22
1 Resting state activity in patients with disorders of consciousness Andrea Soddu 1 , Audrey Vanhaudenhuyse 1 , Athena Demertzi 1 , Marie-Aurélie Bruno 1 , Luaba Tshibanda 2 , Haibo Di 3 , Mélanie Boly 1,2 , Steven Laureys 1,2 , Quentin Noirhomme 1 1 Coma Science Group, Cyclotron Research Centre, University of Liège, Belgium 2 Neurology Department, CHU Sart Tilman Hospital, University of Liège, Belgium 3 Vegetative State and Consciousness Science Institute, Hangzhou Normal University, China Corresponding author: Andrea Soddu Coma Science Group, Cyclotron Research Centre, University of Liège, Belgium Email: [email protected] Tel: +32 4 366 23 35; Fax: +32 4 366 29 46 Vanhaudenhuyse A: [email protected] Demertzi A: [email protected] Bruno MA: [email protected] Tshibanda L: [email protected] Di H: [email protected] Boly M: [email protected] ; Laureys S: [email protected] ; Noirhomme Q: [email protected] ; 1

Resting state activity in patients with disorders of consciousness

Embed Size (px)

Citation preview

1

Resting state activity in patients with disorders of consciousness

Andrea Soddu1, Audrey Vanhaudenhuyse1, Athena Demertzi1, Marie-Aurélie Bruno1, Luaba

Tshibanda2, Haibo Di3, Mélanie Boly1,2, Steven Laureys1,2, Quentin Noirhomme1

1Coma Science Group, Cyclotron Research Centre, University of Liège, Belgium

2Neurology Department, CHU Sart Tilman Hospital, University of Liège, Belgium

3Vegetative State and Consciousness Science Institute, Hangzhou Normal University, China

Corresponding author:

Andrea Soddu

Coma Science Group, Cyclotron Research Centre, University of Liège, Belgium

Email: [email protected]

Tel: +32 4 366 23 35; Fax: +32 4 366 29 46

Vanhaudenhuyse A: [email protected]

Demertzi A: [email protected]

Bruno MA: [email protected]

Tshibanda L: [email protected]

Di H: [email protected]

Boly M: [email protected];

Laureys S: [email protected];

Noirhomme Q: [email protected];

 

2

Abstract

Recent progress in studying spontaneous brain activity has demonstrated activity patterns that

emerge without any task performance or sensory stimulation, holding promise for the study of

higher order associative network functionality. Additionally, such advances are argued to be

relevant in pathology, such as disorders of consciousness (DOC; i.e., coma, vegetative and

minimally conscious states). Recent studies on resting state activity in DOC, measured with

functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging (fMRI) techniques, show that the functional

connectivity is disrupted in the task-negative or the default mode network. However, the two

main approaches employed in the analysis of resting–state functional connectivity data (i.e.,

hypothesis-driven seed-voxel and data driven Independent Component Analysis) present

multiple methodological difficulties, especially in non-collaborative DOC patients.

Improvements in motion artifact removal and spatial normalization are needed before fMRI

resting state data can be used as proper biomarkers in severe brain injury. However, we

anticipate that such developments will boost clinical “resting state” fMRI studies, allowing for

easy and fast acquisitions and ultimately improve the diagnosis and prognosis in the absence

of DOC patients’ active collaboration in data acquisition.

Key Words: Coma, consciousness, resting state, spontaneous activity, functional magnetic

resonance imaging, default network, vegetative state.

Introduction

According to present-day understanding, the vacuum state (also called the ‘quantum

vacuum'), is by no means a “simple empty space” (Lambrecht 2002) or some “absolutely

empty void” (Ray 1991). In fact, the modern theory of quantum mechanics tells us that the

vacuum state contains fleeting electromagnetic waves and particles that pop into and out of

 

3

existence. Thanks to this new theory of vacuum it is now possible to predict with a

remarkable precision the most “intimate” properties of the atoms. If a proper theory of

vacuum in physics was an irreplaceable step to develop a comprehensive modern theory of

the “particle” world, we hypothesize that in neuroscience to develop a proper theory of what

is called the “baseline” activity of the brain could be a critical step in a deeper understanding

of the “neuronal” world.

