22
r Academy of Management Perspectives 2017, Vol. 31, No. 4, 309330. https://doi.org/10.5465/amp.2016.0166 A R T I C L E S ITS ABOUT TIME: NEW PERSPECTIVES AND INSIGHTS ON TIME MANAGEMENT BRAD AEON Concordia University HERMAN AGUINIS George Washington University Time management has helped people organize their professional lives for centuries. The existing literature, however, reveals mixed findings and lack of clarity as to whether, when, how, and why time management leads to critical outcomes such as well-being and job performance. Furthermore, insights relevant to time management are scattered across various disciplines, including sociology, psychology, and behavioral economics. We address both issues by synthesizing and integrating insightful elements from various fields and domains into three novel perspectives on time management. First, we draw on the sociology of time to describe two key concepts: time structures and time norms. We illustrate how time structures and time norms operate at the team, organizational, and national levels of analysis in influencing time management outcomes. Second, we draw on the psychology of time to show how individual differences including time-related beliefs, attitudes, and preferences affect the way people manage time and, consequently, time management outcomes. Third, we rely on the behavioral economics literature to describe how cognitive biases influence individual time management decisions. In- tegrating insights from a diverse set of fields results in a better understanding of past research and allows us to reinterpret conflicting results prevalent in the time manage- ment literature. Finally, we offer directions for future research and discuss implications for how organizations and individuals can implement interventions resulting in a stron- ger and positive relationship between time management and desirable outcomes. The Roman philosopher Seneca (50/2014, p. 118) lamented that people trifle with time, because time is an immaterial thing that doesnt appear to the eyes, and for that reason its valued very cheaply.Two thousand years later, people still have neither the necessary economic sophistication nor the percep- tual apparatus to account for time in the same way as they account for money(Soman, 2001, p. 171). Systematically accounting for time, or what is commonly referred to as time management, has been studied by numerous disciplines. For example, in sociology, researchers have examined the effect of managing ones time on social coordination among people (Southerton, 2003). A typical approach adopted by sociologists is Giddenss (1984) struc- turation theory, which posits that people are simul- taneously constrained by but also shape socially constructed schedules (e.g., Flaherty, 2011). In de- velopmental psychology, researchers have looked at how family stability in early childhood later in- fluences adultstime management (Malatras, Israel, Sokolowski, & Ryan, 2016). Researchers in de- velopmental and other psychology subfields usually study time management through the lens of time- related constructs such as discounted utility (e.g., onig & Kleinmann, 2007) and procrastination (e.g., Ariely & Wertenbroch, 2002; Van Eerde, 2015). In history, some have argued that the industrial age came about not because of the steam engine, but be- cause of our increasing willingness to abide by our own schedules (Mumford, 2010; Thompson, 1967). Historians often use a power perspective to frame We thank Mike Wright and two Academy of Manage- ment Perspectives anonymous reviewers for constructive comments on previous drafts. 309 Copyright of the Academy of Management, all rights reserved. Contents may not be copied, emailed, posted to a listserv, or otherwise transmitted without the copyright holders express written permission. Users may print, download, or email articles for individual use only.

2017, Vol. 31, No. 4, 309 330

  • Upload
    others

  • View
    4

  • Download
    0

Embed Size (px)

Citation preview

Page 1: 2017, Vol. 31, No. 4, 309 330

r Academy of Management Perspectives2017, Vol. 31, No. 4, 309–330.https://doi.org/10.5465/amp.2016.0166

A R T I C L E S

IT’S ABOUT TIME: NEW PERSPECTIVES AND INSIGHTSON TIME MANAGEMENT

BRAD AEONConcordia University

HERMAN AGUINISGeorge Washington University

Timemanagement has helped people organize their professional lives for centuries. Theexisting literature, however, reveals mixed findings and lack of clarity as to whether,when, how, andwhy timemanagement leads to critical outcomes such as well-being andjob performance. Furthermore, insights relevant to time management are scatteredacross various disciplines, including sociology, psychology, and behavioral economics.We address both issues by synthesizing and integrating insightful elements from variousfields and domains into three novel perspectives on timemanagement. First, we draw onthe sociology of time to describe two key concepts: time structures and time norms. Weillustrate how time structures and time norms operate at the team, organizational, andnational levels of analysis in influencing time management outcomes. Second, we drawon the psychology of time to show how individual differences including time-relatedbeliefs, attitudes, and preferences affect the way people manage time and, consequently,time management outcomes. Third, we rely on the behavioral economics literature todescribe how cognitive biases influence individual time management decisions. In-tegrating insights from a diverse set of fields results in a better understanding of pastresearch and allows us to reinterpret conflicting results prevalent in the time manage-ment literature. Finally, we offer directions for future research and discuss implicationsfor how organizations and individuals can implement interventions resulting in a stron-ger and positive relationship between time management and desirable outcomes.

The Roman philosopher Seneca (50/2014, p. 118)lamented that people triflewith time, because time is“an immaterial thing that doesn’t appear to the eyes,and for that reason it’s valued very cheaply.” Twothousand years later, people still have “neither thenecessary economic sophistication nor the percep-tual apparatus to account for time in the same wayas they account for money” (Soman, 2001, p. 171).

Systematically accounting for time, or what iscommonly referred to as timemanagement, has beenstudied by numerous disciplines. For example, insociology, researchers have examined the effect ofmanaging one’s time on social coordination amongpeople (Southerton, 2003). A typical approach

adopted by sociologists is Giddens’s (1984) struc-turation theory, which posits that people are simul-taneously constrained by but also shape sociallyconstructed schedules (e.g., Flaherty, 2011). In de-velopmental psychology, researchers have looked athow family stability in early childhood later in-fluences adults’ time management (Malatras, Israel,Sokolowski, & Ryan, 2016). Researchers in de-velopmental and other psychology subfields usuallystudy time management through the lens of time-related constructs such as discounted utility (e.g.,Konig & Kleinmann, 2007) and procrastination(e.g., Ariely &Wertenbroch, 2002; Van Eerde, 2015).In history, some have argued that the industrial agecame about not because of the steam engine, but be-cause of our increasing willingness to abide by ourown schedules (Mumford, 2010; Thompson, 1967).Historians often use a power perspective to frame

We thank Mike Wright and two Academy of Manage-ment Perspectives anonymous reviewers for constructivecomments on previous drafts.

309

Copyright of the Academy of Management, all rights reserved. Contents may not be copied, emailed, posted to a listserv, or otherwise transmitted without the copyright holder’s expresswritten permission. Users may print, download, or email articles for individual use only.

Page 2: 2017, Vol. 31, No. 4, 309 330

time systems (e.g., calendars, work hours) as a locusof struggle andnegotiation between opposing groups(e.g., Martineau, 2015).

In the particular case of the field of management,Peter Drucker (1967, p. 22) wrote, “Everything re-quires time. It is the one truly universal condition.All work takes place in time and uses up time.” Un-surprisingly, then, time management plays an im-portant role in numerous subdomains such aswork–life conflict (Adams & Jex, 1999), job perfor-mance (Barling, Cheung, & Kelloway, 1996), cross-cultural management (Nonis, Teng, & Ford, 2005),stress (Hafner, Stock, Pinneker, & Strohle, 2014),creativity (Zampetakis, Bouranta, & Moustakis,2010), life satisfaction (Macan, Shahani, Dipboye, &Phillips, 1990), and even unemployment (Wanberg,Griffiths, & Gavin, 1997).

The description above shows that key theoreticaland empirical insights that can enhance our un-derstanding of time management are dispersedacross many disciplines, including sociology(Flaherty, 2003), psychology (Burt & Kemp, 1994),childhood education (Liu, Rijmen, MacCann, &Roberts, 2009), management (Claessens, VanEerde, Rutte, & Roe, 2004), and consumer behav-ior (Feldman & Hornik, 1981), to name a few. Thescattered state of the literature hinders progressbecause insights from some disciplines and do-mains are seldom taken into account by others. Forinstance, Britton and Tesser (1991) studied timemanagement from an educational psychology per-spective, and according to Web of Science over 91%of this paper’s citationswere linked topsychology andeducation outlets; only 2% were made by sociology-focused publications. Likewise, Zerubavel’s (1979b)sociology-based paper on time garnered over 61% ofits total citations fromsociologyoutlets andamere5%from psychology-oriented outlets. Furthermore, aswe describe below, the time management literaturereveals conflicting findings as to whether time man-agement leads to critical and highly desirable out-comes suchasenhancedwell-being and improved jobperformance.

Given these issues, the goal of our article is tosynthesize and integrate theoretical and empiricalinsights relevant to time management from multipledisciplines anddomains in away that is accessible toa nonspecialist audience as well as scholars insideand outside the field of management. This inte-gration of diverse theories and advances based onempirical evidence provides a framework that in-forms timemanagement in newways, allowing us toreinterpret and better understand variations and

conflicting findings in past studies and also guidefuture conceptual and empirical research.

Our article is organized as follows. First, we definetime management and dispel some common myths.We show that, for instance, contrary to popular be-lief, time management is no fad from the 1970s;rather, time management has been of interest tophilosophers, businesspeople, and politicians forcenturies. Second,we review the evidence regardingthe relationship between time management and twocritical and widely examined outcomes in manage-ment and other fields: well-being and performance.Results exhibit much variation, are often contradic-tory, and reveal a murky literature in need of clari-fication. Third, we integrate dispersed insights frommultiple disciplines to offer three novel perspectivesthat advance our understanding of time manage-ment. In particular, we focus on time structures andnorms, two key concepts from the sociology of timethat help us understand how environmental factorsinfluence time management. Then we discuss time-related individual differences—key concepts fromthe psychology of time that heavily influence peo-ple’s temporal behaviors. We subsequently proceedto examine how temporal decision making, a sub-field of behavioral economics that is also informedby psychology and social cognition, sheds light onthe underlying dynamics of time management.

Fourth, using our framework based on our in-tegration of insights from these multiple disciplines,wemake sense of seemingly conflicting findings. Forinstance, our perspective on time structures andnorms shows how employees who exhibit stellartime management might not necessarily receiveexcellent performance appraisals in organizationswhere time norms prioritize long work hours and“face time” rather than actual efficiency. As a result,studies that fail to take into account organizations’time structures and norms produce inconsistentfindings on whether time management does, in fact,have a positive effect on job performance. Similarly,our time-related individual differences perspectiveshows that some people simply are less likely tobenefit from time management training than others.Thus, failing to consider individual differencescompromises the validity of statements regardingtime management training effectiveness and its var-ious outcomes.

What’s more, our temporal decision-making per-spective shows that some by-products of time man-agement training (e.g., the temporal sunk-cost effect)might actually be detrimental to job performance. Insum, our framework based on diverse disciplines

310 NovemberAcademy of Management Perspectives

Page 3: 2017, Vol. 31, No. 4, 309 330

and management domains, with a multilevel per-spective, allows us to gain new perspectives andinsights regarding the meaning of time managementas well as whether, when, how, and why time man-agement leads to critical outcomes such as well-being and job performance. Finally, we conclude byoffering directions for future research on time man-agement as well as practical implications for orga-nizations and individuals.

TIME MANAGEMENT: DEFINITIONAND OUTCOMES

Timemanagement predates themodernGregoriancalendar and the mechanical clock. As mentionedabove, scholars and laypeople alike have reflectedfor centuries on how to best use time (e.g., Alberti,1444/1971; Aurelius, 167/1949; Bennett, 1910;Franklin, 1757/1964; Penn, 1794; Seneca, 50/2014;St. Benedict, 530/1975), a fact that attests to the pe-rennial pervasiveness of timemanagement. There is,however, no widely established definition of timemanagement (Claessens et al., 2007). Some define itas a “combination of time assessment, goal setting,planning, and monitoring activities” (Hafner &Stock, 2010, p. 430) or a “self-controlled attempt touse time in a subjectively efficient way to achieveoutcomes” (Koch & Kleinmann, 2002, p. 201), whileothers do away with the need to define time man-agement altogether (e.g., Barling, Cheung, & Kelloway,1996; Trueman & Hartley, 1996).

