Clinical Neurophysiology Volume Issue 2013 [Doi 10.1016_j.clinph.2013.12.094] Kim, Soyeon; Liu, Zhongxu; Glizer, Daniel; Tannock, Rosemary; Wo -- Adult ADHD and Working Memory- Neural

Embed Size (px)

Citation preview

  • 8/13/2019 Clinical Neurophysiology Volume Issue 2013 [Doi 10.1016_j.clinph.2013.12.094] Kim, Soyeon; Liu, Zhongxu; Glizer

    1/33

  • 8/13/2019 Clinical Neurophysiology Volume Issue 2013 [Doi 10.1016_j.clinph.2013.12.094] Kim, Soyeon; Liu, Zhongxu; Glizer

    2/33

    WM encoding in ADHD

    1

    Adult ADHD and Working Memory: Neural Evidence of1

    Impaired Encoding2

    3

    Soyeon Kim, Zhongxu Liu, Daniel Glizer, Rosemary Tannock*, Steven Woltering*4

    5

    Department of Applied Psychology & Human Development, OISE, University of Toronto,6

    252 Bloor Street West, Toronto, Ont., Canada, M5S 1V67

    8

    E-mails: Soyeon Kim ([email protected]), Zhong-Xu Liu ([email protected]),9

    Daniel Glizer ([email protected]), Rosemary Tannock ([email protected]),10

    StevenWoltering ([email protected]).11

    12

    * Corresponding authors:13

    Steven Woltering14

    Department of Applied Psychology & Human Development, OISE, University of Toronto, 25215

    Bloor Street West, Toronto, Ont. Canada, M5S 1V616

    Tel: +1-416-978-092317

    [email protected]

    19

    or20

    21

    Rosemary Tannock22

    Department of Applied Psychology & Human Development, OISE, University of Toronto, 25223

    Bloor Street West, Toronto, Ont. Canada, M5S 1V624

    E-mail:[email protected]

    26

    27

    28

    mailto:[email protected]:[email protected]:[email protected]:[email protected]:[email protected]:[email protected]:[email protected]:[email protected]:[email protected]:[email protected]:[email protected]://ees.elsevier.com/clinph/viewRCResults.aspx?pdf=1&docID=8582&rev=3&fileID=398911&msid={E12BE538-7556-4FFF-93A2-9DF8EE7A69C9}mailto:[email protected]:[email protected]:[email protected]:[email protected]:[email protected]:[email protected]
  • 8/13/2019 Clinical Neurophysiology Volume Issue 2013 [Doi 10.1016_j.clinph.2013.12.094] Kim, Soyeon; Liu, Zhongxu; Glizer

    3/33

    WM encoding in ADHD

    2

    Highlights29

    30

    ADHD college students are compared to their typically developing peers on neural and31behavioural indices of working memory (WM).32

    Neural indices of working memory (P3) are examined during working memory encoding33

    using EEG34

    ADHD group showed lower P3 during working memory encoding, suggesting an35

    inefficient encoding process in WM.36

    37

    38

    39

    40

    4142

    43

    44

  • 8/13/2019 Clinical Neurophysiology Volume Issue 2013 [Doi 10.1016_j.clinph.2013.12.094] Kim, Soyeon; Liu, Zhongxu; Glizer

    4/33

    WM encoding in ADHD

    3

    Abstract45

    46

    Objectives: To investigate neural and behavioural correlates of visual encoding during a working47

    memory (WM) task in young adults with and without Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD).48

    Methods: A sample of 30 college students currently meeting a diagnosis of ADHD and 25 typically49

    developing students, matched on age and gender, performed a delayed match-to-sample task with low50

    and high memory load conditions. Dense-array electroencephalography was recorded. Specifically, the51

    P3, an event related potential (ERP) associated with WM, was examined because of its relation with52

    attentional allocation during WM. Task performance (accuracy, reaction time) as well as performance53

    on other neuropsychological tasks of WM was analyzed.54

    Results: Neural differences were found between the groups. Specifically, the P3 amplitude was smaller55

    in the ADHD group compared to the comparison group for both load conditions at parietal-occipital56

    sites. Lower scores on behavioral working memory tasks were suggestive of impaired behavioural WM57

    performance in the ADHD group.58

    Conclusions: Findings from this study provide the first evidence of neural differences in the encoding59

    stage of WM in young adults with ADHD, suggesting ineffective allocation of attentional resources60

    involved in encoding of information in WM.61

    Significance: These findings, reflecting alternate neural functioning of WM, may explain some of the62

    difficulties related to WM functioning that college students with ADHD report in their every day63

    cognitive functioning.64

    65

    66

    Keywords:Working memory, P3, adults with ADHD, encoding.67

    68

    69

  • 8/13/2019 Clinical Neurophysiology Volume Issue 2013 [Doi 10.1016_j.clinph.2013.12.094] Kim, Soyeon; Liu, Zhongxu; Glizer

    5/33

  • 8/13/2019 Clinical Neurophysiology Volume Issue 2013 [Doi 10.1016_j.clinph.2013.12.094] Kim, Soyeon; Liu, Zhongxu; Glizer

    6/33

    WM encoding in ADHD

    5

    framework of a "context updating theory" which proposes that P3 is increased when there is a need to95

    update, change, or refresh the current contents of WM. Researchers also focus on the intricate role of96

    attentional processes in working memory functioning. For instance, some suggest that WM capacity is97

    determined by an executive attentional process (Kane et al., 2007; Vogel, 2005), or the capacity of the98

    focus of attention (i.e. the scope of attention; Cowan, 2001). Ford, White, Lim, and Pfefferbaum (1994)99

    suggested that P3 amplitude reflects the effort to allocate attention (see also, Polich, 2007). Similarly,100

    Kok (2001) proposed that the P3 reflects the attentional capacity invested in the categorization of task101

    relevant events. In sum, these studies suggest a possible link between P3 amplitude and allocation of102

    attentional resources that subserve working memory functioning. Thus, in the current study, we interpret103

    P3 activation as reflecting the direction of attentional resources towards encoding information into WM,104

    such as categorizing events or updating mental representations. However, attention acts at many levels105

    of processing during working memory, including early perceptual processes (Rutman et al., 2010). For106

    instance, ERP components such as the P1 are believed to represent sensory responses elicited by visual107

    stimuli as early as 100 ms (Luck, 2005). Thus, we also measure the P1 component to ascertain whether108

    perceptual/sensory processes are intact or altered in college students with ADHD.109

    A recent meta-analysis of six studies investigating the P3 component in adults with ADHD found110

    significantly reduced P3 amplitude compared to controls (Szuromi et al., 2011). However, all of the111

    studies used a GoNoGo paradigm: none used a WM task. Moreover, a recently published EEG study of112

