78
Biomathematics 170A Medical Statistics Jeff Gornbein Office:Life Science 5202 [email protected] 310-825-4193 Office hrs by appt – strongly encouraged 1

Biomathematics 170A Medical Statistics Jeff Gornbein Office:Life Science 5202 [email protected] 310-825-4193 Office hrs by appt – strongly encouraged 1

Embed Size (px)

Citation preview

Page 1: Biomathematics 170A Medical Statistics Jeff Gornbein Office:Life Science 5202 gornbein@ucla.edu 310-825-4193 Office hrs by appt – strongly encouraged 1

Biomathematics 170A

Medical Statistics

Jeff Gornbein

Office:Life Science 5202

[email protected]

310-825-4193

Office hrs by appt – strongly encouraged

1

Page 2: Biomathematics 170A Medical Statistics Jeff Gornbein Office:Life Science 5202 gornbein@ucla.edu 310-825-4193 Office hrs by appt – strongly encouraged 1

Biostatistics – tools for evidence based medicine

Cedars-Sinai Medical Center

Jeff Gornbein, DrPH

Stat/Biomath Consulting Clinic (SBCC)

UCLA Dept of Biomathematics

[email protected]

310-825-4193

gornbein.bol.ucla.edu/cedarassign.htm

2

Page 3: Biomathematics 170A Medical Statistics Jeff Gornbein Office:Life Science 5202 gornbein@ucla.edu 310-825-4193 Office hrs by appt – strongly encouraged 1

Suggested Texts• Medical Statistics at a Glance, 3nd ed

Petrie A, Sabin C, Wiley-Blackwell Pub, 2009 thin, quick & cheap

• Designing Clinical Research. 3rd edHully S, Cummings S, Browner W, Grady D, Newman T

Lippincott Williams & Wilkins, 2006

mostly clinical, good sample size tables

• Wheelen C, Naked Statistics, Norton 2013 – Fun!

• Statistical Reasoning in Medicine-L Moye

Springer, 2000 -written by an MD3

Page 4: Biomathematics 170A Medical Statistics Jeff Gornbein Office:Life Science 5202 gornbein@ucla.edu 310-825-4193 Office hrs by appt – strongly encouraged 1

Notes Contents (subject to change) Section topic I Study design, Confounding & Bias Stratification & adjustment II Descriptive statistics for continuous & binary data (including survival) III Population distributions- Gaussian, Binomial, Poisson IV Sampling distribution, Confidence Intervals and hypothesis testing V Sample size and power VI Comparing means & ANOVA VII Comparing proportions & chi-square VIII Simple linear regression and introduction to multiple regression IX Logistic regression & quantal response (or non parametric testing)

4

Page 5: Biomathematics 170A Medical Statistics Jeff Gornbein Office:Life Science 5202 gornbein@ucla.edu 310-825-4193 Office hrs by appt – strongly encouraged 1

Confounding, bias & Study Design

5

Page 6: Biomathematics 170A Medical Statistics Jeff Gornbein Office:Life Science 5202 gornbein@ucla.edu 310-825-4193 Office hrs by appt – strongly encouraged 1

6

Page 7: Biomathematics 170A Medical Statistics Jeff Gornbein Office:Life Science 5202 gornbein@ucla.edu 310-825-4193 Office hrs by appt – strongly encouraged 1

Important Risk Information About VYTORIN:VYTORIN is a prescription tablet and isn’t right for everyone, including women who are nursing or pregnant or who may become pregnant, and anyone with liver problems. Unexplained muscle pain or weakness could be a sign of a rare but serious side effect and should be reported to your doctor right away.VYTORIN may interact with other medicines or certain foods, increasing your risk of getting this serious side effect. So, tell your doctor about any other medications you are taking. Your doctor may do simple blood tests before and during treatment with VYTORIN to check for liver problems. Side effects included headache and muscle pain. VYTORIN contains two cholesterol medicines, Zetia (ezetimibe) and Zocor (simvastatin), in a single tablet. VYTORIN has not been shown to reduce heart attacks or strokes more than Zocor alone.(emphasis added) 7

Page 8: Biomathematics 170A Medical Statistics Jeff Gornbein Office:Life Science 5202 gornbein@ucla.edu 310-825-4193 Office hrs by appt – strongly encouraged 1

THE EVIDENCE GAPFor Widely Used Drug, Question of Usefulness Is Still

Lingering (NY Times, 1 Sept 2008)By ALEX BERENSON

When the Food and Drug Administration approved a new type of cholesterol-lowering medicine in 2002, it did so on the basis of a handful of clinical trials covering a total of 3,900 patients. None of the patients took the medicine for more than 12 weeks, and the trials offered no evidence that it had reduced heart attacks or cardiovascular disease, the goal of any cholesterol drug.

The lack of evidence has not stopped doctors from heavily prescribing that drug, whether in a stand-alone form sold as Zetia or as a combination medicine called Vytorin. Aided by extensive consumer advertising, sales of the medicines reached $5.2 billion last year, making them among the best-selling drugs in the world. More than three million people worldwide take either drug every day.

