36
Language Variation and Change, 9 (1997), 1-36. Printed in the U.S.A. © 1997 Cambridge University Press 0954-3945/97 $9.00 + .10 Contraction of negatives as evidence of variance in register-specific interactive rules MALCAH YAEGER-DROR University of Arizona ABSTRACT This study investigates the contraction of negatives in a carefully chosen cor- pus of discourse and writing, to permit comparison of the relative influences of various linguistic and social parameters on contraction. Evidence is presented that negative contraction is conditioned by interactional and other register vari- ables. The point is made that the pragmatic as well as morphological interpre- tation of negatives entails that negative contraction and auxiliary contraction should be distinguished from each other. Although a Cognitive Prominence Principle predicts noncontraction when the negative conveys semantically focal information, a Social Agreement Principle predicts contraction. This is because it would be face-threatening (and, therefore, in conversation analysis terms "dis- preferred") to focus on disagreement, which is most often the semantic infor- mation conveyed by negatives. This hypothesis is examined using corpora which differ along several dimensions. The most important of these (for this study) appear to be the interactional versus informational register dimensions (Fine- gan, 1994). Data from instructional (workshop presentations), confrontational (political debates), and casual conversational material are contrasted with com- parable reading style materials. The following general results are predicted. The Cognitive Prominence Principle will take over in informational contexts when disagreement is acceptable or neutralized. The Social Agreement Principle will take over in more interactional contexts where disagreement is not acceptable. The results are of interest to the student of focus, the sociolinguist concerned with dialect, register, and style variation, and even the speech technician. From a sociolinguistic perspective, any analysis of linguistic data must con- sider the influence of the situations of use on variation in given linguistic fea- tures; the characteristic pattern for the use of language in a particular situation determines what is now referred to as "register" (Finegan, 1994). Most sociolinguistic studies isolate one social situation to analyze; fortu- nately, the linguistic variable to be discussed here—contraction—has been analyzed in various registers. The present study takes advantage of previous I would like to thank Manny Schegloff and Gail Jefferson for access to the conversational data and transcriptions, Bea Oshika for access to the SCRL recordings, and Karen Adams for access to the debate data and transcriptions. I would also like to thank Henrietta Cedergren, Molly Dies- ing, Scott Jacobs, Patti Jo Price, two anonymous reviewers, and the editors for helpful suggestions. Any remaining limitations are clearly to be laid at the door of Malcah Yaeger-Dror, Department of Linguistics, University of Arizona, Tucson, AZ 85721. Email: [email protected] 1

Contraction of negatives as evidence of variance in

  • Upload
    others

  • View
    1

  • Download
    0

Embed Size (px)

Citation preview

Page 1: Contraction of negatives as evidence of variance in

Language Variation and Change, 9 (1997), 1-36. Printed in the U.S.A.© 1997 Cambridge University Press 0954-3945/97 $9.00 + .10

Contraction of negatives as evidence of variancein register-specific interactive rules

MALCAH YAEGER-DROR

University of Arizona

ABSTRACT

This study investigates the contraction of negatives in a carefully chosen cor-pus of discourse and writing, to permit comparison of the relative influencesof various linguistic and social parameters on contraction. Evidence is presentedthat negative contraction is conditioned by interactional and other register vari-ables. The point is made that the pragmatic as well as morphological interpre-tation of negatives entails that negative contraction and auxiliary contractionshould be distinguished from each other. Although a Cognitive ProminencePrinciple predicts noncontraction when the negative conveys semantically focalinformation, a Social Agreement Principle predicts contraction. This is becauseit would be face-threatening (and, therefore, in conversation analysis terms "dis-preferred") to focus on disagreement, which is most often the semantic infor-mation conveyed by negatives. This hypothesis is examined using corpora whichdiffer along several dimensions. The most important of these (for this study)appear to be the interactional versus informational register dimensions (Fine-gan, 1994). Data from instructional (workshop presentations), confrontational(political debates), and casual conversational material are contrasted with com-parable reading style materials. The following general results are predicted. TheCognitive Prominence Principle will take over in informational contexts whendisagreement is acceptable or neutralized. The Social Agreement Principle willtake over in more interactional contexts where disagreement is not acceptable.The results are of interest to the student of focus, the sociolinguist concernedwith dialect, register, and style variation, and even the speech technician.

From a sociolinguistic perspective, any analysis of linguistic data must con-sider the influence of the situations of use on variation in given linguistic fea-tures; the characteristic pattern for the use of language in a particularsituation determines what is now referred to as "register" (Finegan, 1994).Most sociolinguistic studies isolate one social situation to analyze; fortu-nately, the linguistic variable to be discussed here—contraction—has beenanalyzed in various registers. The present study takes advantage of previous

I would like to thank Manny Schegloff and Gail Jefferson for access to the conversational dataand transcriptions, Bea Oshika for access to the SCRL recordings, and Karen Adams for accessto the debate data and transcriptions. I would also like to thank Henrietta Cedergren, Molly Dies-ing, Scott Jacobs, Patti Jo Price, two anonymous reviewers, and the editors for helpful suggestions.Any remaining limitations are clearly to be laid at the door of Malcah Yaeger-Dror, Departmentof Linguistics, University of Arizona, Tucson, AZ 85721. Email: [email protected]

1

Page 2: Contraction of negatives as evidence of variance in

2 MALCAH YAEGER-DROR

analyses of negation and supplements them with data from previously unstud-ied registers in order to draw conclusions about the importance of register asa sociolinguistic variable and the importance of social-interaction variablesto an understanding of register.

A critical semantic characteristic of the contractions to be analyzed is thatthey express negation; because negation is often used to express interactionaldisagreement, key tenets of linguistic and pragmatic (conversational) theo-ries are concerned with the information carried by negatives and propose con-flicting hypotheses for the realization of negatives in different registers.Appropriate manipulation of register can permit evaluation of the relativeimportance of semantic (linguistic) and interactional (pragmatic) informationto a given register.

COGNITIVE PROMINENCE PRINCIPLE

Most linguists agree that "focal" or "new" information in a discourse shouldbe realized with accent on the focal word (e.g., Bolinger, 1978; Brown, 1983;Carlson, 1984; Coker & Umeda, 1971; Fowler, 1988; Fowler & Housum,1987; Halliday & Hasan, 1976; Hirschberg, 1990; Koopmans-Van Beinum,1992; Leech, 1980; Nevalainen, 1992; Nooteboom & Kruyt, 1987; Prince,1981), although this is not always the case (Nevalainen, 1987). Accent is pro-duced with both pitch prominence and increased vowel duration and periph-erality (as well as increased amplitude; cf. Goldberg, 1978; Granstrom &Nord, 1992).

Many previous studies of negatives have considered negation primarilyfrom this linguistic-theoretical perspective, assuming that negatives should besemantically regarded as focal information, and therefore they should bepitch and amplitude prominent and lengthened. This conclusion will be re-ferred to as the Cognitive Prominence Principle.1 Supporting it, key quan-titative phonetic studies have shown that in read materials negatives are pitchprominent even when contracted (Hirschberg, 1990; O'Shaughnessy & Allen,1983).

O'Shaughnessy and Allen (1983) elicited sentences from three MIT lin-guists to determine whether pitch was prominent on negative-bearing ele-ments; as the examples in (1) demonstrate, they compiled their corpus topermit the contrast between contracted and uncontracted negatives.

(1) a. Which books was Joe not studying?a'. Which books wasn't Joe studying?b. Which books weren't the boys studying?b'. Which books were the boys not studying?c. Joe (adverb) would not have studied,c'. Joe (adverb) wouldn't have studied.

Both the sentence composition and the overall experimental design entailedthat only very careful, isolated sentence-reading speech was used, and that

Page 3: Contraction of negatives as evidence of variance in

CONTRACTION OF NEGATIVES 3

the pitch was overwhelmingly prominent on the negative in the read sen-tences, whether or not the negative was contracted. We can conclude that,in such a careful context, the Cognitive Prominence Principle is dominant,even if the negative itself is contracted. Similarly, Hirschberg (1990, 1992)determined that pitch was prominent on both contracted and uncontractednegatives of news broadcasts by National Public Radio (henceforth, NPR—the U.S. equivalent of the BBC).2

This type of study disregards the possible relevance of contraction to thethesis. If contraction is considered as a binary coding of vowel duration, withcontraction as the nonprominent (unaccented) option, then a contracted neg-ative is seen to contradict the underlying Cognitive Prominence Principle.Any evidence of durational reduction of the negative undermines the Cog-nitive Prominence Principle. Also undermining the Cognitive ProminencePrinciple is evidence from English that tfo-support was introduced to coun-teract the cognitively counterproductive erosion of negation by reduction(Labov, 1994). Similarly, discussion of southeast England's vernacular nega-tion reveals the extent to which older eroded forms of negation have beenreplaced by hardier all-purpose forms like ain't and never (Cheshire, Edwards,& Whittle, 1989), and Outer Banks vernacular supplants eroded negativesusing distinct verbs with negative (weren't) and non-negative (was) modals,demonstrating that speakers sacrifice the more marginal distinction of num-ber to reestablish the more central semantic category of negation (Schilling-Estes & Wolfram, 1994).3 Obviously, negative contraction must severelyreduce the negative if the vernacular must find ways to counteract it, yet lin-guists have not questioned why contraction occurs, if the semantic informa-tion carried by the negative is so important as to be consistently prominentphonetically, and why the negative needs support. Since there is varied evi-dence that cognitively critical information supplied by negation is systemat-ically eroded by negative reduction, other factors must be relevant to thediscussion.

SOCIAL AGREEMENT PRINCIPLE

A quite different perspective is proposed by conversational analysts. Schegloff,Jefferson, and Sacks (1977), in their important Language article, presentedevidence that in conversational speech there is a "preference for agreement,"to which speakers adapt their speech.4 They showed that, all other thingsbeing equal, in conversation, speakers minimize the extent to which they dis-agree with coparticipants and emphasize signs of agreement. We will refer tothis as the conversational analysis Social Agreement Principle. Although theCognitive Prominence Principle favors emphasis—pitch, amplitude, and dura-tional prominence—on highly significant information, such as that providedby a negative, the Social Agreement Principle favors deemphasis —orminimization—of information which is not supportive of coparticipants; often

Page 4: Contraction of negatives as evidence of variance in

MALCAH YAEGER-DROR

TABLE 1. Data from various spoken registerswere analyzed in Yaeger-Dror (1985)

Code Content Register Dialect

Interactional (H. Sacks; G. Jefferson)1.1 CH-4 DinnerParty Casual Yuppie NY1.1 TG Telephone Casual NY1.2 GTS-5 Therapy session Casual SAE"1.3 GTS-5 Therapist Therapeutic SAE

Instructive/task oriented (B. Oshika)

3.1 SCRL DARPA tutorial Lectures SAE

"For consistency, the term SAE (Standard American English) is being usedarbitrarily to designate those dialects which prefer negative contraction to aux-iliary contraction, which use contraction almost categorically in questions andimperatives and do not permit any negative-contraction of am not.

negatives are the key semantic evidence for such lack of agreement (but seeSchegloffetal., 1977).

Comparing the two principles we see that the first favors prosodically sali-ent negatives, whereas the second favors reduction (or even elimination) ofnegatives. Presumably, to the degree that the Cognitive Prominence Princi-ple holds, all negatives which provide new information should be prosodicallyprominent. To the degree that the Social Agreement Principle holds, sup-portive negatives will be prominent more often than interactively remedialnegatives.

