12
This article was downloaded by: [New York University] On: 23 November 2014, At: 16:13 Publisher: Routledge Informa Ltd Registered in England and Wales Registered Number: 1072954 Registered office: Mortimer House, 37-41 Mortimer Street, London W1T 3JH, UK Theory & Research in Social Education Publication details, including instructions for authors and subscription information: http://www.tandfonline.com/loi/utrs20 A Social Perspective on Student Learning Ronald L. Vansickle a a University of Georgia , USA Published online: 09 Jul 2012. To cite this article: Ronald L. Vansickle (1982) A Social Perspective on Student Learning, Theory & Research in Social Education, 10:2, 21-31, DOI: 10.1080/00933104.1982.10505422 To link to this article: http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/00933104.1982.10505422 PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE Taylor & Francis makes every effort to ensure the accuracy of all the information (the “Content”) contained in the publications on our platform. However, Taylor & Francis, our agents, and our licensors make no representations or warranties whatsoever as to the accuracy, completeness, or suitability for any purpose of the Content. Any opinions and views expressed in this publication are the opinions and views of the authors, and are not the views of or endorsed by Taylor & Francis. The accuracy of the Content should not be relied upon and should be independently verified with primary sources of information. Taylor and Francis shall not be liable for any losses, actions, claims, proceedings, demands, costs, expenses, damages, and other liabilities whatsoever or howsoever caused arising directly or indirectly in connection with, in relation to or arising out of the use of the Content. This article may be used for research, teaching, and private study purposes. Any substantial or systematic reproduction, redistribution, reselling, loan, sub-licensing, systematic supply, or distribution in any form to anyone is expressly forbidden. Terms & Conditions of access and use can be found at http:// www.tandfonline.com/page/terms-and-conditions

A Social Perspective on Student Learning

Embed Size (px)

Citation preview

Page 1: A Social Perspective on Student Learning

This article was downloaded by: [New York University]On: 23 November 2014, At: 16:13Publisher: RoutledgeInforma Ltd Registered in England and Wales Registered Number: 1072954 Registered office: Mortimer House,37-41 Mortimer Street, London W1T 3JH, UK

Theory & Research in Social EducationPublication details, including instructions for authors and subscription information:http://www.tandfonline.com/loi/utrs20

A Social Perspective on Student LearningRonald L. Vansickle aa University of Georgia , USAPublished online: 09 Jul 2012.

To cite this article: Ronald L. Vansickle (1982) A Social Perspective on Student Learning, Theory & Research in SocialEducation, 10:2, 21-31, DOI: 10.1080/00933104.1982.10505422

To link to this article: http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/00933104.1982.10505422

PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE

Taylor & Francis makes every effort to ensure the accuracy of all the information (the “Content”) containedin the publications on our platform. However, Taylor & Francis, our agents, and our licensors make norepresentations or warranties whatsoever as to the accuracy, completeness, or suitability for any purpose of theContent. Any opinions and views expressed in this publication are the opinions and views of the authors, andare not the views of or endorsed by Taylor & Francis. The accuracy of the Content should not be relied upon andshould be independently verified with primary sources of information. Taylor and Francis shall not be liable forany losses, actions, claims, proceedings, demands, costs, expenses, damages, and other liabilities whatsoeveror howsoever caused arising directly or indirectly in connection with, in relation to or arising out of the use ofthe Content.

This article may be used for research, teaching, and private study purposes. Any substantial or systematicreproduction, redistribution, reselling, loan, sub-licensing, systematic supply, or distribution in anyform to anyone is expressly forbidden. Terms & Conditions of access and use can be found at http://www.tandfonline.com/page/terms-and-conditions

Page 2: A Social Perspective on Student Learning

Theory and Research in Social Education Summer, 1982. Volume X Number 2, pp. 21-31 a by The College and University Faculty Assembly of the National Council for the Social Studies

A Social Perspective on Student Learning Ronald L. VanSickle University of Georgia

Student learning is frequently viewed as a function of the interaction be- tween a learner and a task situation and the interaction between a learner and a teacher. However, these are only two perspectives. One's perspectives largely determine how one attempts to describe and explain a phenomenon.. The questions asked, the Concepts used to ask the questions, and the meth- ods and data viewed as appropriate for answering the questions are based on the perspectives. It is important to know how the assumptions under- lying these perspectives limit our understanding of learning in schooIs and what other perspectives are available to study learning.

