Children's Views on Science

Embed Size (px)

Citation preview

  • 7/29/2019 Children's Views on Science

    1/23

    Childrens Views of Science

    Assignment One

    Tarryn Fisher

    21158408.

  • 7/29/2019 Children's Views on Science

    2/23

    Section One

    Determining Prior Knowledge: In teaching any subject, determining the students prior

    knowledge before teaching is very important to effectively plan any and all units of teaching

    and to ensure the content be taught to tailor the students learning needs (Kavanagh &

    Sneider., 2007). When obtaining prior knowledge it is not only important to ask children

    what their ideas are, but to ask them to provide you with evidence of how they know this to

    be true, even if the idea they present is not a scientific concept (Skamp., 2007). This gives

    you an indication of how they are thinking and what scientific and alternate concepts they

    hold in their ideas. It is important to determine this prior knowledge through allowing the

    children to express their ideas in hands-on activities, particularly for younger children as it

    creates engagement and allows them to apply their knowledge in different situations

    (Darling., 2012). Questioning the children and asking for evidence of why they believe

    certain ideas can place them in a position where they need to examine their beliefs and reflect

    upon their understanding of the concept, which may enable them to formulate their ideas and

    possibly see situations where their ideas do not apply (Skamp., 2007).

    Determining childrens prior knowledge is also advantageous as it allows identification of

    areas where they may hold alternate, rather then scientific conceptions on a particular subject.

    The use of hands-on activities is not only engaging for children but advantageous for

    teachers, as many find it hard to predict what misconceptions their students may have

    (Kavanagh & Sneider., 2007). It is possible that alternate conceptions, particularly physics

    related ones, are intuitively formed and therefore research into childrens priorknowledge of

    scientific topics is vital to ensure that the childs educational instruction is related to their

    knowledge and prior conceptions (Wilkening & Huber., 2002).

    The science topic selected for this assignment was the topic of gravity. Two year 5 students,

    Luke and Matthew (pseudonyms), both aged nine, were interviewed on this topic.

  • 7/29/2019 Children's Views on Science

    3/23

    Section Two

    Information on the Topic of Gravity: The topic of Gravity is one that is important to

    children in their everyday life; they can identify it as the force which brings them back to

    Earth when they jump with a skipping rope or on a trampoline (Darling., 2012). A force is

    a push or a full which can cause an object to move, stop, change direction or change speed

    (Darling., 2012). Gravity is thought of as a field force, as it does not contact the object it

    acts upon, such as friction acting on an object would, instead it surrounds the object. (Skamp.,

    2007). Scientifically, the idea of gravity is a fundamental aspect of physics, and is explored at

    great depth through Newtons theory of gravity, and added to by Einsteins theory of

    relativity, which is why it is a key area of learning in science throughout both primary and

    high school years (Kavanagh & Sneider., 2007). The term gravity is not mentioned in the

    Australian Curriculum: Science until year 7, however it is looked at from approximately year

    2 in the physical sciences strand when investigating push and pull forces (ACARA.,

    2012). In primary school, the term gravity is not necessarily the most important aspect for

    students to learn, it is more important that they should learn that it is a pulling force that can

    act on objects from a distance and can make things fall (Kavanagh & Sneider., 2007).

    Without looking at the concept in depth near to that of Newton or Einstein, gravity is a

    pulling force; it pulls things towards the centre of the Earth (Kavanagh & Sneider., 2007).

    The force of gravity, or gravitational pull, is an invisible force such, as magnetism, but differs

    in that gravitational pull is not as strong but can work over much longer, even infinite,

    distances. This can be demonstrated by the fact that gravitational pull exists between planets

    and stars over large distances within space (Woodford., 2004). However, it should be noted

    that as the distance between the objects increases the gravitational pull decreases quickly

    (Cain., 2009). Gravitational pull between planets and the Sun is one of the foundations of the

    Solar System and one of the reasons that the Solar System stays in a particular shape, the

  • 7/29/2019 Children's Views on Science

    4/23

    orbit of the planets around the Sun can be likened to that of a ball on a roulette wheel

    spinning around the centre of gravity (Kavanagh & Sneider., 2007).

    Gravitational force is related to mass, the more mass something has the more gravitational

    force it will exert, which is why the Earth orbits the Sun, and not the other way around

    (Woodford., 2004). Mass is the amount of matter within an object and never changes, as

    opposed to an objects weight, which changes depending on the forces, such as gravitational

    pull, acting on it (Allen., 2010). Although mass and weight are not the same, under a constant

    gravitational force, such as that exerted on Earth, the weight of an object will be proportional

    to its mass, the larger the mass the heavier it weighs ( Wilkening & Huber., 2002). For

    example, one cubic metre of lead will weigh more than one cubic metre of aluminium, as it

    has heavier particles and these particles are more tightly packed, giving it a larger mass, as

    seen in Figure 1 (Allen., 2010).