Much of what is currently known about brain function derives from studies in which a

task or stimulus is administered and the resulting changes in neuronal activity and behavior

are measured. Recent studies on brain energy metabolism, however, have shown that task-

related increases in neuronal metabolism are usually small when compared to resting energy

consumption (Fox and Raichle, 2007). Hence, it becomes clear that in order for one to fully

investigate the brain’s activity, one also needs to study its baseline energy consuming

features. Recent progress in studying spontaneous brain activity (Biswal et al., 1995; Cordes

et al., 2000; Damoiseaux et al., 2006; Fox and Raichle, 2007; Fox et al., 2005; Greicius et al.,

2003; Lowe et al., 1998; Mitra et al., 1997; Nir et al., 2006; Vincent et al., 2007; Xiong et al.,

1999; Beckmann et al., 2005) has demonstrated activity patterns that emerge with no task

performance or sensory stimulation, holding promise for the study of higher order associative

network functionality and its potential implication in stimulus-induced activity (e.g. see Boly

et al., 2007) and pathology (Boly et al., 2008a,b, 2009; Greicius et al., 2004; Rombouts et al.,

2009).

In this paper we will review studies on resting state activity measured with functional

Magnetic Resonance Imaging (fMRI) techniques in patients with disorders of consciousness

(DOC; coma, vegetative and minimally conscious states). While progress has been made in

describing DOC from the clinical perspective (Majerus et al, 2005), we will here focus on

examining DOC patients from the point of view of recent developments deriving from studies

 

4

on the healthy human brain. In our view, new insights obtained by studying Blood Oxygen

Level Dependent (BOLD) resting state activity (Raichle et al., 2001; Raichle and Snyder,

2007) are relevant to a better understanding of DOC, allowing for a better diagnosis, as well

as potential prognosis and treatment (Giacino et al., 2006; Laureys et al., 2006; Schiff, 2006a,

2006b).

Disorders of consciousness

Disorders of consciousness (DOC) encompass a spectrum of clinical conditions

involving profound disruption in global consciousness due to massive brain lesions (Bernat,

2006; Giacino et al., 2002; Laureys et al., 2004; Schiff, 2006b). Clinical characterization of

the different DOC is based on two main distinct components of human consciousness: arousal

and awareness (Plum and Posner, 1972). If arousal refers to the behavioral alternation of sleep

and wakefulness, awareness refers to the collective thoughts and feelings of an individual

(Laureys, 2005). Coma is characterized by the absence of arousal and hence of awareness.

Vegetative state patients are aroused but unaware of the environment and of themselves

(Jennett and Plum, 1972). Minimally conscious state patients show reproducible behavioral

evidence of awareness of environment or self but are unable to reliably communicate (Giacino

et al., 2002). Locked-in syndrome patients (Plum and Posner, 1972) are fully conscious but

are completely paralyzed except for small movements of the eyes or eyelids. Behavioral

assessment is one of the main methods used to detect awareness in severely brain injured

patients recovering from coma (Majerus et al., 2005). However, detecting unambiguous signs

of conscious perception in such patients can be very challenging as it is illustrated by the high

frequency of misdiagnosis (up to 43%) in these patients (Andrews et al., 1996; Childs and

Mercer, 1996; Schnakers et al., 2009b). New technical methods such as fMRI offer specific

 

5

assessment procedures and the possibility to determine objectively whether an unresponsive

patient is aware without explicit verbal or motor response.

Spontaneous fMRI activity patterns as a diagnostic tool in DOCs

The potentially powerful approach of functional brain imaging and particularly of

fMRI has been shown in the diagnosis (Hirsch, 2005; Laureys et al., 2000; Schiff et al., 2005)

and prognosis (Di et al., 2008; Coleman 2009) of DOC. However, in the absence of a full

understanding of the neural correlates of consciousness, even a near-to-normal activation in

response to passive stimulations (e.g. auditive, tactile or visual stimuli) cannot be considered

as a proof of preserved awareness. Instead, all that can be inferred is that a specific brain

region is, to some degree, still able to activate and process relevant sensory stimuli. In the

absence of a cerebral activation during passive fMRI paradigms, it could be argued that the

patient may have intact cognition but failing peripheral sensory systems (e.g., may be deaf or