Two Psychological Perspectives

The individual psychological perspective views learning as a function of a person's mental characteristics and/or behaviors interacting with a task situation. For example, Ausube1(1963) used the concept of cognitive struc- ture (i.e., the organization, stability, and clarity of knowledge) to explain meaningful verbal learning. Given this perspective a reasonable educational treatment is ta alter a learner's cognitive structure prior to a learning task by using general concepts and propositions and making explicit the internal logic and organization of verbal material (Joyce and Weil, 19'72). Another individual psychological view of learning is Kohlberg's theory of moral thought development through a sequence of intellectual stages (Galbraith and Jones, 1976). Given Kohlberg's interpretation, a learner must observe reasoning at one level higher than his or her current stage if development is

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

New

Yor

k U

nive

rsity

] at

16:

13 2

3 N

ovem

ber

2014

Page 3: A Social Perspective on Student Learning

t o be facilitated. Rot hkopf? attention to mat hemagenic behavior in learn- ing from prose focuses on individual response modes (e.g., note-taking, underlining) and the placement of questions in reading material (Faw and Waller, 1976). Skinner's use of operant conditioning is another well- known approach to changing individual learner behavior through rein forcement of selected behaviors (Anderson and Faust, 1973).

All of these approaches to understanding and facilitating learning share a common assumption: Learning is a function of individual mental or be- havioral factors. This might be generally true when a learner is working alone. When instructional prescriptions are made from this perspective, a teacher is assumed; however, the teacher is at the periphery of the scene. In much research, efforts are made to contra1 nuisance variables such as teach- ers, numerous student characteristics, and class effects by randomizing or removing them altogether. Applications of the results of such research have been less productive than anticipated because nonindividual factors p r o b ably overwhelm the individual psychologicall factors. Learning in school is generally a social psychological experience rather than an individual one.

The dyadic perspective views learning as a function of the interaction between a learner and a teacher, This perspective incorporates the inter- personal nature of most school learning situations. An example of this perspective is Flanders' study of teacher influence through interaction analysis (Dunkin and Biddle, 1974). Rosenshine and Furst's review of teacher effectiveness research (1 97 1) identified teacher clarity, enthusiasm, business-like behavior, and instruciional variability as promising variables for affecting student achievement. The efforts of the immense number of studies done from this perspective have not produced many clear prescrip- tions for effective teacher behavior.

Gage, dm al. (1976) conducted a major experimental study on teaching clarity (i.e., structuring, soliciting, and reacting) which illustrates the prob- lems of this perspective. Efforts were made to build on previous research, avoid earlier errors, and produce a definitive statement. The modest conclu- sions were: (1) low cognitive level soliciting increased achievement of both high and low cognitive objectives; (2) high structuring produced some in- crease in achievement; and (3) high reacting produced some increase in achievement. The treatment effects, student aptitudes, teacher effects, and aptitude treatment interactions together accounted for less than 50% of the variance In student performance. Gage, et. a/. concluded that one must either view teacher behavior studies as of questionable value or believe that greater degrees of research design conlplexity and statistical sophistication will reveal more powerful treatment effects in this area.

An assumption underlying all this work is that learning is a function of psychological factors manipulated by a teaches in a dyadic relationship. It is further assumed that the dyadic explanation will generalize t o typical class- room conditions in which a teacher attempts to work with 20 to 35 students

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

New

Yor

k U

nive

rsity

] at

16:

13 2

3 N

ovem

ber

2014

Page 4: A Social Perspective on Student Learning

simu1taneously. However, the explanations for student learning are concep- tualized in terns of a two-person tutoring interaction. Conceptually and empirically, the larger social environment is at the periphery of attention and is often viewed as a complex set of nuisance variables.

Unless school learning is viewed from a socially coherent perspective, the insights gained from psychologicaI and teacher effectiveness research can- not be utilized as productively as possible. School learning is generally learning in groups. Consequently, many students might not be participating in the instructional interaction of a classroom. Various psychological and teaches effectiveness instructional approaches probably fail or have modest effects because the treatments occur only sporadically, if at all, for many students. Classroom research on these approaches might be assessing in- structionaI non-events for many students, thus producing equivocal or neutral 'results. In addition, student-student effects are usualy not even considered. The two research perspectives discussed thus far do not concep- tualize school learning in an adequately comprehensive fashion.

