Ertmer & Newby 1993 Teo Apr y Di

  • Upload
    ferucha

  • View
    223

  • Download
    0

Embed Size (px)

Citation preview

  • 8/10/2019 Ertmer & Newby 1993 Teo Apr y Di

    1/23

    Ertmer, P. & Newby, T. (1993) 'Behaviorism, cognitivism, constructivism:comparing critical features from an instructional design perspective',Performance

    Improvement Quarterly, 6(4): 50-72.

    PerfOT mfJlu:e

    Improvement

    Qu.c:wterly.

    6(4)

    pp

    6(} 12

    PeggyA

    rtmer

    Purdue

    University

    Timothy

    J.

    Newby

    Poroue

    University

    bstract

    The

    way we

    define learning and what we believe about the way learning occurs has

    important implications for situations in which we want to facilitate changes in what

    people know and do. Learning theories provide instructional designers with verified

    instructional strategies and techniques for facilitating learning, as well as a foundation for

    intelligent strategy selection. Yet many designers are operating under the constraints of a

    limited theoretical background. This paper is an attempt to familiarise designers with

    three relevant positions

    on

    learning (behavioural, cognitive and constructivist) which

    provide structural foundations for planning and conducting instructional design activities.

    Each learning perspective is discussed in terms

    of

    its specific interpretations

    of

    the

    learning process and the resulting implications for instructional designers and educational

    practitioners. The information presented here provides the reader with a comparison

    of

    these three different viewpoints and illustrate how these differences might

    be

    translated

    into practical applications in instructional situations.

    The need

    for

    a bridge betweenba-

    sic

    learningresearch

    and

    educational

    practice has long been discussed. To

    ensure

    a

    strong

    connection between

    these

    two

    areas,

    Dewey (cited

    n

    Reigeluth, 1983) called for the

    cre-

    ation and development of a linking

    science ;

    Tyler

    (1978) a 'middleman

    position ; and Lynch (1945) for em

    ploying

    an

    engineering analogy

    88

    an aid for translating

    theory

    into

    practice.

    In each

    case, the respective

    author highlighted the information

    and

    potential contributions of avail

    able learning theories, the

    pressing

    problems faced

    by those dealingwith

    practicalleaming issues,

    and

    a gen-

    50

    aral lack of

    W ing

    the former to facili

    tate

    801utionsforthe

    latter.

    The

    value

    of such a bridgi,ng 'function would be

    its ability

    to tranldate

    relevant as

    pects

    of

    the learning theories into

    optimal instructiona l actions. As

    de

    scribed

    by

    Reigeluth (1983, p. 6), the

    field

    of Instructional

    Design per-

    forms this role.

    Instructiona l designers

    have been

    charged with

    translating

    prindples

    ofleamingandinstruction intoepeci

    fications for instructional materials

    andactivities (Smith& Ragan.

    1995)

    p. 12 . To achieve this goal,

    two

    sets

    of

    skills and lm.owledge

    are

    needed.

    First,

    the designer

    must

    understand

  • 8/10/2019 Ertmer & Newby 1993 Teo Apr y Di

    2/23

    the

    position

    of the

    practitioner.

    n

    this regard, the rollowiq questions

    would e relevant: What are the

    situational and contextual con-

    straints of

    the

    application?

    What is

    the degree of individual clliferences

    among

    the

    learners?

    What form.

    of

    solutions will or wiD

    not

    e accepted

    by the

    learners as well as

    by those

    actually teaching the materiaia?

    The

    designer

    must

    have the ability to di-

    agnose and analyze practical learn

    ro problems. Just as a doctor cannot

    prescribe

    an

    eft'ective remedy with-

    out a proper diagnosis, the instruc-

    tional designer caMot properly rec-

    ommend

    an

    effective prescriptive so-

    lution

    withoutanaceurate

    analysiBof

    the

    instructional problem.

    n

    addition to understanding and

    n lyzing the problem. a second oore

    of knOWledge and

    skills

    is

    needed

    to

    bridge or link application with

    :research-that

    of undemanding

    the

    potential sources of80lutions (i.e.,the

    theories of human learning).

    Through

    thie

    understanding,

    a

    proper prescriptive solution

    can be

    matched

    with

    a given diagnosed

    problem.

    The

    critical link. therefore,

    is not between the design ofinetruc-

    tion and. an autonomous body of

    knowledge about instructional pbe.

    nomena, but between instructional

    de ip inues and the theories ofhu

    man

    learning

    Why this emphasis

    on

    leaming

    theory and

    :reselU Ch?

    First, te ming

    theories are a source

    of

    verified in-

    stroctional

    strategies, tactics,

    and

    techniques. Kuowledgeofavarietyof

    such

    strategies

    is

    critical when

    at

    tempting to select

    an

    effective

    pre.

    scription for overooming a given

    in

    struct ional problem. Second, learn-

    ing

    theories provide

    the

    foundation

    for intelligent

    and

    reasoned strategy

    VOLUME 6,

    NUMml4/1993

    selection

    Designers

    must

    have

    an

    adequate repertoi1 6

    of strategies

    available.and possess

    the

    knowledge

    of when andwhy to employ each.

    This

    knowledge depends on the designers

    ability to match the demands

    of

    the

    task with an

    instructional

    strategy

    that

    help; the leamer. Third,

    inte-

    gration

    of

    the selected strategy

    within the instructional context

    is

    of

    critical importance. Learning theo-

    ries

    and

    :research often provide infor-

    mation

    about relationships among

    instructional

    components

    and

    the

    design

    of instruction, indicating how

    specific techniques/strategies might

    best fit within a

    given context

    and

    with specific learners (Keller, 1979).

    Finally, the ultimate role

    of

    a theory

    is

    to allow for reliable prediction

    (Richey, 1986). Effective solutions to

    practical instructional problems

    are

    ofl:enconstrained

    by

    limited

    time

    and

    resources. It

    s paramount

    that those

    strategies selected and implemented

    have

    the

    highest

    chance for success.

    As

    suggested

    by

    Warnes

    (1990), a

    selection based on strong research

    is

    much

    more reliable th n one based

    on

    instructional

    phenomena.

    The tuk of

    translating

    leaming

    theory into

    practical

    applications

    would be greatly simplified if the

    learning process were relatively

    simple and straightforward. Unfor-

    tunately, this is not the case. Learn-

    ing is a oomplex process

    that

    has

    generated numerous interpretations

    and theories

    of

    how it

    is

    effectively

    accomplished.

    Of

    these many

    theo-

    ries, which should receive the

    atten

    tion of he instructional designer?

    Is

    it better to choose one theory

    when

    designing

    instruction

    or to

    draw

    ideas from different theories? This

    article presents

    three

    distinct per-

    spectives of the learning process

    (be-

    51

  • 8/10/2019 Ertmer & Newby 1993 Teo Apr y Di

    3/23

    haviorai,

    cognitive,

    and

    oonstructivist)

    and

    althougheach.

    many unique features,

    it is

    OW belief

    that each stilldescribes the same phe-

    nomena Uearning).

    In

    selecting the

    theory whose associated instruc@

    tionsl strategies offers

    the

    optimal

    means for

    achieving

    desired ont-

    comes,

    the

    degree

    of

    cognitive

    p r ~

    cessing required of

    he learner

    by the

    specific task

    appears

    to

    be a critical

    factor. Therefore, as emphasized

    by

    Snelbeeker

    our

    knowledge

    of

    modem

    l ~

    theories. The mam.

    intent iB

    to

    pm-

    vide dempeN with some familiarity

    with

    three relevant positions on

    leaming (behavioral, cOlllitive. and

    constructivist) which should provide

    a more

    structured

    fooodation for

    planning

    and

    conducting

    instruc-

    tional

    design

    activities. The idea is

    that i we understand some of the

    deep principles

    of the

    theories of

    learning. we can extrapolate to

    the

    particulars

    (1983), indi-

    viduals

    ad-

    dr e s s ing

    prac t ica l

    l e a r n i ng

    problems

    cannot

    af-

    ford

    the

    luxury

    of

    restricting

    themselves

    to only one

    Less

    th n

    two perce ,t of

    the courses offered.

    in

    university cu . ..;.crila.

    in the

    geruJral area.

    of

    educa.ti.onal

    teehnol.ol1Y

    empluuliu

    theory

    a OM

    of heir he

    cOilwept

    as

    needed.

    As Bruner

    1971)

    states,

    You

    don't

    need to

    encounter

    everything

    in nature in

    order to

    know na-

    ture (p. 18).

    theoretical position...

