24
pre-publication copy Vietnamese Australians’ perceptions of the trustworthiness of police 1. Helen McKernan 2. Leanne Weber Abstract Trust is a central concern in the policing of ethnically and racially identified communities. A challenge for contemporary policing research on trust, as for related concerns such as confidence or cooperation, is to relate the quality of personal police trust encounters to public views about the trustworthiness of the policing institution. Within the criminological literature on policing, many quantitative empirical studies are directed towards the measurement and comparison of how confidence, trust, effectiveness and fairness are related to community perceptions of police practice. Overall there is a general scarcity of integrated frameworks applied to empirical studies of trustworthiness although the organisation and leadership literature offer some models. In this qualitative study, a sociological framework is used to explore Vietnamese Australian’s perceptions of police trustworthiness. The paper investigates the key factors that influence Vietnamese Australians’ perceptions of police trustworthiness using dimensions of trustworthiness described by Sztompka, namely: reputation, performance, appearance, and accountability. Keywords Trust; trustworthiness; community policing; Vietnamese; ethnic groups Introduction Gaining trust is a paramount concern in policing, particularly in relation to ethnically and racially identified communities. Because trust is deeply implicated in community feelings of safety it’s central to the reform of policing institutions in postmodern societies (Goldsmith, 2005). The building of trust is especially difficult in multi-ethnic environments when communities are characterised by low social capital, social disadvantage or face complex security problems (Hulse and Stone, 2007). In addition building trust in ethnically or racially identified communities may be affected by past negative experiences of police or authorities and refugee trauma. Policing approaches are challenged when ethnic community values and normative behaviours are not reflected in policing practice. Tankebe (2013) suggests that shared values and common beliefs underpin normative behaviours to cooperate or not cooperate with police. Other scholarship suggests policing operations perpetuate rather than address existing social exclusions and inequalities by reinforcement of disadvantage through ethnic or racial profiling or a failure to address the specific security needs of minority communities (Bowling, 2007). Innes et al. (2009) warn police practitioners against treating all communities as if they are the same; instead they suggest an assessment of the trust building assets necessary for each ethnic or racial community. Other scholars argue that empirical research that focuses on location-specific

Vietnamese Australians' perceptions of the trustworthiness of police

  • Upload
    monash

  • View
    0

  • Download
    0

Embed Size (px)

Citation preview

pre-publication copy

Vietnamese Australians’ perceptions of the trustworthiness of police

1. Helen McKernan 2. Leanne Weber

Abstract

Trust is a central concern in the policing of ethnically and racially identified communities. A challenge for contemporary policing research on trust, as for related concerns such as confidence or cooperation, is to relate the quality of personal police trust encounters to public views about the trustworthiness of the policing institution. Within the criminological literature on policing, many quantitative empirical studies are directed towards the measurement and comparison of how confidence, trust, effectiveness and fairness are related to community perceptions of police practice. Overall there is a general scarcity of integrated frameworks applied to empirical studies of trustworthiness although the organisation and leadership literature offer some models. In this qualitative study, a sociological framework is used to explore Vietnamese Australian’s perceptions of police trustworthiness. The paper investigates the key factors that influence Vietnamese Australians’ perceptions of police trustworthiness using dimensions of trustworthiness described by Sztompka, namely: reputation, performance, appearance, and accountability.

Keywords

Trust; trustworthiness; community policing; Vietnamese; ethnic groups

Introduction

Gaining trust is a paramount concern in policing, particularly in relation to ethnically and racially

identified communities. Because trust is deeply implicated in community feelings of safety it’s central

to the reform of policing institutions in postmodern societies (Goldsmith, 2005). The building of trust

is especially difficult in multi-ethnic environments when communities are characterised by low social

capital, social disadvantage or face complex security problems (Hulse and Stone, 2007). In addition

building trust in ethnically or racially identified communities may be affected by past negative

experiences of police or authorities and refugee trauma. Policing approaches are challenged when

ethnic community values and normative behaviours are not reflected in policing practice. Tankebe

(2013) suggests that shared values and common beliefs underpin normative behaviours to cooperate

or not cooperate with police. Other scholarship suggests policing operations perpetuate rather than

address existing social exclusions and inequalities by reinforcement of disadvantage through ethnic or

racial profiling or a failure to address the specific security needs of minority communities (Bowling,

2007). Innes et al. (2009) warn police practitioners against treating all communities as if they are the

same; instead they suggest an assessment of the trust building assets necessary for each ethnic or

racial community. Other scholars argue that empirical research that focuses on location-specific

pre-publication copy

relations between ethnic communities and police is essential to the implementation of

neighbourhood policing in ethnically or racially differentiated communities (Murphy and Cherney,

2011a; Bowling and Phillips, 2003b; Pickering et al., 2007; Grossman et al., 2013).

This study of Vietnamese Australian communities in Melbourne investigates how culture, structural

factors and experiences of police affect perceptions of the trustworthiness of police. The study

employs sociological understandings of trust to address empirical issues of trust, an approach which

Jackson (2007) suggests is underutilised in criminological research. The study makes a contribution to

the literature on police relations from the perspective of an ethnic minority; a field of interest because

of the increasing complexity in policing multi-ethnic cities.

Trust and trustworthiness

Integrated studies of trustworthiness are challenged by a general lack of frameworks or models in the

applied literature, although some trust models are found in the organisation and leadership literature

(Mayer and Norman, 2004). Trust is common to human experience but in empirical studies has proven

elusive and difficult to investigate. Fukuyama (1995: 26) defines trust as 'the expectation that arises

within a community of regular, honest, and cooperative behavior, based on commonly shared norms,

on the part of other members of the community'. Sztompka (1999: 26) uses a straightforward

definition of trust as 'committing ourselves to action’ and the metaphor, ‘placing a bet’ to indicate the

anticipatory nature of trust acts. Mayer, Davis, and Schoorman (1995: 712) define trust as the

'willingness of a party to be vulnerable to the actions of another party based on the expectation that

the other will perform particular action important to the trustor, irrespective of the ability to monitor

or control that other party'.

Trust is differentiated from trustworthiness as trust as a behavioral intention and trustworthiness a

judgment made by the trustor ( the actor who creates a trust) about the trusted (actor or agency in

whom trust is invested) (Mayer et al., 1995). The decision by a potential trustor as to whether or not

to confer trust in another party, is based on the other party’s perceived trustworthiness (Sztompka,

1999). Trustworthiness according to these conceptions, integrates the act of trusting by the trustor

with the expected trustworthiness of the trusted. While trustworthiness is mostly relational, it may

include psychological and cultural elements (Hardin, 2002; Sztompka, 1999). The perceived

trustworthiness of an organisation affects strategic outcomes and the capacity to form partnerships

(Mayer and Norman, 2004).

pre-publication copy

Luhmann (1980) argues that trust and risk should be considered together, since trust arises only in

situations where there is recognised risk. While the act of trust requires the trustor to make a positive

assessment of the trustworthiness of the trusted, the trusted does not necessarily have to make a

similar assessment of the trustor. The one-way nature of trust thus entails a risk to the trustor, for the

reason that the act of the trustor occurs ‘irrespective of the ability to monitor or control that other

party' (Mayer et al., 1995: 712; Mayer and Norman, 2004). Distrust is described by Sztompka (1999:

26) as the ‘negative mirror image of trust’. Because trust investments in the untrustworthy are risky

and perhaps even dangerous, some scholarship suggests distrust should not be viewed as problematic

but instead as a necessary and functional aspect of daily living (Hardin, 2002; Sztompka, 1999). In the

case of policing, Goldsmith (2005: 447) suggests distrust of police by ethnic or other marginalised

groups is a necessary means of self-protection and if police intend to overcome distrust, they need to

show ‘tangible indicators’ of increased trustworthiness.