with hearing impairments) or, for visual stimuli, did not look at or fixate the presented visual

targets (the latter needing visual eye tracking devices which are difficult to calibrate in non-

collaborative patients). Resting state fMRI acquisitions are easy to perform (i.e., do not need

auditory, visual or somatosensory stimulation equipment in the fMRI environment) and could

have a potentially broader and faster translation into clinical practice. Indeed, recent progress

in investigating spontaneous brain activity showed the clinical relevance of these studies in

the absence of the patients’ collaboration, particularly important in vegetative and minimally

conscious patients. Cauda and co-workers (2009), for example, showed a dysfunctional

default mode network (defined as a set of area encompassing posterior-cingulate/precuneus,

anterior cingulated/mesiofrontal cortex and tempero-parietal junctions, showing more activity

at rest than during attention-demanding tasks; Raichle et al., 2001) in three patients in a

vegetative state. They found decreased connectivity in several brain regions, including the

 

6

dorsolateral prefrontal cortex and anterior cingulated cortex, especially in the right

hemisphere. Boly and co-workers (2009) demonstrated absent cortico-thalamic functional

connectivity (i.e. cross correlation of BOLD signal between precuneal areas and medial

thalamus) but partially preserved cortico-cortical connectivity within the default network in a

vegetative state patient studied 2.5 years following cardio-respiratory arrest. In this patient,

anticorrelations could also be observed between posterior cingulate/precuneus and a

previously identified task-positive cortical network, but both correlations and anticorrelations

were significantly reduced as compared to healthy controls. In the same study, a brain death

patient studied two days after a massive cranial hemorrhage and evolution to a comatose state,

showed no residual functional connectivity. Brain death is an irreversible coma with absent

brainstem reflexes – complimentary examinations such as the electroencephalogram shows an

isoelectric pattern and (MR) angiography shows absent intracranial arterial flow (Wijdicks,

1995). For brain death, measuring a non-zero BOLD signal in a brain lacking any neuronal

activity and arterial blood flow indicates the artifactual nature of the signal. In fact, it could be

shown that head motion (see figure 1), due to the arterial blood flow up the lower carotid

level, was the main source of the measured BOLD signal as confirmed by comparing its

power spectrum with the power spectrum of the motion curves obtained by the alignment

procedure of the echo planar images. The absence of any connectivity pattern in brain death

as published by Boly et al. (2009) is an important indication that the generally used

preprocessing technique (Fox et al., 2005) employed in resting state data and seed-voxel

connectivity analyses approach, as also applied for this study, are quite effective and reliable.

In a more comprehensive study (Vanhaudenhuyse et al., 2009), fourteen non-

communicative brain-damaged patients and fourteen healthy controls participated in a resting

state fMRI protocol. Connectivity was investigated by using probabilistic independent

component analysis (ICA) and an automated template-matching component selection

 

7

approach. Functional connectivity in all default network areas was found to be non-linearly

correlated with the degree of consciousness, ranging from healthy volunteers and locked-in

syndrome, to minimally conscious, vegetative and comatose patients. Furthermore,

connectivity in the precuneus was found to be significantly stronger in minimally conscious

patients compared to vegetative state patients, while locked-in syndrome patients’ default

network connectivity was shown to be not significantly different from healthy control

subjects.

Methodological issues in fMRI resting state assessment

There are two main ways to analyze resting–state functional connectivity MRI (rs-

fcMRI): (1) hypothesis-driven seed-voxel (Fox et al., 2005) and (2) data driven Independent

Component Analysis (ICA) approaches (McKeown et al., 1998) – each offering their own

advantages and limitations. The seed-voxel approach consists in extracting the BOLD time

course from a region of interest and determines the temporal correlation between this signal

(seed) and the time course from all other brain voxels (Fox et al., 2005). To better visualize

the correlation pattern and better analyze its properties, the seed approach can be integrated

with graph-theory methods (e.g., Fair et al., 2008; Hagmann et al., 2008). In graphical rs-

fcMRI representations, the resting-state BOLD time series for each of the region of interest

(ROI) extracted from the network under investigation are correlated among each other giving

a correlation matrix which can be represented as a weighted graph. To reduce spurious

variance unlikely to reflect neuronal activity, the BOLD signal is pre-processed by temporal

band-pass filter, spatial smoothing, and by regressing out head motion curves, whole brain

signal and ventricular and white matter signal (and each of their first-order derivative terms)

(Fox et al., 2005). This method, which is quite straightforward and gives very intuitive results

has been widely adopted and seems to give very consistent results (Fox and Raichle, 2007).