Yaeger-Dror (1985) analyzed conversational negatives to determinewhether the Cognitive Prominence Principle would also influence pitch vari-ation in interactive speech, or whether the Social Agreement Principle wouldbe the stronger influence in such a situation.5 Table I6 lists the corpora usedfor that study. Given that all of the corpora were composed of fairly stan-dard Northern American casual speech, the vast majority of the negativeswere contracted wherever possible, and a very low percentage of the caseswere pitch prominent, supporting the claim that the Social Agreement Prin-ciple influences both the form and prosody of negatives in discourse.

Tottie (1991:329) presented evidence from the London-Lund Corpus ofSpoken [British] English (Svartvik, 1990) that confirms for British speech thepitch evidence that Yaeger-Dror found in American speech. Although Tot-tie initially assumed that pitch prominence would co-occur with negation,quantitative analysis of the London-Lund Corpus showed no preference forpitch prominence (defined as "focus") on negatives. Rather, both studies con-cluded that the Social Agreement Principle outweighs the Cognitive Promi-nence Principle, both in U.S. (Yaeger-Dror, 1985) and British (Tottie, 1987,

Page 5: Contraction of negatives as evidence of variance in

CONTRACTION OF NEGATIVES 5

1991) taped conversations. Although contraction was not analyzed simulta-neously, to the degree that reduction (i.e., contraction or pitch nonpromi-nence) is the rule, it would also support the Social Agreement Principle at theexpense of the Cognitive Prominence Principle.

As a result, it is relevant to compare conversational material with oral(relatively unplanned) material that is instructional: that is, material wherethe speaker's role is (at least primarily) as conveyer of information rather thanas interactant. Yaeger-Dror (1985) contrasted the conversational data withdata from a more instructional situation, assuming that the Cognitive Prom-inence Principle would be more important in the latter register. This permit-ted a three-way comparison among read materials, workshop tutorials, andconversations. She determined that in a tutorial workshop-instructional reg-ister (cited as the SCRL corpus), which is less planned and more interactionalthan the O'Shaughnessy or Hirschberg data,7 informational needs took pre-cedence more frequently than in conversations: pitch was more often prom-inent, although pitch prominence was far from categorical. SCRL examplesare found in (2) and (3).

In all of the examples cited here, the word (or locution) at issue is under-lined; boldfaced text indicates that it was realized as relatively louder, colonsimply lengthening of the immediately preceding element, and text in italicsis pitch prominent. Pauses, where not implied by punctuation, are noted witha hyphen (which signifies a "hitch," where there is a pause with no pitch con-tour) or with the length of the pause marked in parentheses: e.g., (1.5) equals1500 msec. All samples cited are taken directly from the transcribed corpusdenoted in parentheses. Although the Jeffersonian use of "eye dialect" hasbeen both criticized and defended, in the present article it will be retained notonly because it is the definitive transcript, but also because it permits thereader to see where radical reduction has taken place, even in the absence ofan audio record. Such distinctions cannot otherwise be simply conveyed tothe reader.

(2) Instructional register, pitch prominent, uncontracted examples (SCRL)a. DK: The rule itself is not.b. W: If they're ngt_ arbitrary...c. W: That's no£ the case . . .d. DK: Th't- that he's not learning to do phonetic recognition.

(3) Instructional register, pitch prominent, contracted examples (SCRL)a. DK: There isn't any reason for not making. . . ^b. DK: Real speech isn't clean.c. DK: They don't ((pause)) phonemicize. They hear what's there.

Yaeger-Dror (1985) concluded that instructional and interactional situa-tions should be coded separately. She found that, in settings where informa-tional needs were primary, the Cognitive Prominence Principle was more

Page 6: Contraction of negatives as evidence of variance in

6 MALCAH YAEGER-DROR

relevant than the Social Agreement Principle. Even when the negative wascontracted, as in (3), pitch was more likely to be prominent than was true formore interactional data. O'Shaughnessy's MIT linguists and Hirschberg'sNPR broadcasters also used pitch prominence almost categorically on neg-atives in read material, and the research design (with read discourse) neutral-ized the importance of contraction. Yaeger-Dror (1985) found that researchersused prominence on negations in the tutorial setting less than half the timeand contraction approximately half the time. In more interactionally involvedconversations, both pitch and duration on the negative were much more likelyto be reduced.

Interactional variation and theSocial Agreement Principle

Yaeger-Dror (1985) also presented evidence that, within the context of inter-actional settings, another important parameter is whether the negative isface-threatening to other participants in the interaction (Brown & Levinson,1978; Goffman, 1967). Not all negatives are remedial; Goffman's (1971) con-trast between "supportive" and "remedial" interchanges reflects the fact thatnegatives can be used supportively (s), remedially (ft), or neutrally (n). Reme-dial interchanges, which are referred to as "repairs" (Schegloff et al., 1977)or "face-threatening acts" (Brown & Levinson, 1978), are strongly dispre-ferred by the Social Agreement Principle in most situations, but supportiveinterchanges display agreement with and support the position of copartici-pants. Supportive interchanges occasionally make use of superficial disagree-ment ("no, you're not fat!") or replay negatives used by the previous speaker,as in (6), in order to display support. When there is no face threat, the neg-ative is relatively more likely to retain its cognitive prominence, but when theinformation can be seen as threatening the face of a coparticipant, as in (4),the speaker is more likely to reduce the negative, to conform to the conver-sational analysis Social Agreement Principle, as in (4').

(4) Dinner party conversation; Wife-1, a biologist, explains to two skepticalhusbandsWife-1: And suddenly. They do:n*t- they don't=

[°I should have realized that just from-frm wartching them,0)8

(4')ft V they don't swoop from the front.9 (CH-4)

Both in the tutorial setting and in the more casual interactions, if a speakerwas self-correcting, as in (5), the negative provides contrastive new informa-tion (favoring Cognitive Prominence), but cannot be heard as threateningother parties to the conversation, and so need not be reduced. Pitch was morelikely to be prominent, and contraction of the negation itself was less likelyto occur. Although it has been lavishly documented that listener intuitionsare not infallible, it may be instructive to compare the actual sentences withtheir undocumented "logical" equivalent, marked with a u:

Page 7: Contraction of negatives as evidence of variance in

CONTRACTION OF NEGATIVES 7

(5) Self-correction, in the middle of an explanation of a new technique beingadvocatedJSH: That's not necessarily the best way, b u t . . . (SCRL)

(5M) JSH: That isnX necessarily the best way, b u t . . . (SCRL)

Contraction on .the negative itself was even less likely to occur, but pitchwas even more likely to be prominent in supportive responses, like those in(6a') and (6b'). Note that, in (6a) and (6b), which can be considered inter-actionally neutral, and which have cognitively informative negatives, the neg-atives are both contracted and nonprominent, undermining the CognitiveProminence Principle and possibly supporting the Social Agreement Princi-ple, as the speaker does not know the hearer's opinion. In such data, pitchwas generally nonprominent; percentages for face-threatening data were evenlower. In (6a') and (6b'), the negative is clearly cognitively redundant andcannot be construed as providing either new or contrastive information, butit is socially supportive, expressing the speaker's agreement with a precedingstatement. In such situations, the Cognitive Prominence Principle predictsthat the negative should not be prominent, whereas the Social AgreementPrinciple predicts that the negative should be prominent. Since noncontrac-tion and pitch prominence of the negative occurred almost categorically inthe noninformative supportive turn, the data support the Social AgreementPrinciple.

(6) Supportive interchanges (GTS-5)a. Therapist: You say you d'wanna be an engineer? [i.e., You say you don't

want t o . . . ]a'. Ken: (.3) No:, (.5) I do:nt. (GTS-S)b. Rojer: You see the fault that I'm getting at- at the: a t - . . . wi'th'

schools's, they haven't got an answer to the problem,b'. Therapist: No::\ ((Falsetto)) They do:n'tl10 (GTS-S)

Compare (6) with unattested (6M):

(6M) a. Therapist: You say you do not_ wanna be an engineer?a'. Ken: (.3) (.5) I do no£. (GTS-5)b. Rojer: You see the fault that I'm getting at- at the: a t - . . .

wi'th' schools's, they have not got an answer to the problem,b'. Therapist: They do:n't\ (GTS-5)

The unattested (6Ma) and (6Mb) now appear rude, whereas (6Ma') and (6Mb')appear infelicitous. Yaeger-Dror (1985) found that, in conversation, sup-portive negatives are significantly more likely to be prominent. Similarly, instudies of Spanish and Italian, researchers have found that the use of a prefa-tory isolated (and consequently pitch prominent) negative is most likely tooccur in supportive interchanges (Mendoza-Denton, 1996, and literature citedtherein). This can be understood when one considers that the negative pro-

Page 8: Contraction of negatives as evidence of variance in

8 MALCAH YAEGER-DROR

vides strong agreement with a preceding statement and so may not even pro-vide new information which should receive cognitive prominence.11

Pitch prominence was less likely to occur in a potentially neutral statement,like those in (1), (6a), and (6b), and least likely in a statement that could beconstrued as a face-threatening correction, like those in (7). Note that, incases like those in (7), there is a clear conflict between the Cognitive Promi-nence Principle and the Social Agreement Principle. The information sup-plied by the negative is either new or contrastive and therefore favors pitchprominence, but this information also disagrees with the position (i.e., threat-ens the face) of coparticipants and therefore disfavors pitch prominence.

(7) Disagreement, maintaining that the explanation begun in (4b) does not soundplausiblea. Husband-2: That's what I can't make out. uh, y'know.

If that's true, some of the birds are ahead of 'im.a'. c'n't even see 'im.

Husband-1: Right. (CH-4)

Not only does the speaker mitigate the face-threatening implications of hisdisagreement by morphologically and prosodically neutralizing the negative,but, when no self-correction occurs, he does a certain amount of interactionalwork to imply that the fault here is probably his own (what / can't make out),and hedges (uh, y'know. If that's true), before actually introducing the(reduced) negative (as described in Schegloff et al., 1977).

Thus, Yaeger-Dror (1985) found that the interactional intent of the speakerhad to be considered in addition to whether the setting was instructional orinteractional. Theoretically, let us say that the instructional situation andthe interactive/neutral situation (where the Cognitive Prominence Principleis primary) and the interactional/supportive situation (where the Social Agree-ment Principle is primary) should allow for similar percentages of pitch prom-inence and contraction on the negative, whereas the highest percentages ofpitch prominence and uncontracted negatives should occur when a speakerself-corrects, since the two rules then reinforce each other. In contrast, thetwo rules are clearly in conflict when a "remedial" (Goffman, 1971) or face-threatening (Brown & Levinson, 1978) statement is being made; negative con-traction should occur most consistently in face-threatening situations. Therelative strength of the two rules can be most efficiently compared byjuxtaposing the instructional (or neutral) and supportive data with face-threatening data.

REGISTER VARIATION:INFORMATIONAL VERSUS INTERACTIVE SITUATIONS

Studies that compare the use of contraction in different registers have dem-onstrated that negatives are commonly contracted, except in very "planned"written registers (Ochs, 1979; Tannen, 1985; Tottie, 1987, 1991). The emerg-

Page 9: Contraction of negatives as evidence of variance in

CONTRACTION OF NEGATIVES 9

ing analysis of register distinctions takes the contrast between informational(cognitive) and interactive (social) situations as one of the primary registerdimensions; not surprisingly, contraction is one linguistic variable that is rel-evant to the analysis of what Biber (1988) defined as the "informational-involved" dimension.12 He found that contractions13 (like other interactionallinguistic parameters) occurred most frequently in the conversational datathat he analyzed, least frequently in academic texts, and with intermediatefrequencies in news reporting, radio broadcasts, and public speeches. Thus,the more important information processing is, the more likely speakers areto realize negatives; the more relevant social interaction is, the more likelyspeakers are to contract them.