Sztompka’s (1999) integrated conceptualisation of trustworthiness is based on experiential elements

that provide clues for trustor actors to make assessments of the degree of trustworthiness of trusted

actors. The indicators or trustworthiness are affected by contextual factors in which the relations

between trustor and trusted are enacted. The key elements directly influencing trustworthiness

assessments of the trusted are reputation, performance and appearance. These elements are

influenced by indirect contextual factors that include culture and accountability.

Of the three elements that establish trustworthiness, reputation is considered the main mechanism,

particularly in traditional cultures. Sztompka (1999: 71) defines reputation as ‘the record of past

deeds’ of individuals, institutions or other social objects. Performance, as defined by Stompka (1999),

includes effectiveness and the motivation that drives the trusted to perform. Unlike reputation that

hinges on whether the trusted is acting in or out of character, performance relates to the current

perceptions of conduct. Sztompka (1999) contends performance gives trustors less reliable clues of

trustworthiness than reputation because it requires an evaluation of the potential future

trustworthiness of trustees, based on their current actions. Predictable and consistent actions are

claimed to create a sense of certainty and enable people to assess whether the trusted have behaved

in accordance with expectations. Appearance, as utilised by Sztompka (1999) refers to the external

characteristics of the physical body, civility and dress and their influence on trust judgements.

Sztompka (1999) contends that when trustors make observations about the trusted’s appearance,

they unconsciously use these to make prejudgements about personality, identity or status (Sztompka,

1999). A significant factor affecting trustworthiness cues is whether the trusted is perceived to have

pre-publication copy

cultural or values alignment with the trustor. Accountability mechanisms provide incentives and

motivation for the trusted to demonstrate trustworthy behaviours and impose sanctions to

discourage unacceptable behaviours. The effectiveness of the accountability process relies on the

power to act effectively, to ensure the trusted acts in the interest of trustors. While these monitoring

systems may maintain the anonymity of the complainant, Sztompka (1999) states anonymity cannot

be a condition for the trusted.

Other scholarship endorses the significance of individual aspects of Sztompka’s conception and

framework for understanding trustworthiness. Hardin (2002) similarly prioritises reputation as a key

factor in trust relations and further suggests a good reputations is self-reinforcing because it’s an

incentive to live up to expectations. Connerton (1989) claims past observations, memories,

biographies, rumours and stories form the fabric of reputation and consequently history as a general

rule, cannot be severed from reputation. Other scholarship links competency and consistency of

performance to trustworthiness and whether institutional measures of performance measures meet

community expectations of effectiveness (Mayer et al., 1995; Nooteboom, 2002; A K Mishra, 1996;

Goldsmith, 2005). Loader et al. (2007: 101) describe the maintenance of a public image of neutrality

and consistency as ‘a precarious reputational prize’. Local accountability and involvement in the police

process are rated by Goldsmith (2005) as the main imperative for building trust in police in societies

emerging from conflict and authoritarian rule. Lewis et al.(2008) similarly contend that how

complaints are handled and how the outcomes are communicated to the public, strongly influences

satisfaction with police. Goldsmith (2005: 464) advocates the need to build a reflective type of trust

whereby ‘“reasons to trust”need to be identified, reiterated and built upon in the context of

police/citizen contacts and relationships’

Empirical studies of factors affecting trustworthiness of police in ethnic minorities

Racially and ethnically identified minorities consistently report more negative interactions with police

than other communities (Weitzer and Tuch, 2004; Lai, 2010). The most negative evaluations of police

occurred when groups or individuals believed they were targeted by police. Bowling et al. (2003a) link

distrust to discriminatory policing including aggressive policing tactics, excessive use of stop and

search powers and stereotypical attitudes of police towards members of racially identified

communities. Studies in Cabramatta similarly found that over-zealous drug policing strategies

implemented for Vietnamese Australian drug users and offenders had deleterious effects on the

targeted individuals and damaged relations in the wider Vietnamese Australian community (Dixon and

pre-publication copy

Maher, 2002). Young people in Australia from ethnically and racially identified communities claimed

they were targeted by police and treated with a lack of respect (Grossman and Sharples, 2010;

Grossman et al., 2013).

A study by Bradford and Jackson (2010) attributed trust to three areas: effectiveness of the system

and individual officers; fairness; and demonstration of values that align with those of the community.

Overriding all these factors they found low trust towards police in communities impeded trust

relations even where strenuous efforts are made to demonstrate procedural fairness. Other studies

show similar trends across the U.K., U.S. and Australia for ethnic and racially identified cultural groups

with consistently low levels of trust towards police (Murphy and Cherney, 2011a; Pickering et al., 2007;

Sharp and Johnson, 2009; Tyler, 2005; Fagan and Davies, 2000). Rosenbaum et al. (2005) concluded

that prior attitudes towards police far outweighed more current contrary experiences, which led him

to conclude the establishment of trust with disenfranchised ethnic groups posed a ‘formidable

challenge’ (Rosenbaum et al., 2005: 362).

Stereotypes of police may be reinforced through ‘confirmation bias’ whereby only information that is

consistent with established perceptions is selected and opposing information is ignored (Rosenbaum

et al., 2005). Asymmetry in the effects of good and bad encounters with police has also been found to

affect evaluations of police performance, with far greater impact arising from bad experiences

(Skogan, 2006). In fact, Skogan’s research shows that both favourable and unfavourable encounters

with police lowers people’s appraisal of police and no contact at all with police has the most positive

effect on public attitudes. The study by Rosenbaum et al (2005: 354) of the public’s vicarious

experiences of police officers was more predictable in that they found when people indirectly received

positive or negative information about police, it instilled in the recipient, respectively, more positive

or negative attitudes towards police. Unexpectedly, when contrasted with Skogan’s (2006) study,

Rosenbaum et al (2005) found positive vicarious experiences had greater impact than negative

vicarious experiences on changing public attitudes toward police.