A Social Perspective

A social perspective views learning as a function of relationships between individuals and groups within a classroom context. An individual student's academic motivation and expectations are shaped by social structures, sometimes overlapping, composed of status systems and their associated r e ward structures and roles. The characteristics and relationships of students as a group or groups in a classroom can structure the quantity and quality of teacher-student, student-student, and student-task interaction. Research- ers have commonly studied characteristics of individuals, such as aptitudes and social class background, in order to predict and explain greater or lesser achievements in schools. Consequently, problems in schools have been interpreted as failures to help particular categories of students (Cohen, 1W2b). Less commonIy studied are social variables which characterize no particular student but do meaningfully describe a number of people simul- taneously.

Educational researchers should explore the implications of social vari- ables for individuaI student achievement in order to understand more ade- quately student learning in schools. A set of social variables clusters around the concept of status. Status refers to a criterion used by group members to rank other members from better to worse according to their possession or lack of possession of a particular quality, such as a desirable race, sex, or academic or socia1 skill. Reward structures (i.e,, the system of distributing valued goods, services, or approvd) affect the interaction of teachers and students. Students can be important participants in the distribution of re- wards in a classroom. Other group variables include cohesion, norms, au- thority, and roles (Schmuck and Schmuck, 1975). Numerous possibilities

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

New

Yor

k U

nive

rsity

] at

16:

13 2

3 N

ovem

ber

2014

Page 5: A Social Perspective on Student Learning

exist for analyzing classroom learning groups in terms of these and other sociological concepts.

A social perspective assumes that much learning in schools occurs in group contexts. The beliefs and evaluations of other students as we11 as those of the teacher can affect how much a student participates in learning activities and how much effort he or she will expend to complete an assign- ment on an individual basis. Instructional prescriptions resulting from this perspective differ from those described earlier. Instead of attempting di- rectly to change individual students' mental or behavioral characteristics, the classroom group is treated. Perhaps a status system which discourages active involvement by minority students must be Atered or neutralized. Per- haps a reward system must be modified to make rewards distributed by the teacher and students more accessible to students who have experienced little satisfying response to their efforts.

In summary, the three perspectives focus attention on different aspects of learning and teaching. The individual psychological perspective emphasizes students' internal mental characteristics andtor individual behaviors which facilitate learning. The dyadic perspective emphasizes teacher behavior and the interaction between a student and a teacher. The social perspective em- phasizes relationships between individual and groups within a classroom context and the implications of those relationships for individual students' efforts and learning. The three perspectives are complementary and should dl be used for an adequate understanding of most instructional situations. Specid consideration is given here to the social perspective since it is under- utilized frequently.

In order to illustrate the social perspective, two Iines of inquiry will be de- scribed briefly, One line, based on status characteristics and expectation states theory (Berger, Conner, and Fisek, 1974), has been largely directed or influenced by Elizabeth G. Cohen at the Center for Educational Research at Stanford University. The other line, based on the concepts of team learning and reward structures, has been carried out largely through the Student Team Learning Project of the Center for the Social Organization of Schools at Johns Hopkins University. Both have produced important research find- ings and both hold significant implications for research on school learning.

Status Chamcteristics and Expectation States

Status characteristics and expectation states theory is a complex, refined theory. The following is a brief overview. A general status characteristic (e,g., race) becomes socially significant in a collective task situation when it enables group members to decide whose contributions are likely to be most helpful in accomplishing the group task. A general status characteristic has a general performance expectation associated with it. Consequently, group members with a positive ranking on the status characteristic will be ex- pected, in the absence of contradictory information, to have more of what-

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

New

Yor

k U

nive

rsity

] at

16:

13 2

3 N

ovem

ber

2014

Page 6: A Social Perspective on Student Learning

ever specific ability is required to aceompIish the group's task. In this so- ciety, for example, white students are likely to be viewed by other students as generally more competent than black students at an academic task. In group work, the higher status students are likely to make more suggestions, criticize others' ideas more frequently, receive less criticism, and have their suggestions accepted more frequently than lower status students (Cohen, 1972a). This generalization has been repeatedly observed in various status situations for black and white students (Cohen, 1972a; Cohen, 1973; Cohen & Roper, 1972; Lohrnan, 1972; Cohen, Lockheed & Lohman, 1976), Ea t - ern and Western Israelis (Cohen & Sharen, 19771, females and males (Lock- heed & Hall, 19761, and average and above average readers (Stulac, 1976; Morris, 1977).

