39
ion of Learning : ning learning we could refer simply to overt behavi , if I see you riding a bicycle I can assume that y that skill. PSYCHOLOGY 2250 LEARNING

Definition of Learning: In defining learning we could refer simply to overt behavior. For ex., if I see you riding a bicycle I can assume that you’ve learned

Embed Size (px)

Citation preview

Page 1: Definition of Learning: In defining learning we could refer simply to overt behavior. For ex., if I see you riding a bicycle I can assume that you’ve learned

Definition of Learning:

In defining learning we could refer simply to overt behavior.

For ex., if I see you riding a bicycle I can assume that you’ve learned that skill.

PSYCHOLOGY 2250LEARNING

Page 2: Definition of Learning: In defining learning we could refer simply to overt behavior. For ex., if I see you riding a bicycle I can assume that you’ve learned

A good definition of learning should have both components: Overt behavior and internal state of knowledge

Definition of Learning:

In defining learning we could also refer to an internal state of knowledge.

For ex., you all know the 10 provinces (but I can’t tell that just by looking at you).

So, behavior, or performance, is important.

Page 3: Definition of Learning: In defining learning we could refer simply to overt behavior. For ex., if I see you riding a bicycle I can assume that you’ve learned

Learning as knowledge acquisition

Animals learn about stimuli in their environmentSuch stimuli serve as signals for some important outcome

for ex., a particular odor could indicate that there is food or a predator nearby.

Animals also learn about their own behaviorA certain action will produce a particular outcome for ex., running to escape from a predator

Usually both types of knowledge occur together. For ex., an animal detects a certain odor that tells it a predator is nearby and this odor evokes an escape response, namely running, to avoid being attacked by the predator.

Page 4: Definition of Learning: In defining learning we could refer simply to overt behavior. For ex., if I see you riding a bicycle I can assume that you’ve learned

Learning: is an inferred change in the organisms’ mental state which results from experience and which influences in a relatively permanent fashion the organisms’ potential for subsequent adaptive behavior.

Why is learning defined this way?

Page 5: Definition of Learning: In defining learning we could refer simply to overt behavior. For ex., if I see you riding a bicycle I can assume that you’ve learned

1. Learning is inferred from performance. If there is no behavior to observe then we can’t say for sure

whether or not learning has occurred.

2. Learning involves a change in the mental state of an organism We can’t see the neurological structures that underlie this

mental state but, in theory, they must exist. Acquired knowledge must somehow be coded or represented in the brain.

3. Learning stems from experience This distinguishes learning from instinct, which refers to

behaviors present at birth (i.e., imprinting in certain species of birds)

Key components to definition of Learning

Page 6: Definition of Learning: In defining learning we could refer simply to overt behavior. For ex., if I see you riding a bicycle I can assume that you’ve learned

Key components to definition of Learning

4. Learning is relatively permanent Learning persists through time. This part of the definition

guards against mistaking a temporary change in behavior, due to fatigue for example, for real learning.

5. Learning is a change in the potential to behave. An animal could acquire knowledge and yet not perform in

such a way as to demonstrate that knowledge. The organism could have the potential to behave even though the behavior is not occurring.

Page 7: Definition of Learning: In defining learning we could refer simply to overt behavior. For ex., if I see you riding a bicycle I can assume that you’ve learned

Other points:Learning is assessed as a change in behavior —

this could mean an increase or decrease in behaviorFor ex., In presence of light — barpress for food

In presence of tone — barpress for shock

What other things could influence behavior or performance (other than learning)?

Fatigue — temporary change so not learningMotivation — maze example; learning occurs but rat not motivated to performMaturation — could also affect performance but wouldn’t call it learning (i.e., kid reaching cookie jar on top of counter)

Page 8: Definition of Learning: In defining learning we could refer simply to overt behavior. For ex., if I see you riding a bicycle I can assume that you’ve learned

Domjan’s definition of Learning:

Learning is an enduring change in the mechanisms of behavior involving specific stimuli and/or responses that results from prior experience with those or similar stimuli and responses (p. 14)

Emphasize distinction between learning and performancefatiguematurationmotivation

Page 9: Definition of Learning: In defining learning we could refer simply to overt behavior. For ex., if I see you riding a bicycle I can assume that you’ve learned

Much of behavior occurs in response to stimuli, that is, it is elicited, as opposed to spontaneously produced.