The problem of defining time management iscompounded by the fact that different disciplineshave slightly different takes on what time manage-mentmeans. In sociology, for instance, the emphasismight be on the structure of personal time, whereasin psychology the emphasismight be on the ability tostick to plans and make accurate estimates of howlong a task will take. For this reason, we need a defi-nition that subsumes, integrates, and applies toa wide range of disciplines. We adopt a person-centered perspective in which we conceptualizeindividuals as proactive and intentional agents(Aguinis &Glavas, in press; Rupp, 2011). In linewiththis perspective, we contend that individuals makedecisions about how they allocate time. Accord-ingly, we define time management as a form of de-cision making used by individuals to structure,protect, and adapt their time to changing conditions.This definition is consistent with an agentic per-spective of time (Granqvist & Gustafsson, 2016).

Indeed, calendars, schedules, holidays, semesters,clock time, and weekends are not “brute physical

facts” (Searle, 1995); rather, they are social con-structions subject to change and negotiation (Berger& Luckmann, 1966; Zerubavel, 1981). At the indi-vidual level of analysis, people are arguably free toorganize their time as they see fit (Zerubavel, 1976)by drawing on existing time models (Orlikowski &Yates, 2002) or creating their own unique timestructures (Flaherty, 2003; Kreiner, Hollensbe, &Sheep, 2009). In fact, sociological research suggeststhat even when people complain of having littlecontrol over their time, the reality is that they oftendo, but prefer to absolve themselves of responsibilityby denying their ability tomanage it (Flaherty, 2011).

We conducted a selective review of the literatureby searching for “time management” in EBSCO’sBusiness Source Complete database. We also gath-ered additional sources by identifying papers thatused time management measures (e.g., Britton &Tesser, 1991; Macan et al., 1990) and articles fea-tured in Claessens et al.’s (2007) overview of the lit-erature. Virtually all of the studies focused on twomain outcomes of timemanagement: well-being andperformance. This emphasis is not surprising be-cause these are key outcomes for individuals andorganizations and the focus of most managementtheories as well as the target of many interventionsand practices (Aguinis, Davis, et al., 2016; Grant,Christianson, & Price, 2007; Salas, Kozlowski, &Chen, 2017). We divided the articles we gathered inour search into two groups of 20 empirical articles,one about well-being and the other about job per-formance (see Tables 1 and 2, below). In the follow-ing sections,we assess how timemanagement affectsthese two outcomes.Our intent is not to cover the fullbreadth of the literature. Rather, because our paper’sgoal is to integrate and offer new perspectives andinsights, we focus on the core conclusions of theliterature.

Time Management and Well-Being

Table 1 includes summaries of studies that exam-ined the relationship between timemanagement andwell-being.Well-being is the experience of “pleasantemotions, low levels of negativemoods, and high lifesatisfaction” (Diener, Lucas, & Oishi, 2002, p. 63).Bond and Feather (1988) were among the first tostudy the impact of time management—using theirTime Structure Questionnaire, which measures thedegree to which people’s time is structured andpurposeful—on various facets of well-being. Theauthors administered the questionnaire to a cohort ofpsychology students and found time management

2017 311Aeon and Aguinis

Page 4: 2017, Vol. 31, No. 4, 309 330

TABLE 1Summary of Selective Research on the Relationship Between Time Management and Well-Being

Authors Sample Research design and measures Conclusions

Adams and Jex(1999)

522workingadults/part-timestudents

Self-report questionnaire Time management correlates with health(r 5 .39) and job satisfaction (r 5 .27)indirectly through perceived control of timeand a reduction of work–family conflict.

Bond and Feather(1988)

Undergraduate psychologystudents (sample 1 5336; sample 25 193;sample 3 5 217)

Self-report questionnaire Time management is positively associatedwith better health (r 5 .27), a sense ofpurpose (r 5 .65), and optimism (r 5 .31),and negatively related to depression(r 5 –.44), psychological distress (r5 –.37),and anxiety (r 5 –.56).

Chang and Nguyen(2011)

111 undergraduate students Self-report questionnaire Time management correlates positively withjob satisfaction (r 5 .31) and psychologicalwell-being (r 5 .31).

Claessens et al.(2004)

70 R&D engineers Self-report questionnaire Time management is associated with jobsatisfaction (r 5 .30) and work strainthrough perceived control of time (r5 –.58).

Hafner and Stock(2010)

71 employees (tradingcompany)

Experiment (time managementtraining)

Time management training is negativelyrelated to stress and increases perceivedcontrol of time.

Hafner, Stock, et al.(2014)

177 undergraduate students Experiment (time managementintervention)

Timemanagement training reduces perceivedstress (partial h2 5 .03) and increasesperceived control of time (partial h2 5 .03).

Hafner et al. (2015) 23 undergraduate students Non-equivalent dependent variabledesign (time managementintervention)

Time management training increasesperceived control of time and reducesperceived stress.

Jex and Elacqua(1999)

525 full-time employees/part-time students

Self-report questionnaire Time management is negatively associatedwith strain (r 5 –.15 to –.42).

Kelly (2003) 130 undergraduate students Self-report questionnaire Time management is negatively related toworry (r 5 –0.21), although an alternativemeasure of time management showed nosignificant correlation (r 5 .04).

Lang (1992) 96 full-time and part-timeemployees (taking eveningbusiness classes)

Self-report questionnaire Timemanagement correlateswith less anxiety(r 5 –.22) but not depression and somaticsymptoms.

Macan (1994) Study 1: 353 employees(various organizations);study 2: 341 undergraduatestudents

Self-report questionnaire Time management is related to perceivedcontrol of time (r 5 –.04 to .43), which inturn relates to increased job satisfaction(r 5 .29) and reduced stress (r 5 –.32).

Macan (1996) 44 employees (social serviceagency)

Quasi-experimental field study(in-house time managementtraining)

Time management training increasesperceived control of time and reducessomatic tensions.

Macan et al. (1990) 165 graduate andundergraduate students

Self-report questionnaire and gradepoint average

Time management is associated with less roleambiguity (r 5 –.47) and somatic tension(r5 –.26), and with greater job (r5 .26) andlife satisfaction (r 5 .23).

Misra and McKean(2000)

249 university students Self-report questionnaire Time management correlates with lessacademic stress (r 5 .006 to –.39).

Nonis and Sager(2003)

201 sales representatives Self-report questionnaire Time management correlates negatively withstress (r 5 –.19 to –.32).

Nonis, Teng, andFord (2005)

205 MBA students (U.S. andSri Lanka)

Self-report questionnaire Time management correlates positively withjob satisfaction (r 5 .18 to .39).

Peeters and Rutte(2005)

123 elementary teachers Self-report questionnaire Time management correlates with emotionalexhaustion in people who have lowautonomy and high work demands(r 5 –.17).

Van Eerde (2003) 37 trainees Quasi-experiment Time management reduces worrying(h2 5 .08).

312 NovemberAcademy of Management Perspectives

Page 5: 2017, Vol. 31, No. 4, 309 330

to be associated with a stronger sense of purpose,higher self-esteem, better health, and optimism.They also found that managing time was associatedwith lower levelsofdepression,psychologicaldistress,anxiety, and hopelessness, with effect sizes rangingfrom r5 –.13 for anxiety to r5 .65 for sense of purposein life. Subsequent studies found time management tobeassociatedwithgreater life satisfaction (Macanet al.,1990) and job satisfaction (Macan, 1994), lower anxiety(Lang, 1992), and lower strain (Jex & Elacqua, 1999).

Many of these early studies used passive obser-vation designs, so conclusions about causality wereambiguous (Aguinis & Vandenberg, 2014). Could itbe that there is reverse causality and, actually, thathigher levels of well-being lead to improved timemanagement? Macan (1996) was among the pio-neers who used experiments to determine whethertime management training enhances well-being.She studied workers from a large social serviceagencywhohad attended a two-day training sessiontaught by an in-house instructor and comparedthem with co-workers who had not undergonetime management training. The training sessionincluded time management techniques such aslearning how to handle interruptions (Lakein,1973). Her quasi-experimental field study revealedthat time management training reduced job-relatedsomatic tensions and increasedperceived control oftime. A subsequent series of experiments by Hafnerand colleagues involving German workers anduniversity students showed that time managementtraining reduced perceived stress (Hafner & Stock,2010; Hafner, Stock, & Oberst, 2015; Hafner, Stock,et al., 2014).

Overall, nonexperimental and experimental find-ings suggest that time management can improvepeople’s quality of life, lower stress, boost job satis-faction, and enhance other facets of well-being (seeTable 1). However, suggestive though they may be,results are far from conclusive, and the existing lit-erature is not sufficiently large to conduct meta-analyses (Aguinis, Pierce, Bosco, Dalton, & Dalton,2011). For example, if we focus only on studies thatlinkMacan et al.’s (1990) timemanagement measureto job satisfaction, we note quite a few differences ineffect sizes. Claessens et al. (2004) reported an effectsize of r 5 .30; Macan’s (1994) ranged from r 5 .10(statistically nonsignificant) to r5 .19 depending onthe subscale; andNonis, Teng, andFord (2005) failedto find statistically significant effects in theirU.S. sample. In other words, while results point totime management being a potential well-being en-hancer, results exhibit substantial variability.

In sum, our review suggests that, overall, timemanagement may be useful for well-being enhance-ment and stress relief.Wenow turn to the question ofwhether time management improves performance.

Time Management and Performance

Table 2 includes a summary of studies that haveexamined the relationship between time manage-ment and performance. In their pioneering study, forinstance, Hall and Hursch (1982) studied the effectsof reading a time management manual on four uni-versity faculty and staff members. The manual’s aimwas to help people spend more time on high-prioritytasks and less time on meetings and interruptions. The

TABLE 1(Continued)

Authors Sample Research design and measures Conclusions

Van Hoye andLootens (2013)

231 unemployed people Self-report questionnaire Time structuring correlates withpsychological well-being duringunemployment (r 5 –.12 to .52).

Wanberg, Griffiths,and Gavin (1997)

243 unemployed andemployed individuals

Self-report questionnaire(longitudinal)

Time structuring correlates with better mentalhealth among unemployed people (r5 .19).

Note: Effect sizes indexed by r2 (Pearson’s correlation squared or coefficient of determination, typically used in nonexperimental research)and h2 (h-squared, typically used in experimental research) indicate proportion of variance explained. h-squared is the proportion of totalvariation attributable to the factor, and partial h-squared is the proportion of total variation attributable to the factor partialing out other factorsfrom the total nonerror variation (Pierce, Block, & Aguinis, 2004). To put effect sizes in this table in perspective, Bosco, Aguinis, Singh, Field,and Pierce (2015) reviewed 30 years of articles published in the Journal of Applied Psychology and Personnel Psychology, and, based on a totalof about 150,000 r’s, they reported an average r 5 .16 (absolute value), with the 33rd and 67th percentile values of .09 and .26, respectively.However, interpreting the importance of effect sizes requires that they be put in context. Therefore, labels such as “small,” “medium,” and“large” should not denote importance and should be based on only the size of the correlation. For example, the same effect size of r5 .20wouldbe interpreted as beingmuchmore important if the dependent variable is heart failure compared to self-reported job dissatisfaction on a 7-pointLikert-type scale.

2017 313Aeon and Aguinis

Page 6: 2017, Vol. 31, No. 4, 309 330

TABLE 2Summary of Selective Research on the Relationship Between Time Management and Performance

Authors Sample Research design and measures Conclusions

Barling et al. (1996) 102 salespeople (car sales) Self-report questionnaire Time management alone does notcorrelate with job performanceas measured by objective sales,although time management doesinteract with achievementstriving in predicting sales (r 5.32).

Britton and Tesser (1991) 90 undergraduate students Self-report questionnaire(longitudinal)

Time management correlateswith academic achievementas measured by GPA(r 5 –.10 to .39).

Claessens et al. (2004) 70 R&D engineers (semiconductorindustry)

Self-report questionnaire Time management is associatedwith self-reported jobperformance (r 5 .33).

Hafner, Oberst, et al.(2014)

96 undergraduate students Experiment (time managementintervention)

Time management interventionreduces procrastination(partial h2 5 .21).