Both Bell (1991a) and Ochs (1979) pointed out another factor that re-inforces the influence of the Cognitive Prominence Principle in read mate-rial like O'Shaughnessy's or Hirschberg's: "[contraction] has been largelyignored in the study of linguistic variation, partly because it is so subject toconscious control" (Bell, 1991a). To the degree that speech and reading (letalone writing) can be compared, the relative percentages may already beskewed in favor of speech as contractable (Ochs, 1979; Tannen, 1985; Zwicky& Pullum, 1982). Tottie (1983) noted that negation is generally more frequentin discourse than in written texts, and that what negation there is in text ismore likely to be "affixal" (i.e., lexicalized, as in 'w/m'kely') and, conse-quently, not contractable. She found that, although there is more affixalnegation in writing, overall, even including affixal negation, there is twice asmuch negation in speech as in writing (1991).

As Bell (1984, 1991a) found in his study of contraction in the speech ofNew Zealand news announcers, BBC (overseas) announcers used no con-traction whatsoever. In contrast, announcers on New Zealand's most "care-ful" stations used contraction at least 259b of the time, announcers on theintermediate stations used contraction more than 50% of the time, and an-nouncers on the pop music station with the most (pseudo)interactional styleof presentation used contraction more than 70% of the time. This finding isconsistent with studies of other variables carried out by Bell himself (1991b),by Brunei (1970), and by Yaeger-Dror (1988, 1993); all of which show that,even in the speech of a single newscaster, stigmatized or casual variables areused less when reporting for prestigious stations, where informational con-tent is primary, than when reporting for more popular stations, where thereporters are more (pseudo)interactively "involved" with listeners.

Within the larger theoretical context, all these analyses can be interpretedas demonstrating that more "planned," informational registers are more con-cerned with conveying information and therefore more likely to be governedby the Cognitive Prominence Principle, whereas more "unplanned" registersare generally more interactional and consequently more likely to be governedby the Social Agreement Principle. This implies a complex relationship be-tween contraction and focus. It is obvious that the context of the talk influ-ences how it is produced for linguistic as well as for sociolinguistic reasons.Comparing results across larger contextual/register gaps (as Biber did) dem-

Page 10: Contraction of negatives as evidence of variance in

10 MALCAH YAEGER-DROR

TABLE 2. Alternate forms for have not and be not in English

Uncontracted

I have notyou have nots/he has notwe have notthey have not

Neg.Contraction

I haven'tyou haven'ts/he hasn'twe haven'tthey haven't

Aux.Contraction

I've notyou've nots/he's notwe've notthey've not

Uncontracted

I am notyou are nots/he is notwe are notthey are not

Neg.Contraction

•(I ain't)you aren'ts/he isn'twe aren'tthey aren't

Aux.Contraction

I'm notyou're nots/he's notwe're notthey're not

onstrates that the more informational the context, the more the focal importof the negative requires noncontraction; the more speaker and addressee areinteractively involved, the less critical cognitive prominence is and the morecritical social agreement is, which militates against prominence on the nega-tive, even to the extent of contracting and reducing the key informative word.

DIALECT VARIATION

The evidence presented so far implies that planned/informational data aremore governed by the Cognitive Prominence Principle and more likely to beunreduced, whereas unplanned and interactional data are governed by theSocial Agreement Principle and more likely to be reduced unless used sup-portively. Obviously, to the extent that negatives are contracted in a givencontext, they cannot be regarded as prosodically prominent, but even in a reg-ister with many contractions, negative contractions can be contrasted withcontractions where the auxiliary is contracted and negatives are unreduced.Tottie (1991) reported Biber (1988) as having quantitative evidence that Britishspeakers are less likely to use contraction than Americans, but unfortunately,Biber (personal communication, 1994) has no quantitative evidence compar-ing different contraction strategies, because his analysis coded for both con-traction types together. Other studies have shown that auxiliary contractionand negative contraction function as dialect variables (Hazen, 1996; Labov,1969; Trudgill, 1986). The distinction is found in Table 2.

Some dialects are more likely to contract the auxiliary, whereas othersmore often contract the negative. Trudgill (1986) claimed that reduction ofthe negative element of a contraction {we haven't) is the rule in AmericanEnglish, but that contraction of the auxiliary, leaving the negative intact{we've not), is more common in at least some Southern British dialects.Unfortunately, no quantitative study of different contraction types in Brit-ish English is yet available, but Hazen (1996) presented quantitative evidencethat speakers from the Outer Banks (Ocracoke, NC) contract the auxiliary,leaving the negative intact.14 Consequently, mere evidence of contraction isnot immediate proof that the Social Agreement Principle takes precedence.The relative number of contractions is generally conceded to correlate with

Page 11: Contraction of negatives as evidence of variance in

CONTRACTION OF NEGATIVES 11

context (register and style) and code (dialect), but future analyses should dif-ferentiate between negative contraction and auxiliary contraction, particularlybecause a theoretical perspective on contraction predisposes us to expect neg-atives, which carry important information, to be unreduced, whereas auxil-iaries, which do not, can be contracted without compromising informationretrieval.

Taking all the literature into consideration, then, the following variablesare relevant to analysis of contraction: dialect, register, and interactive intent.The following sections discuss the corpora chosen for the present study, itsdesign, and the results of the analysis of contraction within each of the cor-pora. The final section draws conclusions and suggests future areas ofresearch.

Given the initial understanding that the Cognitive Prominence Principleand the Social Agreement Principle conflict in most social situations, one pur-pose of the present study is to determine the relative importance of the twoprinciples in new social contexts, or registers, by analyzing variation in con-traction usage. Addressing the theoretical problem initially posed, the Cog-nitive Prominence Principle entails that contraction of the negative shouldbe inversely correlated with the importance of the information that it con-veys, whereas the Social Agreement Principle entails that contraction of thenegative should be directly correlated with the potential for face threatimplied by a negative. Earlier studies support both principles:

1. Negatives are prominent in registers that convey critical information with-out any need for social agreement (Hirschberg, 1990, 1992; Hirschberg &Grosz, 1994; O'Shaughnessy & Allen, 1983).

2. Negatives are not prominent in conversational interactions when theythreaten the face of coparticipants, especially when the negative conveys crit-ical new information (Yaeger-Dror, 1985).

3. Negatives are prominent in conversational interactions when they are sup-portive of coparticipants, whether or not they convey new information(Yaeger-Dror, 1985).

In order to test further the domain of these principles, other registersneed to be studied, and the connection between contraction and pitch prom-inence should be compared. The following corpora were chosen because theychallenge the two principles, and because they permit us to compare thedegree to which negative contraction and other forms of reduction are usedconsistently.

DESCRIPTION OF THE CORPORA

Fiction

This register was chosen because it presents a clear contrast with both actualconversational material and actual informational (e.g., news broadcast) read-ings. Although earlier register analyses considered fiction to be a single reg-

Page 12: Contraction of negatives as evidence of variance in

12 MALCAH YAEGER-DROR

TABLE 3. Corpora analyzed here

2.1 Cleary

2.2 Wharton2.3 Atwood

2.3 Tyler2.4 Keillor

3.2 Rio3.2 M/B3.1'

Prose

Ramona QuimbyRamona the Pest (S. Channing)SummerLife Before ManBluebeard's Egg (Atwood)Breathing Lessons (Eikenberry)Lake Wobegon Days (Keillor)

Confrontational? (K. Adams)

Phoenix Rio Salado debateMecham/Babbitt (M-L)Above debates

Children's prose

1890s prose1970s prose198819881985, 1986(monologue)

DebateDebateModerator/interviewer

SAE

SAESCE?SCE?SAESAE

SWESWE

Note: SCE (Standard Canadian English) and SWE (South West English) appear to be indistin-guishable from SAE with regard to these morphosyntactic generalizations, but for the presentthe distinctions are maintained in the table.

ister (Biber, 1988; Svartvik, 1990), one purpose of the present study was todetermine the degree to which different subregisters can or should be isolated.Earlier studies based on informational corpora proposed that all "planned"registers require uncontracted negatives to be prosodically prominent. How-ever, Yaeger-Dror (1996a) hypothesized that analysis of pseudointeractive sit-uations would reflect either the absence of true interaction (the informationalresults would then be similar to O'Shaughnessy's and Hirschberg's) or theauthor's (and reader's) impression of truly interactive speech. The data wasclosely scrutinized to evaluate the importance of this register dimension onnegatives and to determine the degree to which that variation reveals thedomain of the Social Agreement and/or the Cognitive Prominence Princi-ples. In each of the readings, I have included descriptive prose passages(referred to as "prose") and a great deal of dialogue in order to discoverwhether dialogue followed informational or interactional principles. Table3 shows the specific read corpora chosen for comparison and analyzed here.

Authors were chosen for their similarities and differences to the NPREnglish model. One children's author was chosen (Cleary, 1968, 1981), be-cause fiction for children might be even more emphatically and information-ally presented than fiction for adults, or, alternatively, it might permit a morechildlike contracted presentation.15 An older novel was chosen (Wharton,1917) because older fiction was assumed to be less contracted than more re-cent fiction (Biber, 1988). Atwood (1979, 1983)16 and Tyler (1988) were cho-sen because they are similar both to each other and to the NPR model, andthey permit us to consider the possibility that Canadian fiction might be lesscontracted than U.S. fiction. Keillor (1985) was chosen primarily because hisbooks are compendia of monologues originally presented over NPR, and

Page 13: Contraction of negatives as evidence of variance in

CONTRACTION OF NEGATIVES 13

because his monologue style epitomizes my own internalized concept of theideal NPR voice and would, I assumed, come closest to an NPR informa-tional presentation. However, Keillor's book turns out to include two differ-ent monologue styles. One style is literary (L), with little dialogue; examplesof this style are "Forebears" (where most of the quotes are actually from let-ters) and "Protestants," major portions of which are in fact read aloud onthe tape. The second, monologue (M) style, using ample dialogue, provideschildhood reminiscences such as in "Home" and "Sumus."

The comparison of written and oral versions of fiction permitted analy-sis of actual contractive reduction of negatives as well as of pitch prominenceon negatives (Yaeger-Dror, 1996a). Initially, I assumed (following Ochs,1979) that the oral version would permit negative contraction significantlymore often than the written version. However, this expected shift did notoccur at all, whereas the shift from contracted (in the written version) touncontracted (in the oral version) actually did occur in one of the Keillormonologues (1 of 716 tokens).

The published versions of the books were used as transcripts, while thetaped versions were compared with them to provide prosodic information.The initial hypothesis was that informational criteria (prominence on nega-tion) would outweigh interactive ones (decreased prominence on negation) inthe neutral descriptive prose segments, resulting in less contraction of the neg-atives, but that the more interactional Social Agreement Principle would berelevant in dialogue.

