23
This is the published version Neoh, Jenni and Mellor, David 2010, Shared parenting : adding children's voices and their measures of adjustment to the evaluation, Journal of child custody, vol. 7, no. 3, pp. 155-175. Available from Deakin Research Online http://hdl.handle.net/10536/DRO/DU:30031272 Reproduced with the kind permission of the copyright owner Copyright: 2010, Taylor & Francis

Shared Parenting: Adding Children's Voices and Their Measures of Adjustment to the Evaluation

  • Upload
    deakin

  • View
    0

  • Download
    0

Embed Size (px)

Citation preview

This is the published version Neoh, Jenni and Mellor, David 2010, Shared parenting : adding children's voices and their measures of adjustment to the evaluation, Journal of child custody, vol. 7, no. 3, pp. 155-175. Available from Deakin Research Online http://hdl.handle.net/10536/DRO/DU:30031272 Reproduced with the kind permission of the copyright owner Copyright: 2010, Taylor & Francis

PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE

This article was downloaded by:On: 22 November 2010Access details: Access Details: Free AccessPublisher RoutledgeInforma Ltd Registered in England and Wales Registered Number: 1072954 Registered office: Mortimer House, 37-41 Mortimer Street, London W1T 3JH, UK

Journal of Child CustodyPublication details, including instructions for authors and subscription information:http://www.informaworld.com/smpp/title~content=t792306888

Shared Parenting: Adding Children's Voices and Their Measures ofAdjustment to the EvaluationJennifer Neoha; David Mellora

a Deakin University, Burwood, Victoria, Australia

Online publication date: 23 September 2010

To cite this Article Neoh, Jennifer and Mellor, David(2010) 'Shared Parenting: Adding Children's Voices and TheirMeasures of Adjustment to the Evaluation', Journal of Child Custody, 7: 3, 155 — 175To link to this Article: DOI: 10.1080/15379418.2010.512230URL: http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/15379418.2010.512230

Full terms and conditions of use: http://www.informaworld.com/terms-and-conditions-of-access.pdf

This article may be used for research, teaching and private study purposes. Any substantial orsystematic reproduction, re-distribution, re-selling, loan or sub-licensing, systematic supply ordistribution in any form to anyone is expressly forbidden.

The publisher does not give any warranty express or implied or make any representation that the contentswill be complete or accurate or up to date. The accuracy of any instructions, formulae and drug dosesshould be independently verified with primary sources. The publisher shall not be liable for any loss,actions, claims, proceedings, demand or costs or damages whatsoever or howsoever caused arising directlyor indirectly in connection with or arising out of the use of this material.

Shared Parenting: Adding Children’sVoices and Their Measures of Adjustment

to the Evaluation

JENNIFER NEOH and DAVID MELLORDeakin University, Burwood, Victoria, Australia

Shared parenting has been advocated to be a better arrangementfor children than sole residence and access arrangements afterparental separation. Although there is some research on this issue,studies have been restricted in their reliance on the reports ofothers. In this paper, we report on a study in Australia, in whichchildren in each of these arrangements were compared withchildren in intact families on a range of adjustment measuresand with each other in relation to their responses to their parents’separation, using both self- and parent-reporting. We found thatthere was little difference between children in the three family con-figurations, suggesting that shared parenting is not necessarilyassociated with better outcomes for the child. On other aspects ofadjustment, the children in shared parenting and sole residence=access families did not differ. We also found that parents in allgroups underestimated the emotional problems reported by chil-dren. In separated families, they also overestimated the children’sdesire for parents to re-unite. Finally, we found that parents inshared parenting families are more satisfied with their situationthan are their children, and fathers are particularly so. The find-ings suggest that the promotion of shared parenting as the bestpost-separation family structure is contestable.

KEYWORDS child adjustment, parental separation, shared care

Address correspondence to Jennifer Neoh, D. Psych., School of Psychology, DeakinUniversity, 66 Mount Street, Burwood, Victoria 3084, Australia. E-mail: [email protected]

Journal of Child Custody, 7:155–175, 2010Copyright # Taylor & Francis Group, LLCISSN: 1537-9418 print=1537-940X onlineDOI: 10.1080/15379418.2010.512230

155

Downloaded At: 02:48 22 November 2010

The idea of shared equal time parenting for children whose parents haveseparated has been taken up around the western world with enthusiasm inrecent times. In Australia, this has been enshrined in legislation throughamendments to the Family Law Act 1975. However, of some concern, therhetoric and arguments that tend to inflame discussions on this topic arerarely informed by sound research. In particular, children themselves haverarely been given a voice in evaluations of the benefits of shared parentingarrangements relative to other arrangements. Even the report of theAustralian House of Representatives Standing Committee on Family andCommunity Affairs (2003) inquiry into shared parenting, which used chil-dren’s graphic depictions of their experience of separation on the cover, paidscant attention to children’s perspectives.

This lack of attention to children’s views is reflective of the literature onthis topic, which also includes very little research on children’s experiencesof shared parenting. When children’s experiences are investigated, data areoften derived from parent or teacher reports, and the studies are generallyunderpinned by three assumptions. The first assumption is that childrenare incompetent or too immature to provide relevant information on theirown family situations (some exceptions are Gollop, Smith, & Taylor, 2000;Smart, Neale, & Wade, 2001; Sviggum, 2000; Pruett & Kline Pruett, 1999).These types of studies also assume that parent or teacher reports are reliableand valid measures of children’s adjustment. Secondly, research that reportsparental satisfaction with living arrangements rests on the assumption that ifparents are satisfied, then children will also be satisfied and well-adjusted.Finally, other research uses parent’s behavior as the benchmark, such asthe degree of parental conflict or the parents’ geographical proximity toeach other, a position similar to the Family Court’s, to investigate the impactof shared parenting on children. However, these studies rarely involvechildren’s perspective and, as a result, treat children as passive recipientsof parental behavior.