    The distance between objects will also influence the gravitational pull; the closer the objects

    the stronger the pull (Woodford., 2004). Gravitational force is exerted by all objects, large

    ones such as the gravitational pull the

    Earth has to the people on it, or small ones,

    such as the pull between two blocks of

    metal (Woodford., 2004). In the case of

    the two metal blocks, if they were on

    Earth, although they have gravitational

    pull, this force is overcome by the larger

    pull that is the Earth pulling them to it

    (Allen., 2010). On the Earth it is not only

    objects which are pulled towards the

    centre of the Earth, gravity also ensures

    Figure 1: Two cubes of different metals will weigh

    different amounts and have different masses, even

    though they are the same size. In this case, as the

    iron has more mass than the aluminium it would

    have more gravitational pull than the aluminium. This

    diagram was taken from Allen (2010) p 138.

  • 7/29/2019 Children's Views on Science

    5/23

    that the Earths atmosphere does not escape (Nardelli, Saffin & Taylor., 2003). Although

    gravitational pull draws people to the centre of the Earth, as you get further from the centre of

    the Earth the pull decreases, for example at the top of Mt Everest there is 0.28% less gravity,

    which although is small continues to decrease until there is 90% less gravity when at the

    International Space Station (Cain., 2009). It is important to note that although the

    gravitational pull decreases throughout the atmosphere, gravity is not caused by or related to

    the atmosphere.

    It was Isaac Newton who provided an explanation of gravity after he famously observed an

    apple falling from a tree (Woodford., 2004). As previously explained, all objects have

    gravitational pull, so in the case of the apple it is not only the apple being pulled to the Earth,

    the apple is also exerting its own gravitational pull on the Earth, but in this case its pull is too

    small to measure so it appears that the Earth is the only object with gravitational pull

    (Woodford., 2004). This difference between the gravitational pull of a person for example, as

    compared to that of the Earth means that a person can fall into the Earth due to the

    gravitational pull of the Earth, but the reverse does not occur.

    It is true that objects of different mass will have different gravitational force acting on them,

    however this force acting on them does not increase the acceleration of the object when it is

    falling, so if two items are dropped from the same height, they will land on the ground at the

    same time (assuming that air resistance is not present) (Allen.,2010). This relates to another

    force, that of inertia, so although there is more gravitational pull on the heavier object, it also

    requires more energy to move it, and will still land at the same time as a lighter object

    dropped from the same height, as seen in Figure 2 (Allen., 2010). The pull of gravity towards

    the centre of the Earth on people means that we are constantly falling towards the centre of

    the Earth, however the Earths surface keeps us from continual free fall into the Earths

    centre (Cain., 2009).

  • 7/29/2019 Children's Views on Science

    6/23

    Sports are a good example of the

    different forces acting upon people

    due to gravity (Nardelli, Saffin &

    Taylor., 2003) and one way in

    which gravity can be explained to

    students in younger grades.

    Gravity is present in many

    sporting activities such as

    skydiving, which involves free

    fall, to rock climbing (moving up

    against the force of gravity), to

    activities such as surfing, which

    requires balance between gravity

    and other forces. An example is

    the gravitational pull on the surfer onto the board equalling the forces such as buoyancy

    pushing up on the board and surfer, which means the surfer is able to balance and stay on the

    board (Nardelli, Saffin & Taylor., 2003).

    Section Three

    Alternate Conceptions Relating to Gravity: Alternate conceptions can perpetuate from a

    number of sources such as poor teaching, intuitive assumptions, mis-representation of a

    scientific concept in the community or even the students mis-understanding a scientific

    concept and formulating their own conceptions about how things happen (Skamp., 2007).

    There are numerous alternate conceptions related to physics based concepts such as gravity.

    Some alternate conceptions in relation to gravity are; (1) there is no gravity on the Moon, (2)

    Figure 2: objects with a different mass will still land

    simultaneously. There is more gravity acing on the heavier

    object, but it also takes more force to make it move, which

    balances out and means that the objects of different mass will

    fall at the same time and lad simultaneously (provided that isno air resistance and they are dropped from the same height.

    The diagram was taken from Allen (2010) p124.

  • 7/29/2019 Children's Views on Science

    7/23

    gravity is part of the Earths atmosphere, (3) heavier objects fall faster than light objects from

    the same height, (4) objects have more gravity on them when they are dropped from a higher

    height, and (5) objects at rest have no gravity acting upon them.

    The alternate conception that there is no gravity on the Moon links with another alternate

    conception that gravity is part of the Earths atmosphere, gravitational pull comes from the

    atmosphere and therefore finishes at the surface of the Earth (Gilbert & Watts., 1983). This

    relates to gravity on the Moon because if the learner believes that gravitational pull is caused

    by the atmosphere then, as the Moon has no atmosphere, it therefore also has no gravity

    (Gilbert & Watts., 1983). The alternate conception that there is no gravity on the Moon has

    perpetuated through the many images of weightlessness in space provided through science

    fiction films and other popular culture outlets that children may be exposed to such as books

    and television shows (Allen., 2010). Although popular culture suggests otherwise, there is

    gravity on the Moon, however as the properties of mass and gravitational force are related

    and the Moons mass is less than the Earths the gravitational pull is therefore less. The Moon

    has only 17% of the mass of the Earth, which means it has only 17% of the same

    gravitational pull at its surface (Allen., 2010). As there is less gravitational pull on astronauts

    on the Moon, they are able to jump higher and bound further than they are able to on Earth,

    which may be one of the reasons for the idea that there is no gravity on the Moon, as the

    astronauts are not seen to be pulled to the surface in the same way they do on Earth (Allen.,

    2010).