I intended the corpora from Table 1 (actual social interactions) to providea direct contrast with the dialogue data from taped fiction listed in Table 3(the writers' version of an interactional register). To that end, the texts I choseare equivalent (middle-class American), the proposed emotional content iscomparable (midway along a continuum between "informational" and "inter-active"), but the speech register differs (polite interactions vs. taped politepseudointeractions). The Hirschberg data may provide a closer comparisonwith the narrative prose passages of the taped books: again, the speakers andproposed position along the informational-interactive continuum are equiv-alent, but the registers differ. The corpora listed in Table 3 differ from bothconversational speech and the citation register favored by phoneticians.Whereas proponents of the Cognitive Prominence Principle as well as theSocial Agreement Principle have assumed negative contraction to be irrele-vant to their purposes, the present study differentiates more realisticallyamong speech registers, using variability regarding negatives to determine thedegree to which register is related to the principles. The results also show thatthe interactional variables important for conversational speech should not beignored in fiction or in semiscripted confrontational situations, any more thanin conversational interactions.17

Political debates

Political debates were chosen to permit analysis of an informationally max-imally emphatic, but possibly interactionally sensitive, register.18 The two

Page 14: Contraction of negatives as evidence of variance in

14 MALCAH YAEGER-DROR

debates considered here were chosen because of their apparent comparabilitywith earlier corpora; they are shown in Table 3. The "Rio Salado" debate,which was carried over a local NPR affiliate, is concerned with a politicalproblem local to the Phoenix area: this debate features a man and a womandebating the merits of passing a certain amendment. The second debatewas presented on the MacNeil-Lehrer Report,19 with two (then) Arizonapoliticians—Mecham and Babbitt. All four debaters are from the Southwest;contractions are primarily negative contractions. On a scale of planned-unplanned, the debates are more planned than casual conversations, but lessplanned than the published texts or books on tape. On the informational-interactive dimension, the debates should be both highly informational andinteractive. On the other hand, if all interactional situations are governed bythe Social Agreement Principle, political debates should require reduced face-threatening negatives. The degree to which they permit prominence on neg-atives will permit us to characterize this principle more exactly. Whereas thedebating politicians were initially assumed to share the same register, theinterviewers were not only different from the debaters, but also quite differ-ent from each other.20

Previous research into debate "stance" (dayman, 1988, 1992) and "foot-ing" (Goffman, 1981) found that the Social Agreement Principle is neutral-ized in this register:21 that is, both supportive and face-threatening negativesappear to be prominent. In the debate corpora used here, the majority of thepoliticians' negatives were intended to be face-threatening and were stylisti-cally marked by repetition and bombastic repetitive intonation (Tannen,1989); however, negatives were also realized as uncontracted and pitch prom-inent in the 1988 Democratic Convention debates, where they were usedsupportively.22

The fiction and debate corpora are now contrasted with conversationaldata from Yaeger-Dror (1985). Each of the subcorpora is discussed in turn,and conclusions about both contractions and pitch prominence are used todetermine the relative importance of informational and interactive input tothe analysis of prosody.

ANALYSIS

The primary analysis here is of negative contraction patterns; the results arecompared with studies of pitch prominence (Yaeger-Dror, 1996a, 1996b).Auxiliary contraction does not bear on the contrast between the principles,so only negative contraction cases are considered in this article. To limit dia-lect variation in contraction patterns, all of the speakers (and writers) forwhom data are compared are U.S. or Canadian Standard dialect speakers,for whom negative contraction is more common than auxiliary contraction.Comparison of contraction percentages in different contexts provides evi-dence to evaluate the relative importance of the two principles.

Page 15: Contraction of negatives as evidence of variance in

CONTRACTION OF NEGATIVES 15

Pitch/Contraction

+ Prominent

— Prominent

TABLE 4 .

Cognitive

Contraction and dialogue

- Contracted

Prominence Principle

[mixed] Social

+ Contracted

[mixed]

Agreement Principle

Variables

Lexical/morphological variables. Presumably, the most emphatic real-ization of the negative in English is the uncontracted word not, with ampli-tude and pitch prominence. Some examples were given in (2).

Relexification. In formal registers, the negative can be lexicalized differ-ently {unstressed, dissimilar), but only what Tottie (1983, 1991) referred toas not negation is considered here, because only not negation is contracta-ble, and because only contractable tokens are considered in the analysis. Therequirement that a negative be contractable eliminates from the analysisnot only tokens like unwilling but also those of I'm not, given that, in North-ern American Standard English of the late 20th century, the equivalent con-tracted form I ain't is not acceptable.23 Those tokens of not which could becontracted were divided into those that were and those that were not.

Negative contraction. When the negative becomes less emphatic, it iscontracted, as in (3). Only contraction of the negative itself provides evidencefor the conflict between the Cognitive Prominence Principle and the SocialAgreement Principle.

Syntactic constraints. In North American Standard English of the late20th century contractions are almost categorical, even in fictional imperatives("Do not eat the daisiesl") and questions ("are they no/?"), so imperativesand questions are excluded from the analysis.

In short, the analysis is limited to wof-negatives in positions where nega-tive contraction is variable in NPR English. Cases of / am not and negativesin questions and imperatives are disregarded. By factoring out structural anddialect variation, I attempt to determine the degree to which contraction isinfluenced by the Cognitive Prominence and Social Agreement Principles andby register variation.

Pitch and contraction. The spoken data were independently coded forpitch prominence, because the information from pitch prominence reinforcesthat of noncontraction, whereas nonprominence reinforces contraction. Thefour options available are shown in Table 4, and examples of each can befound in Appendix 1. Examples like those in (8) are not totally consistent witheither principle.

Page 16: Contraction of negatives as evidence of variance in

16 MALCAH YAEGER-DROR

(8) "Inconsistent" examplesPitch nonprominent, uncontracteda. This belief is not encouraged. (Keillor)Pitch prominent, contractedb. An' roughly twennyfive percent do:n'wanna state holiday, (M/B:Mechaml9)c. an' doesn V want, and is using his power, eh . . . (M/B:Babbitt; PT lb)d. Oh:::, I do::n't care! (Keillor; PT la)

(See the first three examples in Appendix 1.) On the other hand, if, as in (9),the pitch is nonprominent and the negative is contracted, this minimizes anyface threat, reflecting the Social Agreement Principle. (See the last threeexamples in Appendix 1.)

(9) Reduced examplesa. b't the truth is, we sti:tt don't know. (Keillor; PT Id)b. If o:ther towns have o:ne, we don* know about it. (Keillor; PT le)

Analysis of pitch variation for these data has appeared elsewhere (Yaeger-Dror, 1996a, 1996b; Yaeger-Dror & Nunamaker, 1992); an analysis of con-traction follows.

Subregisters. In the fiction corpora, prose passages were coded sepa-rately from dialogue, which was further divided into different interactionalsituations.

Interactive footing. The separate coding of dialogue as neutral, face-threatening, or supportive permits an unbiased investigation of the SocialAgreement Principle. Contrasting pseudo-interactional dialogue with actualinteractional speech permits a more nuanced understanding of the importanceof the differences between registers. Whereas dialogue provides all threeoptions, the four debaters use only face-threatening negations. Each nega-tive was coded for contraction (±), register (informational or interactional),discourse type (actual or scripted), and footing (supportive, neutral, or face-threatening).

Analysis technique

To study contraction variation in these different corpora, one need only gothrough all the negatives in one or two chapters of fiction or enough pagesof transcript to contain 100 contractable negatives in order to determinewhether (on this first cut) the negative is contracted and whether fictional dia-logue is presumed to be interactive—either with other characters (in thebooks) or with the listeners (in Keillor's monologues), and if so, whether thefooting is supportive, neutral, or face-threatening. In what follows, I choseto classify as dialogue those fiction segments that were clearly delineated byquotes; material considered dialogue was further coded as neutral, face-threatening, or supportive.241 expected supportive negatives, as in (6a') and

Page 17: Contraction of negatives as evidence of variance in

CONTRACTION OF NEGATIVES 17

(6b'), to be less likely to lead to contraction than face-threatening ones,because the latter conflict with the Social Agreement Principle. Percentagesof uncontracted cases were compared separately for each coded group.

RESULTS

Two corpora were used for the analysis. The literary texts were chosen fortheir apparent/superficial similarity to the informational texts studied byHirschberg and her associates (Hirschberg, 1990; Hirschberg & Litman, 1990).Public political debates were chosen as superficially antipodal to the informa-tional texts. Both corpora were much more complex than initially projected.

Contraction in fiction

Consider our expectations for the likelihood of contraction in the differentfiction segments. Earlier literature has already shown that more contractionsoccur in speech than in writing, leading one to expect more contractions indialogue than in neutral prose passages and fewer contractions in fiction forchildren (Cleary, 1968, 1981) than in fiction for adults. Examples of con-tracted and uncontracted negatives in dialogue and neutral narrative prose,as well as examples of each dialogue type, can be found in Appendix 2.

Prose. Figure 1 presents the percentages for uncontracted negatives foreach of the authors in descriptive prose passages and in dialogue; the con-nected line connects percentages for prose passages. Remember that accord-ing to the Cognitive Prominence Principle negatives, as bearers of importantinformation, should not be reduced. Yet it is clear that, even in this infor-mational context, negatives are contractable. Both Cleary and Wharton usedmore than 90% uncontracted tokens in narrative prose, but recent adultauthors used much lower percentages: Atwood (1979) and the Keillor-M sub-corpus used less than 40% uncontracted negatives. Thus, even in the leastinteractional written/planned fiction register sampled here, between 10 and60% of the declarative negatives are contracted. Table 5 presents the infor-mation underlying Figure 1 in tabular form.

The relative positions of different authors along the informational (uncon-tracted)/interactional (contracted) continuum are not surprising. It washypothesized that a children's author might choose to be more informational(and therefore contract less) than an author for adults, and the Cleary (1968,1981) prose data support this hypothesis. A comparison of the two booksrevealed that the earlier book (Cleary, 1968), which is about (and for) youn-ger children, has a higher percentage of uncontracted negatives in both proseand dialogue than any of the other texts, including Cleary (1981). Biber (1988)proposed that fiction is becoming more involved, and Wharton (1917) usedless contraction in narrative prose than more recent authors for adults.

Part of the difference between older and more recent authors may stemfrom a greater tendency for recent authors (at least the ones used here) to

Page 18: Contraction of negatives as evidence of variance in

18 MALCAH YAEGER-DROR

100%

un

ouca

uu

80%-

60%-

40%-

20%-

0%

•o-Prose

Neutral Dialogue

Supportive Dialogue

FTA Dialogue

(a)

Author

M

50%

40%-

30%-

u

c

§ 20%

c4>U

10%-

-

(

/

o

' T 1

•*

a

o o 4

— * — p y A Dialogue

+ Supponive Dialogue

o Neutral Dialogue

(b)

o IAuthor

J3u

HGURE 1. (a) Contractions in fiction for five American authors, (b) Contrac-tions in dialogue for the five authors by "footing."

Page 19: Contraction of negatives as evidence of variance in

CONTRACTION OF NEGATIVES 19

TABLE 5. Percent uncontracted negatives for each reading category,for each of the four authors analyzed

Name

AtwoodKeillor/M["Home"/"!

TylerKeillor/L("Forebears

Cleary/8WhartonCleary/P

...

Type

ClassicSatire

sumus"]

ClassicSatire

"/"Protestant")

Children'sClassicChildren's

[Total)

(303)(124)

(325)(184)

(169)(246)(320)

<7o

3333

4662

939697

Prosen

(N)

(267)(83)

(118)(163)

(114)(112)(106)

Supports

«7o (N)

38 (13)(0)

24 (17)(0)

11 (9)28 (18)14 (7)

Neutraln

%

1413

1340

08

30

(N)

(14)(24)

(78)(15)

(10)(46)(99)

Face-Threatening

ft

%

2241

3733

191343

(N) !

(9) i(17)

012)(6)

(36)(70)

(108)

Note: Number of tokens of negatives in each category are in parentheses.

use internal monologue for at least one character. Since internal monologuein these books is not enclosed in quotes, the present analysis merged self-involved monologue with informational/descriptive prose passages. It ispartly a result of this change in literary technique that both Biber (1988) andthe present study found more recent authors to be more interactional, al-though Biber used only British material and the present study only NorthAmerican English. Future studies should distinguish internal/interactivemonologue from information as well as from dialogue.