The simplest form of elicited behavior is reflexive behavior. -knee-jerk reflex -a loud noise causes a startle response -puff of air at the cornea makes the eye blink

These are all reflexes.

Habituation and Sensitization

Page 10: Definition of Learning: In defining learning we could refer simply to overt behavior. For ex., if I see you riding a bicycle I can assume that you’ve learned

A reflex involves 2 closely related events:-eliciting stimulus-corresponding response

The response and stimulus are linked-presentation of the stimulus leads to the response and the response rarely occurs in the absence of the stimulus (i.e., you don’t go around kicking your leg out unless someone taps you on the knee).

Page 11: Definition of Learning: In defining learning we could refer simply to overt behavior. For ex., if I see you riding a bicycle I can assume that you’ve learned

The specificity of the relation between stimulus and response is a consequence of the organization of the nervous system.

Simple reflexes involve three neurons:

(1). Sensory— afferent— to spinal cord(2). Motor— efferent— to muscles(3). Interneuron — sensory and motor neurons often don’t communicate directly.

This is known as the reflex arc (see Fig. 2.1)

Page 12: Definition of Learning: In defining learning we could refer simply to overt behavior. For ex., if I see you riding a bicycle I can assume that you’ve learned
Page 13: Definition of Learning: In defining learning we could refer simply to overt behavior. For ex., if I see you riding a bicycle I can assume that you’ve learned

Other forms of elicited behavior

Two of the simplest and most common forms of behavioral change are:

(1). Habituation—defined as a progressive decrease in the vigor of an elicited response that may occur with repeated presentations of the eliciting stimulus.

(2). Sensitization— defined as an increase in the vigor of elicited behavior that may result from repeated presentations of the eliciting stimulus.

Page 14: Definition of Learning: In defining learning we could refer simply to overt behavior. For ex., if I see you riding a bicycle I can assume that you’ve learned

Habituation and Sensitization occur in a wide variety of response systems and are therefore fundamental properties of behavior.

Because elicited behavior involves a very close relationship between the eliciting stimulus and the resulting response, people often think that the behavior is invariant, or fixed.

The common assumption is that elicited behavior will occur the same way every time the stimulus is presented--not true!

Behavior is plastic — it changes— it does not occur the same way every time.

Page 15: Definition of Learning: In defining learning we could refer simply to overt behavior. For ex., if I see you riding a bicycle I can assume that you’ve learned

Procedures used to study Habituation and Sensitization(examples of repeated stimulation):

1. Visual attention in human infantsIn babies, visual cues elicit a looking response which can

be measured by how long an infant keeps her eye on one object before shifting her gaze.

A study by Bashinski, Werner & Rudy (1985)-described on p. 37 of Domjan.

2. Startle response in ratsA study by Davis (1974)-described on p.38 of Domjan

The startle response is a defensive response in many species(i.e., present loud noise, you jump).

Page 16: Definition of Learning: In defining learning we could refer simply to overt behavior. For ex., if I see you riding a bicycle I can assume that you’ve learned

In rats, we can measure startle in a stabilimeter chamber

Rat jumps, chamber bounces and sensors detect the amount of movement

Page 17: Definition of Learning: In defining learning we could refer simply to overt behavior. For ex., if I see you riding a bicycle I can assume that you’ve learned

Davis investigated startle in rats by presenting a loud tone.