    [They] are

    urged

    to

    examine

    each

    of

    the

    basic

    science theories which have

    been

    de-

    veloped

    by

    psychologists in

    the

    study

    of learning and to select those

    prin.

    ciples and conceptions which

    seem

    to

    be ohalne for one's particulareduca-

    tional situation (p. 8),

    lfknowledge

    of

    he various

    leam-

    ing theories is so important for in-

    structional designers,

    to what

    degree

    are

    they

    emphasized. and promoted?

    As reported by Johnson

    (1992), less

    than two percent of the courses of-

    fered

    in

    university curricula

    in

    the

    general area of educational technol-

    ogy

    emphasize theory

    as

    oneof

    heir

    key concepts.

    It

    appears

    that the

    real

    benefits of heoretical knowledge are,

    at present, not being realized.

    This

    article is an attempt

    to

    fill in

    some of

    the

    gaps that

    may

    exist in

    5

    A basic 00

    derstanding of the learning theories

    can

    provide

    you

    with

    a canny strat-

    egy whereby you could know a great

    deal about a lot

    of

    hings while keep-

    ing very little in

    mind

    (p.

    18).

    It

    is expeeted

    that

    after reading

    this article, instructional designers

    and educat ional practitiOD.eN should

    be

    better informed consumers of

    he

    strategies

    suggested by each. view-

    point.

    The

    concise information pre-

    sented here can serve as an initial

    bue

    oCknowledge for making impor-

    tant decisions regarding instruc-

    tional objectives

    and

    strategies.

    Lean1iD1 Defined

    Learning

    has

    been defined in

    nu-

    merous

    ways

    bymany different theo-

    rists,

    researchers

    and

    educational

    praeti.tioners. Although universal

    agreement on

    any

    single definition is

  • 8/10/2019 Ertmer & Newby 1993 Teo Apr y Di

    4/23

    n.onemtent, many

    definitions

    em-

    ploy common elements. The follow-

    ing

    definition by Shull (as inter

    pretedby Schunk, 1991) incorporates

    these

    main ideas: LeIU'Ding

    is an

    enduring change in behavior, or in

    the

    capacity

    o

    behave

    n

    a

    given fash

    ion, which resulte from practice or

    other

    forms ofexperience (p.

    2).

    Undoubtedly, some

    leaming

    the0-

    ristswill disagree on the definitionof

    learning presented here. However, it

    is

    not

    the definition itselfthat sepa

    rates a

    given theory from

    the

    rest.

    The m ~ o l differences among theo

    ries lie more

    in

    interpretation

    than

    they do in definition. These differ

    ences :revolve around a

    number

    ofkey

    issues

    that

    ultimately delineate the

    instructional prescriptions that flow

    from each theoretical perspective.

    Schunk (1991) lists five definitive

    questions

    that

    serve

    to

    distinguish

    each leamingtheoryfrom the others:

    (1)How does learning occur?

    (2)

    Which

    factors

    n l ~

    (S)What is the role ofmemo:ry?

    (4) How does transfer occur? and

    (5)What types

    ofleaming are best

    explained by the theory?

    Expanding on this original list, we

    have

    included two additional ques

    tions important to the instructional

    designer:

    (S)What basic assumptions/prin

    ciples

    of this

    theory are relevant to

    instructional design?

    and

    (7) How should instruction be

    structured to facilitate learning?

    n this article. each of these ques

    tions is answered from three distinct

    viewpoints: behaviorism. oognitivism,

    and constructivism. Although. learn-

    VOLUME

    6,

    NUM ER 4/1993

    ingtheories

    typically

    are

    divided into

    two

    categories-behavioral

    and

    cog-

    nitive-a third category, construc

    tive,

    is

    added

    here

    because of its :re-

    cent emphasis

    in the

    instructional

    design

    literature e.g.,

    Bednar,

    Cunningham, Duffy,

    Peny,

    1991;

    Duffy

    : Jonassen, 1991; Jonassen,

    1991b; Winn, 1991). n many ways

    these viewpoints overlap; yet they

    are

    distinctive enough to be treated

    as

    separate approaches

    to

    under

    standing and describing learning.

    These

    three

    part icular positions were

    chosen because of their importance,

    both

    historically and cUlTently. to the

    field of instructional design.

    It

    is

    hoped that the

    answers to

    the first

    five questions will provide

    the

    reader

    with

    a basic understanding of how

    theseviewpoints differ. The answers

    to

    the last two questions will trans

    late these differences into practical

    suggestions

    and

    recommendations

    for the applicationof hese principles

    in

    the design

    of

    instruction.

    These

    seven questions

    provide

    the

    basis for the article's structure. For

    each

    of

    the three theoretical posi

    tions, the questions

    are

    addressed

    and an example

    is

    given to illustrate

    the application

    of hat

    perspective.

    It

    is

    expected

    that

    this approach will

    enable

    the

    reader to compare and

    contrast

    the different viewpoints

    OD.

    each

    of the seven issues.

    As

    is

    common

    in

    any

    attempt to

    compare and contrast similar prod

    ucts,

    processes,

    or

    ideas, differences

    are

    emphasized

    in

    order

    to

    make

    dis

    tinctions clear. 'This

    is

    not

    to

    suggest

    that

    there are no similarities among

    these

    viewpoints or that

    there are

    no

    overlapping features.

    n fact,

    differ

    ent

    learning theories will often pre

    scribe

    the

    same instructional meth

    ods or the same situ.a.tions (only with

    53

  • 8/10/2019 Ertmer & Newby 1993 Teo Apr y Di

    5/23

    different terminology

    and

    pouibly

    with different intentions).

    This

    8.1 -

    tide

    outlines the major differences

    between

    the

    three

    positions in an at

    tempt

    to

    facilitate comparison. It is

    OW hope that the reader will p n

    greater

    insight into

    what

    each view

    point offersin terms of he designand

    presentation of materials. as weD as

    the types of les:ming activities

    that

    might be prescribed.

    Historical FoundatioDB

    Current learning

    theories

    have

    roots that extend far into the put.

    The prob-

    B.C.),

    empiricists have

    the

    view

    that

    knowledge

    t

    derived from

    S8W3Ol Y

    impressions.

    Tho

    impres

    sions when associated o n ~ y

    in time and/or space. can be hooked

    together to form compleJ: ideas. For

    ex.ampls,

    the complex

    idea

    of

    a tree,

    as illustrated by Hulse, Egeth, and

    Deese (1980), can be built

    from

    the

    less complex ideas of branches and

    leaves,

    which

    in

    tum

    are built from

    the ideas olwood and fiber. which are

    .built

    from basic sensations such as

    greenn.esa,

    woody

    odor,

    and

    so forth.

    From this perspective, critical in-

    structional

    lems with

    w

    hi h

    today's theo

    rists and

    re

    searchers

    grapple

    and

    struggle are

    not

    new but

    simply

    variat ions

    The

    gO l

    ofiutnuJoon; for

    the behavion,t

    is

    to

    elicit

    the desired

    re poue

    from

    the kamer w o is

    presen ted with a tareet

    stimulus

    design is

    sues

    focus

    on how

    to

    manipulate

    the environ

    ment

    in

    or-

    der

    to

    im

    prove and

    ensure the

    on a timeless theme: Where

    does

    knowledge come

    from

    and

    how do

    people come to know? Two opposing

    positions on

    the

    origins of knowl

    edge-empiricism and rationalism

    have

    existed for centuries

    and are

    still evident, to varying degrees, in

    the learning theories

    of

    oday. Abrief

    description of these views is included

    here as a background for comparing

    the

    modem learning viewpoints of

    behaviorism, cognitivism, and

    constructivism.

    Empiricism

    is

    the

    view

    that

    expe-

    rience isthe primary soUl'ceofknowl

    edge (Schunk, 1991).

    That is,

    organ

    isms are born with basically no

    knowledge

    and

    anything learned is

    gained through interactions and

    as-

    sociations with the environment.

    Beginning with Aristotle (884322

    54

    occurrence

    of

    proper

    associations.

    Rationalism

    is the view

    that

    knowledge derives from

    reason

    with

    out the

    aid of the senses (Schunk,

    1991). This fundamental beliefin the

    distinction between mind

    and

    matter

    originated with Plato

    (e.

    427-347

    B.C.), and is reflected

    in

    the view

    point that humans learn by recalling

    or discovering what already exists

    in the

    mind. For example,

    the

    direct

    experience with a tree duril:lg one's

    lifetime simply serves

    to

    reveal

    that

    which

    is

    already

    in

    the

    mind.