But there is still no proof that the drugs help patients live longer or avoid heart attacks. This year Vytorin has failed two clinical trials meant to show its benefits. Worse, scientists are debating whether there is a link between the drugs and cancer.

8

Page 9: Biomathematics 170A Medical Statistics Jeff Gornbein Office:Life Science 5202 gornbein@ucla.edu 310-825-4193 Office hrs by appt – strongly encouraged 1

August 19, 2012 NY Times

Testing What We Think We Know By H. GILBERT WELCH• BY 1990, many doctors were recommending

hormone replacement therapy to healthy middle-aged women and P.S.A. screening for prostate cancer to older men. Both interventions had become standard medical practice.

• But in 2002, a randomized trial showed that preventive hormone replacement caused more problems (more heart disease and breast cancer) than it solved (fewer hip fractures and colon cancer). Then, in 2009, trials showed that P.S.A. screening led to many unnecessary surgeries and had a dubious effect on prostate cancer deaths.

Page 10: Biomathematics 170A Medical Statistics Jeff Gornbein Office:Life Science 5202 gornbein@ucla.edu 310-825-4193 Office hrs by appt – strongly encouraged 1

10

Page 11: Biomathematics 170A Medical Statistics Jeff Gornbein Office:Life Science 5202 gornbein@ucla.edu 310-825-4193 Office hrs by appt – strongly encouraged 1

Cant reproduce findingsBegley(Amgem)-Nature 2012, 483 p 531-533

Fifty-three papers were deemed ‘landmark’ studies. It was acknowledged from the outset that some of the data might not hold up, because papers were deliberately selected that described something completely new, such as fresh approaches to targeting cancers or alternative clinical uses for existing therapeutics. Nevertheless, scientific findings were confirmed in only 6 (11%) cases. Even knowing the limitations of preclinical research, this was a shocking result.

11

Page 12: Biomathematics 170A Medical Statistics Jeff Gornbein Office:Life Science 5202 gornbein@ucla.edu 310-825-4193 Office hrs by appt – strongly encouraged 1

Section I - Study Design

Two essential questions in clinical & experimental medicine: 1. What is the best therapy/treatment? 2. What is the cause of disease? – Epi

(not talking about mechanisms)

Threats to study integrity Confounding Bias

Designs

Experiments – Clinical Trials Observational Studies

12

Page 13: Biomathematics 170A Medical Statistics Jeff Gornbein Office:Life Science 5202 gornbein@ucla.edu 310-825-4193 Office hrs by appt – strongly encouraged 1

Working definition of causality (or efficacy) The requirement for "proof"

Definition: We say that “X causes Y” when, all other factors associated with the outcome held constant, a change in predictor X, the "cause" (more frequently) leads to a change in the outcome (or effect) Y. This usually implies a temporal ordering (the cause must happen before the effect) and/or a dose response (the higher the dose of ionizing radiation the higher the probability of getting cancer. So, to establish causality (for disease) or efficacy (for a treatment) there are at least three requirements:

I. The comparison groups must be comparable (no bias, no confounding). This does not happen unless the study had the proper design.

II. The association must not be due to chance alone. This is where inferential statistics (p values, CIs) are useful.

III. The temporal ordering must be correct (cause comes before effect). This is a bigger issue in observational studies.

13

Page 14: Biomathematics 170A Medical Statistics Jeff Gornbein Office:Life Science 5202 gornbein@ucla.edu 310-825-4193 Office hrs by appt – strongly encouraged 1

Bradford Hill “causation” criteria1. Consistency: Same finding observed by different persons in different places with different samples

2. Specificity: Causation is likely if seen in a very specific population at a specific site and disease with no other likely explanation. The more specific an association between a factor and an effect is, the bigger the probability of a causal relationship.

3. Temporality: The effect has to occur after the cause. If there is an expected delay between the cause and expected effect, then the effect must occur after that delay.

4. Biological gradient: Greater exposure should generally lead to greater incidence. However, in some cases, the mere presence of the factor can trigger the effect. In other cases, an inverse proportion is observed: greater exposure leads to lower incidence. Sometimes called the “dose-response” effect. Can be “U” shaped.

5. Plausibility: A plausible mechanism between cause and effect is helpful, but not required.

 

6. Coherence: There is coherence (agreement) between epidemiological and laboratory findings .

7. Experiment: Relationship can be investigated in an experiment. Not always possible.

8. Analogy: The effect of similar factors may be considered. 14

Page 15: Biomathematics 170A Medical Statistics Jeff Gornbein Office:Life Science 5202 gornbein@ucla.edu 310-825-4193 Office hrs by appt – strongly encouraged 1

Confounding

X outcome (Y)

Confounder Important-A confounder is

1) associated with risk factor X (double arrow)

2) an independent risk factor for Y (single arrow pointed at Y)

15

Page 16: Biomathematics 170A Medical Statistics Jeff Gornbein Office:Life Science 5202 gornbein@ucla.edu 310-825-4193 Office hrs by appt – strongly encouraged 1

Confounding

Diet Weight loss

Exercise

Key

= causation (uni direction)

= association (bi direction)

16

Page 17: Biomathematics 170A Medical Statistics Jeff Gornbein Office:Life Science 5202 gornbein@ucla.edu 310-825-4193 Office hrs by appt – strongly encouraged 1

Not a confounder–intermediate risk factor

(mediator)

smoking serum nicotine lung cancer

When looking at lung cancer risk due to smoking, we would not control for serum nicotine. This would remove or reduce the effect we were trying to study.