2 groups of rats-each received 100 tones at 30 sec intervals (110-dB)-noise generator that provided background noise

Group 1 Group 2soft background noise loud background noise (60-dB) (80-dB; not as intense as the tone)

Page 18: Definition of Learning: In defining learning we could refer simply to overt behavior. For ex., if I see you riding a bicycle I can assume that you’ve learned

Repeated presentations of the tone did not always elicit the same response

With soft background noise, repetitions of the tone resulted in weaker startle response (i.e., habituation)

In contrast, when the background noise was louder, repetitions of the tone resulted in a bigger startle response (i.e., sensitization)

With the same tone, see 2 different patterns depending on other circumstances.

Results:

Page 19: Definition of Learning: In defining learning we could refer simply to overt behavior. For ex., if I see you riding a bicycle I can assume that you’ve learned
Page 20: Definition of Learning: In defining learning we could refer simply to overt behavior. For ex., if I see you riding a bicycle I can assume that you’ve learned

These 2 studies show that increases or decreases in responding can occur with repeated presentations of stimuli

Decreases in responsiveness by repeated stimulation = HabituationIncreases in responsiveness by repeated stimulation = Sensitization

Lots of everyday example: grandfather clocks, trains

Page 21: Definition of Learning: In defining learning we could refer simply to overt behavior. For ex., if I see you riding a bicycle I can assume that you’ve learned

Must distinguish habituation from:

response fatigue sensory adaptationmuscles become incapacitated sense organs becomeby fatigue temporarily insensitive

(i.e.,won’t respond to visual cues if you’re temporarily blinded by a bright light)

Habituation is a decline in the response that was initially elicited by a stimulus

However, habituation is not the only effect that can produce a decrease in response

Page 22: Definition of Learning: In defining learning we could refer simply to overt behavior. For ex., if I see you riding a bicycle I can assume that you’ve learned

Habituation is stimulus-specific-if you present a different stimulus, the animal will make the response-shows that they are not fatigued if they can still make the response-rules out response fatigue

Habituation is response-specific-an animal may stop responding to a stimulus in one aspectof its behavior but continue to respond in other ways -e.g., orienting response to mother’s voice may habituate butstill listen to what she is saying-rules out sensory adaptation

Page 23: Definition of Learning: In defining learning we could refer simply to overt behavior. For ex., if I see you riding a bicycle I can assume that you’ve learned

Sense organ

Muscle

CentralNervousSystem

Site of sensoryadaption

Site of responsefatigue

Site of habituationand sensitization

Sensoryneuron

Motorneuron

Page 24: Definition of Learning: In defining learning we could refer simply to overt behavior. For ex., if I see you riding a bicycle I can assume that you’ve learned

Habituation and Sensitization effects are changes in behavior or performance

But what factors are responsible for such changes?

The Dual-Process Theory (Groves & Thompson, 1970) was an attempt to get at this issue

The DPT assumes that different types of underlying neural processes are responsible for increases and decreases in response to stimuli.

Dual-Process Theory

Page 25: Definition of Learning: In defining learning we could refer simply to overt behavior. For ex., if I see you riding a bicycle I can assume that you’ve learned

The habituation process produces decreases in responding

The sensitization process produces increases in responding

These 2 processes are not mutually exclusive— they may be activated at the same time.

The behavioral outcome depends on which process is stronger.

Page 26: Definition of Learning: In defining learning we could refer simply to overt behavior. For ex., if I see you riding a bicycle I can assume that you’ve learned
Page 27: Definition of Learning: In defining learning we could refer simply to overt behavior. For ex., if I see you riding a bicycle I can assume that you’ve learned

Net effect = summation of habituation and sensitization processes(not to be confused with habituation and sensitization effects)

process = underlying neural process/mechanismeffect = behavior (what you actually observe)

if you observe habituation, might still have sensitization process activated, but its not very strong

Page 28: Definition of Learning: In defining learning we could refer simply to overt behavior. For ex., if I see you riding a bicycle I can assume that you’ve learned

Groves & Thompson suggested that habituation and sensitization processes occur in different parts of the nervous system.