In the following, we will first consider the arguments as to why sharedparenting arrangements should be better for children whose parents separateand refer to the limited research that reports on the adjustment of childrenwho experience such arrangements. We will show that the majority of thisresearch has been informed by parent’s reports and that parents’ views arenot always congruent with those of their children. As parental separation isa circumstance in which this discrepancy may be exaggerated, we suggestthat research should pay more attention to the views of children. Few studieshave done this well. We will then describe a study in which we investigatedthe adjustment of children in different living arrangements after parental sep-aration compared with children whose parents had not separated. We alsoexamine the beliefs and attitudes of children whose parents had separatedabout their parents’ separation.

156 J. Neoh and D. Mellor

Downloaded At: 02:48 22 November 2010

WHY SHOULD SHARED PARENTING ARRANGEMENTS BEBETTER FOR CHILDREN?

Proponents of shared parenting (e.g., Bauserman, 2002; Clancy, 1990; Elkin,1991; Greif, 1979; Kruk, 1993) contend that shared parenting provides a pro-tective condition that ameliorates the effects of parental separation andresults in better adjustment outcomes for children. Therefore, children inshared parenting arrangements have been expected to show more favorableadjustment and outcomes than children who live with one parent and havecontact visits with the other. For example, Greif (1979) posited that children’sgreater contact with fathers1 after separation allowed children to have ‘‘morenatural, casual relationships’’ (p. 316) with them, and allowed fathers to havemore input into intellectual, moral, religious, and emotional developmentthan more conventional arrangements after separation. Other benefits thathave been proposed are that shared parenting facilitates the maintenanceof links with extended family members (Brown, 1994), improves the chancesof child support being paid (Luepnitz, 1991), avoids circumstances wherechildren are forced to choose one parent over the other, reduces litigationbetween parents and decreases the risks of child snatching (Schwartz,1987). Shared parents report that children tend to have better relationshipswith both parents, whereas parents report that children from more traditionalstyles of post-separation arrangements tend to have poorer relationships withtheir other parent, which tend to be fathers (Arditti, 1992; Kruk, 1993).

CHILDREN’S ADJUSTMENT AND RESPONSE TOSHARED PARENTING

Despite the aforementioned arguments, the question remains as to whetherchildren in shared parenting arrangements are better adjusted. This issue wasaddressed by Bauserman (2002) in a meta-analysis of 33 studies. He con-cluded that children from shared parenting families were better adjusted(across measures of emotional and behavioral adjustment, self esteem, familyrelations, academic performance, and specific adjustment to the separation)than children in sole residence families, and they were very similar tochildren in intact families. He attributed these findings to the fact that chil-dren from both shared parenting families and intact families have ongoingrelationships with both parents. However, it should be noted that 23 of thesestudies were unpublished, and the analysis combined shared parentingsamples with those families that had joint legal custody. It is broad methodo-logical issues such as these that make comparisons of research difficult andevaluations of shared parenting, even from parental reports of children’sadjustment, elusive.

Shared Parenting Impact on Children 157

Downloaded At: 02:48 22 November 2010

Some indication that there may be other differences in families that arenot considered in these types of comparative studies was mooted by Pearsonand Thoennes (1991). They found, in three large US studies, that it was notshared parenting that produced the better adjusted children. Rather otherfactors, such as co-operation between parents, financial stability, and theabsence of violence, were more important. Shared parenting in itself, withchildren’s more frequent contact with both parents, and parents that sharedchild responsibilities did not increase the likelihood that children were betteradjusted. Child adjustment in both of these studies was measured with theChild Behaviour Checklist, a measure of disruptive behaviour, agitation,and depression, which was completed by parents.

In contrast to the optimistic picture painted by the aforementioned stu-dies, other research has suggested that shared parenting may have a negativeimpact on children’s adjustment. Johnston, Kline, & Tschann (1989) found, ina longitudinal study across two years, that there was generally no differencein children’s well-being whether they were in one primary residence arrange-ment or shared parenting families. However, they found that children, whospent equal time with both parents (n¼ 35), were rated by their parents asmore depressed, withdrawn, and uncommunicative than children in soleresidence families (n¼ 65).

Another study by McKinnon and Wallerstein (1991) of 26 young chil-dren living in a shared parenting arrangement, found that 73% were noton course developmentally. These children, aged between one and fiveyears, were followed over three years. Parent and teacher reports suggestedthat the difficulties children experienced included distress on parental separ-ation, enuresis, anxiety, nightmares, and social difficulties. Although thesefindings were based on a sample of families referred to a counseling service,this appears to be contrary to the view that shared parenting protectschildren from the stress of parental separation.

In summary, it would appear that the assumed benefits of both parentsbeing actively involved in the care of their children has not been consistentlydemonstrated in research. Some studies using parental reports of their chil-dren’s adjustment found that shared parenting does have benefits for chil-dren. Other research has produced results that are more equivocal. Oneexplanation for these mixed results, or at least a contributing factor, mightbe that parents, and particularly separated parents who might be dealingwith a multitude of emotional and practical problems at the same time, mightnot have a full understanding or appreciation for their children’s perspective.

DISCREPANCIES BETWEEN CHILD AND PARENT REPORT

Although it has been found that often parent’s and teacher’s reports concur(De Los Reyes & Kazdin, 2004; Teja & Stolberg, 1993), some studies

158 J. Neoh and D. Mellor

Downloaded At: 02:48 22 November 2010

(e.g., Lengua, Wolchik, Sandler, & West, 2000; Tebbut, Swanston, Oates, &O’Toole, 1997) have found that there is generally little correlation betweenparent and child self-report measures of internal states such as depressionand self-esteem. For example, De Los Reyes and Kazdin (2004) found in aclinical sample of 389 children and their parents that different informantsproduced very different results.

As suggested previously, parental separation may be a particular circum-stance in which parent and child reports are unlikely to be in accord. Forexample, in an Australian Institute of Family Studies project conducted byAmato (1987), 141 separated mothers claimed that their children had a ‘‘neu-tral’’ reaction to their parents’ separation. However, when the children wereasked directly, most reported negative reactions. Similarly, Howell, Portes, &Brown (1997) asked 433 resident mothers to rate their child’s adjustment andcompared these ratings to children’s reactions to the separation and childratings of self esteem. Although their research focused on age and gendereffects, it was noted that parent responses were not consistent with children’sreported reactions to their parents’ separation. These results suggest that par-ents may be motivated to believe that children are not affected by parentalseparation, and importantly, that research that depends on parent reportsmay not capture children’s experience.