Neutral and face-threatening dialogue. As initially projected, Figure 1shows that in most cases authors contract more consistently in dialogue thanin descriptive prose: all authors contract more than 60% of the negatives indialogue, but less than 60% of the negatives in descriptive prose. In fact,Wharton (1917), who was projected to retain more uncontracted tokens evenin dialogue, contracts more than 90% of nonsupportive dialogue tokens, farmore than the other adult authors. It was also proposed that deary's dialoguemight exhibit more contraction than that of adult authors in order to con-form to adult intuitions about children's speech; this hypothesis is partly sup-ported. Thus, the widest contrast between descriptive prose and dialogueoccurs for Wharton and children's writers.

Supportive dialogue. Whereas numbers for all other groups analyzed arequite acceptable, information on supportive interchanges is sparse, varyingfrom a low of 0 for Keillor's satire to a high of 14 tokens for Tyler's novel.Perhaps for this reason, even though there is a significant difference betweencontraction in prose and dialogue, the differences between scripted interactive

Page 20: Contraction of negatives as evidence of variance in

20 MALCAH YAEGER-DROR

n

•au

sou

8 0 % -

6 0 % -

4 0 % -

2 0 % -

8

D

§

n ClearyTest'

O Cleary'Age8'

• Cleary

D

B

O

I 3

"ZDialogue

Interactive variables

2CU

Nondialogue

FIGURE 2. Uncontracted negatives in Cleary's writing for children in differentsubregisters.

footings is less clear than in the other factors. Figure l(b) shows that support-ive scripted interchanges between adults are actually more contracted thannegatives in face-threatening dialogue, but less than neutral negatives. Itappears that, despite a significant contrast between prose and dialogue, thepercentages in dialogue, though not identical to those for conversation, areconsistent with conversational contraction percentages, but that contractiondifferences caused by interactional intent are not realized.

Figure 2 presents a more detailed picture of the Cleary data. Prose pas-sages are almost categorically uncontracted in both books, but neutral dia-logue is almost categorically contracted, and supportive tokens are almostalways contracted as well. One of the devices that probably lends flavor tothe dialogue is that contraction is used less by "pesty" children towards teach-ers or parents, where it is a face-threatening act, than towards a character'sjuniors, where it is not. While this may not reflect social reality, the socialfantasy is apparently not lost on the audience.

The conclusion can be drawn that negation in dialogue is interactivelybiased, whereas prose passages reflect a more informationally biased presen-tation, which is partly neutralized in recent adult fiction when monologuetakes over.

Page 21: Contraction of negatives as evidence of variance in

CONTRACTION OF NEGATIVES 21

80.07*

6O.QV8 -

t•2z2

40.0V. -

2O.O°7« -

O.OVo

M % uncontncted face thrcau

M

(debates)

M M

M (casual conversations)

o eo

56

U <1 i % 1* 3 I

II

l 1Author/Speaker

FIGURE 3. Percent uncontracted face threats in conversations, dialogue, anddebate.

Contraction in political debates

The debate corpus was chosen because the degree to which the data could beconsidered interactive provided counterevidence to the notion that negativesshould be more reduced at the interactive end of the continuum. Althoughspeakers in political debates appear to speak extemporaneously, speeches areprobably based on notes and can be considered planned compared to con-versational data. However, although the speakers are probably fairly con-cerned with their speech, the style is obviously not as planned as the readfiction, much less Hirschberg's "newsspeak" or the O'Shaughnessy data. Thepurpose is to give information to the listeners (so the Cognitive ProminencePrinciple should be important), and the speakers are expected to contrast theirpositions (so the Social Agreement Principle is likely to be neutralized). Sincethe two principles are less likely to conflict in political debates than in anyother interactional situation used, uncontracted negatives should thereforeoccur even more frequently in political debates than in scripted dialogue.

Results of the analysis of contracted negatives from the debates can befound in Figure 3 and in Table 6. Figure 3 permits comparison with the face-threatening dialogue and conversational data. The Moderator (30% uncon-tracted tokens) and Mecham (37%) differ less from scripted face-threateningdialogue than they do from the Rio Salado debaters, who use uncontracted

Page 22: Contraction of negatives as evidence of variance in

22 MALCAH YAEOER-DROR

TABLE 6. Percent uncontracted negatives for each interactive category,for the debate material analyzed

Supportive Neutral Face-Threatening

Speaker

Rio SaladoMale debaterFemale debater

Mechami'BabbittMechamBabbittMacNeil

LJIUIUgUC(Total)

(44)(27)

(27)(13)(3)

Vo

——

——0

(N)

(1)

——

——

100

(N)

(2)

Vo

7555

3746—

(N)

(44)(27)

(27)(13)

Note: Number of tokens of negatives in each category are in parentheses.

tokens more consistently than any of the scripted dialogues, let alone the con-versational material studied. The greater emphasis on negatives is not relatedto their information-bearing significance (signaling a greater need for theCognitive Prominence Principle), but rather to the altered requirements ofdebate register. In debates the Social Agreement Principle is modified orabrogated, permitting a speaker to emphasize negatives that in other regis-ters would be reduced.

In the debates analyzed, negative contraction and other forms of prosodicreduction were highly correlated for all speakers. The apparent paradox musttherefore be considered within the larger register framework being workedout by conversation analysts and other ethnomethodologists rather thanwithin the narrow contextual principles developed by sociolinguists. Publicdebate cannot be considered a purely informational context, but research hasshown (dayman, 1988,1992; Heritage & Greatbatch, 1991; Hutchby, 1992a,1992b) that throughout Western society the debate format requires elaboraterhetorical flaunting of the rituals of social agreement. Consequently, noncon-traction of negatives, like pitch prominence on negatives or interruption(Adams, 1992; Adams & Edelsky, 1990), is consistent with the modified inter-active footing specific to debates.

Variation in contraction (and other prosodic reduction) among debatespeakers was also consistent with other social variables. The female politicianin the Rio Salado debate contracted much more than her male counterpart,but less than Mecham and Babbitt; she prosodically reduced negatives muchmore consistently than the men, but less than lecturers or conversationalists.Whether this is considered evidence of a female speaker's inability to main-tain the appropriate footing for debates or of a greater female sensitivity tothe Social Agreement Principle, the consistency of the evidence with thatgathered for other interactional variables implies that an elaborated study

Page 23: Contraction of negatives as evidence of variance in

CONTRACTION OF NEGATIVES 23

100%

u

2 go%-

I* 60%

BO 4 0 % -

*2 0 % -

1%

Uncontracted dialogue

Uncontracted prose

Pitch Prominence (FTA)

g (Casual Conversations)

.s .a•5 at

£

Speaker or Author

FIGURE 4. Percent uncontracted negatives in descriptive prose and neutral dia-logue (or conversation), compared with pitch prominence on face-threateningdisagreements.

of register variation is incomplete if it does not consider personal socialvariables.

Figure 4 compares negative noncontraction with pitch prominence,because the two measures should be directly correlated. It shows that for themost part noncontraction is more directly correlated with pitch prominencethan with a simple assessment of planned versus unplanned, and that bothare more directly correlated with the acceptability of social disagreement thanwith the cognitive significance of disagreement.

DISCUSSION

This study was initiated to determine the relative effect of the CognitiveProminence Principle (of informational content) and the Social AgreementPrinciple (of interactional intent) on the contraction of negatives in differ-ent registers of North American English. Preliminary evidence had shownthat prosodic variation was consistent with the understanding that these tworules interact with register variation in complex ways, and further studiesshould broaden our understanding of the evidence presented here.

1. Uncontracted and pitch prominent realization is favored where informationalintent is primary, as proposed by the Cognitive Prominence Principle.

Page 24: Contraction of negatives as evidence of variance in

24 MALCAH YAEGER-DROR

2. Uncontracted pitch prominent realization is also favored in the actual inter-actional (Biber's "involved") register dimension, when the interactional intentis supportive (Goffman, 1971), but the contraction effect is not found inscripted supportive negatives, although pitch prominence is somewhat moreconsistent, even in dialogue.

3. Reduction is favored in interactional (Biber's involved) or even pseudo-interactional registers if the content of the negative is either neutral or face-threatening (Brown & Levinson, 1978); in such cases, the Social AgreementPrinciple is primary. The prominence distinctions related to interactionalintent found in actual interactions are not realized in the dialogue contrac-tion percentages, but only in the pitch prominence.

The present article initially proposed that fictional dialogue would reflectthe pattern found in conversation, whereas fictional informational prose pas-sages would conform more consistently to the Cognitive Prominence Prin-ciple. Figure 1 and Table 4 substantiate the claim that narrative prose is muchless likely than dialogue to exhibit contraction. However, neutral dialogue isfor some writers more contracted than disagreements. The presumed SocialAgreement Principle designed hierarchy of different dialogue contexts doesnot reflect the actual hierarchy based on polite conversational interactions.For most of the authors, neutral and supportive negatives are more often con-tracted than face-threatening negatives. This may reflect a deterioration inverisimilitude in dialogue. On the other hand, it could reflect the fact that theauthors are portraying family or childhood peer arguments, whereas the con-versational data consist of polite (but generally nonintimate) interactions. TheSocial Agreement Principle is neutralized in political debates, where high ratesof uncontracted pitch prominent negatives are found. However, even the mostcombative debater did not prosodically focus on the negations categorically.

Other studies of contraction have shown that specific registers favorgreater or lesser contraction, depending on their formality or plannedness.The present study chose to foreground the fact that negative contraction, asa form of extreme reduction of the negative, is not merely correlated with aplanned-unplanned dimension, but should also be correlated with those fac-tors that favor prosodic reduction of the negative. When viewed from thisricher perspective, data for negative contraction demonstrate that, asexpected, all forms of negative reduction are highly correlated with style, reg-ister, and interactional intent. Comparing contraction from all corpora, mosthypotheses proposed are supported. With regard to style, all things beingequal, contractions are more likely to occur in less planned language. In termsof register, the more informational the writing, the more consistently contrac-tion is avoided. Regardless of the focal information conveyed by a negative,contractions are more likely to occur in scripted speech than in purely descrip-tive passages. They are also more likely to occur in more interactive registers(or even scripted pseudoregisters) than in more informational registers. Leastreduction occurs in older neutral fiction and in fiction for children. As faras intent is concerned, following the Social Agreement Principle, contractions

Page 25: Contraction of negatives as evidence of variance in

CONTRACTION OF NEGATIVES 25

should occur in conflictual interactive situations more than in neutral situa-tions, but the hypothesis is not supported by the fictional dialogue sampledhere. The Social Agreement Principle is neutralized under unusual circum-stances like debates, where negatives are prominent more consistently.

The results support the hypothesis that negative contraction is correlatedwith the factors that favor prosodic reduction of the negative. In the case ofprosodic pitch prominence (Yaeger-Dror, 1996a), more informational contextsare more likely to be pitch prominent, even if they are contracted. In thisstudy of contraction, the more informational contexts are less likely to becontracted, and the more involved contexts are more likely to be contracted(Yaeger-Dror, 1996a, found they are also more likely to be prosodicallyreduced), unless the negative is supportive. This evidence demonstrates thatnegative contraction, far from being a simple factor correlated with planned-ness, as described in earlier studies, can be fruitfully considered as one param-eter of prosodic reduction. The results also provide further support for aninformational-interactional register dimensional continuum like that foundin Yaeger-Dror (1985) and later proposed as a general register dimension byBiber (1988) and Finegan (1994); at the same time, the evidence shows thatthe informational-interactional continuum cannot be interpreted in an overlysimplistic way. Further studies of other written registers and other writers(and readers) should permit a clearer perspective of the variables involved andthe likelihood of negative reduction in different situations.