    The

    real

    nature

    of the

    tree

    (greenness,

    woodiness, and otherchar&etemties>

    beeomes known,

    not

    through

    the

    ex

    perience, but

    through

    a reflection on

    one's idea about the given instance of

    a tree. Although later rationalists

    differed

    on

    some

    of Plato's

    other

  • 8/10/2019 Ertmer & Newby 1993 Teo Apr y Di

    6/23

    ideo, the

    centra l beliefremam.ed.

    the

    Ilmle: thatknowledp arises

    through

    the

    mind. From

    this

    perspective,

    structional design issues focus on

    how best to structure new informa-

    tion in order

    to

    facilitate

    (1)

    theleam-

    enood.ing

    of hisnew

    information,

    as

    well

    as

    (2)

    the

    recalling of that

    which is

    already

    known.

    The empiricist, or associationist,

    mindset

    provided

    the

    framework

    for

    many learning theories during the

    first

    half of

    thiIB century. and it was

    against

    this

    background

    that

    behav-

    iorism became

    the

    leading psycho-

    logical viewpoin.t (Schunk, 1991).

    Because behaviorism

    was

    dominant

    when instructional theory

    wu

    initi-

    ated (around 1950), the

    instructional

    design. (ID) technology that arose

    alongside

    it

    was naturaDy influenced

    by many orits basic assumptions

    and

    characteristics. Since ID has its roots

    in

    behavioral theory.

    it seems

    appro-

    priate that we turn

    oW'

    attsntion

    to

    behaviorism first.

    Behaviomm

    Bow o e l I l e ~ o e c v

    Behaviorism

    equates

    learning

    with changes in

    either

    the form or

    frequencyofobservable performance.

    teaming

    is

    aecompUshed when a

    proper response is demonstrated fol

    lowing the presentation of a specific

    environmental

    stimulus.

    For

    Imlple. when presented with a math

    flashcard

    showing the

    equation

    "'2

    +4

    1

    the

    learner

    replies

    with

    the

    an-

    swer of

    "S."

    The equation is the

    stimulusand the proper ruwweris the

    associated response. The

    y

    ele-

    mente

    are

    the stimulus, the response.

    and the association between the two.

    Ofprimmy ooncem

    is

    how the

    8SSr

    ciation between

    the

    stimulW l and

    :re-

    VOLuME 6 NUMl ER

    4/1993

    IllPOnse

    ill

    made,

    strengthened, and

    maintained.

    Behmorismfocu.seson the impor-

    tance of the consequences of those

    performances and contends that reo

    IPO:nses that are

    fonowed by rein-

    forcement are more likely to

    recur

    in

    the future. No attempt is made

    to

    determine the stru.ctu.re

    ofa

    student s

    knowledge

    nor

    to assess which

    men-

    tal

    processes it

    ia

    necessary for

    them

    to use (Winn, 1990). The learner is

    characterized as being reactive to

    conditions

    in

    the

    environment

    as

    op-

    posed to

    taking an

    active role in

    dis-

    covering the en.vironment.

    Which

    facton

    mtluu.ee leanWlg?

    Although

    both learner and

    envi-

    ronmental factors

    are

    considered m-

    portantbybehaviorists, environmen-

    tal conditions receive the greatest

    emphasis. Behaviorists assess the

    learners to

    determine at what point

    to begin instruction

    as

    well as to de-

    termine which reinforcers

    are most

    effective for a

    particular student.

    The

    most

    critical factor, however,

    is

    the

    arrangement of stimuli and conse-

    quences

    within the environment.

    What

    is t.he role of

    memory?

    Memory, as oommonly defined by

    the layman, is

    not

    typicaJ1y addressed

    by behaviorists.

    Although the

    acquisi-

    tion of habits is discussed, little at-

    tention is given

    as

    to how these habits

    are stored or

    rsca1led

    for

    future

    use.

    Forgetting is attributed

    to the

    "nonuse" of

    a

    response over

    time.

    The

    Wile of periodic practice or review

    serves to maintain a leamer s readi-

    ness

    to

    respond (Schunk, 1991).

    ow dou 1 occur?

    Tnmsf er refers

    to the application

    ofleamedknowledge

    in

    new ways or

    55

    behaviorism is

    derived from

    empiricism, a focuson knowledge

    derived from

    environment

  • 8/10/2019 Ertmer & Newby 1993 Teo Apr y Di

    7/23

    situations,

    as

    well

    as

    to

    how prior

    learning affects new

    learning. In be-

    havioralleamingtheories, transferia

    a result ofgeneralization. Situationa

    involving identical

    or

    similar fea

    tures allow behaviors to transfer

    across common elements.

    For

    ex

    ample, the

    student

    who has

    leamed

    to

    recognize and classify elm

    trees

    demonstrates

    transfer

    when

    8)he

    classifies maple trees using

    the

    same

    process.

    The

    similarities between

    the

    elmand maple trees allow the leamer

    to

    apply

    the

    previous elm

    tree

    classi

    fication learning experience

    to

    the

    maple tree classification task.

    What t :ypM

    of

    ieam.ml u e bu t

    expJamed by this position?

    Behaviorists attempt

    to

    prescribe

    strategies that are

    most useful

    for

    building and

    strengthening stimu

    lus-response associations Winn,

    1990), including

    the

    use of instruc

    tional cues, practice, and reinforce

    ment. These prescriptions have gen

    erally been proven reliable

    and

    effec

    tive in facilitating learning

    that in-

    volves discriminations recalling

    facts), generalizations defining and

    illustrating concepts), associations

    applying explanations). and chaining

    automatically performing a

    specified.

    procedure). However, it is generally

    agreed that

    behavioral principles can

    not adequately explain the acquisition

    of higher level skills or those that r

    quire a greater depth of processing

    e.g., language development, problem

    solving,

    inference generating, critical

    thinking)

    Schunk,

    1991).

    What basic auumptiou

    prmcipiea of

    thh I theory U e

    relevant to iutmetionai

    design?

    Many of the basic assumptions

    and characteristics of behaviorism

    56

    ate

    embedded

    in

    CW T8nt.

    instruc

    tional deIrip practices. Beha:riorism

    was

    used as the basis

    for

    many

    of

    he early audio-visual

    mate

    m

    rials

    and gave rise

    to

    many related

    teaching strategies, such as

    Skinners

    teachin.g machines md

    programmed texts. More :recent ex-

    amples include principles utilized

    within

    computer-asaistedinstruction

    CAl) and mastery leammg.

    Specific assumptions

    or

    principles

    that have direct relevance

    to

    inst;ruc..

    tional design include

    the

    following

    possible current ID applications are

    listed

    in

    brackets [ ]

    fonowing the

    listed principle):

    1m. emphasis on producing ob

    servable

    and

    measurable

    out;..

    comes in

    students

    [behavioral

    objectives, task analysis, crite

    rion-referenced assessment]

    9 Pre-assessment ofstudents

    tode

    termine where instruction

    should begin [learner analysis]

    l P

    Emphasis on mastering

    early

    steps

    before

    progressing

    to more

    complex levels

    of

    performance

    [sequencing of Mtructional pI'&-

    sentation, mastery learning]

    Use of reinforcement to

    impact

    performance {tangible rewards,

    informative feedback]

    Use

    of

    cues, shapingand practice

    to ensure a strong stimulut-re

    aponas auociation [simple to

    complex sequencing of practice.

    use of prompts]

    Bow iutnlciion be

    ~ ?

    The

    goal ofinatruc tion for the

    be-

    haviorist

    is

    to elicit

    the

    desired re

    sponse from

    the

    learner who

    is

    pre

    sented with

    a target stim:w.u. To

    aecomplish this, the learner must

  • 8/10/2019 Ertmer & Newby 1993 Teo Apr y Di

    8/23

    knoW

    hoW

    to execute

    the

    proper

    re-

    spmwEI. u well u the conditions un

    der wbicb

    that

    response should

    be

    made. Therefore, instrudion

    is

    structured around the presentation

    oCtile

    target stimulus

    Md the provi-

    sionofoppommitiesfortheleamel'to

    practice making the proper re

    sponse.

    To facilitate the

    linking

    of stimu.lus-response

    pairs,

    in

    struction frequently uses cues to

    initiaUy prompt the

    delivery

    of

    the

    re-

    manager have

    the

    capability to

    make

    the correct

    response.

    However.

    with

    the repeated presentation

    of

    cues

    (e.g completed templates of

    past

    agendas. bltmk

    templates ammgen

    in standard format) paired wi th the

    verbal command stimulus, the man

    ager begins

    to make

    the appropriate

    responses. Although the initial re

    aponsesmaynotbe in

    he final

    proper

    form, repeated practice

    and

    rein

    f ~ e n t s h a p e t h e r e s p o n s e u n t i l i t

    is correctly

    sponss)

    and

    reinforce

    ment to

    strengthen

    correct

    re

    sponding in

    the presence

    of the

    target

    stimulus).