 

8

On the other hand, it has raised some controversial issues mostly related to the pre-processing

procedure, especially concerning the regressing out of the global activity from the BOLD

signal (an important step to obtain zero connectivity in brain death, Boly et al. 2009) which

might induce spurious anti-correlations (Fox et al., 2009; Murphy et al., 2009).

Contrary to the seed-voxel connectivity-assessment, ICA-based analyses (McKeown

et al., 1998) do not require an a priori definition of regions of interest. ICA analyzes the entire

BOLD dataset and decomposes it into components that are maximally statistically

independent (Hyvarinen et al., 2001). A number of studies have shown that ICA is a powerful

tool which can simultaneously extract a variety of different coherent neuronal networks (De

Luca et al., 2006; Esposito et al., 2008; Greicius et al., 2003; Greicius and Menon, 2004;

Greicius et al., 2004; McKeown et al., 1998) and separate them from other signal

modulations, such as those induced by head motion or physiological confounds (e.g., cardiac

pulsation, respiratory cycle and slow changes in the depth and rate of breathing, Birn et al.,

2008; Perlbarg et al., 2007; Perlbarg and Marrelec, 2008).

Patients with chronic DOC develop brain atrophy and secondary hydrocephalus (i.e.,

ex-vaquo dilated ventricles) and traumatic brain injuries and focal hemorrhages will largely

deform the brain. This implies that even if a normalization procedure of the brain has been

performed, the selection of a proper seed region becomes difficult and requires visual

inspection of an expert eye. Instead, the ICA approach (as illustrated in the minimally

conscious patient shown in figure 2 is able to automatically identify a cerebral network even if

the brain is distorted and if the regions are not localized as expected. To recognize the

network (even if it involves only half a brain as in the reported case) becomes an easier task.

Of course the problem of automatically selecting the right independent component in cases

like the one reported remains a delicate issue. Simply performing a similarity test (Esposito

2005) has shown to be not successful for choosing the right component (e.g., the component

 

9

presented in figure 2 was selected by visual inspection). Different approaches could be

implemented, such as masking, which can restrict the similarity test only to the part of the

brain which by expert inspection of structural imaging or other neuroimaging data are

considered less damaged and, hence, still able to present a pattern of activity similar to

healthy subjects.

Another important issue in the study of spontaneous BOLD signal fluctuations,

especially for patients that show a significantly reduced neuronal activity, is the possible

contamination by artifact and noise sources (Birn et al., 2006; Chuang and Chen, 2001;

Cordes et al., 2000). When dealing with non-collaborative patients, major confounds in

resting state fMRI acquisitions and analysis are movement, pulse and respiration artifacts.

ICA seems to offer the advantage to better isolate physiological artifacts from the neuronal

components and this is why data-driven approaches are now being commonly adopted in this

field (Beall and Lowe, 2007; Birn et al., 2008; Perlbarg et al., 2007). Clinical studies should

preferably perform heart rate and respiratory rate recording (Gray et al., 2009) and

simultaneous real movement monitoring which would improve resting state analyses.

Conclusions and future directions

Recent progress in the study of spontaneous brain activity not only sheds new light on

the understanding of the human brain functionality but offers new opportunities for clinical

investigations of the most disparate neurological pathologies. The world of DOC research

started taking advantage from the study of spontaneous activity and studies of resting

metabolism and spontaneous electrical activity at the scalp level in patients with disorder of

consciousness can now be supported by BOLD activity studies during rest. What makes fMRI

approaches very appealing is the large diffusion in medical facilities of new generation MRI

scanners, able now not only to perform structural but also functional sequences. Easy as a

 

10

resting state protocol using a common echo planar imaging sequence may appear, and thus

suitable for clinical purposes, rapid transient “clonic” motions during the acquisition (which

induce big artifacts on the BOLD measurements) and DOC patients’ highly deformed brains

(making comparisons with healthy normal brains difficult) challenge data analysis and

interpretation of these data.