Hafner and Stock (2010) 71 employees (trading company) Experiment (time managementtraining)

Time management training has noimpact on performance asassessed by supervisors.

Hall and Hursch (1982) 4 participants (university facultyand staff)

Time management interventionwithout control group

Time management is associatedwith an increase in time spent onhigh-priority tasks and self-ratedeffectiveness.

Kaser et al. (2013) 196 university students Experiment Dedicating uninterrupted time towork on some tasks (i.e. quiettime) leads to lowerperformance.

Konig, Kleinmann, andHohmann (2013)

27 managers (financial sector) Experimental diary study Dedicating uninterrupted time towork (i.e. quiet time) leads tohigher self-reported jobperformance (b 5 .83).

Macan (1994) Study 1: 353 employees; study 2:341 undergraduate students

Self-report questionnaire Time management is notassociatedwith jobperformance.

Macan (1996) 44 employees (social serviceagency)

Quasi-experimental field study(time management training)

Time management training doesnot lead to more timemanagement behaviors and doesnot increase job performance.

Macan et al. (1990) 165 graduate and undergraduatestudents

Self-report questionnaire Time management is associatedwith higher self-reportedperformance as measured bothby perceptions (r5 .32) andGPA(r 5 .23).

Nonis, Fenner, and Sager(2011)

201 salespeople (various sectors) Self-report questionnaire Time management correlatespositively with self-reported jobperformance (r 5 .13 to .43).

Nonis et al. (2005) 205 MBA students (U.S. and SriLanka)

Self-report questionnaire Time management is associatedwith higher self-reported jobperformance (r 5 .06 to .26).

Orpen (1994) 52 supervisors (manufacturingsector)

Experiment (training program) Time management trainingincreases job performance asassessed by managers’ appraisalof participants’ activity diaries.

Rapp et al. (2013) 212 employees and 41 supervisors(hospitality industry)

Self-report questionnaire Time management correlates withthe influenceofhelpingbehavior(r 5 .16) on job performance.

314 NovemberAcademy of Management Perspectives

Page 7: 2017, Vol. 31, No. 4, 309 330

authors asked participants to record the time they spentonmeetings and high- and low-priority activities and toreport their weekly subjective assessment of effective-ness at work. The main effect of the intervention, theauthors found, was a marked increase in time spent onhigh-priority tasks.With no control group and a sampleof only four people, however, results from this studyprovided preliminary evidence only. Subsequent re-search by Claessens et al. (2004) and Nonis, Teng, andFord (2005) demonstrated a positive relationship be-tween time management and performance (r 5 .33and r 5 .25, respectively) but similarly relied onpeople’s self-reported performance (i.e., employeeswere asked to assess their own performance comparedto thatof colleaguesalongsuchdimensionsascustomerrelations, communication, and job-related expertise).

Conclusions about the positive effects of timemanagement on performance have not been consis-tent when studies relied on other types of designsand measures. For example, Macan (1996) used su-pervisory ratings (as opposed to self-reports) in herquasi-experimental study and found that time man-agement training failed to boost job performance.

More recently, Hafner and Stock’s (2010) experi-mental intervention—consisting of a one-day trainingsession featuring exercises and practical cases—revealed that time management had no impacton indicators such as timely project completion andoverall performance as assessed by supervisors.Barling et al. (1996) used sales as an objective mea-sure of performance in car dealerships and found nodirect effect of time management on job perfor-mance. Research by Kaser, Fischbacher, and Konig(2013) showed that using quiet time, a time man-agement technique in which people dedicate un-interrupted time to work on important tasks (i.e., aform of time protection), actually lowered job per-formance as measured by the number of errors ina given task.

Clearly, results concerning timemanagement andperformance conceptualized as results or outcomes(e.g., sales, project completion) aremixed.However,individual performance can also be conceptualizedas behaviors rather than results (Aguinis, O’Boyle,Gonzalez-Mule, & Joo, 2016; Joo, Aguinis, & Bradley,2017). For instance, Rapp, Bachrach, and Rapp’s

TABLE 2(Continued)

Authors Sample Research design and measures Conclusions

Slaven and Totterdell(1993)

32 employees (various sectors) Time management intervention(no control group)

Time management training is notassociated with motivation,commitment, and time spent onhigh-priority tasks.

Trueman and Hartley(1996)

293 university students Self-report questionnaire Time management is associatedwith overall academicperformance (r 5 .21).

Van Eerde (2003) 37 trainees Quasi-experiment Time management reducesprocrastination (h2 5 .10).

Woolfolk and Woolfolk(1986)

81 pre-service teachers(undergraduate seniors)

Experiment (time managementtraining)

Time management training doesnot increase performance ratingsas assessed by cooperatingteachers and supervisors.

Zampetakis et al. (2010) 186 undergraduate students Self-report questionnaire Time management is positivelyassociated with creativity(r 5 .48).

Note: GPA5 grade point average. Effect sizes indexed by r2 (Pearson’s correlation squared or coefficient of determination, typically used innonexperimental research) andh2 (h-squared, typically used in experimental research) indicate proportion of variance explained.h-squared isthe proportion of total variation attributable to the factor, and partial h-squared is the proportion of total variation attributable to the factorpartialing out other factors from the total nonerror variation (Pierce, Block, & Aguinis, 2004). To put effect sizes in this table in perspective,Bosco, Aguinis, Singh, Field, and Pierce (2015) reviewed 30 years of articles published in the Journal of Applied Psychology and PersonnelPsychology, and, based on a total of about 150,000 r’s, they reported an average r5 .16 (absolute value),with the 33rd and67thpercentile valuesof .09 and .26, respectively. However, interpreting the importance of effect sizes requires that they be put in context. Therefore, labels such as“small,” “medium,” and “large” should not denote importance and should be based on only the size of the correlation. For example, the sameeffect size of r5 .20would be interpreted as beingmuchmore important if the dependent variable is heart failure compared to self-reported jobdissatisfaction on a 7-point Likert-type scale.

2017 315Aeon and Aguinis

Page 8: 2017, Vol. 31, No. 4, 309 330

(2013, p. 674) study on time management and orga-nizational citizenship behavior concluded thatskilled time managers “do a better job of managingtheir citizenship contributions as well as the re-ciprocal exchanges that emerge as a consequence ofthese behaviors.” Another study linked time man-agement to higher levels of creativity (Zampetakiset al., 2010). Last, a handful of experiments haveshown time management training to reduce pro-crastination (Van Eerde, 2003; Hafner, Oberst, &Stock, 2014).

In sum, the existing evidence suggests a complexrelationship between time management and perfor-mance. Time management seems to have more con-sistent effects on performance defined as behaviorscompared to performance defined as results or out-comes. In what follows, we outline novel perspec-tives on time management that will allow us todiscuss and interpret the findings highlighted above.

INTEGRATING INSIGHTS FROM SOCIOLOGY,PSYCHOLOGY, AND BEHAVIORAL ECONOMICS

The mixed results described in the previous sec-tion mean that we need to better understand the dy-namics of time management. To do so, we focus onthree research perspectives: time norms and struc-tures, individual time-related differences, and tem-poral decision making. Each of these perspectivesaddresses often-neglected aspects in time manage-ment research: Time structures and norms considerenvironmental influences; individual differencesdescribe how time preferences, beliefs, and attitudesaffect time management behaviors; and temporaldecision making sheds light on the underlying dy-namics of time management itself. By virtue of theirfocus on different levels of analysis, the perspectiveswe offer pave the way for bridging micro and macrodomains (Aguinis, Boyd, Pierce, & Short, 2011) infuture time management research. We elaborate oneach of these three perspectives in what follows.

Time Structures and Time Norms

Macan (1994) found a relationship between jobtype and timemanagement: Themore structured thejob (e.g., maintenance staff), the less likely peoplewere to engage in time management (r 5 –.20) orattend timemanagement training seminars (r5 –.25).Unsurprisingly, people who occupied highly struc-tured jobs also reported lower levels of perceivedcontrol over time. More recently, Claessens et al.(2004) demonstrated that job characteristics such as

job autonomy and workload influenced one’s per-ceived control of time and, by extension, job satis-faction, job performance, and stress. A subsequentstudy by Nonis et al. (2005) took a higher-level ap-proach and examined the effects of national cultureon time management practices. The authors foundthat different aspects of time management have dif-ferent effects on job performance (i.e., self-reportedeffectiveness in customer relations, sales, and otherperformance dimensions) and satisfaction dependingon whether employees were in the United States orSri Lanka.

These findings highlight the importance of timestructures and time norms, two key concepts in thesociology of time often overlooked in time researchin themanagement and psychology literatures. Timestructures are “those external aspects . . . that can bedescribed more or less reliably by an independentobserver” (Barley, 1988, p. 128), such as the timing,frequency, sequence, and duration of events (Flaherty,2003; Moore, 1963; Zerubavel, 1976). Business hours,project timelines, cleaning schedules, and holidaysexemplify time structures; they are explicit and for-malized. Time structures affect individual time man-agement by laying out a system around which peoplecan organize their time. For instance, when an em-ployee manages her time, she has to take into accounther team’s deadlines and the organization’s hoursof operation. Even global entrepreneurs (Markman,Devinney, Pedersen, & Tihanyi, 2016), who seeminglyenjoy unfettered autonomy, must operate within thebounds of their international clients’ different timezones.

Time norms, in contrast, are intangible and sharedpatterns of expected temporal activity (Ancona,Goodman, Lawrence, & Tushman, 2001; Bergmann,1992) that become more salient once they arebreached. They constrain time management behav-ior through social pressures. For instance, an em-ployee may want to quickly wrap up his sales pitchand leave the office early, but unwritten time rulesdictate that a meeting with an important clientshould not be rushed lest the client take offense atbeing given short shrift. What distinguishes timenorms from time structures is themoral connotationsattached to time norms (Zerubavel, 1979a, 1979b). Ateam leader’s proposal to set a project deadline onMay 23rd (i.e., an element of time structure) mightarouse disagreement among teammembers, but theywill likely not think of the deadline as a moral issue.In contrast, a supervisor can afford to arrive 15minutes late to a meeting because lateness signalspower and status (i.e., a time norm); a direct report

316 NovemberAcademy of Management Perspectives

Page 9: 2017, Vol. 31, No. 4, 309 330

running late, however, signals lack of assiduity andcommitment, both of which are typically judgedfrom a moral standpoint.

We contend that time norms influence individualtime management in two ways. First, a time normbreach, such as calling one’s supervisor at 3 a.m. orleaving work 20 minutes early in a “workaholic”culture, can elicit strong reactions from peers, withsubstantial consequences for the violator (e.g., loss ofreputation, ostracism, and even termination). Thus,time norms act as a deterrent for engaging in tem-poral behaviors that are frowned upon. Second, evenin the absence of prohibitive sanctions, time normsaffect time management by making individuals takecertain behaviors for granted. Taken-for-grantednessensures adherence to local customs. Such adherenceto time norms is mainly done through early sociali-zation (Ancona, Okhuysen, & Perlow, 2001; Berger &Luckmann, 1966; Schneider, Ehrhart, & Macey,2011) and is, over time, maintained through habit.

Time structures and norms operate at many levelsof analysis, and thus afford a conceptual frameworkto study the dynamics of time management beyondthe individual level. In what follows, we offer ex-amples of how time structures and norms influencetime management at the team, organization, andcountry levels. As a preview, Table 3 includes asummary of the discussion that follows.