The importance of procedural justice is widely recognized within the literature although some

research indicates procedural justice may be less effective in encouraging cooperation with police

among different cultural groups (Tyler, 2005; Tyler and Huo, 2002; Murphy and Cherney, 2011b;

Tankebe, 2013). In a study by Innes et al. (2007), police effectiveness was a key concern to young

Muslim men and led to lowered perceptions of police trustworthiness. The study demonstrated how

‘reassurance policing’ overcame ‘trust deficits’ through ‘cycles of trust building’ by targeting and fixing

pre-publication copy

crimes of concern to the local communities (Innes et al., 2007). Other scholarship casts doubt on a

direct relationship between effectiveness and trust mainly because reassurance policing has covert

intentions of increasing intelligence flows which may compromise motivational trust (Pickering et al.,

2008). Public perceptions of the effectiveness of police depend on whether citizens or police initiated

the contact, with police effectiveness being more highly rated in citizen-initiated contacts (Schafer et

al., 2003; Skogan, 2006). The English-speaking language proficiency of citizens also affects the

frequency and type of contacts with police officers (Skogan, 2005).

The ethnic or racial representation of police officers is a highly politicised aspect of appearance. In

advanced democracies government and police recruitment policies have espoused the value of police

forces that reflect the broader society. Strategies have met with some successes, although generally

for police forces in Australia and other English speaking countries proportional representation of the

racial and ethnic groups present in the wider community has not been achieved (Victoria Police, 2003;

Victoria Police, 2009; Sharp and Atherton, 2007). The most visible aspect of a police officer’s

appearance and identity, the uniform, is intended to signal to the public the presence of an effective

police force (Bradford and Jackson, 2010). Innes (2004) claims the symbolic power of policing is as

important as its instrumental functions and that the uniform along with the foot patrol and other

practices are the cultural symbols that connote protection. However, police uniforms cannot be

assumed to be a signal for reliability and trustworthiness, as for some ethnic groups the signal may

have a different or opposite effect. A study in the U.S. found that officers wearing traditional uniforms

were perceived by the white public as helpful and competent when compared with officers in casual

attire and yet recognisable as police. In contrast, for black residents in the same vicinity the uniform

reinforced negative and hostile attitudes to police (Mauro, 1984: :55). Studies show that it is

commonplace for police to make assessments of various publics based on visible cues for race,

ethnicity or social status which subsequently influences who police officers subject to stop and search

procedures (Quinton, 2011; Bowling and Sheptycki, 2011; Phillips and Bowling, 2003).

Methodology

The paper draws on qualitative data from an ARC Linkage Project1 on trust relations between police

and Vietnamese Australian communities in Melbourne and the implications for policing. The project

1 ARC Linkage Project LPO776899 Exploring the experience of security in the Australian Vietnamese

community: practical implications for policing

pre-publication copy

investigated Victorian police officers’ experience of professional relations with members of

Vietnamese Australian communities and the attitudes of Vietnamese Australians to police, crime,

security and community policing. This paper analyses the data from the Vietnamese Australian

participants to investigate how direct and indirect experiences of police and contextual factors shaped

perceptions of police trustworthiness. The study was conducted in three Police Service Areas with high

populations of people with Vietnamese heritage.

The research tools were focus groups and semi-structured interviews that incorporated investigative

and grounded theory approaches to allow for the emergence of unexpected or new insights into old

problems (Strauss and Corbin, 1997). Eleven focus groups were conducted and involved 100

Vietnamese Australian community members. The interviews and focus groups were conducted in

Vietnamese or English dependent on participants’ primary language. Participants for the focus groups

included generation one and two residents; women and men; employed and unemployed people; and

representatives from three localities. Generation one Vietnamese Australians were defined in this

study as people born in Vietnam and generation two Vietnamese Australians as born in Australia with

at least one parent born in Vietnam and also for this study includes people who were born in Vietnam

and migrated to Australia under the age of eleven. This definition of generation two accommodates

the way in which the respondents thought about themselves and incorporates the influence of social

and political contexts for determining membership of a generation (Wyn and Woodman, 2006). Youth

in the study referred to any respondents who were 24 years of age or younger. In response to advice

from the reference groups, separate focus groups were conducted for generation one and generation

two and separate focus groups for men and women in generation one. Table 1 provides a summary of

the key characteristics of the participants and the methods used for data collection. Because social

justice workers were interviewed in regard to their professional knowledge of the crime and policing

issues in the community and not their personal experiences as members of the community, gender is

the only demographic provided for this data group.

(INSERT TABLE 1[HM1])

Snowball sampling was used to recruit participants for the focus groups from Vietnamese Australian

networks including: committees and volunteers associated with the Buddhist religion; professional

networks; mothers’ groups; community workshop groups; and social networks. Researchers attended

the focus groups at the venues provided by Vietnamese community organisations to administer the

ethics requirements and answer questions. The focus group schedule asked participants about

pre-publication copy

situations that fostered feelings of lack of safety and insecurity in their everyday lives, their reporting

of crime, and their views on how police dealt with crime and other issues of concern. A training session

for Vietnamese Australian facilitators was conducted at the university in order to attain a degree of

consistency and expose any potential cultural misunderstandings in the focus group schedules.

Nineteen semi-structured interviews were conducted with selected Vietnamese Australians working

in the legal, social or community sectors who are referred to collectively in this article as social justice

workers. Interviewees from the community sector worked in community liaison positions at county

courts or for Vietnamese Australian associations or did paid or voluntary work at drug rehabilitation

or needle exchange NGOs. The sample of social justice workers also included social workers and

lawyers. The snowball sampling method was used to recruit interviewees as the approach is a useful

technique for groups that are closed or reticent to be involved in research (Corbetta, 2003). The

researchers from the project conducted the interviews all of which were in English. An additional 13

semi-structured interviews were conducted with Vietnamese Australian past offenders all of whom

had multiple drug offences and many of whom had past prison sentences. The interviewees were

recruited by Vietnamese Australian facilitators who used the snowball method to find participants

from community organisations or drug rehabilitation organisations. The interview schedule explored

offenders’ experiences of police and the justice sector. All interviews with past offenders were

conducted in Vietnamese and their responses if recorded translated into English. For offenders who

did not agree to a recorded interview, the interviewer wrote their responses in English.

The data analysis reported factors affecting perceptions of police trustworthiness by general

community members, social justice workers and past offenders. After initially coding emergent

themes, it became apparent that the data showed a strong compatibility with Sztompka’s (1999) three

elements of trustworthiness which were subsequently applied as a framework for the analysis. The

study addresses two research questions, the first is: What are the main factors that influence

Vietnamese Australians assessments of the trustworthiness of police in the areas of reputation,

performance and appearance? The second related research question is: How are these assessments

affected by contextual factors? We do not claim that the research represents the views of all

Vietnamese Australians or can be generalised to other Vietnamese Australians. This is a specific

account that provides insights into how and why Vietnamese Australians in several regions within

Melbourne evaluate police trustworthiness.

