply the principles and procedures learned during the instruction to a realistic

economic problem. In other words, the assessment questions did not require learners to

fully activate knowledge acquired during the learning process to generate the solution to

a complex problem.

Another possible explanation is the amount of learner practice provided in the

study. Cognitive Load researchers have argued that worked examples alone are not a

sufficient instructional strategy for learning complex skills (Darabi, Nelson, & Palanki,

2007). Accordingly, the design of the instructional materials in this study included either

three practice problems for the worked example groups or seven practice problems for

the conventional problem solving group.

The practice problems used in this study were developed to allow participants to

apply the knowledge acquired during the instructional segments (i.e., lecture or lecture

and worked examples). For example, both of the worked example strategies were

designed with four worked examples and three practice problems with feedback, whereas

the conventional problem strategy presented participants with seven practice problems

with feedback. Considering eight-six percent of the participants (n=291) had previously

completed at least a high school economics course and forty-three percent (n=147)

completed a college-level course, it can be argued that participants were not novices and

entered the course with at least a partially developed basic schema (Pollock et al., 2002).

Page 99: Florida State University Libraries254119/datastream/PDF/download/...one of three instructional strategies (process-oriented, product-oriented, or conventional problem solving) developed

87

As a result, simply having the opportunity to practice an authentic problem task may have

been sufficient to promote learning with these participants.

A related explanation deals with the effectiveness of the practice feedback. As

previously stated, all three instructional strategy conditions provided practice problems

with feedback to participants. In each case, the solution feedback was provided on the

page subsequent to the problem. Recently, in a study of principled-based and procedure-

based instructional conditions, Mahan (2007) argued that the availability of feedback

limited the learner’s use of an inefficient search process (i.e., means-end analysis). The

study concluded that the feedback acted as an instructional prompt to encourage the

generation of self-explanations, which is a form of mental dialog learners have when

studying a worked example (Clark et al., 2006) that have been linked to improved

learning. Similarly, several studies that evaluated the effectiveness of different

combinations of instructional text and diagrams on learning found that if the text alone is

self-explanatory then it may be more beneficial to drop diagrams or other visual and text

combinations for experienced learners (Mayer & Gallini, 1990; Kalyuga et al., 1998,

2000). Based on these assertions, the detailed feedback received by participants in the

conventional problem solving strategy group may have acted as a sufficient prompt to

encourage self-explanation behavior. Therefore, the practice feedback may have had a

greater influence on learning than the type of instructional strategy. Consequently, if the

feedback provided to conventional problem solving participants enhanced their self-

explanation behavior similar to those in a worked example strategy, then the practice

feedback may have limited the potential benefit of the worked example strategies with

these participants.

The Expertise Reversal Effect on Learning and Transfer Performance

A critical design factor in determining what information is relevant and how

information should be presented is a learner’s level of prior knowledge and experience.

Surprisingly, many design recommendations “proceed without an explicit reference to

learner knowledge levels” (Kalyuga, Ayres, Chandler, & Sweller, 2003, p.23). The

design principle of prior knowledge, in the context of Cognitive Load Theory, offers an

explanation of the underlying cognitive mechanisms that impact the effectiveness of a

particular instructional strategy. Specifically, the principle of prior knowledge states that

Page 100: Florida State University Libraries254119/datastream/PDF/download/...one of three instructional strategies (process-oriented, product-oriented, or conventional problem solving) developed

88

the level of learner prior knowledge influences how information is processed, organized,

and retrieved during the learning process (Kalyuga, 2005). For example, novices lack the

domain-specific schemas necessary to handle the multiple elements associated with a

complex task or problem. As a result, the learner’s cognitive capacity may become

overloaded. For this reason, instructional guidance in the form of worked examples is an

instructional method designed to improve learning by freeing cognitive capacity that can

be allocated to attend to the principles and procedures necessary to solve a problem.

Conversely, advanced learners possess more well-developed schemas that allow

them to assimilate new information as well as access stored information more efficiently.

Consequently, they require less external support (e.g., instructional guidance) to solve a

complex problem. Kalyuga et al. (2003) argued that when advance learners are

confronted with instruction that may be redundant to their existing schema, they may

disregard the external support. Furthermore, researchers suggested that as the knowledge

level of the learner increases during the learning process they require less support. As a

result, the relative usefulness of the instructional material declines or reverses, an effect

known as the expertise-reversal effect (Kalyuga et al., 2003).

In chapter four, participants previous economic course demographic data and

scores on the prior knowledge assessment (M=12, SD= 1.98) established that a majority

of students in this study possessed at least a basic understanding of economic principles.

Consequently, it is argued that few of the participants in the study were novice learners

and, therefore, did not require the additional supportive information provided by worked

examples. This is a critical factor since earlier research has established that a worked

example strategy is superior for novice learners but that a worked examples advantage

over conventional problem solving quickly erodes as a learners knowledge increases

(Kalyuga, Chandler, Tuovinen, & Sweller, 2001; Kalyuga, Chandler, & Sweller, 2001).

In the present study, hypotheses two and three predicted that participants with

lower prior knowledge in the process-oriented strategy would perform better on both the

achievement test and the transfer test than those in either the product-oriented or the

conventional problem solving strategy. The findings did not support the expected

outcome. In addition, the prediction that high prior knowledge participants in the process-

oriented strategy would show no significant difference on achievement test and transfer

Page 101: Florida State University Libraries254119/datastream/PDF/download/...one of three instructional strategies (process-oriented, product-oriented, or conventional problem solving) developed

89

test performance was inconclusive. However, examination of achievement and transfer

test mean scores of high prior knowledge participants did show higher scores from the

conventional problem solving group when compared to participants in both worked

example strategy groups. More specifically, the use of process-oriented or product-

oriented worked example strategies had a negligible affect on learning and transfer

outcomes, in part, because many of the participants appeared to have developed schema

relevant to the domain that allowed them to manage the learning process without

requiring additional instructional support. The author contends that because of their

higher level of knowledge participants may have chosen to eliminate the redundant

information by focusing only on either the visual diagram or textual practice feedback

(Kalyuga, Chandler, & Sweller, 1998), thus skipping the additional information provided

by the worked examples, relying instead on their existing schema to solve the problem.

Hence, no differences were obtained between instructional strategies on learning and

transfer.

Effect of Instructional Strategy on Mental Effort

Extraneous Load. Previous research on worked examples has demonstrated their

effectiveness in improving learning outcomes while limiting the amount of mental effort

invested in solving a problem (Paas, 1992; Paas & van Merrienboer, 1994). Accordingly,

hypothesis four predicted that participants in a worked example strategy would report

lower mental effort ratings than those in a conventional problem solving strategy.

Analysis results revealed that participants across all three instructional conditions

reported exerting “rather low mental effort” (equivalent to an approximate rating of four

on a nine-point scale). Specifically, mean mental effort rating scores ranged from a low

of 3.78 (process-oriented) to a high of 4.13 (product-oriented). Two possibilities may

offer an explanation for the generally low mental effort reported by participants. First, the

the instructional materials were designed to minimize extraneous load by managing (1)

split-attention effect, (2) modality effect, and (3) redundancy effect (refer to Chapter Two

for an overview). By limiting the amount of extraneous load imposed on a participant,

more of their cognitive capacity could be used to construct a schema of how to solve a

problem (Clark et al., 2006). Perhaps, as a result of the effective design of the materials,

the mental load place on the participants was minimized.

Page 102: Florida State University Libraries254119/datastream/PDF/download/...one of three instructional strategies (process-oriented, product-oriented, or conventional problem solving) developed

90

A second possible explanation relates to participant prior knowledge. As

previously discussed, many of the participants had prior economic course work and

scored relatively high on the prior knowledge assessment (M=12, SD=1.98). Thus, prior

knowledge may have mediated the complexity of the instructional activity, thereby

reducing the participants’ intrinsic load. In other words, the participants’ level of prior

knowledge allowed them to operate more efficiently by activating relevant domain-

specific information to the problem as well as focus their attention on the key visual and

textual information necessary to solve the problem with minimal instructional support.

Germane Load. More recently, Cognitive Load researchers have focused their

attention toward strategies that increase germane or effective load on learners by the use

of process-oriented worked examples. Process-oriented worked examples provide

learners with the steps, the “how”, necessary to solve a problem as traditional worked

examples do. In addition, process-oriented examples also provide the underlying

principles and heuristics or the “why” behind the problem solution. Previous research has

shown that the processing of the additional information can increase mental load (van

Gog et al., 2006). Consequently, hypothesis five predicted that participants in the

process-oriented worked example strategy would report higher mental effort ratings than

those in the product-oriented worked example strategy. Contrary to expectations, the

hypothesis was not supported.

A possible explanation may be that participants have already developed the

necessary schemas to solve the problems presented during the learning tasks. In other

words, participants experienced a lower intrinsic cognitive load (Sweller, 2006);

therefore, any additional information presented to a participant by the process-oriented

strategy had a minimal or neutral additive effect on overall cognitive load. As a result, no

differences were found between the process-oriented and product-oriented worked

example strategies.

Effect of Instructional Strategy on Learner Attitude

Cognitive Load research has traditionally examined the cognitive processes and

structures that comprise the human architecture as the essential factors in the design of

effective and efficient learning and training programs. However, an increasing number of

studies are beginning to explore the importance of improving a learner’s attitude (i.e.,

Page 103: Florida State University Libraries254119/datastream/PDF/download/...one of three instructional strategies (process-oriented, product-oriented, or conventional problem solving) developed

91

motivation) toward the instruction as a desirable outcome goal. In fact, Paas, Tuovinen,

van Merrienboer, and Darabi (2005) argued that “meaningful learning” (pp.26) can occur

only if learners are motivated to do well. As such, creating materials that positively

influence learner attitude toward the learning process should be an essential aspect of

instructional design.

In the present study, worked example strategies (i.e., process-oriented and

product-oriented) were predicted to promote more positive attitudes toward the

instruction than those in the conventional problem solving strategy group. The results

indicated that all participants had a generally positive overall attitude toward the

instruction. As a result, no differences were found between instructional strategy groups.

Findings for attitude sub-scales, based on a five-point Likert-type scale, showed that

ratings of confidence were the most positive with the product-oriented worked example

group exhibiting the highest mean rating score (M=3.84), followed by the conventional

problem solving group (M=3.83), and the process-oriented group (M=3.75). Conversely,

attention ratings were comparatively low to the confidence sub-scale and overall learner

attitude (process-oriented: M=2.65; product-oriented: M=2.65; conventional: M=2.60).

The fact that strategies comprising minimal instructional support had higher

confidence scores and all participants reported low attention scores may indicate the

presence of an important relationship between a learner’s level of prior knowledge and

confidence toward the instruction. Accordingly, Cognitive Load Theory suggests that as

learners become more knowledgeable during the learning process the instructional

strategy must change to accommodate the change in knowledge. As a learner’s expertise

increases the influence of a particular instructional strategy on the components that make

up their attitude toward instruction also change. For example, Alexander, Jetton, and

Kulikowich (1995) proposed a motivational stage model that describes the link between

knowledge and interest, a component factor of attitude. The model suggests that as a

learner’s level of expertise increased from novice to expert their interest shifted from

situational interest (extrinsic factors) to individual interest (intrinsic factors).

The motivation literature defines situational interest as interest elicited by various

aspects of a situation. Individual interest refers to individual preferences for different

topics, tasks, or contexts and how they influence learning. In the context of this study,

Page 104: Florida State University Libraries254119/datastream/PDF/download/...one of three instructional strategies (process-oriented, product-oriented, or conventional problem solving) developed

92

situational interest would likely be elicited by the instructional material design. As such,

for novice learners worked examples may draw a learner’s interest (i.e., attention) more

effectively. However, as the learner gains expertise their individual interest (i.e., intrinsic

motivation) may supplant the need for external motivational forces. Consequently, the

prior knowledge level of participants in the present study may have contributed to the

relatively low attention scores. This is because their individual interest level in the

domain area did not require additional stimulation by the material to gain or maintain

their attention during the instruction.

The influence of prior knowledge may also offer a plausible explanation for the

lack of difference in confidence rating scores between instructional strategies. As

previously discussed, novices and high-knowledge learners learn differently based on

how they process information, a principle called the “individual difference principle”

(Mayer, 2001). Because learners with more prior knowledge can learn more efficiently,

instruction that presents additional information creates redundancy that may compete

with existing schemas imposing greater extraneous cognitive load (Kalyuga et al., 2003).

As a result, high prior knowledge learners may find that the additional process

information presented in a process-oriented worked example interferes with the learning

activity. In a recent commentary, Sweller (2006) offered that the researchers reconsider

when it is appropriate to provided additional information while studying worked

examples. Therefore, in this study, the use of an instructional strategy that limits the

amount of information for learners to process, such as product-oriented and conventional

problem solving strategies, may have contributed to slightly higher confidence and

overall attitude scores among these participants.

Incidental Findings

Effect of Learner Attitude on Mental Effort

As part of an exploratory secondary analysis, the impact of a learner’s attitude on

their investment of mental effort was examined. The results showed a correlational

relationship between a participant’s attitude toward the instruction and the amount of

mental effort they exerted during the instructional activity (i.e., treatment condition). In

other words, participants reporting a more positive attitude toward the instruction were

Page 105: Florida State University Libraries254119/datastream/PDF/download/...one of three instructional strategies (process-oriented, product-oriented, or conventional problem solving) developed

93

more likely to invest a greater amount of mental effort during the instructional activity. In

particular, the product-oriented strategy elicited the most positive attitude scores, as did

high prior knowledge participants. Because the product-oriented example provided

minimal instructional guidance in a visual form, participants with high prior knowledge

found this format easier to integrate information into their existing schemas because it

was similar to the example presented during the lecture.

For most participants, a more confident attitude was correlated with an increased

investment of mental effort on the instructional activity. This is not surprising

considering the ability of a participant to effectively assimilate information into existing

schema may have increased their self-efficacy (Holladay & Quinones, 2003; Bandura,

1982) toward the content, thereby, positively affecting their confidence toward the

instruction. As a result, the participant is willing to invest more effort to work through the

instructional activity.

Effect of Instructional Strategy on Instructional Efficiency

An additional exploratory analysis was conducted to investigate the influence of

instructional strategy on instructional efficiency. Accordingly, the present study

examined the instructional efficiency data of participants in the various instructional

strategies by comparing and then plotting their achievement test scores and their self-

reported ratings of mental effort. Findings revealed that the product-oriented group had

higher relative achievement scores while reporting higher mental effort ratings.

Conversely, the process-oriented group had lower relative achievement scores and

reported lower mental effort ratings. In addition, the effect of prior knowledge on

instructional efficiency was examined. Not surprisingly, results showed that participants

with higher prior knowledge performed the learning task more efficiently.

In explaining the importance of examining instructional efficiency, Paas et al.

(2003) argued that learners tend to balance their cognitive capacity limits by adjusting the

level of mental effort exerted on a complex task. This allows them to maintain a constant

level of performance. In other words, as the complexity of a task increases the cognitive

load, the learner must manage the complexity by investing more mental effort to

complete the learning task. For example, to understand the influence an instructional

strategy can have on instructional efficiency one must recognize that two learners can

Page 106: Florida State University Libraries254119/datastream/PDF/download/...one of three instructional strategies (process-oriented, product-oriented, or conventional problem solving) developed

94

achieve the same level of performance yet one learner invests more or less effort than the

other. Consequently, determining the most appropriate strategy that will improve learning

efficiency for a particular learner becomes an essential consideration for designers.

The worked example strategies used in the present study failed to demonstrate

differences in efficiency outcomes between instructional strategy groups because no

differences were found between strategy groups in both achievement scores and mental

effort ratings. Differences in efficiency results between groups mediated by the level of

participant prior knowledge reaffirmed that high prior knowledge participants operate at

higher levels of efficiency due to their more highly developed schemas.

Limitations of the Study

In review of the study results, it would be easy to infer that prior knowledge accounted

for outcome differences. However, to suggest that participant prior knowledge alone

accounts for the learning outcomes reported in the study with no influence by the type of

instructional strategy employed would overextend the inferences from these results.

Several reasons other than participant prior knowledge that might account for the

outcome results deserve discussion. The reasons include: (a) extraneous interference of

participant attendance and environmental factors, (b) brevity of the instructional

intervention, (c) use of a structured multiple-choice question format to assess problem

solving performance, (d) lack of participant pre-training to promote learning and transfer,

and (e) the inability to accommodate unmotivated learners.

Extraneous Interference Factors

Several factors inherent in classroom-based research bring in abnormalities that

may have resulted in extraneous interference in the study’s outcomes. In this study,

participants were enrolled in a classroom environment of approximately 500 students. As

a result, the researcher had limited control over participant attendance. Because the study

was conduct over a period of several weeks, the level of participant attendance fluctuated

on a regular basis. Although measures were taken to improved attendance, such as

offering additional participation points for students that attended the sessions, purposely

scheduling the data collection around university events (e.g., sporting events and

university required exams) that may significantly disrupt attendance, participation still

varied from session to session. Regardless of the researcher’s attempts to encourage

Page 107: Florida State University Libraries254119/datastream/PDF/download/...one of three instructional strategies (process-oriented, product-oriented, or conventional problem solving) developed

95

attendance, the lack of control over this facet of the study remained a limitation

throughout the data collection process.