    WM functioning in adult ADHD utilized an n-back task and mainly investigated time-frequency113

    measures (Missonier et al., 2013). Among other findings in that study, Missonier and colleagues (2013)114

    reported reduced low frequency oscillations (e.g., theta) in the ADHD group during the period in which115

    the P3 would have occurred (e.g., between 200-500 ms), suggesting that both time-and frequency116

    domain measures reflect similar underlying neural processes.117

    Only one study to date has investigated the P3 component in the context of a delayed ma tch-to-118

    sample WM paradigm and this involved a comparison of participants with and without Schizophrenia119

  • 8/13/2019 Clinical Neurophysiology Volume Issue 2013 [Doi 10.1016_j.clinph.2013.12.094] Kim, Soyeon; Liu, Zhongxu; Glizer

    7/33

    WM encoding in ADHD

    6

    (Haenschel et al., 2007). In this paradigm, the to-be-remembered target stimuli were presented in a120

    sequence that varied in length (e.g., 2 versus 3 stimuli) to measure effects of WM load. Longer121

    sequences place greater demands on WM in terms of storage and updating information compared to122

    short sequences. Then, after a delay period, the participant is asked whether a test-stimulus matched one123

    of the targets. The task uses irregular shapes as stimuli to avoid verbal/semantic processing.124

    Significantly reduced mean P1 (an early visual component) and P3 amplitudes were found in individuals125

    with Schizophrenia compared to controls. The load effect was present only for the P1 component. The126

    authors interpreted the reduced P1 activity as an early visual processing deficit whereas the decreased127

    P3 amplitudes were interpreted as evidence of reduced ability in categorizing or evaluating stimuli.128

    To investigate WM, the present study adapted Haenschels WM paradigm (Haenschel et al.,129

    2007) and used it with a sample of college students with ADHD. This paradigm allowed us to130

    investigate the neural effects of encoding stimuli presented in sequence under different load conditions131

    (low versus high load). Specifically, the current task required participants to allocate attention to the132

    stimulus, register it, and then quickly shift attention to the next stimulus while keeping the previous one133

    in mind. This process requires both earlier attention allocation to sensory/perception of the stimuli and134

    later allocation of attention to evaluate the stimulus and update the mental representation. To the best of135

    our knowledge, no previous study has investigated the encoding phase of WM using a delayed-match-136

    to-sample task in participants with ADHD. A complete understanding of ADHD necessitates the study137

    of different subpopulations of this clinical condition, which represent different levels of functioning.138

    Our sample is unique in that it consists of college students with ADHD who are relatively high139

    functioning and successful academically, but who continue to show impairment (DuPaul et al., 2009). A140

    better understanding of mechanisms underlying their difficulties may inform interventions designed to141

    increase academic achievement and educational attainment in this population.142

    To determine whether participants with ADHD are impaired during WM encoding, we measured143

    P1 and P3 amplitudes, which allowed us to ascertain at which stage (early vs. later visual processing)144

  • 8/13/2019 Clinical Neurophysiology Volume Issue 2013 [Doi 10.1016_j.clinph.2013.12.094] Kim, Soyeon; Liu, Zhongxu; Glizer

    8/33

    WM encoding in ADHD

    7

    any impairment occurred. Furthermore, to test whether any impairment might vary according to the145

    level of WM demand, we utilized two load conditions (low, high) in the delayed match-to-sample WM146

    task. We predicted that individuals with ADHD would show impaired WM performance and reduced P3147

    amplitude and that these group differences would be most pronounced under the high-load WM148

    condition.149

    150

    METHOD151

    1) Participants:152A total of 32 unmedicated individuals with ADHD (47% male; aged 19-35) and 25 control153

    participants (44% male; aged 19-32) participated in the study. Participants with ADHD were recruited154

    from University Student Disability Services in a major urban area, via email lists and flyers. Inclusion155

    criteria were; 1) current enrolment in a post-secondary program, 2) a previous diagnosis of ADHD, 3)156

    registration with respective university or college Student Disability Services, which requires157

    documented evidence of a previously confirmed diagnosis of ADHD (typically, but not invariably in158

    elementary school), and 4) aged 19-35. Exclusion criteria were; 1) uncorrected sensory impairment, 2)159

    major neurological dysfunction and psychosis, and 3) current use of sedating or mood altering160

    medication including stimulant medication for ADHD. Amongst the clinical sample, 4 reported161

    comorbid learning disability.Participants in the comparison group were recruited through campus162

    advertisements: they were required to have no history or current presentation of mental health disorders.163

    All participants completed the Adult ADHD Self-Report Scale (ASRS v1.1), the Symptom164

    Assessment-45 (SA-45; Maruish, 1999) and the Cognitive Failures Questionnaire (CFQ; Broadbent,165

    Cooper, FitzGerald & Parkes, 1982; Wallace et al., 2002) to quantify current symptoms of ADHD,166

    additional psychopathology, and associated levels of cognitive impairment, respectively. The ASRS is a167

    reliable and valid scale for evaluating current symptoms of ADHD in adults (Adler et al., 2006). The168

    ASRS v1.1 consists of eighteen questions based on the criteria used for diagnosing ADHD in the DSM-169

  • 8/13/2019 Clinical Neurophysiology Volume Issue 2013 [Doi 10.1016_j.clinph.2013.12.094] Kim, Soyeon; Liu, Zhongxu; Glizer

    9/33

    WM encoding in ADHD

    8

    IV-TR. The SA45 is a brief assessment of psychiatric symptom.It is based on the well-validated170

    longer version (SCL90R) to create a brief, yet thorough, measure of psychiatric symptoms.The CFQ171

    measures self-reported failures in perception, memory, and motor function in everyday life. This 25-172

    item questionnaire uses a 5-point Likert scale and has a good external validity (Broadbent, Cooper,173

    FitzGerald & Parkes, 1982; Wallace et al., 2002). Questions require participants to rank how often these174

    failures in cognition occur. Both ASRS and CFQ instruments yield a total score whereby a higher score175

    represents more ADHD symptoms and more cognitive failures, respectively (Table 1).176

    The study was approved by the Research Ethics Boards of the participating universities. All177

    participants provided informed written consent before starting the study.178

    179

    >180

    181

    2) Tasks182Neuropsychological tasks measuring WM performance183

    Three neuropsychological tasks were used to measure WM performance. The Digit Span subtest from184

    the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-Fourth Edition (WAIS-IV) was used to assess verbal working185

    memory ability (Wechsler, 2008). The Spatial Span (SSP) and Spatial Working Memory (SWM) tasks186

    from the Cambridge Neuropsychological Testing Automated Battery (CANTAB) were used to assess187

    visual spatial WM. The SSP required participants to remember the spatial sequence of squares flashed188

    briefly one at a time on the screen. The scores represent the highest level at which the participant189

    reproduces at least one correct sequence. The SWM task requires participants to find a blue token in a190

    series of displayed boxes and use these to fill up an empty column, taking care not to return to boxes191

    where a blue token had been previously found. The standardized z-score for total number of errors for192

    the SWM was used.193

    194

  • 8/13/2019 Clinical Neurophysiology Volume Issue 2013 [Doi 10.1016_j.clinph.2013.12.094] Kim, Soyeon; Liu, Zhongxu; Glizer