Findings on reputation of police

pre-publication copy

Comparisons with police in Vietnam

Victorian police were favourably compared to police from Vietnam. Officers from the Department of

Police Vietnam were said to be untrustworthy because of their character, their relationship with the

government or corruption. They were described as puppets of the communist regime and frequently

referred to as generally bad people. The reputation of police in Vietnam was damaged by the violent

and discriminatory pre-migration experiences of Vietnamese people who left the country as refugees

or migrated following the end of the war in Vietnam in 1975. More recent experiences for many family

members in the Republic of Vietnam rekindled a distrust in police because of persistent corruption, a

view consistent with the location of Vietnam in 2010 on an International, Corruption Perceptions Index

with a given ranking of 116 in a list of 178 countries (Transparency International, 2012). When asked

if there was any similarity between the local police in Australia and police in Vietnam a social justice

worker responded:

Nothing at all. We are only afraid of [Vietnamese police]. We don't respect them. And we hate them

because we think that they have all the powers and they can do anything to you any way they want. So

normal people would avoid the police and the criminals would buy the police.(INC02)

The difference between police forces in Vietnam and Australia was explained by research participants

as primarily related to the distinction between a communist and democratic country. Vietnamese

Australians believed that in Australia police were mandated to keep people safe whereas in Vietnam

people were vulnerable to political and localised manipulation. Understanding the relationship of

police to the state was important to Vietnamese Australians and was a reason why they chose to live

in a democratic country. Vietnamese Australian men, many of whom were past members of the South

Vietnamese army, strongly endorsed a role for police in maintaining the security of the State. For many

men the trustworthiness of police in Australia was related to an expected role to ‘maintain order in

society’. Nonetheless, the male supporters who commented on the role of police in protecting society,

also said that the force consisted of good and bad characters, which for one respondent meant

wariness was essential when dealing with police. Another respondent’s view was the police were not

any different from any social group: ‘As with any group of people, there exist good and bad characters.

The police are no exception. Citizens must put their trust in the law, the police and the authorities’

(FGS03).

Research participants were clear that they understood the different roles of police in the two

countries. In Vietnam police were viewed as oppressors whereas in Australia police were part of

democratic governance. Similarly Dixon and Maher (2002) found in their study that Vietnamese

Australians did not conflate Australian police with police from their country of origin and held high

pre-publication copy

expectations for the behaviour of police in a liberal democracy. While Vietnamese Australians

respondents in this study emphasised they understood police operations in Australia were different

from Vietnam, it was apparent the adverse reputation of police in Vietnam affected their trust in the

local police in Australia. Some people spoke of persistent feelings of uncertainty and lack of

trustworthiness that emanated from Vietnam but remained with them in relation to police in

Australia:

I think in any culture trust in relationships is important. I think coming from Vietnam, which is a country

of authority, power held by the police force and government, people have a negative attitude to the

police. So some people may bring with them and carry with them that attitude.(INC17)

The historical memory of police in the Republic of Vietnam was revealed in patterns of distrust and

discordance between the cognitive and the emotional responses. Overcoming damaged trust from

the past is difficult according to Sztompka (1999)as it is embedded as psychological reactions which

need remediation through new experiences of consistent performance from the trusted whereby new

reputations are built over time. Traumatic experiences in Vietnam with police and other authorities

had left a deep-seated, perhaps sublimated, fear of police.

Corruption claims

Negative attitudes were not solely due to experiences in Vietnam. Research participants claimed the

police force in Melbourne was not corruption-free but agreed only a minority of police officers were

corrupt. Some community members in the study showed their preconceptions of corruption of police

changed when the police force acted in the 1990s to remove corruption from their neighbourhood.

Even so, one participant suggested ‘there are still a small number of corrupt police officers’ (FGY01).

Interviewees and focus group members were not questioned directly about corruption, but the issue

emerged in general responses about perceptions and attitudes towards police. Corrupt practices were

almost entirely recounted in relation to the policing of drug crimes in the three neighbourhoods in the

study. The least number of comments on corruption were raised in the focus groups and as they were

comprised of people from across the communities they were more representative of normative

attitudes. Most comments on corruption were made in interviews with social justice workers based

on their work with drug offenders or by the drug offenders themselves, most of whom had multiple

convictions and whose life experiences were based at the coalface of drug use, crime and policing. No

details were given on the sources of information about the corruption and the researchers have no

way of verifying whether the accounts are authentic. Social justice workers claimed they felt excluded

from reporting corrupt practices because of the lack of a safe public reporting process. Lodging a

pre-publication copy

formal complaint was explained as counter-cultural because deference to authority was a cultural

disposition in the Vietnamese community. Some explained the reluctance to report originated from

feeling they didn’t have the right to question officers.

Given that Vietnamese Australian communities are highly networked, incidents of police corruption

achieved wide circulation. Even a small element of corruption was experienced vicariously by the

wider community and appeared to have a long lasting and damaging effect on the reputation of police.

This is consistent with Rosenbaum et al.’s (2005: 360) study that found vicarious experience to be an

important factor in shaping attitudes towards police, particularly for racially and ethnically defined

groups. For one social justice worker, the major impact of accounts of corruption was rekindling

awareness of past insecurities experienced in Vietnam.

Because they come from a system that was so corruptible, what faith do they have about our system in

Australia? Especially when there is corruption as well, I’m not saying the whole Police Force is, I’m saying

there is (some).(INC14)

Past offenders appeared less concerned by police corruption than the social justice workers and

community members. This finding aligns with Rosenbaum’s (2005: 347) de-sensitivity hypothesis,

which the authors encapsulate as: ‘This is nothing new to us—we expect it’. The strongest criticism

was expressed by social justice workers and arose from the opposite position: they expected

professionalism. The asymmetry in the attention given to negative over positive stories and negative

bias towards police were contributing factors to perceptions of police corruption (Rosenbaum et al.,

2005; Skogan, 2006; Brown and Benedict, 2002).

Findings on police performance

Policing home burglaries

When asked about safety and crimes of concern to their community, participants in focus groups

designated home burglaries as a crime of concern. While police may have regarded home burglaries

as a crime not warranting their attention unless it involved involve harm to an individual, the

participants clearly used it as a crime on which to judge policing effectiveness. Respondents used three

indicators to judge performance in policing home burglaries: police response times to their calls; the

care or concern shown for the victim; and police’s capacity to catch the offender or find the stolen

goods. Stories of prompt attendance left good impressions of police performance even if they

occurred many years previously such as in the following incident:

pre-publication copy

Burglars also broke into my house ten years ago. At the time, the police came right after I called. They

were good and enthusiastic. The burglars broke my door to get in the house so the door was locked from

inside. The police were willing to get in through the window. … The police seemed nice, they were quite

big but still went through the small window to get in and opened the door for me.(FGS03)

Non-attendance at the crime scene was understood by many respondents as evidence of lack of

concern for Vietnamese Australians. A respondent said: ‘I call them up, but they don’t seem to care

and help’ (FGS05) another claimed it was racism ‘I think it’s more likely racial discrimination, if I were

white they would come quickly’ (FGS03). Many Vietnamese Australians did not know why police did

not attend or assist with home burglaries and interpreted this as lack of interest or motivation.