Additionally, during the administration of the instructional conditions some

participants arrived late to the session while others either progressed through the activity

more quickly or simply chose not to participate. The resulting disruption occurred as a

consequence of the ebb and flow of students entering and exiting the classroom. For

example, one participant commented to the researcher that she could not “concentrate”

due to the noise level. Lastly, some participants did not follow the proper instructions

required to complete the instructional activity. For example, some participants failed to

provide the appropriate identification necessary to match them to their completed

instructional packet. A few other participants failed to complete either the mental effort

ratings or the attitude survey. However, none of these factors should have imposed a

greater impact on one group more than another. Any factors that may have contributed to

errors in the instructional conditions should have been distributed randomly across all

groups.

Brevity of the Instructional Intervention

Another limitation was the brevity of the instructional intervention. For the

purpose of this study, instructional time includes two instructional segments, (1) the

course instructor-led lecture, approximately 20 minutes, and (2) the instructional strategy

condition (i.e., instructional activity), approximately 50 minutes. The available total

instructional time was approximately 70 minutes. However, the actual time required to

complete the instructional activity varied by participant. Even though participants were

asked to record their start and stop time manually at specific points during the

instructional packet, the prior knowledge level of many of the participants may have

rendered any increase in instructional time inconsequential. More specifically, the content

may not have been at the level of complexity that would have required extended time to

assimilate the material into existing schemas or require the development of new schemas.

Use of a Structured Multiple-Choice Question Format to Assess Problem Solving

Performance

The structure of the performance assessment in comparison to the instructional

tasks practiced by participants during the instructional activity (i.e., treatment condition)

Page 108: Florida State University Libraries254119/datastream/PDF/download/...one of three instructional strategies (process-oriented, product-oriented, or conventional problem solving) developed

96

was a significant limitation in the present study. The items used in the performance

assessment were multiple-choice questions selected from a test bank constructed by the

course instructor. The main limitation of the assessment was that the performance

assessment did not present participants the problem tasks in a way that required them to

explicitly demonstrate the solution steps necessary to arrive at the correct solution. More

specifically, the multiple choice format of assessment questions required the participants

to recall declarative knowledge of the content rather than demonstrating procedural

knowledge by the application of the principles and procedures learned during the

instruction on a complex problem. In other words, the assessment questions may not have

required the participants to fully activate the knowledge acquired during the learning

process. In addition, the preliminary item analysis and the subsequent analysis of the

performance assessment scores revealed that participants had little difficulty with

successfully answering the questions. As a result, determining the actual effectiveness of

a particular instructional strategy on learning and transfer performance was problematic

at best.

Lack of Participant Pre-Training to Promote Learning and Transfer

Participants in the present study were expected to learn how to apply their

knowledge of microeconomic principles of supply and demand and the impact of taxes

on market behavior from an introductory lecture and a subsequent in-class instructional

activity in which participants were assigned to a particular instructional strategy. Worked

examples have been well established as a technique to improved learning performance by

reducing extraneous load (Sweller et al., 1998). However, some researchers have argued

that learning from worked examples is a suboptimal technique when taking into

consideration learner differences in processing example-based material (Renkl &

Atkinson, 2003). In fact, in an earlier study, Renkl (1997) found that many learners

studying worked examples do not optimize their available cognitive capacity on the

appropriate [germane] information through self-explaining. In a follow up study,

researchers found that self-explanation behavior was significantly enhanced by providing

a short training immediately prior to studying worked examples designed to ensure

optimization of cognitive capacity during example study (Renkl, Stark, Gruber, & Mandl,

1998).

Page 109: Florida State University Libraries254119/datastream/PDF/download/...one of three instructional strategies (process-oriented, product-oriented, or conventional problem solving) developed

97

Participants in the present study had no benefit of pre-training prior to utilizing

any one of the worked example strategies. It could be argued that participants were not

aware of how to utilize the materials in a manner that would promote learning. In future

studies on process-oriented example strategies, the use of pre-training should be

examined as a mediating variable of participant learning. Considering the use of real

world problems and examples (e.g., supply and demand curve graphs), the domain of

economics provides a viable discipline in which to further explore the use of worked

examples as an effective instructional technique.

Inability of the Design to Accommodate Unmotivated Learners

The underlying assumption of the design of the materials is that the participants

have a desire to learn and perform well. However, some participants may have chose to

pay cursory attention to the requirements of the instructional activity, such as reading the

directions, completing each section, showing their work, and recording start and stop

times. As indicated earlier, some participants worked more expeditiously through the

instructional activity. In a post-treatment condition review of completed activity packets

some participants’ work products showed minimal effort on the practice problems. For

example, one participant wrote the numeric answers to the practice problems without

showing the steps or calculations used to arrive at the answer. This would suggest the

individual either copied the answer from the feedback page or was able to solve the

problem without the benefit of working it out on paper. In either instance, the solution to

the problem was derived without devoting study time to the worked example. The

inability of the researcher to directly observe each participant during the completion of

the activity as well as the lack of consequences for non-compliance, such as a poor grade,

may have provided the an opportunity for some participants not to invest their full effort

during the activity. Thus, it is important for designers, instructors, and researchers to take

motivational attributes and self-regulation factors (Nelson, 2006) into account during the

design of instruction.

There are multiple factors that have contributed to the result findings of the

present study. It is important to note that the results of the study cannot be generalized

beyond the investigated population of undergraduate students with similar individual and

class characteristics. By recognizing and adjusting for the limitations described above,

Page 110: Florida State University Libraries254119/datastream/PDF/download/...one of three instructional strategies (process-oriented, product-oriented, or conventional problem solving) developed

98

the hypothesized effects might be found with the same learning objectives. Consequently,

the limiting factors should be readdressed during the planning process in a follow-up

study.

Implications for Instructional Design Practice

The purpose of the present study was to examine the effect of process-oriented

and product-oriented worked examples as viable instructional strategies for improving

problem solving and learner attitude. The results showed no difference in the learning

outcomes between the three instructional strategies. The initial impressions of an

experimental study that produces non-significant results would be that it has no

substantive implications for instructional design practice. However, the findings do

provide insight that can inform practitioners of essential elements to consider during the

design process.

For each dependent measure as well as learning efficiency, the prior knowledge

level of the participant was a significant predictor. Likewise, analysis of the prior

knowledge effect within instructional strategies showed that prior knowledge had a

significant influence on the learning outcomes examined by the present study. While

other factors were likely contributors to the study’s results, the influence of prior

knowledge was an essential element that affected performance scores and contributed to

differences in attitude toward the instruction.

For practitioners this finding is of particular importance. In certain scenarios, an

instructor may be presented with all novice learners. Similarly, they may find themselves

in a scenario with more advance participants. In either one of these scenarios, because of

the homogenous composition of the class, the instructor can more easily adjust the

structure of content and the delivery of information. A more likely situation, however, is

that learners will comprise a mixture of novice and more experienced participants. This

potentially increases the complexity required in the instructional design. Accordingly, the

designer will need to conduct a more detailed learner analysis in order to develop a

comprehensive instructional strategy.

Cognitive load research has argued that when developing instruction for novices it

is important to include supportive information, such as process-oriented worked

examples, that aid the learner in developing relevant schemas while managing cognitive

Page 111: Florida State University Libraries254119/datastream/PDF/download/...one of three instructional strategies (process-oriented, product-oriented, or conventional problem solving) developed

99

(i.e., extraneous and germane load) capacity (van Gog et al., 2004). In contrast, advanced

learners require less instructional support, often preferring more opportunity to engage in

actual problem solving in order to strengthen previously developed schemas. Moreover,

when learners are either more experienced, as was the case in the present study, or will

increase their competency during the instructions, adjustments need to be made in the

amount of instructional support provided in a dynamic way that considers the

characteristics and needs of the learners.

Despite the present study findings that worked example strategies had no

significant effect on learning or transfer, Clark et al. (2006; p. 295) suggested that if prior

knowledge is low, a designer should seek to minimize extraneous load due to the

learner’s lack of developed schemas. On the contrary, for high prior knowledge

participants, alternative instructional approaches that avoid presenting redundant

information (e.g., practice problems or guided discovery as opposed to worked examples)

should be used. In instances that the prior knowledge level of the learners is mixed, the

instructional strategy should seek to accommodate differences, where possible, through

the development adaptive instructional systems that make use of multiple instructional

techniques, such as measuring the level of learner knowledge with a pre-work or pre-tests

and modularized instructional sessions that can present basic and advanced lessons.

Another implication from this study is the use of a sequenced strategy that fades

the use of worked examples in stages toward problem-solving with no external support.

At each stage, supportive information is removed in conjunction with an increase in

knowledge level obtained by the learner. Recently, van Gog, Paas, & van Merrienboer

(2008) proposed that an optimal training sequence for novices would progress from

initially studying process-oriented examples, to studying product-oriented examples (with

self explanation prompts), to completing problems with an increasing number of blanks,

and to finally solving a conventional problem. More research needs to examine the most

effective use of process-oriented and product-oriented worked examples in the delivery

of an effective sequencing strategy for learners of all knowledge levels.

Recently, researchers have begun to acknowledge the need to expand the

conceptualizations of CLT to include the influence of the motivational attributes of a

particular strategy in the development of an efficient and effective learning environment

Page 112: Florida State University Libraries254119/datastream/PDF/download/...one of three instructional strategies (process-oriented, product-oriented, or conventional problem solving) developed

100

(Paas, Tuovin, van Merrienboer, & Darabi, 2005; Darabi et al., 2006; Moreno, 2006,

Gerjets et al., 2003). The findings in this study presented correlational evidence that a

participant’s attitude toward the instruction influences the amount of effort they are

willing to invest. More specifically, an instructional design that focuses on increasing the

learner’s confidence in their ability to perform a learning task may enhance learning and

ultimately improve performance outcomes. For practitioners, this means developing

instructional materials that help a learner maintain a high level of mental investment

during a learning activity. Maintaining a sufficient amount of mental effort is even more

critical when learning tasks that are high in complexity.

Another implication for instructional design practice relates to the motivational

and cognitive benefits of feedback. The design of feedback can play an essential role in

influencing learner attitude. As previously suggested, participants in the present study

received detailed feedback in the form of a correct answer for each practice problem

presented. As a product of the structure and detail of the feedback as well as their prior

knowledge, participants in the present study may have been able to develop the necessary

cognitive connections between the content principles to perform sufficiently on the

performance assessment without the need of additional instructional support. A

secondary effect of studying the feedback may have been an increase in learner

confidence toward the material. These factors point to the connection between motivation

and effort. Hattie and Timperley (2007) suggested that a learner’s use of “error correction

strategies” was dependent on their motivation to diminish the gap between their existing

knowledge and the learning goal. If this is assumption is correct, then practitioners need

to incorporate sufficient time and effort to develop feedback in the context of its

motivational and cognitive benefits to the learner.

Lastly, results of the present study showed that prior knowledge had a significant

effect on the learning outcomes. Since prior knowledge influences the complexity of the

task to be learned as well as the amount of support the learner needs there are practical

benefits for developing adaptive instructional systems. These systems have shown to be

more effective and efficient than fixed sequenced instructional systems (Salden, Paas, &

van Merrienboer, 2006). The value of an adaptive system is that the sequence of tasks,

the level of complexity, and pace of the instruction adjust to the needs to the learner.

Page 113: Florida State University Libraries254119/datastream/PDF/download/...one of three instructional strategies (process-oriented, product-oriented, or conventional problem solving) developed

101

More research is needed in this area to determine the viability of adaptive systems in a

variety of learning settings and the role worked examples will take within these emerging

forms of instructional systems.

Future Research

The findings of this study offer several possible directions for future research.

One direction would be to continue to expand research on the effect of worked example

sequencing on learner knowledge progression. While various factors in the present study

revealed non-significant findings related to the effectiveness of a process-oriented and a

product-oriented instructional strategy, recent studies have found that process-oriented

worked examples initially foster higher levels of efficiency in novice learners (van Gog et

al., 2006; 2008), based on equal performance with lower mental effort investment.

Researchers have also found that the benefit of a process-oriented worked example

quickly dissipates as the learner’s level of knowledge increases (van Gog et al., 2008).

These findings support the general principle of expert-reversal effect (Kalyuga et al.,

2003) and suggest the need to advance the fields understanding of how to effectively

integrate instructional techniques and methods in a manner that optimizes knowledge

progression.

A similar area for future research is investigating how to enhance instructional

guidance through the use of adaptive instructional systems that base task selection on a

learners performance and mental effort ratings (van Gog et al., 2008; Kalyuga, 2006;

Salden et al., 2006). Research has already begun exploring instructional models that

adapt the level of instructional support and difficulty for the learning task on the basis of

assessing the learner’s expertise (Kalyuga & Sweller, 2004).

Recently, Cognitive Load researchers have identified three types of task selection

models that provide adaptive instructional approaches based on the learner’s level of

expertise (van Merrienboer, Sluijsmans, Corbalan, Kalyuga, Paas, & Tattersall, 2006).

The identified models are: (1) systems-controlled models for task selection (Camp, Paas,

Rikers, & van Merrienboer, 2001), which determined task selection on the basis of the

learner’s performance of the previous task, (2) shared responsibility models (Corbalan,

Kester, & van Merrienboer, 2006) in which the instructional system selects the next

learning task from an available set of tasks but the learner has the control over final

Page 114: Florida State University Libraries254119/datastream/PDF/download/...one of three instructional strategies (process-oriented, product-oriented, or conventional problem solving) developed

102

selection of the learning task, and (3) advisory models (Kicken, Brand-Gruwel, & van

Merrienboer, 2008) which act as an advisor to the learner on the recommended task to

select but the learner has full control over task selection. Research will likely continue to

explore the uses of adaptive systems, in parallel with new advances in technology, in both

educational and organizational settings.

Future research efforts need to advance our understanding and measurement of

mental efficiency. The present study explored instructional efficiency. This approach was

adapted form of the original efficiency model (Paas & van Merrienboer, 1993) that

captured both mental effort and performance scores during the testing phase. In the

present study’s instructional efficiency model, mental effort ratings were capture during

the instructional phase while performance scores were captured during the test phase.

Van Gog and Paas (2008) argued that the original efficiency model (Paas & van

Merrienboer, 1993), now referred to as either performance or mental efficiency, is the

most effective measure of efficiency because it examines performance and mental effort

during the test phase. They suggest that because a learning environment is usually

designed to minimize cognitive load and optimize learning, taking mental load ratings

during the learning process will result in mental effort ratings that do not accurately

reflect the cognitive load experienced during a performance assessment (e.g., test).

Contrary to learning efficiency, mental efficiency measures performance and mental

effort based on the learner’s ability to recall information on a real assessment. Future

research should consider investigating the effect of process-oriented strategies on a

novice learner’s mental efficiency.

Finally, future studies should include the collection of more qualitative data. The

results of the study left many questions unanswered about the effectiveness of a process-

oriented instructional strategy. At the conclusion of the instructional activity phase,

participant feedback to both the researcher and course instructor was positive. For

example, one participants expressed to the researcher that, “the material [a process-

oriented instructional packet] really helped me put everything together”, referring to

improving his understanding of the effect of taxes on market activity. However, despite

positive feedback, it was difficult to gain explanatory insight into what the participants

really learned from the materials. In the future, using small groups and qualitative

Page 115: Florida State University Libraries254119/datastream/PDF/download/...one of three instructional strategies (process-oriented, product-oriented, or conventional problem solving) developed

103

techniques such as think-aloud protocol and individual interviews may provide additional

information on the effects worked example strategies have on the learning outcomes

examined in the present study and offer further direction for Cognitive Load research.

Conclusion

The purpose of this study was to explore the effect of worked example

instructional strategies on problem solving and learner attitude as well as the mediating

effect of prior knowledge on the stated learning outcomes. Findings failed to support the

prediction that worked example strategies would enhance learning and transfer,

particularly for low prior knowledge participants. Although the results failed to reject the

null hypothesis, a review of the mean scores of high prior knowledge participants showed

slightly higher achievement test scores for conventional problem solving participants

compared to participants in either of the two worked example groups. The results may

indicate the presence of expertise reversal effect. Contrary to expectations no differences

were found in performance scores and mental effort ratings between instructional

strategies with low prior knowledge participants. However, analysis results indicated that

the mediating effect of prior knowledge was found to be a significant factor that affected

learner performance and mental effort as well as attitude and learning efficiency.

Incidental findings showed that motivational factors, such as a learner’s attitude

and confidence to complete a task, can have a significant effect on the level of mental

effort a participant will invest on an instructional activity. Several areas of future research

have been discussed that will expand our knowledge of the potential benefit of a worked

example instructional strategy. The implications for instructional design practice and

future research offer suggestions to move cognitive load research in a direction the

leverages several decades of research in order to enhance learning effectiveness and

efficiency for learners across a wide-spectrum of educational and organizational contexts.