Recent progress in data analysis has made it possible to increase our understanding of

the BOLD signal properties and its spatial patterns in cohorts of healthy subjects, where head

motions are controllable and the brain’s structure does not have a too large between subject

variability. For clinical purposes, we need to go from group level analysis to reliable

individual assessments. Before the technique can be employed in clinical routine as a

diagnostic or prognostic biomarker at the single subject level, we will need to tackle the

problems of spatial normalization (e.g., novel warping methods, Ashburner, 2007) and

movement correction.

Acknowledgment: SL is Senior Research Associate; AS, MB, QN are Post-doctoral Fellows

and MAB is Research Fellow at the Fonds de la Recherche Scientifique (FRS). AV was

funded by French Speaking Community Concerted Research Action (ARC 06/11-340). AD is

funded by the DISCOS Marie-Curie Research Training Network. This work was also

supported by European Commission (Mindbridge, DECODER, COST-CATIA), McDonnell

10 

 

11

Foundation, Mind Science Foundation, FRS, Reine Elisabeth Medical Foundation and

University and University Hospital of Liège.

References

Andrews, K., Murphy, L., Munday, R. and Littlewood, C. (1996). Misdiagnosis of the

vegetative state: retrospective study in a rehabilitation unit. BMJ, 313 (7048):13-16.

Ashburner, J. (2007). A Fast Diffeomorphic Image Registration Algorithm. NeuroImage 38

(1), 95-113

Beall, E. B., and Lowe, M. J. (2007). Isolating physiologic noise sources with independently

determined spatial measures. Neuroimage 37, 1286-300.

Beckmann, C.F., DeLuca, M., Devlin, J.T., Smith, S.M., (2005). Investigations into resting-

state connectivity using independent component analysis. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci

360, 1001-1013.

Bernat, J. L. (2006). Chronic disorders of consciousness. Lancet 367, 1181-92.

Birn, R. M., Diamond, J. B., Smith, M. A., and Bandettini, P. A. (2006). Separating

respiratory-variation-related fluctuations from neuronal-activity-related fluctuations in fMRI.

Neuroimage 31, 1536-48.

Birn, R. M., Murphy, K., and Bandettini, P. A. (2008). The effect of respiration variations on

independent component analysis results of resting state functional connectivity. Hum Brain

Mapp 29, 740-50.

Biswal, B., Yetkin, F. Z., Haughton, V. M., and Hyde, J. S. (1995). Functional connectivity in

the motor cortex of resting human brain using echo-planar MRI. Magn.Reson.Med. 34, 537-

541.

11 

 

12

Boly, M., Balteau, E., Schnakers, C., Degueldre, C., Moonen, G., Luxen, A., Phillips, C.,

Peigneux, P., Maquet, P., and Laureys, S. (2007). Baseline brain activity fluctuations predict

somatosensory perception in humans. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 104, 12187-92.

Boly, M., Tshibanda, L., Vanhaudenhuyse, A., Noirhomme, Q., Schnakers, C., Ledoux, D.,

Boveroux, P., Garweg, C., Lambermont, B., Phillips, C., Luxen, A., Moonen, G., Bassetti, C.,

Maquet, P., and Laureys, S. (2009) . Functional connectivity in the default network during

resting state is preserved in a vegetative but not in a brain dead patient. Human Brain

Mapping 30, 2393-400

Boly M, Phillips C, Tshibanda L, Vanhaudenhuyse A, Schabus M, Dang-Vu TT, Moonen G,

Hustinx R, Maquet P, Laureys S (2008a) Intrinsic brain activity in altered stated of

consciousness – how conscious is the default mode of brain function? Annals of the New York

Academy of Sciences, 1129, 119–129.

Boly M, Phillips C, Balteau E, Schnakers C, Degueldre C, Moonen G, Luxen A, Peigneux P,

Faymonville ME, Maquet P, Laureys S (2008b) Consciousness and cerebral baseline activity

fluctuations Human Brain Mapping 29, 868-74.

Cauda, F., Micon, B.M., Sacco, K., Duca, S., D’Agata, F., Geminiani, G., Cannavero, S.