    Behav

    ioral

    theo

    ries imply

    Cognitive

    theone,

    emphmriu mriking

    kMwledge meonillKfu,1

    nd kelpin.g

    learM'I $

    orgell; .

    nd relate

    new

    i n f o n n a t i o l l t o ~ t i n g

    knowledge ill memory.

    executed.

    F in a l l y

    learning is

    d em o n -

    str ted

    when, upon

    the com

    mand to for

    mat

    a

    meet

    ing agenda,

    the

    manager

    that

    b.ejoboftha

    teacher/desiper is

    to

    1)

    determine

    which

    cues

    can

    elicit

    the desired responses; (2)

    liIl l ange

    practice situations in

    which

    prompts

    are

    paired with

    the

    target stimuli

    that imtially have no eliciting power

    butwhich

    will

    beexpected

    toelicitthe

    responses in

    the natmal

    (perfor

    mance) setting; and (3) arrange envi

    ronmental conditions so that

    stu

    dentscan make the

    correcl .

    responses

    in hepresence of hose

    target

    stimuli

    and receive

    reinforcement or those

    responses (Gropper, 1987).

    For

    example, a newly-hired

    m -

    apr of

    human

    resources

    may

    be ex

    pected

    to

    organize a meeting agenda

    according to the company's specific

    format. The target stimulus (the ver

    bal command

    to

    format a meeting

    agenda )

    does not initially elicit the

    correct

    response

    nor does

    the

    new

    .

    VOLUME 6,

    NUMER

    4/1993

    reliably -

    ganizes the agenda according

    to

    com

    pany

    standards

    and

    does so without

    the use of previous examples or

    mod-

    els.

    Copitivism

    In

    the

    late 1950's,

    learning

    theory

    began

    to

    make a shift; away from the

    use of behavioral models to

    an

    ap

    proach

    that

    relied on leaming theo

    ries and mode1s

    from. the

    cognitive

    sciences. Psychologists

    and

    educa

    tors began to de-emphasize a concern

    with overt, observable behavior and

    stressed insteadmore com plex cogni-

    tive processes suchas thinking, prob

    lem solving, language, concept for

    mation and information processing

    (Snelbecker,1983). Within the

    past

    decade, a number

    of

    authors

    in

    the

    field of instructional design have

    openlyandconsciously rejecteti many

  • 8/10/2019 Ertmer & Newby 1993 Teo Apr y Di

    9/23

  • 8/10/2019 Ertmer & Newby 1993 Teo Apr y Di

    10/23

    dift enmce

    can

    be

    dei;ecl;ed

    between

    thee two theories. However. the 41ae-

    tive" utureo he leunerisperceived

    quite differently. The

    cognitive

    8.p

    proachfocuses

    on

    themental

    activities

    ofthelearnertMtleaduptoarespoW le

    and

    aclmowledps

    the processes

    of

    mental plmming. o a J . ~ and

    pnizationai strategies(Shull. 1986).

    Cognitive theories contend

    that

    envi

    ronmental

    Ieues l t

    and instructional

    components alone cannot

    accwn.t

    for

    aU the learning that :results from an

    iMt:rnctional

    situation. Additional

    key elements include the way that

    learners attend

    to,

    code.

    transform,

    :rehearse,

    store and retrieve informa-

    tion. Learners'

    thoughts,

    beliefs, atti

    tudes. and values are also considered

    tobemfiuentialintheleamingp:rocess

    (winne,

    1985). The

    real

    fOCl lB of the

    cognitive app:roac.h

    is on ch nging

    the

    byencourcging him her touse

    appropMte

    le rning

    stmtegies

    What Us the role of memory?

    As

    indicated above,

    memory

    is

    liven a prominent role

    in the

    lear:ning

    procesa. Leamingresultswhen infor-

    mationis

    stored

    in m.emory in an

    r p ~

    Dized. m a n n e : r Teacheml

    designers

    are

    responsible forusisting

    lea:mers in organizing that informa

    tion in some optimal way. Designers

    use techniques such as advance orga

    nizers, analogies, hierarchical rem

    D

    tionships, and matrices to

    help

    learn

    ers relate

    new

    information

    to

    prior

    knowledge.

    Forgetting is the inability

    to

    :retrieve information

    from

    memory

    because o interi'erenee. memory loss,

    01

    missing or madequate cues needed

    to access information.

    B o w d . ~ r ~

    According to cognitive theories,

    transfer sa function ofhow

    infol 1. l18-

    .

    VOLUM

    6, NUMBEl4./1993

    tiOD

    is

    stored

    in

    memory

    (Schunk,

    1991). When

    a

    learner undel'Stlmds

    how to apply knowledge in different

    contexts, then transfer has occurred.

    Understandingis seen as being

    com

    posedo a lmowledgebase

    in

    the form

    of

    rules,

    concepts, and discrimina-

    tions (Duffy&Jonassen,l99 ). Prior

    knowledgeiaused o establishbound

    ary constraints

    for

    identifying the

    Bimilinities and differences of novel

    information. Not only

    must

    the

    mowledge itselfbe stored nmemory

    but

    the

    uses

    of that

    knowledge

    as

    well. Specific instructional or real

    world events will. trigger particular

    responses, but the learner must be

    lieve

    that

    the knowledge

    is

    useful

    n

    a

    given situation before

    he

    willactivate

    it.

    What

    o l l ~

    are

    best

    ~ hJ thlI poRtion?

    Because of the emphasis on men

    tal strnctures, cognitive theories are

    UBually considered more appropriate

    for explaining complex forms

    of

    learning

    (reasoning, problem-sol

    v-

    mg,

    information-processing)thanare

    those of a more behavioral perspec

    tive (Schunk.

    1991).

    However, it is

    important

    to indicate

    at

    this point

    that

    the

    actual goal ofinst:ruction for

    both of these viewpoints is often the

    same:

    to communicate

    or

    transfer

    knowledge tothe students in the most

    effi.ci.ent.

    effective manner

    possible

    {Bednaretal

    1991). Twoteclmiques

    used by bothcamps in achieving this

    effectiveD.eas

    and efficiency of know

    1

    edge transfer

    are

    simplification and

    standardization

    That is, knowledge

    can be

    analyzed, decomposed, and

    simplified into basic building blocks.

    KnOWledge

    transfer

    is expedited i f

    irre levant information

    is

    eliminated.

    For e:mmple, trainees attending a

    59

  • 8/10/2019 Ertmer & Newby 1993 Teo Apr y Di

    11/23

    workshop on effective management

    skills would be presented With

    :Infor.

    mation

    that

    is

    sized and chmlked

    in such

    a way that

    they can

    aumu-

    late and/or accommodate the new in-

    formation

    as quickly

    and as

    easily as

    possible.

    Behaviorists

    would

    focuson

    the design of he environment toopti-

    mize

    that

    transfer,

    while

    cognitivists

    would stress efficient processing

    strategies.

    What basic unmpt iou /prin_

    ciples

    of

    thla

    theol')'

    a n

    NI-

    eVmlt

    to

    atru.dicm.al

    Many

    of

    the

    instructional strat&

    gies

    advocated

    and

    utilized

    by

    cognitivists

    are also emphasized

    by

    behaviorists, yet usually for different

    reasons.

    An

    obvious commonality is

    the use of

    feedback.

    A

    behaviorist

    uses feedback (reinforcement) to

    modify

    behavior in the desired

    direc-

    tion. while cognitivists make use of

    feedback

    (knowledge of results)

    to

    guide and support aOOW'ate

    mental

    connections (Thompson. Simonson.

    Hargrave. 1992).

    Leamer

    and

    task analyses are

    also critical

    to

    both cognitivists and

    behaviorists,

    but

    once again. for dif-

    ferent

    reasons.

    Cognitivists look at

    the

    learner

    to determine hislher

    pre-

    disposition to

    learning, (i.e.,

    How

    does the learner activate,

    maintain,

    anddirecthlslher eanrlng?) (Thomp-

    son

    at at 1992).

    Additionally.

    cognitivists examine the

    leamer

    to

    determine

    how to

    design

    instruction

    so

    that it can be readily assimilated

    (i.e., What are

    the leamer's

    existing

    mental

    structures?).

    In contrast, the

    behaviorists look. at earners

    to

    deter

    mine where the

    lesson

    should

    begin

    ( i.e.,

    At what level are they currently

    performing successfully?)

    and

    which

    :reinforcers should be most effective

    60

    (i.e., What

    ccmsequeneu

    are

    moat

    desired by the learner?).