    Historically, it was thought by philosophers such as Aristotle that the speed at which

    something falls is proportional to its weight (Kavanagh & Sneider., 2007). This has been

    proved scientifically incorrect, but is still often held as an alternate conception relating to

    gravity and free-fall (Allen., 2010). Studies into students in a US first-year college physics

    degree showed that 4 out of 5 students, even at tertiary level of study and having taken

  • 7/29/2019 Children's Views on Science

    8/23

    physics at high school, still held this alternate conception (Kavanagh & Sneider., 2007). The

    widespread belief and tenacity with which this conception perseveres, demonstrates why it is

    important to teach these conceptions scientifically in primary school, rather than allow the

    alternate conception to predominate throughout schooling. This particular alternate

    conception is held because it appears that the heavier of the two items being dropped has

    more gravity acting upon its larger mass (Allen., 2010). A learner who holds this conception

    is likely to believe that as an object increases in mass the amount of gravity acting on it also

    increases, which affects its acceleration (Allen., 2010). This idea, that heavier object will

    have more gravity acting upon it is scientifically correct, however it also requires more

    energy to make it move, which means these forces cancel each other out, resulting in it

    landing at the same time to the lighter object (Naylor and Keogh., 2000). It is also important

    to note that in some cases the opposite alternate conception is held, that lighter objects, such

    as a feather, will fall quicker as they have less air resistance when falling (Allen., 2010).

    The alternate conception that objects dropped from a higher height have more gravity acting

    on them than objects dropped from lower heights comes from stories such as if one drops a

    coin from the top of a skyscraper and it hit someone it would kill them (Allen., 2010). The

    conception that there is more gravity acting on objects if they are higher in the Earths

    atmosphere, which therefore gives them more force when dropped is incorrect, as it is

    actually the weight of the object which causes the acceleration and force with which the

    object lands (Allen, 2010). Scientifically, the higher and further out of Earths atmosphere

    you get the less you weigh (for example if you are on an aeroplane you weigh less), which is

    the opposite of what people with the alternate conception believe (Allen., 2010).

    The final alternate conception in this paper is that objects at rest have no forces acting upon

    them. Students of high school age (12-17) were interviewed on this topic and were found to

    hold this alternate conception, that objects working had more gravity acting upon them than

  • 7/29/2019 Children's Views on Science

    9/23

    those at rest (Kavanagh & Sneider., 2007). It was also found through a different study run by

    Palmer (2001) (as sited in Kavanagh & Sneider., 2007), that 34% of students in year 6 do not

    believe that gravity acts upon stationary objects (Kavanagh & Sneider., 2007). This

    conception develops through children thinking of a force as something which makes things

    move, which is not always the case (Darling., 2012).

    Section Four

    Selection of Children: The children were selected to be interviewed initially because they

    were known to one of the interviewers and therefore easy to contact and arrange to interview.

    The children were also selected because as Kavanagh & Sneider (2007) suggests, children

    between the ages of approximately 9-13 begin to see concepts such as falling not only

    related to the fact that there is nothing supporting the object, or that it is simply heavy, but

    begin to incorporate the idea of gravity as an unseen force to explain why things fall. Both

    the boys interviewed were 9 years old and fell into this age category, so it was a good

    opportunity to see how they viewed concepts such as gravity.

    Ethical Issues: Historically, research in many areas including social sciences, where children

    have been involved, has not always been to the advantage of the child, but to the advantage of

    the researcher(s) involved (Harcourt & Conroy, 2005). The idea that children are citizens and

    able to willingly participate in research of their own consent is a recent recognition point in

    child-related research, one that has come about after the United Nations Convention on the

    Rights of the Child in 1989 (Einsrsdottir., 2009). As part of the rights of the child the consent,

    or informed assent in the case of children, is of key importance when engaging with children

    in ethical research, as participants should have the right to decide whether they would like to

    participate or not (Harcourt & Conroy, 2005) . This choice to participate provides children

  • 7/29/2019 Children's Views on Science

    10/23

    with the feeling that their knowledge and ideas are valid and important, which can lead to

    information gathered from the research interview being of a high quality (Harcourt & Conroy

    2005). The ethical issues involved with interviewing children for research purposes include

    confidentiality (that information or identity of the participant will not be revealed for any

    purposes other than those agreed to) (Einsrsdottir., 2009), that participants feel as though they

    can pull out at any time, and that they dont have to give a particular answerdue to the power

    relationship between the (adult) interviewer and the child. The interviews carried out in this

    research abided by these ethical rules, and as in Einsrsdottir (2009), as well as the childs own

    written informed consent, and the consent of their parents, the children were reminded prior

    to the start of the interview that they had the right to not answer a question or to terminate the

    interview at any time. Research using childrens ideas and knowledge are used as the basis of

    curriculum and other informational documents in education, and to keep this aspect of

    educational research, childrens participation in research must be voluntary and the purpose

    of the research must be known to the child (Harcourt & Conroy 2005).