Team level. Barker’s (1993) ethnographic studyrevealed that teams tend to work out rules that, overtime, ossify into structures that constrain the be-havior of existing members and newcomers. Manysuch structures are time-based, which shouldn’t besurprising because a defining feature of teams is

interdependence (Cannon-Bowers & Bowers, 2011),and the best way to ensure seamless interdependenceis temporal coordination (Janicik & Bartel, 2003;Okhuysen & Bechky, 2009). This is why teams devisesundry time structures to control teammembers’ timemanagement. As a rather drastic example, a partici-pant in Barker’s (1993, p. 428) study reported that “ifyou are more than five minutes late, you’re dockeda day’s pay.”Here, the teamhad comeupwith a stricttime structure (i.e., work starts at a certain time andyou can’t bemore than fiveminutes late) that affectedindividuals’ timemanagement choices. By deductinga whole day’s pay for a five-minute delay, the teamalso implicitly established a draconian norm ofpunctuality. The timenormconveyeda clearmessageto existing teammembers and newcomers: “Our timeis extremely precious, so you had better show up at 8o’clock sharp and notwaste a singleminute.” In otherwords, what seemed like a simple rule (i.e., if you’remore than5minutes late you lose your day’s pay)wasin reality amanifestationof the team’snormativeviewof timeuse (i.e., we expect you tomake optimal use ofyour time) that could dramatically affect a person’stime management choices.

Organization level. Like teams, organizations usetime structures to align the efforts of employees withorganizational goals. The primary time structure inorganizations is hours of operation (e.g., 9 to 5), andorganizations increasingly experiment with differ-ent ways to structure those hours. Intel, for instance,had 300 engineers turn off their communication de-vices and pin “do not disturb” signs to their officedoors every Tuesday morning for four hours (Stone,2008). The purpose of this “quiet time” practice was

TABLE 3Summary of Why Time Structures and Time Norms Affect Time Management at Different Levels of Analysis

Level of analysis Time structures Time norms

Team Teams agree on rules via consensus. Such rules can betime-related (e.g., work starts at 8 a.m. sharp) andinfluence the time management behaviors ofindividual members.

Teams develop implicit time-related norms that canconstrain individual time management (e.g., time isprecious in our team, andwasting it will be severelypunished).

Organization Organizations use time structures (e.g., businesshours, project timelines) to standardize and controlindividual time management practices.

Through socialization and reward systems,organizations instill time norms in employees tochannel their individual time managementpractices toward organizational goals.

Country Cultures and institutions have different ways oforganizing time—hence the differences in timezones, business days, and other time structuresacross countries and institutions. Individuals suchas travel executives and global entrepreneurs mustbe mindful of those differences to seamlesslycoordinate their global operations.

Different cultures and institutions have wildlydifferent norms with regard to time. To avoidconflict, the frequently traveling employee must betime-culturally savvy.

2017 317Aeon and Aguinis

Page 10: 2017, Vol. 31, No. 4, 309 330

to determine whether four hours of undisturbedwork (i.e., a time structure)would affect productivityand creativity. Perlow (1999) conducted a similarintervention (also dubbed “quiet time”) on softwareengineers at a Fortune 500 company. The in-tervention, she found, had tremendous implicationsfor time management. In her words, “the engineersdiscovered that they were not well prepared to workalone and needed help from a colleague to continue.It often turnedout thatwhat theyneededcouldeasilyhave been prepared ahead of time, but the engineerswere not used to planning ahead. By the third phase,they indicated that they were more accustomed toquiet time and were better able to prepare for non-interactive periods” (Perlow, 1999, p. 73).

Organizational time norms, too, affect individualtime management. But, because time norms are nottypically explicit or formalized, they can be difficultto quantify and study. Nevertheless, Schriber andGutek’s (1987) study uncovered a set of time normsthatmadeup the time cultures of the organizations intheir sample. Such norms included expectations re-garding punctuality, emphasis on scheduling anddeadlines, temporal boundaries between work andhome, work speed, and autonomy of time use. Morerecently, Burt et al. (2010) developed a scale tomeasure the extent to which organizational normsfacilitate time management practices. Items includesuch statements as “Productive use of time is a keyvalue” and “Making time to plan the day’s work isencouraged.” Such norms have direct implicationsfor individual time management, and the authorsfound that employees in organizations with less“time management–friendly” norms had higherstress and turnover intentions.

Country level. Global entrepreneurs (Markmanet al., 2016), traveling executives (DeFrank,Konopaske, & Ivancevich, 2000), virtual team mem-bers (Malhotra, Majchrzak, & Rosen, 2007), andemployees on international assignments (Shaffer,Kraimer, Chen, & Bolino, 2012) must all adjust theirtime management practices to take into accountdifferent time zones, business hours, and how othertime structures vary around the world. But an evenmore complex terrain to navigate is that of timenorms in foreign cultures (Graham, 1981; Hofstede,2001; Levine & Norenzayan, 1999). Executives mustfactor in, for example, that being 45minutes late to anofficial meeting is considered acceptable in Mexico.When invited over for dinner in Greece, it is rude toask locals for a specific dinner time; what matters isthat one shows up, not when (Hall, 1959). In Brazil,a country characterized by high in-group culture,

foreign team leaders are expected to spendenormousamounts of time with team members and cajolepeople in positions of power to be successful(Javidan, Dorfman, de Luque, & House, 2006). In-ternational entrepreneurs must similarly keep inmind that different cultural and institutional con-texts have different time orientations, “potentiallyinfluencing entrepreneurs’ tenacity and persistenceas well as investment horizons when making in-vestments and resource allocations” (Zahra &Wright, 2011, p. 73). Such differences in time ori-entations can also foster varying levels of per-ceived urgency, which influence entrepreneurs’priorities and time management choices (Zahra &Wright, 2011).

In summary, the above examples highlight theimportance of using time structures and norms asa lens to study time management at the team, orga-nization, and country levels of analysis. Timestructures and norms, however, are not immutable.We mentioned earlier that clock time, calendars,weekends, and so on are social constructs, whichmeans they are changeable. In other words, whilehigher-level time norms such as organizationalculture can affect individual time management(i.e., top-down influence; Bluedorn, 2000), indi-viduals can also change higher-level norms andstructures through concerted action (i.e., bottom-upinfluence; Perlow,Mazmanian, &Hansen, 2016) andmake their environment more adapted to their timemanagement style.

Individual Differences

Individual differences in personality, values, andbeliefs are known to influence various organiza-tional outcomes such as job satisfaction and perfor-mance (Barsade & Gibson, 2007; Sackett, Lievens,Van Iddekinge, & Kuncel, 2017). Similarly, individ-ual differences—specifically, individual differencesin time attitudes, beliefs, and preferences (Vinton,1992)—play a critical role in affecting time manage-ment outcomes.

Time-related individual differences abound. Forinstance, perceived control over time (Macan et al.,1990), sometimes called temporal self-efficacy(Britton & Tesser, 1991), refers to people’s beliefthat they are in charge of their time. This belief hasbeen well researched from a time management per-spective, but it is often studied as an outcome of timemanagement (Hafner & Stock, 2010). Other time-related differences might better predict time man-agement outcomes. For instance, some people prefer

318 NovemberAcademy of Management Perspectives

Page 11: 2017, Vol. 31, No. 4, 309 330

to do one thing at a time; others prefer to multitask.The latter have a polychronic time preference; theformer have a monochronic time preference(Bluedorn, Kaufman, & Lane, 1992; Hecht & Allen,2005). These differences in individual preferencesaffect time management outcomes: Monochronicpeople are more upset by schedule changes and en-gage in more planning; polychronic people dealbetter with schedule changes and can easily in-tegrate different activities (Kaufman-Scarborough& Lindquist, 1999).

In the work–life balance literature, there is a dis-tinctionbetween“segmenters” and “integrators”: Theformer like to set boundaries between work time andfamily time, while the latter prefer to blend the two(Nippert-Eng, 1996; Rothbard & Ollier-Malaterre,2016). It is easy to imagine that time managementtraining will have different outcomes dependingon whether the trainee is a segmenter or anintegrator—the former will benefit from a rigid stylethat sets boundaries between work and home, whilethe latterwill require a flexible timemanagement stylethat facilitates seamless integration of both domains.

In short, there is a host of time-related individu-aldifferencessuchas timepreferences (i.e.,polychronicvs. monochronic preference), time boundary styles(i.e., segmenting vs. integrating), and others (e.g., tem-poral orientation; Shipp, Edwards, & Lambert, 2009;Zimbardo&Boyd, 1999). Researchersneed topaymoreattention to how individual differences—especiallythose related to time—can moderate or mediate(Aguinis, Edwards, & Bradley, 2017) the effects of timemanagement on various outcomes. To illustrate, wenext discuss the individual difference of temporalawareness in greater detail.

Temporal awareness. Temporal awareness is thebelief that time is a real, finite resource that needs tobe budgeted. To avoid any conceptual confu-sion (Suddaby, 2010), we emphasize that tempo-ral awareness is not time perception (i.e., people’ssubjective perception of time passing; Flaherty,1999), but a belief regarding the nature of time.

Half a century ago,Drucker (1967) observed that high-performing executives think primarily in terms of time,not activities or strategies. Such executives, he noted,have a sense of how much time they have and wheretheir time actually goes. A series of experiments bySoman (2001) showed that many people can engage inrational budgeting formoneybutnot for time, suggestingthat people do not readily conceive of time as a finiteresource.

Temporal awareness is not mere time tracking ortemporal accounting, although these two behaviors

are likely outcomes of temporal awareness. Rather,temporal awareness is the understanding that thereare only so many hours in a day and that activitiescome at a temporal cost, hence the need forbudgeting—temporal awareness is a resource-basedconception of time. Granted, most people wouldagree with the statements “time is finite” and “timeshould be budgeted like money,” but not everyoneactually conceives of time that way in daily life.

Low temporal awareness (i.e., weak or nonexistentbelief that time is a resource) severely underminespeople’s ability to manage time effectively, muchlike an inability to see credit card funds as creditleads to poor finance management. People high intemporal awareness think of their available time asa time budget—finite and nonrenewable. The cur-rency of their psychological economy is time. Howmuch will this movie cost me in time? Can I rea-sonably expect to finish this report before attendingthe meeting? How long will it take you, the reader ofour manuscript, to finish it? These are the kind ofquestions that temporally aware individuals rou-tinely ask themselves. As a result, temporal aware-ness can influence how and to what extent peoplemanage their time. A person high in temporalawareness, for instance, might have more of a pro-clivity for time tracking and scheduling than some-one low in temporal awareness, whomight usemoreof an improvisational time management style. Peo-ple with high temporal awareness, by virtue ofthinking mainly in terms of time, might also be morenaturally drawn to time management as a way toimplement their preference for a time-based organi-zation system.

In short, people high in temporal awareness willlikely enjoy better time management outcomes. Thechiefmechanism,we contend, is a reduction of time-based conflict, defined as the impossibility of com-pleting multiple activities because of insufficienttime (Greenhaus & Beutell, 1985). Indeed, hightemporal awareness helps people make more re-alistic assessments of what they can and cannot do.When people know how much time they have andtreat time as a resource, they are less likely to committo new activities requiring more time than they ac-tually have. As a result, people high in temporalawareness are likely to experience lower levels oftime-based conflict between their activities. The re-duced level of conflict between a person’s differentroles and activities may, in turn, increase well-beingand performance.

Having discussed perspectives and insights re-garding time structures andnorms, and the influence

2017 319Aeon and Aguinis

Page 12: 2017, Vol. 31, No. 4, 309 330

of individual differences (e.g., temporal self-efficacy,monochronic vs. polychronic time preference, seg-menters vs. integrators, temporal awareness), wenow turn to how decision making, when applied totime decisions, can affect time management.

Temporal Decision Making

As our definition of time management implies,time management is a form of temporal decisionmaking. This means, fundamentally, that timemanagement is about making decisions—consciousor otherwise—relating to how we use our time. Inthis section, we discuss how temporal decisionmaking—the examination of how peoplemake time-related decisions—enhances our understanding oftime management.

The time management literature implicitly adoptsa “rational timemanager”model, according towhichindividuals make optimal time decisions. Otherfields, such as economics and strategicmanagement,have also traditionally treated individuals as ratio-nal, optimal decision makers. However, devel-opments in behavioral economics (Kahneman,2003) and strategy-as-practice (Vaara&Whittington,2012) have upturned some of these assumptions. Inthe same way, insights from the temporal decision-making literature can elucidate how people actuallymanage their time.Our goal isnot to comprehensivelysurvey the field of temporal decision making. Rather,we aim to illustrate how a synthesis of findings intemporal decision making can inform our under-standingof timemanagementbydrawingona fewkeyperspectives and insights.