    Specific assumptioWl

    or principles

    that

    have direct relevance

    to n s t r u ~

    tional design

    include the

    following

    (possible current

    m

    applications are

    listed in

    brackets

    ] fonowing the

    listed principle);

    9 Emphasis on

    the

    active involve-

    ment of he learner

    in

    the

    leam

    a

    ing process [leamer

    control,

    metacognitive traiWng(e.g., self-

    plmming,

    o m t o r i n ~

    and

    revis-

    ing teclmiques)]

    4

    Use

    of

    hlemrehical analyses

    to

    identify

    and illustrate prerequi-

    site

    relationships [cognitive task

    analysis procedures]

    III

    Emphasis on structuring. orga-

    nizing,

    and sequencing

    informa-

    tion

    to facilitate

    optimal proceu-

    ing

    [use of cognitive strategies

    such

    as

    outlining. summaries,

    synthesizers

    t

    advance

    organiz

    w

    ers, etc.l

    $

    Creation of leaming environ

    ments that allow and

    encourage

    students to

    make

    connections

    with

    previously learned

    material

    [recall

    of prerequmte skills; use

    of relevant examples. analogies]

    Bow mould

    t i o D . 'be

    ~ U f t d ,

    Behavioral theories

    imply that

    teachers ought

    to

    arrange environ-

    mental conditions

    1 that

    students

    reBpond properly to presented

    stimuli.

    Cognitive

    theories empha

    D

    size making knowledge m.emingful

    and.

    helping

    learners

    organize

    and

    :relate new

    information

    to eDsting

    knowledge

    in

    memory. Instruction

    must

    be

    based on a studemts msting

    mental structures. or schema,

    to

    be

    effective.

    It should

    organize

    inform .-

  • 8/10/2019 Ertmer & Newby 1993 Teo Apr y Di

    12/23

    tion

    in

    such

    a manner

    that

    learners

    are able to oonned; new information

    with existing knowledge

    in

    some

    meaningful way. Analogies

    and

    metaphors

    are

    examples of this

    type

    of

    cognitive

    strategy. For

    eumple,

    inatmetional design textbooks

    fre

    quanay

    draw

    an analogy between the

    familiar architect s profUsion

    and

    the

    unfamiUar instruetio Ml design

    profMlion to help the novice learner

    conceptualize, organize and retain

    the major

    auimilated

    and/or accommodated

    within

    the leamer s cognitive struc

    ture

    (Stepich

    & Newby.

    1988).

    Conmderthe fonowing

    example

    of

    a

    leaming situation

    utilizing a

    cogni

    tive

    approach:

    A manager in the

    training

    departm.ent

    of a large corpo

    ration

    had

    been asked

    to teach a new

    intern to complete a cost-benefit

    analysis

    for an

    upcoming

    develop

    ment project. In this ease, it

    is

    as

    sumed that

    the

    intern bas

    no pl evi-

    ous experi

    duties

    and

    functions

    of

    an instruc-

    tional

    de

    signer (e.g.,

    Reigeluth,

    1983, p. 7).

    Other

    cogni-

    tive strate-

    gies may

    in

    clude

    the use

    of framing,

    outl ining.

    mnemonics,

    c o n c e p t

    mapping

    advance

    01 -

    Although

    the

    emphui8

    on

    p e r b ~ c e

    end

    imtruetion htH proven

    effective in teeching

    be ic

    .killEl

    in

    relatively

    BtrI.u tured

    knowledge

    domai lU, mueh ofwhat

    needs to be learned

    involves advanced

    luaowledee

    in

    ill

    tructured

    domai:M.

    ence

    with

    cost-benefit

    analysis in a

    bus iness

    s e t t i n g .

    However, by

    relating

    this

    new

    task to

    highly

    simi

    lar proce

    dures

    with

    which the

    intern

    bas

    had

    more ex

    per ience.

    the manager

    ganizers and

    111

    forth (West. Farmer,

    & Wolft'.

    1991).

    Such

    cognitive

    emphases imply

    that

    ~ r taw of the teacher/de

    signer include

    1)

    undentandi,ng

    that individuals b:ring vari01.US leam

    ingexperiences

    to

    the teamingsitua-

    tion

    which can impact learning auto

    comes; (2)

    determining

    the

    moat ef-

    fective

    manner in

    which to

    organize

    and

    structure

    new information to

    tap

    the learners previously acquired

    knowledge. abilities.

    and

    experi

    ences;

    and (3)

    IU'l aDging practice

    with feedback 10 that the

    new

    infor

    mation is effectively and efficiently

    VOLUME 6 NtJMIBR 4/1993

    can facili

    tate

    a

    smooth and efficient assimilation of

    this new procedure into memory.

    These

    familiar

    procedures

    may

    in

    clude

    the

    process

    by which

    the

    indi

    vidual allocates

    his monthly

    pay

    check, bow (e)he makes a

    huy/noobuy

    decision

    Ngarding the

    purchase of a

    1 I. IXID Y

    item or even how one s week-

    end

    spending activities might

    be

    de

    termined and

    prioritized.

    The proce

    dures for such activities may

    not ex

    actly

    match those of the cost..benefi.t

    analysis, but the si:milarlty between

    the

    activities allows for

    the

    unfamil.

    iar

    information

    to

    be put within a

    familiaroontext.

    ThUll

    processingre-

    61

  • 8/10/2019 Ertmer & Newby 1993 Teo Apr y Di

    13/23

    quirements

    are

    reduced

    and

    the

    p0-

    tential effectiveness

    of recaD

    cues is

    increased.

    Coutmctinsm

    The philosophical

    assumptions

    underlying

    both

    the behavioral

    and

    cognitive

    theories

    are primarily ob

    jectivistic;

    that

    is the

    world

    is

    real,

    external to

    the

    leamer. The goal

    of

    instruction

    is to

    map

    the

    structure

    of

    the world onto

    the

    leamel' (Jonassen,

    1991b). A

    Bow

    dOH

    leam.m,

    o ~

    is a

    th ory that

    equatesleamingwithcreatingmeu

    ing

    from experience (Bednar

    et

    aI.,

    1991).

    Eventhoughconstruetivismis

    considered

    to

    be a

    branch

    of

    cognitivism

    (both

    conceive

    of learn

    ing as a mental activity), it distin

    guishes

    itself

    from traditional

    cogni-

    tive theories in a

    number

    of ways.

    Most

    cognitive

    psychologists thinkof

    the

    mind

    as

    areferencetool tu

    eal

    world

    number

    of

    contempo

    rary cogni*

    t ive

    theo

    rists have

    begun to

    qu s t i on

    this

    basic

    objectivis

    tic

    assump

    t ion and

    are start

    ingtoadopt

    a more con

    structivist

    approach

    to

    AB one 1IWve

    aJong

    the

    behavwmt....... opitivi.t-

    COMtnwti vuf

    continuum,

    the

    foeus

    ofin traction

    shills from teach;; g to

    eami:nll, from

    the passive

    believe

    that

    the

    mind

    fil-

    ters input

    from the

    world to

    pr0-

    duce its

    own

    u n i q u

    reality

    Jonassen,

    1991a . Lib

    with t be ra-

    tionalists

    of

    Plato's

    time.

    the mind is

    transfer

    of oc'ltl a

    routi.nes to the active

    application of .deo.s to

    problems

    learning and understanding:

    knowledge

    is a function of how

    the

    individual creates

    meaning

    from his or her own experiences

    p.lO). Constructivism

    is not a

    totally

    new approach to learning.

    Like

    most

    other

    learning

    theo

    ries,

    constructivism

    has multiple

    roots

    in

    the philosophical and

    pay.

    chological viewpoints

    of his

    century,

    specifically in

    the

    works of Piaget.

    Bruner, and

    Goodman

    Perkins,

    IS9l).

    In

    recent years, however. con

    structivism has become a hot issue

    as

    it has

    begun to receive increased

    attention

    in

    a number of different

    disciplines, including instructional

    design Bednar

    at al., 1991).

    62

    believed to

    be the

    source of

    all

    meaning, yet

    like

    the

    empiricists, individual, direc t ex

    periences

    with the

    environment

    are

    considered critical. Constructivism

    crosses both

    categories byemphasiz

    ingtheinteracticm between

    thesetwo

    variables.

    Constructivists do not share with

    cognitivists

    and

    behavi.oriD

    the be-

    liefthat knowledge

    is

    mind-indepen

    dent

    and

    can

    be

    mapped

    onto a

    leamer.

    Constructivists

    do

    not deny

    the

    existence of

    the real

    world

    but

    contend

    that what

    we know of the

    world stems from

    ourowuinterpreta

    tiona

    of our

    experiences.