More congruence has been found when parents and children are askedabout overt behavior, such as conflict. For example, Strangeland, Pellegreno,and Lunholm (1989) found in a comparison of 33 children of separated par-ents and 28 children of intact families, that children and parents agreed whenasked about frequency of overt conflict and amount of time spent with par-ents. These behaviors could be objectively measured. However, childrenwhose parents had separated reported they had sleep difficulties, school pro-blems, increased anxiety, and feelings of insecurity of which their parentswere unaware.

CHILDREN’S PERSPECTIVES ON SHARED PARENTING

A small number of empirical studies have attempted to consider children’sperspectives on the experience of shared parenting. However, these studieshave been limited by either their extremely narrow focus, or if they take abroader aspect with qualitative interviews, they tend to concentrate on anextremely small number of families (e.g., Sharpley & Webber, 1989 inter-viewed just two children). Some early studies, using highly selective samples,found children much better adjusted after parental separation when theirparents shared responsibility for them. For example, Neugebauer (1989)reported that in a comparison of 40 children, 34 children in sole residence,and 6 in shared parenting families, children in shared arrangements experi-enced much less initial distress at parental separation. Four years later, the

Shared Parenting Impact on Children 159

Downloaded At: 02:48 22 November 2010

children reported in interviews that they had not experienced the feeling ofloss of one parent, nor adjustment problems. Further, they had more positiveviews of divorce than sole residence children. Arbanel (1979) also found thatchildren from shared parenting families reported positive effects of their fam-ilies’ arrangements and were well adjusted according to teachers’ reports.Furthermore, even in this very small sample (n¼ 6), one 12 year old childfound shared parenting to be disruptive, confusing and had contributed tohis difficulties with peer relationships.

Studies that appear to have more rigorous methodology that examinedchildren’s adjustment in shared parenting families from the child’s perspec-tive have found that shared parenting has little or no advantages over soleresidence. One of the few larger studies that asked children from shared par-enting about their experiences was a study undertaken in the United Statesby Buchannan, Maccoby, and Dornbusch (1991) who conducted telephoneinterviews with 522 children, aged between 10 and 18 years. This studyfound no difference on behavioral, emotional, or social well-being betweenthose living with predominantly one parent (n¼ 469) and those involved inshared parenting (n¼ 53). A smaller study conducted in the United Statesby Luepnitz (1991) compared a total of 91 children from 32 sole residencefamilies and 11 shared parenting families (the exact sample sizes were notreported), and found that children’s ratings of self concept was not associatedwith post-separation family structure.

A further study from the US by Glover and Steele (1989) matched 24children from sole residence, intact2 and shared parenting families. Childrenfrom shared parenting families were found to be just as ambivalent abouttheir fathers as sole residence children, a surprising finding considering thatsome have argued that increased involvement of fathers in shared parentingis a prominent factor in improving children’s experience (e.g., Kruk, 1993).Glover and Steele’s (1989) conclusions that shared parenting, from children’sperspective, is ‘‘as least as beneficial’’ (p.185) as sole residence arrangementsfor children is hardly a resounding recommendation for shared parenting.

In a larger study, Smart et al. (2001) interviewed 117 British childrenaged between 4 and 20 years. They found that children who live with sharedparenting (n¼ 65) complained of things such as moving house every week,keeping to inflexible schedules, and confusion resulting from constantchanges. Younger children described the distress of missing the parent theywere not with. Older children felt constrained by the demands of the change-over schedule, missing out on opportunities with friends, and losing freetime. This research provides some insight into the lives of children who livein shared parenting arrangements, however, it did not look at siblings rela-tionships, had vague definitions of shared parenting (co-parenting definedas if a child felt both parents were involved in their upbringing), and presum-ably relied on some retrospective accounts as one participant was aged 20years.

160 J. Neoh and D. Mellor

Downloaded At: 02:48 22 November 2010

In summary, as demonstrated previously, the bulk of post separationresearch has relied on parents for information about children’s adjustment.The few studies that do canvass children’s experience do not provide clearevidence that shared parenting provides a better environment for childrenthan an environment with one parent caregiver. Research on shared parent-ing, both adult focused and that using children’s perspectives has also beencharacterized by the presence of methodological limitations that tend toqualify any conclusions that may be drawn.

THE CURRENT STUDY

The aim of our study is to investigate children’s adjustment in shared parent-ing arrangements relative to that of children in other family arrangements.On the basis of the aforementioned research, we expected that:

. Using the Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire, both parents andchildren from intact families would report better child adjustment thanboth separated family groups. However, parents from all family groupswere expected to underestimate children’s self-reported internal states.

. Using the Children’s Beliefs about Parental Separation Scale, separatedparents would underestimate children’s self reported reactions to theirparents’ separation in both separated family groups.

. Members of intact families would be less stressed and more satisfied thanshared parenting families, but members of shared parenting families wouldbe less stressed and more satisfied than members of sole residencefamilies.

METHOD

Participants

The sample comprised 68 families. There were a total of 94 parent parti-cipants: 31 shared parents (16 mothers and 15 fathers), 36 parents from soleresidence families (27 resident parents, 24 mothers and 3 fathers; and 9contact parents, 2 mothers and 7 fathers), and 27 parents from intact families(13 mothers and 14 fathers).

In the sample of 88 children, there were 27 children from shared parent-ing families (13 females; 14 males; Mean age 10.75 years; SD¼ 2.50 years, agerange 8–15 years), 37 children from sole residence and contact families (19females; 18 males; Mean age 10.51 years; SD¼ 2.54 years, age range 8–15years) and 24 children from intact families (16 females; 8 males; Mean age10.29 years; SD¼ 2.31 years, age range 8–15 years).