(2009). Disrupted intrinsic functional connectivity in the vegetative state. J. Neurol.

Neurosurg. Psychiatry 80, 429-431

Childs, N.L. and Mercer, W.N. (1996). Misdiagnosing the persistent vegetative state.

Misdiagnosis certainly occurs [letter; comment]. BMJ 313 (7062), 944.

Chuang, K. H., and Chen, J. H. (2001). IMPACT: image-based physiological artifacts

estimation and correction technique for functional MRI. Magn Reson Med 46, 344-53.

12 

Coleman, M.R., Davis, M.H., Rodd, J.M., Robson, T., Ali, A., Owen, A.M., Pickard, J.D.

(2009). Towards the routine use of brain imaging to aid the clinical diagnosis of disorders of

consciousness. Brain. 132 (Pt 9), 2541-52

 

13

Cordes, D., Haughton, V.M., Arfanakis, K., Wendt, G.J., Turski, P.A., Moritz, C.H., Quigley,

M.A., Meyerand, M.E., (2000). Mapping functionally related regions of brain with functional

connectivity MR imaging. AJNR Am J Neuroradiol 21, 1636-1644.

Damoiseaux, J.S., Rombouts, S.A., Barkhof, F., Scheltens, P., Stam, C.J., Smith, S.M.,

Beckmann, C.F., (2006). Consistent resting-state networks across healthy subjects. Proc Natl

Acad Sci U S A 103, 13848-13853.

De Luca, M., Beckmann, C.F., De Stefano, N., Matthews, P.M., Smith, S.M., (2006). fMRI

resting state networks define distinct modes of long-distance interactions in the human brain.

Neuroimage 29, 1359-1367.

Di H., Boly M., Weng X., Ledoux D., Laureys S., (2008) Neuroimaging activation studies in

the vegetative state: predictors of recovery? Clinical Medicine, 8, 502-507.

Esposito, F., Aragri, A., Pesaresi, I., Cirillo, S., Tedeschi, G., Marciano, E., Goebel, R., Di

Salle, F., 2008. Independent component model of the default-mode brain function: combining

individual-level and population-level analyses in resting-state fMRI. Magn Reson Imaging 26,

905-913.

Esposito, F., Scarabino, T., Hyvarinen, A., Himberg, J., Formisano, E., Comani, S., Tedeschi,

G., Goebel, R., Seifritz, E., Di Salle, F., (2005). Independent component analysis of fMRI

group studies by self-organizing clustering. Neuroimage 25, 193-205.

Fair, D.A., Cohen, A.L., Dosenbach, N.U., Church, J.A., Miezin, F.M., Barch, D.M., Raichle,

M.E., Petersen, S.E., Schlaggar, B.L., 2008. The maturing architecture of the brain's default

network. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 105, 4028-4032.

Fox, M. D., and Raichle, M. E. (2007). Spontaneous fluctuations in brain activity observed

with functional magnetic resonance imaging. Nat Rev Neurosci 8, 700-11.

13 

 

14

Fox, M. D., Snyder, A. Z., Vincent, J. L., Corbetta, M., Van Essen, D. C., and Raichle, M. E.

(2005). The human brain is intrinsically organized into dynamic, anticorrelated functional

networks. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 102, 9673-8.

Fox, M.D., Zhang, D., Snyder, A.Z., Raichle, M.E., (2009). The Global Signal and Observed

Anticorrelated Resting State Brain Networks. J Neurophysiol. 101 (6), 3270-83.

Giacino, J. T., Ashwal, S., Childs, N., Cranford, R., Jennett, B., Katz, D. I., Kelly, J. P.,

Rosenberg, J. H., Whyte, J., Zafonte, R. D., and Zasler, N. D. (2002). The minimally

conscious state: definition and diagnostic criteria. Neurology 58, 349-53.

Giacino, J. T., Hirsch, J., Schiff, N., and Laureys, S. (2006). Functional neuroimaging

applications for assessment and rehabilitation planning in patients with disorders of

consciousness. Arch Phys Med Rehabil 87, S67-76.

Greicius, M. D., Krasnow, B., Reiss, A. L., and Menon, V. (2003). Functional connectivity in

the resting brain: a network analysis of the default mode hypothesis. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S

A 100, 253-8.