Page 116: Florida State University Libraries254119/datastream/PDF/download/...one of three instructional strategies (process-oriented, product-oriented, or conventional problem solving) developed

104

APPENDIX A

HUMAN SUBJECTS COMMITTEE APPROVAL MEMORANDUM

Page 117: Florida State University Libraries254119/datastream/PDF/download/...one of three instructional strategies (process-oriented, product-oriented, or conventional problem solving) developed

105

Page 118: Florida State University Libraries254119/datastream/PDF/download/...one of three instructional strategies (process-oriented, product-oriented, or conventional problem solving) developed

106

APPENDIX B

REQUEST FOR STUDENT DEMOGRAPHIC DATA

November 3, 2008 University Registrar Florida State University Office of the Registrar 282 Champions Way P.O. Box 3062480 Tallahassee, Florida 32306-2480 Re: Request for ECO2023-01 Student Demographic Information Dear University Registrar: Per your instructions, I am requesting the following demographic information on students enrolled in the fall 2008, ECO2023 section 01 Principles of Microeconomics course at Florida State University under the following column headers:

Student name Blackboard Username Gender Race/ethnicity Student date of birth FSU student classification (e.g., freshman, sophomore, junior) Academic major

The requested information will be used in during the analysis phase of my dissertation project under the direction of Associate Professor Aubteen Darabi, College of Education, Department of Educational Psychology and Learning Systems. The study examines the effectiveness of example-based instructional strategies on student performance and motivation. I have enclosed a more detailed summary for your review. All necessary precautions will be taken to protect the privacy of student information. Once received from the Registrar, the data will be secured as a password protected file. For analysis purposes, student performance indicators and motivation ratings will be matched with demographic data based on student name and blackboard username. The results will be reported in aggregate form to discuss the students’ performance and motivation as a group. No individual student will be identified and no information of any individual will be revealed. At the completion of the study, the individual identifiers including names and usernames will be destroyed. If you need additional information or have questions about the study, please contact me. Thank you for your assistance. C. Darren Brooks A. Aubteen Darabi Doctoral Candidate Dissertation Chair

Page 119: Florida State University Libraries254119/datastream/PDF/download/...one of three instructional strategies (process-oriented, product-oriented, or conventional problem solving) developed

107

APPENDIX C

CLASSROOM OBSERVATION FORM Instructor: ________________ Date: ________________ Course No. _______________ Start Time: ___________ Course Title: ______________ End Time: ________________________ Class session Topic: _______________ Number of Students: ________________ Directions: In each of the following sections indicate the presence of the following actions and behaviors with a check mark or + mark. Leave unobserved items blank. In the space provided below document any examples or observations of the actions or behaviors that occur during the instructional period. Variety and Pacing of Instruction The instructor: ___ Uses more than one form of instruction ___ Pauses after asking questions ___ Encourages student responses ___ Demonstrates active listening ___ Specifies how learning tasks will be evaluated (if at all) ___ Allows sufficient time for students to complete tasks ___ Provides opportunities and time for students to practice Examples and observations of instructor and student behaviors relating to the above ratings. Organization and Presentation of Instruction The instructor: ___ Begins on time ___ Relates the content of the lecture to previous information ___ Provides instructional goals or objectives for the class session ___ Utilizes various media forms during the class session ___ Makes transitional statements between instructional segments ___ Follows the stated structure of the class section ___ Conveys purpose of each activity ___ Periodically summarizes content and emphasizes important topics Examples and observations of instructor and student behaviors relating to the above ratings

Page 120: Florida State University Libraries254119/datastream/PDF/download/...one of three instructional strategies (process-oriented, product-oriented, or conventional problem solving) developed

108

Presentation and Clarity of Instruction The instructor: ___ Reviews new terms and concepts ___ Elaborates and/or simplifies complex information ___ Uses examples to explain content ___ Uses visual aids effectively to reinforce or explain concepts or complex content ___ Makes explicit statements or uses cues to draw student attention to certain ideas ___ Pauses during explanations to ask and answer questions ___ Communicates the reasoning process behind operations and/or concepts Examples and observations of instructor and student behaviors relating to the above ratings Instructor-Student Interaction The instructor: ___ Invites student participation and comments, when possible ___ Provides periodic feedback ___ Uses positive reinforcement The students: ___ Ask questions ___ Engage in instructor-led activities Examples and observations of instructor and student behaviors relating to the above ratings Describe the general classroom setting and features

Page 121: Florida State University Libraries254119/datastream/PDF/download/...one of three instructional strategies (process-oriented, product-oriented, or conventional problem solving) developed

109

APPENDIX D

CONVENTIONAL PROBLEM SOLVING INSTRUCTIONAL ACTIVITY MATERIAL

Note: This appendix contains only the instructional portion of the activity. The Mental Effort Rating and Learner Attitude Survey are displayed in separate appendices.

SUPPLY AND DEMAND APPLICATION AND EXTENSIONS:

THE IMPACT OF A TAX

Student Name (Print): _____________________________ (Last name, first initial)

Blackboard Username: ____________________________

Page 122: Florida State University Libraries254119/datastream/PDF/download/...one of three instructional strategies (process-oriented, product-oriented, or conventional problem solving) developed

110

MICROECONOMICS 2023 – SUPPLY AND DEMAND APPLICATION ___________________________________________________________ Introduction Taxes affect how the market exchanges goods and services. When

governments tax goods and services who bears the burden or incidence? Economists use the term tax incidence to indicate how the burden of a tax is actually shared between the buyer and seller. While the government can impose a tax statutorily on either the buyer or seller, the individual paying the tax is not always the one who ultimately bears the burden of the tax. Recall from the lecture that the burden of the tax is shared between the buyer and the seller according to the elasticity of supply and demand. This activity provides you with the opportunity to apply the knowledge you learned from the lecture by completing seven practice problems.

____________________________________________________________ Learning Goals

Following the completion of this instructional activity, you will be able to:

Determine the effects of a tax in a market

Calculate the tax incidence of buyers and sellers

Analyze the effects of supply and demand elasticity on the tax incidence between buyers and sellers

____________________________________________________________ Activity Material

This self-guide instructional activity includes the following material:

Seven Practice Problems

Mental Effort Rating Scale

Instructional Materials Survey

____________________________________________________________

Page 123: Florida State University Libraries254119/datastream/PDF/download/...one of three instructional strategies (process-oriented, product-oriented, or conventional problem solving) developed

111

MICROECONOMICS 2023 – SUPPLY AND DEMAND APPLICATION ___________________________________________________________

Activity Instructions

This activity will comprise two sections. Section One: In Section One, you will work through seven practice examples that require you apply the information you learned by solving economic impact analysis problems that require the application of the following concepts and principles:

Principles of supply and demand

Tax incidence

Supply and demand elasticity As you work through the practice problems you may use a calculator, if needed, to complete the calculations. You may not use your notes or other lecture material during the activity. You must show all your work. Section Two: In Section Two, you will complete two surveys designed to evaluate the effectiveness of the activity.

On the first survey, Mental Effort Scale, you will rate the amount of mental effort you exerted while studying the worked out examples and completing the practice problems.

The second survey, Instructional Materials Survey, will require you answer questions about the effectiveness of the activity materials on a 5-point scale. Record your answers on the scantron form.

You must complete both sections of this activity. If you have questions about any of the tasks you are performing, raise your hand for a Teaching Assistant to assist you. After completing both sections of the activity, turn in your booklet to the Instructor.

____________________________________________________________

Getting Started

In the next section you will be presented with seven practice problems. On the first three problems, you will determine the impact of a tax on both the seller and the buyer. On the remaining four problems, you will determine the effect of supply and demand elasticity on the tax incidence. Remember, you must show all your work.

____________________________________________________________ Continue to Section 1 on the next page

Page 124: Florida State University Libraries254119/datastream/PDF/download/...one of three instructional strategies (process-oriented, product-oriented, or conventional problem solving) developed

112

MICROECONOMICS 2023 – SUPPLY AND DEMAND APPLICATION ___________________________________________________________

SECTION ONE

Record Start Time: _______________ ____________________________________________________________

Continue to the next page

Page 125: Florida State University Libraries254119/datastream/PDF/download/...one of three instructional strategies (process-oriented, product-oriented, or conventional problem solving) developed

113

MICROECONOMICS 2023 – SUPPLY AND DEMAND APPLICATION ___________________________________________________________

List of Key Concepts

The following is a list of the relevant key concepts and principles underlying tax incidence and supply and demand elasticity.

Law of Demand – The principle that states there is an inverse relationship between the price of a good or service and the quantity of it that the buyers are willing to purchase. In other words, as the price of a good increase, buyers will wish to purchase less of it.

Law of Supply – The principle that states there is a direct relationship between the price of a good or service and the quantity of it that sellers are willing to supply.

Equilibrium – The state in which the conflicting forces of supply and demand are in balance. At this point, the decisions of consumers and producers are brought into harmony with one another, and the quantity supplied will equal the quantity demand.

Tax burden (incidence) – The incidence describes the actual burden of who bears the burden of a tax, buyers or sellers.

Demand Elasticity – The degree of responsiveness to a change in price is demonstrated by the steepness of the demand curve. The more responsive buyers are to a change in price, the flatter or more elastic the demand curve will be. Conversely, the less responsive to price changes, the steeper or inelastic the demand curve will be.

Supply Elasticity – The responsiveness to a change in price is shown by the steepness of the supply curve. The more willing sellers are to alter the quantity supplied in response to a change in price, the flatter or more elastic the supply curve. Conversely, the less willing sellers are to alter the quantity supplied in response to a price change, the steeper or inelastic the supply curve will be.

____________________________________________________________

Review of the Impact of a Tax

Economic analysis indicates that the actual burden of a tax does not depend on whether the tax is statutorily placed on either the buyer or the seller. The true burden of the tax is shared between the buyer and the seller according to the elasticity of supply and demand. What determines the incidence or burden of the tax? The incidence of a tax depends on the responsiveness of buyers and sellers to a change in price. When buyers respond to changes in price by leaving the market and buying other things, they will not be willing to accept a price that is higher than it was prior to the tax. Similarly, if sellers respond to a reduction in what they receive by shifting their goods and resources to other markets, or by going out of business, they will not be willing to accept a smaller payment net of taxes. Therefore, the burden of a tax tends to fall more heavily on the side of the market that is less sensitive to price changes.

Continue to Practice Problem 1 on the next page

Page 126: Florida State University Libraries254119/datastream/PDF/download/...one of three instructional strategies (process-oriented, product-oriented, or conventional problem solving) developed

114

Practice Problem 1: The Impact of a Tax Imposed on the Seller

Scenario: The seller of used cars is interested in selling a used car for any price over $6,000. However, a recent $1,000 tax has been placed on the sale of used cars. The seller would prefer to pass the tax increase on to buyers but recognizes that the higher price may result in fewer car sales. As a result, the seller and the buyer have to share the burden of the new tax. Assume the before-tax price of a used car is $7,000 resulting in the sale of 750 units. Used car prices increase by $400 which reduces demand by 250 units.

Determine the amount of the tax burden each party will bear because of the new tax. Show YOUR work in the space provided below.

____________________________________________________________ ____________________________________________________________

Continue the next page to check the answer for Practice Problem1

Page 127: Florida State University Libraries254119/datastream/PDF/download/...one of three instructional strategies (process-oriented, product-oriented, or conventional problem solving) developed

115

Solution to Practice Problem 1

Answer: In this scenario, the government passed a $1,000 tax on the seller of used cars. Despite the imposed tax on the seller, both the seller and buyer share the actual tax burden. Each $1,000 of tax revenue paid to the government imposes a burden of $400 on the buyer in the form of higher used-car prices and a $600 burden on the seller in the form of lower net revenue received from the sale of a used car.

____________________________________________________________ ____________________________________________________________

Continue to Practice Problem 2 on the next page

Page 128: Florida State University Libraries254119/datastream/PDF/download/...one of three instructional strategies (process-oriented, product-oriented, or conventional problem solving) developed

116

Practice Problem 2: The Impact of a Tax Imposed on the Buyer

Scenario: Continuing with the automobile scenario, a buyer is interested in purchasing a used car. However, recently the government placed a $1,000 tax on the buyer of used cars. Assume the before tax price of a used car is $7,000 which results in the sale of 750 units. Used car prices increase by $400 for the buyer causing demand to drop by 250 units. Determine the amount of the tax burden each party will bear because of the new tax. Show YOUR work in the space provided below.

____________________________________________________________

____________________________________________________________

Continue the next page to check the answer for Practice Problem2

Page 129: Florida State University Libraries254119/datastream/PDF/download/...one of three instructional strategies (process-oriented, product-oriented, or conventional problem solving) developed

117

Solution to Practice Problem 2

Answer: In this scenario, the government imposed a $1,000 tax on the buyers of used cars. Despite the imposed tax on the both, both the seller and buyer share the actual tax burden. Each $1,000 of tax revenue paid to the government imposes a burden of $400 on the buyer in the form of higher used-car prices and a $600 burden on the seller in the form of lower net revenue received from the sale of a used car. The result is the same whether the government places the tax on the buyer or seller.

____________________________________________________________

____________________________________________________________

Continue to Practice Problem 3 on the next page

Page 130: Florida State University Libraries254119/datastream/PDF/download/...one of three instructional strategies (process-oriented, product-oriented, or conventional problem solving) developed

118

Practice Problem 3: The Impact of a Tax Imposed on the Seller

Scenario: The seller of laptop computers sells computers for $1,000 each. A tax of $200 has been imposed on the sale of computers. This raises the price of a laptop computer to $1,100. Assume the price of laptop computers before the tax is $1,000 which results in the sell of 500 units. The increase in price impacts demand negatively by 150 units. Determine the amount of the tax burden each party will bear as a result of the tax. Show YOUR work in the space provided below.

____________________________________________________________ ____________________________________________________________

Continue the next page to check the answer for Practice Problem 3

Page 131: Florida State University Libraries254119/datastream/PDF/download/...one of three instructional strategies (process-oriented, product-oriented, or conventional problem solving) developed

119

Solution to Practice Problem 3 Answer: As a result of the $200 tax imposed on the sale of laptop computers, both the

seller and buyers share the burden of the tax equally. The seller bears $100 and the buyer bears $100 of the tax burden.

____________________________________________________________ ____________________________________________________________

Continue to Practice Problem 4 on the next page

Page 132: Florida State University Libraries254119/datastream/PDF/download/...one of three instructional strategies (process-oriented, product-oriented, or conventional problem solving) developed

120

Practice Problem 4: Elastic Supply and Inelastic Demand

Scenario: Recently, the government imposed at tax of $0.50 on the sale of a gallon of milk. The seller has to determine whether or not to pass the tax increase on to buyers. Before making the decision the seller needs to consider the impact of higher costs on sales revenue. Milk is a product that buyers have difficulty finding inexpensive alternatives. Therefore, demand is relatively inelastic while there is ample supply. Assume the before tax price of a gallon of milk is $4.60 resulting in the sale of 100 units. As a result of the tax, milk prices increase by $0.40 and demand drops by 10 units. Determine the amount of the tax burden each party will bear because of the new tax. Show YOUR work in the space provided below.

____________________________________________________________ ____________________________________________________________

Continue the next page to check the answer for Practice Problem 4

Page 133: Florida State University Libraries254119/datastream/PDF/download/...one of three instructional strategies (process-oriented, product-oriented, or conventional problem solving) developed

121

Solution to Practice Problem 4 Answer: In this problem, the government imposed a $0.50 tax on the seller of milk. As a

result, demand is more inelastic than supply. As a result, each $0.50 of tax revenue paid to the government creates a burden of $0.40 is imposed on the buyer in the form of higher milk prices and a $0.10 burden is maintained by the seller in the form of lower net revenue received from the sale of a gallon of milk.

____________________________________________________________

____________________________________________________________

Continue to Practice Problem 5 on the next page

Page 134: Florida State University Libraries254119/datastream/PDF/download/...one of three instructional strategies (process-oriented, product-oriented, or conventional problem solving) developed

122

Practice Problem 5: Elastic Supply and Inelastic Demand and the Burden of a Tax

Scenario: The government has placed a $.50 tax per gallon of gas sold. Unfortunately,

buyers have few options to switch to when gas prices increase. Before the tax was imposed a gallon of gas was $3.60 which resulted in the sale of 200 gallons of gas. After the tax, gas rose to $4.00 per gallon causing demand to drop to 190 gallons. Determine the amount of the tax burden each party will bear as a result of the tax. Show YOUR work in the space provided below.

____________________________________________________________ ____________________________________________________________

Continue the next page to check the answer for Practice Problem 5

Page 135: Florida State University Libraries254119/datastream/PDF/download/...one of three instructional strategies (process-oriented, product-oriented, or conventional problem solving) developed

123

Solution to Practice Problem 5

Answer: As a result of the $.50 per gallon gasoline tax imposed the buyer will bear more of the tax burden than the seller. This is because demand is relatively inelastic. Specifically, the buyer bears $.40 and the seller bears $.10 of the tax burden.

____________________________________________________________ ____________________________________________________________

Continue to Practice Problem 6 on the next page

Page 136: Florida State University Libraries254119/datastream/PDF/download/...one of three instructional strategies (process-oriented, product-oriented, or conventional problem solving) developed

124

Practice Problem 6: Elastic Demand and Inelastic Supply

Scenario: You own a business selling custom made t-shirts. Recently, a tax of $3.00 has

been imposed on the sale of clothing. Buyers of t-shirts have numerous alternatives to choose from. Therefore, as the seller you have to determine how much of the increased cost you will pass on to the buyer. For this problem, assume the before tax price of a t-shirt is $20.00. At this price you can sell 150 units. As a result of the tax, t-shirt prices increase by $1.00 per shirt. Concurrently, your t-shirt demand drops by 50 units. Determine the amount of the tax burden each party will bear because of the new tax. Show YOUR work in the space provided below.