Shared Parenting Impact on Children 161

Downloaded At: 02:48 22 November 2010

Measures

The Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire (SDQ) is a widely used, highlyreliable, and valid measure to screen for emotional and behavioral problemsin children. This scale has been used extensively in previous research withseparated parents (McMunn, Nazroo, Marmot, Boreham, & Goodman,2001), to compare parent and child assessments of children’s adjustment(Goodman 1997), and has been validated in research against the CBCL(Goodman & Scott, 1999). Parents completed a questionnaire for each oftheir children who had consented to participate in the study and childrencompleted the child report measure. This 25-item questionnaire incorporatesfive subscales: Prosocial, Hyperactivity, Emotional, Conduct and Peer pro-blems. Responses range from not true (0), somewhat true (1) to certainly true(2) Summed subscales range from 0 to 10. The summated total difficultiesscale did not include the prosocial subscale, as this factor is considered tobe a protective factor improving prognosis. The summated total scale rangesfrom 0 to 40 and greater scores indicate greater levels of maladjustment. Theinternal reliability of the total scale was high (Total SDQ a¼ .82) (a> .75 isconsidered to be good). Cronbach’s alpha for parents ranged from .60 to.91 for all subscales and children’s ranged from .82 to .89.

The Children’s Beliefs about Parental Separation Scale (CBAPS),(CBAPS; Kurdek & Berg, 1983), a subscale of the Parent Separation Inven-tory, is a parent report of children’s separation experience that measuresthe same domains as the child version of Children Beliefs about ParentalSeparation Scale. It has been used in a number of mostly North AmericanStudies. While the validity and reliability of this scale is not so well established,it is one of the fewmeasures for this population. This scale consists of 16 itemsacross four subscales, Peer Ridicule, Paternal Blame, Maternal Blame, andHope of Reunification. Two of the original subscales, Fear of Abandonmentand Self Blame, were not used in this study as they were felt to be insensitive.Parents responded with yes, scored as 1, or no, scored as 0, to items such as‘‘my child feels that the separation of their parents is something to be ashamedof,’’ and ‘‘my child seems to interact with children less now than before theseparation.’’ The truncated scale scores ranged from 0 to 12. High subscalescores represented greater degrees of the subscale factors. Cronbach’s a sug-gested that the scale was internally reliable (a¼ .79). Subscales ranged froma¼ .65 to .84. This scale was administered to separated-families only.

Satisfaction

We created five items tomeasure parental and child satisfaction. Each itemwasused independently in the analyses. The items assessed global happiness,levels of stress, and life satisfaction. Two additional items, given to separatedfamilies only, assessed parent and child satisfaction with living arrangements

162 J. Neoh and D. Mellor

Downloaded At: 02:48 22 November 2010

and satisfaction with the time they spent with their children or parents. Allitemswere rated from 0 to 100 from very low to very high, with 50 representingaverage. Higher numbers indicated greater satisfaction, happiness, or stress.

Procedure

Human Research Ethics Committee approval for the study was obtained fromDeakin University. Participants were then recruited in three ways: throughschool newsletters, editorial comment in community newspapers, and snow-ball sampling beginning with initial parent participants. Interested parentscontacted the researcher by voicemail if they wished to be included in thestudy. All interested parents were then contacted by telephone and askeda few short screening questions to ensure they met inclusion criteria.

Families were classified as shared parenting families if they had one ormore children between the ages of 8 to 15 years who spent greater than 40%of time each parents’ household. Sixty-six percent of shared parenting childrenin the study had an arrangement whereby they spent a week with each parent.Families were classified as Sole Residence families if they included one or morechildren between the ages of 8 to 15 yearswho spentmost of their timewith oneparent and 30% or less with the other. Families were classified as intact if adultparticipants were the biological parents of their children and lived with them.

Families were not included if parents did not consent to completingquestionnaires, or did not consent to their child=ren’s participation andagreed to confidentiality for their child=ren’s responses. Children’s fullyinformed consent was also essential (see as follows).

A follow up telephone call was made to answer any questions and toarrange a time for an interview for those participants who continued toexpress interest in participation. A number of families were excluded or selfexcluded at this stage (n¼ 11). A convenient interview time and place wasthen arranged. All participants were interviewed in their own homes. Chil-dren from Sole Resident families were interviewed in their usual home andchildren from Shared Parenting families were interviewed in the home theywere in at the time of the assessment.

Parent Interview

After fully informed consent was confirmed at the first parent interview, part-icipants were asked to respond to the verbally presented questionnaires.Each parent signed a consent form on behalf of their children who wantedto participate, as required under Australian ethics protocols3.

Child Interview

At the child interview, some time was spent in building rapport and getting toknow each child. In acknowledging that children’s participation in research

Shared Parenting Impact on Children 163

Downloaded At: 02:48 22 November 2010

is often decided by their parents (Mason, 2000), the expectations, meaning,and possible consequences of participating in the study were explainedto each child individually (specifically covering anonymity, an overview ofthe questions they would be asked, their right to stop at any time, how thedata were to be used, and options for them if they felt upset or distressedby participation). Children were then asked if they would like to participate.Three children declined at this point. Signed consent was obtained beforeany data were collected. All participating children were interviewed privatelyin their own home. Children’s responses were not shared with parents.

RESULTS

The data were analyzed with SPSS=PC (Version 14) using Multivariate Analy-sis of Variance, a statistical technique that allows for the detection of defensesbetween groups and over time. Before the hypotheses were tested, a rangeof potential covariates, including age of the parents, average weekly income,length of parental relationship, years of education, number of children ineach family, time since separation for separated families, and the age of chil-dren, were analyzed. Differences across the groups were found to be insig-nificant, so the analyses proceeded as described in the following section.

Children’s and Parents’ Reports of Adjustment

Means for all groups for each of the SDQ subscales are shown in Table 1. A3� 3 between subjects MANOVA was performed on five dependent variables(the subscales of the SDQ). The analysis involved two independent variables,family composition (shared, sole residence and intact families), and role inthe family member (mother, father, and child).

Global main effects were found on the best linear combination of thesubscales for informant, FPillais(10, 410)¼ 2.23, p¼ .01 and for family struc-ture, FPillais(10, 410)¼ 2.01, p¼ .032. There was no significant main effectfor the interaction of these variables although power appeared to beadequate at .72.