Greicius, M. D., Srivastava, G., Reiss, A. L., and Menon, V. (2004). Default-mode network

activity distinguishes Alzheimer's disease from healthy aging: evidence from functional MRI.

Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 101, 4637-42.

Gray, M.A., Minati, L., Harrison, N.A., Gianaros, P.J., Napadow, V., Critchley, H.D., (2009).

Physiological recordings: basic concepts and implementation during functional magnetic

resonance imaging. Neuroimage 47, 1105-1115.

Hagmann, P., Cammoun, L., Gigandet, X., Meuli, R., Honey, C.J., Wedeen, V.J., Sporns, O.,

2008. Mapping the structural core of human cerebral cortex. PLoS Biol 6, e159.

Hirsch, J. (2005). Functional neuroimaging during altered states of consciousness: how and

what do we measure? Prog Brain Res 150, 25-43.

Hyvarinen, A., Karhunen, J., Oja, E., (2001). Independent Component Analysis.

14 

 

15

Jennett, B., and Plum, F. (1972). Persistent vegetative after brain damage. Lancet 1, 4.

Lambrecht, A. (2002). Observing mechanical dissipation in the quantum vacuum: an

experimental challenge. Laser physics at the limits (Hartmut Figger, Dieter Meschede, Claus

Zimmermann Eds.). Berlin/New York: Springer, 197.

Laureys, S., Giacino, J. T., Schiff, N. D., Schabus, M., and Owen, A. M. (2006). How should

functional imaging of patients with disorders of consciousness contribute to their clinical

rehabilitation needs? Curr Opin Neurol 19, 520-7.

Laureys, S., Lemaire, C., Maquet, P., Phillips, C., and Franck, G. (1999). Cerebral

metabolism during vegetative state and after recovery to consciousness. J Neurol Neurosurg

Psychiatry 67, 121.

Laureys, S., Faymonville, M.E., Degueldre, C., Fiore, G.D., Damas, P., Lambermont, B.,

Janssens, N., Aerts, J., Franck, G., Luxen, A., Moonen, G., Lamy, M., Maquet, P., (2000).

Auditory processing in the vegetative state. Brain 123 ( Pt 8), 1589-1601.

Laureys, S., Owen, A. M., and Schiff, N. D. (2004). Brain function in coma, vegetative state,

and related disorders. Lancet Neurol 3, 537-46.

Laureys S (2005) The neural correlate of (un)awareness: lessons from the vegetative state

Trends Cogn Sci, 9 556-559 .

Lowe, M. J., Mock, B. J., and Sorenson, J. A. (1998). Functional connectivity in single and

multislice echoplanar imaging using resting-state fluctuations. Neuroimage 7, 119-32.

Majerus, S., Gill-Thwaites, H., Andrews, K., and Laureys, S. (2005). Behavioral evaluation of

consciousness in severe brain damage. Prog Brain Res 150, 397-413.

McKeown, M.J., Makeig, S., Brown, G.G., Jung, T.P., Kindermann, S.S., Bell, A.J.,

Sejnowski, T.J., 1998. Analysis of fMRI data by blind separation into independent spatial

components. Hum Brain Mapp 6, 160-188.

15 

 

16

Mitra, P.P., Ogawa, S., Hu, X., Ugurbil, K., 1997. The nature of spatiotemporal changes in

cerebral hemodynamics as manifested in functional magnetic resonance imaging. Magn Reson

Med 37, 511-518.

Nir, Y., Hasson, U., Levy, I., Yeshurun, Y., and Malach, R. (2006). Widespread functional

connectivity and fMRI fluctuations in human visual cortex in the absence of visual

stimulation. Neuroimage 30, 1313-24.

Perlbarg, V., Bellec, P., Anton, J. L., Pelegrini-Issac, M., Doyon, J., and Benali, H. (2007).

CORSICA: correction of structured noise in fMRI by automatic identification of ICA

components. Magn Reson Imaging 25, 35-46.

Plum, F., and Posner, J. B. (1972). The diagnosis of stupor and coma. Contemp Neurol Ser

10, 1-286.