Yaeger-Dror (1985, 1996b) suggested that a more elaborated understand-ing of register should incorporate the vectors of power and solidarity (Brown& Gilman, 1960). These are difficult to access using the present corpora,except in the analysis of the Cleary fiction, where the power vector was in-versely correlated with negative prominence. Further analysis of a varied cor-pus of political debates would enrich our understanding of the degree towhich the Social Agreement Principle can be abrogated by register-specificfooting and by other social variables, such as gender, ethnicity, and power.

Further analysis is clearly necessary to disentangle the roots of register vari-ation, to clarify our understanding of the theoretical and practical impli-cations of the Social Agreement Principle's precedence over the CognitiveProminence Principle, and to permit quantification of the prominence vari-ables for practical needs, such as speech recognition and synthesis. For exam-ple, sociolinguists can do speech technology a service by attending to theimmediate technological needs without losing sight of the larger question offormulating a realistic register theory.25 Although our prototheory of style(like the technologists') prefers that even task-oriented (Labov, 1989; Yaeger,1974) reading should be realized with pitch prominence on negatives, thepresent study shows that a wider range of reading tasks, or registers, shouldbe isolated from each other and analyzed separately. The results would havedirect impact on speech technology while fine-tuning our understanding ofsociolinguistic variation. Rickford and McNair-Knox (1994) demonstratedthat isolating speech for separate audiences (in the sense of Bell, 1984) and

Page 26: Contraction of negatives as evidence of variance in

26 MALCAH YAEGER-DROR

topics (as used by Hindle, 1979, or Coupland, 1980) also permits us to eval-uate different aspects of the interactional context in our understanding ofregister.

It goes without saying that any analysis of reading should be comparedwith an analysis of the type of interaction that such reading is meant tomimic, and that any analysis of debates should be compared with the anal-ysis of conversational confrontation and disagreement strategies.26 The anal-ysis of a wider range of debate settings and perhaps even the categorizationof different dialogues relative to audience design and topic would permit anelaboration of our understanding of these social parameters and should beattempted in the near future. The greater accessibility of the recorded datafor reading and debates, along with the greater practical applications for suchdata, entails that as much evidence as possible should be gleaned from suchsources before attacking the larger questions that will arise in more casualconversational settings.

Looking back at Table 2, we are reminded that some locutions are morelikely to be contracted on the negative (we haven't} than on the verb (we'venot), but that this is less true for some verbs (we aren't vs. we're not).A thorough variable rule analysis of this issue should therefore distinguishauxiliary verbs from each other even in NPR English. Another theoreticalextension of the work here would require analysis of at least one dialect thatis primarily auxiliary-reducing. The analysis of negative-reducing dialectsdemonstrates that both contraction and pitch reduction on negatives are gen-erally triggered by the Social Agreement Principle. Analysis of auxiliary-reducing dialects may demonstrate that the relationship between the CognitiveProminence Principle and the Social Agreement Principle is different in dia-lects that do not favor negative contraction, or (more likely) that pitch re-duction on negatives is even more noticeable in these dialects, as a way ofcompensating for dialectal morphosyntactic preferences. Data from dialectslike Ocracoke, where negative contraction is less likely, should be comparedwith data from more standard dialects. Finally, given the discussion in theliterature of cultures that consider confrontation a sign of social involvementrather than an interactional breakdown (Schiffrin, 1984; Tannen, 1984), thecomparison of interactions and (say) debates from other cultures could befruitfully compared with those analyzed here in order to determine the rela-tive tendency toward the culture of considerateness or involvement (Tannen,1984, 1985). This information in turn could serve both theoretical and prac-tical ends.

NOTES

1. For another perspective on negation, see Mulkay (1986).2. A more careful (but cumbersome) designation might be Standard North American English

(as spoken by educated speakers); given that the locution "BBC English" is used for the Britishequivalent, MNPR English" will be used here.

Page 27: Contraction of negatives as evidence of variance in

CONTRACTION OF NEGATIVES 27

3. Recent studies also support a theory that negatives are progressively reduced in French aswell. For example, Hirschbuhlcr and Labelle (1994) pointed out that the reduction of non to cliticne is first attested in the 14th century, and although the first case of point support attested datesto 1372, until the end of the 16th century ne was generally the only negative in a clause. Ate-support(by par or point) then became common, presumably to permit disambiguation, because the neg-ative ne was generally reduced. Slowly, point itself was more generally reduced to pas, and ininfinitival clauses the supporting clement (point/pas) was fronted (before the verb). If this istrue even for written (and potentially more informational) usage, reduction of negation was pre-sumably even more advanced in speech. Today, at least in Canadian French, ne is most oftenabsent (Labov, 1994; Vincent & Sankoff, 1977). Yet despite the fact that pas now carries thiscritical information, the duration of the pay vowel is systematically shorter than other (a) vow-els; in fact, it is generally so reduced as to alter vowel color as well as duration (Lennig, 1978;Yaeger, 1979). One tentative conclusion is that French (like English) favors the Social Agree-ment Principle over the Cognitive Prominence Principle.4. This concept was further developed in later work by Pomerantz (1984).5. Other prosodic register analyses include, for example, Brazil (1984, 1985), Bruce (1992),

Engstrand (1989), Ghadessy (1993), Goodwin (1992), Granstrom and Nord (1992), Halliday(1992), Halliday and Hasan (1976, 1989), Hirschberg and Litman (1990), Levin et al. (1982),Nevalainen (1987, 1992), Selling (1988, 1994, 1995), Ure (1982).6. The conversational corpora, transcribed by Gail Jefferson, were made available to me by

Jefferson, Sacks, and Schegloff. Their code designations are mnemonic: e.g., TG = Two Girls,GTS-5 = fifth Group Therapy Session. The SCRL corpus comes from a U.S. governmentAdvanced Research Projects Agency (ARPA) workshop on speech recognition and synthesis,held at the Speech Communication Research Lab (hence SCRL) in Santa Barbara. It was tapedby Bea Oshika and transcribed by myself. I owe thanks to Oshika and the other SCRL research-ers for their generosity and assistance in collecting the data.

7. Indeed, the corpus was transcribed and analyzed to reflect more directly the prosodic pat-terns of the O"Shaughnessy speakers in an instructional but task oriented setting (cf. Labov, 1989;Yaeger, 1974) and to evaluate the Cognitive Prominence Principle on the basis of the speech ofthose who had proposed it.8. In the cited examples, curly brackets indicate that a segment was realized parenthetically.

Unless otherwise specified, the section so annotated was spoken more quickly and with loweramplitude and narrower pitch range than the surrounding speech (cf. Hirschberg, 1992); degreesigns indicate that the segment was delivered very softly.

9. Note that (4) begins informationally, but (given that others will not agree with her) thespeaker self-corrects, both by inserting a parenthetical implication that by herself she would nothave known this ({"I should have realized that just from-frm wa:tching them,")) and by repeat-ing, prosodically correcting toward a prosodically neutral negative {'n'they don't swoop fromthe front).10. Supportive interchanges using negatives need not involve direct agreement as in the aboveexamples. Following the logic of Brown and Levinson (1978) and Goffman (1971), whether theaction is supportive (You're not fat!) or face-threatening (You're not right!) is considered moreinteractionally significant than whether it is an agreement or a disagreement on the surface level.11. On the other hand, the question might well be asked why the negative formulation is pre-ferred (i.e., occurs much more frequently) over an equally available positive formulation, suchas / agree!12. In Yaeger (1985) I drew the distinction between instructional and interactional. I find Biber'sterm informational more apt than the former term, since many contexts are informational with-out being instructive; but interactional is more apt than Biber's term involved, since, for exam-ple, even pseudointeractional materials to be discussed here follow a pattern similar to Biber'sinvolved register. Moreover, the more politely interactive (and less obviously involved) an inter-action is, the more likely the Social Agreement Principle is to counteract the Cognitive Promi-nence Principle. In short, I believe the register-dimension continuum should be characterized asinformational-interactional, and the contrasting poles will be referred to here using thatterminology.13. Note that the measure of contraction used by previous studies is not directly comparableto that used here, since negative-contraction (like isn't) and modal contraction (like 's not) arepooled; however, because most contractions are negative contractions, the evidence is relevant.14. In fact, the African American English Vernacular preference for deleting the auxiliary (we

Page 28: Contraction of negatives as evidence of variance in

28 MALCAH YAEOER-DROR

not) rather than contracting the negative (we haven't) (cf. Labov, 1969) may be seen as a devel-opment from the British pattern of auxiliary contraction (we've not) rather than the negative-contracting Northern U.S. dialect pattern. The present study should thus be seen as analyzingcontraction variation of urban U.S. "Standard" speakers only.15. Cleary's popularity for generations of children provides evidence that her particular choiceof contracted and uncontracted negatives is appropriate for both parent-presenters and the child-readers of Ramona books.16. Ultimately, only one book of Atwood's was used for the analysis of contraction, since thesecond book contained almost no negatives. This provides one more piece of evidence that writ-ers radically manipulate the presence of negatives as well as their prominence (by choosing whetherto contract them or lexicalize them so as to minimize prominence) to achieve specific literary ends.17. Similarly, a recent study of the use of Montreal French Vernacular variables showed thatplaywright Michel Tremblay's dialogue is consistent with actual conversational usage: "The morestigmatized forms are over-represented in the plays [relative to interview data], while the less stig-matized ones pretty well match real use by local speakers" (Fonollosa, 1995:39).18. The tapes and transcripts were kindly donated by Karen Adams of Arizona State Univer-sity, and the data have been used for her own work as well (Adams, 1992; Adams & Edelsky,1988, 1990). Initially, a third debate was included in the corpus: the 1988 Democratic Conven-tion. However, close perusal of that corpus showed that the participants' footing differed radi-cally from the other two debates, so the corpus will not be analyzed in detail here.19. At the time the data were collected, the MacNeil-Lehrer Report was the primary televisedequivalent of NPR speech.20. For example, MacNeil (M/B) was fairly consistently (pseudo)neutral:

((Moderator denying any negative implications in what he's said:))Mecham: . . . So I think the representation that everybody is opposed to what I've

done,.h isn't quite correct.MacNeil: I don't think we meant to suggest that! (M/B)

MacNeil's role is similar to that of the therapist in a group therapy session analyzed by Yaeger-Dror (1985) (GTS-5); in that study as well, the therapist's register, like the moderator's here, wasquite different from the speech of other coparticipants. In contrast, the other moderator (Rio)did elaborate displays of disbelief of what either debater said.

((Moderator denying any trust in either debater:))Moderator: OK. She: says you have the power, you: say you don't have any power?,

unless you go through all these checks and balances?Mr. Dewitt: Exactly.Moderator: OK, it's — Folks, it's only who you believe here! You make up your own

minds, because /•• don't know myself! (Rio)

More detailed discussion of the footing or stance of the moderator toward speakers can be foundin Button (1987), dayman (1988, 1992), Drew (1992), Goffman (1981), Greatbatch (1992), Her-itage (1985), Heritage and Greatbatch (1991), and Hutchby (1992a, 1992b).21. Confrontation itself is the focus of a number of recent studies. A review of recent literatureon confrontation highlights the fact that in interactional registers, confrontations may not usenegatives at all (Jacobs, 1992; Jacobs & Jackson, 1992; Newell & Stutman, 1988; Pomerantz,1989; Schegloff, 1989). The debate material, in contrast, used negatives more prominently thanany other register studied. A thorough study would thus extrapolate from the continuum dis-cussed here (pitch prominent negatives to pitch reduced and contracted negatives) to include thevarious indirect means whereby a speaker avoids the use of any negative phrasing at all (asdescribed in Schegloff et al., 1977).22. Supportive and face-threatening debate negatives can often be distinguished by copartici-pants' reactions. In the 1988 Democratic Convention, emphatic-prominent statements were sureto be met with vociferous agreement by coparticipants and loud applause from the audience.