17

Page 18: Biomathematics 170A Medical Statistics Jeff Gornbein Office:Life Science 5202 gornbein@ucla.edu 310-825-4193 Office hrs by appt – strongly encouraged 1

Collider

Artifactual relationships may appear even though there is no causation or association. Example:

Flu Fever food poisoning

One incorrectly thinks getting the flu is associated with food poisoning since both cause fever.

18

Page 19: Biomathematics 170A Medical Statistics Jeff Gornbein Office:Life Science 5202 gornbein@ucla.edu 310-825-4193 Office hrs by appt – strongly encouraged 1

Easy to be mislead when one does not control for confounding

cholesterol in mg/L No apparent gender difference

Statistic Males Females Mean 205 205 SD 30 29 n 100 100 SEM 3.0 2.9

19

Page 20: Biomathematics 170A Medical Statistics Jeff Gornbein Office:Life Science 5202 gornbein@ucla.edu 310-825-4193 Office hrs by appt – strongly encouraged 1

Cholesterol (mg/dl) in males and females - No apparent gender difference

variable Male Female

mean age 30 40

mean chol 205 205

But Controlling for age, males are higher than females

The mean cholesterol ignoring age is the same in male & females

110

130

150

170

190

210

230

250

270

15 20 25 30 35 40 45 50 55

age

cho

l

males

female

M F

20

Page 21: Biomathematics 170A Medical Statistics Jeff Gornbein Office:Life Science 5202 gornbein@ucla.edu 310-825-4193 Office hrs by appt – strongly encouraged 1

Depression in males vs female

Depression score from 0 (good) to 100 (bad)

Gender mean depression score

Males 66

Females 76

p < 0.001

21

Page 22: Biomathematics 170A Medical Statistics Jeff Gornbein Office:Life Science 5202 gornbein@ucla.edu 310-825-4193 Office hrs by appt – strongly encouraged 1

variable Male Female

income 17,000 12,000

mean depr 66 76

Males seem to have lower depression than females

50

55

60

65

70

75

80

85

10000 11000 12000 13000 14000 15000 16000 17000 18000 19000 20000

income

dep

r

males

females

means

F

M

Controlling for income, depression is the same in males and females

Ex 2 – Depression scores in males versus females

22

Page 23: Biomathematics 170A Medical Statistics Jeff Gornbein Office:Life Science 5202 gornbein@ucla.edu 310-825-4193 Office hrs by appt – strongly encouraged 1

Effect modification When effect is not the same at all levels of the confounder (non parallel, interactions), confounder

is often called an effect modifier (moderator)

effect modification

100120140160180200220240260

15 20 25 30 35 40 45 50 55

age

cho

l

males

female

When young, chol is higher in males but gap narrows with age 23

Page 24: Biomathematics 170A Medical Statistics Jeff Gornbein Office:Life Science 5202 gornbein@ucla.edu 310-825-4193 Office hrs by appt – strongly encouraged 1

Can’t assume additive thinkingRelationships are not necessarily linear or

additive. May be “ok” to look at one factor at a time if relation is of the form

Outcome(Y)=bo + b1 age + b2 gender + …

ex: HDL = 46 + 0.15 age -10 male

In real life, not all factors are linear or additive (interactions, synergisms,

antagonisms)

24

Page 25: Biomathematics 170A Medical Statistics Jeff Gornbein Office:Life Science 5202 gornbein@ucla.edu 310-825-4193 Office hrs by appt – strongly encouraged 1

Is lumpectomy bad?

Breast Cancer survival (unpublished)

0%

10%

20%

30%

40%

50%

60%

70%

80%

90%

100%

0 6 12 18 24 30 36 42 48 54 60 66 72 78 84months of follow up

Lumpectomy

Mastectomy

Radical Mastectomy

25

Page 26: Biomathematics 170A Medical Statistics Jeff Gornbein Office:Life Science 5202 gornbein@ucla.edu 310-825-4193 Office hrs by appt – strongly encouraged 1

Fisher et. al. Oct 2002 NEJM p1233

BackgroundIn 1976, we initiated a randomized trial to determine whether lumpectomy with or without radiation therapy

was as effective as total mastectomy for the treatment of invasive breast cancer.

MethodsA total of 1851 women for whom followup data were

available and nodal status was known underwent randomly assigned treatment consisting of total mastectomy, lumpectomy alone, or lumpectomy

and breast irradiation. Kaplan–Meier and cumulative- incidence estimates of the outcome were obtained.