Raichle, M. E., MacLeod, A. M., Snyder, A. Z., Powers, W. J., Gusnard, D. A., and Shulman,

G. L. (2001). A default mode of brain function. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 98, 676-82.

Raichle, M. E., and Snyder, A. Z. (2007). A default mode of brain function: a brief history of

an evolving idea. Neuroimage 37, 1083-90; discussion 1097-9.

Ray, C., (1991). Time, space and philosophy. London/New York: Routledge 10, 205.

Rombouts, S. A., Damoiseaux, J. S., Goekoop, R., Barkhof, F., Scheltens, P., Smith, S. M.,

and Beckmann, C. F. (2009). Model-free group analysis shows altered BOLD FMRI networks

in dementia. Hum Brain Mapp 30, 256-66.

Schiff, N. D. (2006a). Measurements and models of cerebral function in the severely injured

brain. J Neurotrauma 23, 1436-49.

Schiff, N. D. (2006b). Multimodal neuroimaging approaches to disorders of consciousness. J

Head Trauma Rehabil 21, 388-97.

16 

 

17

Schiff, N. D., Rodriguez-Moreno, D., Kamal, A., Kim, K. H., Giacino, J. T., Plum, F., and

Hirsch, J. (2005). fMRI reveals large-scale network activation in minimally conscious

patients. Neurology 64, 514-23.

Schnakers, C., Vanhaudenhuyse, A., Giacino, J., Ventura, M., Boly, M., Majerus, S.,

Moonen, G. and Laureys, S. (2009b). Diagnostic accuracy of the vegetative and minimally

conscious state: clinical consensus versus standardized neurobehavioral assessment. BMC

Neurol. 9, 35.

Vanhaudenhuyse A, Noirhomme Q, Tshibanda L, Bruno MA, Boveroux P,Schnakers S,

Soddu A, Perlbarg V, Ledoux D, Brichant JF, Moonen G, Maquet P, Greicius M, Laureys S,

Boly M (2009) Default network connectivity reflects the level of consciousness in non-

communicative brain damaged patients. Brain, in press

Vincent, J. L., Patel, G. H., Fox, M. D., Snyder, A. Z., Baker, J. T., Van Essen, D. C., Zempel,

J. M., Snyder, L. H., Corbetta, M., and Raichle, M. E. (2007). Intrinsic functional architecture

in the anaesthetized monkey brain. Nature 447, 83-6.

Xiong, J., Parsons, L. M., Gao, J. H., and Fox, P. T. (1999). Interregional connectivity to

primary motor cortex revealed using MRI resting state images. Hum Brain Mapp 8, 151-6.

Wijdicks, EF. (1995): Determining brain death in adults. Neurology 45(5):1003-11.

17 

 

18

Figure 1: Independent component (IC) selected as default mode in brain death (female, age

50 y). The selection criteria were based on similarity testing with an averaged default mode

map obtained from an independent control group of 20 healthy volunteers (black and white

contour). Similarity value was 0.11. (a) Spatial map of the IC selected as default mode; (b)

Motion curves illustrating translation parameters (in mm) for x (red), y (green) and z (blue)

and rotation (in °) for pitch (yellow), roll (purple) and yaw (cyan) parameters together with

the time course of the IC selected as default mode; (c) Power spectrum of the motion curves

and of the time course of the IC selected as default mode; (d) Graphical representation (i.e,

fingerprint) of default mode temporal properties showing five frequency bands, temporal

entropy and one-lag autocorrelation and spatial properties (spatial entropy, skewness, kurtosis

and clustering) (normalized values).

18 

 

19

19 

 

20

Figure 2: Independent component selected as default mode in a healthy subject (male, age 39

y), a locked-in syndrome patient (female, age 24 y with a brainstem stroke), a minimally

conscious state (MCS) patient (male, age 24 y post-traumatic) and a vegetative state (VS)

patient (male, age 87 y post-traumatic). The selection criteria were based on similarity testing

with an averaged default mode map obtained from an independent control group of 20 healthy

volunteers (black and white contour). Similarity values were respectively 0.65, 0.52 and 0.10.

Note that for the presented minimally conscious patient this method failed to select the proper

default mode because of the very asymmetrical brain damage and network selection; hence, it

was based on visual inspection of both the spatial maps and time courses.

20 

 

21

21 

 

22

22