Temporal escalation of commitment.Wealreadymentioned Soman’s (2001) experiments, whichshowed that people do notmentally account for timetheway theydomoney.Another result fromSoman’s(2001) experiments was that the sunk-cost effect—awell-established psychological bias that inducespeople to throw good money after bad (Drummond,2014)—seems to hold for money but not for time.Interestingly, when experimental manipulationsmade people more likely to account for time (e.g., byproviding a wage rate or lecturing participants abouteconomic approaches to time), the sunk-cost effectappeared. Results of this study draw attention toa potentially counterproductive effect of time man-agement training. Specifically, when people engagein timemanagement, they aremore likely to accountfor time in a systematic way. According to Soman’sstudy, this means that timemanagers are more likelyto fall prey to the temporal sunk-cost bias. As a result,

this might hamper people’s ability to “stop un-profitable routines and activities” (Britton & Tesser,1991, p. 409) and lead to a counterproductive esca-lation of commitment—that is, throwing good timeafter bad. A key implication we can draw fromSoman’s (2001) work is that time managementtraining can have unintended consequences, such asfostering a sunk-cost bias that can undermine thevery purpose of time management.

Value of time. Another important aspect of tem-poral decision making is time valuation. How wevalue our time bears relevance to howwemanage it.Consider independent contractors, who are oftentouted as the epitome of professional freedom(Aguinis & Lawal, 2013; Barley & Kunda, 2006).Contractors tend to sell their services by the hour. Asa result, they are intensely aware of the economicvalue of their time and face “an ever-present choiceof how to spend every hour. . . .When choosing howto spend their time, contractors could calculate to thepenny the opportunity costs of every unbilled orleisure hour” (Evans, Kunda, & Barley, 2004, p. 21).The value people place on their time is thus con-sequential for time management because time man-agement may induce people to consciously orunconsciously strive to make the most of their time,to get the most bang for their minute. This may bea potentially detrimental side effect.

DeVoe and Pfeffer (2007), for instance, concludedthat because hourly payment makes people keenlyaware of the value of their time, it makes non-remunerative activities, such as volunteering, muchless attractive. Similarly, the higher the perceivedeconomic value of time, themore people feel pressedfor time (DeVoe & Pfeffer, 2011), which defeats thevery purpose of timemanagement. Counterintuitively,therefore, being overly conscious of time’s economicvalue may not lead to effective time management. Ifanything, it maymake peoplemore harried. In supportof this conclusion, research shows that being generouswith one’s time (e.g., devoting time to helping people)actually makes people feel as though they have moretime, not less (Mogilner, Chance, & Norton, 2012).

We have barely scratched the surface of howtemporal decision making can inform time manage-ment, and a full review of the temporal decision-making literature is beyond the scope of this paper.However, time is a very peculiar resource withunique characteristics, warranting more attentionto temporal decision making. We elaborate on thispoint in what follows.

The importance of a temporal approach to de-cisionmaking.Webelieve that timedeserves its own

320 NovemberAcademy of Management Perspectives

Page 13: 2017, Vol. 31, No. 4, 309 330

decision-making literature because it is a resourcelike no other. Unlike money, time is possessed inequal amounts by everyone (McGrath & Rotchford,1983) but is still subject to theft in the workplace bypeople who tend to non-work-related tasks duringwork hours (Brock, Martin, & Buckley, 2013; Martin,Brock, Buckley, & Ketchen, 2010) and abuse bypeople who excessively solicit coworkers’ time(Perlow, 1999).Unlike energy, time is not renewable,recoverable, or substitutable (Fritz, Lam, &Spreitzer,2011; Jaques, 1982; Moore, 1963). Furthermore, thevalue of time is ambiguous. Everybody knows thata dollar is a dollar, but an hour can mean differentthings to different people (Okada & Hoch, 2004).

Time is also unique by virtue of being the funda-mental resource—people need time to acquire otherresources. For example, at the individual level ofanalysis, people are not able to acquire new knowl-edge, skills, and abilities if they do not have suffi-cient time (Aguinis & Kraiger, 2009), which impairstheir results- and behavior-based performance(Markman, 2012). At the firm level of analysis, or-ganizations are not able to acquire valuable and in-imitable resources that give them a competitivemarket advantage if theydon’t have sufficient time todo so (Perlow, 1999). Organizational actions occur intime and unfold in a path-dependent way throughtime, which has implications even for seeminglydisconnected events such as the subsequent ven-tures of serial entrepreneurs (Wright, Robbie, &Ennew, 1997; Zahra & Wright, 2011). Given time’speculiar nature, then, it shouldn’t be surprisingthat unlike other resources, time is a resource thatmost people have a hard time processing (Saini &Monga, 2008).

DISCUSSION

Our review of the literature suggests that thelinks between time management and well-beingand performance are not clear; the relationshipbetween time management and well-being ex-hibits much variability, and the link between timemanagement and performance seems to dependon whether performance is measured as resultsor behaviors. Our review also suggests that in-sights relevant to the time management literatureare fragmented and dispersed across various dis-ciplines such as sociology, psychology, and be-havioral economics. We proposed three novelperspectives that integrate cross-disciplinary andmultilevel insights. This integration identifies sev-eral critical factors that may enhance or suppress

the effects of timemanagement and sheds light on thehitherto ambiguous links between time management,well-being, and performance.

To illustrate how our cross-disciplinary and mul-tilevel perspectives can shed light on past research,let’s focus on the link between timemanagement andperformance (see Table 2). We can start by usinga time structures and norms lens. In her study ontime management in a software engineering com-pany, Perlow (1999, p. 69) observed that “at the endof the calendar year, in a confidential meeting, themanagers ranked their software engineers. . . . For allof the top ten engineers, the comments mentionedthe long hours that the engineers worked. . . . Incontrast, the comments about those at the bottom ofthe list all referred negatively to the engineer’s levelof commitment as assessed by hours worked. . . .Clearly, managers noticed the hours that the engi-neers worked and used these observations as a crite-rion in ranking them.” In otherwords, organizationaltime norms in Perlow’s organization equate longhours with performance, a norm arguably antitheti-cal to efficient timemanagement. In such conditions,an employee who excels at time management mightfinish her job in less time than her peers and yet geta negative performance review. In such cases, em-pirical analyses blind to the importance of timestructures and norms might wrongly conclude thattime management is not related to performance.

Orpen’s (1994) experiment corroborated this view.After randomlyassigning agroupof supervisors to anintensive, customized three-day time managementtraining program, the author asked participants tokeep track of how they spent each 30 minutes of ev-ery workday in an activity diary. The author thenasked three managers familiar with the demands ofthe job to rate the employees’ performance based ontheir activity diaries (i.e., a behavior-based perfor-mance assessment). Critically, activity diaries wereanonymous and did not disclose whether the em-ployee had undergone training or not. In otherwords, Orpen’s (1994) experimental design miti-gated the influence of time norms and other con-founding variables by shrouding diaries in anonymity.The nature of Orpen’s (1994) study might explainwhy it is among the rare experiments to find a clearlypositive link between time management and jobperformance.

As a second perspective, people’s individualdifferences, especially time-related individual dif-ferences, likely play a big role in whether timemanagement boosts job performance. Consider,for instance, the fact that in some experiments

2017 321Aeon and Aguinis

Page 14: 2017, Vol. 31, No. 4, 309 330

participants reported engaging in more timemanagement behaviors after training (e.g., Hafner& Stock, 2010), while in other experiments manyparticipants did not seem to manage their time atall after they had been trained (e.g., Macan, 1996).This could very well be due to participants’ indi-vidual differences. A handful of researchers havestudied the effects on time management of indi-vidual time-related differences, such as timediscounting (i.e., choosing immediate small re-wards over larger but delayed payoffs; Koch &Kleinmann, 2002; Konig & Kleinmann, 2005). Re-sults from these studies show that people whodiscount time steeply engage less in time man-agement (Konig & Kleinmann, 2006) and pay lessattention to future deadlines (Konig & Kleinmann,2007). Therefore, if these individual differencesare not considered explicitly, it is difficult toconclude whether time management does influ-ence job performance, and to what extent. But-tressing this view, Barling et al. (1996) found thatwhile time management alone did not predict carsales (a key measure of performance among cardealers), achievement striving (admittedly nota time-related construct, but an individual differ-ence nonetheless) significantly interacted withtime management to predict job performance.

A third perspective that helps usmake sense of theexisting literature is temporal decision making. Ourdiscussion of temporal decision making outlinedhow timemanagement training itself can undermineperformance. Consider Macan’s (1994, p. 388) con-clusion that the “size of the path coefficients . . .suggests that time management training may notexplain much of the reported variance in the be-haviors.” Consider, further, Macan’s (1994, p. 389)assertion that “respondents in the present studywho practiced time management behaviors suchas making lists and scheduling activities did notnecessarily perceive greater control over theirtime. . . . When a person does not complete theprojects listed, the perception of having little con-trol over how time is spentmay result.”This relatesto how framing effects—a core bias in the decision-making literature—affect our perception of time andwork. Researchers have found that thinking in termsof “time spent” and “work left” is often seen as a sug-gestion to rev up the pace; on the other hand, “workdone” and “time left” indicate that there is no needto rush (Teigen & Karevold, 2005). Depending onwhether participants in Macan’s (1994) study framedtheir projects in terms of remaining time (e.g., ona schedule) or work left (e.g., on a to-do list), their

feelings of being in control of their time varied tre-mendously. This is why there is a need to open the“black box” of time management training: With fewexceptions (e.g., Hafner & Stock, 2010; Van Eerde,2003), experimenters typically provide only a vagueoutline of the contents of time management train-ing programs, mentioning covered topics only inpassing (e.g., Macan, 1996; Orpen, 1994; Woolfolk& Woolfolk, 1986).

Consider another illustration. Kaser et al. (2013)concluded that when people engaged in quiet time,their performance actually decreased. But, as the au-thors themselves acknowledged, the nature of the ex-periments heavily influenced participants’ temporaldecision-making processes. As the authors put it:

In search of an explanation, we find that . . . the [quiettime participant]’s performance decreases signifi-cantly when the number of time spans with inter-ruptions and without interruptions increases (r 5–.42, p , .05). This means that people who havechosen to frequently alternate between interruptiontime and non-interruption time performed morepoorly than people who divided their time into fewerand therefore longer time spans with and withoutinterruptions. Themeannumber of time spanswithorwithout interruptions . . . lasted 4.7 minutes. A theo-retical explanation for the poorer performance mightbe that selecting more and therefore shorter timespans without interruptions generated a higher cog-nitive load because this was harder [for participants]to track. (Kaser et al., 2013, p. 301)

In other words, it is not quiet time per se that de-creases performance; it is the frequent task switchingthat undermines people’s temporal decision makingand, by extension, their performance.

In short, the three perspectives improve our un-derstanding of the relationship between time manage-mentandwell-beingandperformanceandaffordaclearframework for making sense of the literature. Next, weturn to their potential for guiding future research.

Suggestions for Future Research

Based on our three perspectives, we offer the fol-lowing directions for future research. First, futureresearch can focus on time structures and norms.The vast majority of existing studies use quantita-tive methods, which seems fitting given the osten-sibly practical and efficiency-oriented nature oftime management. The reality, however, is thatpeople who manage their time are, like all people,embedded in an intricatewebof social relationshipsand constraints. To understand how the complexity

322 NovemberAcademy of Management Perspectives

Page 15: 2017, Vol. 31, No. 4, 309 330

of social life influences time management, re-searchers need to approach time management froma complementary qualitative angle and study thickdescriptions of people’s experiences. Qualitativedesigns enable researchers to fully explore the na-ture, antecedents, and outcomes of time structuresand norms; this could not only shed light on thedynamics of time structures and norms, but alsounearth facets of time management that are yetunexplored.