Cohen and her associates have developed approaches to neutralizing the negative effects of student status systems through a program of natural ob- servation, highly controlled laboratory experiments, less contrslIed experi- mental educational programs, and, most recently, field experiments in con- ventional, desegregated schooIs. Most of the successful efforts to neutralize negative status effects in educational settings had a common feature. The low status students were given the opportunity to demonstrate competent behavior to the high status students. .Usually, arrangements were made for the low status students to demonstrate competence superior to the high status students. This was often accomplshed by having the low status stu- dents teach the high status students to perform tasks the latter students valued. A general status characteristic (e.g., race, reading ability) was neu- tralized by generating a relevant specific status characteristic (e.g., gaming ability, language ability, radio building skill) which ranked the students in reverse order to the general characteristic (Vansickle, 1979). Both high and low status s'tudents observed that the low status students could perform competentIy in the classroom context. This treatment producd equal-status or nearly equal-status participation (e.g., initiation rates, response rates, in- fluence rates) between the high and low status groups of students.

Recent studies. in the classrooms of desegregated schools have added a new degree of complexity to this line of inquiry. Rosenholtz (1979) neu- tralized a status system based on reading ability by implementing a multiple abilities curriculum. By convincing students that reading skill did not ac- curately predict performance on many academic tasks, high and Iow reading status students moved toward equal-status interaction. Rosenholtz focused the curriculum and accompanying evaluation system on visual ability, in- tuitive thinking, and reasoning. Cohen (1979) attempted to use the multiple abilities curriculum in the commonly encountered situation in which race and academic status systems both exist. The results were much more i im- plex since an uncontroIled social power status system also existed in' the classroams which was more powerful than the experimental treatm&i.. While treatment effects could be observed, they were very modest. Much

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

New

Yor

k U

nive

rsity

] at

16:

13 2

3 N

ovem

ber

2014

Page 7: A Social Perspective on Student Learning

theoretical and empirical work needs to be done, particularly in relating participation and cognitive learning, but the educational significance of this line of inquiry is clear.

Reward Structures

The Student Team Learning Project has explored the academic and social effects of cooperative learning efforts and group and individual reward structures. Three techniques have been studied: (1) Teams-Games-Touma- ment ; (2) Student Teams-Achievement Divisions; and (3) Jigsaw. The Teams-Games-Tournament (TGT) approach groups students in mixed abil- ity teams of four or five students who help each other achieve instructional objectives, Representatives of each team compete in three-person, equal ability tournaments. Each team member contributes his tournament score t o the total team score, Additional rewards are made to those teams with high total performance. In a review of ten experiments comparing TGT to traditional instruction, DeVries and Slavin (1978) reported generally positive effects on academic achievement, interpersonal concern, race rela- tions, and peer norms helpful in acadernic achievement.

The second technique, Student Teams- Achievement Divisions (STAD), is similar t o TGT except that the tournaments are replaced with individual quizzes with the scores totaled to produce team scores. Slavin (1978) re- viewed four experiments that compared STAD to traditional instruction and reported that STAD increased cross-racial attraction and interaction, liking for others, feelings of being liked, and peer norms for academic achievement. Academic achievement effects were less clear; two studies re- ported no difference, one indicated a large positive effect on black students' achievement, and one showed a positive effect on a treatment-specific achievement measure. It was determined that group rewards and their effect on achievement norms were more important for learning than peer-tutoring.

The Jigsaw method, the third technique, differs in that cooperative effort and individual rewards are used. Five or six students, who are each respon- sible for a segment of a Iesson, are grouped. Students from different groups responsible for a given segment meet and check each other's understanding of their common assignment. Next, each student teaches his or her segment t o the other group members. Each student in the group is dependent on all the others t o help him or her combine the pieces of the lesson into a whole. Unlike the other techniques, students are evaluated and rewarded individ- ually. Aronson, Bridgeman, and Geffner (1 978) reviewed several field studies related to the Jigsaw method and concluded that experimental sub- jects demonstrated increases in self-esteem, liking for classmates, and lik- ing for school. Also, minority students and low-achieving white students achieved more than students receiving conventional instruction while high- achieving whites performed as usual.