    Humans

    create

    meaning

    as

    opposed

    to

    6,C Juir.

    m it.

    Since

    there are

    many possible

  • 8/10/2019 Ertmer & Newby 1993 Teo Apr y Di

    14/23

    meuinp

    to

    glean from

    any

    experi

    ence, we cmmot achieve a

    predeter

    mined,

    I I ~ meaning. Leam.ers

    do not tnmsfer

    knowledge

    from the

    extemal

    world into

    their

    memories;

    rather they

    build personal interpre

    tations

    of the

    world

    based

    on indi

    vidual experiences

    and

    interactions.

    Thus, the

    internal

    representation of

    knowledge

    is

    constantly open

    to

    change;

    there snot

    Ul

    objective real

    ity that learners strive

    to

    know.

    Knowledge emerges in contexts

    within which

    it

    is relevant. There

    fore,inordertounderstandtheleam-

    ins which has taken place witbin an

    individual, the actual

    experience

    must be examined (Bednar et at.

    1991).

    Which faeton

    m f i _ D e ~

    oth

    leamer

    and environmental

    factors are critical to the

    constructivist,

    al i t s the

    specific -

    ter ction between these two vari

    ables

    that

    creates

    knowledge.

    Constructivists argue that behavior

    is situationaUy determined

    (JOllallHn,

    1991a).

    ust as the learn

    ing

    of

    new vocabulary words

    is

    en

    banced by eJq)owre and subsequent

    interaction with

    those words in

    con-

    text

    (as

    opposed to learning

    tbeir

    meanings from a dictionary), liken

    wise

    it is essential

    that

    content

    knowledge

    be

    embedded

    in the

    situa

    tion

    in which it is

    used. Brown.

    Collins, and Duguid (1989) suggest

    that

    situations

    actually

    co-produce

    knowledge (along with cognition)

    througb

    activity. Every action is

    viewed 88 an interpretation of

    the

    cUm nt

    situation hued

    on an

    entire

    history

    of

    previous interactions

    (Clancey. 1986).

    Just

    8S shades of

    meaninp

    of given words

    are

    con-

    VOLUME 6, N U M B

    4/1993

    stantly

    changing a

    leamer's

    cur

    rent undenta:nding

    of

    a word, so

    too

    will

    concepts continually evolvewith

    each new use.

    For

    this reason,

    it

    is

    critical

    that

    leaming occur in realis

    tic settings

    and

    that

    the

    selected.

    learning taW be relevant to

    the

    stu-

    dents ' lived experience.

    w.bat

    u the 1'0118

    of memory?

    The goal of instruction is not to

    ensure

    that

    individuals know partic

    war facts but rather that theyelaoo.

    rate on

    and

    interpret

    information.

    Understanding

    is

    developed

    through

    continued,

    situated

    use

    ..

    and does not crystallize into a

    categorical definition that can be

    called

    up from memory (Brown

    et

    al.,

    1989, p. 33). As mentioned earlier, a

    concept

    will continue to

    evolve

    with

    each new useas new situations, nego

    tiations,

    and

    activities recast

    it in

    a

    different, more densely textured

    form. Therefore, memory is always

    under constrocl.ion

    as

    a

    cumulative

    history

    of

    interactions. Representa

    tions

    of

    experiences

    are not

    formal

    ized or st:rm:tured into a single piece

    of declarative knowledge and then

    stored

    in the

    head. The em.phasis

    is

    not on retrieving int ct knowledge

    structures,but on

    providing

    learners

    with the m.eans to create novel and

    situation-specific understandings by

    assembJ.ing prior

    knowledge from

    diverse sources appropriate

    to the

    problem

    at

    hand.

    For

    example,

    the

    knowledge

    of

    design activities

    has

    to

    be

    used

    by a practitioner in

    too

    many

    dift'erentways for them all tobe

    anticipated

    in

    advance. Constructiv

    iatsempbuize the flexible use ofpre

    existing

    knowledge

    rather than the

    recall of

    prepackaged

    schemas (Spi-

    ro, Feltovich. Jacobson, Coulson,

    1991). Mental representat ions devel-

    63

    all this emphasis on

    situated, context-specific

    meaning is very much Gee

    ???

    need for skill-building,

    differentiation, and learner-

    specific knowledge

    activation in a problem

  • 8/10/2019 Ertmer & Newby 1993 Teo Apr y Di

    15/23

  • 8/10/2019 Ertmer & Newby 1993 Teo Apr y Di

    16/23

    edge

    dome.ms

    much

    ofwhaineeds

    to

    be leamed involves advanced knowl

    edge in ill-structured domains.

    Jonassen (1991a) has described three

    stages of knowledge acquisition (in

    troductory, advanced, and

    expert

    and argues that conatru.etive leam

    ing

    envircmmenta are

    most

    effective

    for the stage of advanced. knowledge

    acquisition, where initial misconcep

    tions and biases acquired during the

    introdw::tory stage can

    be discovered,

    negotiated,

    and

    if necessary, modi

    fied

    andlor

    removed.

    JonasssD

    agrees

    that introductory lm.owledge

    acquisition is better supported by

    more objectivistic approaches (be

    havioral and/or cognitive) but sug

    gests I i transition to constructivistic

    approaches

    as

    leamers

    acquire

    more

    knowledpwhich provides them.with

    the conceptual power needed

    to

    deal

    withcomplex Maiill-structured prob

    lems.

    What bale uwmptimulpriD

    cipkil

    o

    thU

    theory

    e

    rel

    evant

    to

    i:utnetioDal deadp?

    The constru.etivist designer speci

    fies

    o n l

    methods and strat

    egies

    that

    will assist learners

    in

    ac

    tively exploring complex topics/envi

    ronments and that

    will

    move them

    into thinking in a given content

    area

    as M

    expert

    user of that

    domain

    might think. Knowledge

    is

    not ab

    stract but is

    linked

    to

    the context

    Wlder study and to the experiences

    that

    the

    participants

    bring to

    thecon

    text.

    As

    such,

    learners

    are

    encour

    aged

    to construct their own under

    standings

    and then to validate,

    through

    social negotiation,these

    new

    perspectives. Content is

    not

    pre

    specified; information from mmy

    sources

    is

    essential.

    For

    example, a

    typical constructivist's goal would

    VOLUME 6 NUMBml4/1993

    not

    be

    to

    teach

    novice

    m

    students

    straight facti about

    instructional de

    sign, but to prepare students to use

    ID facts

    as an

    instructional designer

    might

    u.ee

    them. As such. perfor

    mance

    objectives

    are

    not related so

    much to

    the

    content as they are

    to the

    pro esses

    of

    construction.

    Some

    of he

    specific s trategies uti

    lized by COnli1tru.ctivists include situ

    atingtasksin:realworld contexts,

    USe

    of cognitive apprenticeships (model

    ing and coaching a student toward

    expert

    performance), presen tation

    of

    multiple perspectives

  • 8/10/2019 Ertmer & Newby 1993 Teo Apr y Di

    17/23

  • 8/10/2019 Ertmer & Newby 1993 Teo Apr y Di

    18/23

    such activities

    in

    a new

    light

    which

    guides them towards conceptual

    reframing

    (learning).

    Students gain

    familiarity

    with analysis and action

    in: complex

    situations

    and conse

    quently

    begin to expand their hori

    zons: they encounter relevant

    boob,

    attend conferences and seminars,

    discwas issues with other students,

    and

    use their knowledge

    to

    interpret

    numerous situations around them

    (not

    only re-

    lea:m.ing

    pr OCeSIlI

    itself

    is

    constantly

    ~ both in nature

    and

    diver

    sity,

    as it progresses (Shuell,

    1990>.

    What

    mightbe most effective for nov

    ice learners encountering a complex

    body

    of knowledge for the

    irst

    time,

    would not be effective, efficient or

    stimwatingfor a learner who is more

    fa.miliar

    with

    the content. Typically,

    one does not teach facts

    the

    same way

    that concepts or problem-solving are

    taught; like-

    lated to spe

    cific

    design

    issues). Not

    only have

    the

    learners

    been in -

    volved in dif-

    ferent

    types

    of learning

    as they

    moved from

    being

    nov-

    ices to bud

    ding ex-

    perts,'

    but

    the nature

    of

    What

    might

    be

    mo t

    effective for novice

    leamen

    encoun teri n a a

    comples

    body

    of

    knowledge for the first

    time would

    not

    be

    effective eflkien t

    0. .

    wise, one

    teaches dif

    ferently

    de

    pending

    on

    the profi

    ciency level

    of

    the leam-

    ers involved.