    To put Luke and Matthew at ease in this interview situation we chose to interview them both

    on the same afternoon, so they both experienced the interviews at a similar time and also at

    the same place. The interviews were conducted after school time and afternoon tea was

    provided for the children to make them feel comfortable. Ensuring the child feels comfortable

    in the surrounding environment is important when interviewing children as, unlike adults,

    they often do not know what to expect in an interview situation (Einsrsdottir., 2009). One of

    the interviewers was already well known to the students; however the other interviewer was

    introduced in an informal manner while afternoon tea was being served so the children felt

    comfortable in the presence of both interviewers. Interviewing children in a well-known

    environment to them and having a familiar person in the interviews are similar to some of the

    methods used and suggested in Einsrsdottir (2009) to ensure that children feel safe and

  • 7/29/2019 Children's Views on Science

    11/23

    empowered to share their knowledge in an interview situation. The interview was conducted

    in a hands-on manner, so that the children did not feel like it was a quiz. There were no

    written questions, only drawing and reading (as well as the questions being read aloud by the

    interviewer to the students), so that if the student had weak reading or writing skills they were

    still able to express themselves. This is similar to methods used in Harcourt & Coroy (2005),

    as they allowed for the children to choose less-conventional methods of expressing

    themselves (other than reading and writing) so that they were participants rather than

    subjects in the research. Sorting and drawing were used in this interview to provide hard

    evidence of the childrens thoughts, and to provide activities for them to engage with and

    actively participate in.

    Interview Process: The interview ran for approximately 15 minutes with each child, with the

    child and two interviewers present. One interviewer interacted with the child and asked

    questions, while the other took notes and photographs of the interview for recording

    purposes. The children were asked general questions about gravity and what they

    understood, then questions relating to particular areas where misconceptions are commonly

    held. Each area was examined differently, for example when being asked about gravity acting

    on falling objects the child was asked to respond to the concept cartoon presented of two

    bungy jumpers (cartoon fromNaylor and Keogh., 2000). Follow up questions such as why?

    orhow? were often asked to clarify the childrens responses and allow them to display their

    knowledge. This concept also involved a physical sort where the children were asked to sort

    pictures of inanimate objects into which would fall faster or slower when dropped from a

    particular height. When asking this question the interviewers made sure to provide children

    with the option to put the objects in a row if they thought they would all land at the same

  • 7/29/2019 Children's Views on Science

    12/23

    time, so as not to bias the answers by suggesting to the children that there was a fast/slow

    gradient for these objects.

    When looking at the idea of gravity on the Moon the children were first asked for their

    knowledge of gravity on the moon and were then shown a clip on YouTube of astronauts on

    the Moon (http://www.youtube.com/watch?v=efzYblYVUFk). The children were asked

    questions about how the astronauts moved, the forces acting upon them and how they stayed

    on the Moon. The children were then asked about the forces acting upon them in the room

    right now; were there the same forces/gravity acting on them as on the book on the table or

    the rug on the floor?

    The most interactive part of the interview related to asking children to explain the gravity

    acting upon a tennis ball when dropped from different heights. The children dropped a tennis

    ball close to the floor, then from standing height, from the top of a chair and then off a

    balcony and were asked to explain the gravity acting upon the tennis ball and how it

    compared from the different heights.

    To finish off the interview the students were asked to draw a picture of the Earth and to draw

    arrows in the direction that a tennis ball would go if they dropped it from different sides of

    the Earth, similar to the task given to year 3 and 6 students in research by Tao, Oliver and

    Venville (2012).

    Results and Interpretation: Both students showed a mixture of alternate and scientific

    conceptions across different areas of the interview (Table 1). While Luke was steadfast and

    confident in his answers, when Matthew was answering he showed evidence of cognitive

    conflict with what he thought to be true but how that could be true based on what he knows,

    which caused him to second-guess his ideas and change his mind as he thought of new

    evidence. For example, when talking about gravity on the Moon, Matthew said that there was

    http://www.youtube.com/watch?v=efzYblYVUFkhttp://www.youtube.com/watch?v=efzYblYVUFkhttp://www.youtube.com/watch?v=efzYblYVUFkhttp://www.youtube.com/watch?v=efzYblYVUFk
  • 7/29/2019 Children's Views on Science

    13/23

    no gravity in space, but then later, when describing how the astronaut stays on the Moon,

    changed his mind and said that there is a little bit of gravity (see notes in Appendix 1.1).