In addition, the issue of time structures and normsopens the door to myriad opportunities for cross-cultural timemanagement research. Inparticular,wecontend that, because of cultural differences, theway a person manages time at home in no wayguarantees success when transferred to anothercountry. One way to mitigate this issue—and a prom-ising direction for future research—would be to pairexpatriateswith localhostswho, as culturalmediators,can familiarize expatriates with local time norms andstructures (Cooper, Doucet, & Pratt, 2007; DeNisi& Toh, 2005). Studying how expatriates and frequenttravelers can better adapt to different time structuresand norms can not only enhance organizational per-formance, but also avert the numerous pitfalls thatthreaten the well-being of expatriates and theirfamilies.

Second, future research could investigate the ex-tent to which time-related individual differences,and particularly temporal awareness, affect timemanagement. A first step in this direction would betodevelopameasureof temporal awareness. Suchaninstrument would allow us to determine whethertime management training yields better results inpeople with high temporal awareness. Researcherscan also determine if temporal awareness alone (thatis, in the absence of typical time management be-haviors such as scheduling) contributes to outcomessuch as job performance and satisfaction. Most im-portant, future research candetermine towhat extenttemporal awareness is dispositional. If temporalawareness is a crucial prerequisite for good timemanagement, then the degree to which temporalawareness can be learned has important implica-tions for time management training. By the same to-ken, research can also examine how much timemanagement itself can be learned. There is modestevidence that time management might be a disposi-tional aspect of individual personality (Shahani,Weiner, & Streit, 1993), but results are insufficient todrawa firmconclusion.More recently,Malatras et al.(2016) showed that people who grow up in stablefamilies tend to have better time management skills.

One way to conclusively assess the extent to whichtemporal awareness and time management are dis-positional constructs would be to conduct a twinstudy (Boomsma, Busjahn, & Peltonen, 2002).

Third, aswenoted earlier, the field ofmanagementin general would benefit from more research ontemporal decision making. For instance, we shouldnot assume that time management training is nec-essarily beneficial because it can foster decision-making biases—such as the sunk-cost effect (Soman,2001)—that ultimately undermine time manage-ment outcomes. Furthermore, existing research ontime valuation—a key parameter in temporal de-cision making—draws attention to an importantmethodological implication for time managementresearch. Specifically, dopeoplewho take the time toparticipate in time management studies differ sig-nificantly from people who don’t? In other words,is there a potential nonresponse bias (Rogelberg &Stanton, 2007) inherent to time management re-search? We believe there is.

Goodman, Cryder, and Cheema (2013) comparedMechanical Turk (MTurk, Amazon’s online laborsystem)participantswithpeople fromamiddle-classurban neighborhood on various measures. Theyfound that the MTurk participants valued their timeless than the offline sample, which is not that sur-prising given that the MTurk participants agreed tocomplete a 15-minute survey for a paltry $0.20. Asresearch on time valuation suggests (e.g., DeVoe &Pfeffer, 2007), thismeans that peoplewhovalue theirtime more might be less likely to participate in timemanagement studies, which, as a result, createsa nonresponse bias and compromises the validity ofresults. For instance, in Macan’s (1996) quasi-experiment, there were significant preexisting dif-ferences between participants who volunteered toparticipate in time management training and thosewho did not. One way to address this would be todevise shorter time management measures—Macanet al.’s (1990) TMB scale comprises 46 items; Brittonand Tesser’s (1991) TMQ measure contains 35.Shorter scales would likely attract participants whowould have otherwise declined to take part in stud-ies they deemed too lengthy.

Implications for Practice

Time management is a topic well suited to bridgethe practice–research gap (Bansal, Bertels, Ewart,MacConnachie, & O’Brien, 2012). We offer somepractical observations based on the three perspec-tives advanced in our article.

2017 323Aeon and Aguinis

Page 16: 2017, Vol. 31, No. 4, 309 330

First, timemanagement training iswildly popular inorganizations and is often touted as a silver bullet thatwill fix sluggishness and other corporate woes. How-ever, this is an ill-advised approach to time manage-ment that will likely fail if it ignores organizationaltime structures and norms (Jacobs & Gerson, 2004;Perlow, 1999). Indeed, the literature we reviewedsuggests thatmanytimemanagement interventions failto translate into job performance. As we have dis-cussed, chief among the reasons for this are the orga-nizational time structures and norms that hinderemployees’ effective time management. Employeesoftenhave to contendwith temporal expectations fromtheir managers and coworkers that, in fact, discouragegood time management practices (e.g., Perlow, 1999).

From an employee’s perspective, organizationaltime structures and norms are hard to resist, let alonechange, and this can often lead to frustration anddysfunctional turnover. Leaders, on the other hand,can reengineer their organization’s time structuresand norms in a way that accommodates effectivetime management practices. As a first step, leaderscan use existing measures of time structures andnorms (e.g., Burt et al., 2010; Schriber &Gutek, 1987)as a diagnostic tool. Judging by the available evi-dence, regardless of whether people manage theirtime or not, organizational cultures that are moretime management–friendly tend to cause less stressand turnover intentions among employees (Burtet al., 2010), which further highlights the impor-tance of time structures and norms for practitioners.

Second, time-related individual differences mayor may not lead to positive time management out-comes. As the literature suggests, time managementis not for everyone (e.g., Barling et al., 1996). Thisdoes not mean that time-related individual differ-ences cannot be changed. Zimbardo and Boyd(2008), for instance, argued that it is possible forpresent-oriented people (i.e., people who live in themoment, like to take risks, and loathe thinkingabout the future) to become more future-oriented(i.e., become more forward-thinking, plan ahead,take more calculated risks, and so on) with propertraining. For this reason, timemanagement programsshould expand their curriculum to more than justtraditional aspects of timemanagement such as to-dolists and scheduling and include modules that targettime-related individual differences. For example,training programs may include content for peoplelow in temporal awareness to make them moremindful of the “resource” dimension of time.

Another strategy would be to tackle individualdifferences not by changing them but by screening

them out. Different organizations have different timenorms that may or may not accommodate certaintime-related individual differences. This suggestsa need to consider person–environment fit (Edwards,Cable, Williamson, Lambert, & Shipp, 2006) froma temporal perspective (Francis-Smythe & Robertson,2003; Kaufman, Lane, & Lindquist, 1991). For humanresource management practitioners, this means thatemployee selection can be used to screen out can-didates whose time attitudes, beliefs, or preferencesdo not fit the organization’s time norms. Selection, insome cases, might be beneficial because diversity intime-related individual differences can backfire—arecent body of research shows that temporal di-versity in teams can hurt performance (Mohammed& Nadkarni, 2011).

Third, developers of time management trainingprograms might want to draw lessons from the tem-poral decision-making literature. As time manage-ment research shows, the “mechanics” of timemanagement (e.g., using to-do lists, schedulers, andcalendars) are not always related to time manage-ment outcomes, suggesting that time managementtools will likely prove ineffective if people makecounterproductive time decisions. When peoplebecome more mindful of the potential biases loom-ing over their decision making, they become lesslikely to fall prey to them. The possibility of alteringpeople’s likelihood to succumb to a temporaldecision-making bias has been shown experimen-tally (e.g., Soman, 2001), which offers hope that timemanagement training can be improved if developersincorporate decision-making elements in theirprograms.

CONCLUSION

Everybody needs to manage time. Entrepreneurs,executives, expatriates, and academics alike dependon it to organize their professional and personallives. However, our review of the literature revealsa rather scattered body of knowledge and in-consistent findings regarding the relationship be-tween timemanagement and the critical outcomes ofwell-being and performance. By integrating timemanagement research from different domains, wedistilled three perspectives that help us make senseof the mixed findings: time structures and norms,time-related individual differences, and temporaldecision making. With the advent of the knowledgeeconomy, work has become ever more flexibleand the burden of time management is graduallyshifting from organizations to employees, making

324 NovemberAcademy of Management Perspectives

Page 17: 2017, Vol. 31, No. 4, 309 330

time management an increasingly vital skill. Wehope our manuscript will help make time manage-ment research accessible to a wide range of scholarsand that the perspectives offered here will stimulatemuch-needed research andpractices on this importanttopic.

REFERENCES

Adams, G. A., & Jex, S. M. (1999). Relationships betweentime management, control, work-family conflict, andstrain. Journal of Occupational Health Psychology, 4,72–77.

Aguinis, H., Boyd, B. K., Pierce, C. A., & Short, J. C. (2011).Walking new avenues in management researchmethods and theories: Bridging micro and macrodomains. Journal of Management, 37, 395–403.

Aguinis,H., Davis,G. F., Detert, J. R., Glynn,M.A., Jackson,S. E., Kochan, T., & Kossek, E. E., et al. (2016). Usingorganizational science research to addressU.S. federalagencies’ management and labor needs. BehavioralScience & Policy, 2, 67–76.

Aguinis, H., Edwards, J. R., & Bradley, K. J. (2017). Im-proving our understanding of moderation and media-tion in strategic management research. OrganizationalResearch Methods, 20, 665–685.

Aguinis, H., & Glavas, A. (in press). On corporate socialresponsibility, sensemaking, and the search formeaningfulness through work. Journal of Manage-ment. doi:10.1177/0149206317691575.

Aguinis, H., & Kraiger, K. (2009). Benefits of training anddevelopment for individuals and teams, organiza-tions, and society. Annual Review of Psychology, 60,451–474.

Aguinis, H., & Lawal, S. O. (2013). eLancing: A review andresearch agenda for bridging the science–practice gap.Human Resource Management Review, 23, 6–17.

Aguinis, H., O’Boyle, E., Gonzalez-Mule, E., & Joo, H.(2016). Cumulative advantage: Conductors and in-sulators of heavy-tailedproductivity distributions andproductivity stars. Personnel Psychology, 69, 3–66.

Aguinis, H., Pierce, C. A., Bosco, F. A., Dalton, D. R., &Dalton, C. M. (2011). Debunking myths and urbanlegends about meta-analysis. Organizational Re-search Methods, 14, 306–331.

Aguinis, H., & Vandenberg, R. J. (2014). An ounce of pre-vention is worth a pound of cure: Improving researchquality before data collection. Annual Review of Or-ganizational Psychology and Organizational Behav-ior, 1, 569–595.

Alberti, L. B. (1971).TheAlbertis of Florence: LeonBattistaAlberti’s Della Famiglia. Lewisburg, PA: BucknellUniversity Press. (Original work published 1444.)

Ancona,D.G., Goodman,P. S., Lawrence,B. S., &Tushman,M. L. (2001). Time: A new research lens. Academy ofManagement Review, 26, 645–663.

Ancona, D. G., Okhuysen, G. A., & Perlow, L. A. (2001).Taking time to integrate temporal research. Academyof Management Review, 26, 512–529.

Ariely, D., & Wertenbroch, K. (2002). Procrastination,deadlines, and performance: Self-control by pre-commitment. Psychological Science, 13, 219–224.

Aurelius, M. (1949).Meditations. Chicago: Henry RegneryCompany. (Original work published 167.)

Bansal, P., Bertels, S., Ewart, T., MacConnachie, P., &O’Brien, J. (2012). Bridging the research–practice gap.Academy of Management Perspectives, 26, 73–92.

Barker, J. R. (1993). Tightening the iron cage: Concertivecontrol in self-managing teams. Administrative Sci-ence Quarterly, 38, 408–437.

Barley, S. R. (1988). On technology, time, and social order:Technically inducedchange inthe temporalorganizationof radiological work. In F. A. Dubinskas (Ed.), Makingtime: Ethnographies of high-technology organizations(pp. 123–169). Philadelphia: Temple University Press.

Barley, S. R., & Kunda, G. (2006). Contracting: A new formof professional practice. Academy of ManagementPerspectives, 20, 45–66.

Barling, J., Cheung, D., & Kelloway, E. K. (1996). Timemanagement and achievement striving interact topredict car sales performance. Journal of AppliedPsychology, 81, 821–826.

Barsade, S. G., & Gibson, D. E. (2007). Why does affectmatter in organizations? Academy of ManagementPerspectives, 21, 36–59.

Bennett, A. (1910). How to live on 24 hours a day. NewYork: George H. Doran Company.

Berger, P. L., & Luckmann, T. (1966). The social construc-tion of reality: A treatise in the sociology of knowledge.New York: Doubleday.