26

Page 27: Biomathematics 170A Medical Statistics Jeff Gornbein Office:Life Science 5202 gornbein@ucla.edu 310-825-4193 Office hrs by appt – strongly encouraged 1

Fisher et. al. Oct 2002 NEJM p1233

27

Page 28: Biomathematics 170A Medical Statistics Jeff Gornbein Office:Life Science 5202 gornbein@ucla.edu 310-825-4193 Office hrs by appt – strongly encouraged 1

Bias (internal bias)

Bias: Usually caused by action taken (or not taken) by the investigator

Confounding: Usually due to a patient variable/action rather than the action of the investigator

28

Page 29: Biomathematics 170A Medical Statistics Jeff Gornbein Office:Life Science 5202 gornbein@ucla.edu 310-825-4193 Office hrs by appt – strongly encouraged 1

Major Types of bias• Variable observer bias - The apparent effect is due to a

difference in the observers (ie. the MD) and not to a true difference in the outcome. “Calibration” bias.

• Hawthorne effect - The subject (patient) changes his response in the presence of the questioner (physician). Showing interest in a patient changes their response.

• Response bias - The way and conditions under which the question is asked affect the answer. Hawthorne effect is a specific response bias.

• Diagnostic accuracy bias - The accuracy of the diagnosis changes (usually improves) over time. Causes apparent disease incidence to change.

29

Page 30: Biomathematics 170A Medical Statistics Jeff Gornbein Office:Life Science 5202 gornbein@ucla.edu 310-825-4193 Office hrs by appt – strongly encouraged 1

Survival / dropout bias -Only those healthy enough to survive until data is collected can provide data.

Ex – WBC toxicity in chemo

Treatment A Treatment B

Mean WBC 5600 4200

Sample size (n) 67 89

Is B really more toxic than A (lower WBC)?

The n is smaller in A since more died.

30

Page 31: Biomathematics 170A Medical Statistics Jeff Gornbein Office:Life Science 5202 gornbein@ucla.edu 310-825-4193 Office hrs by appt – strongly encouraged 1

Dropouts in a clinical trial are a major potential source of bias even though

patients may be randomized to treatment.

Must report dropouts, compare baseline characteristics in dropouts versus non

dropouts to see if dropouts are at random or are systematic (ie older, sicker more

likely to drop out)

31

Page 32: Biomathematics 170A Medical Statistics Jeff Gornbein Office:Life Science 5202 gornbein@ucla.edu 310-825-4193 Office hrs by appt – strongly encouraged 1

Some sources of bias

Study design: Absence of a control group Wrong type of controls used Lack of control for other prognostic factors

Sample selection: Poor eligibility (inclusion/exclusion) criteriaCan’t generalize to population of interest from "grab"

(convenience) samples (external bias)Refusals – sickest persons may not agree to participate

Conduct of study: Differential dropouts – More/sicker dropouts in one group (like survival bias)

Poor and differential diagnosis and supportive care Patients in treatment group get more attention than controlsInadequate evaluation methodsPoor data quality, errors and missing data

32

Page 33: Biomathematics 170A Medical Statistics Jeff Gornbein Office:Life Science 5202 gornbein@ucla.edu 310-825-4193 Office hrs by appt – strongly encouraged 1

External bias / lack of validity

(non representative sample)

The term "bias" is also used when the study sample is not representative of the target population of interest. This is "external" bias or "selection" bias as noted above. Often, groups may be comparable within a study but results cannot be generalized to a wider population.

33

Page 34: Biomathematics 170A Medical Statistics Jeff Gornbein Office:Life Science 5202 gornbein@ucla.edu 310-825-4193 Office hrs by appt – strongly encouraged 1

How to deal with confounding?

• 1. By study design (inclusion/exclusion, randomization …)

• 2. By stratification (group matching) or individual matching (can be part of the

design)

• 3 By statistical modeling

(regression is one example)

34

Page 35: Biomathematics 170A Medical Statistics Jeff Gornbein Office:Life Science 5202 gornbein@ucla.edu 310-825-4193 Office hrs by appt – strongly encouraged 1

Experiments = clinical trialsFor assessing treatments

• Premeditated nonstandard treatment intervention

• Primary purpose to evaluate the relative efficacy of the treatments.

• Study is an experiment when the main reason for treatment assignment is to make

comparisons possible and at least one of the treatments is not part of the standard therapy.

• Does not require randomization (quasi expt) or blinding to be an experiment

35

Page 36: Biomathematics 170A Medical Statistics Jeff Gornbein Office:Life Science 5202 gornbein@ucla.edu 310-825-4193 Office hrs by appt – strongly encouraged 1

Experimental designs

Randomized controlled trial (RCT)

Crossover trial

Quasi-experiment= Parallel group trial

Self control, before and after trial

(no controls-”case series”)

External or Historical controls

Diagnostic assessment study (medical test)

36

Page 37: Biomathematics 170A Medical Statistics Jeff Gornbein Office:Life Science 5202 gornbein@ucla.edu 310-825-4193 Office hrs by appt – strongly encouraged 1

RCTExample: Breast cancer patients are randomized to

surgery with standard chemo (group A) vs surgery with standard chemo + Herceptin (group B)

Group A

Screen ->Enroll & randomize

Group B

Primary Outcome: Disease free survival

37

Page 38: Biomathematics 170A Medical Statistics Jeff Gornbein Office:Life Science 5202 gornbein@ucla.edu 310-825-4193 Office hrs by appt – strongly encouraged 1

Parallel groups-Quasi ExptExample: Those taking aspirin are compared to those not taking aspirin. Patients gets to decide if they take aspirin (self assigned). NOT randomized but ascertained at the same calendar times (parallel in time).