    10/33

    WM encoding in ADHD

    9

    The ERP delayed match-to-sample task195

    The delayed match-to-sample task was adapted from Haenschel et al (2007). Thirty-six different196

    abstract figures were used as stimuli. The task consisted of 2 WM load conditions (144 trials total): a197

    high load condition in which a sequence of 3 stimuli was presented (72 trials) and a low load condition198

    with a sequence of 2 stimuli (72 trials). The low load (2-stimulus) condition is shown in Figure 1). Our199

    task differed from that of Haenschel et al (2007) in that we made the task self-paced to better optimize200

    participants readiness on computer-paced tasks which are known to be difficult in the ADHD201

    population (e.g., interference effects of set shifting between trials)and thereby obtain a purer measure202

    of working memory. Furthermore, we were also hoping to reduce the loss of trials due to artifacts.203

    Participants initiated each trial by pressing the space bar on a keyboard, which served to optimize their204

    readiness for the start of each trial. Trials were presented in blocks of 12 trials each of either a low or205

    high load. The blocks of high (3 Stimuli) and low (2 Stimuli) loads were randomly distributed. Before206

    each block, participants were shown which condition to expect (e.g., low or high load) and after each207

    block, feedback on accuracy was provided (as the percentage of trials that were correct). After the space208

    bar was pressed, a clear time of 600 ms was introduced before the fixation stimulus was presented for209

    400 ms. Each target stimulus appeared in the center of the screen for 600 ms (visual angle, 1.34) with an210

    inter-stimulus interval of 400 ms. Two seconds after the last target stimulus appeared (delay;211

    maintenance), a probe stimulus was shown for two seconds (target; retrieval: see figure 1). Participants212

    were asked to indicate whether the target stimulus was one of the previously presented stimuli by213

    pressing the keys labelled as Same or Different on the keypad using their dominant hand. Response214

    accuracy was emphasized, but to maintain a reasonable pace of processing, participants were required to215

    respond within a 2 second time frame, after which the trial would be terminated and scored as incorrect.216

    However, response time was recorded. E-prime 1.2 software (Psychology Software Tools, Inc.) was217

    used to control stimulus presentation and timing as well as to record the accuracy and latency of218

    responses.219

  • 8/13/2019 Clinical Neurophysiology Volume Issue 2013 [Doi 10.1016_j.clinph.2013.12.094] Kim, Soyeon; Liu, Zhongxu; Glizer

    11/33

    WM encoding in ADHD

    10

    Trial time varied between 4-6 seconds for low load and 5-7 seconds for the high load. Since220

    between-trial inter-stimulus intervals would vary because of the self-pacing (participant pressed space bar221

    to start the next trial), total time for task completion was calculated and compared between groups. The222

    analysis indicated that there were no significant differences between the two groups in terms of total time223

    to complete the task (see table 2).224

    225

    >226

    227

    3) Neural data acquisition, processing:228ERP data229

    The EEG was recorded with a 128-channel Geodesic Hydrocel Sensor Net at a 500 Hz sampling rate,230

    using Netstation stand-alone software (Electrical Geodesics Inc, Eugene, Oregon). Netstation was used231

    to filter (FIR, 0.5-30 Hz, 60 Hz notch) and segment the data (400 ms before stimulus onset and 2000232

    ms for low load, or 3000 ms for high load, after stimulus onset).233

    Segments containing artifacts were removed using automatic algorithms to detect eye blinks and234

    eye movements, as well as large drifts or spikes in the data. Eye blinks were detected when the vertical235

    eye channels exceeded a threshold of 150 v (max-min) within a 160 ms (moving) time window236

    within each trial after running a 20 ms moving-average smoothing algorithm across the entire trial237

    period. Eye movements were detected when horizontal eye channels exceeded a threshold of 100 v238

    (max-min) over a 200 ms time window. Channels were automatically marked bad when they exceeded239

    a transition threshold of 200 v over the entire segment (max-min). Segments containing more than240

    20% bad channels were automatically removed. In addition, all epochs were visually inspected by241

    trained research assistants blind to the hypotheses. Bad channels were replaced by values interpolated242

    from neighbouring channel data using spherical splines. Results were verified manually by a research243

    assistant, who was unaware of the study objectives or hypotheses. Because we used a relatively low244

  • 8/13/2019 Clinical Neurophysiology Volume Issue 2013 [Doi 10.1016_j.clinph.2013.12.094] Kim, Soyeon; Liu, Zhongxu; Glizer

    12/33

    WM encoding in ADHD

    11

    high-pass filter, a linear detrending algorithm was applied to individual epochs to eliminate any left-245

    over linear drift if it existed.246

    The data were then averaged for each subject and stimulus, average referenced, and coded247

    within the segment. The P1 components were scored as the maximum positivity between 80-160 ms248

    after stimulus-onset for each stimulus (e.g., 3 stimuli for high load) for electrode sites 75 (O z), 70 (O1)249

    and 83 (O2). Similarly, P3 components were scored as the mean positivity between 250-500 ms after250

    stimulus onset for electrode sites 65, 66, 84, and 90 (See Supplementary Figure S1 for electrode map)251

    compared to the baseline. The mean activity was chosen to represent P3 because this is a more252

    elongated component with peaks (often several) that are less clearly defined compared to perceptual253

    components (see also, Luck, 2005; Woltering et al., 2011). Electrodes were selected based on the254

    literature (e.g., see Polich, 2007, for rationale) as well as the maximal positivities in the grand average255

    waveforms (relative to the 200 ms baseline). Latency ranges were determined based on inspection of256

    the individual ERPs as well as Haenschels et al., 2007 study. We acknowledge that, considering the257

    paucity of ERP studies using delayed-match-to-sample tasks, our component labeling may vary from258

    convention used in other tasks. Before exportation, data were baseline corrected for 200 ms prior to259

    stimulus onset, separately, for each individual stimulus. Only correct trials were used in the analysis.260

    Averages across electrode sites were used for the P1 and P3 analysis.261

    262

    4) Data analysis:263

    Independent one-way ANOVAs were used to test for group differences in performance on the264

    neuropsychological tasks. Separate repeated measures ANOVAs were conducted to test for group265

    differences in accuracy and reaction time on the delayed match-to-sample task, with Load as the266

    repeated measure (High load vs. Low) and Group as a between participant factor (ADHD vs. Control).267

    By contrast, the incomplete factorial design of the Delayed-Match-To-Sample task (2 stimuli in Low268

    Load; 3 stimuli in High Load) precluded a Group (2) x Load (2) x (Stimulus; 2 or 3) repeated measures269

  • 8/13/2019 Clinical Neurophysiology Volume Issue 2013 [Doi 10.1016_j.clinph.2013.12.094] Kim, Soyeon; Liu, Zhongxu; Glizer