In deciphering the global main effect, there was only one significantunivariate main effect of informant detected. Only emotional problems wassignificant, F(2, 208)¼ 5.64, p¼ .004; g2¼ .05. It would appear that thechildren in the sample saw themselves as having more emotional problems(M¼ 4.10) than mothers (M¼ 3.05) or fathers (M¼ 2.68) across all familygroups. Scheffe post hoc tests of informant found significant mean differ-ences between mothers and children (M¼ 1.10, p¼ .022) and betweenfathers and children (M¼ 1.57, p¼ .003). However, the difference betweenmothers and fathers on emotional measures of the SDQ was not significant.

164 J. Neoh and D. Mellor

Downloaded At: 02:48 22 November 2010

TABLE1

Mean

san

dStan

dardDeviationsforSu

bscalesoftheSD

QforFam

ilyMembers

inEachFam

ilyGroup

Fam

ilygroup

Sharedparenting

Sole

residence

Intact

Inform

ant

Mother

Father

Child

Mother

Father

Child

Mother

Father

Child

n25

17

27

31

937

23

24

24

Prosocial

Behaviours

8.60(1.8)

8.18(2.0)

8.89(1.3)

7.81(2.7)

7.78(2.4)

8.32(1.8)

8.65(1.5)

7.67(2.2)

8.63(1.5)

Hyperactiveproblems

2.84(2.8)

3.12(2.4)

3.74(2.6)

4.10(3.2)

6.00(2.9)

5.03(2.6)

2.78(2.4)

3.50(2.7)

3.58(2.5)

PeerProblems

2.40(2.1)

2.47(2.7)

3.19(3.4)

2.84(2.3)

3.44(1.7)

3.03(2.4)

2.57(2.5)

2.33(2.2)

2.67(2.3)

Conduct

Problems

1.76(1.9)

2.85(3.0)

1.59(1.6)

2.55(2.6)

1.33(1.0)

3.14(2.5)

1.91(2.4)

2.42(3.2)

2.42(2.4)

Emotional

Problems

2.84(2.3)

2.82(2.8)

3.89(3.0)

3.06(2.2)

3.00(1.4)

4.32(3.1)

3.17(2.4)

2.21(1.8)

4.08(2.6)

165

Downloaded At: 02:48 22 November 2010

Also, detected within the global main effect of family composition wasone univariate main effect for family composition on hyperactivity F(2,208)¼ 5.64, p¼ .004; g2¼ .05. Mothers, fathers and children in sole residencefamilies (M¼ 5.00) were more likely to report hyperactive behaviors thanmembers of shared parenting (M¼ 3.24) or intact (M¼ 3.29) families. Scheffepost hoc tests revealed significant mean differences between sole residentand contact families and shared parenting families (M¼�1.43, p¼ .007)and sole residence families and intact families (M¼�1.40, p¼ .007). Therewere no differences between shared parenting and intact families on hyper-activity measures of the SDQ. There were no significant main effects of familygroup found across the other subscales of the SDQ.

Parents’ and Children’s Reports of Children’s Reactions to TheirParents’ Separation

Whether separated parents underestimated children’s reports of their reac-tions to their parents’ separation was tested in a 2� 3 between subjectsMANOVA conducted on the four subscales of the CBAPS- peer ridicule,paternal blame maternal blame, and hope of reunification as dependent vari-ables. Both separated family groups (shared parenting and sole residencefamilies) and informant (mother, father, or child) served as independent vari-ables. Planned contrasts were used to examine the effects of informant toincrease the power of the analysis. The first contrast compared children withmothers and fathers; the second compared mothers and fathers. Descriptivestatistics may be viewed in Table 2.

The analysis revealed significant global main effects on the best linearcombination of the subscales of the CATD scale for informant FPillai’s(4,137)¼ 13.00, p< .001, and explained 27% of the difference across the roleof the family member, partial g2¼ .27. The interaction between informantand family composition, and the global main effect of family compositionwere not significant. There appeared to be adequate power to detect aneffect (.42).

TABLE 2 Means for Subscales of the CBAPS for Family Members in Each Family Group

Family groupShared parenting Sole residence

Informant Mother Father Child Mother Father Childn 25 17 27 31 9 37

Maternal Blame 0.36 (0.5) 0.18 (0.8) 1.22 (1.4) 0.68 (0.8) 0.11 (1.4) 1.68 (1.5)Paternal Blame 0.28 (0.8) 0.35 (0.4) 1.00 (2.0) 0.39 (0.8) 0.89 (0.3) 1.03 (1.9)Peer Ridicule 0.48 (1.0) 0.76 (1.1) 2.15 (0.9) 1.00 (0.5) 0.56 (0.9) 2.49 (1.2)Hope ofReunification

1.68 (1.0) 1.47 (0.9) 0.70 (1.4) 1.48 (0.6) 1.22 (0.4) 0.92 (1.1)

166 J. Neoh and D. Mellor

Downloaded At: 02:48 22 November 2010

For the main effect of role in the family, planned contrasts found thatchildren in both family compositions (shared parenting and sole residencefamilies) reported different reactions to their parents separation than theirparents believed across all subscales of the CBAPS. However parents heldsimilar views to each other. The second contrast was not significant forany subscale and found that mothers and fathers from both separated familygroups held similar understandings about their children’s beliefs.

Children felt significantly more peer ridicule over their parents separ-ation (M¼ 2.3) than mothers (M¼ .71) or fathers (M¼ .66) believed their chil-dren did, F(2, 140)¼ 122.34, p< .001, g2¼ .48. Both mothers and fathersacross shared and sole residence families felt that children were more openwith their friends than children in separated families themselves reported.

Children tended to blame both their fathers (Paternal blame M¼ 1.43),F(2, 140)¼ 20.11, p< .001, g2¼ .13 and their mothers (Maternal BlameM¼ 1.01, F(2, 140)¼ 7.00, p¼ .009, g2¼ .07 more than either of their parentsbelieved. Mother and fathers of separated children tended to believe similardegrees of maternal and paternal blame (Mothers, Paternal Blame M¼ .48;Maternal Blame M¼ .33; Fathers, Paternal Blame M¼ .14; Maternal BlameM¼ .62).

Mothers and fathers in both separated family groups also concurredthat they felt their children would like them to be reunited. However, chil-dren themselves felt significantly less Hope of Reunification (M¼ .81) thanmothers (M¼ 1.58) or fathers (M¼ 1.35) believed their children felt, F(2,140)¼ 13.42, p< .001, g2¼ .13.