Duk: Well, the first thing I wouldn't do is trade arms to the Ayatola!((wild applause; agreement by codebaters in immediately succeeding turns))

Page 29: Contraction of negatives as evidence of variance in

CONTRACTION OF NEGATIVES 2 9

Duk: You've got to understand one thing: You never, ever make concessions to terrorists!No exceptions! No exceptions!((wild clapping))

Gep: There's not gonna be progress,there's not gonna be a continuation of Camp David,and there's not gonna b e . . . ((agreement in succeeding moves))

The examples here are all supportive interchanges and should not be classed with the emphaticdisagreements voiced in the Rio and M/B debates, yet both emphatically supportive and emphat-ically face-threatening debate turns use prominent negatives, implying that the Social AgreementPrinciple is neutralized.23. In contrast, Hazen (1995) found high percentages of ain't in Ocracoke. Wharton's ruralheroine also uses ain't. Trollope's use of ain't by upper-class British speakers of his own gener-ation demonstrates that U.S. Standard morphological taboos are not universal.24. Both earlier work (Yaeger-Dror, 1985, 1996a, 1996b) and the present study differ fromTottie (1991) by including a neutral (informational) option in interaction or dialogue. Even inconversations, much speech is merely intended as informational and cannot be considered as eitherface-threatening or supportive of coparticipants in a conversation.25. Recently, synthesis researchers have acknowledged their own need to pay closer attentionto register variation (Bladon et al., 1987; Bruce, 1992; Cahn, 1990; Carlson et al., 1990; Eng-strand, 1989,1992; Granstrom, 1992; Hirschberg & Grosz, 1994; Liberman, 1992; Shockey, 1995),even for fairly simple tasks like book reading (Fant & Kruckcnberg, 1989).26. See note 21.

REFERENCES

Adams, K. (1992). Accruing power on debate floors. In K. Hall, M. Bucholtz, & B. Moonwomon(Eds.), Locating power (pp. 1-10). Berkeley: University of California Press.

Adams, K., & Edelsky, E. (1988). Male and female styles in political debates. In Ferrara et al.(Eds.), Linguistic change and contact. Austin: University of Texas Press. 18-24.

(1990). Creating inequality: Breaking the rules in debates. Journal of Language and SocialPsychology 9:171 -190.

Atwood, M. (1979). Life before man. New York: Fawcett.(1983). Bluebeard's Egg. New York: Fawcett. [1990. Read by M. Atwood. Toronto: Ran-

dom House.]Bell, A. (1984). Language style as audience design. Language in Society 13:145-204.

(1991a). Audience of accommodation in the mass media. In H. Giles, N. Coupland, &J. Coupland (Eds.), Contexts of accommodation. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.69-102.

(1991b). Language in the news media. Oxford: Blackwell.(1992). Hit and miss: Referee design in the dialects of New Zealand television advertise-

ments. Language and Communication 12:327-342.Biber, D. (1988). Variation across speech and writing. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

(1994). An analytical framework for register studies. In D. Biber & E. Finegan (Eds.),Perspectives on register: Situating register variation within sociolinguistics. Oxford: OxfordUniversity Press. 31-56.

Bladon, A., Carlson, R., Granstrom, B., Hunnicutt, S., & Karlsson, I. (1987). A test-to-speechsystem for British English and issues of dialect and style. In J. Laver & M. Jack (Eds.), Euro-pean conference on speech technology: Vol. 1. Edinburgh: CEP Consultants. 55-58.

Bolinger, D. (1978). Intonation across languages. In J. Greenberg (Ed.), Universals of humanlanguage: Vol. 2. Stanford: Stanford University Press. 471-524.

Brazil, D. (1984). The intonation of sentences read aloud. In D. Gibbon & H. Richter (Eds.),Intonation, accent and rhythm. Berlin: de Gruyter. 46-66.

(1985). The communicative value of intonation in English. Birmingham: English Lan-guage Research.

Page 30: Contraction of negatives as evidence of variance in

30 MALCAH YAEOER-DROR

Brown, G. (1983). Prosodic structures and the given/new distinction. In A. Cutler & R. Ladd(Eds.), Prosody: Models and measurements. Berlin: Springer-Verlag. 67-77.

Brown, R., & Gilman, A. (1960). The pronouns of power and solidarity. In T. Sebeok (Ed.),Style in language. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. 253-276.

Brown, P., & Levinson, S. (1978). Universals of language usage: Politeness phenomena. In E.Goody (Ed.), Questions and politeness: Strategies in social interaction. Cambridge: CambridgeUniversity Press. 256-289.

Bruce, G. (1992). On the analysis of prosody in spontaneous speech. Speech Communication11:453-458.

Brunei, G. (1970). Lefrancais radiophonique a Montreal. Master's thesis, University de Montreal.Button, G. (1987). Answers as interactional products: Two sequential practices used in interviews.

Social Psychology Quarterly 50:160-171.Cahn, J. (1990). The generation of affect in synthesized speech. / . American Voice I/O Soc

8:1-19.Carlson, L. (1984). Focus and dialogue games. In L. Vaina & J. Hintikka (Eds.), Cognitive con-

straints on communication. Dordrecht: Reidel. 295-333.Carlson, R., Granstrom, B., & Hunnicutt, S. (1990). Multi-lingual text-to-speech development

and applications. In A. Ainsworth (Ed.), Advances in speech, hearing and language process-ing. London: JAI. 269-296.

Cheshire, J., Edwards, V., & Whittle, P. (1989). Urban British dialect grammar. English WorldWide 10:185-225.

Clayman, S. (1988). Displaying neutrality in television news interviews. Social Problems35:474-492.

(1992). Footing in the achievement of reality. In P. Drew & J. Heritage (Eds.), Talk atwork. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. 163-198.

Cleary, B. (1968). Ramona the pest. New York: Scholastic Books. [1990. Read by S. Channing.Listening Library.]

(1981). Ramona Quimby, age 8. New York: Dell.Coker, C , & Umeda, N. (1971). Toward a theory of stress and prosody in American English.

Proceedings of the 7th International Congress of Acoustics. Budapest. 137-140.Coupland, N. (1980). Style shifting in a Cardiff work setting. Language and Society 9:1-12.Drew, P. (1992). Contested evidence in courtroom cross-examination. In P. Drew & J. Heritage

(Eds.), Talk at work. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Engstrand, O. (1989). Fo correlates of tonal word accents in spontaneous speech: Range and sys-

tematicity of variation. Phonetic Experimental Research, Institute of Linguistics, Universityof Stockholm (PERILUS) 10:1-12.

(1992). Systematicity of phonetic variation in natural discourse. Speech Communication11:337-346.

Fant, G., & Kruckenberg, A. (1989). Preliminaries to the study of Swedish prose reading andreading style. STL-QPSR 2.

Finegan, E. (1994). Language: Its structure and use. San Diego: Harcourt Brace Jovanovich.Finegan, E., & Biber, D. (1994). Parallel patterns in social dialect and register variation: Towards

an integrated theory. In D. Biber & E. Finegan (Eds.), Perspectives on register: Situating reg-ister variation within sociolinguistics. Oxford: Oxford University Press. 315-350.

Fonollosa, M.-O. (1995). The representation of spoken French in Quebec theater. Paper pre-sented at NWAVE-XXIV, University of Pennsylvania.

Fowler, C. A. (1988). Differential shortening of repeated content words produced in various com-municative contexts. Language and Speech 31:307-319.

Fowler, C. A., & Housum, J. (1987). Talkers* signaling of "new" and "old" words in speech andlisteners' perception and use of the distinction. Journal of Memory and Language 26:489-504.

Ghadessy, M. (Ed.). (1993). Register analysis: Theory and practice. London: Pinter.Goffman, E. (1967). The presentation of self in everyday life. Harmondsworth: Penguin.

(1971). Relations in public. New York: Harper & Row.(1981). Forms of talk. Oxford: Blackwcll.

Goldberg, J. (1978). Amplitude shift: A mechanism for the affiliation of utterances in conver-sational interaction. In J. Schenkein (Ed.), Studies in the organization of conversational inter-action. New York: Academic.

Goodwin, M. (1992). He-said-she-said. Bloomington: Indiana University Press.Granstrom, B. (1992). Use of speech synthesis in exploring different speech styles. Speech Com-

munication 11:347-356.

Page 31: Contraction of negatives as evidence of variance in

CONTRACTION OF NEGATIVES 31

GranstrSm, B., & Nord, A. (1992). Neglected dimensions of speech synthesis. Speech Commu-nication 11:459-462.

Greatbatch, D. (1992). The management of disagreement between news interviewees. In P. Drew& J. Heritage (Eds.), Talk at work. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. 268-301.

Halliday, M. (1992). How do you mean? In L. Ravelli & M. Davies (Eds.), Advances in systemiclinguistics. London: Pinter.

Halliday, M., & Hasan, R. (1976). Cohesion in English. London: Longman.(1989). Language, context and text. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Hazen, K. (1995). Linguistic preference and prescriptive dictum: On the phonological and mor-phological justification of ain't. Paper presented at NWAVE-XXIII.

(1996). Linguistic preference and prescriptine dictum. In J. Arnold et al. (Eds.), Sociolin-guistic variation: Data, theory, and analysis. Stanford: CSLI. 101-112.

Heritage, J. (1985). Analyzing news interviews: Aspects of the production of talk for an over-hearing audience. In T. van Dijk (Ed.), Handbook of discourse analysis: Vol. 3. London: Aca-demic. 95-117.

Heritage, J., & Greatbatch, D. (1991). On the institutional character of institutional talk: Thecase of news interviews. In D. Boden & D. Zimmerman (Eds.), Talk and social structure. Cam-bridge: Polity. 47-98.

Hindle, D. (1979). The social and situational conditioning of phonetic variation. Doctoral the-sis, University of Pennsylvania.

Hirschberg, J. (1990). Accent and discourse context: Assigning pitch accent in synthetic speech:The given/new distinction and deaccentability. Proceedings of the Eighth National Confer-ence on Artificial Intelligence. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. 952-957.

(1992). Prosodic variation for text-to-speech synthesis. Journal of the Acoustical Soci-ety of America 94:1841 (A).

Hirschberg, J., & Grosz, B. (1994). Intonation and discourse structure in spontaneous and readdirection giving. In Proceedings of the international symposium on prosody. Yokohama.103-109.

Hirschberg, J., & Litman, D. (1990). Disambiguating cue phrases in text and speech. Proceed-ings of the 13th International Conference on Computational Linguistics.

Hirschbuhler, P., & Labelle, M. (1994). Changes in verb position in French negative infinitivalclauses. Language Variation and Change 6:149-178.

Hutchby, I. (1992a). The pursuit of controversy: Routine skepticism in talk on talk radio. Sociol-ogy 26:673-694.

(1992b). Confrontation talk: Aspects of "interruption" in argument sequences on talkradio. Text 12:343-371.

Jacobs, S. (1992). Argumentation without advocacy. In F. van Eemeren, R. Grootendorst, J.Blair, & C. Willard (Eds.), Argumentation illuminated. Dordrecht: SICSAT. 270-280.

Jacobs, S., & Jackson, S. (1992). Relevance and digression in argumentative discussion. Argu-mentation 6:\6\-\12.

Jones, K. (1994). Masked negotiation in a Japanese work setting. In A. Firth (Ed.), The discourseof negotiation: Studies of language in the workplace. Oxford: Pergamon.

Keillor, G. (1985). Lake Wobegon days. New York: Penguin. [1986. Read by G. Keillor. Min-nesota Public Radio.].

Koopmans-Van Beinum, F. (1992). The role of focus words in natural and synthetic continuousspeech. Speech Communication 11:439-452.