____________________________________________________________

____________________________________________________________

Continue the next page to check the answer for Practice Problem 5

Page 137: Florida State University Libraries254119/datastream/PDF/download/...one of three instructional strategies (process-oriented, product-oriented, or conventional problem solving) developed

125

Solution to Practice Problem 6 Answer: In this scenario, the government imposed a $3.00 tax on the seller of t-shirts.

In this scenario, supply is more inelastic than demand. As a result, each $3.00 of tax revenue paid to the government creates a burden of $1.00 is imposed on the buyer in the form of t-shirt prices and a $2.00 burden is on the seller in the form of lower net revenue received from the sale of a custom t-shirt.

____________________________________________________________

____________________________________________________________

Continue to Practice Problem 7 on the next page

Page 138: Florida State University Libraries254119/datastream/PDF/download/...one of three instructional strategies (process-oriented, product-oriented, or conventional problem solving) developed

126

Practice Problem 7: Inelastic Supply and Elastic Demand and the Burden of a Tax

Scenario: You the owner of a bicycle shop that specializes in selling custom youth bicycles. Recently, a luxury tax of $100 has been imposed on the sell of bicycles. Because you sell custom bicycles buyers have numerous low-end alternatives to choose from. Therefore, as the seller you have to determine how much of the increased cost from the tax you will pass on to the buyer. For this problem, assume the before tax price of a bicycle is $500. At this price you can sell 100 bicycles. As a result of the tax, your prices increase by $25.00 per bicycle. The price increase causes demand to drop by 50 units. Determine the amount of the tax burden each party will bear as a result of the tax. Show YOUR work in the space provided below.

____________________________________________________________ ____________________________________________________________

Continue the next page to check the answer for Practice Problem 7

Page 139: Florida State University Libraries254119/datastream/PDF/download/...one of three instructional strategies (process-oriented, product-oriented, or conventional problem solving) developed

127

Solution to Practice Problem 7

Answer: As a result of the $100 luxury tax the seller will bear more of the tax burden than the seller. This is because demand is relatively elastic. Specifically, the buyer bears $25 and the seller bears $75 of the tax burden.

____________________________________________________________ ____________________________________________________________

Record Stop Time: ___________

Page 140: Florida State University Libraries254119/datastream/PDF/download/...one of three instructional strategies (process-oriented, product-oriented, or conventional problem solving) developed

128

APPENDIX E

PROCESS-ORIENTED INSTRUCTIONAL ACTIVITY MATERIAL

Note: This appendix contains only the instructional portion of the activity. The Mental Effort Rating and Learner Attitude Survey are displayed in separate appendices.

SUPPLY AND DEMAND APPLICATION AND EXTENSIONS:

THE IMPACT OF A TAX

Student Name (Print): _____________________________ (Last name, first initial)

Blackboard Username: ____________________________

Page 141: Florida State University Libraries254119/datastream/PDF/download/...one of three instructional strategies (process-oriented, product-oriented, or conventional problem solving) developed

129

Research Study Participant Consent

Dear Student,

I am a doctoral student under the direction of Professor A. Aubteen Darabi of the College of Education, Department of Educational Psychology and Learning Systems at Florida State University. I am conducting a study to investigate the effectiveness of the use of learning strategies aimed at improving problem solving skills in some topic areas of instructions in this course.

In this research, you may be asked to study a series of worked out examples and/or complete a few practice activities as part of a regularly scheduled 50-minute classroom lesson. In addition, you will also be asked to evaluate the amount of mental effort you invested during these activities: studying the worked out examples and/or completing the practice activities. At the end of the session, you will be asked to complete a 21-question survey designed to evaluate the instructional materials influences of the use of these strategies.

Your participation in this study is voluntary; you must be at least 18 years of age to participate. If you choose not to participate or to withdraw from the study at any time, there will be no penalty, and it will not affect your grade. This study is not a test or an evaluation of your course performance. The results of this activity will only be used to assess the efficiency and effectiveness of the instructional strategies used in the study. The data from this research may be published in aggregate form and by no means would you be individually identified in a publication.

There are no risks to you if you agree to participate in this study.

Microeconomics is a new area for conducting this type of research. By your participation you are contributing to the development of instructional strategies designed to improve student learning and understanding of microeconomics.

If you have any questions concerning this research study, please call me at (850) 878-3085 or email me at [email protected] or the faculty member supervising the research A. Aubteen Darabi at 850-644-5652.

Sincerely,

C. Darren Brooks

By signing below, I acknowledge that I am at least 18 years of age and I give my consent to participate in the study described above.

_______________________________ (student signature) _______________________ (date)

If you have any questions about your rights as a subject/participant in this research, or if you feel you have been placed at risk, you can contact the Chair of the Human Subjects Committee, Institutional Review Board, through the Vice President for the Office of Research at (850) 644-8633.

Page 142: Florida State University Libraries254119/datastream/PDF/download/...one of three instructional strategies (process-oriented, product-oriented, or conventional problem solving) developed

130

MICROECONOMICS 2023 – SUPPLY AND DEMAND APPLICATION ___________________________________________________________ Introduction Taxes affect how the market exchanges goods and services. When

governments tax goods and services who bears the burden or incidence? Economists use the term tax incidence to indicate how the burden of a tax is actually shared between the buyer and seller. While the government can impose a tax statutorily on either the buyer or seller, the individual paying the tax is not always the one who ultimately bears the burden of the tax. Recall from the lecture that the burden of the tax is shared between the buyer and the seller according to the elasticity of supply and demand. This activity provides you with the opportunity to study four worked out examples that demonstrate how to perform an impact analysis of a tax on a buyer and a seller. After studying each worked out example you will have the opportunity to apply your new knowledge by completing three practice problems.

____________________________________________________________ Learning Goals

Following the completion of this instructional activity, you will be able to:

Determine the effects of a tax in a market

Calculate the tax incidence of buyers and sellers

Analyze the effects of supply and demand elasticity on the tax incidence between buyers and sellers

____________________________________________________________ Activity Material

This self-guide instructional activity includes the following material:

Four Worked Out Examples

Three Practice Problems

Mental Effort Rating Scale

Instructional Materials Survey

____________________________________________________________

Page 143: Florida State University Libraries254119/datastream/PDF/download/...one of three instructional strategies (process-oriented, product-oriented, or conventional problem solving) developed

131

MICROECONOMICS 2023 – SUPPLY AND DEMAND APPLICATION ___________________________________________________________

Activity Instructions

This activity is comprised of two sections. Section One: In Section One, you will study four different worked examples designed to teach you how to solve an impact analysis problem requiring the application of the following concepts and principles:

Principles of supply and demand

Tax incidence

Supply and demand elasticity After studying a worked out example you will have the opportunity to apply your knowledge on a practice problem similar to the example you studied. If needed, you may use a calculator to complete the calculations. You must show all your work. Section Two: In Section Two, you will complete two surveys designed to evaluate the effectiveness of the activity.

On the first survey, Mental Effort Scale, you will rate the amount of mental effort you exerted while studying the worked out examples and completing the practice problems.

The second survey, Instructional Materials Survey, will require you answer questions about the effectiveness of the activity materials on a 5-point scale. Record your answers on the scantron form.

You must complete both sections of this activity. If you have questions about any of the tasks you are performing, raise your hand for a Teaching Assistant to assist you. After completing both sections of the activity, turn in your booklet to the Instructor.

____________________________________________________________

Getting Started

In the next section, you are presented four worked out examples and three practice problems. The first two worked out examples and practice problem expose you to the impact of a tax on both the seller and the buyer. The next two worked out examples and practice problems help you learn about the effect of supply and demand elasticity on the tax incidence. As you work the practice problems, show all your work.

____________________________________________________________ Continue to Section 1 on the next page

Page 144: Florida State University Libraries254119/datastream/PDF/download/...one of three instructional strategies (process-oriented, product-oriented, or conventional problem solving) developed

132

MICROECONOMICS 2023 – SUPPLY AND DEMAND APPLICATION ___________________________________________________________

SECTION ONE

Record Start Time: ____________ ____________________________________________________________

Continue to the next page

Page 145: Florida State University Libraries254119/datastream/PDF/download/...one of three instructional strategies (process-oriented, product-oriented, or conventional problem solving) developed

133

MICROECONOMICS 2023 – SUPPLY AND DEMAND APPLICATION ___________________________________________________________

List of Key Concepts

The following is a list of the relevant key concepts and principles underlying tax incidence and supply and demand elasticity.

Law of Demand – The principle that states there is an inverse relationship between the price of a good or service and the quantity of it that the buyers are willing to purchase. In other words, as the price of a good increase, buyers will wish to purchase less of it.

Law of Supply – The principle that states there is a direct relationship between the price of a good or service and the quantity of it that sellers are willing to supply.

Equilibrium – The state in which the conflicting forces of supply and demand are in balance. At this point, the decisions of consumers and producers are brought into harmony with one another, and the quantity supplied will equal the quantity demand.

Tax burden (incidence) – The incidence describes the actual burden of who bears the burden of a tax, buyers or sellers.

Demand Elasticity – The degree of responsiveness to a change in price is demonstrated by the steepness of the demand curve. The more responsive buyers are to a change in price, the flatter or more elastic the demand curve will be. Conversely, the less responsive to price changes, the steeper or inelastic the demand curve will be.

Supply Elasticity – The responsiveness to a change in price is shown by the steepness of the supply curve. The more willing sellers are to alter the quantity supplied in response to a change in price, the flatter or more elastic the supply curve. Conversely, the less willing sellers are to alter the quantity supplied in response to a price change, the steeper or inelastic the supply curve will be.

____________________________________________________________

Review of the Impact of a Tax

Economic analysis indicates that the actual burden of a tax does not depend on whether the tax is statutorily placed on either the buyer or the seller. The true burden of the tax is shared between the buyer and the seller according to the elasticity of supply and demand. What determines the incidence or burden of the tax? The incidence of a tax depends on the responsiveness of buyers and sellers to a change in price. When buyers respond to changes in price by leaving the market and buying other things, they will not be willing to accept a price that is higher than it was prior to the tax. Similarly, if sellers respond to a reduction in what they receive by shifting their goods and resources to other markets, or by going out of business, they will not be willing to accept a smaller payment net of taxes. Therefore, the burden of a tax tends to fall more heavily on the side of the market that is less sensitive to price changes.

Continue to Example 1 on the next page

Page 146: Florida State University Libraries254119/datastream/PDF/download/...one of three instructional strategies (process-oriented, product-oriented, or conventional problem solving) developed

134

Example 1: The Impact of a Tax Imposed on the Seller

Scenario: The seller of used cars is interested in selling a used car for any price over $6,000. However, a recent $1,000 tax has been placed on the sale of used cars. The seller would prefer to pass the tax increase on to buyers but recognizes that the higher price may result in fewer car sales. As a result, the seller and the buyer have to share the burden of the new tax. Assume the before-tax price of a used car is $7,000 resulting in the sale of 750 units. Used car prices increase by $400 which reduces demand by 250 units. Determine the amount of the tax burden each party will

bear because of the new tax by following the steps below and their provided rationale.

Answer: In this scenario, the government passed a $1,000 tax to the seller of used cars. Despite the imposed tax on the seller, both the seller and buyer share the actual tax burden. Each $1,000 of tax revenue paid to the government imposes a burden of $400 on the buyer in the form of higher used-car prices and a $600 burden on the seller in the form of lower net revenue received from the sale of a used car.

Continue to Example 2 on the next page

S No Tax

D

Q No Tax

$7,000

750

P No Tax

S + Tax

$7,400

$6,400

500

$1,000

Q With Tax

Pc

Ps

Step 1: Draw the intersection of the Supply and Demand Curves and determine the price (P) and quantity (Q) equilibrium before the tax is imposed. The equilibrium price is the state in which the conflicting forces of supply and demand are in balance. The quantity supplied will equal the quantity demanded. Changes in price disrupt the market balance resulting in a new market equilibrium.

Step 5: Determine the tax burden of the seller and buyer. It is important to recognize that the actual burden of a tax (or share of the burden) does not depend on whether the tax is statutorily placed on the seller or the buyer. Supply and demand elasticity determines how the tax is distributed between the buyer and seller. Remember, trade results in mutual gains for both sellers and buyers. Therefore, passing the entire tax burden on to the buyer would result in higher prices and some buyers may choose not to buy a used car. As a result, the seller finds it more profitable to accept some of the tax burden.

Seller’s Burden

(PNo Tax - Ps) $600

Buyer’s Burden

(Pc - PNo Tax )

$400

Step 3: Plot price consumers (Pc) are willing to pay after tax is applied. The new equilibrium price represents the seller’s acceptance of some of the tax burden to minimize sales losses.

Step 4: Calculate and determine the price

the seller will receive (Pp) after paying the

tax. Calculation:

$7,400 (Pc) - $1,000 (tax) = $6,400 (Ps)

This point represents the price the seller actually receives as a result of bearing some of the tax burden. Prior to the tax the seller was receiving $7,000 for each used car sold.

Step 2: Shift the supply curve upward by the amount of the tax. When a tax is statutorily imposed on the seller the supply curve shifts.

Page 147: Florida State University Libraries254119/datastream/PDF/download/...one of three instructional strategies (process-oriented, product-oriented, or conventional problem solving) developed

135

Example 2: The Impact of a Tax Imposed on the Buyer

Scenario: Continuing with the automobile scenario, a buyer is interested in purchasing a used car.

However, recently the government placed a $1,000 tax on the buyer of used cars. Assume the before-tax price of a used car is $7,000, which results in the sale of 750 units. Used car prices increase by $400 for the buyer causing demand to drop by 250 units. Study the following steps and their rationale to determine the amount of the tax burden each party will bear because of the new tax.

Answer: In this scenario, the government imposed a $1,000 tax on the buyers of used cars. Despite the imposed tax, both the seller and buyer share the actual tax burden. Each $1,000 of tax revenue paid to the government imposes a burden of $400 on the buyer in the form of higher used-car prices and a $600 burden on the seller in the form of lower net revenue received from the sale of a used car. The result is the same whether the government places the tax on the buyer or seller.

Continue to the Practice Problem on the next page

S

D

Q No Tax

$7,000

750

P No Tax

$7,400

$6,400

500

$1,000

Step 1: Draw the Supply and Demand Curves and plot the price and quantity equilibrium before the tax is imposed. The equilibrium price is when supply and demand forces are in balance. Disruptions in price or demand result in a new equilibrium.

Step 3: Plot price sellers (Pp) receive

before the tax is applied. This represents the new equilibrium price.

This also represents the price the buyer is willing to pay given the addition of the tax. Before the tax was imposed the buyer would be willing to pay $7000.

Step 2: Shift the demand curve downward by the amount of the $1000 tax. When a tax is statutorily placed on the buyer, the demand curve shifts.

Buyer’s Burden

(Pc - PNo Tax )

$400

Seller’s Burden

(PNo Tax - Pp) $600

Q With Tax

Pc

Pp

D - Tax

Step 4: Calculate and plot the total price the buyer will

pay (Pc) after of the tax.

$6,400 (Pp) + $1,000 (tax) = $7,400 (Pc)

This point represents the price paid by the buyer as a result of bearing some of the tax burden.

Step 5: Determine the tax burden of the seller and buyer. Regardless of whether the tax is placed statutorily on the seller or the buyer the result of the actual tax burden is the same. Supply and demand elasticities determine how the tax is distributed between the buyer and seller. In this case, the seller must drop car prices or lose sales due to the buyer incurring a tax.

Page 148: Florida State University Libraries254119/datastream/PDF/download/...one of three instructional strategies (process-oriented, product-oriented, or conventional problem solving) developed

136

Practice Problem for Examples 1 and 2: The Impact of a Tax Imposed on the Seller

Scenario: The seller of laptop computers sells computers for $1,000 each. A tax of $200 has been imposed on the sale of a computer. This raises the price of a laptop computer to $1,100. Assume the price of a laptop computer before the tax is imposed $1,000 which results in the sell of 500 units. The increase in price impacts demand negatively by 150 units. Determine the amount of the tax burden each party will bear as a result of the tax. Show YOUR work in the space provided below.

____________________________________________________________ ____________________________________________________________

Continue to the next page for the answer to the Practice Problem

Page 149: Florida State University Libraries254119/datastream/PDF/download/...one of three instructional strategies (process-oriented, product-oriented, or conventional problem solving) developed

137

Solution to Practice Problem for Examples 1 and 2 Answer: As a result of the $200 tax imposed on the sale of laptop computers, both the

seller and buyer share the burden of the tax equally. The seller bears $100 and the buyer bears $100 of the tax burden.

____________________________________________________________ ____________________________________________________________

Continue to Example 3 on the next page

Page 150: Florida State University Libraries254119/datastream/PDF/download/...one of three instructional strategies (process-oriented, product-oriented, or conventional problem solving) developed

138

Example 3: Elasticity and the Burden of a Tax: Elastic Supply and Inelastic Demand

Scenario: Recently, the government imposed at tax of $0.50 on the sale of a gallon of milk. The seller has to determine whether or not to pass the tax increase on to buyers. Before making the decision the seller needs to consider the impact of higher costs on sales revenue. Milk is a product that buyers have difficulty finding inexpensive alternatives. Therefore, demand is relatively inelastic while there is ample supply. Assume the before tax price of a gallon of milk is $4.60 resulting in the sale of 100 units. As a result of the tax, milk prices increase by $0.40 and demand drops by 10 units. Study the steps below to determine the amount of the tax burden each party will bear because of the new tax.