Satisfaction and Stress

A 3� 3 between subject’s MANOVA was performed on participants’responses to their rating of global satisfaction with life, their global happi-ness, and their general feelings of stress in their lives (see Table 3). Therewere two independent variables: family composition (shared parenting, soleresidence, and intact families) and informant (mother, father, and child).

As satisfaction with life and global happiness were very highly corre-lated (greater than r¼ .81 for all groups), they were averaged for all parti-cipants to create one variable.

A family composition by informant interaction effect was found,FPillais(8, 322)¼ 3.85, p< .001, g2¼ .09. The main effect of family compo-sition was also significant FPillais(4, 322)¼ 3.55, p¼ .007, g2¼ .04. Multivari-ately, the main effect of informant was not significant, although powerappeared to be adequate to detect an effect at .72.

The interaction between family composition and informant was foundon the dependent variable stress. Shared parenting fathers reported feelingless stress (M¼ 29.05) than mothers (M¼ 60.47) and children (M¼ 57.63)in their families; shared parenting fathers also reported less stress than

Shared Parenting Impact on Children 167

Downloaded At: 02:48 22 November 2010

TABLE3

Mean

ofMother’s,Father’san

dChildren’sScoresofSatisfactionWithLife

andHap

piness

andLevelsofStress

byFam

ilyGroup

Fam

ilygroup

Sharedparenting

Sole

residence

Intact

Role

inthefamily

Mother

Father

Child

Mother

Father

Child

Mother

Father

Child

n17

11

27

21

637

13

14

24

Satisfactionwith

life

andhap

piness

69.26(20.40)

76.36(17.9)

63.94(23.2)

72.08(17.4)

57.08(24.1)

66.89(24.4)

81.54(10.0)

79.75(12.4)

80.71(13.5)

Levelsofstress

60.47(18.3)

29.05(19.2)

57.63(24.3)

50.48(33.0)

67.50(10.8)

26.11(19.4)

42.00(25.4)

46.79(26.5)

34.79(27.0)

168

Downloaded At: 02:48 22 November 2010

mothers (M¼ 50.48) and fathers (M¼ 67.50) in sole residence families; andalso mothers (M¼ 42.00), fathers (M¼ 46.79), and children (M¼ 34.79) inintact families, F(4, 161)¼ 7.92, p< .001, g2¼ .16.

There was also a significant main effect of family composition on theaveraged variable of satisfaction with life and general feelings of happiness.Intact families felt much more satisfied with their life and felt generallyhappier (M¼ 80.67) than shared (M¼ 70.57) or sole residence families(M¼ 65.13), F(2, 161)¼ 7.04, p¼ .001, g2¼ .08. Post hoc Scheffe testsrevealed that there were significant differences between intact and sharedparenting families (mean difference¼ 10.10, p¼ .013) and also betweenintact and sole residence parents (mean difference¼ 15.54, p< .001).

Separated Families’, Mother’s, Father’s, and Children’s Feelings ofSatisfaction

A 2� 3 between subject’s MANOVA was conducted to determine how differ-ent family members (mothers, fathers, and children) in different separatedfamily compositions (sole residence or shared parenting families) differedin their satisfaction with their current parenting arrangements and how satis-fied they were with the amount of time they spend in each other’s company(Table 4).

The analysis revealed a multivariately significant interaction effectbetween role in the family and the family composition FPillais(4,226)¼ 3.13, p¼ .01, g2¼ .05. There were no significant main effects for rolein the family or family composition alone. Power for these analyses was .58and .42, respectively. Univariate analyses revealed that the interaction effectbetween the role in the family and separated family group was on satisfactionwith child arrangements, F(2,114)¼ 2.71, p¼ .01, g2¼ .05. While mothers,fathers, and children in sole residence and contact families gave similar esti-mates of the satisfaction with the parenting arrangements and indicated theywere only moderately happy with the agreed arrangements, shared parents,both mothers and fathers, were much more satisfied with their form ofarrangement than their children, who reported similar levels to that of chil-dren from sole residence and contact families.

DISCUSSION

The current study explored how children feel about their living arrange-ments, adjustment and satisfaction with life after their parents’ separation,and compared this to their parent’s perspectives. Such research into sharedparenting and especially research including children’s experiences of sharedparenting, has important repercussions for Family Law and policy making,and can aid future decision-making for separating families.

Shared Parenting Impact on Children 169

Downloaded At: 02:48 22 November 2010

TABLE4

Mean

s(andstan

darddeviations)

ofFam

ilyMember’sFeelingsofSatisfactionWiththeArran

gementsin

Place

forChildrenan

dtheTim

eParents

andChildrenSp

endin

EachOthers

Compan

yAcross

SeparatedFam

ilyGroups

Fam

ilygroup

Sharedparenting

Sole

residence

andco

ntact

Role

inthefamily

Mother

Father

Child

Mother

Father

Child

n17

11

27

21

637

Satisfactionwith

arrangements

for

children

80.24(27.0)

86.36(16.1)

60.22(33.7)

56.67(28.5)

68.33(45.8)

65.81(34.0)

Satisfactionwith

Parent=ch

ildtime

70.88(32.6)

80.91(19.1)

57.19(36.2)

66.90(25.9)

44.17(29.2)

49.70(40.50)

170

Downloaded At: 02:48 22 November 2010

On all domains of the SDQ, except hyperactivity, there were no differ-ences between children from the three groups, whether by child self-report,mother report, or father report. However, by all sources of report, children insole residence families were more likely to have symptoms of hyperactivity=inattention. By this measure, shared parenting would appear to be associatedwith better outcomes for children when their parents separate than soleresidence arrangements.

The second important finding was that although parents and children inall family groups concurred about peer difficulties, overt conduct problemsand prosocial behaviors, they disagreed in relation to emotional difficulties.Across all family groups, mothers and fathers tended to underestimate thedegree of emotional problems that children report, in that the SDQ parentreport scores were less than the SDQ self-report scores.. This suggests thatparents, in general, are not good at estimating children’s internal, subtleexperiences, and, in particular, supports the view that research into chil-dren’s adjustment after their parents’ separation that is based on parentreporting may be misleading.