    13/33

    WM encoding in ADHD

    12

    ANOVA to investigate effects on the P1 and P3 ERP components. Thus we used a multi-step approach270

    to disentangle the effects of Load, Stimulus, and Group on our ERP components (P1, P3). In Step-1, to271

    investigate the effects of Group and WM Load on the P3 and P1, we collapsed across Stimulus, and ran272

    a Group by Load Repeated Measures ANOVA. For Step-2 we ran a Group by Load Repeated Measures273

    ANOVA for the first and last Stimuli separately. The last stimuli of each load would represent the274

    situation in which a participant would hold the maximum amount of information in mind for each275

    condition when encoding new information. This analysis involved a comparison of the second (last)276

    stimulus of the low load with the third (last) stimulus of the high load: thesecondstimulus of the high277

    load was not compared to either the first or the second stimulus of the low load as it was not278

    conceptually equivalent to those stimuli. In Step-3, we ran separate Group by Stimulus repeated279

    measures ANOVAs for each load. For the high load, contrasts were run to test for differences between280

    the three stimuli. This step allowed us to investigate sequence effects of stimulus presentation, which281

    may provide insights in the effects of neural resources being allocated during encoding with increasing282

    amount of stimuli to be held in mind.283

    Two participants, whose ERP data yielded less than 10 trials, were excluded from this analysis.284

    Data points (behavioural and ERP) with SD's >3 were regarded as outliers and adjusted using the285

    winsorizing technique (Tabachnick and Fidell, 2001). Three data points in ADHD group and 2 data286

    points in control group were winsorized. No significant differences were found in trial-counts [Low287

    load:F(1, 49) = .258,p=.553, High load:F(1, 49) = .006,p=.938] between the groups.288

    Partial eta-squared values (2) were computed to ascertain effect size (ES). According to Vacha-289

    Haase and Thompson (2004),ESbased on2= .01 corresponds to a small effect, 2= .10 corresponds to a290

    medium effect, and 2= .25 represents a large effect.291

    292

    293

    RESULTS294

  • 8/13/2019 Clinical Neurophysiology Volume Issue 2013 [Doi 10.1016_j.clinph.2013.12.094] Kim, Soyeon; Liu, Zhongxu; Glizer

    14/33

    WM encoding in ADHD

    13

    1. Behavioral Data295

    Analysis of demographic and clinical variables confirmed that the ADHD and control groups296

    were comparable in terms of age[F(53) = 1.358,p=.25] and sex [ (1, 21) = .039,p= 1.00]. As297

    expected, ADHD participants reported significantly more ADHD symptoms [F (54) =1,p< .001] and298

    cognitive failures in everyday life than controls [F(42) = 59.91,p< .001]. The ADHD group also299

    reported more obsessive-compulsive symptoms [F(43) = 25.22,p< .001] on the SA-45, compared to the300

    Control group. Means and standard deviations are presented in Table 1.301

    Analysis of neuropsychological tests of WM performance revealed, as expected, that the ADHD302

    group obtained significantly lower scores on the CANTAB-SWM [F(1, 44) = 4.54,p= .039], compared303

    to the control group. ADHD group also showed poorer scores in WAIS Digit Span, albeit a non-304

    significant difference [F(1, 48) = 3.29,p = .076]. Groups did not differ on CANTAB-SSP. The mean305

    scores for the ADHD group on these two tests of WM fell in the low average range: their mean SWM z-306

    score was -.10, which corresponds to a scaled score of just below 7 (16thpercentile); and their mean307

    Digit Span scaled score was 8.31 (25thpercentile). By contrast, scores for the comparison group fell308

    well within the average range: mean Digit Span scaled score of 9.95 corresponds to the 50 thpercentile309

    and the SWM z-score was .51, which corresponds to a scaled score of 10 or the 50thpercentile.310

    As predicted, analysis of the Delayed-Match-To-Sample Task, revealed a main effect of Load311

    on performance accuracy [F(1, 53) = 49.88,p= .000, ES= .49]. Specifically, all participants were less312

    accurate during the high WM load compared to the low WM load condition, indicating the success of313

    the experimental manipulation. The groups did not differ significantly in terms of either their accuracy314

    or response times (to the target stimulus). There was no significant Group-by-Load interaction for315

    either response accuracy or response times. Moreover, there were no group differences in terms of intra-316

    individual variability of response times and neither the main effect of Load nor the Group-by-Load317

    interaction was significant. Means and standard deviations of the neuropsychological tasks and the318

    delayed match to sample task measures are shown in Table 2.319

  • 8/13/2019 Clinical Neurophysiology Volume Issue 2013 [Doi 10.1016_j.clinph.2013.12.094] Kim, Soyeon; Liu, Zhongxu; Glizer

    15/33

    WM encoding in ADHD

    14

    320

    >321

    322

    2) ERP data323

    The waveforms for each stimulus are presented in Figure 2, in Panel A for the low load and Panel B324

    for the high load. Table 3 shows the means and standard deviations for the ERP components for each325

    load and for each group collapsed across stimulus. The waveforms and (difference) topoplots for the P3,326

    collapsed across stimulus, are presented in Figure 3, Panel A for the low load and Panel B for the high327

    load. As can be seen in the topoplots, the difference in P3 amplitude between the ADHD and control328

    group was observed primarily in the parietal/occipital regions (see Supplementary Figure S2 for a t-plot).329

    330

    P1 Analyses331

    For P1 amplitude, Step-1 analysis, which examined the effects of Group and Load collapsed332

    across stimuli, showed a significant main effect of Load [F (1, 53) = 8.26,p= .006,ES= .14] with333

    higher amplitude for high load. There was no main effect of Group or Group-by-Load interaction. Step-334

    2 analyzed Group and Load effects separately for the first and last stimuli. For the first stimulus there335

    was a significant Group-by-Load interaction [F (1, 53 = 6.27,p= .015,ES= .106] in the absence of336

    main effects for either Group or Load. Post-hoc tests revealed that participants with ADHD had337

    increased P1 amplitudes for the High compared to the Low load, whereas the comparison group did not338

    show this effect (p< .05). For the last stimulus, a main effect of Load was present [F (1, 52) = 5.76,p339

    = .02,ES= .10] but neither the main effect of Group nor the Group-by-Load interaction was significant.340

    Step-3 analysis of P1 during the low load condition revealed a main effect of Stimulus, [F (1, 53) = 9.31,341

    p= .004,ES= .15] with lower P1 amplitudes for the first stimulus compared to the last. No Group, or342

    Group-by-Stimulus interaction effect was found. The high load also revealed a main effect of Stimulus,343

    [F (1, 51) = 13.61,p= .000,ES= .35], with lower P1 amplitudes for the first Stimulus compared to the344

  • 8/13/2019 Clinical Neurophysiology Volume Issue 2013 [Doi 10.1016_j.clinph.2013.12.094] Kim, Soyeon; Liu, Zhongxu; Glizer