Bergmann, W. (1992). The problem of time in sociology:An overview of the literature on the state of theory andresearch on the “sociology of time,” 1900–82. Time &Society, 1, 81–134.

Bluedorn, A. C. (2000). Time and organizational culture. InN. M. Ashkanasy, C. P. M. Wilderom, & M. F. Peterson(Eds.),Handbookof organizational culture andclimate(pp. 117–128). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications.

Bluedorn, A. C., Kaufman, C. F., & Lane, P. M. (1992). Howmany things doyou like todo at once?An introductionto monochronic and polychronic time. Academy ofManagement Executive, 6, 17–26.

Bond, M. J., & Feather, N. T. (1988). Some correlates ofstructure and purpose in the use of time. Journal ofPersonality and Social Psychology, 55, 321–329.

2017 325Aeon and Aguinis

Page 18: 2017, Vol. 31, No. 4, 309 330

Boomsma, D., Busjahn, A., & Peltonen, L. (2002). Classicaltwin studies and beyond.Nature Reviews Genetics, 3,872–882.

Bosco, F. A., Aguinis, H., Singh, K., Field, J. G., & Pierce,C. A. (2015). Correlational effect size benchmarks.Journal of Applied Psychology, 100, 431–449.

Britton, B. K., & Tesser, A. (1991). Effects of time-management practices on college grades. Journal ofEducational Psychology, 83, 405–410.

Brock, M. E., Martin, L. E., & Buckley, M. R. (2013). Timetheft in organizations: The development of the timebanditry questionnaire. International Journal of Se-lection and Assessment, 21, 309–321.

Burt, C. D. B., & Kemp, S. (1994). Construction of activityduration and time management potential. AppliedCognitive Psychology, 8, 155–168.

Burt, C. D. B., Weststrate, A., Brown, C., & Champion, F.(2010). Development of the time management envi-ronment (TiME) scale. Journal of Managerial Psy-chology, 25, 649–668.

Cannon-Bowers, J. A., & Bowers, C. (2011). Team develop-mentand functioning. InS.Zedeck (Ed.),APAhandbookof industrial and organizational psychology, Vol. 1:Buildinganddeveloping theorganization (pp.597–650).Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.

Chang, A., & Nguyen, L. T. (2011). Themediating effects oftime structure on the relationships between timemanagement behaviour, job satisfaction, and psycho-logical well-being. Australian Journal of Psychology,63, 187–197.

Claessens, B. J. C., Van Eerde, W., Rutte, C. G., & Roe, R. A.(2004). Planning behavior and perceived control of timeatwork. JournalofOrganizationalBehavior,25, 937–950.

Claessens, B. J. C., Van Eerde, W., Rutte, C. G., & Roe, R. A.(2007). A review of the time management literature.Personnel Review, 36, 255–276.

Cooper, D., Doucet, L., & Pratt,M. (2007).Understanding inmultinational organizations. Journal of Organiza-tional Behavior, 28, 303–325.

DeFrank, R. S., Konopaske, R., & Ivancevich, J. M. (2000).Executive travel stress: Perils of the road warrior.Academy of Management Perspectives, 14, 58–71.

DeNisi, A. S., & Toh, S. M. (2005). A local perspective toexpatriate success. Academy of Management Execu-tive, 19, 132–146.

DeVoe, S. E., & Pfeffer, J. (2007). Hourly payment andvolunteering: The effect of organizational practices ondecisions about time use. Academy of ManagementJournal, 50, 783–798.

DeVoe, S. E., & Pfeffer, J. (2011). Time is tight: How highereconomic value of time increases feelings of timepressure. Journal of Applied Psychology, 96, 665–676.

Diener, E., Lucas, R. E., & Oishi, S. (2002). Subjective well-being: The science of happiness and life satisfaction.In C. R. Snyder & S. J. Lopez (Eds.), Handbook ofpositive psychology (pp. 63–73). Oxford, UK: OxfordUniversity Press.

Drucker, P. F. (1967). The effective executive. Oxford, UK:Butterworth-Heinemann.

Drummond,H. (2014). Escalation of commitment:When tostay the course? Academy of Management Perspec-tives, 28, 430–446.

Edwards, J. R., Cable, D. M., Williamson, I. O., Lambert,L. S., & Shipp, A. J. (2006). The phenomenology of fit:Linking the person and environment to the subjectiveexperience of person-environment fit. Journal ofApplied Psychology, 91, 802–827.

Evans, J. A., Kunda, G., & Barley, S. R. (2004). Beach time,bridge time, and billable hours: The temporal struc-ture of technical contracting. Administrative ScienceQuarterly, 49, 1–38.

Feldman, L. P., & Hornik, J. (1981). The use of time: Anintegrated conceptual model. Journal of ConsumerResearch, 7, 407–419.

Flaherty, M. G. (1999).Awatched pot: How we experiencetime. New York: New York University Press.

Flaherty, M. G. (2003). Time work: Customizing temporalexperience. Social Psychology Quarterly, 66, 17–33.

Flaherty, M. G. (2011). The textures of time: Agency andtemporal experience. Philadelphia: Temple Univer-sity Press.

Francis-Smythe, J. A., & Robertson, I. T. (2003). The im-portance of time congruity in the organisation.Applied Psychology, 52, 298–321.

Franklin, B. (1964). Poor Richard’s almanack. New York:Paddington Press.

Fritz, C., Lam, C. F., & Spreitzer, G. M. (2011). It’s the littlethings that matter: An examination of knowledgeworkers’ energy management. Academy of Manage-ment Perspectives, 25, 28–39.

Giddens, A. (1984). The constitution of society: Outline ofthe theory of structuration. Berkeley, CA: Universityof California Press.

Goodman, J. K., Cryder, C. E., & Cheema, A. (2013). Datacollection in a flat world: The strengths and weak-nesses of Mechanical Turk samples. Journal of Be-havioral Decision Making, 26, 213–224.

Graham,R. J. (1981).Theroleofperceptionof timeinconsumerresearch. Journal of Consumer Research, 7, 335–342.

Granqvist,N.,&Gustafsson,R. (2016).Temporal institutionalwork.AcademyofManagement Journal,59, 1009–1035.

Grant, A. M., Christianson, M. K., & Price, R. H. (2007).Happiness, health, or relationships? Managerial

326 NovemberAcademy of Management Perspectives

Page 19: 2017, Vol. 31, No. 4, 309 330

practices and employee well-being tradeoffs. Acad-emy of Management Perspectives, 21, 51–63.

Greenhaus, J. H., & Beutell, N. J. (1985). Sources of conflictbetween work and family roles. Academy of Man-agement Review, 10, 76–88.

Hafner, A., Oberst, V., & Stock, A. (2014). Avoiding pro-crastination through time management: An experi-mental intervention study. Educational Studies, 40,352–360.

Hafner, A., & Stock, A. (2010). Time management trainingand perceived control of time at work. Journal ofPsychology, 144, 429–447.

Hafner, A., Stock, A., & Oberst, V. (2015). Decreasing stu-dents’ stress through time management training: Anintervention study.European Journal of Psychology ofEducation, 30, 81–94.

Hafner, A., Stock, A., Pinneker, L., & Strohle, S. (2014).Stressprevention througha timemanagement trainingintervention: An experimental study. EducationalPsychology, 34, 403–416.

Hall, B., &Hursch, D. (1982). An evaluation of the effects ofa time management training program on work effi-ciency. Journal of Organizational Behavior Manage-ment, 3, 73–96.

Hall, E. T. (1959). The silent language. Garden City, NY:Anchor Books.

Hecht, T. D., & Allen, N. J. (2005). Exploring links be-tween polychronicity and well-being from theperspective of person-job fit: Does it matter if youprefer to do only one thing at a time? Organiza-tional Behavior and Human Decision Processes,98, 155–178.

Hofstede, G. (2001).Culture’s consequences: Comparingvalues, behaviors, institutions, and organizationsacross nations (2nd ed.). Thousand Oaks, CA: SagePublications.

Jacobs, J. A., & Gerson, K. (2004). The time divide: Work,family, and gender inequality. Cambridge, MA: Har-vard University Press.

Janicik, G. A., & Bartel, C. A. (2003). Talking about time:Effects of temporal planning and time awarenessnormsongroupcoordination andperformance.GroupDynamics, 7, 122–134.

Jaques, E. (1982). The form of time. New York: Crane,Russak & Company.

Javidan,M., Dorfman, P.W., de Luque,M. S., & House, R. J.(2006). In the eye of the beholder: Cross cultural les-sons in leadership from Project GLOBE. Academy ofManagement Perspectives, 20, 67–90.

Jex, S. M., & Elacqua, T. C. (1999). Time management asa moderator of relations between stressors and em-ployee strain. Work and Stress, 13, 182–191.

Joo, H., Aguinis, H., & Bradley, K. J. (2017). Not all non-normal distributions are created equal: Improvedtheoretical and measurement precision. Journal ofApplied Psychology, 102, 1022–1053.

Kahneman, D. (2003). A perspective on judgment andchoice: Mapping bounded rationality. American Psy-chologist, 58, 697–720.

Kaser, P. A. W., Fischbacher, U., & Konig, C. J. (2013).Helping and quiet hours: Interruption-free time spanscan harm performance. Applied Psychology, 62,286–307.

Kaufman, C. F., Lane, P. M., & Lindquist, J. D. (1991). Timecongruity in the organization: A proposed quality-of-life framework. Journal of Business andPsychology, 6,79–106.

Kaufman-Scarborough, C., & Lindquist, J. D. (1999). Timemanagement and polychronicity: Comparisons, con-trasts, and insights for the workplace. Journal ofManagerial Psychology, 14, 288–312.

Kelly, W. E. (2003). No time to worry: The relationshipbetweenworry, time structure, and timemanagement.Personality and IndividualDifferences, 35, 1119–1126.

Koch, C. J., & Kleinmann, M. (2002). A stitch in time savesnine: Behavioural decision-making explanations fortime management problems. European Journal ofWork and Organizational Psychology, 11, 199–217.

Konig, C. J., &Kleinmann,M. (2005). Deadline rush: A timemanagement phenomenon and its mathematical de-scription. Journal of Psychology, 139, 33–45.

Konig, C. J., & Kleinmann, M. (2006). Individual differ-ences in theuseof timemanagementmechanics and intime discounting. Individual Differences Research, 4,194–207.

Konig, C. J., & Kleinmann, M. (2007). Time managementproblems and discounted utility. Journal of Psychol-ogy, 141, 321–334.

Konig, C. J., Kleinmann,M., &Hohmann,W. (2013). A fieldtest of the quiet hour as a timemanagement technique.Revue Europeenne de Psychologie Appliquee, 63,137–145.

Kreiner, G., Hollensbe, E., & Sheep, M. (2009). Balancingborders and bridges: Negotiating the work-home in-terface via boundary work tactics. Academy of Man-agement Journal, 52, 704–730.

Lakein, A. (1973).How to get control of your time and yourlife. New York: Signet.

Lang, D. (1992). Preventing short-term strain through time-management coping. Work and Stress, 6, 169–176.

Levine, R. V., & Norenzayan, A. (1999). The pace of life in31countries. Journal ofCross-Cultural Psychology,30,178–205.

2017 327Aeon and Aguinis

Page 20: 2017, Vol. 31, No. 4, 309 330

Liu, O. L., Rijmen, F., MacCann, C., & Roberts, R. (2009).The assessment of timemanagement inmiddle-schoolstudents. Personality and Individual Differences, 47,174–179.

Macan, T. H. (1994). Time management: Test of a processmodel. Journal of Applied Psychology, 79, 381–391.

Macan,T.H. (1996). Time-management training: Effects ontime behaviors, attitudes, and job performance. Jour-nal of Psychology, 130, 229–236.

Macan, T. H., Shahani, C., Dipboye, R. L., & Phillips, A. P.(1990). College students’ time management: Correla-tions with academic performance and stress. Journalof Educational Psychology, 82, 760–768.

Malatras, J. W., Israel, A. C., Sokolowski, K. L., & Ryan, J.(2016). First things first: Family activities and rou-tines, time management and attention. Journal ofApplied Developmental Psychology, 47, 23–29.