    Both the in

    structional

    strategies

    employed

    and

    the

    con

    tent

    ad-

    dressed (in

    Btimulating for

    a kame .

    who

    i

    more familia .

    with

    the

    con.ten.t.

    the lem:ning process

    has

    changed as

    well.

    Ge:n.eral DiseWMion

    It is

    apparent that

    students ex

    posed to the three instructional ap-

    preaches described in the examples

    abovewouldgaindifferentcompeten

    eiea. This

    leads nstructom/designers

    to

    ask

    two significant questions:

    Is

    there

    a single best

    approach

    and is

    one approach more efficient

    than the

    others? Given

    that

    learning is a

    o m ~

    plex, dl'awn-out process

    that

    seems to

    be

    strongly influencedby

    ooe 8

    prior

    knowledge, perhaps the best answer

    to these questions is

    it

    depends.

    Because learning is influenced by

    many

    facton

    from many sources,

    the

    VOLUM 6, NUMDU 4/1993

    both

    depth

    and breadth)

    would

    very based on the

    level of the

    leaman.

    So how does a

    designerfacilltate i

    proper match between leamer, con

    tent, and strategies? Consider,

    l in t

    of aU, how

    learners'

    knowledge

    changes

    as

    they become more famil

    iar

    with

    a

    given content. As people

    acquire more experience

    with

    a given

    content, they progressalong a low-to

    high knowledge continuum from 1)

    being

    able

    to

    recognize

    and

    apply

    the

    standard

    mies, facts,

    and

    operations

    o a profession (knowing what), to 2)

    thinking like a professional to ex

    o

    trapolate from these general rules to

    particulu,

    problematic cases (know

    inghow),

    to

    S)developing

    and.

    testing

    new forms of understanding and Be-

    67

    confirmation of my

    own prior thoughts

    about the role of

    context in

    determining which

    approach is most

    appropriate

  • 8/10/2019 Ertmer & Newby 1993 Teo Apr y Di

    19/23

    tions

    when familiar categories m d

    ways of

    thinking

    fail (reftection-in

    action) (Schon, 1987).

    In

    a

    HMe ,

    the

    points along this continuum mirror

    the points of

    he

    leamingtheory o n ~

    tinuumdescribedearlier. Depending

    on where

    the

    learners sit on the

    continuum

    in

    terms of

    the d e v e l o ~

    ment of heir professional

    knowledge

    (knowing

    what VB

    knowing

    how VB

    reflectionGin-action), themost appro

    priate insl:ructional approach for ad

    vancing

    the learners'

    knowledge

    at

    that

    particular level would

    be

    the

    one

    advocated

    by the

    theory

    that

    corre

    sponds to that point on

    the

    con

    tinuum.

    That

    is, a behavioral ap

    proach can effectively facilitate mas-

    tery of the content of a profession

    (knowing what); cognitive

    strategies

    are

    useful

    in

    teaching problem-solv

    ing tactics

    where defined

    facts and

    rules are applied in unfamiliar situa

    tions

    (knowing

    how); and

    constructivist strategies

    are

    espe

    cially suited

    to

    dealing

    with

    ill-de

    M

    fined problems

    through

    reflection-in

    action.

    A

    second consideration depends

    upon

    the

    requirementso

    he task to

    be learned. Based on

    the

    level of

    cognitive processing required,

    strate-

    gies from

    different theoretical

    per-

    spectives may be needed. For ex

    ample, tasks requiringalowdegreeof

    processing (e.g., basic paired associa

    tions, discriminations, rote memori

    zation) seem

    tobe

    facilitatedby

    strat-

    egies

    moot

    frequently associated

    with

    a behavioral outlook (e.g., stimulus

    response, contiguity of feedback/re

    inforcement). Tasks

    requiring an in

    creased levelo processing (e.g., clas

    sifications, rule

    or

    procedural execu

    tions) ar e primarily associated with

    strategies having a stronger

    o g n i ~

    tiveemphasis (e.g., C h e m a t i C o r g a n i ~

    68

    zation, analogical

    reuonma

    algo

    ritbmie problem solving),

    Tub

    de

    manding

    high

    l e v of

    prooeum,

    (e.g., heuris tic problem solving, per

    sonal nlecticn andmonitoringofcog-

    nitive strategies) are frequently best

    learned with strategies advanced

    by

    the oonstmetivist perspective (e.g.,

    situated

    learning, cognitive appren

    ticeships,

    social

    negotiation).

    We

    believe

    that the

    critical

    que.

    tioninst:ructional desipersmWlt

    ask

    is

    not

    Which is the best theory? but

    -which

    theory

    is

    the

    most

    eft'ective

    in

    fostering mastery

    of

    specific

    tasks by

    specific learners? Prior to

    strategy(ies) selection, consideration

    must

    be

    made

    of

    oth the learners

    and

    the task. An attempt is made in

    Figure

    1

    to

    depict

    these

    two continua

    (learners level

    ofmow

    edge and cog

    nitive processing demands) and to

    illustrate

    the

    degree to which strate-

    gies offered

    by

    each

    altha

    theoretical

    perspectives appear applicable. The

    figure is useful

    in

    demonstrating: (a)

    that

    the

    strategies promoted

    by

    the

    different perspectives overlap

    in

    cer

    tain instances (i.e., one strategy may

    be relevant for each of

    the

    different

    perspectives. given

    the

    proper

    amount of

    prior knowledge

    and the

    cor:responding amount of cognitive

    processing),

    and (b) that strategies

    are

    concentrated along different

    points of the

    continua

    due to the

    unique

    focus

    of each of the leaming.

    theories. Thismeans

    that

    when inte

    grating

    my strategies

    into the

    in

    structional design process,

    the

    na-

    ture

    of

    the

    learning

    task

    (i.e.,

    the

    level of cognitive

    processing re-

    quired)

    and the

    proficiency level

    of

    the learne rs involved must both be

    conJidered before selecting one

    ~

    proach

    over another. Depending on

    the demands of the task and where

  • 8/10/2019 Ertmer & Newby 1993 Teo Apr y Di

    20/23

    High

    0

    cO

    A

    C

    0

    iii.

    0

    0

    ffigh

    Level

    of CopiUve i l l l e q W r e d

    bytheT.uk

    fisure

    1.

    CompaND" of the t e inmudiooal strategies of the

    beMvioml, mpitWe, and cOMtrudMlt viewpoints baRd the

    le&'lmers level

    of

    wac lmowIedp II'Id the

    level

    of cognitive Pl oceumg

    requfimfi

    by tM

    wk.

    the leamers are

    in terms

    of

    the

    con

    tent to be

    delivered/discovered,

    dif-

    ferent strategies

    based

    on

    different

    theories appear obe

    necessary.

    Pow-

    erful

    frameworks for

    instruction

    have been developed by designers

    in

    spired

    by each of

    these perspectives.

    In fact, succ:essful instructional prac

    tices have features that are

    sup

    ported by virtually

    aU

    three perspec

    tives

    (e.g., active

    participation

    and

    interaction. practice and feedback).

    For

    this reason,

    we

    have con

    sciously

    chosen

    not to

    advocate

    one

    theory over

    the

    others, but to

    mess

    instead

    the usefulness of

    being

    well

    versed in each. This is not to suggest

    that one should work without

    a

    theory,

    but rather that

    one must be

    able

    to intelligently choose, on the

    basis of

    information

    gathered about

    the learners' present level of compe

    tence and the

    type

    of leaming

    tok

    VOLuM 6 NUM BER 4/1993

    the

    appropriate methods for aehiev

    ingoptimW inltructiona loutcomesin

    that

    situation.

    As stated by Smith and. Ragan

    (1993,

    p. viii : "Reasoned and

    vali

    dated theoretical

    eclecticism

    has

    been

    a

    key

    strength of

    our field be-

    cause no single theoretical base

    pro

    vides complete prescriptive prin-

    ciples for the entire design process."

    Someof he

    most crucial

    design

    tasks

    involve being able to decide which

    strategy to use, for

    what

    content, for

    which

    students. and

    at

    what point

    during

    the instruction. Knowledgeo

    this sort

    is

    an

    example of conditional

    knowledge, where "thinking like" a

    designer

    becomesa

    neceasary

    compe

    tency. It

    mould

    be noted however.

    that to be an

    eelectic.

    one :must

    know

    a lot, not a little, about the theories

    being

    combined.

    A thorough

    under

    standingof he learning

    theories pre-

    69

  • 8/10/2019 Ertmer & Newby 1993 Teo Apr y Di

    21/23

    sentedabove seems to

    be

    essential for

    professional designers whomustcon

    stantly

    make

    decisions for

    which

    no

    design model provides precise rules.