The Student Team Learning Project has been based on a variety of the-

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

New

Yor

k U

nive

rsity

] at

16:

13 2

3 N

ovem

ber

2014

Page 8: A Social Perspective on Student Learning

oretical and empirical inquiries. Consequently, its theoretical sophistication is less than that of the status characteristics and expectation states approach and the explanations for the effects of the cooperative learning techniques are still somewhat unclear. Slavin (1977) identified a critical component of these techniques which represented an advance over earlier work comparing cooperative and competitive techniques. He observed that students must have important resources which they can choose to share or withhold. Otherwise, individual reward structures are more effective than cooperative structures for increasing achievement although the social and attitudinal benefits are largely lost.

The two lines of inquiry described here developed independently. How- ever, there are similarities in effects that theoretical analysis might explain, Understanding the effects of social variables, such as staters and reward structures, might be increased through such efforts.

Implications for Soda1 Studies Educational Research

Research on student learning in schools from a social perspective has sev- eral implications for social studies education. First, social studies teachers usually instruct classes of students with wide ranges of academic ability and achievement. This great diversity often makes individualizing instruction impractical. Methods developed from a social perspective, such as the stu- dent team learning techniques described earlier, can reorganize relationships among students in a feasibIe way which promotes academic achievement. Research on the instructional effects of team learning in Iow achieving so- cial studies classes indicates that positive achievement effects will probably result in homogeneously grouped classes as wd'I (Allen and Vansickle, in press). Much more research and development is possible with a focus on altering peer norms and expectations for high achievement.

Ssond, decision-making in groups is a prime domain for the appIication of a social research perspective. Widespread student participation is es- pecially important in light of research on smalI group decision-making, Groups are more likely to make correct decisions when decisions are arrived at by consensus or by a decision-maker who uses the advice of others than when one person decides alone. "Correct decisions," in this case, refer to judgments of accuracy, effectiveness, or efficiency; they do not refer to moral judgments. Often the group decision will be more adequate than the decision of any individual group member (Piper, 1974).

Fraenkel(198 13 suggested that group dismssions of moral issues can pos- sibly lead to personal behavior more consistent with an individual's moral thought if individual decisions rather than group decisions are required. A variety of cooperative group activities which involve individual account- ability could give students greater self-confidence in their beliefs while en- abling them to benefit from others'hdp and insights. Fraenkel cited several social psychological studies that make his proposals promising; he reom-

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

New

Yor

k U

nive

rsity

] at

16:

13 2

3 N

ovem

ber

2014

Page 9: A Social Perspective on Student Learning

mended that ducationa1 researchers and developers explore the possibili- ties. Instructional research from a social perspective is in order.

A third implication involves the goal of many social educators to teach students to participate effectively in discussions by focusing on systematic discussion skills (Newmann and Oliver, 1970). Among the discussion be- haviors which are probably sensitive to social conditions, such as status di f- ferences, are: stating issues; challenging inconsistency; and making personal attacks. Also, Morris (1977) observed that the frequencies of requesting and offering reasons in group decision-making were related to perceived reading ability status differences even though the subjects were objectively equal. Social research could contribute to making discussion and decision-making goals more feasible to attain for students and teachers.

A fourth implication is the effect of social variables on teacher behavior in teacher-Ied discussions. In a research review, Smith (1979) noted that teachers tend to select a small number of students in a class to answer ques- tions. The selection criteria are often student characteristics which form status hierarchies, such as race, social class, physical attractiveness, and, especially, academic ability. In an experimental study which compared g r s dictable and unpredictable teacher solicitation patterns, Smith (1980) o b served that student examination of stimulus material and concentration on the substance of a discussion were greater under the unpredictable solicita- tion condition. This finding leads to the hypothesis that teachers whose solicitation patterns are predictable strengthen status systems among stu- dents that produce negative instructional outcomes. Research is needed to investigate this hypothesis and to identify ways teachers can neutralize neg- ative status effects in group discussions.

Fifth, some educators recommend curriculum goals which include coc operative group action and analysis of group organization and performance in the school and the community (Gillespie and Lazarus, 1976, 1979). For such instructional programs to be effective, social conditions which militate against social participation must be treated. Further, students must also un- derstand these troublesome social conditions if learning is to transfer be- yond specially treated, equal-status classroom conditions. Available r s search on these topics is virtually nonexistent.