Group A

Screen ->Enroll Group B

Outcome: Time to first heart attack

38

Page 39: Biomathematics 170A Medical Statistics Jeff Gornbein Office:Life Science 5202 gornbein@ucla.edu 310-825-4193 Office hrs by appt – strongly encouraged 1

Before-after trial paired trial (“case series”)

bacteria before - mouthwash - bacteria after

Acne on left side – placebo treatment

Acne on right side – antibiotic treatment

In these studies, same person is measured twice (or many times – repeated measures)

There is no control group – Often assume the behavior of the outcome is known with no treatment.

39

Page 40: Biomathematics 170A Medical Statistics Jeff Gornbein Office:Life Science 5202 gornbein@ucla.edu 310-825-4193 Office hrs by appt – strongly encouraged 1

Example: before-after trialNonconventional treatment for pain

(see Bausell)

pain by time (arbitrary pain units)

01234567

0 3 6 9 12 15 18 21 24 27 30 33

day

pain

40

Page 41: Biomathematics 170A Medical Statistics Jeff Gornbein Office:Life Science 5202 gornbein@ucla.edu 310-825-4193 Office hrs by appt – strongly encouraged 1

Crossover trial

Treatment A – washout - Treatment BScreen-> enroll &randomize

Treatment B – washout – Treatment A

Historical controlsExample: Breast cancer survival in those before herceptin was introduced in 1997 Is compared to with survival in those given herceptin after 1997.

***************************************************************************

41

Page 42: Biomathematics 170A Medical Statistics Jeff Gornbein Office:Life Science 5202 gornbein@ucla.edu 310-825-4193 Office hrs by appt – strongly encouraged 1

Diagnostic assessment

One diagnostic test is compared to another or to a “gold standard”.

Example: Colposcopy is compared to pap smear for cervical cancer.

Gold standard is biopsy. Hard to do since all women must be biopsied in order to fairly estimate sensitivity, specificity and not just predictive values.

42

Page 43: Biomathematics 170A Medical Statistics Jeff Gornbein Office:Life Science 5202 gornbein@ucla.edu 310-825-4193 Office hrs by appt – strongly encouraged 1

No B BLow A Y YMed A Y YHigh A Y Y

No B BLow A Y YMed A Y YHigh A Y Y

Factorial experimental designEvaluate several factors at same time

No C

C

43

Page 44: Biomathematics 170A Medical Statistics Jeff Gornbein Office:Life Science 5202 gornbein@ucla.edu 310-825-4193 Office hrs by appt – strongly encouraged 1

Survival at 3 years in MI patients on standard treatment plus anti arrhythmic

and/or NSAID

.

treatment low dose NSAID high dose NSAID

no anti arrhythmic tx 60% 80%

anti arrhythmic tx 70% ??

44

Page 45: Biomathematics 170A Medical Statistics Jeff Gornbein Office:Life Science 5202 gornbein@ucla.edu 310-825-4193 Office hrs by appt – strongly encouraged 1

Survival at 3 years in MI patients on standard treatment plus anti arrhythmic

and/or NSAID

Factorial design can identify interactions.Not discovered if only one factor varied and

the others held constant.

treatment low dose NSAID high dose NSAID

no anti arrhythmic tx 60% 80%

anti arrhythmic tx 70% 65%

45

Page 46: Biomathematics 170A Medical Statistics Jeff Gornbein Office:Life Science 5202 gornbein@ucla.edu 310-825-4193 Office hrs by appt – strongly encouraged 1

Repeated measure design

Time 1 Time 2 Time 3

Treatment A Y Y Y

Treatment B Y Y Y

Each subject measured repeatedly over time. A paired comparisonis a special case. Treatment is the “between group” factor, and time is

the “within group” factor. Measuring the same person four times is NOT the same as measuring four different groups once so the between group and

within group comparisons have different statistical properties.

Page 47: Biomathematics 170A Medical Statistics Jeff Gornbein Office:Life Science 5202 gornbein@ucla.edu 310-825-4193 Office hrs by appt – strongly encouraged 1

Order Period 1 Period 2

T-P 43%(Timolol)

27%(Placebo)

P-T 27%(Placebo)

43%(Timolol)

Cross over designOutcome- pct with relief from chronic migraine headache

Ideal result- No period effects, no carry over (order) effects

There is a 43%-27%=16% improvement due to Timolol

47

Page 48: Biomathematics 170A Medical Statistics Jeff Gornbein Office:Life Science 5202 gornbein@ucla.edu 310-825-4193 Office hrs by appt – strongly encouraged 1

Order Period 1 Period 2

T-P 43%(Timolol)

37%(Placebo)

P-T 27%(Placebo)

53%(Timolol)

Cross over designOutcome- pct with relief from chronic migraine headache

Period effect

There is a 16% improvement due to Timolol and a 10%Improvement due to time period