    16/33

    WM encoding in ADHD

    15

    second and third. No Group or Group-by-Stimulus interaction was found. Last, the P1 did not correlate345

    with any clinical questionnaire or behavioral measures.346

    For P1 latency, Step-1 analysis revealed no main effect of Load, Group, or Load-by-Group347

    interaction for either the high or low load conditions. On the Step-2 analysis, no main effects of Group,348

    Load, or a Stimulus-by-Group interaction were found, nor were there any significant effects for the first349

    stimulus. On the Step-3 analysis, neither the main effects of Stimulus or Group, nor the Stimulus-by-350

    Group interaction was significant.351

    352

    P3 Analyses353

    For P3 amplitude, our Step-1 analysis showed a significant main effect of Group, [F (1, 53) =354

    6.30,p= .015,ES= .11], with the ADHD group showing smaller P3 amplitudes than the control group.355

    There was also a main effect of Load, [F (1, 53) = 4.04,p= .049,ES= .07], with larger P3 amplitudes356

    for the high load confirming that our P3 index was sensitive to effects of load. No Group-by-Load357

    interaction was found.358

    The Step-2 analysis tested the Group by Load effects separately for the first and last Stimuli.359

    There was a main effect of Group for the first, [F (1, 53) = 5.31,p= .025,ES= .09], and last [F(1, 52)360

    = 5.76,p= .020,ES= .10], stimuli suggesting the group difference was present across stimuli. There361

    was a main effect of Load for the first [F (1, 53) = 6.55,p= .013,ES= .11] but not for the last stimulus,362

    suggesting our general load effect was driven mostly by the first stimulus. Also, no significant Group-363

    by-Load interaction was found for both first and last stimuli.364

    Our Step-3 analysis, testing for sequence effects of stimuli, revealed a main effect of Stimulus,365

    [F(1, 53) = 11.76,p= .001,ES= .18], and a main effect of Group, [F (1, 53) = 4.89,p= .031,ES= .08],366

    during the low load. No Group-by-Stimulus interaction effect was found. The high load also revealed a367

    main effect of Stimulus, [F (1, 53) = 3.17,p= .05,ES= .11], and a main effect of Group, [F (1, 52) =368

    7.63,p= .008,ES= .13], with no Group-by-Stimulus interaction. In general, for both WM loads,369

  • 8/13/2019 Clinical Neurophysiology Volume Issue 2013 [Doi 10.1016_j.clinph.2013.12.094] Kim, Soyeon; Liu, Zhongxu; Glizer

    17/33

    WM encoding in ADHD

    16

    responses to the first stimulus showed lower amplitudes than to the other stimuli (ps < .05). For the370

    high load, pairwise comparisons showed that the second and third stimulus did not differ. The ADHD371

    group showed lower P3 amplitudes for all three stimuli compared to the control group.372

    For the P3 latency, the Step-1 latency analysis indicated no main effect of Group, a marginally373

    significant effect for Load, [F (1, 53) = 3.23,p= .078,ES= .058], with shorter latencies for the high374

    load, and no Load-by-Group interaction. Step-2 analysis revealed a main effect of Load for the first [F375

    (1, 53) = 8.16,p= .006,ES= .133], but not for the last stimulus. No main effect of Group or Group-by-376

    Load interaction was found for either the first or last stimulus. The Step-3 analysis showed a main effect377

    of Stimulus for the low load condition [F (1, 53) = 5.47,p= .023,ES= .093]. Post-hoc tests revealed378

    that the ADHD group showed increased P3 amplitude for the second stimulus compared to the first,379

    whereas the comparison group did not show this difference (p= .03). No main effect of Group or380

    Group-by-Stimulus interaction was found for both first and last stimuli.381

    382

    >383

    384

    >385

    386

    >387

    388

    DISCUSSION389

    The present study is the first to investigate neural changes in P3 amplitudes and latencies during390

    the encoding phase of WM in ADHD, using a delayed match-to-sample task. The main finding was a391

    reduced P3 amplitude for the ADHD group, regardless of WM load or stimulus sequence (i.e., first or392

    last), compared to the control group. Behavioral results showed that all participants were less accurate393

  • 8/13/2019 Clinical Neurophysiology Volume Issue 2013 [Doi 10.1016_j.clinph.2013.12.094] Kim, Soyeon; Liu, Zhongxu; Glizer

    18/33

    WM encoding in ADHD

    17

    during the high WM load condition compared to the low load condition, which attests to the success of394

    our experimental manipulation of WM load.395

    Changes in P3 amplitudes during WM tasks have been associated with differences in the396

    allocation of attention necessary for WM functioning (Donchin and Coles, 1988; Polich, 2007; Kok,397

    2001). Our findings of lower P3 amplitude in the ADHD group compared to the control group,398

    independent of load or stimulus order, suggests a more general problem of attentional allocation in the399

    encoding phase of WM rather than a more specific problem associated with an increasing demand on400

    WM. The reduced P3 amplitude in college students with ADHD found in this study using a WM task is401

    consistent with and extend findings from a recent meta-analysis on P3 in adults with ADHD (to WM402

    encoding during a WM task), which revealed a decreased P3 amplitude recorded during Go-Nogo tasks403

    in ADHD (Szuromi et al., 2011; and also see, Woltering et al., 2013, using a similar sample to that in404

    the present study). Go-Nogo tasks, which traditionally are used to index response inhibition, do involve405

    WM, albeit low-level demands, and some require constant updating of working memory.406

    We believe that the decreased P3 amplitude observed in individuals with ADHD can be407

    interpreted as deficient attentional allocation, which leads to inefficient encoding of information in408

    memory. Theoretically, the notion of P3 reflecting attentional allocation is not that different to Koks409

    notion of capacity since recent insights in the field of working memory have linked attentional processes410

    and working memory capacity closely together (Awh and Vogel, 2008). A functional relationship411

    between P3 amplitude and attentional resource allocation has also been reported in the literature (Ford,412

    White, Lim, and Pfefferbaum, 1994; Kok, 2001).Also, many studies found the P3 to be related with413

    brain regions thought to be involved in attention, such as the parietal lobe, the temporoparietal junction,414

    lateral prefrontal areas and the cingulate gyrus (Linden, 2005; Verleger et al., 1994). Similarly, a study415

    that explored the cortical basis of ERP components revealed an impaired ventral attentional pathway in416

    ADHD participants during the time of the P3 (Helenius et al., 2011). Furthermore, in their meta-analysis,417

    Szuromi et al (2011) interpreted the overall reduction of P3 amplitude in adults with ADHD as a418

  • 8/13/2019 Clinical Neurophysiology Volume Issue 2013 [Doi 10.1016_j.clinph.2013.12.094] Kim, Soyeon; Liu, Zhongxu; Glizer

    19/33

    WM encoding in ADHD

    18

    dysfunction of the ventral attention network since the P3 related to target detection is thought to be419

    generated from the temporoparietal junction. In sum, previous research suggests a possible link between420