    Both Luke and Matthew initially possessed the alternate conception that there was no gravity

    on the Moon, and gave some form of astronauts clothes (boots, belt), which stopped the

    astronaut from floating away. Even after being presented with the footage of the astronaut

    walking on the moon and coming back down Lukes alternate conception was unchanged, he

    still said it was the boots bringing the astronaut down, but as previously mentioned, when the

    Concept explored Luke Matthew

    What is Gravity? A force, keeps things on the

    ground and stops everything from

    floating. All around the Earth but

    not found anywhere else.

    Gravity pulls you down towards the

    Earth, it is all around the Earth and

    acts on all things. If there was no

    gravity then everything, even the

    ocean, would be floating.

    Free fall of objects

    (refer to Figure 3 and 4)

    Small objects go faster and larger

    ones go slower. Two Bungy

    jumpers would go at the same

    speed. The heavier bungy jumper

    would go further down.

    Serious consideration to the topic,

    re-thought answers to the

    question. Larger objects go faster,

    the large jumper would go further

    and faster.

    Gravity on the Moon Adamant that there was no

    gravity on the moon. The

    astronaut has heavy boots to

    keep him coming back down to

    surface of the moon. The

    bouncing walk of the astronaut is

    because there is no gravity.

    Space has no gravity, which is why

    you float in space. After careful

    consideration he dicided that there

    is a little bt of gravity on the moon

    and that astronauts need more

    weight to keep them on the

    surface, as there is less gravity, that

    is what the space belt does for an

    astronaut.

    Gravity at different heights There is more gravity acting on

    the ball the higher you drop it

    from. This links in with the idea of

    the free fall, as Luke mentioned

    that the ball falls faster the higher

    it is dropped from.

    Identified that gravity is pulling the

    ball down towards the Earth. Holds

    alternate conception that as it goes

    higher there is more gravity acting

    on it. Matthew mentions that the

    ball falls faster at a lower height.

    Gravity on resting objects Understands that the same

    gravity is acting on him, as well as

    other objects and large structures

    such as houses.

    Still developing understanding of

    this concept, had cognitive conflict

    with this idea and kept changing his

    answer as he thought of different

    examples.

    Table 1: A summary of notes taken in the interview showing both Luke and Mathews responses to

    questions asked and their ideas relating to the gravity concepts explored.

  • 7/29/2019 Children's Views on Science

    14/23

    clip was shown to Matthew he changed his mind about gravity on the Moon, saying that there

    was a little bit of gravity to bring the astronaut down again (Table 1). He then followed this

    up by saying that if there was less gravity the heaviness of the astronauts belt would be

    needed to bring him back to the surface of the Moon, the belts weight was to compensate for

    less gravitational pull. These conflicting ideas, and Matthews change in opinion, suggests he

    is slightly confused about the topic, as his realising that there must be gravity on the Moon to

    bring the astronaut down conflicted with his previous idea that there was no gravity at all.

    Both Luke and Matthew held the alternate conception that larger objects would fall faster as

    when presented with a sort of random objects both boys ranked objects which they thought

    would fall faster or slower, and although they both said that particular objects would fall at

    the same time, they did not mention that all objects would land at the same time (Figures 3

    and 4). In this area the boys sorted objects differently, Matthew into a linear progression of

    items fastest to slowest, based largely on the size of the objects (notes in Table 1 and Figure

    3), while Luke chose to sort into two categories, slower and faster, choosing not to

    differentiate within these categories (Figure 4). This may have indicated that Luke, rather

    than believing that each item had an individual speed at which it fell and would land,

    believed closer to the scientific concept, that in fact at least some of these objects would land

    simultaneously, as his categories indicate that all of these objects within each group would

    land at the same time. To further illustrate this point, when presented with the concept

    cartoon from Naylor and Keogh (2000) of two bungy jumpers (a large and a small one) and

    asked who would fall faster, Matthew said that the larger person would fall faster (Appendix

    1.1), but Luke said that they would fall at the same time (Appendix1.2). Lukes choice that

    the two jumpers would fall at the same time does not align completely with his sort of

    inanimate objects (Figure 3), so it is possible that his conception of gravity acting upon

    objects changes in relation to the context in which it is presented. It is important to note that

  • 7/29/2019 Children's Views on Science

    15/23

    in this activity both boys also selected the third

    option, that the heavier person would go down further

    on their bungy cord, which is in fact scientifically

    correct (Naylor & Keogh., 2000).

    Both Luke and Matthew held the alternate conception

    relating to dropping a tennis ball from different

    heights, both of them saying that if dropped from a

    higher point there is more gravity acting on the tennis

    ball.

    When asked about whether or not gravity was acting

    on objects and themselves at the time of the interview Luke demonstrated that he held the

    scientific conception that forces do act on stationary objects, rather than the alternate

    conception. Luke told that gravity was acting upon him at that time, and even went so far as

    to say that gravity was acting on him the same as on the book on the table, or even the house

    on the ground (Table 1). Matthew was not as

    confident in his understanding of this topic (Table

    1). When initially discussing gravity in general he

    had stated that it acts on everything, however when

    it came to making the distinction between gravity

    working on objects as rest compared to working

    objects he was not able to state whether or not

    gravity acted on all of the objects and changed his

    mind many times as he continued to think about

    this.