Sztompka (1999), as does Tyler (2005), emphasises the public’s understanding of police’s motivations

is a critical aspect of performance and trustworthiness. One person stressed the failure to attend the

crime was experienced as a failure to help them:

When you call the police, they not going to come, or they can’t solve this problem, so half the Vietnamese

they have this same perception too, or they have this incident and they call, they’re not coming, so they’re

not helping.(INC16)

Comments by police officers stated that the stolen possessions were unlikely to be returned were

understood as poor performance. The return of stolen goods which were often cash, gold jewellery

or other items traditionally kept as an investment were important for Vietnamese Australians. Data

on Australian household burglaries shows money (19%) and jewellery (18%) were the most commonly

stolen items (AIC, 2011). Police did not provide updates on progress and other studies show that if

police share information on progress victims are more satisfied with police (Tyler and Huo, 2002;

Sunshine and Tyler, 2003; Skogan, 2006). Many Vietnamese Australians said they did not have home

property insurance and this was commonplace in the community. Consequently Vietnamese

Australians had different expectations concerning the policing of burglaries from Anglo Australians,

the majority of whom report burglaries to submit an insurance claim and do not expect a police

response.

The perceived lack of performance discouraged reporting; one man expressed the disincentive as

making people feel ‘tired’ and ‘not trust police’ (FGS04). Another person in the same focus group

believed it would lead to a more insecure and dangerous society: ‘If incidents like this keep increasing

then it would be really dangerous, society should take a serious view of this matter’ (FGS04). Home

burglaries were crimes on which the community based judgements about their exposure to collective

risks and could be considered a signal crime from their perspective. Innes (2004: 162) defines signal

pre-publication copy

crimes as ‘any criminal incident that causes change in public behaviour and/or beliefs’. Police’s

apparent apathy to respond contributed further to feelings of social disorder. Innes (2004; 2009)

claims police have to shift their standpoint from dictating crime priorities to understand instead the

incidents they find troublesome and the visible signal crimes that generate feelings of social order.

The insecurity beyond the immediate incidents of home burglaries were expressed by this respondent:

Although it's a minor incident but if it is allowed to continually happen again and again, it will reduce our

confidence in relying on police security. This time we may have lost our property, but in the future, it may

be our lives that are lost.(FGS04)

Stompka (1999)claims consistency is critical in building reputation and police’s failure to respond to

calls consequently contributed directly to a reputation of unreliability. In the few accounts provided

where police did arrive promptly, usually described as within half an hour, the action created a good

impression, although Skogan’s (2006) asymmetry hypothesis raises questions about the relative

impact of the reported positive and negative experiences .

Policing drug crimes

The policing of drug crimes was almost exclusively police-initiated and was judged by the community

members as ineffective. Vietnamese Australians were reticent to report drug crimes but all the same

expected outcomes because they were highly concerned by issues of morality and the social

consequences of these crimes. Generation one and two gave different reasons for the lack of police

performance. Generation one men disagreed with policing approaches, sentencing periods and what

they saw as too-comfortable jail conditions, adding that none of these were deterrents. Generation

two did not agree harsher penalties would solve the drug-related crime, and instead, supported a

multi-agency response.

Reasons for not reporting drug-related crime included fear of reprisals and refusal to be witnesses.

Some participants described a cultural preference to not get involved: ‘People usually think that since

this does not concern us, we should not get involved’ (FGS04). Generation two respondents were

more willing to report crimes to police, but similarly to generation one, respondents did not want to

be witnesses or have any further contact with police. A generation two male said he would be too

frightened to be a witness for fear of payback from the offender’s gang.

pre-publication copy

We should just report what we feel is happening and then hang up the phone; we should only cooperate

with the police to that extent. In the event that the police ask us to remain there and be witnesses, I would

not do so as if the gangs see me calling the police, I would be a dead man.(FGF08)

Vietnamese Australian respondents did not feel safe to make contact with police to pass on

information or report drug-related insecurities experienced in the high rise public housing including

theft, intimidation, exposure of children to drug injecting in public areas and requests for young

mothers to hide drugs in their baby’s rooms. Adopting a no-contact approach with police prevented

further visits from police to their premises or requests for information, as most claimed they feared

further damage to their property such as their car windows being smashed by offenders. Other

contributing factors to non-reporting were a lack of English and Vietnamese face-saving behaviours

that kept crime or drug issues hidden in families.

Prejudice and bias

A widespread criticism in the community generally was that police stereotyped Vietnamese

Australians as belonging to a drug-crime culture; many claimed to be treated as if they were guilty first

rather than innocent. People generally accepted that it was not only police who made these

assumptions but the broader community. In particular, they blamed the media for stereotyping

Vietnamese people as associated with drugs through casting them in drug-related roles in police

television dramas. Others said the pre-judgements by police were racial in that being Asians was

sufficient to be criminalised; ‘they think of crime and the Asians’ (FGY11): ‘Just say if they’re white,

then they’ll treat them with respect and it’s like us Asians, they’d be like downgrading us and just not

respecting us’(FGY11).

In minor car accidents, some generation one drivers claimed police gave preference to a white

person’s account of an incident over a Vietnamese Australian’s account. Anglo Australians, they said,

were forthright in giving their opinion and usually managed to gain the full attention of police.

Vietnamese Australians preferred to only answer questions when asked by an officer, rather than

volunteering information. They claimed to be further disadvantaged in explaining technical aspects of

traffic laws with limited English without an interpreter which police often did not provide for minor

car accidents. The lack of opportunity to tell their side of the story to police led to strong feelings of

unfairness, which is broadly consistent with research demonstrating the priority placed on procedural

fairness (Skogan, 2005; Tyler, 2001).

pre-publication copy

In contrast to generation one for whom police prejudice was a general belief, generation two and

youth with Vietnamese heritage were divided on the issue. Some generation two respondents thought

that while the policing institution itself was not prejudiced against people with Vietnamese

backgrounds, some individual police officers were biased. This differentiation between interpersonal

bias and institutional bias, allowed this group to accept a degree of racism among certain officers

without developing a negative view of all police. Their bicultural position enabled them to make

observations from Australian and Vietnamese cultural standpoints. They believed that because police

already had a suspicious attitude towards Vietnamese people, cultural misunderstandings occurred in

communications involving police officers with limited experience of other cultures.

But I think there are still misunderstandings, so cultural differences that some officers… I think in a

group when I think of the police aren’t like that, but I think it does come down to individuals as well

and some may not be as exposed to different cultures or have an understanding there (FGS10).

Male youths who participated in the study vociferously claimed they were subjected to police bias in

stop and check processes. More male youth than female youth said that police officers were biased,

with male youth expressing the most negative views of police. The most positive views of police were

expressed by female university students who said that some perceptions of being targeted were

simply police following procedures. This segmentation of views of fairness is consistent with the

empirical findings in the wider study that showed perceptions of targeting on the basis of ethnicity,

race or visible appearance whether true of not, damages trust and trustworthiness of police (Weber,

2010).

Unethical behaviours

In Australia 58.6% of Vietnamese-born people belong to the Buddhist religion (Ben-Moshe and Pyke,

2012)and a requirement of respectful behaviours by police for all people whatever their background

featured as an important theme among respondents. Recounted incidents of unethical behaviours by

some police were circulated in the network and damaged the reputation of police some localities.

Whereas corruption was usually spoken of as indirect experiences by community members, the

accounts given of unethical behaviours were first hand observations or accounts from family

networks. One participant observed an incident whereby police allegedly rough handled an Aboriginal

drug user: ‘he had a spray can and he was sniffing it, the police came and they were really rough with

the man’ (FGY11). These views are supported by Tyler’s (2005) contention that institutional trust is

pre-publication copy

contingent on police exercising their professional responsibilities for every citizen.