Answer: In this scenario, the government imposed a $0.50 tax on the seller of milk. In this scenario, demand is more inelastic than supply. As a result, each $0.50 of tax revenue paid to the government creates a burden of $0.40 is imposed on the buyer in the form of higher milk prices and a $0.10 burden is maintained by the seller in the form of lower net revenue received from the sale of a gallon of milk.

Continue to the Practice Problem on the next page

S No Tax

D

Q No Tax

$4.60

100

P No Tax

S + Tax

$5.00

$4.50

90

$0.50

Step 1: Draw the Supply and Demand Curves and plot the price and quantity equilibrium before the tax is imposed. The demand curve represents consumer demand for a good or service. The concept of elasticity determines how much the quantity of a good or service responds to a change in price. In this scenario, the good is inelastic because consumers view milk as a necessity. Consequently, price changes have limited affect on consumer demand.

Step 3: Plot price consumers (Pc) are willing to pay after tax is applied. The new equilibrium price represents the seller’s and buyer’s acceptance of a portion of the tax burden. When demand is inelastic the consumer accepts a larger portion of the tax burden.

Step 2: Shift the supply curve upward. A tax imposed on the seller causes the supply curve to shift upward.

Step 4: Calculate and plot the price the

seller will receive (Pp) net of taxes.

$5.00 (Pc) - $0.50 (tax) = $0.50 (Pp) This point represents the price the seller receives after adjusting the price due to the tax. Prior to the tax, the original price for a gallon of milk was $4.60.

Buyer’s Burden

(Pc - PNo Tax )

$0.40

Seller’s Burden

(PNo Tax - Pp) $0.10

Q With Tax

Pc

Pp

Step 5: Determine the tax burden of the seller and buyer. Remember, the actual burden of a tax (or share of the burden) does not depend on whether the tax is statutorily placed on the seller or the buyer. The supply and demand elasticity determines how the tax is distributed between the buyer and seller. Because the effect of price on the quantity demanded is minimal, the seller is able to pass on a larger portion of the tax burden to the consumer.

Page 151: Florida State University Libraries254119/datastream/PDF/download/...one of three instructional strategies (process-oriented, product-oriented, or conventional problem solving) developed

139

Practice Problem for Example 3: Elastic Supply and Inelastic Demand

Scenario: The government has placed a $.50 tax per gallon of gas sold. Unfortunately, buyers have few options to switch to when gas prices increase. Before the tax was imposed, a gallon of gas was $3.60 which resulted in the sale of 200 gallons of gas. After the tax, gas rose to $4.00 per gallon causing demand to drop to 190 gallons. Determine the amount of the tax burden each party will bear as a result of the tax. Show YOUR work in the space provided below.

____________________________________________________________ ____________________________________________________________

Continue to the next page for the answer to the Practice Problem

Page 152: Florida State University Libraries254119/datastream/PDF/download/...one of three instructional strategies (process-oriented, product-oriented, or conventional problem solving) developed

140

Solution to the Practice Problem for Example 3

Answer: As a result of the $.50 per gallon gasoline tax imposed the buyer will bear more of the tax burden than the seller. This is because demand is relatively inelastic. Specifically, the buyer bears $.40 cents and the seller bears $.10 cents of the tax burden.

____________________________________________________________ ____________________________________________________________

Continue to Example 4 on the next page

Page 153: Florida State University Libraries254119/datastream/PDF/download/...one of three instructional strategies (process-oriented, product-oriented, or conventional problem solving) developed

141

Example 4: Elasticity and the Burden of a Tax: Elastic Demand and Inelastic Supply

Scenario: You own a business selling custom made t-shirts. Recently, a tax of $3.00 has been imposed on the sale of clothing. Buyers of t-shirts have numerous alternatives to choose from. Therefore, as the seller you have to determine how much of the increased cost you will pass on to the buyer. For this problem, assume the before tax price of a t-shirt is $20. At this price you can sell 150 units. As a result of the tax, t-shirt prices increase by $1.00 per shirt. Concurrently, your t-shirt demand drops by 50 units. Study the following steps and their rationale to determine the amount of the tax burden each party will bear because of the new tax.

Answer: In this scenario, the government imposed a $3.00 tax on the seller of t-shirts. In this scenario, supply is more inelastic than demand. As a result, each $3.00 of tax revenue paid to the government creates a burden of $1.00 is imposed on the buyer in the form of t-shirt prices and a $2.00 burden is on the seller in the form of lower net revenue received from the sale of a custom t-shirt.

Continue to the Practice Problem on the next page

S No Tax

D

Q No Tax

$20.00

150

P No Tax

S + Tax

$21.00

$18.00

100

$3.00

Step 3: Plot price consumers (Pc) are willing to pay after tax is applied. The new equilibrium price represents the seller’s and buyer’s acceptance of a portion of the tax burden. When demand is elastic the consumer is not willing to purchase a good when price increases.

Step 2: Shift the supply curve upward. A tax imposed on the seller causes the supply curve to shift upward.

Step 4: Calculate and plot the price the

seller will receive (Pp) net of taxes.

$21.00 (Pc) - $3.00 (tax) = $18.00 (Pp)

This point represents the price the seller receives after adjusting the price due to the tax. Prior to the tax, the original price for a t-shire was $20.

Buyer’s Burden

(Pc - PNo Tax )

$1.00

Seller’s Burden

(PNo Tax - Pp) $2.00

Q With Tax

Pc

Pp

Step 1: Draw the Supply and Demand Curves and plot the price and quantity equilibrium before the tax is imposed. The demand curve represents consumer demand for a good or service. The concept of elasticity determines how much the quantity of a good or service responds to a change in price. In this scenario, the good is elastic because consumers view t-shirts as a non-essential item. Consequently, price changes have a greater influence on consumer demand.

Step 5: Determine the tax burden of the seller and buyer. Remember, the actual burden of a tax (or share of the burden) does not depend on whether the tax is statutorily placed on the seller or the buyer. The supply and demand elasticity determines how the tax is distributed between the buyer and seller. Because the increase in price drastically affects the quantity demanded, the seller must absorb a larger portion of the tax burden.

Page 154: Florida State University Libraries254119/datastream/PDF/download/...one of three instructional strategies (process-oriented, product-oriented, or conventional problem solving) developed

142

Practice Problem for Example 4: Inelastic Supply and Elastic Demand

Scenario: You are the owner of a bicycle shop that specializes in selling custom youth bicycles. Recently, a luxury tax of $100 has been imposed on the sell of bicycles. Because you sell custom bicycles buyers have numerous low-end alternatives to choose from. Therefore, as the seller you have to determine how much of the increased cost from the tax you will pass on to the buyer. For this problem, assume the before tax price of a bicycle is $500. At this price you can sell 100 bicycles. As a result of the tax, your prices increase by $25 per bicycle. The price increase causes demand to drop by 50 units. Determine the amount of the tax burden each party will bear as a result of the tax. Show YOUR work in the space provided below.

____________________________________________________________ ____________________________________________________________

Continue to the next page for the answer to the Practice Problem

Page 155: Florida State University Libraries254119/datastream/PDF/download/...one of three instructional strategies (process-oriented, product-oriented, or conventional problem solving) developed

143

Solution to Practice Problem for Example 4

Answer: As a result of the $100 luxury tax, the seller will bear more of the tax burden than the seller. This is because demand is relatively elastic. Specifically, the buyer bears $25 and the seller bears $75 of the tax burden.

____________________________________________________________ ____________________________________________________________

Record Stop Time: ___________

Page 156: Florida State University Libraries254119/datastream/PDF/download/...one of three instructional strategies (process-oriented, product-oriented, or conventional problem solving) developed

144

APPENDIX F

PRODUCT-ORIENTED INSTRUCTIONAL ACTIVITY MATERIAL

Note: This appendix contains only the instructional portion of the activity. The Mental Effort Rating and Learner Attitude Survey are displayed in separate appendices.

SUPPLY AND DEMAND APPLICATION AND EXTENSIONS:

THE IMPACT OF A TAX

Student Name (Print): _____________________________ (Last name, first initial)

Blackboard Username: ____________________________

Page 157: Florida State University Libraries254119/datastream/PDF/download/...one of three instructional strategies (process-oriented, product-oriented, or conventional problem solving) developed

145

MICROECONOMICS 2023 – SUPPLY AND DEMAND APPLICATION ___________________________________________________________ Introduction Taxes affect how the market exchanges goods and services. When

governments tax goods and services who bears the burden or incidence? Economists use the term tax incidence to indicate how the burden of a tax is actually shared between the buyer and seller. While the government can impose a tax statutorily on either the buyer or seller, the individual paying the tax is not always the one who ultimately bears the burden of the tax. Recall from the lecture that the burden of the tax is shared between the buyer and the seller according to the elasticity of supply and demand. This activity provides you with the opportunity to study four worked out examples that demonstrate how to perform an impact analysis of a tax on a buyer and a seller. After studying each worked out example you will have the opportunity to apply your new knowledge by completing three practice problems.

____________________________________________________________ Learning Goals

Following the completion of this instructional activity, you will be able to:

Determine the effects of a tax in a market

Calculate the tax incidence of buyers and sellers

Analyze the effects of supply and demand elasticity on the tax incidence between buyers and sellers

____________________________________________________________ Activity Material

This self-guide instructional activity includes the following material:

Four Worked Out Examples

Three Practice Problems

Mental Effort Rating Scale

Instructional Materials Survey

____________________________________________________________

Page 158: Florida State University Libraries254119/datastream/PDF/download/...one of three instructional strategies (process-oriented, product-oriented, or conventional problem solving) developed

146

MICROECONOMICS 2023 – SUPPLY AND DEMAND APPLICATION ___________________________________________________________

Activity Instructions

This activity is comprised of two sections. Section One: In Section One, you will study four different worked out examples designed to teach you how to solve an impact analysis problem requiring the application of the following concepts and principles:

Principles of supply and demand

Tax incidence

Supply and demand elasticity After studying a worked out example you will have the opportunity to apply your knowledge on a practice problem similar to the example you studied. If needed, you may use a calculator to complete the calculations. You must show all your work. Section Two: In Section Two, you will complete two surveys designed to evaluate the effectiveness of the activity.

On the first survey, Mental Effort Scale, you will rate the amount of mental effort you exerted while studying the worked out examples and completing the practice problems.

The second survey, Instructional Materials Survey, will require you answer questions about the effectiveness of the activity materials on a 5-point scale. Record your answers on the scantron form.

You must complete both sections of this activity. If you have questions about any of the tasks you are performing, raise your hand for a Teaching Assistant to assist you. After completing both sections of the activity, turn in your booklet to the Instructor.

____________________________________________________________

Getting Started

In the next section, you are presented with four worked out examples and three practice problems. The first two worked examples and practice problem expose you to the impact of a tax on both the seller and the buyer. The next two worked out example and practice problems help you learn about the effect of supply and demand elasticity on the tax incidence. As you work the practice problems, show all your work.

____________________________________________________________ Turn to the next page to begin Section 1

Page 159: Florida State University Libraries254119/datastream/PDF/download/...one of three instructional strategies (process-oriented, product-oriented, or conventional problem solving) developed

147

MICROECONOMICS 2023 – SUPPLY AND DEMAND APPLICATION ___________________________________________________________

SECTION ONE

Record Start Time: ____________ ____________________________________________________________

Continue to the next page

Page 160: Florida State University Libraries254119/datastream/PDF/download/...one of three instructional strategies (process-oriented, product-oriented, or conventional problem solving) developed

148

MICROECONOMICS 2023 – SUPPLY AND DEMAND APPLICATION ___________________________________________________________

List of Key Concepts

The following is a list of the relevant key concepts and principles underlying tax incidence and supply and demand elasticity.

Law of Demand – The principle that states there is an inverse relationship between the price of a good or service and the quantity of it that the buyers are willing to purchase. In other words, as the price of a good increase, buyers will wish to purchase less of it.

Law of Supply – The principle that states there is a direct relationship between the price of a good or service and the quantity of it that sellers are willing to supply.

Equilibrium – The state in which the conflicting forces of supply and demand are in balance. At this point, the decisions of consumers and producers are brought into harmony with one another, and the quantity supplied will equal the quantity demand.

Tax burden (incidence) – The incidence describes the actual burden of who bears the burden of a tax, buyers or sellers.

Demand Elasticity – The degree of responsiveness to a change in price is demonstrated by the steepness of the demand curve. The more responsive buyers are to a change in price, the flatter or more elastic the demand curve will be. Conversely, the less responsive to price changes, the steeper or inelastic the demand curve will be.

Supply Elasticity – The responsiveness to a change in price is shown by the steepness of the supply curve. The more willing sellers are to alter the quantity supplied in response to a change in price, the flatter or more elastic the supply curve. Conversely, the less willing sellers are to alter the quantity supplied in response to a price change, the steeper or inelastic the supply curve will be.

____________________________________________________________

Review of the Impact of a Tax

Economic analysis indicates that the actual burden of a tax does not depend on whether the tax is statutorily placed on either the buyer or the seller. The true burden of the tax is shared between the buyer and the seller according to the elasticity of supply and demand. What determines the incidence or burden of the tax? The incidence of a tax depends on the responsiveness of buyers and sellers to a change in price. When buyers respond to changes in price by leaving the market and buying other things, they will not be willing to accept a price that is higher than it was prior to the tax. Similarly, if sellers respond to a reduction in what they receive by shifting their goods and resources to other markets, or by going out of business, they will not be willing to accept a smaller payment net of taxes. Therefore, the burden of a tax tends to fall more heavily on the side of the market that is less sensitive to price changes.

Continue to Example 1 on the next page

Page 161: Florida State University Libraries254119/datastream/PDF/download/...one of three instructional strategies (process-oriented, product-oriented, or conventional problem solving) developed

149

Example 1: The Impact of a Tax Imposed on the Seller

Scenario: The seller of used cars is interested in selling a used car for any price over $6,000. However, a recent $1,000 tax has been placed on the sale of used cars. The seller would prefer to pass the tax increase on to buyers but recognizes that the higher price may result in fewer car sales. As a result, the seller and the buyer have to share the burden of the new tax. Assume the before-tax price of a used car is $7,000 resulting in the sale of 750 units. Used car prices increase by $400 which reduces demand by 250 units. Determine the amount of the tax burden each party will

bear because of the new tax by following the steps below and their provided rationale.

Answer: In this scenario, the government passed a $1,000 tax to the seller of used cars. Despite the imposed tax on the seller, both the seller and buyer share the actual tax burden. Each $1,000 of tax revenue paid to the government imposes a burden of $400 on the buyer in the form of higher used-car prices and a $600 burden on the seller in the form of lower net revenue received from the sale of a used car.

Continue to Example 2 on the next page

S No Tax

D

Q No Tax

$7,000

750

P No Tax

S + Tax

$7,400

$6,400

500

$1,000

Step 1: Draw the Supply and Demand Curves and plot the price and quantity equilibrium before the tax is imposed.

Step 3: Plot price

consumers (Pc) are

willing to pay after tax is applied.

Step 2: Shift the supply curve upward by the

amount of the tax.

Step 4: Calculate and plot the price the

seller will receive (Pp) net of taxes.

$7,400 (Pc) - $1,000 (tax) = $6,400 (Pp)

Step 5: Determine the tax burden of the seller and buyer.

Buyer’s Burden

(Pc - PNo Tax )

$400

Seller’s Burden

(PNo Tax - Pp) $600

Q With Tax

Pc

Pp

Page 162: Florida State University Libraries254119/datastream/PDF/download/...one of three instructional strategies (process-oriented, product-oriented, or conventional problem solving) developed

150

Example 2: The Impact of a Tax Imposed on the Buyer

Scenario: Continuing with the automobile scenario, a buyer is interested in purchasing a used car. However, recently the government placed a $1,000 tax on the buyer of used cars. Assume the before-tax price of a used car is $7,000, which results in the sale of 750 units. Used car prices increase by $400 for the buyer causing demand to drop by 250 units. Study the following steps and their rationale to determine the amount of the tax burden each party will bear because of the new tax.

Answer: In this scenario, the government imposed a $1,000 tax on the buyers of used cars. Despite the imposed tax, both the seller and buyer share the actual tax burden. Each $1,000 of tax revenue paid to the government imposes a burden of $400 on the buyer in the form of higher used-car prices and a $600 burden on the seller in the form of lower net revenue received from the sale of a used car. The result is the same whether the government places the tax on the buyer or seller.

Continue to the Practice Problem on the next page

S

D

Q No Tax

$7,000

750

P No Tax

$7,400

$6,400

500

$1,000

Step 1: Draw the Supply and Demand Curves and plot the price and quantity equilibrium before

the tax is imposed.

Step 3: Plot price sellers (Pp) receive before the tax is applied. This represents the

new equilibrium price.

Step 2: Shift the demand curve downward by the amount of the $1000 tax.

Step 5: Determine the tax burden of the seller and buyer.

Buyer’s Burden

(Pc - PNo Tax )

$400

Seller’s Burden

(PNo Tax - Pp) $600

Q With Tax

Pc

Pp

D - Tax

Step 4: Calculate and plot the total price

the buyer will pay (Pc) after of the tax.