    When asked to draw where the tennis ball would go

    Figure 4: Lukes sort of how fast

    random objects would fall; he has

    sorted them into 2 groups, faster and

    slower.

    Figure 3 : The order which Matthew

    sorted random objects based upon

    how fast he thought they would fall

    when dropped.

  • 7/29/2019 Children's Views on Science

    16/23

    when dropped in terms of them on the Earth Matthews drawing demonstrated that he

    understood that gravity was not only in the atmosphere, but that it pulled the ball to the centre

    of the Earth, from no matter where on Earth the ball was dropped (Appendix 1.1). Lukes

    response to this question did not demonstrate this scientific idea, rather the alternate

    conception, as his drawing of the figure at the bottom of the Earth showed the ball dropping

    down, away from the surface of the Earth, which suggests that his understanding of gravity

    is that it pulls things down (Appendix 1.2).

    Section Five

    Comparison to the literature: The alternate conception held by the Luke and Matthew in

    relation to how astronauts stay on the Moon appears to be consistent with many childrens

    beliefs that astronauts have some item of clothing/equipment (such as shoes or a belt) which

    keep them from floating away from the surface of the Moon. A British study asked similar

    questions to students about gravity acting on the Moon and one of the responses by a 12 year

    old boy was almost identical to that given by Luke in the interview; that astronauts have

    heavy boots which would prevent them from floating off into space (Kavanagh & Sneider.,

    2007). This response has also been seen in many adults, and even in some teachers who have

    been interviewed on this subject, which shows how widespread this alternate conception is

    (Kavanagh & Sneider., 2007).

    The difficulty Luke had with the drawing activity, where a tennis ball would go if dropped

    straight down, is not an uncommon difficulty for students, as it can be hard for children to

    relate the ground they stand on as part of the Earth as a whole (Gilbert & Watts., 1983).

    When comparing Matthews and Lukes responses to this question, Matthew had a better

    awareness of the surface of the earth being part of the Earth as a whole, and he also identified

    that the pull of gravity was not that of the atmosphere, but of the core of the Earth. In

  • 7/29/2019 Children's Views on Science

    17/23

    comparison, Lukes drawing suggested he did not hold these understandings and held the

    alternate conception that gravity does not come from the Earths centre, and that the

    atmosphere and gravity are linked, which is a common alternate conception held among

    children (Gilbert & Watts., 1983). In studies by Tao, Oliven and Venville (2012), when

    students in year 6 were asked to complete this drawing task, the majority of students

    answered similarly to Luke, they did not mention gravity, and answered that the ball would

    fall to the ground. This indicates that Lukes conceptions are similar to many children of his

    age and he may not have considered the effect of gravity on the ball, or that he does not hold

    the scientific understanding that gravity is from the centre of the Earth. He instead subscribes

    more to the alternate conception that gravity is found in the atmosphere. Matthews response

    to this question, having the scientific understanding that the ball would go to the centre of the

    Earth, was a response given by only four of the eighteen year 6 students interviewed in Tao,

    Oliver and Venville (2012). Although his response is a more scientific conceptualisation of

    what is happening, it is clearly a less commonly held idea among students his age.

    Luke held the correct scientific understanding that objects at rest do have forces, including

    gravitational pull, acting on them. As previously mentioned this concept is not always an

    obvious one for children to grasp, and 34% of students in year 6 were found to hold the

    alternate conception that objects at rest are not being acted on by gravity (Palmer, 2001, as

    cited in Kavanagh & Sneider., 2007). Matthew still appeared to be one of this 34% of

    students who did not yet distinguish between gravity pulling on all objects as compared to

    just objects which are working.

    Section Six

    Rationale for teaching: It has been seen that alternate conceptions can persist, studies in

    Portugal of students after 4 years of a physics course showed that many still believed that

  • 7/29/2019 Children's Views on Science

    18/23

    larger objects would free-fall faster because they were heavier, sharing these beliefs with year

    10 students who had had no formal teaching in this area (Kavanagh & Sneider., 2007). These

    university students believed these conceptions so strongly that they could provide

    mathematical equations which they believed proved their alternate conception (Kavanagh &

    Sneider., 2007). In teaching concepts where alternate conceptions are held two different

    methods can be used, the evolutionary method, where small bits of knowledge are added by

    the learner to encourage change over a period of time, and the revolutionary method, which

    is more of a constructivist approach to learning and creates cognitive conflict to encourage re-

    structuring of the learners ideas based upon new evidence (Gilbert & Watts., 1983). For

    some students the idea of evolutionary change works best to change their conceptions about

    a topic (Skamp., 2007). Providing time for evaluation, discussion and reflection, for example

    in the form of a science journal and group and class discussion, can sometimes be more

    effective in eliciting conceptual change for some students, although teachers do still need to

    be aware if the alternate conception persists (Skamp., 2007).