    Being knowledgeable about each

    of

    these

    theories provides designers

    with

    the flexibility needed tobe spon

    taneous and creative

    when

    a first

    attempt doesn't; work or

    when

    they

    find themselves limitedby time, u d ~

    get, and/or personnel

    constraints.

    The practitioner cannot afford to ig

    nore any theories that might provide

    practical implications.

    Given the

    myriad

    of

    potential

    design situat ions.

    the

    designer's best approach.

    may

    not ever be identical

    to any

    previous

    approach,

    but will truly depend

    upon the context. his type of in

    structional cherry-picking has been

    termed systematic eclecticism' and

    has had

    a great deal

    of

    support in the

    instructional design literature

    (Snelbecker, 1989).

    Inclosing, we would liketoexpanci

    on a quote by P. B. Drucker, (cited in

    Snelbecker, 1983): These old centro..

    versies have

    been

    phonies all along.

    We need the behaviorist's triad of

    practice/reinforcementlfeedback to

    enlarge learning

    and

    memory.

    We

    need purpose, decision,

    values, un

    derstanding-the cognitive catego

    ries-lest learning be mere behav

    ioral activities rather

    than

    action (p.

    203).

    And

    to

    this we would add that we

    also need adaptive

    learners

    who are

    able to function well

    when

    optimal

    conditions do

    not

    exist,

    when

    situa

    tions

    are unpredictable and

    task de.

    mands change, when

    the

    problems

    are

    messy and ~ f o r m e d and the

    80

    lutions depend on inventiveness,

    im-

    provisation, discussion, and social

    negotiation.

    70

    Wenneu

    B e d I W , A . K , ~ , D

    DUftY T.

    M., Perry,J. D.

    (1991).

    Themymto

    practice:

    How do

    we link? In

    O.

    J.

    Anglin (Ea.), InBtruetional technol-

    ogy: Past. present, and future.

    Englewood, CO: Libmriea Unlimited.

    Bower, G. H., Hilgard,

    E.

    B. (1981).

    Theories

    of

    learning

    (6th

    ed.>.

    Englewood Cliffs,

    NJ: Prentice-Hall

    Brown, J. S

    CoDins,

    A., Duguid,

    P.

    (1989). Situated cognition and the

    culture

    of leaming. Educctional

    Re-

    searcher,

    18(1).3242.

    Bnmer.J.

    S.

    (1971).

    The

    proceu

    ofedu

    cation revisited. Phi Delta

    KGppcm,

    53,1821.

    Clancey, W. J. US8S). Review of

    Winograd a

    Flores untknttmdiNil

    compute1 8

    OM

    cognition: A ftworable

    interpretation. (STAN..cS.87U73)

    Palo Alto,

    CA:

    Department of Com

    puter

    Science. Stanford

    University.

    Cunningham, D.

    J.

    (1991).

    Assessing

    constructions

    and

    constructing u -

    86ssmente: A dialogue. EducatioiU1.l

    Teclmolol3l.

    31(5),13-17.

    Duffy, '1'. M., Jonassen,

    D.

    (1991).

    Constructivism: Newimplicationafor

    instructional technology?

    duca-

    tional

    Teckno/,olJy.

    31(5},812.

    Gropper, G. L.

    (1987).

    Alesson baaed on

    a

    behaviOl'al

    approaen to inatruetional

    design. In C. M. Reigeluth

    M) ,

    In

    structional theories in action (pp. 45-

    112). Hillsdale, NJ:

    LaW1'ence

    Erlbaum Associates.

    Hulse,S.H.,Egeth,H., &Deese,J. (1980).

    The psychology of learning

    (5th eel.).

    New Y ork: McGraw-Bill.

    Jo'lmaon. J. K. (1992). Advancingby de -

    grees:

    Trends inmuter'sanddoetoral

    programs

    in educational

    oommUl'1ica-

    tions and technology. Tech

    Trends,

    31(2), 13-16.

    Jonusen, D.

    H.

    (1991 ).

    Evaluating

    construetiviatic learning. duca-

    tioiU1 l Technology, 81(9).28-33.

    Jonassen. D.

    H.

    (1991b). Objeetivimn va

    constTuct.ivism:

    Do

    we need i new

    philosophical paradigm.

    Educational

  • 8/10/2019 Ertmer & Newby 1993 Teo Apr y Di

    22/23

    Techno10m

    &'fcrcb.

    cmd

    Deuelop-

    ment,

    39(3), 514.

    ~ n e r

    J.

    M.

    (1979). Motivation

    and

    in

    structionai desip: A theoretical per

    spective. o u m o l o f l ~ i o n a l De

    velopment. 2(4),

    26-34..

    Lynch,

    J.

    M. (1945). The applicability of

    psychological research. to

    education.

    o u ~

    of

    Ed.u.t:ational PlYChology,

    48,289-296.

    MenilJ.. M. D.,

    &Walia,

    T., Wilson, B.

    G.

    1981).

    Instructiorml

    design n

    transition.

    In F. B. Farley, I

    N. J.

    GoNon (Ede.),

    PlYCholojJy andeduca

    oon:

    The

    tate of

    he

    union

    (pp. 298-

    348). Berkeley: McCutchan.

    Perkins, D.

    N.

    (1991).

    Technology

    meets

    constructivism:

    Do

    they

    make

    a mar

    riage?

    Educational

    Techno10m .

    31 5),

    18-23.

    Reigeluth, C.M. (1983).

    InstrnctioMl

    Design: What ll i t and

    why

    s it? In C.

    M. Reigelutb (Ed.),

    Instructional

    theories

    in

    action

    (pp.

    3-36). Hillsdale,

    NJ: Lawrence Erlbllnm AellOOiatu.

    Reigeluth,

    C.

    M. (1989). Educational

    technology at

    the

    crouroade: New

    min.tIs

    and

    new directions.

    Educa

    tional Technology Baea rch and

    lJe

    velopment,

    37(1), 6780.

    Resnick,

    L.

    B.

    1987).

    Learning in school

    and out.

    Educational

    Research.er,

    16(9), 1320.

    Richey, R. D.

    1986).

    The

    theoretical and

    coneepmal

    base, of

    instr1.wtional

    de

    sign. New York: Nichobi.

    Schunk,

    D. H.

    1991).

    Learning theories:

    n

    educational perspective.

    New

    York:

    Macmillan.

    ShueU, T.

    J.

    (1986). Cognitive concep

    tions

    of

    learning.

    Reuiew

    of 1.lduca-

    tional Research.

    56. 411-436.

    Shuell, T.J.(l990).

    Plw6lll0fmeaningM

    learning. Review

    ofEducational Re

    search. 60,581-547.

    Smith, P. L.,

    & Ragan,

    T. J. (1998).

    In

    structional deaiBn.

    New York:

    Macmillan.

    hon,

    D. A. (1987). Educating

    the

    reflec

    tive

    practitioner. Sm

    Francisco

    Jouey-Bass.

    VOLuMa

    6,

    NUIvJ IlBR 4/1993

    Sneibeeker,

    O.

    E. (1983).

    Learning

    theory, instructional theory, and

    PlIYchoedw:a.tional

    design. New

    York:

    McGraw-Hill.

    Snelbecker,

    G. E.

    (1989). Contrasting

    and

    complementary approaches to

    in

    structioMl

    design.. In

    C. M. Reigeluth

  • 8/10/2019 Ertmer & Newby 1993 Teo Apr y Di

    23/23

    Winne, P.

    H.

    (1985). Cognitive

    proc:euing

    in the

    claslll OOm.

    InT.Husen

    6;

    T.N.

    Postlethwaite

    (Eels.), The

    In rM-

    tionalEncyclopediaof Education

    (Vol.

    2, pp. 795-8(8). Oxford: Pergamon.

    PEGGYA ERTMEBhuaMuters

    Degree in Special Education-

    Learning Disabilities

    and

    is cur

    rently a doctoral student n In

    structional Development. otUne

    address: Educational Computing

    and Instroctional Research and

    Development.

    1442

    L E B ~

    Room

    3134, Purdue University. West

    Lafayette, IN 47907-1442.

    72

    fIMOTBY

    J

    N E Q f

    hi

    an

    uooi-

    ate Professor n Educational Com-

    puting

    and

    Instru.etiOMl

    Develop-

    ment

    at

    Purdue University. He

    CWTently conducts research

    n the

    areas

    of

    humanmotivation

    and

    in-

    structional strategies.

    ailinead

    dress: Educational Computingand

    Instructional Development, 1442

    LAEB,

    Room

    3146. Purdue Univer

    sity,

    West Lafayette, IN 47907-

    1442.