    P3 amplitude and attentional resource allocation. Hence, based on previous findings, we speculate that421

    weak activation in attention pathways might play a role in the defective allocation of attentional422

    resources during activities involving WM, accounting for the observed decreased P3 amplitudes in423

    college students with ADHD.424

    Our analyses of P1 amplitude revealed no significant group effects except for one: namely that425

    the ADHD group seems to manifest an increase in P1 amplitude from the low to the high WM load426

    condition in response to the first stimulus only. Compared to Haenschel et al. (2007), who did find427

    robust P1 group differences in Schizophrenia patients, our findings suggest that differences in WM428

    encoding between the ADHD and non-ADHD peer group were due primarily to differences in429

    attentional allocation during the WM encoding phase as opposed to early visual processing.430

    Only one other study to date has investigated neural differences in WM between ADHD adults431

    and their peers using EEG (Missonnier et al., 2013). Despite the use of different methodologies (n-back432

    versus delayed-match-to-sample task, respectively; smaller sample size, and the focus on different433

    neurophysiological measures, such as time-frequency domain), conclusions are similar. That is, both434

    conclude that individuals with ADHD manifest differences in neural correlates linked to the attentional435

    network involved in the allocation of attention subserving WM.436

    Behavioral differences between groups were found during the WM task: namely the ADHD437

    group had slower response times to the retrieval cue, albeit marginally significant, but did not differ in438

    response accuracy. It is possible that the slower response times in the ADHD group indicate their need439

    for more time to make accurate decisions compared to their peers. In this sense, the longer response440

    times may be another manifestation of slow processing speed or a compensatory mechanism. Although441

    accuracy in our ADHD group was comparable to that of their typically developing peers, cognitive442

    mechanisms of processing, such as the allocation of attention, could still function less optimally. It is443

  • 8/13/2019 Clinical Neurophysiology Volume Issue 2013 [Doi 10.1016_j.clinph.2013.12.094] Kim, Soyeon; Liu, Zhongxu; Glizer

    20/33

    WM encoding in ADHD

    19

    possible that this deficit may show in contexts other than within the experimental confines of the current444

    task. This suggestion is strengthened by other findings from this study: namely that for the ADHD445

    group, their mean scores for WAIS Digit Span and CANTAB SWM were in low average range, whereas446

    those of the control group were in the average range; the T-scores for the ADHD group (mean = 41.5)447

    were in the mildly impaired range whereas those for the control group (mean = 50) were within the448

    well-functioning range. Note that the two neuropsychological tasks not only require the maintenance but449

    also the updating of the spatial representations, which renders their WM demands similar to those of the450

    delayed-match-to-sample task. Furthermore, these behavioral and neuropsychological differences were451

    found despite the fact that the participants with ADHD in this study are high functioning (mostly college452

    students) and the task was self-paced to exclude potential effects of poor sustained attention453

    confounding the WM result.454

    In light of our neural results, the lack of differences in behavioral accuracy in the delayed-455

    match-to-sample task could suggest that our group differences found in neural processing indicate456

    inefficient means of attentional allocation; a detriment that may not always be observed in behavioural457

    accuracy. The P3 could be a neural indicator of this inefficient mechanism.458

    From a clinical perspective, the WM impairment (albeit modest) in these college students with459

    ADHD compared to their college-peers may well help explain why this relatively successful subgroup460

    continues to manifest functional impairments in academic, social functioning during college years.461

    Furthermore, these results support findings from a recent meta-analysis suggesting long-term memory462

    difficulties in adult ADHD may be attributable to difficulties in the encoding phase (Skodzik et al.,463

    2013). Our findings highlight the importance of studying WM encoding and in this newly emergent464

    subgroup of young adults with ADHD.465

    This study has several limitations, which should be taken into consideration when interpreting466

    the findings. First of all, we did not have a direct measure of IQ, or scholastic ability, which limits the467

    characterization of our sample. However, these college students must be of at least average intelligence468

  • 8/13/2019 Clinical Neurophysiology Volume Issue 2013 [Doi 10.1016_j.clinph.2013.12.094] Kim, Soyeon; Liu, Zhongxu; Glizer

    21/33

    WM encoding in ADHD

    20

    (as suggested by the WAIS Digit Span) to be able to successfully complete high school and gain college469

    entrance. Furthermore, we did not measure alcohol and recreational drug use in this sample of college470

    students, which could influence allocation of attention or WM or both. Also, the current analyses were471

    restricted to the encoding stage and further research is warranted to characterize the maintenance and472

    retrieval stages of WM.473

    In conclusion, keeping in mind the aforementioned limitations, our results suggest differences in474

    neural processing in WM with adult ADHD. Our results may suggest, based on what is known about475

    changes in P3 amplitudes in the ERP literature, that weak activation in attentional pathways might play476

    a role in defective attention resource allocation to updating representations in working memory.477

    Moreover, our results concerning the P1 versus P3 suggest a temporal as well as syndrome specific478

    neural index of adult ADHD. These findings could have important implications for understanding the479

    cognitive difficulties ADHD adults face, as well as intervention.480

    481

    482

    483

  • 8/13/2019 Clinical Neurophysiology Volume Issue 2013 [Doi 10.1016_j.clinph.2013.12.094] Kim, Soyeon; Liu, Zhongxu; Glizer

    22/33

    WM encoding in ADHD

    21

    Acknowledgements484

    We thank Dr. Corinna Haenschel and Niko Kriegeskorte for letting us use the task stimuli. Also, we485

    appreciate Alex Lamey for his programming assistance with the E-prime task and Peter Fettes for486

    helping with parts of the data analysis. We would like to thank Dr. Marc Lewis for the use of his487Canadian Foundation for Innovation (CFI #482246) funded EEG lab. This research was supported488

    financially in part by a CIHR Operating grant (# 245899, Tannock & Lewis) and by the Canada489

    Research Chair program (RT).490

    491

    492

  • 8/13/2019 Clinical Neurophysiology Volume Issue 2013 [Doi 10.1016_j.clinph.2013.12.094] Kim, Soyeon; Liu, Zhongxu; Glizer

    23/33

    WM encoding in ADHD

    22

    References493

    Adler LA, Spencer T, Faraone SV, Kessler RC, Howes MJ, Biederman J, et al. Validity of pilot Adult494

    ADHD Self- Report Scale (ASRS) to Rate Adult ADHD symptoms. Ann Clin Psychiatry495

    2006;18:145-148.496

    Awh E, Vogel EK. The bouncer in the brain. Nat Neurosci 2008; 11:56.497

    Baddeley A. Working memory: Looking back and looking forward. Nat Rev Neurosci 2003; 4:829-839.498

    Baddeley AD, Hitch GJ. Developments in the concept of working memory. Neuropsychology499