    It has been suggested that to cause conceptual change in students they need a supportive,

    social environment where they feel safe to discuss what they think (and are encouraged to do

    so), engaging activities, first-hand experiences which challenge their conceptions, and

    encouragement to explain what they see and what they think is happening, after witnessing

    the same concept in a variety of different situations (Kavanagh & Sneider., 2007).

    Constructivist methods, which challenge peoples pre-conceived thoughts and ideas on a

    particular topic, do so by creating a conceptual conflict in the mind of the learner, as there is

    conflict between what they believe to be correct and what they are experiencing (Kavanagh &

    Sneider., 2007). Teaching in a constructivist way requires the students to reconstruct their

    own ideas of a topic based upon new knowledge, rather than a teacher-transmitted telling of

    new ideas and concepts (Skamp., 2007). This can sometimes be difficult, particularly with

  • 7/29/2019 Children's Views on Science

    19/23

    younger students, as it requires metacognitive thought that may not be well developed in

    younger children (Kuhn., 2002). The constructivist view of teaching however, has proved to

    be effective when trying to alter an alternate conception held by a student, and has been seen

    to be particularly effective in teaching about gravity (Kavanagh & Sneider., 2007). Not only

    do students find a conflicting idea through this method of teaching, but they are required to

    include these new conflicts and the evidence for them in restructuring their own ideas about a

    concept (Gilbert & Watts., 1983).

    Teaching to challenge alternate concepts, or to introduce new, scientific ones, is best taught

    when applying concepts to real-world situations (Kavanagh & Sneider., 2007). An example

    of a constructivist teaching activity to address the misconception of the heavier items falling

    faster could be to set the situation of the bungee jumpers such as presenting the concept

    cartoon by Naylor & Keogh (2000) and to then have everyone share their thoughts (and their

    evidence for this), discussing this in groups providing why students think particular things

    will happen. The next step is to actually perform the bungee activity in groups, using

    heavy/light materials and elastic bands, then to provide them with other situations where they

    can test this multiple times, and finally to return and see if the conception has changed based

    on these experiences/ new information by a class or group discussion. Requiring children to

    provide evidence for why they think something will happen encourages the metacognitive

    thinking that will allow students to look at their own beliefs and reconstruct their own ideas

    when presented with new evidence (Kuhn., 2002).

    Time for sharing and discussion is of critical importance when addressing alternate

    conceptions, as hands-on experiences are not always enough to prove or disprove something,

    students need to be able to rationalise and communicate what it is that is happening in these

    experiences (Driver., 1981). Through discussion, the teacher has the opportunity to talk with

    the learner about what they have seen and how their ideas may have changed, which is

  • 7/29/2019 Children's Views on Science

    20/23

    particularly important, especially because the learners conceptions may not have not

    changed through the experiences. It is possible that even if new information and theories are

    presented to the learner, if they are not done so in a way that allows the learner to experience

    cognitive conflict and reconstruct their knowledge with their existing conceptions, then the

    information may be disregarded or assimilated into the persisting alternate conception

    (Kuhn., 2002).

    Another gravity-related activity which could challenge students and make them apply their

    understanding and provide evidence for their decisions would be to discuss what would

    happen if there was no gravity (looking at the effect on buildings and natural formations such

    as oceans and people). This reverse-application of the concept to students lives would be a

    good starting point for discussion about gravity and how it affects students everyday lives.

    Learners should not only have the opportunity to prove new ideas, but to disprove old ones

    (Driver., 1981), which is another reason why multiple activities, with opportunities to prove,

    disprove and apply ideas, will provide students with a solid understanding of scientific

    concepts and will aim to disprove their alternate conceptions. If/once a correct scientific

    conception is held by the student the teacher should re-enforce this knowledge by challenging

    the students to apply their knowledge and see which situations their understandings apply and

    where it does not (Kavanagh & Sneider., 2007).

  • 7/29/2019 Children's Views on Science

    21/23

    Reflection:

    Reporting The assignment requirements were to design and perform an interview with two

    children to determine their prior knowledge about gravity and then take this information to

    compare their knowledge and any alternate conceptions they had to published literature.

    Responding Although I have a science background I do not have a very good knowledge or

    understanding of physics concepts, so this assignment gave me the opportunity to learn about

    these concepts. The interview-style format including the ethics process prior to the interview

    was also a new experience for me, one which I found sometimes challenging but overall

    enjoyed and found to be very informative.

    Relating The process of basing an assignment on information taken from in interview which

    I co-designed was a new experience for me and one which taught me a lot about the interview

    process, obtaining prior knowledge from children, and childrens knowledge about the topic

    of gravity. Unfortunately after conducting the interviews it was found that the audio

    recording did not work and we were unable to use any of the audio recordings for the

    purposes of the assignment. Although this presented an unanticipated challenge, in designing

    the interview we had planned to use a note-taker in addition to the audio recording. This

    choice proved to be beneficial, as in the end the results from our interviews used in the

    assignment were based upon the notes taken in the interview (seen in Appendix).