48

Page 49: Biomathematics 170A Medical Statistics Jeff Gornbein Office:Life Science 5202 gornbein@ucla.edu 310-825-4193 Office hrs by appt – strongly encouraged 1

Order Period 1 Period 2

T-P 43%(Timolol)

41%(Placebo)

P-T 27%(Placebo)

43%(Timolol)

Cross over designOutcome- pct with relief from chronic migraine headache

Carryover (order) effect

Giving Timolol “cures” 14-16% of patients. Only period 1 gives unbiased estimate

49

Page 50: Biomathematics 170A Medical Statistics Jeff Gornbein Office:Life Science 5202 gornbein@ucla.edu 310-825-4193 Office hrs by appt – strongly encouraged 1

Experiments - Disadvantages

• Experiments are very costly in time and money. • Many research questions can’t be addressed because of ethical problems or disease

is too rare • Physicians and patients often unwilling to participate, particularly in randomized

trials. • Inappropriate use of historical controls or no controls can produce major errors! (less

of a problem with concurrent controls) • Answers from standardized clinical trials may be different from the behavior in

general practice. For example only a single fixed dose may be evaluated in a trial, whereas the general practice uses many doses.

• Trials tend to restrict the scope and the questions under study.

Experiments - Advantages Experiments are usually in the correct temporal order

• Properly controlled and designed experiments produce strongest evidence for cause & effect or lack thereof. May be unethical to give a treatment that does not work. Important in an era of proliferating medical technology.

• Randomized trials are best for assuring comparability and best for controlling confounding and bias.

• Sometimes required by the Govt. (FDA and new drugs)

• Can be faster and cheaper in the long run if they put a controversy to rest. 50

Page 51: Biomathematics 170A Medical Statistics Jeff Gornbein Office:Life Science 5202 gornbein@ucla.edu 310-825-4193 Office hrs by appt – strongly encouraged 1

Criteria for the “best” experiments/trials (Bausell R, Snake Oil Science, Oxford Univ Press, 2007)

1. Randomized Trial

2. Double blind (if applicable)

3. Large sample size (at least 50/group)

4. No more than 25% dropouts in any group

5. Published in high quality peer reviewed Journal

51

Page 52: Biomathematics 170A Medical Statistics Jeff Gornbein Office:Life Science 5202 gornbein@ucla.edu 310-825-4193 Office hrs by appt – strongly encouraged 1

Observational studies

Cohort/prospective/longitudinal

Historical Cohort (some call “retrospective”)

Cross sectional-survey

Case-Control (true “retrospective”)

“Ecologic” – aggregate units

52

Page 53: Biomathematics 170A Medical Statistics Jeff Gornbein Office:Life Science 5202 gornbein@ucla.edu 310-825-4193 Office hrs by appt – strongly encouraged 1

Cohort: Coffee vs Pancreatic cancer(Michaud et. al., Cancer Epi Biomark, May 2001)

1980 Nurses Health study, 1986 Health professionals study

136,593 persons. Most followed to 1996+

n=35,738 no coffee, n=27,012 w/ 4+ cups

RR=0.62,

95% CI for true RR (0.27, 1.43)

For 4+ cups/day vs no coffee

53

Page 54: Biomathematics 170A Medical Statistics Jeff Gornbein Office:Life Science 5202 gornbein@ucla.edu 310-825-4193 Office hrs by appt – strongly encouraged 1

COHORT - advantages• Establishes sequence of events

• Avoids bias in measuring predictors• Avoids survival bias

• Can study several outcomes• Yields incidence, relative risk, risk

difference• Gives control of selection of subjects and

over what to measure• Outcome not likely to affect the selection

of subjects (no selection bias)

54

Page 55: Biomathematics 170A Medical Statistics Jeff Gornbein Office:Life Science 5202 gornbein@ucla.edu 310-825-4193 Office hrs by appt – strongly encouraged 1

COHORT – disadvantages

• Usually need large sample size

• Not feasible for rare outcomes/diseases

• May have long duration

• May have dropouts/loss to follow up

• Does not guarantee comparability

55

Page 56: Biomathematics 170A Medical Statistics Jeff Gornbein Office:Life Science 5202 gornbein@ucla.edu 310-825-4193 Office hrs by appt – strongly encouraged 1

Cross sectional example:

MESA data in FY 2000log HOMA Insulin resistance (IR) By BMI

n=750, r= -0.45, rs= -0.46, p < 0.001

-3

-2

-1

0

1

log H

OM

A IR

20 25 30 35 40 45 50 55

BMI

56

Page 57: Biomathematics 170A Medical Statistics Jeff Gornbein Office:Life Science 5202 gornbein@ucla.edu 310-825-4193 Office hrs by appt – strongly encouraged 1

Cohort effect in cross sec study

57

20 25 30 35 40 45-1.8

-1.6

-1.4

-1.2

-1.0

-0.8

-0.6

-0.4

-0.2

log IR vs BMI

old middle young

BMI

log

IR

Red descending line is misleading

Page 58: Biomathematics 170A Medical Statistics Jeff Gornbein Office:Life Science 5202 gornbein@ucla.edu 310-825-4193 Office hrs by appt – strongly encouraged 1