Vietnamese Australian social justice workers claimed their clients with drug-related offences were

often subjected to rough handling or violence by police officers. Social justice workers differentiated

between breaches of interpersonal trust through unprofessional behaviours from individual officers

and a wider belief they held in the institutional trustworthiness of the police force (Tyler, 2005;

Bradford and Jackson, 2010). One social justice worker commented: ‘I don’t mean every policeman is

bad, there’s lot of good policemen out there, but there are a few bad police to make the Vietnamese

feel very different about them because of the treatment’ (INC06). Another social justice worker

believed that it was inevitable and part of the job that police developed prejudice towards ex-

offenders, or offenders. ‘But I reckon say if you are in the police force for a while, you would develop

that kind of mind. You would look at junkies as junkies, offenders as offenders’ (INC02). Views on

police were modified when relationships were formed between social justice workers and police

officers through participation in community policing activities such as youth camps. Collaboration with

officers provided opportunities for demonstrating alignment of values in mutual concern for youth

and enabled a re-evaluation of the bad stories heard from offender clients about police, as an

inevitable part of the job. A Vietnamese Australian working in the legal system who understood the

pressures on police in handling difficult criminals, nonetheless made the following observation about

the loss of trust through rough handling, trickery and coercion:

Coppers, I don’t blame them, they deal with criminals and criminal activity on a daily basis so their view’s

not that great of offenders, they treat them very roughly and they don’t treat them with respect. I’m not

blaming them, I mean you get difficult people, who’re drugged out, alcohol fuelled, so they’re

manhandled quite a lot, most of the time, I mean they trick them into talking, try to get them on board

by just being friendly with them, but then get all the answers out of them in a recorded interview so,

they’re not completely upfront with them and I can’t see how that can illicit any trust, or respect. (INC12)

The fifteen past-offenders claimed to have experienced police abuse which they said included

intimidation or violence. Past-offenders spoke of expecting nothing better; they spoke as if violence

or abuse from police was a fact of life. Incidents of humiliation appeared to cause the most anxiety,

particularly acts that shamed them in front of their community such as a reputed incident where an

offender claimed to be walked in his underwear through public areas by officers and claims from other

past offenders of public strip-searches. Rough handling was more of a concern to the offenders than

claims of venality, which some offenders described as a transactional aspect of their dealings with

police officers. Past-offenders referred to police officers as Anh Hai (big brother). One past-offender

spoke of the mismatch between calling police officers big brother and their behaviours: ‘They’re not

pre-publication copy

really any big brother really. They just take too much, because they think they have got authority they

kind of, you know, take advantage of it’ (INF007). Incidents to build trust among past offenders were

few but powerful when they occurred, as shown by one past-offender’s memory of an officer who

took the time to speak with him as an individual:

One time and after he sit down, after he arrest me so many time he’s just, or actually questioned me so

many times, or harassed me so many times he just sat down and talked to me, ‘Why you keep on doing

this?’.(INOF07)

These findings support the view that trust remains a paramount concern even for sub-groups that are

more vulnerable to misuse of police powers and where trust is likely to be low (Goldsmith, 2005).

Findings on appearance

The two main components of Sztompka’s (1999) appearance dimension, civility and visible

appearance, were both commented on negatively. Disrespect was commented on by generation one

and two respondents in various settings including a Buddhist religious centre, homes, on the street or

at the police station and the incidents involved rudeness, dismissiveness or patronising behaviours

from police officers. Some respondents objected to the language used by police, in particular,

swearing in front of family members which people claimed sometimes insulted the entire extended

family. One Vietnamese woman interpreted disrespectful behaviours from police officers as implying

Vietnamese Australians were from a socially subordinate group: ‘the police officer just waved his hand

and did not answer her question implying that we are of lower class in this country’ (FGF07).

Inappropriate behaviours by police reportedly included not speaking to the oldest family member or

using incorrect family titles such as addressing any older women as grandma.

The visible appearance of Victoria Police was perceived by respondents as a white Anglo force. This is

consistent with the statistics at the time that showed extremely low numbers of Vietnamese

Australian officers in the force and an over-representation of white Anglo officers (McKernan, 2008).

Differences in race and ethnicity between officers and people of Vietnamese heritage, was perceived

as a basis for unfair treatment: ‘Here the policemen are almost exclusively Western, we are

Vietnamese, Asian, and so we feel that there is a certain degree of discrimination’ (FGS04). It was

widely believed that Asian or Vietnamese Australian police would not show prejudice towards

Vietnamese Australians. Rather contradictorily research participants claimed more Vietnamese

Australian police would be beneficial because they would understand the culture, parents nonetheless

pre-publication copy

strongly discouraged their daughters or sons to pursue a career as a police officer. The general reason

given by generation two for their parent’s resistance to them becoming a police officer was because

it was considered a low status and dangerous occupation. In addition, height was perceived as a

barrier to entry into the police force by Vietnamese Australians despite the changes made to

recruitment in 2001 that reduced/eliminated height requirements.

The physical appearance of police officers was also criticised with gender distinctions evident in the

different reasons given by women and men for their poor appraisals. Women commented on the

general countenance of officers as frightening. One Vietnamese woman who worked as a counsellor

spoke of the conflicting feelings of fear and yet respect for the work of police, as illustrated by this

example:

I respect the police, I admire the young women police…….. I saw the young women police, I wanted to talk

to them but they had a cold face. I know they need to be strong to do their job. I wanted to tell them that

‘Oh yes I support you’.(INC01)

A focus group participant contrasted the frightening aspects of the physical appearance of police

officers with their verbal communications which she did not find frightening: ‘They're big they look

scary, when they talk they don't talk like that’ (FGFAC). Women who could not speak English, did not

have the opportunity to experience a potentially different aspect of the officers’ appearance. In

contrast to these women’s perceptions of police officers as intimidating, prior research showed police

officers viewed both male and female members of Vietnamese Australian communities as

unintimidating and respectful; assessments based on physical characteristics and general demeanour.

Both generation one and two women described the prospect of walking into a police station as ‘very

intimidating’ (FGFAC). Some men referred to police cars and weapons as invoking fear.

Generation one men related trust assessments to inadequate physical attributes and a consequent

lack of capacity to defend civilians or catch offenders. They described police officers as not fit and

lacking in muscular strength. Strong bodies and high levels of fitness were attributes that promoted

confidence in their ability. Local police officers were also described as lazy by some Vietnamese

Australian men, a behaviour they linked to an inferior and ineffective police service. Australian police

were compared unfavourably to police in the U.S. where American police were stereotyped as strong

and athletic in appearance and Australian police as weak and overweight with ‘very big bellies’

(FGS03). Another respondent in the same focus group suggested: ‘our police need to improve to be

stronger, be athletic and more muscular’ (FGS03). Consistent with Sztompka’s (1999: 58) scholarship

on gender differences in attributes associated with trust, this study found gender differences in trust

pre-publication copy

attributes. Women related trust to ‘soft’ attributes such as benevolence and helpfulness and men

related trust to ‘hard’ attributes, in particular physical effectiveness.