Sixth, simulation gaming provides a feasible means of manipulating class- room social variables, Assigning students to simulation roles with statuses inconsistent with their actual statuses can produce observable changes in student interaction, There is very little research that has attempted to mea- sure the nature or strength of the impact of simulation gaming on student status systems, norms, expectations, and interaction. In a study of simula- tion game design characteristics, Vansickle (1977) observed that the distri- bution of game resources affected the satisfaction students experienced in working with their fellow group members. Interest in the real-life analogue of the simulation game was positively affected by a moderate level of small

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

New

Yor

k U

nive

rsity

] at

16:

13 2

3 N

ovem

ber

2014

Page 10: A Social Perspective on Student Learning

group integration rather than high or low levels. These findings support the possibility that appropriately designed simulation games could be a means of altering classroom social structural variables.

Seventh, an important god for many social educators is to provide stu- dents with opportunities to develop skills of engaging in effective interper- sonal and interethnic group interaction (Banks, CortCs, Gay, Garcia, and Ochoa, 1976). Problems of status, performance expectations, and norms tend to make group intefaction mirror the negative features of intergroup relations In the larger society. Close proximity does not guarantee positive relationships. The two lines of inquiry described in this paper involving stu- dent statuses and reward structures show that classroom social conditions can be created which break down stereotypes and actualize the values of human 'dignity and the worth of the individual. Enough research has been done to support instructional development efforts aimed at producing, feas- ible school programs.

These implications cover a broad range of questions and problems which concern social studies educators, although they do not exhaust the possi- bilities. Social studies professionals need to incorporate a sophisticated social understanding of school learning into their instruction. By virtue of their social scientific and historical orientations, they should be especially well-prepared to understand and use a social perspective.

InstructionaI programs based on psychologically oriented research and development are more likely to demonstrate benefits if social condidons are arranged to support them. Due to the individual and dyadic perspectives underlying most school learning research and the tendency of educators to interpret student behavior as a function of individual characteristics, social research on learning is an underdeveloped field (SchIechty, 1976). Oppor- tunities for research and development are numerous in exploring the social aspects of school learning and identifying the social conditions under which psychologically ba.sed instructional procedures can operate effectively.

References

Alen, W. H, and VanSickle, R. L. Instructional effects of learning teams for low achieyhg students. Social Edumtion, in press.

Anderson, C. and Faust, W. E$ucationaZpsychoIogy: Tke science of instmction and learning. New York: Dodd, Mead and Company, 1973.

Aronson, E., Bridgeman, D., and Geffner. R, Interdependent interactions and prosocial behavior. Journal of Research and Development in Education, 1978, 12{1), 16-27.

Ausubel, D. P. The psychology of meaningful wrba! learning. New York: Grune and Stratton, 1963. '

Banks, J. A., CortQ, E., Gay, G,, Garcia, L., and Ochoa, S, Curriculum guidelines

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

New

Yor

k U

nive

rsity

] at

16:

13 2

3 N

ovem

ber

2014

Page 11: A Social Perspective on Student Learning

for rnulfierhnic education. Washington, D.C. : National Council for the Social Studies, 1976.

Berger, J., Conner, T. L., and Fisek, M. H. Expectation states theory: A theoretical research ptwgmm. Cambridge, Massachusetts: Winthrop Publishers, Inc., 1974.

Cohen, E. G., Lockheed, E., and Lohman, R. The Center for Interracial Coopera- tion: A field experiment. Sociology of Education, 1976, 49111, 47-58.

Cohen, E. G. Interracial interaction disability. Human Relufions, 1972a, 25(1), 9-24.

Cohen, E. G. and Roper. S. Modification of interracial interaction disability: An application of status characteristic theory. American Sociological Review, 1972, 37(6$, 643-657.

Cohen, E. G. and Sharen, S. Modifying status relations in Israe/i youth. Paper p r e sented at the meeting of the American Educational Research Association, New York, ApriI, 1977.

Cohen, E. G. Modifying the effects of social structure. American Behavioral Scien- fist, 1973, 16(6), 861-878.

Cohen, E. G. Sociology and the classroom: Setting the conditions for teacher- student interaction. Review of Educational Research, 1972b, 42(4), 441-452.

Cohen, E. G. S tam equalization in the desegregated school. Paper presented at the meeting of the American Educational Research Association, San Francisco, April, 1979.