Cross-sectional: advantages

Can study several outcomes at same time

Can study several exposures at same time

Short study duration

Provides prevalence (not incidence)

Can be front end of a cohort study

58

Page 59: Biomathematics 170A Medical Statistics Jeff Gornbein Office:Life Science 5202 gornbein@ucla.edu 310-825-4193 Office hrs by appt – strongly encouraged 1

Cross sectional:disadvantagesDoes not establish temporal order

Exposure info from memory may not be accurate (recall bias)

Only survivors can be measured – survival bias

Not feasible for rare diseases

Can’t distinguish between predictors of disease occurrence vs disease progression

Can’t provide incidence

Assumes observed associations across persons are the same as associations across time within a

person(In Miami, young Cuban males grow up to be old Jewish males)

59

Page 60: Biomathematics 170A Medical Statistics Jeff Gornbein Office:Life Science 5202 gornbein@ucla.edu 310-825-4193 Office hrs by appt – strongly encouraged 1

Case control : exampleCoffee & Pancreas cancer

(MacMahon et. al. NEJM, March 1981)

369 with histologic confirmed cancer

644 controls (no cancer)

OR=2.7

95% CI (1.6 to 4.7)

For 3+ cups/day vs no coffee

60

Page 61: Biomathematics 170A Medical Statistics Jeff Gornbein Office:Life Science 5202 gornbein@ucla.edu 310-825-4193 Office hrs by appt – strongly encouraged 1

Case-Control: advantages

Feasible for rare diseases

Short duration

Inexpensive - easy to do

Can evaluate many risk factors at once

61

Page 62: Biomathematics 170A Medical Statistics Jeff Gornbein Office:Life Science 5202 gornbein@ucla.edu 310-825-4193 Office hrs by appt – strongly encouraged 1

Case-control:disadvantages

Bias from sampling possibly two populations-not one population with or

without disease (where do we get appropriate controls?)

Does not establish temporal order

Recall bias

Survival bias

Can’t estimate incidence or prevalence

Case control is weakest design but easiest to do

62

Page 63: Biomathematics 170A Medical Statistics Jeff Gornbein Office:Life Science 5202 gornbein@ucla.edu 310-825-4193 Office hrs by appt – strongly encouraged 1

Exploratory vs ConfirmatoryExperiments & observational studies can be

classified as exploratory or confirmatory

Exploratory study -> hypothesis generating

(“fishing expedition”)

Liberal criteria ok for “significance”

Ex: Phase I and II trials

Confirmatory study->hypothesis validating

Need strict criteria for confirmation

Ex: Phase III and IV trials

63

Page 64: Biomathematics 170A Medical Statistics Jeff Gornbein Office:Life Science 5202 gornbein@ucla.edu 310-825-4193 Office hrs by appt – strongly encouraged 1

Controlling for confounding–stratification

I. False effect- A not really higher than B

Tx alive dead total

A 74 (74%) 26 (26%) 100

B 26 (26%) 74 (74%) 100

younger only

A 72 (90%) 8 (10%) 80

B 18 (90%) 2 (10%) 20

older only

A 2 (10%) 18 (90%) 20

B 8 (10%) 72 (90%) 80

64

Page 65: Biomathematics 170A Medical Statistics Jeff Gornbein Office:Life Science 5202 gornbein@ucla.edu 310-825-4193 Office hrs by appt – strongly encouraged 1

II Treatment efficacy obscured

(Simpson’s paradox- A is higher than B)

Tx alive dead total

A 50 (50%) 50 (50%) 100

B 50 (50%) 50 (50%) 100

younger only

A 30 (75%) 10 (25%) 40

B 48 (60%) 32 (40%) 80

older only

A 20 (33%) 40 (67%) 60

B 2 (10%) 18 (90%) 20

65

Page 66: Biomathematics 170A Medical Statistics Jeff Gornbein Office:Life Science 5202 gornbein@ucla.edu 310-825-4193 Office hrs by appt – strongly encouraged 1

III Interaction

Tx alive dead total

A 60 (60%) 40 (40%) 100

B 60 (60%) 40 (40%) 100

younger only- A is higher

A 54 (90%) 6 (10%) 60

B 36 (60%) 24 (40%) 60

older only – B is higher

A 6 (15%) 34 (85%) 40

B 24 (60%) 16 (40%) 40

66

Page 67: Biomathematics 170A Medical Statistics Jeff Gornbein Office:Life Science 5202 gornbein@ucla.edu 310-825-4193 Office hrs by appt – strongly encouraged 1

Statistical methods to control for confounding

Stratification (group matching)

Rate adjustment

Regression (linear, logistic, proportional hazard, ANOVA, Poisson…)

Propensity scores

This is needed when one can’t randomize or match/pair.

67

Page 68: Biomathematics 170A Medical Statistics Jeff Gornbein Office:Life Science 5202 gornbein@ucla.edu 310-825-4193 Office hrs by appt – strongly encouraged 1

Rate adjustment

UC Berkeley Admissions – 1973

(Friedman)

males females

Applied 2691 1835 (num app)

Admitted 1198 557

Percent 45% 30%

Admitted

Sexist?