    1994; 8:485.500

    Barkley RA, Cook E, Diamond A, Zametkin A, Thapar A, Teeter A. International consensus statement501

    on ADHD. Clin Child Fam Psych 2002; 5:89-111.502

    Carrasco M. Visual attention. Vision Res 2011; 51:1484-1525.503

    Cowan N. The magical number 4 in short-term memory: A reconsideration of mental storage capacity.504

    Behav Brain Sci 2001; 24:87-114.505

    Donchin E, Coles MG. Is the P300 component a manifestation of context updating? Behav Brain506

    Sci1988:11:357-374.507

    DuPaul G, Weyandt L, ODell S, Varejao M. College students with ADHD: Current Status and future508

    directions. J Atten Disord 2009; 13:234.509

    Fabiani M, Karis D, Donchin E. P300 and recall in an incidental memory paradigm.510

    Psychophysiology 1986; 23:298-308.511

    Faraone SV, Biederman J, Mick E. The age-dependent decline of attention deficit hyperactivity512

    disorder: a meta-analysis of follow-up studies. Psychol Med 2006; 36:159-166.513

    Ford JM, White P, Lim KO, Pfefferbaum A. Schizophrenics have fewer and smaller P300s: A single-514

    trial analysis. Biol Psychiat 1994; 35:96-103.515

    Frazier TW, Youngstrom EA, Glutting JJ, Watkins MW. ADHD and achievement meta-analysis of the516

    child, adolescent and adult literatures and a concomitant study with college students. J Learn517

  • 8/13/2019 Clinical Neurophysiology Volume Issue 2013 [Doi 10.1016_j.clinph.2013.12.094] Kim, Soyeon; Liu, Zhongxu; Glizer

    24/33

    WM encoding in ADHD

    23

    Disabil 2007; 40:49-65.518

    Haenschel C, Bittner RA, Haertling F, Rotarska-Jagiela A, Maurer K, Singer W, et al. Contribution of519

    impaired early-stage visual processing to working memory dysfunction in adolescents with520

    schizophrenia: a study with event-related potentials and functional magnetic resonance imaging.521

    Arch Gen Psychiat 2007; 64:1229.522

    Heiligenstein E, Guenther G, Levy A, Sevino F, Fulwiler J. Psychological and academic523

    functioning in college students with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. J Am Coll Health524

    1999; 47:181-185.525

    Helenius P, Laasonen M, Hokkanen L, Paetau R, Niemivirta M. Impaired engagement of the ventral526

    attentional pathway in ADHD. Neuropsychologia 2011; 49:1889-1896.527

    Hervey AS, Epstein JN, Curry JF. Neuropsychology of adults with attention-Deficit/hyperactivity528

    disorder: a meta-analytic review. Neuropsychology 2004; 18:485.529

    Karis D, Fabiani M, Donchin E. P300 and memory: Individual differences in the von Restorff effect.530

    Cognitive Psychol 1984; 16:177-216.531

    Kane MJ, Conway AR, Hambrick DZ, Engle RW. Variation in working memory capacity as variation in532

    executive attention and control. Oxford: Oxford University Press, 2007.533

    Kok A. On the utility of P3 amplitude as a measure of processing capacity. Psychophysiology534

    2001; 38:557-577.535

    Kok A, Ramautar JR, De Ruiter MB, Band GP, Ridderinkhof KR. ERP components associated with536

    successful and unsuccessful stopping in a stopsignal task. Psychophysiology 2004; 41:9-20.537

    Linden DEJ. The P300: where in the brain is it produced and what does it tell us? Neuroscientist538

    2005; 11:56376.539

    Luck SJ. An introduction to the event-related potential technique (cognitive neuroscience).540

    Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, 2005.541

    Martinussen R, Hayden J, Hogg-Johnson S, Tannock R. A meta-analysis of542

  • 8/13/2019 Clinical Neurophysiology Volume Issue 2013 [Doi 10.1016_j.clinph.2013.12.094] Kim, Soyeon; Liu, Zhongxu; Glizer

    25/33

    WM encoding in ADHD

    24

    working memory impairments in children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. J543

    Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 2005;44:377-384.544

    Missonnier P, Hasler R, Perroud N, Herrmann FR, Millet P, Richiardi J, et al. EEG anomalies in adult545

    ADHD participants performing a working memory task. Neuroscience 2013; 241:135-146.546

    Polanczyk G, Jensen P. Epidemiologic considerations in attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: a547

    review and update. Child Adol Psych Cl 2008; 17:245-260.548

    Polanczyk G, Rohde LA. Epidemiology of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder across the lifespan.549

    Curr Opin Psychiatry 2007;20:386-389.550

    Polich J. Updating P300: an integrative theory of P3a and P3b. Clin Neurophysiol 2007; 118:2128-2148.551

    Rutman AM, Clapp WC, Chadick JZ, Gazzaley A. Early topdown control of visual processing predicts552

    working memory performance. J Cognitive Neurosci 2010; 22:1224-1234.553

    Shaw-Zirt B, Popali-Lehane L, Chaplin W, Bergman A. Adjustment, social skills, and self-esteem in554

    college students with symptoms of ADHD. J Atten Disord 2005; 8:109-120.555

    Skodzik T, Holling H, Pedersen A. Long-Term Memory Performance in Adult556

    ADHD: A Meta-Analysis. J Atten Disord 2013; doi 10.1177/1087054713510561.557

    Szuromi B, Czobor P, Komlosi S, Bitter I. P300 deficits in adults with attention deficit558

    hyperactivity disorder: a meta-analysis. Psychol Med 2011; 41:1529.559

    Tabachnick BG, Fidell LS. Multivariate analysis of variance and covariance using multivariate statistics.560

    Boston: Allyn and Bacon, 2001.561

    Verleger R, Heide W, Butt C, Kmpf D. Reduction of P3b in patients with temporo-parietal lesions.562

    Brain Res 1994; 2:103-116.563

    Vogel EK, McCollough AW, Machizawa MG. Neural measures reveal individual differences in564

    controlling access to working memory. Nature 2005; 438:500-503.565

    Wallace JC, Kass SJ, Stanny CJ. The Cognitive Failures Questionnaire revisited: Dimensions and566

    correlates. J Gen Psychol 2002; 129:238-256.567

  • 8/13/2019 Clinical Neurophysiology Volume Issue 2013 [Doi 10.1016_j.clinph.2013.12.094] Kim, Soyeon; Liu, Zhongxu; Glizer

    26/33

    WM encoding in ADHD

    25

    Woltering S, Granic I, Lamm C, Lewis MD. Neural changes associated with treatment outcome in568

    children with externalizing problems. Biol Psychiat 2011; 70:873-879.569

    Woltering S, Liu Z, Rokeach A, Tannock R. Neurophysiological differences in inhibitory control between570

    adults with ADHD and their peers. Neuropsychologia 2013; 51: 1888-1895.571

    572

    573

    574

  • 8/13/2019 Clinical Neurophysiology Volume Issue 2013 [Doi 10.1016_j.clinph.2013.12.094] Kim, Soyeon; Liu, Zhongxu; Glizer