Habituation processes are assumed to occur in the S-R system-the shortest neural path connecting the stimulus

and the response (sense organs and muscles)-similar to the reflex arc

Each presentation of the stimulus activates the S-R system and causes some build up of habituation

Page 29: Definition of Learning: In defining learning we could refer simply to overt behavior. For ex., if I see you riding a bicycle I can assume that you’ve learned

Sensitization processes are assumed to occur in the state system -this system consists of other parts of the nervous system

that determine the organism’s general level of responsiveness or readiness to respond

-only arousing events activate state system; not necessarily activated with every stimulus presentation.

Page 30: Definition of Learning: In defining learning we could refer simply to overt behavior. For ex., if I see you riding a bicycle I can assume that you’ve learned

The state system determines the animal’s readiness to respond, whereas the S-R system enables the animal to make the specific response that is elicited by the particular stimulus

Changes in behavior that occur with repeated presentations of a stimulus reflect the combined actions of the S-R and state systems

Page 31: Definition of Learning: In defining learning we could refer simply to overt behavior. For ex., if I see you riding a bicycle I can assume that you’ve learned
Page 32: Definition of Learning: In defining learning we could refer simply to overt behavior. For ex., if I see you riding a bicycle I can assume that you’ve learned

Back to Startle Response in rats:

When the rats were tested with the quiet background noise,there was little to arouse them – the state system was probably not activated

- repeated presentations of the tone activated only the S-R system and the result was habituation of the startle response

When the rats were tested with the loud background noise, the state system was activated and the result was an increase in the startleresponse to the same tone.

Page 33: Definition of Learning: In defining learning we could refer simply to overt behavior. For ex., if I see you riding a bicycle I can assume that you’ve learned

The State and S-R systems are activated differently by repeatedpresentations of a stimulus

The S-R system is activated every time a stimulus elicits a response-it is the neural circuit that conducts impulses from sensoryinput to response output

The state system only becomes involved in special circumstances-e.g., when stimulus is intense

Page 34: Definition of Learning: In defining learning we could refer simply to overt behavior. For ex., if I see you riding a bicycle I can assume that you’ve learned

Characteristics of Habituation and Sensitization

Time course

Sensitization is usually temporary-sensitization can last for up to a week but not generally a long-term effect. -with a stronger stimulus, the effects last longer.

Habituation can be short-term or long-term, depending on presentation and interval between stimuli.

Page 35: Definition of Learning: In defining learning we could refer simply to overt behavior. For ex., if I see you riding a bicycle I can assume that you’ve learned

Short-term habituation:-rapid presentations of a stimulus with a short interval between presentations-results in habituation quickly but see spontaneous recovery-the degree of spontaneous recovery depends on length of rest interval.

Long-term habituation:-one stimulus presentation a day -see more long-term effects-see less spontaneous recovery

Page 36: Definition of Learning: In defining learning we could refer simply to overt behavior. For ex., if I see you riding a bicycle I can assume that you’ve learned

Tones Blocks of 30 Tones Tones

Tones Once a DayTones Every 3 seconds on 1 day

Tones Once a Day

Leaton, 1976; see page 46 of Domjan

Page 37: Definition of Learning: In defining learning we could refer simply to overt behavior. For ex., if I see you riding a bicycle I can assume that you’ve learned

Stimulus specificity

Habituation is stimulus-specific-if you change the stimulus, see recovery of the response

Sensitization is not highly stimulus-specific -if an animal is aroused, it is usually aroused to a variety of cues

Page 38: Definition of Learning: In defining learning we could refer simply to overt behavior. For ex., if I see you riding a bicycle I can assume that you’ve learned

Effects of strong extraneous stimuli

If you change the nature of the eliciting stimulus you see recovery of the habituated response. Can also see recovery of the response if the animal is given a rest period = spontaneous recovery.

The response can also be restored by presenting a strong stimulus— this is called dishabituation.

Dishabituation refers to recovery of the response to thehabituated stimulus following presentation of a different, novel stimulus.

Page 39: Definition of Learning: In defining learning we could refer simply to overt behavior. For ex., if I see you riding a bicycle I can assume that you’ve learned