Labov, W. (1969). Contraction, deletion, and inherent variability of the English copula. Lan-guage 45:715-762.

Keillor, G. (1985). Lake Wobegon days. New York: Penguin. [1986. Read by G. Keillor. Min-nesota Public Radio.]

Koopmans-Van Beinum, F. (1992). The role of focus words in natural and synthetic continuousspeech. Speech Communication 11:439-452.

Labov, W. (1969). Contraction, deletion, and inherent variability of the English copula. Lan-guage 45:1 \S-162.

(1989). Exact description of the speech community. In R. Fasold & D. Schiffrin (Eds.),Language change and variation. Philadelphia: Benjamins. 1-57.

(1994). Foundations of linguistic change. Oxford: Blackwell.Leech, G. (1980). Exploration in semantics and pragmatics. Amsterdam: Benjamins.Lennig, M. (1978). Acoustic measurements of linguistic change: The modern Paris vowel sys-

tem. Philadelphia: U.S. Regional Survey.

Page 32: Contraction of negatives as evidence of variance in

32 MALCAH YAEGER-DROR

Levin, H., Schaffer, C , & Snow, C. (1982). The prosodic and paralinguistic features of read-ing and telling stories. Language and Speech 25:43-54.

Liberman, M. (1992). Comments on various papers, IRCS Workshop on Prosody in NaturalSpeech, IRCS Technical Report 92-37.

Mendoza-Denton, N. (1996). Multiple membership and stance taking in Latina adolescents'con-versation. Paper presented at NWAVE-XXIII.

Mulkay, M. (1986). Agreement and disagreement in conversations and letters. Text 5:201-227.Nevalainen, T. (1987). Adverbial focusing and intonation. Lingua 73:141-165.

(1992). Intonation and discourse type. Text 12:397-427.Newell, S., & Stutman, L. (1988). The expression of dissatisfaction. Communication Monographs

55:266-285.Nooteboom, S.G., & Kruyt, J.G. (1987). Accents, focus distribution, and the perceived distri-

bution of given and new information: An experiment. Journal of the Acoustical Society ofAmerica 82:1512-1524.

Ochs, E. (1979). Planned and unplanned discourse. In T. Givon (Ed.), Syntax and semantics:Vol. 12. New York: Academic. 51-80.

O'Shaughnessy, D., & Allen, J. (1983). Linguistic modality effects on fundamental frequency.Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 74:1155-1171.

Pomerantz, A. (1984). Agreeing and disagreeing with assessments: Some features of preferred/dispreferred turn shapes. In J. Atkinson & J. Heritage (Eds.), Structures of social action: Stud-ies in conversation analysis. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

(1989). Constructing skepticism. Research on Language and Social Interaction22:293-314.

Prince, E. (1981). Toward a taxonomy of given-new information. In P. Cole (Ed.), Radical prag-matics. New York: Academic. 223-256.

Rickford, J., & McNair-Knox, F. (1994). Addressee and topic influenced style shift. In D. Biber& E. Finegan (Eds.), Perspectives on register: Situating register variation within sociolinguis-tics. Oxford: Oxford University Press. 235-276.

Schegloff, E. (1989). From interview to confrontation: Observations of the Bush-Rather encoun-ter. Research on Language and Social Interaction 22:215-240.

Schegloff, E., Jefferson, G., & Sacks, H. (1977). Preference for self-correction in the organi-zation of repair in conversation. Language 53:361-382.

Schiffrin, D. (1984). Jewish argument as sociability. Language in Society 13:311-335.Schilling-Estes, N., & Wolfram, W. (1994). Convergent explanation and alternative regulariza-

tion patterns: Were/weren't leveling in a vernacular English variety. Language Variation andChange 6:273-302.

Selling, M. (1988). The role of intonation in the organization of repair and problem handlingin sequences in conversation. Journal of Pragmatics 12:293-322.

(1994). Emphatic speech style—With special focus on the prosodic signalling of height-ened emotive involvement in conversation. Journal of Pragmatics 22:375-408.

(1995). Prosody as an activity type distinctive cue in conversation: 'Astonished' questionsin repair initiation. In E. Couper-Kuhlen & M. Selting (Eds.), Prosody in conversation. Cam-bridge: Cambridge University Press.

Shockey, L. (Ed.). (1995). University of Reading Speech Research Laboratory Work in Progress8. Department of Linguistic Science, University of Reading, Whiteknights.

Svartvik, J. (1990). (Ed.). The London-Lund Corpus of Spoken English. Lund: Lund Univer-sity Press.

Tannen, D. (1984). Conversational style: Analyzing talk among friends. Norwood, NJ: Ablex.(1985). Relative focus on involvement in oral and written discourse. In D. Olson, N. Tor-

rance, & A. Hildyard (Eds.), Literacy, language and learning. Cambridge: Cambridge Uni-versity Press. 124-147.

(1989). Talking voices. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Tottie, G. (1983). Much about 'not'and 'nothing': A study about the variation between analytic

and synthetic negation in contemporary American English. Lund: CWK Gleerup.(1987). Rejections, denials and explanatory statements. Studia Linguistica 4:154-164.(1991). Negation in English speech and writing. San Diego: Academic.

Trudgill, P. (1986). Dialects in contact. Oxford: Blackwell.Tyler, A. (1988). Breathing lessons. [1990. Read by J. Eikenberry. New York: Random House.]Ure, J. (Ed.). (1982). Issue on the Study of Register Range. International Journal of the Sociology

of Linguistics 35.

Page 33: Contraction of negatives as evidence of variance in

CONTRACTION OF NEGATIVES 33

Vincent, D., & Sankoff, G. (1977). L'emploi productif du 'ne* dans le francais montrdalais. LeFrancois Moderne 45:244-256.

Wharton, E. (1917 [1981]). Summer. New York: Berkley.Yaeger, M. (1974). Speaking style: Some etic realizations and their significance. Pennsylvania

Working Papers on Linguistic Change and Variation 1.(1979). Context-determined variation in Montreal French vowels. Doctoral thesis, Uni-

versity of Pennsylvania.Yaeger-Dror, M. (1985). International prominence on negatives in English. Language and Speech

28:197-230.(1988). The influence of changing group vitality on convergence toward a dominant lin-

guistic norm. Language and Communication 8:285-306.(1993). Linguistic analysis of dialect "correction" and its interaction with cognitive sali-

ence. Language Variation and Change 5:189-224.(1996a). Register as a variable in prosodic analysis: The case of the English negative.

Speech Communication 19 (in press).(1996b). Intonation and register variation. Paper presented at NWAVE-XXIII.

Yaeger-Dror, M., & Nunamaker, J. (1992). Negatives and Register. Journal of the AcousticalSociety of America 91:S3288(A).

Zwicky, A., & Pullum, G. (1982). Cliticization vs. inflection: English n't. Bloomington: 1ULC.

APPENDIX 1

PITCH PROMINENT, CONTRACTED DATA (PT l a )

oh::; c a r e . [Keillor 1985:1161

0.000 s 0,050 ««c./tlc 0-500 Hz Tl»a Domain fO Plot

I 0.8301 »0.0159.Pitch fr«qu«nct| - 133 Hz* Dui-lZOmsec

PITCH PROMINENT, CONTRACTED DATA (PT l b )

ari thestii want an'is w ing his power,

Pitch fragutneu • 130 Hi -lOOHz*Dur-359msec

-100

eh-

3.20O *

t 1.3771

fall!

3.2191

g

Midi* ^

.200 see, /tic 0-500 Hz Tl Ri« Domqln f 0 Plot

» 3

(BabWn( Screen 55)

Page 34: Contraction of negatives as evidence of variance in

34 MALCAH YAEGER-DROR

NONPROMINENT PITCH, CONTRACTED DATA/PITCH PROMINENT, CONTRACTED DATA (PT l c )

•IOOT-

{•itisn'any thing*)m

r

uu

it isa* anything of my doing*

1.90Di LOT f »c Jt ic 0-SDO Kt Tin Ocmin fO Plot 4.197,

I 0.HII Pichfreq-160*Dta*15O '2.8m 3.U77 (Kfprham 3 .2) .

NONPROMINENT PITCH, CONTRACTED DATA (PT I d )

ioo— 100

\Jb'tthe trwth is , we sti:ll don't* know.

3.000 t 0.203 f C / t l c O-5OO Ht fO Plol

[KeQlor 1985:119]

t 1.0131 Q.DDOO

•Pithfrn»70hz

NONPROMINENT PITCH, CONTRACTED DATA (PT l e )

100

If o:tber tovns have one, don'» knov about it.lKefflorl985)

o.ooo s 0.200 ««c./tlc 0-500 Hr T lw Domain fO Plot

Pi«hfreqwncy»120H2* Dm* 140msec

Page 35: Contraction of negatives as evidence of variance in

CONTRACTION OF NEGATIVES 3 5

A P P E N D I X 2

EXAMPLES OF CONTRACTED AND UNCONTRACTEDTOKENS IN THE BOOK DATA

Nondialogue, uncontracted

a. (Wharton, 1917)Charity was not very clear about the mountain.She was not very clear as to his purpose.The roads were not plowed up by hauling.She was not an expert work woman.

b. (Cleary, 1981)She did not want anything to spoil this exciting day.who was not going to let her sister get away with . . .She had not actually started third grade.

c. (Atwood, 1979)The crack will not widen and split.She does not trust the world.To remove all toys because some do not have them is not the answer.

d. (Tyler, 1988)In high school Maggie had not much liked him.They were not the dark Lotharios you would expect.if Serena believed that marriage was not a Doris Day movie

e. (Keillor-L, 1985)Women did not speak in meeting, [italics in original]Some Bretheren . . . were not so strict.were not content to worship in peace

f. (Keillor-M, 1985)The Dieners had not been getting full use of the dining room.

Dialogue, contracted

a. (Wharton, 1917)"You don't seem strong on architecture""you couldn't get a good draught through it.""and you weren't there""you needn't t rouble. . . it won't be this time tomorrow."

b. (Cleary, 1981)"I know it isn't easy.""We carrt both be the lady!""But I don't want to be the dog!"

c. (Atwood, 1979)"It wouldn't have happened.""I wouldn't have been forced."

d. (Tyler, 1988)"I haven't sung for 20 years or more.""I couldn't sing even then."

Page 36: Contraction of negatives as evidence of variance in

36 MALCAH YAEGER-DROR

e. (Keillor-M, 1985)"guys like you . . . have a lot of weaknesses that you don't find out about untilit's too late.""You don't get along with other people. You don't make an effort."

Prose dialogue(n = neutral, ft = face-threat, s = support)

a. (Wharton, 1917)"you couldn't get a good draught through it." (architect explaining how to fixhouse)—n"and you weren't there" (gloss 'You weren't on the job.')—ft"You don't seem strong on architecture..." (gloss 'You don't know what youhave here.')—ft"you needn't trouble . . . it won't be this time tomorrow."—s

b. (Cleary, 1981)"We car£t both be the lady!"-ft"But I don't want to be the dog!"-^"t"I know it isn't easy."—s

e. (Keillor-L, 1985)"We did not know what would become of us," wrote Oskar.—n"He does not care, he is a butcher and a barbarian," Magnus wrote to hisfamily.—nNote: Even if these are quotes from letters, as the text implies, the letters wouldhave been written in Norwegian, and the uncontracted/contracted distinctioncould not have been carried over from there.

f. (Keillor-M, 1985)"guys like you . . . have a lot of weaknesses that you don't find out about untilit's too late."-ft"You don't get along with other people. You don't make an effort. . . "—ft"You're so dumb you're going to hell and you don't even know it!"—ft"What are you doing! You can't shoot people here!"—ft [italics in original]