    Identifying students prior knowledge before teaching is of great importance, because it

    allows teachers to identify areas where students may need specific teaching and design

    learning experiences which will do this (Kavanagh & Sneider., 2007). Simply assuming that a

    student understands gravity because they can tell you that gravity keeps us on the Earth does

    not mean that they do not hold alternate conceptions on the topic. This was seen through our

    research, although both boys interviewed knew that gravity pulled us to the Earth they both

    held different alternate conceptions as to different aspects of the topic.

  • 7/29/2019 Children's Views on Science

    22/23

    The completion of this work highlighted that identification of these alternate conceptions

    through determining students prior knowledge is of great importance in a primary science

    classroom, as alternate conceptions can persist into adulthood. When researching the alternate

    conceptions and reading about studies in which alternate conceptions have persisted into

    adulthood, particularly in Kavangah & Sneider (2007), I was confronted by how influential

    an alternate conception can be if left unchallenged. This has important implications for

    learners throughout their lives and is the responsibility of the teacher to attempt to resolve the

    alternate conceptions held by children. Obviously this can be a problem when teachers

    themselves do not hold the correct scientific conception on the topic, as it has been seen that

    they sometimes do not (Kavanagh & Sneider., 2007). When completing the theory aspect of

    the assignment relating to the scientific concept of gravity, I discovered that I myself held

    some of these alternate conceptions, and found some of the concepts relating to gravity to be

    challenging, so although it is desirable that teachers possess all the correct scientific

    conceptions, this may not always be the case.

    Reconstructing When teaching I will apply the knowledge gained through this assignment in

    terms of content, alternate conceptions and prior knowledge determination to create as many

    opportunities as possible for my students to explore their conceptions and gather evidence to

    enable them to reconstruct their own understanding. I will also attempt to make use of as

    many different activities as possible to determine areas of alternate conception and prior

    knowledge so that I am able to give students experiences to further their own learning and

    develop their scientific thinking. This assignment has allowed me to realise that I also hold

    some alternate conceptions. Although I cannot know everything about all the different areas

    of science, as a teacher attempting to address alternate student conceptions, it is my

    responsibility to ensure that I am providing students with correct information to help them

    challenge their own conceptions and construct new ones.

  • 7/29/2019 Children's Views on Science

    23/23

    References

    Australian Curriculum and Reporting Agency (2012) Australian Curriculum: Science

    Allen, M. (2010)Misconceptions in Primary Science. Maidenhead: Mc Graw-Hill Education.

    Cain, F. (2009). Gravity of the Earth. The Universe Today.

    http://www.universetoday.com/26775/gravity-of-the-earth/. (accessed 31 March 2013).

    Darling, G. (2012) How does force affect motion? Science and Children. 50 (2).

    Driver, R. (1981) Pupils alternative frameworks in science.European Journal of Science

    Education. 3 (1). 93-101.

    Einsrsdottir, J. (2007). Research with children: methodological and ethical challenges.

    European Early Childhood Education. 15 (2). 197-211.

    Gilbert, J., and Watts, D. (1983). Concepts, misconceptions and alternative conceptions:

    changing perspectives in science education. Studies in Science Education. 10 (1). 61-98.

    Harcourt, D., and Conroy, H. (2005) Informed assent: ethics and processes when researching

    with young children.Early Child Development and Care. 175 (6). 567-577.

    Kuhn, D. (2002). What is Scientific Thinking and How Does it Develop? In Goswami, U.

    (Ed).Blackwell Handbook of Childhood Cognitive Development. (349-370). doi:

    10.1002/9780470996652

    Nardelli,D., Saffin, P., and Taylor, P. (2003). Science Alive . Australia: Jacaranda. p 220,

    231.

    Naylor, S., and Keogh, B. (2000). Cartoon Concepts in Science Education. Cheshire, UK:

    Millgate House Publishing.

    Kavanagh, C., and Sneider, C. (2007). Learning about gravity I. Free fall: a guide for teachers

    and curriculum developers.Astronomy Education Review. 5 (2).

    Skamp, K. (2007) Teaching Primary Science Constructively. South Melbourne, Victoria:

    Cengage Learning.

    Tao, Y., Oliver, M., and Venville, G. (2012). Chinese and Australian childrens

    understanding of the Earth: a cross cultural study of conceptual development. Cultural

    Studies of Science Education.doi: 10.1007/s11422-012-9415-1

    Wilkening, F., and Huber, S. (2002). Childrens Intuitive Physics. In Goswami, U. (Ed).

    Blackwell Handbook of Childhood Cognitive Development. (349-370). doi:10.1002/9780470996652

    Woodford, C. (2004)Routes of Science: Gravity. London: Brown Reference Group.

    http://www.universetoday.com/26775/gravity-of-the-earth/http://www.universetoday.com/26775/gravity-of-the-earth/http://www.universetoday.com/26775/gravity-of-the-earth/