    27/33

    WM encoding in ADHD

    26

    Figure Legends575

    576

    Figure 1.Schematic outline of the delayed match-to-sample task for low load condition.577

    Working memory paradigm adopted and modified from Haenschel et al (2007). Two stimuli were578

    presented in low load condition, and 3 stimuli were shown in high load condition for encoding for 600579

    ms each with an inter-stimulus interval of 400 ms. After a 2000 ms delay, a probe stimulus was580

    presented for the participants to judge whether they saw the target among the initial set of the stimuli or581

    not. Participants initiated the next trial by pressing the space bar. 200 ms of clear time was allowed582

    before each trial.583

    584

    Figure 2. ERP waveforms at site P3 (averaged across all P3 electrodes) for (A) Low and (B) High585

    load conditions for each stimulus [(A) left: first stimulus, right: second stimuli (B) first, second586

    and last stimuli].587

    588

    Figure 3. ERP waveforms atsite P3 (averaged across all P3 electrodes)and topoplots (250-500 ms)589

    for (A) Low and (B) high load condition across the stimuli.590

    591

  • 8/13/2019 Clinical Neurophysiology Volume Issue 2013 [Doi 10.1016_j.clinph.2013.12.094] Kim, Soyeon; Liu, Zhongxu; Glizer

    28/33

    Encoding (600ms per stimuli) Maintenance (delay: 2s) Retrieval (target:2s)

    600ms 600ms 2000ms

    ure 1

  • 8/13/2019 Clinical Neurophysiology Volume Issue 2013 [Doi 10.1016_j.clinph.2013.12.094] Kim, Soyeon; Liu, Zhongxu; Glizer

    29/33

    gure 2

  • 8/13/2019 Clinical Neurophysiology Volume Issue 2013 [Doi 10.1016_j.clinph.2013.12.094] Kim, Soyeon; Liu, Zhongxu; Glizer

    30/33

    gure 3

  • 8/13/2019 Clinical Neurophysiology Volume Issue 2013 [Doi 10.1016_j.clinph.2013.12.094] Kim, Soyeon; Liu, Zhongxu; Glizer

    31/33

    Table 1: Mean and Standard deviations of the ASRS (ADHD Self Report Scale), CFQ (Cognitive

    Failures Questionnaire), and SA-45 subscales (Symptom Assessment-45) for the ADHD and the

    Comparison group.

    * p< .05, ** p< .01, *** p< .001. p-values shown reflect one-way ANOVAs.

    ADHD Comparison P-values

    ASRS *** 44.97(9.12) 18.71(7.98) .00

    CFQ *** 53.88(12.74) 27.56(8.09) .00

    SA-45

    Anxiety* 8.41(2.37) 6.75(1.83) .01

    Depression 9.37(3.63) 8.05(3.58) .21

    Obsessive Compulsive *** 14.72(3.77) 9.25(3.45) .00

    Somatization 7.65(2.59) 6.55(1.73) .11

    Phobic Anxiety 5.52(0.80) 5.29(0.72) .30

    Hostility 6.48(1.71) 5.95(1.50) .28

    Interpersonal Sensitivity 8.67(3.16) 7.76(3.85) .38

    Paranoid Ideation 7.93(2.56) 6.62(2.20) .07

    Psychoticism 5.54(1.07) 5.62(1.02) .80

  • 8/13/2019 Clinical Neurophysiology Volume Issue 2013 [Doi 10.1016_j.clinph.2013.12.094] Kim, Soyeon; Liu, Zhongxu; Glizer

    32/33

    Table 2. Mean and Standard deviations for behavioural measures of the delayed match to sample task

    [accuracy, as well as the reaction time (in milliseconds), the intra-individual variability (IIV)], and the

    WM [Digit span, CANTAB: SWM and SSP] neuropsychological tests for the ADHD and Comparison

    group.

    * p< .05,p-values shown reflect one-way ANOVAs, RT (Reaction Time), SWM (Spatial Working Memory), SSP (Spatial

    Span)

    ADHDMean (SD)

    ComparisonMean (SD)

    P-values

    Delayed Match to Sample Task

    Accuracy

    Low load 0.73(0.14) 0.80(0.14) .10

    High load 0.62(0.18) 0.64(0.15) .64

    Reaction Time

    Low load 1260.60(232.85) 1176.17(166.47) .14

    High Load 1309.96(268.49) 1241.70(208.08) .30

    Intra-individual variability (RT)

    Low load 494.32(241.75) 446.06(216.30) .44

    High load 420.31(242.63) 452.05(228.98) .62

    Total duration of the task 23.32(3.26) 21.81(2.57) .07

    Neuropsychological tests

    WAIS Digit Span 8.31(3.31) 9.95(2.94) .08

    CANTAB SWM* -0.10(1.13) 0.51(0.62) .04

    SSP 0.09(0.86) 0.60(1.14) .10

  • 8/13/2019 Clinical Neurophysiology Volume Issue 2013 [Doi 10.1016_j.clinph.2013.12.094] Kim, Soyeon; Liu, Zhongxu; Glizer

    33/33

    Table 3: Means and standard deviations for ERP amplitudes and latency.

    ADHD Control

    Amplitude P1

    Low load 3.27(1.30) 4.02(2.09)

    1ststimulus 2.83(1.51) 3.83(2.35)2ndstimulus 3.70(1.54) 4.21(2.05)

    High load 3.62(1.22) 4.30(2.08)1

    ststimulus 3.20(1.52) 3.58(2.42)

    2ndstimulus 3.87(1.70) 4.33(1.90)

    3rdstimulus 3.83(1.13) 5.05(2.36)

    P3

    Low load 1.96(1.52) 3.02(2.06)1

    ststimulus 1.59(1.70) 2.69(2.61)

    2nd

    stimulus 2.32(1.68) 3.39(1.85)

    High load 2.02(1.45) 3.32(2.07)1ststimulus 1.80(1.42) 3.12(2.20)

    2ndstimulus 2.18(1.60) 3.47(1.99)3rdstimulus 2.04(1.61) 3.29(2.18)

    Latency P1

    Low load 127.27(16.98) 127.92(17.05

    1ststimulus 127.27(16.98) 127.92(17.05)2ndstimulus 124.47(16.19) 124.80(16.30)

    High load 125.58(11.99) 125.65(11.66)1

    ststimulus 124.73(15.21) 127.76(19.41)

    2ndstimulus 124.53(13.47) 123.44(16.16)

    3rdstimulus 126.55(14.19) 125.76(10.30)

    P3

    Low load 360.13(26.42) 357.40(22.59)1

    ststimulus 350.27(42.41) 350.24(37.86)

    2ndstimulus 370.00(38.08) 364.56(23.31)

    High load 468.04(29.31) 362.21(18.94)1ststimulus 372.60(48.80) 363.52(31.73)

    2ndstimulus 366.27(35.64) 360.72(24.66)3

    rdstimulus 365.27(42.75) 362.40(25.23)