68

Page 69: Biomathematics 170A Medical Statistics Jeff Gornbein Office:Life Science 5202 gornbein@ucla.edu 310-825-4193 Office hrs by appt – strongly encouraged 1

UC admission by major males femalesMajor num app % admit num app % admit A 825 62% 108 82% B 560 63% 25 68% C 325 37% 593 34% D 417 33% 375 35% E 191 28% 393 24% F 373 6% 341 7%Total 2691 45% 1835 30%

69

Page 70: Biomathematics 170A Medical Statistics Jeff Gornbein Office:Life Science 5202 gornbein@ucla.edu 310-825-4193 Office hrs by appt – strongly encouraged 1

Total num applicants to each major

Major male female M+F % of total %F

A 825 108 933 20.6% 11.6%

B 560 25 585 12.9% 4.3%

C 325 593 918 20.3% 64.6%

D 417 375 792 17.5% 47.3%

E 191 393 584 12.9% 67.3%

F 373 341 714 15.8% 47.8%

Total 2691 1835 4526 100.0% 40.5%70

Page 71: Biomathematics 170A Medical Statistics Jeff Gornbein Office:Life Science 5202 gornbein@ucla.edu 310-825-4193 Office hrs by appt – strongly encouraged 1

Adjusted (weighted) admission rates

Males

933x62%+585x63%+918x37%+792x33%+584x28%+714x6%

4526

= 39%

Females933x82%+585x68%+918x34%+792x35%+584x24%+714x7%

4526

= 43%

This is an example of adjustment over strata

71

Page 72: Biomathematics 170A Medical Statistics Jeff Gornbein Office:Life Science 5202 gornbein@ucla.edu 310-825-4193 Office hrs by appt – strongly encouraged 1

Outline for assessing an article in the Biomedical literature

(Colton: Statistics in Medicine)

I. Objectives

a. What is the goal or purpose of the study? What scientific hypothesis is being tested?

b. What is the target population – to whom do the investigators wish to apply the results? Who was included and excluded?

72

Page 73: Biomathematics 170A Medical Statistics Jeff Gornbein Office:Life Science 5202 gornbein@ucla.edu 310-825-4193 Office hrs by appt – strongly encouraged 1

II. Study design a. Is the study a planned experiment, quasi experiment or observational study?

b. What is the population from which the sample was selected?

c. How was the sample selected/participants chosen? Are their sources of bias? Are reasons for inclusion and exclusion of study subjects defined?

d. If the study was an experiment, were the subjects randomly assigned to treatment? Was the randomization scheme stated?

e. Was there an adequate control group?

f. Are the groups comparable at baseline? 

g. Was there a sample size calculation in the planning?73

Page 74: Biomathematics 170A Medical Statistics Jeff Gornbein Office:Life Science 5202 gornbein@ucla.edu 310-825-4193 Office hrs by appt – strongly encouraged 1

III. Observations

a. What are the outcome measures? Are they clearly defined?

b. What are the predictors and relevant covariates?

c. Are the measures reproducible (reliable) and understandable?

74

Page 75: Biomathematics 170A Medical Statistics Jeff Gornbein Office:Life Science 5202 gornbein@ucla.edu 310-825-4193 Office hrs by appt – strongly encouraged 1

IV. Analysis a. What statistical hypotheses are being tested? Is this

consistent with the goals in part I?

b. What type of analyses and statistical tests were performed? Are the calculations correct? Are the analysis methods consistent with the nature of the data?

c. What assumptions have been made about the data or design? Are they reasonable?

d. Have important, relevant factors and extraneous influences been accounted for in the analysis? Were confounding factors controlled?

e. Were the analysis results properly interpreted?

f. Were negative results distinguished from inconclusive results? Was the sample size large enough? 

75

Page 76: Biomathematics 170A Medical Statistics Jeff Gornbein Office:Life Science 5202 gornbein@ucla.edu 310-825-4193 Office hrs by appt – strongly encouraged 1

V. Presentation

a. Are the data and findings presented clearly? Is there sufficient detail to allow the reader to judge them?

b. Are the findings internally consistent? Do numbers add up and match in various tables and figures?

 

76

Page 77: Biomathematics 170A Medical Statistics Jeff Gornbein Office:Life Science 5202 gornbein@ucla.edu 310-825-4193 Office hrs by appt – strongly encouraged 1

VI. Conclusions

a. What conclusions do the investigators draw? Do they exceed the data presented?

b. Do the conclusions related to the goals of the study? Do they answer the study questions?

77

Page 78: Biomathematics 170A Medical Statistics Jeff Gornbein Office:Life Science 5202 gornbein@ucla.edu 310-825-4193 Office hrs by appt – strongly encouraged 1

VII. Redesign / reanalysis

If parts of the design or analysis are thought to be inadequate, how would you would redesign the study and/or reanalyze the data. Be practical. That is, recognize that there are financial, time and ethical limits to the types of studies that can be carried out.

78