Similarly, in both separated family types (sole residence families andshared parenting families), parents (mothers and fathers) overestimatedhow comfortable children feel telling friends about their home life andhow much children blame both parents for the separation. Interestingly, bothmothers and fathers also tended to overestimate how much their childrenwanted their parents to reconcile. However, there was no difference betweenchildren from sole residence families and children from shared parentingfamilies on these reactions to their parents’ separation. Again, while theseresults suggest that using children’s own estimates of their adjustment andinternal experience is more informative than using parents to measure chil-dren’s reactions to their parents’ separation, they also cast doubt on claimsthat shared parenting provides better outcomes for children whose parentsseparate.

A third important finding in the present study was that fathers in sharedparenting families reported less stress than all other participant groups (evenintact family members). That is, shared parenting is better for fathers, but notfor children or mothers on this domain. On the other hand, while well withinnormal limits of adjustment, children in shared parenting families reportedmuch more stress than children from sole resident families and children fromintact families. As expected, intact families reported greater general satisfac-tion and happiness than the separated family groups, who did not differ fromeach other in levels of these.

Finally, while children from both shared parenting families and soleresidence families were equally only moderately satisfied with their familyarrangements, parents in shared parenting family reported being much moresatisfied with the arrangement than their children. Sole resident parentsexpressed a similar level of satisfaction to their children. This finding implies

Shared Parenting Impact on Children 171

Downloaded At: 02:48 22 November 2010

that the benefits of shared parenting are more evident to parents than chil-dren, and that children in such families perceive themselves to be no betteroff than children in sole residence families.

Is it the case that parents know best and that shared parenting is betterfor children, but that children only see the immediate trivial irritations? Or, isit the case that parents do not recognize the extra pressure that shared par-enting is placing on their children because of the benefits to themselves andtheir firm beliefs that shared parenting is best for children? Whichever thecase, the findings suggest that parental satisfaction and perceptions of theirchildren’s adjustment appear to be poor metrics for deciding children’s futureand may not be related in any direct way with child adjustment. It is alsorecognized that the converse, parental dissatisfaction, is also unlikely to cre-ate a supportive environment to rear children or to result in optimal childadjustment (King, 1999; Stoneman, Brody, & Burke, 1989). Perhaps, how-ever, assumptions about relationships between parental satisfaction andchild adjustment should be re-evaluated and other factors may need to beconsidered when deciding what is best for children.

In summary, this study demonstrates, in contradiction of arguments tothe contrary (e.g., Bauserman, 2002; Clancy, 1990; Elkin, 1991; Greif, 1979;Kruk, 1993) children in shared parenting are not necessarily better off thanchildren in other post-separation arrangements. Further, as other researchers(e.g., Lengua et al., 2000; Tebbut et al., 1997) have found, there is generallylittle correlation between parent and child self-report measures of emotionalproblems. Consistent with this, children in both shared parenting and soleresidence arrangements often have different views of themselves than theirparents, and what their parents consider to be important may not alwaysbe the most relevant from children’s point of view. Even when parentsbelieve children are doing well, and when parents feel satisfied, childrenmay not be doing well and may not feel the same levels of satisfaction.

Despite these findings, the study has a number of limitations. Our sam-ple was reasonable but not large. A larger sample would allow analyses tocontrol for how much time had passed since separation, and how long chil-dren had been in different care arrangements. This would help to determinewhether the child’s adjustment is associated with the transitions involved inparental separation or specifically with the living arrangements. Additionally,we did not record several variables that may have an impact on children’sadjustment and that of their families. For example, we did not recordsocio-economic status, the quality and nature of the interaction between chil-dren and their parents when in their care, the manner in which the carearrangements were determined, and so on. Third, despite our arguments,there may be concerns with giving substantial weight to children’s views,in that their views tend to change over time, and it may be inappropriateto think that their voice in childhood has a predictive value with respect totheir future adjustment. That is, assessing children’s reactions at a specific

172 J. Neoh and D. Mellor

Downloaded At: 02:48 22 November 2010

moment in time may not provide a stable picture. Clearly, there is a need forstudies of larger populations over the longer term. Having adults look backon their experiences in various living arrangements may provide insight.Looking back over custodial arrangements and offering suggestions forimprovement may provide different information. Finally, it may be interest-ing to include in future studies a group of children whose separated=divorced parents live in the same household.

CONCLUSION

It seems that traditional ideas about what makes shared parenting successfuldo not have the anticipated beneficial impact on children. Simplistic structur-al comparisons of different family groups do not appear to adequatelydescribe what is happening in families and suggest that consistency in rela-tionships and parenting are the most important factors for children, not thecomposition of their family. The unilateral promotion of shared parentingas the best post-separation family structure seems ultimately unsustainable.

NOTES

1. Although this research was with joint legal custody, fathers they also had greater time with children.

2. The term ‘‘intact’’ to delineate not-separated families from separated families has been carefully

considered for this research and not been used lightly. Some criticism has been directed at the use of this

term based on the idea that it connotes separated families as ‘‘broken.’’ This research considered ‘‘unified,’’

‘‘non separated,’’ ‘‘conventional,’’ ‘‘two parent,’’ ‘‘couple,’’ ‘‘biological,’’ or ‘‘nuclear’’ terms as even less

useful alternatives. These terms have negative overtones, do not distinguish between separated and

not-separated families, or repartnered and not repartnered families and create confusion. To conform with

Australian Bureau of Statistics and Australian Institute of Family Studies standards, ‘‘intact’’ is used as the

most effective term.

3. In separated families only one parent’s consent is required as the law recognizes both parents as

equally able to give guardian consent (Mason, 2000). Attempts were made to gain both parents consent

in all families; however, in sole residence families where only one parent was interviewed, the residence

parent alone gave guardian consent.

REFERENCES

Amato, P. R. (1987). Family process in one-parent families: The child’s point of view.Journal of Marriage and the Family, 49, 327–337.

Arbanel, A. (1979). Shared parenting after separation and divorce: A study of jointcustody. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 49, 320–329.

Arditti, J. A. (1992). Differences between fathers with joint custody and noncustodialfathers. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 62, 186–195.