$6,400 (Pp) + $1,000 (tax) = $7,400 (Pc)

Page 163: Florida State University Libraries254119/datastream/PDF/download/...one of three instructional strategies (process-oriented, product-oriented, or conventional problem solving) developed

151

Practice Problem for Examples 1 and 2: The Impact of a Tax Imposed on the Seller

Scenario: The seller of laptop computers sells computers for $1,000 each. A tax of $200 has been imposed on the sale of a computer. This raises the price of a laptop computer to $1,100. Assume the price of a laptop computer before the tax is imposed $1,000 which results in the sell of 500 units. The increase in price impacts demand negatively by 150 units. Determine the amount of the tax burden each party will bear as a result of the tax. Show YOUR work in the space provided below.

____________________________________________________________ ____________________________________________________________

Continue to the next page for the answer to the Practice Problem

Page 164: Florida State University Libraries254119/datastream/PDF/download/...one of three instructional strategies (process-oriented, product-oriented, or conventional problem solving) developed

152

Solution to the Practice Problem for Examples 1 and 2 Answer: As a result of the $200 tax imposed on the sale of laptop computers, both the

seller and buyers share the burden of the tax equally. The seller bears $100 and the buyer bears $100 of the tax burden.

____________________________________________________________ ____________________________________________________________

Continue to Example 3 on the next page

Page 165: Florida State University Libraries254119/datastream/PDF/download/...one of three instructional strategies (process-oriented, product-oriented, or conventional problem solving) developed

153

Example 3: Elasticity and the Burden of a Tax: Elastic Supply and Inelastic Demand

Scenario: Recently, the government imposed at tax of $0.50 on the sale of a gallon of milk. The seller has to determine whether or not to pass the tax increase on to buyers. Before making the decision the seller needs to consider the impact of higher costs on sales revenue. Milk is a product that buyers have difficulty finding inexpensive alternatives. Therefore, demand is relatively inelastic while there is ample supply. Assume the before tax price of a gallon of milk is $4.60 resulting in the sale of 100 units. As a result of the tax, milk prices increase by $0.40 and demand drops by 10 units. Study the steps below to determine the amount of the tax burden each party will bear because of the new tax.

Answer: In this scenario, the government imposed a $0.50 tax on the seller of milk. In this scenario, demand is more inelastic than supply. As a result, each $0.50 of tax revenue paid to the government creates a burden of $0.40 is imposed on the buyer in the form of higher milk prices and a $0.10 burden is maintained by the seller in the form of lower net revenue received from the sale of a gallon of milk.

Continue to the Practice Problem on the next page

S No Tax

D

Q No Tax

$4.60

100

P No Tax

S + Tax

$5.00

$4.50

90

$0.50

Step 1: Draw the Supply and Demand Curves and plot the price and quantity equilibrium before

the tax is imposed.

Step 3: Plot price

consumers (Pc) are willing to pay after

tax is applied.

Step 2: Shift the supply curve upward by the amount of the tax.

Step 4: Calculate and plot the price the

seller will receive (Pp) net of taxes.

$5.00 (Pc) - $0.50 (tax) = $0.50 (Pp)

Step 5: Determine the tax burden of the seller and buyer.

Buyer’s Burden

(Pc - PNo Tax )

$0.40

Seller’s Burden

(PNo Tax - Pp) $0.10

Q With Tax

Pc

Pp

Page 166: Florida State University Libraries254119/datastream/PDF/download/...one of three instructional strategies (process-oriented, product-oriented, or conventional problem solving) developed

154

Practice Problem for Example 3: Elastic Supply and Inelastic Demand

Scenario: The government has placed a $.50 tax per gallon of gas sold. Unfortunately, buyers have few options to switch to when gas prices increase. Before the tax was imposed, a gallon of gas was $3.60 which resulted in the sale of 200 gallons of gas. After the tax, gas rose to $4.00 per gallon causing demand to drop to 190 gallons. Determine the amount of the tax burden each party will bear as a result of the tax. Show YOUR work in the space provided below.

____________________________________________________________ ____________________________________________________________

Continue to the next page for the answer to the Practice Problem

Page 167: Florida State University Libraries254119/datastream/PDF/download/...one of three instructional strategies (process-oriented, product-oriented, or conventional problem solving) developed

155

Solution to the Practice Problem for Example 3

Answer: As a result of the $.50 per gallon gasoline tax imposed the buyer will bear more of the tax burden than the seller. This is because demand is relatively inelastic. Specifically, the buyer bears $.40 cents and the seller bears $.10 cents of the tax burden.

____________________________________________________________ ____________________________________________________________

Continue to Example 4 on the next page

Page 168: Florida State University Libraries254119/datastream/PDF/download/...one of three instructional strategies (process-oriented, product-oriented, or conventional problem solving) developed

156

Example 4: Elasticity and the Burden of a Tax: Elastic Demand and Inelastic Supply

Scenario: You own a business selling custom made t-shirts. Recently, a tax of $3.00 has been imposed on the sale of clothing. Buyers of t-shirts have numerous alternatives to choose from. Therefore, as the seller you have to determine how much of the increased cost you will pass on to the buyer. For this problem, assume the before tax price of a t-shirt is $20. At this price you can sell 150 units. As a result of the tax, t-shirt prices increase by $1.00 per shirt. Concurrently, your t-shirt demand drops by 50 units. Study the following steps and their rationale to determine the amount of the tax burden each party will bear because of the new tax.

Answer: In this scenario, the government imposed a $3.00 tax on the seller of t-shirts. In this scenario, supply is more inelastic than demand. As a result, each $3.00 of tax revenue paid to the government creates a burden of $1.00 is imposed on the buyer in the form of t-shirt prices and a $2.00 burden is on the seller in the form of lower net revenue received from the sale of a custom t-shirt.

Continue to the Practice Problem on the next page

S No Tax

D

Q No Tax

$20.00

150

P No Tax

S + Tax

$21.00

$18.00

100

$3.00

Step 1: Draw the Supply and Demand Curves and plot the price and quantity equilibrium before

the tax is imposed.

Step 3: Plot price

consumers (Pc) are willing to pay after

tax is applied.

Step 2: Shift the supply curve upward by the amount of the tax.

Step 4: Calculate and plot the price the

seller will receive (Pp) net of taxes.

$21.00 (Pc) - $3.00 (tax) = $18.00 (Pp)

Step 5: Determine the tax burden of the seller and buyer.

Buyer’s Burden

(Pc - PNo Tax )

$1.00

Seller’s Burden

(PNo Tax - Pp) $2.00

Q With Tax

Pc

Pp

Page 169: Florida State University Libraries254119/datastream/PDF/download/...one of three instructional strategies (process-oriented, product-oriented, or conventional problem solving) developed

157

Practice Problem for Example 4: Inelastic Supply and Elastic Demand

Scenario: You are the owner of a bicycle shop that specializes in selling custom youth bicycles. Recently, a luxury tax of $100 has been imposed on the sell of bicycles. Because you sell custom bicycles buyers have numerous low-end alternatives to choose from. Therefore, as the seller you have to determine how much of the increased cost from the tax you will pass on to the buyer. For this problem, assume the before tax price of a bicycle is $500. At this price you can sell 100 bicycles. As a result of the tax, your prices increase by $25 per bicycle. The price increase causes demand to drop by 50 units. Determine the amount of the tax burden each party will bear as a result of the tax. Show YOUR work in the space provided below.

____________________________________________________________ ____________________________________________________________

Continue to the next page for the answer to the Practice Problem

Page 170: Florida State University Libraries254119/datastream/PDF/download/...one of three instructional strategies (process-oriented, product-oriented, or conventional problem solving) developed

158

Solution to the Practice Problem for Example 4

Answer: As a result of the $100 luxury tax the seller will bear more of the tax burden than the seller. This is because demand is relatively elastic. Specifically, the buyer bears $25 and the seller bears $75 of the tax burden.

____________________________________________________________ ____________________________________________________________

Record Stop Time: ___________

Page 171: Florida State University Libraries254119/datastream/PDF/download/...one of three instructional strategies (process-oriented, product-oriented, or conventional problem solving) developed

159

APPENDIX G

MENTAL EFFORT SURVEY

Mental Effort Scale

Using the 9-point scale below, rate the amount of mental effort you exerted during the activity you have just finished.

Circle One

1 = Very, very low mental effort

2 = Very low mental effort

3 = Low mental effort

4 = Rather low mental effort

5 = Neither low nor high mental effort

6 = Rather high mental effort

7 = High mental effort

8 = Very high mental effort

9 = Very, very high mental effort

Page 172: Florida State University Libraries254119/datastream/PDF/download/...one of three instructional strategies (process-oriented, product-oriented, or conventional problem solving) developed

160

APPENDIX H

SUBJECT-SPECIFIC PRIOR KNOWLEDGE ASSESSMENT ITEMS

1. The economic way of thinking is a. a set of historical generalizations that indicates what goods should be produced. b. a body of statistical data that indicates how an economy should be organized. c. a set of basic concepts that helps one understand human choices. d. a set of complex, highly abstract theories that provides persons skilled in statistics

with the information necessary to tell others what choices they should make. 2. With voluntary exchange,

a. both the buyer and seller will be made better off. b. the buyer will be made better off, while the seller will be made worse off. c. the seller will be made better off, while the buyer will be made worse off. d. both the buyer and the seller will be made worse off.

3. Steve values his Honda Accord at $10,000, and Jennifer values it at $14,000. If Jennifer

buys it from Steve for $11,000, which of the following is true? a. Steve gains $1,000 of value, and Jennifer gains $3,000 of value. b. Steve gains $11,000 of value, and Jennifer loses $11,000 of value. c. Steve gains $10,000 of value, and Jennifer loses $14,000 of value. d. Steve and Jennifer both gain $11,000 of value.

4. Three basic decisions must be made by all economies. What are they?

a. How much will be produced; when will it be produced; who will produce it? b. What goods will be produced; how will goods be produced; for whom will goods

be produced? c. What will be consumed; how will goods be consumed; for whom will goods be

consumed? d. How will the opportunity cost principle be applied; if the law of comparative

advantage will be utilized, how will it be utilized; will the production possibilities constraint apply?

5. The law of demand refers to the

a. inverse relationship between the price of a good and the willingness of consumers to buy it.

b. price increase that results from an increase in demand for a good of limited supply. c. inverse relationship between the price of a good and the quantity offered for sale.

Page 173: Florida State University Libraries254119/datastream/PDF/download/...one of three instructional strategies (process-oriented, product-oriented, or conventional problem solving) developed

161

6. In which statement(s) are "demand" and "quantity demanded" used correctly? (I) "An increase in the price of coffee will reduce the quantity demanded of coffee." (II) "An increase in the price of coffee will reduce the demand for creamer used in coffee." a. in both statements I and II b. in statement I only c. in statement II only d. in neither statements I nor II

7. Two goods are considered substitutes if

a. a decrease in the demand for one leads to a decrease in the supply of the other. b. an increase in the demand for one leads to a decrease in the supply of the other. c. an increase in the price of one leads to an increase in the demand for the other. d. a decrease in the price of one leads to an increase in the demand for the other, e. a decrease in the supply of one leads producers to switch to production of the

other.

8. According to the law of supply, a. more of a good is desired by consumers as the price falls. b. less of a good is desired by consumers as the price rises. c. more of a good will be offered by suppliers as the price rises. d. less of a good will be offered by suppliers as the price rises.

9. Which of the following events would increase producer surplus?

a. Sellers' costs stay the same and the price of the good increases. b. Sellers' costs increase and the price of the good stays the same. c. Sellers' costs increase and the price of the good decreases. d. All of the above are correct.

10. In which statement(s) are "supply" and "quantity supplied" used correctly?

(I) "An increase in the price of computers will increase the quantity supplied of computers." (II) "A technological advance that lowers the cost of producing computers will increase the supply of computers." a. in both statements I and II b. in statement I only c. in statement II only d. in neither statements I nor II

11. When there is excess demand for a product in a market,

a. price will tend to fall. b. price must be below the equilibrium price. c. price must be above the equilibrium price. d. producers will reduce output and sales will fall.

Page 174: Florida State University Libraries254119/datastream/PDF/download/...one of three instructional strategies (process-oriented, product-oriented, or conventional problem solving) developed

162

12. If we observe an increase in the price of a good and a decrease in the amount of the good bought and sold, this could be explained by a. an increase in the supply of the good. b. an increase in the demand for the good. c. a decrease in the demand for the good. d. a decrease in the supply of the good.

13. How will an increase in the price of coffee affect the market for cocoa, a substitute good?

a. The supply of cocoa will increase, leading to a reduction in the price of cocoa. b. The supply of cocoa will decrease, leading to an increase in the price of cocoa. c. The demand for cocoa will increase, leading to an increase in the price of cocoa. d. The demand for cocoa will decrease, leading to a reduction in the price of cocoa.

14. Suppose demand decreases and supply increases. Which of the following will happen?

a. Equilibrium price will rise, fall, or stay the same while equilibrium quantity will decrease.

b. Equilibrium price will rise, fall, or stay the same while equilibrium quantity will increase.

c. Equilibrium quantity will rise, fall, or stay the same and equilibrium price will increase.

d. Equilibrium quantity will rise, fall, or stay the same while equilibrium quantity will decrease.

e. The change in equilibrium price and quantity cannot be determined. 15. Adam Smith's invisible hand principle stresses

a. that benevolence is a powerful motivator that encourages individuals to engage in productive economic activity.

b. the tendency of the competitive market process to direct self-interested individuals into activities that enhance the economic welfare of society.

c. the potential of government regulation as a means of bringing the self interest of individuals into harmony with the economic welfare of society.

d. the tendency of self-interested individuals to pursue activities that benefit themselves but harm the overall economic welfare of society.

Page 175: Florida State University Libraries254119/datastream/PDF/download/...one of three instructional strategies (process-oriented, product-oriented, or conventional problem solving) developed

163

APPENDIX I

INSTRUCTIONAL MATERIALS MOTIVATION SURVEY __________________________________________________________

Instructional Materials Motivation Survey Instructions

1. There are 21 statements in this questionnaire. Please think about each statement in relation to the instructional materials you have just studied, and indicate how true it is. Give the answer that truly applies to you, and not what you would like to be true, or what you think others want to hear.

2. Think about each statement by itself and indicate how true it is. Do not be influenced by your answers to other statements.

3. Record your responses by circling the appropriate response. Thank you.

____________________________________________________________ Instructional Materials Motivation Survey

Questions

Circle the Appropriate Response

1 (or A) = Not true 2 (or B) = Slightly true 3 (or C) Moderately true 4 (or D) = Mostly true 5 (or E) = Very true

_____________________ 1. When I first looked at this lesson, I had the impression

that it would be easy for me. 1 2 3 4 5

2. There was something interesting at the beginning of this lesson that got my attention.

1 2 3 4 5

3. This material was more difficult to understand than I would like for it to be.

1 2 3 4 5

4. After reading the introductory information, I felt confident that I knew what I was supposed to learn from this lesson.

1 2 3 4 5

5. Many of the pages had so much information that it was hard to pick out and remember the important points

1 2 3 4 5

6. These materials grabbed my attention.

1 2 3 4 5

7. The quality of the writing helped to hold my attention. 1 2 3 4 5 8. This lesson is so abstract that it was hard to keep my

attention on it. 1 2 3 4 5

9. As I worked on this lesson, I was confident that I could learn the content.

1 2 3 4 5

10. The pages of this lesson look dry and unappealing. 1 2 3 4 5 ____________________________________________________________

Continue to the next page

Page 176: Florida State University Libraries254119/datastream/PDF/download/...one of three instructional strategies (process-oriented, product-oriented, or conventional problem solving) developed

164

INSTRUCTIONAL MATERIALS SURVEY ___________________________________________________________ Instructional Materials Motivation Survey

(Continued)

Questions

Circle the Appropriate Response

1 (or A) = Not true 2 (or B) = Slightly true 3 (or C) Moderately true 4 (or D) = Mostly true 5 (or E) = Very true

____________________ 11. The way the information is arranged on the pages

helped keep my attention.

1 2 3 4 5

12. The exercises in this lesson were too difficult. 1 2 3 4 5 13. This lesson has things that stimulated my curiosity. 1 2 3 4 5 14. I learned some things that were surprising or

unexpected. 1 2 3 4 5

15. After working on this lesson for a while, I was confident that I would be able to pass a test on it.

1 2 3 4 5

16. As I worked on this lesson, I was confident that I could learn the content.

1 2 3 4 5

17. The variety of reading passages, exercises, illustrations, etc., helped keep my attention on the lesson.

1 2 3 4 5

18. The style of writing is boring. 1 2 3 4 5 19. There are so many words on each page that it is

irritating. 1 2 3 4 5

20. I could not really understand quite a bit of the material in this lesson.

1 2 3 4 5

21. The good organization of the content helped me be confident that I would learn this material.

1 2 3 4 5

____________________________________________________________

Page 177: Florida State University Libraries254119/datastream/PDF/download/...one of three instructional strategies (process-oriented, product-oriented, or conventional problem solving) developed

165

APPENDIX J

PERFORMANCE ASSESSMENT FOR ACHIEVEMENT AND TRANSFER

1. Along the inelastic portion of a demand curve, a. the change in price will always be less than the change in quantity demanded. b. the percentage change in price will be less than the percentage change in quantity

demanded. c. the change in price will always be more than the change in quantity demanded. d. the percentage change in price will be more than the percentage change in quantity

demanded.

2. A good that takes up a very large percentage of the consumer's budget will tend to have

a. an elastic demand. b. a perfectly elastic demand. c. an inelastic demand. d. an upward-sloping demand curve. e. very many substitutes.