Conclusion

Thirty years on from the settlement of people from Vietnam in Australia, police had not established a

reputation of being trustworthy in these communities. As recognised in the literature, negative

reputational effects from respondents’ past experiences with police in Vietnam played a significant

part in Victorian police being assigned a poor reputation. However it was also evident that

respondents’ past experiences of police and authorities in Vietnam, did not entirely account for the

reputational challenges faced by police in Victoria. The breaches to the trustworthiness expectations

participants held for police in a democratic country, whether true or not, were widely commented on

and significantly undermined trust in police. Reputations of the members of Victoria Police were most

damaged by their reputed involvement in incidents of violence or corruption. When incidents

involving local police in Australia mirrored the bad behaviours of police in Vietnam, the negative

reputational effects for local police were amplified.

While it was agreed that violators of ethical standards may be a small minority of officers, the effects

were widely spread through the closely networked communities. Participants’ lack of trust in police

did not originate from the question of whether police officers were entitled to implement the law; in

fact it was quite the contrary; respondents reiterated that police were a necessary part of the

democratic system. Nonetheless reputations of local officers were fragile and contingent on a

consistent demonstration that police in Australia were different from police in Vietnam particularly in

delivering ethical policing. The findings support other studies that highlight the centrality of ethical

behaviours in shaping perceptions of police officer trustworthiness, particularly among groups

previously exposed to violence and authoritarian rule. The findings indicate the need for ethnically

appropriate and trusted accountability processes that encourage reporting of unethical behaviours.

The research showed that the police crime priorities were implemented without knowledge of local

insecurities. Policing of street-level drug related crimes, a priority crime for police in the three Police

Service Areas was judged as entirely unsuccessful by all participants. While the areas that police

focused on were in the high density ‘Vietnamese’ business shopping streets or markets, few

participants spoke about experiencing risk or fear in these busy trading precincts (McKernan and

Scambary, 2011). Instead, many spoke of insecurities associated with drug dealing after hours in the

immediate vicinities of their flats or residences. Police and the Vietnamese residents experienced the

pre-publication copy

local areas they inhabited entirely differently and the incidents or threats causing fear and emotional

or physical insecurity for Vietnamese Australians in their local environment were not made known to

police. Some viewed the policing of street drug traders and drug users cynically, saying it placated the

non-Vietnamese Australian public by targeting low level Vietnamese drug dealers or users with high

rates of recidivism because they made the public street spaces appear risky for non-Vietnamese

Australians. A fear of police requests for Vietnamese Australians to act as witnesses to crime, and

cultural reticence to speak in public about crime in their family or community contributed further to

the lack of communication and trust between the Vietnamese communities and police. The study

indicated a lack of police who were skilled and/or motivated to uncover the risks and safety concerns

of Vietnamese Australian community members which contributed to a perception of ineffectiveness

in local crime management. It was apparent that the police standpoint highlighted elements of

neighbourhoods considered problematic in the wider Australian community, and these did not align

with the Vietnamese Australian respondents’ perception of the everyday risks and insecurities they

experienced.

A rigid adherence to priorities identified through ‘intelligence-led’ policing approaches, resulted in lost

opportunities for police to regenerate trustworthiness. The policing of home burglaries in these

communities was demonstrated, by all accounts from participants, to be of low importance to police

but was a crime of concern raised by many respondents. When contacted about home burglaries,

police officers’ slow response to calls was interpreted as a lack of concern for Vietnamese Australian

people and inability to solve house burglary crime. The inconsistency in police response to calls was

in itself a factor that directly undermined police reputations, as consistency according to Sztompka

(1999) is foundational to reputation. The poor performance on home burglary crime was extended by

some respondents to questioning the capability of the police force to protect Vietnamese Australians

if faced with more serious safety threats. Performance was the active dynamic of trustworthiness in

which new reputations could have been forged or strengths reinforced, however police rarely built on

the key opportunities offered in these citizen-reported crimes.

The widely held belief that police officers criminalised Vietnamese Australian communities by

stereotyping members as drug-traders was a major impediment to assessing police officers as

trustworthy. Some participants experienced these pre-judgements as a risk in their encounters with

officers because they felt they were judged first as guilty and treated with suspicion, rather than

being more universally trusted, at least from the outset, to be innocent.The risk embedded in the

one-way nature of trust relations whereby Vietnamese Australian trustors are expected to be ‘willing

pre-publication copy

to be vulnerable to the actions of police’, without the ability to control or monitor them, entailed a

positive risk for the Vietnamese Australians who held strong beliefs that police did not trust them

(Mayer et al., 1995: 712). The contention at the heart of this is whether you can trust a person or

organisation you believe does not trust you. This poses a paradox for policing a community located

in high drug use locations and with a significant proportion of offenders, which fosters suspicion for

both parties and is consequently self-reinforcing of low-trust relations.

This study emphasises the powerful influence on perceptions of police trustworthiness of ethical

behaviours and the importance of culturally inclusive processes for making complaints which

provides feedback to complainants. Police reputations are fragile, easily damaged but also malleable

and open to renegotiation if police demonstrate effectiveness, fairness and attentiveness to the

crimes of concern to community members in all encounters.

Police intentions to operationalise the results of this study in order to foster a more trustworthy

relationship with Vietnamese Australians, could integrate the dual partners of improved

effectiveness and reputation to guide action. The actions taken have to reach the Vietnamese

speaking community members, and there were scant examples of information from police reaching

generation one. The daughters and sons, the second generation, also have reservations about the

police force in relation to bias and intention to assist their communities, indicating that action is

required rather than complacent hopes of acculturation. The study indicated [HM2]new professional

skills are required to enable police to respond not only to the most obvious aspects of street-level

drug crime but also the more hidden insecurities faced by residents, and importantly, to understand

how standpoint influences the interpretation of crime and insecurity.

Bibliography

A K Mishra. (1996) Organizational responses to crisis: The centrality of trust. In: Kramer RM and Tyler TR (eds) Trust in organizations. Thousand Oaks, CA.: Sage Publications, 261-287.

AIC. (2011) Household burglary. Available at: http://www.aic.gov.au/crime_types/property%20crime/burglary.html.

Ben-Moshe D and Pyke J. (2012) The Vietnamese Diaspora in Australia: Current and Potential Links with the Homeland. Deakin University: Deakin University.

Bowling B. (2007) Fair and Effective Policing Methods: Towards ‘Good Enough’ Policing. Journal of Scandinavian Studies in Criminology and Crime Prevention 8: 15.

Bowling B and Phillips C. (2003a) Policing ethnic minority communities. In: Newburn T (ed). Willan publishing.

Bowling B and Phillips C. (2003b) Policing ethnic minority communities. In: Newburn T (ed) Handbook of Policing. Devon, UK: Willan Publishing Ltd.

Bowling B and Sheptycki J. (2011) Policing Globopolis. Social Justice 38: 19.

pre-publication copy

Bradford B and Jackson J. (2010) Trust and Confidence in the Police: A Conceptual Review. (accessed December 1 2010).