Bauserman, R. (2002). Child adjustment in joint-custody versus sole-custody arrange-ments: A meta-analytic review. Journal of Family Psychology, 16, 91–102.

Brown, C. (1994). The impact of divorce on families: The Australian experience.Family and Conciliation Courts Review, 32, 149–167.

Shared Parenting Impact on Children 173

Downloaded At: 02:48 22 November 2010

Buchannan, C. M., Maccoby, E. E., & Dornbusch, S. M. (1991). Caught between parents:Adolescents experience in divorced homes. Child Development, 62, 1008–1029.

Clancy, P. (1990). Custody: Sharing the responsibility. Law Institute Journal, 64,607–610.

De Los Reyes, A., & Kazdin, A. E. (2004). Measuring informant discrepancies in clini-cal child research. Psychological Assessment, 16, 330–334.

Elkin, M. (1991). Joint custody: In the best interest of the family. In J. Folberg (Ed.)Joint custody and shared parenting (2nd ed., pp. 11–15). New York: Guildford.

Family Law Act (1975). (Act 56=1975).Glover, R. J., & Steele, C. (1989). Comparing the effects on the child of post-divorce

parenting arrangements. Journal of Divorce, 12, 185–201.Gollop, M. M., Smith, A. B., & Taylor, N. J. (2000). Children’s involvement in custody

and access arrangements after parental separation. Child and Family LawQuarterly, 12, 383–399.

Goodman, R. (1997). The strengths and difficulties questionnaire: A research note.Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 38, 581–586.

Goodman, R., & Scott, S. (1999). Comparing the strength and difficulties question-naire and the child behavior checklist: Is small beautiful? Journal of AbnormalChild Psychology, 27, 17–24.

Greif, J. D. (1979). Fathers, children and joint custody. American Journal of Orthop-sychiatry, 49, 311–319.

House of Representatives Standing Committee on Family and Community Affairs(2003). Every picture tells a story: Report on the inquiry into child custodyarrangements in the event of family separation. Canberra: Parliament of theCommonwealth of Australia.

Howell, S. H., Portes, P. R., & Brown, J. H. (1997). Gender and age differences inchild adjustment to parental separation. Journal of Divorce and Remarriage,27, 141–158.

Johnston, J. R., Kline, M., & Tschann, J. M. (1989). Ongoing postdivorce conflict:Effects on children of joint custody and frequent access. American Journal ofOrthopsychiatry, 59, 576–592.

King, V. (1999). Nonresident father visitation, parental conflict, and mother’s satisfac-tion: What is best for children’s well-being? Journal of Marriage and the Family,61, Retrieved 3 October, 2003 from Ebschost database.

Kruk, E. (1993). Promoting co-operative parenting after separation: A therapeuticinterventionist model of family mediation. Journal of Family Therapy, 15,235–261.

Kurdek, L. A., & Berg, B. (1983). Correlates of children’s adjustment to their parents’divorce. In L. A. Kurdek (Ed.) New directions for child development: Childrenand divorce (pp. 47–60). San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.

Lengua, L. J., Wolchik, S. A., Sandler, I. N., & West, S. G. (2000). The additiveand interactive effects of parenting and temperament in predicting adjustmentproblems of children of divorce. Journal of Clinical Child Psychology, 29,232–244.

Luepnitz, D. A. (1991). A comparison of maternal, paternal, and joint custody. InJ. Folberg (Ed.) Joint custody and shared parenting (2nd. ed., pp. 105–113).New York: Guildford.

174 J. Neoh and D. Mellor

Downloaded At: 02:48 22 November 2010

Mason, J. (2000). Researching children’s perspectives: Legal issues. In A. Lewis &G. Lindsay (Eds.) Researching childrens’ perspectives (pp. 34–45). Buckingham,UK: Open University.

McKinnon, R., & Wallerstein, J. S. (1991). Joint custody and the preschool child. InJ. Folberg (Ed.) Joint custody and shared parenting (2nd ed., pp. 153–166).New York: Guildford.

McMunn, A., Nazroo, J. Y., Marmot, M. G., Boreham, R., & Goodman, R. (2001).Children’s emotional and behavioural well-being and the family environment:Findings from the Health Survey for England. Social Science and Medicine,53, 423–440.

Neugebauer, R. (1989). Divorce, custody, and visitation: The child’s point of view.Journal of Divorce and Remarriage, 14, 153–168.

Pearson, J., & Thoennes, N. (1991). Child custody and child support after divorce.In J. Folberg (Ed.) Joint custody and shared parenting (2nd ed., pp. 185–208).New York: Guildford.

Pruett, K. D., & Kline Pruett, M. (1999). ‘‘Only God decides’’: Young children’s per-ceptions of divorce and the legal system. Journal of the American Acaademy ofChild and Adolescent Psychiatry, 38, 1544–1550.

Schwartz, L. L. (1987). Joint custody: Is it right for all children. Journal of FamilyPsychology, 1(1), 120–134.

Sharpley, C. F., & Webber, R. P. (1989). Coparenting: An alternative to consider inseparation. Australian Journal of Sex, Marriage and Family, 10, 111–117.

Smart, C., Neale, B., & Wade, A. (2001). The changing experience of childhood:Families and Divorce. Malden MA, USA: Polity.

Stoneman, Z., Brody, G. H., & Burke, M. (1989). Marital quality, depression andinconsistent parenting. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 59, 105–117.

Strangeland, C. S., Pellegreno, D. D., & Lunholm, J. (1989). Children of divorced par-ents: A perceptual comparison. Elementary School Guidance & Counseling, 23,167–174.

Sviggum, G. (2000). How children’s view their parents’ divorce [Electronic version].Family Matters, Autumn, 62–72.

Tebbut, J., Swanston, H., Oates, K., & O’Toole, B. I. (1997). Five years after childsexual abuse: Persisting dysfunction and problems of prediction [Electronic ver-sion]. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 36,330–340.

Teja, S., & Stolberg, A. L. (1993). Peer support, divorce, and children’s adjustment.Journal of Divorce and Remarriage, 20, 45–64.

Shared Parenting Impact on Children 175

Downloaded At: 02:48 22 November 2010