3. If the price elasticity of demand is computed for two products, and product A measures .79, and product B measures 1.6, then: a. product A is more price elastic than product B. b. product B is more price elastic than product A. c. consumers are more sensitive to price changes in product A than in product B. d. product B is more price inelastic than product A. e. products A and B must be substitutes.

4. If the price of apples increases, total expenditures on apples will decline if a. the demand for apples is inelastic. b. the demand for apples is elastic. c. the quantity of apples purchased is unresponsive to changes in price. d. there are few good substitutes for apples.

5. A 20 percent increase in the price of sugar reduces sugar consumption by about 10 percent. Such a price increase causes households to a. spend less on sugar. b. spend more on sugar. c. spend the same amount on sugar as before. d. consume more goods like coffee and tea, which are sugar complements.

Page 178: Florida State University Libraries254119/datastream/PDF/download/...one of three instructional strategies (process-oriented, product-oriented, or conventional problem solving) developed

166

6. (Transfer Item 1) Suppose a city that operates local electric and natural gas companies wants to raise revenues by increasing its rates for electricity and natural gas. The price rise will increase city revenues if the elasticity of demand for electricity and natural gas is a. less than 1 in absolute value. b. elastic. c. negative. d. equal to 1.

7. (Transfer Item 2) A recent study on enrollment at a liberal arts college concluded that

demand elasticity is 0.91. The administration is considering a tuition increase to help balance the budget. The revenue-maximizing decision is to a. decrease tuition, which should boost enrollment enough to balance the budget. b. decrease tuition, which would bring in more revenue. c. leave tuition as is-an increase would not help balance the budget. d. increase tuition, which would generate more revenue.

8. The number of CDs purchased increased by 5 percent when consumer income increased

by 10 percent. Assuming other factors are held constant, CDs would be classified as a. inferior goods. b. normal goods. c. elastic goods. d. inelastic goods.

9. The costs of a firm indicate the desire of consumers for

a. the product produced by the firm. b. other goods that might have been produced with the same resources. c. goods that can be easily substituted for the good produced by the firm. d. goods that are complementary with the good produced by the firm.

10. (Transfer Item 3) Which of the following is correct?

a. An increase in the number of students choosing to major in marketing would likely lead to an increase in the wage earned by marketing graduates.

b. A new law allowing nurses to offer patients some of the medical services that are currently only available through doctors would increase the wage rate earned by doctors.

c. An increase in the demand for newly constructed homes would lead to a reduction in the price of lumber.

d. New licensing requirements that substantially lowered the number of plumbers would likely lead to an increase in the wage rate of plumbers.

Page 179: Florida State University Libraries254119/datastream/PDF/download/...one of three instructional strategies (process-oriented, product-oriented, or conventional problem solving) developed

167

11. A tax on the buyers of coffee will a. increase the price of coffee paid by buyers, increase the net price of coffee

received by sellers, and increase the equilibrium quantity of coffee. b. decrease the price of coffee paid by buyers, increase the net price of coffee

received by sellers, and decrease the equilibrium quantity of coffee. c. increase the price of coffee paid by buyers, decrease the net price of coffee

received by sellers, and decrease the equilibrium quantity of coffee. d. increase the price of coffee paid by buyers, decrease the net price of coffee

received by sellers, and increase the equilibrium quantity of coffee.

12. Suppose that a tax is placed on DVDs. If the sellers end up bearing most of the tax

burden, this indicates that the a. demand is more inelastic than supply. b. supply is more inelastic than demand. c. government has required that buyers remit the tax payments. d. government has required that sellers remit the tax payments.

13. If the government wants to raise tax revenue and shift most of the tax burden to the

consumers, it would impose a tax on a good with a: a. flat (elastic) demand curve and a steep (inelastic) supply curve. b. steep (inelastic) demand curve and a flat (elastic) supply curve. c. steep (inelastic) demand curve and steep (inelastic) demand curve. d. flat (elastic) demand curve and a flat (elastic) supply curve.

14. If the federal government placed a 50 cent per pack excise tax on cigarette manufacturers, and if as a result, the price to consumers of a pack of cigarettes went up by 40 cents, the a. actual burden of this tax falls mostly on consumers. b. actual burden of this tax falls mostly on manufacturers. c. actual burden of the tax would be shared equally by producers and consumers. d. tax would clearly be a progressive tax.

15. (Transfer Item 4) The federal government currently levies a 15.3 percent payroll tax

(7.65 percent statutorily on both the employer and employee) on the wages of all workers. If the tax were redefined such that the entire 15.3 percent was statutorily levied on employers, economic analysis suggests that the actual burden of the tax would a. remain unchanged. b. shift more heavily toward employers. c. shift more heavily toward employees. d. be different than if the entire 15.3 percent was statutorily imposed on employees.

Page 180: Florida State University Libraries254119/datastream/PDF/download/...one of three instructional strategies (process-oriented, product-oriented, or conventional problem solving) developed

168

APPENDIX K

ACTIVITY LEADER GUIDE

Activity Material

This instructional activity requires the following material:

Instructional Packet (Three different versions)

Scantron form (approximately 500)

Pencil (for scantron)

____________________________________________________________

Activity Instructions

Good morning! As you can see, I am not Dr. Calhoun. Unfortunately, he was unable to be here today but asked me to facilitate your activity today. My name is Darren Brooks. I am a doctoral student in the Instructional Systems Department in the College of Education. I have been working with Dr. Calhoun for over a year in order to improve the effectiveness of his economics courses. Regarding today’s activity, you will continue your study of the impact taxes have on market exchanges between buyers and sellers. By now, each of you should have received an activity packet and scantron form. We have randomly distributed one of three different versions of the activity to each of you. Please note this is NOT a test but rather an opportunity to apply what you have learned about tax incidence and elasticity. Do the best you can! At the end of today’s session, you will have a chance to evaluate the effectiveness of today’s activity. Please open up your packets to page 2. (Show Consent Form on Document Imager) Since we are collecting evaluation data from you on the effectiveness of the materials used in this activity, you will need to sign and date the consent form, which will allow us to use your feedback in our analysis. This activity material is divided into two sections. Section 1 starts on page 5. In this section, you will study several different scenarios, similar to your PRS questions on Wednesday. This will require you to apply your recently acquired knowledge of tax incidence and elasticity. There are simple calculations throughout the activity. You may use a calculator to complete the calculations, if needed. You must show all your work Be sure to record both your start and stop time for section 1

Page 181: Florida State University Libraries254119/datastream/PDF/download/...one of three instructional strategies (process-oriented, product-oriented, or conventional problem solving) developed

169

Activity Leaders Guide (continued) ____________________________________________________________

In Section 2, you will complete two surveys designed to evaluate the effectiveness of the activity.

(Show ME Rating Scale) On the first survey, you will rate the amount of mental effort you exerted during the activity. You will rate mental effort by circling a choice on the form.

(Show IMMS Survey) On the second survey, you will answer 21-questions about the activity materials. Record your answers on the scantron form.

I will alert you when there is about 10 minutes to go before the end of class so you can complete the survey information.

____________________________________________________________

You must complete both sections of this activity in order to receive the participation points for today. If you have questions during the activity procedure, raise your hand for a Teaching Assistant to assist you. Although we cannot answer questions about the subject matter content. Remember, do the best you can. Once you have completed the activity, place your activity packet and scantron form in the boxes found on each side of the auditorium. You may begin. ____________________________________________________________

Page 182: Florida State University Libraries254119/datastream/PDF/download/...one of three instructional strategies (process-oriented, product-oriented, or conventional problem solving) developed

170

APPENDIX L

ACTIVITY INSTRUCTIONS

Tax Incidence and Elasticity Activity

Course: ECO 2023 Microeconomics Section: 01 Instructor: Calhoun

(Note: This slide was presented as students entered the classroom)

Make sure you have the following materials:

o Activity Packet o Scantron form o Pencil

Print name and blackboard username on cover page of the activity packet Complete course, section, instructor name, date, and your name on the

scantron form Do not start the activity early Instructions will be given before you start the activity You will receive double participation points for completing both sections

of the activity At the conclusion of the activity, review the material to ensure you have

completed all parts of the activity and signed the consent page Encouraging word:

“I am a great believer in luck and I find the harder I work the more of it I

have.” - Thomas Jefferson

Page 183: Florida State University Libraries254119/datastream/PDF/download/...one of three instructional strategies (process-oriented, product-oriented, or conventional problem solving) developed

171

Tax Incidence and Elasticity Activity Course: ECO 2023 Microeconomics Section: 01

Instructor: Calhoun (Note: This slide was presented during the study)

Review and sign the consent form (this allows us to use your survey

responses in our analysis) Start Section One on page 5 Make sure you record your START and STOP time for Section One Section Two:

o Rate your Mental Effort by circling your response in the Activity Packet

o Instructional Materials Survey – Respond to the 21-questions on

the Scantron form Pace yourself so you can complete the entire activity to receive full

participation points This is NOT a test – Do the best you can! At the conclusion of the activity, review the material to ensure you have

completed all parts of the activity and signed the consent page Remember you will receive double participation points for completing

the activity Encouraging word:

“Perseverance and spirit have done wonders in all ages.” - General George Washington

Page 184: Florida State University Libraries254119/datastream/PDF/download/...one of three instructional strategies (process-oriented, product-oriented, or conventional problem solving) developed

172

APPENDIX M

EVALUATION OF PREVIOUS ECONOMIC EXPERIENCE Previous Economics Experience:

Your responses to the following questions will provide an understanding of your prior experience in academic economics courses that may have exposed you to certain topics covered in this section of the course.

Select the appropriate response Yes No

1. Have you previously taken an introductory economics course

in high school?

1 2

2. Have you previously taken a college level introductory economics course?

1 2

3. Have you previously taken college level economics course with this instructor?

1 2

Page 185: Florida State University Libraries254119/datastream/PDF/download/...one of three instructional strategies (process-oriented, product-oriented, or conventional problem solving) developed

173

REFERENCES

Alexander, P. A., Jetton, T. L., & Kulikowich, J. M. (1995). Interrelationship of knowledge, interest, and recall: Assessing a model of domain learning. Journal of

Educational Psychology, 87, 559-575. Anderson, J. R. (1982). Acquisition of cognitive skill. Psychological review, 89, 369-406. Atkinson, R. K., Derry, S. J., Renkl, A., & Wortham, D. (2000). Learning from examples:

Instructional principles from the worked example research. Review of Educational

Research, 70(2), 181-214. Bandura, A. (1982). Self-efficacy mechanism in human agency. American Psychologist,

37, 2, 122-147. Becker, W. E. (2000). Teaching economics in the 21st century. The Journal of Economic

Perspectives, 14 (4), 109-119. Becker, W. E. & Watts, M. (2001). Teaching economics at the start of the 21st century.

Still chalk and talk. American Economic Review, 91 (2), 446-451. Bhaskar, R. & Simon , H. A. (1977). Problem solving in semantically rich domains: An

example from engineering thermodynamics. Cognitive Science, 1, 193-215. Britannica Encyclopedia: Online student edition. Retrieved January 19, 2008

http://student.britannica.com/comptons/article-199909/economics Camp, G., Paas, F., Rikers, R., & van Merrienboer, J. J. G., (2001). Dynamic problem

selection in air traffic control training: A comparison between performance, mental effort and mental efficiency. Computers in Human Behavior, 17, 575-595.

Chandler, P. & Sweller, J. (1996). Cognitive load while learning to use a computer

program. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 10(2), 151-170. Chi, M., Glaser, R., & Farr, M. (Ed.) (1988). The nature of expertise. Hillsdale, NJ:

Erlbaum. Chi, M., Glaser, R., & Rees, E. (1982). Expertise in problem solving. In R. J. Sternberg

(Ed.), Advances in the psychology of human intelligence, 7-75. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

Choi, J. & Hannafin, M. (1997). The effects of instructional context and reasoning

complexity on mathematics problem solving. Educational Technology Research

and Development, 40 (1), 43-55.

Page 186: Florida State University Libraries254119/datastream/PDF/download/...one of three instructional strategies (process-oriented, product-oriented, or conventional problem solving) developed

174

Clark, R. C., Nguyen, F., & Sweller, J. (2006). Efficiency in learning: Evidence-based

guidelines to manage cognitive load. San Francisco, CA: Pfeiffer. Clarke, T., Ayres, P., & Sweller, J. (2005). The impact of sequencing and prior

knowledge on learning mathematics through spreadsheet applications. Educational Technology, Research, and Development, 53(3), 15-24.

Cooper, G. A. & Sweller, J. (1987). Effects of schema acquisition and rule automation on

mathematical problem-solving transfer. Journal of Educational Psychology,

79(4), 347-362. Corbalan, G., Kester, L., & van Merrienboer, J. J. G., (2006). Towards a personalized

task selection model with shared instructional control. Instructional Science, 34, 399-422.

Cortina, J. M. (1993). What is coefficient alpha? An examination of theory and

applications. Journal of Applied Psychology, 78, 98-104. Darabi, A. A., Nelson, D. W., & Palanki, S. (2007). Acquisition of troubleshooting skills

in a computer simulation: Worked example vs. conventional problem solving instructional strategies. Computers in Human Behavior, 23(4), 1809-1819

Darabi, A. A., Sikorski, E. G., Nelson, D. W., & Palanki, S. (2006). Efficient,

motivational, and effective strategies for complex learning: Computer-based simulation experiments in troubleshooting. Technology, Instruction, Cognition

and Learning Journal, 3, 233-247. Field, A. (2005). Discovering statistics using spss. Thousand Oaks, CA:Sage. Gerjets, P., Scheiter, K., & Catrambone, R. (2003). Reducing cognitive load and fostering

cognitive skill acquisition: Benefits of category-avoiding examples. In R. Alterman & D. Kirsch (eds), Proceedings of the 25

th Annual Conference of the

Cognitive Science Society. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. Gerjets, P., Scheiter, K., & Schuh, J. (2008). Information comparisons in example-based

hypermedia environments: supporting learners with processing prompts and an interactive comparison tool. Educational Technology Research and Development,

56 (1), 73-92. Halabi, A. K., Tuovinen, J. E., & Farley, A. A. (2005). Empirical evidence on the relative

efficiency of worked examples versus problem-solving exercises in accounting principles instruction. Issues in Accounting Education, 20, 1, 21-32.

Hattie, J., & Timperley, H. (2007). The power of feedback. Review of Educational

Research, 77, 81-112.

Page 187: Florida State University Libraries254119/datastream/PDF/download/...one of three instructional strategies (process-oriented, product-oriented, or conventional problem solving) developed

175

Holladay, C. L. & Quinones, M. A. (2003). Practice variability and transfer of training: The role of self-efficacy generality. Journal of Applied Psychology, 88, 1094-1103.

Jensen, E. J. & Owen, A. L. (2003). Appealing to good students in introductory economics. Journal of Economic Education, 34, 4, 299-325.

Johnson, D.W., Johnson, R.T., & Holubec, E.J. (1994). Cooperative learning in the

classroom. Alexandria, Virginia: Association for Supervision and Curriculum Development.

Kalyuga, S. (2006). Rapid cognitive assessment of learners’ knowledge structures.

Learning and Instruction, 16, 1-11. Kalyuga, S. (2005). Prior knowledge principle in multimedia learning. In R.E. Mayer

(ed), The Cambridge Handbook of Multimedia Learning, (pp. 325-337). New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.

Kalyuga, S., Ayers, P., Chandler, P., & Sweller, J. (2003). Expertise reversal effect.

Educational Psychologist, 38, 23-31. Kalyuga, S., Chandler, P., & Sweller, J. (1998). Levels of expertise and instructional

design. Human Factors, 40, 1-17. Kalyuga, S., Chandler, P., & Sweller, J. (2001). Learner experience and efficiency of

instructional guidance. Educational Psychology, 21, 5-23. Kalyuga, S., Chandler, P., Tuovinen, J. E., & Sweller, J. (2001). Learner experience and

efficiency of instructional guidance. Educational Psychology, 21(1), 5-23. Kalyuga, S. & Sweller, J. (2004). Measuring knowledge to optimize cognitive load

factors during instruction. Journal of Educational Psychology, 96, 558-568. Keller, J. M. (1987a). Strategies for stimulating the motivation to learn. Performance &

Instruction, 26(8), 1-7. Keller, J. M. (1987b). The systematic process of motivational design. Performance &

Instruction, 26(9), 1-8. Keller, J. M. (1993). Instructional Material Motivational Survey. Florida State

University, Unpublished documents. Keller, J. M. (2006). The ARCS model: Designing motivating instruction [Instructional

Booklet]. Tallahassee, FL: John M. Keller.

Page 188: Florida State University Libraries254119/datastream/PDF/download/...one of three instructional strategies (process-oriented, product-oriented, or conventional problem solving) developed

176

Kicken, W., Brand-Gruwel, S., & van Merrienboer, J. J. G., (2008). Scaffolding advice on task selection: A safe path toward self-directed learning in on-demand education. Journal of Vocational Education & Training, 60, 223-239.

Kirschner, P. A., Sweller, J., & Clark, R. E. (2006). Why minimal guidance during

instruction does not work: An analysis of the failure of constructivist, discovery, problem-based, experiential, and inquiry-based teaching. Educational

Psychologist, 41(2), 75-86. Lim, J.H. (2006). Effects of part-task and whole-task instructional approaches and learner

levels of expertise on learner performance of a complex cognitive task. Dissertation Abstracts International (UMI No. 3216512).