DeVries, D. L. and Edwards, K. J. Teams-games-tournaments: Review of ten class- room experiments. Journal of Research and Development in Education, 1 978, 12(1), 23-38.

Faw, H. W. and Waller, T. Mathemagenic behaviors and efficiency in learning from prose materials: Review, critique, and recornmendatiens. Review of Educational Researrh, 1976, 46(4), 691-720,

Fraenkel, J. R. The relationship between moral thought and moral action: Implica- tions for soci a1 studies education. Theory and Research in Social Education, 9(2): 39-54, 1981.

Gage, N. L., Clark, C. M., Marx, R. W., Peterson, P. L., Stayrook, N. G., and Winne, P. H. A factorially designed experiment on teacher structuring, solicititrg, and reacting. Stanford, California: Stanford Center for Research and Develop- ment in Teaching, Memorandum No. 147, 1976.

Galbrai th, R. E. and Jones, T. M. Moral rmsoning: A teaching handbook for adapr- ing Kohlberg to the clmsmom. Minneapolis: Greenhaven Press, Inc., I 976.

Gi llespie, J. and Lazarus, S. American government: Comparing poIitical experi- ences. Englewood Cliffs, New Jersey: Prentice-Hall Inc., 1979.

Gillespie, J. and Lazarus, S. Teaching political participation skills. Social Educo- tion, 1976, 40(63, 373-378.

Joyce, l3. and Weil, M. Models of teaching. Englewood Cliffs, New Jersey: Prentice- Hall, Inc. 1972.

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

New

Yor

k U

nive

rsity

] at

16:

13 2

3 N

ovem

ber

2014

Page 12: A Social Perspective on Student Learning

Lockheed, M. E. and Hall, K. P. Conceptualizing sex as a status characteristic: A p plications to leadership training strategies. Journal of Social Iwes , 1976, 32(3), 111-124.

Lohman, M, R. Changing a racial status ordering: Some implications for integration efforts. Education and Urban Society, 1972, 4(4), 383-402.

Morris, R. A. Equalizing participation between status groups through a normative intervention. Paper presented at the meeting of the American Educational Re- search Association, New York, April, 1977.

Newrnann, F. M. and Oliver, D. W. Clarvying public controversy. Boston: Little, Brown and Company, 1970.

Piper, D. L. Decision-making: Decisions made by individual vs. those made by group consensus or group participation, Edtlcalional Administration Quarterly, 1974, 10(2), 8 2 9 5 .

Rosenholtz, S. J. Tseating problems of academic stafus. Paper presented at the meeting of the American Educational Research Association, Sm Francisco, 1979.

Rosenshine, B. and Furst, N. Research in teacher performance criteria. In B. 0. Smith (Ed.), Research in teacher education: A symposium. Englewood Cliffs, New Jersey: Prentice-Hall, Inc., 1971.

Schlechty, P. C. Teaching and social behavior: To ward an organizational t heou of instsuction, Boston: Allyn and Bacon, Inc., 1976.

Schmuck, R. A. and Schmuck, P. A. Group processes in the clwroorn. Dubuque, Iowa: Wm. 6. Brown Company Publishers, 1975.

Slavin, R. E. Classroom reward structure: An analytical and practical review. Re- view of Educational Resetzrch, 1977, 47(4), 633-650.

Slavin, R. Student teams and achievement divisions. Journal of Rmeamh and iDevel- opment in Education, 1978 12(1), 39-49,

Smith, B. B. Influence of solicitation pattern, type of practice example, and student response on pupil behavior, commitment to discussion, and concept attainment. Theory and Research in Socia! Educaf ion, 7(4): 1-1 7, 1980.

Smith, B. D. Questioning to involve students actively. Georgia Social Science Jour- nal, 10(1): 4-8, 1999.

Stulac, J. Tke Sew fulfilling Prophecy Wkat you think h what you get. Paper pre- sented at the meeting of the American Educational Research Association, San Francisco, April, 1976.

VanSickle, R. L. Neutralizing status constraints on student performance in mall group activities. Theory and Rarearch in Social Bducution, 1 979, 7(2), 1-33.

VanSickle, R. L. ~esource independence and decision-making procedure specificity in simulation games. Journal of Experimental Educalion, &(I): 47-55, 1977.

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

New

Yor

k U

nive

rsity

] at

16:

13 2

3 N

ovem

ber

2014