Brown B and Benedict W. (2002) Perceptions of the police: Past findings, methodological issues, conceptual issues and policy implications. Policing: An International Journal of Police Strategies & Management 25: 3-22.

Connerton P. (1989) How societies remember, Cambridge England: Cambridge University Press

Corbetta P. (2003) Social Research: Theory, Methods and Techniques, London: Sage Publications. Dixon D and Maher L. (2002) Anh Hai: Policing, Culture and Social Exclusion in a Street Heroin

Market. Policing & Society 12: 93 - 110. Fagan J and Davies G. (2000) Street stops and broken windows: Terry, race and disorder in New York

City. Fordham Urban Law Journal 28. Fukuyama F. (1995) Trust: The social virtues and the creation of prosperity, New York: Free Press.

Goldsmith A. (2005) Police reform and the problem of trust. Theoretical Criminology 9: 443-470.

Grossman M, Bruck D, Stephenson P, et al. (2013) Learning to Engage: A Review of Victoria Police Cross-Cultural Training Practices. Melbourne: Victoria University, 206.

Grossman M and Sharples J. (2010) Don’t Go There: Young people’s perspectives on community safety and policing. Melbourne: Victoria University.

Hardin R. (2002) Trust and trustworthiness: Russell Sage Foundation. Hulse K and Stone W. (2007) Social cohesion, social capital and social exclusion: A cross-cultural

comparison’. Policy Studies 28. Innes M. (2004) Reinventing Tradition?: Reassurance, Neighbourhood Security and Policing.

Criminology and Criminal Justice 4.

Innes M, Abbott L, Lowe T, et al. (2007) Hearts and minds and eyes and ears: reducing radicalisation risks through reassurance-oriented policing. unpublished

Innes M, Abbott L, Lowe T, et al. (2009) Seeing like a citizen: field experiments in 'community intelligence-led policing'. Police Practice and Research 10: 15.

Jackson J and Sunshine J. (2007) Public confidence in policing: A neo‐Durkheimian perspective. British Journal of Criminology 47.

Lai Y-LZ, Jihong Solomon. (2010) The impact of race/ethnicity, neighborhood context, and police/citizen interaction on residents' attitudes toward the police. Journal of Criminal Justice 38: 8.

Lewis C and Prenzler T. (2008) Police Oversight Agencies: Measuring Effectiveness. In: Head BW, Brown AJ and Connors C (eds) Promoting Integrity: Evaluation and Improving Public Integrity Institutions Aldershot: Ashgate,.

Loader I and Walker N. (2007) Civilizing Security. Luhmann N. (1980) Trust: A Mechanism for the Reduction of Social Complexity. Trust and Power.

New York Wiley. Mauro R. (1984) The Constable's New Clothes: Effects of Uniforms on Perceptions and Problems of

Police Officers. Journal of Applied Social Psychology 14. Mayer RC, Davis JH and Schoorman FD. (1995) An Integrative model of organizational trust. Academy

of Management Review 20: 709 - 734. Mayer RC and Norman PM. (2004) Exploring Attributes Of Trustworthiness: A Classroom Exercise.

Journal of Management Education 28. McKernan H. (2008) A multicultural police force? Representation of ethnic minorities in Victoria

Police. The Australian Sociological Association,. University of Melbourne: TASA. McKernan H and Scambary J. (2011) Final Report: Exploring the experience of security in the

Vietnamese Australian community, practical implications for policing. Swinburne Institute of Technology.

pre-publication copy

Murphy K and Cherney A. (2011a) Fostering cooperation with the police: How do ethnic minorities in Australia respond to procedural justice-based policing? Australian & New Zealand Journal of Criminology 44: 22.

Murphy K and Cherney A. (2011b) Understanding Cooperation With Police in a Diverse Society. British Journal of Criminology 52.

Nooteboom B. (2002) Trust: Forms, foundations, functions, failures, and figures, Cheltenham: Edward Elgar.

Phillips C and Bowling B. (2003) Racism, Ethnicity and Criminology. Developing Minority Perspectives. The British Journal of Criminology 43: 21.

Pickering S, McCulloch J and Wright-Neville D. (2008) Counter-terrorism policing: towards social cohesion. Crime Law Society Change 50.

Pickering S, Wright-Neville D and McCulloch J. (2007) Counter-Terrorism Policing and Culturally Diverse Communities. Clayton Victoria: Monash University.

Quinton P. (2011) The formation of suspicions: police stop and search practices in England and Wales. Policing and Society 21: 357-368.

Rosenbaum DP, Schuck AM, Costello SK, et al. (2005) Attitudes Toward the Police: The Effects of Direct and Vicarious Experience. Police Quarterly 8: 22.

Schafer JA, Huebner BM and Bynum TS. (2003) Citizen Perceptions of Police Services: Race, Neighborhood Context, and Community Policing. Police Quarterly 6.

Sharp D and Atherton S. (2007) To Serve and Protect? The Experiences of Policing in the Community of Young People from Black and Other Ethnic Minority Groups. The British

Journal of Criminology 47.

Sharp E and Johnson P. (2009) Accounting for Variation in Distrust of Local Police. Justice

Quarterly 26: 27. Skogan W. (2005) Citizen satisfaction with police encounters’. Police Quarterly 8. Skogan WG. (2006) Asymmetry in the Impact of Encounters with Police. Policing and Society: An

International Journal of Research and Policy 16: 99 - 126. Strauss A and Corbin JM. (1997) Grounded Theory in practice. California: SAGE Publications, Inc. Sunshine J and Tyler TR. (2003) The Role of Procedural Justice and Legitimacy in Shaping Public

Support for Policing. Law & Society Review 37: 513 - 548.

Sztompka P. (1999) Trust: A Sociological Theory, Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Tankebe J. (2013) Viewing things differently: The dimensions of public perceptions of police

legitimacy. American Society of Criminology 51. Transparency International. (2012) The 2012 Corruption Perceptions Index. Available at:

www.transparency.org/cpi2010/results. Tyler TR. (2001) Public trust and confidence in legal authorities: What do majority and minority

group members want from the law and legal institutions? Behavioral Sciences and the Law 19.

Tyler TR. (2005) Policing in Black and White: Ethnic Group Differences in Trust and Confidence in the Police Police Quarterly 8: 20.

Tyler TR and Huo YJ. (2002) Trust in the law, New York: Russell Sage. Victoria Police. (2003) The Way Ahead, Strategic Plan, 2003 - 2008. Melbourne: Victoria Police. Victoria Police. (2009) The Way Ahead: Connecting the Community: 5 year plan Safer Victoria 2008 -

2013. Weber L. (2010) What's trust got to do with it? A review of literature on the determinants of

legitimacy, trust and confidence in policing; why they matter; and what police can do to enhance them. Working papers for Victoria Police. University of New South Wales.

Weitzer R and Tuch SA. (2004) Race and perceptions of police misconduct. Social Problems 51. Wyn J and Woodman D. (2006) Generation, Youth and Social Change in Australia. Journal of Youth

Studies 9.

pre-publication copy