Mahan, W. L. (2007). Effect of prior knowledge and principle-based and procedure-

based instructional strategies on students’ transfer of principles. Dissertation

Abstracts International (UMI No. 3266161) Marks, S. G. & Rukstad, M. G. (1996). Teaching macroeconomics by the case method.

Journal of Economic Education, 27, 139-147. Mayer, R. E., (2001). Multimedia learning. New York: Cambridge University Press. Mayer, R. E. & Gallini, J. (1990). When is an illustration worth ten thousand words?

Journal of Educational Psychology, 82, 715-726. Mayer, R. E. & Wittrock, M. C. (1996). Problem-solving transfer. In R. Calfee & R.

Berliner (eds.), Handbook of educational psychology. New York: Simon & Schuseter, Macmillan.

Miller, G. (1956). The magical number seven, plus or minus two: Some limits on our

capacity for processing information. Psychological Review, 63, 81-97. Moreno, R. (2006). When worked examples don’t work: Is cognitive load theory at an

impasse? Learning and Instruction, 16, 170-181. Moreno, R., & Mayer, R. E. (1999). Cognitive principles of multimedia learning: The

role of modality and contiguity. Journal of Educational Psychology, 91(2), 348-368.

Nelson, D. W. (2006). Effects of practice sequence variations on the transfer of complex

cognitive skills practiced in computer-based instruction. Dissertation Abstracts

International, 64 (04), 115. (UMI No. 3216525) Paas, F. (1992). Training strategies for attaining transfer of problem-solving skill in

statistics: A cognitive load approach. Journal of Educational Psychology, 84, 429-434.

Page 189: Florida State University Libraries254119/datastream/PDF/download/...one of three instructional strategies (process-oriented, product-oriented, or conventional problem solving) developed

177

Paas, F., Renkl, A., & Sweller, J. (2004). Cognitive load theory: Instructional

implications of the interaction between information structures and cognitive architecture. Instructional Science, 32, 1-8.

Paas, F., & van Merriënboer, J. J. G. (1993). The efficiency of instructional conditions: An approach to combine mental-effort and performance measures. Human

Factors, 35, 737-743. Paas, F., & van Merrienboer, J. J.G. (1994). Variability of worked examples and transfer

on geometrical problem solving skills: A cognitive-load approach. Journal of

Educational Psychology, 86, 122-133. Paas, F., Tuovinen, J. E., van Merrienboer, J. J. G., & Darabi, A. A. (2005). A

motivational perspective on the relation between mental effort and performance: Optimizing learner involvement in instruction. Educational Technology Research

and Development, 53 (3), 25-34. Pollock, E., Chandler, P., & Sweller, J. (2002). Assimilating complex information.

Learning and Instruction, 12, 61-86. Pretz, J. E., Naples, A. J., & Sternberg, R. J. (2003). Recognizing, defining, and

representing problems. In J. E. Davidson & R. J. Sternberg (Eds.), The

Psychology of Problem Solving (pp. 3-30). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge. Renkl, A. (1997). Learning from worked-out examples: A study on individual

differences. Cognitive Science, 21(1), 1-29. Renkl, A. (2002). Worked-out examples: Instructional explanations support learning by

self-explanations. Learning and Instruction, 12, 529-556. Renkl, A., & Atkinson, R. K. (2003). Structuring the transition from example study to

problem-solving in cognitive skill acquisition: A cognitive load perspective. Educational Psychologist, 38, 15-22.

Renkl, A., Stark, R., Gruber, H., & Mandl, H. (1998). Learning from worked-out

examples: The effects of example variability and elicited self-explanations. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 23, 90-108.

Rotter, J. B. (1966). Generalized expectancies for internal versus external control of

reinforcement. Psychological Monographs, 80, 1, (Whole No. 609). Salden, R. J. C. M., Paas, F., & van Merrienboer, J. J. G. (2006). Personalised adaptive

task selection in air traffic control: Effects on training efficiency and transfer. Learning & Instruction, 16, 350-362.

Page 190: Florida State University Libraries254119/datastream/PDF/download/...one of three instructional strategies (process-oriented, product-oriented, or conventional problem solving) developed

178

Singley, M. K., & Anderson, J. R. (1989). The Transfer of Cognitive Skills. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

Stark, R., Mandl, H., Gruber, H., & Renkl, A. (2002). Conditions and effects of example

elaboration. Learning and Instruction, 12, 39-60. Sweller, J. (1988). Cognitive load during problem solving: Effects on learning. Cognitive

Science, 12, 257-285. Sweller, J. (2004). Instructional design consequences of an analogy between evolution by

natural selection and human cognitive architecture. Instructional Science, 32, 9-31.

Sweller, J. (2005). The redundancy principle in multimedia learning. In R.E. Mayer (Ed.),

The Cambridge Handbook of Multimedia Learning (pp. 159-167). New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.

Sweller, J. (2006). The worked example effect and human cognition. Learning and

Instruction, 16, 165-169. Sweller, J. & Chandler, P. (1994). Why some material is difficult to learn. Cognition and

Instruction, 12 (3), 185-233. Sweller, J. & Cooper, G. A. (1985). The use of worked examples as a substitute for

problem-solving in learning algebra. Cognition and Instruction, 2(1), 59-89. Sweller, J. & Sweller, S. (2006). Natural information processing systems. Evolutionary

Psychology, 4, 434-458. Sweller, J., van Merrienboer, J. G., & Paas, F. G. W. C. (1998). Cognitive architecture

and instructional design. Educational Psychology Review, 10(3), 251-296. Tarmizi, R. A., & Sweller, J. (1988). Guidance during mathematics problem-solving.

Journal of Educational Psychology, 80(4), 424-436. Tindall-Ford, S., Chandler, P., & Sweller, J. (1997). When two sensory modes are better

than one. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 3(4), 247-287. Tuovinen, J. E. (1997). Cognitive load and discovery learning. Retrieved April 2, 2007,

from http://www.aare.edu.au/97pap/tuovj113.htm . Tuovinen, J. E. & Sweller, J. (1999). A comparison of cognitive load associated with

discovery learning and worked examples. Journal of Educational Psychology,

91(2), 334-341. van Gog, T., & Paas, F. (2008). Instructional efficiency: Revisiting the original construct

in educational research. Educational Psychologist, 43, 16-26.

Page 191: Florida State University Libraries254119/datastream/PDF/download/...one of three instructional strategies (process-oriented, product-oriented, or conventional problem solving) developed

179

van Gog, T., Paas, F. G. W. C., & Van Merrienboer, J. G. (2004). Process-oriented worked examples: Improving transfer performance through enhanced understanding. Instructional Science, 32, 83-98.

van Gog, T., Paas, F., & Van Merriënboer, J. J. G. (2006). Effects of process-oriented

worked examples on troubleshooting transfer performance. Learning and

Instruction, 16, 154-164. van Gog, T., Paas, F., & van Merrienboer, J. G. (2008). Effects of studying sequences of

process-oriented and product-oriented worked examples on troubleshooting transfer efficiency. Learning and Instruction, 18, 211-222.

van Merrienboer, J. J. G. (1997). Training complex cognitive skills: A four-component

instructional design model for technical training. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Educational Technology Publications.

van Merrienboer, J. J. G., Kirschner, P. A., & Kester, L. (2003). Taking the load off a

learner’s mind: Instructional design for complex learning. Educational

Psychologist, 38, 5-13. van Merrienboer, J. J. G., Sluijsmans, D., Corbalan, G., Kalyuga, S., Paas, F., &

Tattersall, C. (2006). Performance assessment and training task selection in environments for complex learning. In J. Elen & R. E. Clarks (Eds.), Handling

complexity in learning environments: Theory and research (pp. 201-220). Elsevier.

Zimmerman, B. J. & Campillo, M. (2003). Recognizing, defining, and representing

problems. In J. E. Davidson & R. J. Sternberg (Eds.), The Psychology of Problem

Solving (pp. 233-262). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge. Zhu, X., & Simon, H. A. (1987). Learning mathematics from examples and by doing.

Cognition and Instruction, 4, 137-166. Zook, K. B. & Di Vesta, F. J. (1989). Effects of overt controlled verbalization and goal-

specific search on acquisition of procedural knowledge in problem solving. Journal of Educational Psychology, 81, 2, 220-225.

Page 192: Florida State University Libraries254119/datastream/PDF/download/...one of three instructional strategies (process-oriented, product-oriented, or conventional problem solving) developed

180

BIOGRAPHICAL SKETCH

EDUCATION Doctor of Philosophy Instructional Systems Florida State University Master of Science Instructional Systems Florida State University Bachelor of Science Psychology Florida State University CERTIFICATIONS

Graduate Certificate in Program Evaluation Florida State University Graduate Certificate in Human Performance Technology Florida State University Professional in Human Resources SHRM PROFESSIONAL EXPERIENCE State of Florida, Department of Children and Families 2007-Present Director of Administrative Services, Information Technologies 2009-Present Direct a staff of 17 professionals in the delivery of budget, human resources, training, communications, revenue management, facilities management, and contracts management. Manage a budget of $67 million in the delivery of IT services to agency staff and external providers of social service and mental health/substance abuse services. Director of Human Resources 2007-2008 Direct a staff of 25 human resource professionals in the development and implementation of full-service HR programs and services to a 13,500 employee state government agency.

Directing the re-design and development of a new employee development and job skill training curriculum.

Centralizing the development and coordination of a firm wide recruitment strategy to bring consistency and responsiveness to the strategic recruitment needs of the Department.

Selected to serve as a Steering Committee Member for the development of a State of Florida Human Resource Strategic Plan.

Hewitt Associates, LLC 2005-2007 Learning Design Manager Direct a team of 20 project managers, instructional designers and an additional 20 contractors, who create learning solutions for client organizations as part of Hewitt’s HR MBPO offering.

Direct the design and development of 11,000+ hours of multi-delivery channel instruction per quarter resulting in approximately $2.4 million in annual revenue.

Develop and implement structural alignment of resources, new design/development processes and procedures, and new design and project management systems to operate in a revenue generation model for start up learning design department.

Negotiate vendor and consultant contracts to ensure consistent and quality service for clients. Consult with clients on learning and non-learning interventions to improve organizational

performance. Serve as a consulting advisor to the Hewitt Learning Research and Development team.

Page 193: Florida State University Libraries254119/datastream/PDF/download/...one of three instructional strategies (process-oriented, product-oriented, or conventional problem solving) developed

181

Wachovia Corporation 1997-2005 Vice President, Senior Learning Manager, Learning Design Team 2003–2005 Manage a team of 14 instructional designer/developers, and up to an additional 17 contractors, who create learning solutions in a variety of delivery mediums from instructor led to highly interactive web based training. Accountability for managing an operating budget of approximately $1 million dollars as well as contractor funding in excess of $1.5 million dollars annually.

Directed the design and development of job-skill and soft-skill instruction in support of multiple business lines and corporate initiatives.

Obtained project funding and allocated resources, oversight of multiple project budgets, ensured projects closed on time and exceeded internal clients expectations.

Consulted with internal clients and partners on most efficient and effective learning intervention to improve line of business performance.

Vice President, Senior Recruiting Manager/Director, Florida Region & 1998-2003 Recruiting Solutions Project Manager, General Bank Recruiting (Corporate-wide) Direct the development and implementation of all General Bank recruiting strategies in the Florida region. Train, develop and manage a staff of 11 Recruiting Consultants and 7 Staffing Coordinators and manage a budget of $1.3 million. Also, responsible for the recruitment of exempt and executive level positions.

Direct the recruiting activity that has resulted in an annual hire rate of 2600+ positions for the each of the past 5 plus years.

Designed and developed a process to identify entry-skill gaps and employment trends of new employees in an effort to increase the effectiveness and efficiency of Wachovia’s selection system.

Co-developed a workforce planning model/staffing report currently used in the General Bank.

Developed a monthly diversity candidate/hiring analysis to analyze diversity recruitment flow activity corporate wide.

Designed, developed and implemented the Recruiting Solutions Service Level Agreement, which recruiters use in establishing expectations between Recruiter and line management. Also, designed a template for developing tactical recruitment plans.

Developed and delivered job search, networking and interview training workshops and seminars.

Support the continuing education and development of employees, resulting in 9 employees sitting for the Professional in Human Resources Certification exam in 2002/2003.

Assistant Vice President, Senior Human Resources Generalist 1997 - 1998 Provided broad generalist services, in a multi-site environment, with specific expertise in recruiting and staffing, training and development, compensation and employee relations.

Recruiting Services Design project team.

Consulted with management to develop Portfolio Management incentive plans.

Authored the performance management section of First Union Corporation's Commercial Bank compensation system manual.

Developed and delivered technical and soft-skill training for Florida Commercial Credit managers and employees.

Page 194: Florida State University Libraries254119/datastream/PDF/download/...one of three instructional strategies (process-oriented, product-oriented, or conventional problem solving) developed

182

Barnett Banks, NA (N.K.A. Bank of America) 1996-1997 Senior Staffing Specialist/Team Leader Directed the recruitment operations of 15 exempt and non-exempt recruiters in support of Barnett's Corporate Business Unit and the Consumer Credit Group. Internal consultant to affiliate management on issues relating to the development of recruitment strategy, compensation and salary negotiation, staffing trends, hiring and training practices and provide management training on the use of recruitment as a vehicle to increase employee retention. First Union National Bank of Florida (N.K.A. Wachovia Corporation) 1993-1996 Branch Manager/Registered Investment Representative TEACHING EXPERIENCE Florida State University, College of Education 2003-2004 Instructor/Teaching Assistant for Dr. Robert Branson’s graduate-level course in Change Management.

COMMITTEES/SERVICE Search Committee, Student Representative 2007 – Performance Improvement and Human Resource Development Faculty Position, The Florida State University, College of Education, Educational Psychology and Learning Systems Department, Dr. Rita Conrad, Committee Chair. Instructional Systems, Masters Curriculum Redesign Task Force, Student Representative 2006, Dr. Bonnie Armstrong, Task Force Chair. Volunteer Instructor and Mentor, Florida State University Career Center

PUBLICATIONS

Brooks, C. D. & Jeong, A. (2006). The Effects of Pre-Structuring Discussion Threads on Group Interaction

and Group Performance in Computer-Supported Collaborative Argumentation. Distance Education,

27 (3), pp. 367-377. INVITED PRESENTATIONS Brooks, C. D. (2007). Socio-Organizational Factors Affecting the Use of Task Analysis by

Instructional Designers in Organizational Settings. Paper presented at the Association for Education Communication & Technology 2007 Conference, Anaheim, California.

Brooks, C. D. (2006). Adult Learning from a Motivational Design Perspective. Presented October 31,

2006 to Wachovia Corporation, Charlotte, North Carolina Brooks, C. D. & Slabon, W. (2006). Are you thinking what I am thinking? Explorations in Share Cognition.

Paper presented at the Florida State University Mental Models workshop, Tallahassee, Florida. Brooks, C. D. & Jeong, A. (2005). The effects of structure versus non-structured discussions on group

interaction patterns and performance in computer-mediated communications. Paper presented at the Association for Educational Communications & Technology 2005 Conference, Orlando, Florida.

Page 195: Florida State University Libraries254119/datastream/PDF/download/...one of three instructional strategies (process-oriented, product-oriented, or conventional problem solving) developed

183

Brooks, C. D. (2005, April). Effective project estimation for complex ID projects: Effort hours versus project duration. Guest expert to Dr. Bonnie Armstrong’s graduate level Managing Instructional Development class, Florida State University, Tallahassee, Florida.

Brooks, C. D. (2002 – present).The Art and Science of Interviewing. Training workshop conducted twice a

year for Mock Interview Mentors at the Florida State University Career Center, Tallahassee, Florida. Brooks, C. D. (2004, April). Managing the instructional design process in large organizations. Guest expert to

Dr. Bonnie Armstrong’s graduate level Managing Instructional Development class, Florida State University, Tallahassee, Florida.

Brooks, C. D. (2001, October). Creating a Sales Culture in a Corporate Recruiting Department. Presenter at

the CareerXroads Colloquium, San Francisco, CA. Brooks, C. D. (2003, October). Preparing an effective resume. Job skills seminar for Veterans Administration

counselors and patients. Jacksonville, Florida. Brooks, C. D. (2003, November). Job networking skills. Job skills seminar for Veterans Administration

counselors and patients. Jacksonville, Florida. PROFESSIONAL AND COMMUNITY AFFILIATIONS International Society for Performance Improvement Academy of Human Resource Development Society for Human Resource Management American Society of Training and Development Association for Educational Communications and Technology Volunteer Youth Athletic Coach

HONORS AND ACTIVITIES Recipient 2008-2009 Gagne/Briggs Outstanding Doctoral Student Award, Instructional System, Finalist 2006-2007; 2007-2008 Recipient 2008-2009 Ruby Diamond Future Professor Award, Instructional Systems, Finalist 2005-2006; 2007-2008 Finalist 2005-2006 Gagne/Briggs Outstanding Service Award, Instructional Systems, Recipient 2002-2003 Gagne/Briggs Outstanding (Masters) Distance-Learning Student, Phi Kappa Phi, National Academic Honor Society Eta Sigma Gamma, National Academic Honor Society Psi Chi, National Academic Honor Society in Psychology Finalist, Howard D. Baker Undergraduate Research Award, Florida State University Captain, Florida State University Varsity Cheerleading Team Outstanding College Students of America, 1989-1992 Eagle Scout, Boy Scouts of America