Malhotra,A.,Majchrzak,A.,&Rosen,B. (2007).Leadingvirtualteams.AcademyofManagement Perspectives, 21, 60–70.

Markman, G. D. (2012). Entrepreneurs’ competencies. InR. J. Baum, M. Frese, & R. A. Baron (Eds.), The psy-chology of entrepreneurship (pp. 67–92). New York:Psychology Press.

Markman, G. D., Devinney, T. M., Pedersen, T., & Tihanyi,L. (2016). Global entrepreneurship: Assessment andchallenges. In G. D. Markman, T. M. Devinney,T. Pedersen, & L. Tihanyi (Eds.), Global entrepre-neurship: Past, present & future (Advances in in-ternational management, Volume 29) (pp. 35–43).Bingley, UK: Emerald Group Publishing.

Martin, L. E., Brock, M. E., Buckley, M. R., & Ketchen, D. J.(2010). Time banditry: Examining the purloining oftime in organizations. Human Resource ManagementReview, 20, 26–34.

Martineau, J. (2015). Time, capitalism and alienation: Asocio-historical inquiry into the making of moderntime. Boston: Brill.

McGrath, J. E., &Rotchford,N.L. (1983). Timeandbehaviorin organizations. InL.L.Cummings&B.M.Staw (Eds.),Research in organizational behavior (pp. 57–101).Greenwich, CT: JAI Press.

Misra, R., & McKean, M. (2000). College students’ aca-demic stress and its relation to their anxiety, timemanagement, and leisure satisfaction. AmericanJournal of Health Studies, 16, 41–51.

Mogilner, C., Chance, Z., &Norton,M. I. (2012). Giving timegives you time. Psychological Science, 23, 1233–1238.

Mohammed, S., & Nadkarni, S. (2011). Temporal diversityand team performance: The moderating role of teamtemporal leadership. Academy of Management Jour-nal, 54, 489–508.

Moore,W.E. (1963).Man, time,andsociety.NewYork:Wiley.

Mumford, L. (2010). Technics and civilization. Chicago:University of Chicago Press.

Nippert-Eng, C. (1996). Calendars and keys: The classifi-cation of “home” and “work.” Sociological Forum, 11,563–582.

Nonis, S.A., Fenner,G.H.,&Sager, J.K. (2011). Revisiting therelationship between time management and job perfor-mance.World Journal of Management, 3, 153–171.

Nonis, S. A., & Sager, J. K. (2003). Coping strategy profilesused by salespeople: Their relationships with per-sonal characteristics and work outcomes. Journal ofPersonal Selling & Sales Management, 23, 139–150.

Nonis, S. A., Teng, J. K., & Ford, C. W. (2005). A cross-cultural investigation of time management practicesand job outcomes. International Journal of Intercul-tural Relations, 29, 409–428.

Okada, E. M., & Hoch, S. J. (2004). Spending time versusspending money. Journal of Consumer Research, 31,313–323.

Okhuysen, G. A., & Bechky, B. A. (2009). Coordination inorganizations: An integrative perspective. Academyof Management Annals, 3, 463–502.

Orlikowski, W. J., & Yates, J. (2002). It’s about time: Tem-poral structuring in organizations. Organization Sci-ence, 13, 684–700.

Orpen, C. (1994). The effect of time-management trainingon employee attitudes and behavior: A field experi-ment. Journal of Psychology, 128, 393–396.

Peeters, M. A. G., & Rutte, C. G. (2005). Time managementbehavior as a moderator for the job demand-controlinteraction. Journal of Occupational Health Psychol-ogy, 10, 64–75.

Penn, W. (1794). Fruits of solitude, in reflections andmaxims relating to the conduct of human life (11thed.). Philadelphia: Printed by Benjamin Johnson.

Perlow, L. A. (1999). The time famine: Toward a sociology ofwork time.Administrative Science Quarterly, 44, 57–81.

Perlow, L. A., Mazmanian, M., & Hansen, E. (2016).Unlocking the temporal paradox: Creating temporalcontrol in an era of the ideal worker. Symposium:Making time for time: Extending theories of tempo-rality in and across organizations. Academy of Man-agement Conference, Anaheim, CA.

Pierce, C. A., Block, R. A., & Aguinis, H. (2004). Cautionarynote on reporting eta-squared values frommultifactorANOVA designs. Educational and PsychologicalMeasurement, 64, 916–924.

Rapp, A. A., Bachrach, D. G., & Rapp, T. L. (2013). Theinfluence of timemanagement skill on the curvilinearrelationship between organizational citizenship be-havior and task performance. Journal of Applied Psy-chology, 98, 668–677.

328 NovemberAcademy of Management Perspectives

Page 21: 2017, Vol. 31, No. 4, 309 330

Rogelberg, S. G., & Stanton, J. M. (2007). Introduction:Understanding and dealing with organizational sur-vey nonresponse. Organizational Research Methods,10, 195–209.

Rothbard, N. P., & Ollier-Malaterre, A. (2016). Boundarymanagement. In T. D. Allen & L. T. Eby (Eds.), TheOxford handbook of work and family (pp. 109–122).New York: Oxford University Press.

Rupp, D. E. (2011). An employee-centered model of orga-nizational justice and social responsibility. Organi-zational Psychology Review, 1, 72–94.

Sackett, P. R., Lievens, F., Van Iddekinge, C. H., & Kuncel,N. R. (2017). Individual differences and their mea-surement: A review of 100 years of research. Journalof Applied Psychology, 102, 254–273.

Saini,R.,&Monga,A. (2008).HowIdecidedependsonwhatI spend: Use of heuristics is greater for time than formoney. Journal of Consumer Research, 34, 914–922.

Salas, E., Kozlowski, S. W. J., & Chen, G. (2017). A centuryof progress in industrial and organizational psychol-ogy: Discoveries and the next century. Journal ofApplied Psychology, 102, 589–598.

Schneider, B., Ehrhart, M. G., & Macey, W. H. (2011). Per-spectives on organizational climate and culture. InS. Zedeck (Ed.), APA handbook of industrial and or-ganizational psychology, Vol. 1: Building and de-veloping the organization (pp. 373–414).Washington,DC: American Psychological Association.

Schriber, J. B., & Gutek, B. A. (1987). Some time di-mensions of work: Measurement of an underlying as-pect of organization culture. Journal of AppliedPsychology, 72, 642–650.

Searle, J. R. (1995). The construction of social reality. NewYork: The Free Press.

Seneca, L. A. (2014). Hardship and happiness. Chicago:University of Chicago Press. (Original work published50.)

Shaffer, M. A., Kraimer, M. L., Chen, Y.-P., & Bolino, M. C.(2012). Choices, challenges, and career consequencesof global work experiences: A review and futureagenda. Journal of Management, 38, 1282–1327.

Shahani, C., Weiner, R., & Streit, M. K. (1993). An in-vestigation of the dispositional nature of the timemanagement construct. Anxiety, Stress, and Coping,6, 231–243.

Shipp, A. J., Edwards, J. R., & Lambert, L. S. (2009). Con-ceptualization and measurement of temporal focus:The subjective experience of the past, present, andfuture.Organizational Behavior andHumanDecisionProcesses, 110, 1–22.

Slaven, G., & Totterdell, P. (1993). Time managementtraining: Does it transfer to the workplace? Journal ofManagerial Psychology, 8, 20–28.

Soman, D. (2001). The mental accounting of sunk timecosts: Why time is not like money. Journal of Behav-ioral Decision Making, 14, 169–185.

Southerton, D. (2003). “Squeezing time”: Allocating prac-tices, coordinating networks and scheduling society.Time & Society, 12, 5–25.

St. Benedict. (1975). The rule of St. Benedict. Garden City,NY: Image Books. (Original work published 530.)

Stone, L. (2008, July 3). Quiet time at Intel. Bloomberg Busi-nessweek. Retrieved fromhttps://www.bloomberg.com/news/articles/2008-07-02/quiet-time-at-intel

Suddaby, R. (2010). Editor’s comments: Construct clarityin theories of management and organization. Acad-emy of Management Review, 35, 346–357.

Teigen, K. H., &Karevold, K. I. (2005). Looking back versuslooking ahead: Framing of time and work at differentstages of a project. Journal of Behavioral DecisionMaking, 18, 229–246.

Thompson, E. P. (1967). Time, work-discipline, and in-dustrial capitalism. Past & Present, 38, 56–97.

Trueman, M., & Hartley, J. (1996). A comparison betweenthe time-management skills and academic perfor-mance of mature and traditional-entry universitystudents. Higher Education, 32, 199–215.

Vaara, E., & Whittington, R. (2012). Strategy-as-practice:Taking social practices seriously. Academy of Man-agement Annals, 6, 285–336.

Van Eerde, W. (2003). Procrastination at work and time man-agement training. Journal of Psychology, 137, 421–434.

Van Eerde, W. (2015). Time management and pro-crastination. In M. D. Mumford &M. Frese (Eds.), Thepsychology of planning in organizations: Researchand applications (pp. 312–333). London: Routledge.

Van Hoye, G., & Lootens, H. (2013). Coping with un-employment: Personality, role demands, and timestructure. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 82, 85–95.

Vinton, D. E. (1992). A new look at time, speed, and themanager. Academy of Management Executive, 6,7–16.

Wanberg, C., Griffiths, R. F., & Gavin, M. B. (1997). Timestructure and unemployment: A longitudinal in-vestigation. Journal of Occupational and Organiza-tional Psychology, 70, 75–95.

Woolfolk, A. E., & Woolfolk, R. L. (1986). Time manage-ment: An experimental investigation. Journal ofSchool Psychology, 24, 267–275.

Wright, M., Robbie, K., & Ennew, C. (1997). Venture capi-talists and serial entrepreneurs. Journal of BusinessVenturing, 12, 227–249.

Zahra, S. A., & Wright, M. (2011). Entrepreneurship’s nextact.AcademyofManagement Perspectives, 25, 67–83.

2017 329Aeon and Aguinis

Page 22: 2017, Vol. 31, No. 4, 309 330

Zampetakis, L. A., Bouranta, N., &Moustakis, V. S. (2010).On the relationship between individual creativity andtime management. Thinking Skills and Creativity, 5,23–32.

Zerubavel, E. (1976). Timetables and scheduling: On thesocial organization of time. Sociological Inquiry, 46,87–94.

Zerubavel, E. (1979a). Patterns of time in hospital life.Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

Zerubavel, E. (1979b). Private time and public time:The temporal structure of social accessibility andprofessional commitments. Social Forces, 58,38–58.

Zerubavel, E. (1981). Hidden rhythms. Berkeley, CA: Uni-versity of California Press.

Zimbardo, P. G., & Boyd, J. N. (1999). Putting time in per-spective: A valid, reliable individual-differencesmetric. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,77, 1271–1288.

Zimbardo, P. G., & Boyd, J. N. (2008). The time paradox:The new psychology of time that will change your life.New York: The Free Press.

Brad Aeon ([email protected]) is a Ph.D. candi-date at the John Molson School of Business at ConcordiaUniversity. He conducts research on time and timemanagement.

Herman Aguinis ([email protected]; www.hermanaguinis.com) is the Avram Tucker Distinguished Scholar andProfessor of Management at George Washington Univer-sity School of Business. His research is interdis-ciplinary and addresses the acquisition and deploymentof talent in organizations and organizational researchmethods. He has published about 150 academic articlesand five books and received numerous awards, includingthe AOM Research Methods Division Distinguished Ca-reer Award for lifetime contributions, Michael R. LoseyHuman Resource Research Award by Society for HumanResource Management Foundation for lifetime achieve-ment in human resource management research, and best-article-of-the-year award from Academy of ManagementPerspectives, Personnel Psychology, Organizational Re-search Methods, and Journal of Organizational Behavior.He is a Fellow of AOM, the American Psychological As-sociation, the Association for Psychological Science, andthe Society for Industrial and Organizational Psychologyand currently serves on the editorial boards of 14 journals.

330 NovemberAcademy of Management Perspectives