66
EMOTION SCRIPTS IN ORGANIZATIONS: A MULTI-LEVEL MODEL DONALD E. GIBSON Dolan School of Business Fairfield University North Benson Road Fairfield, CT 06611 (203) 254-4000, x2841 [email protected]

Emotion regulation and scripts5

Embed Size (px)

DESCRIPTION

 

Citation preview

Page 1: Emotion regulation and scripts5

EMOTION SCRIPTS IN ORGANIZATIONS: A MULTI-LEVEL MODEL

DONALD E. GIBSON

Dolan School of BusinessFairfield UniversityNorth Benson RoadFairfield, CT 06611

(203) 254-4000, [email protected]

Page 2: Emotion regulation and scripts5

Emotion Scripts Organizations: A Multi-Level Model

A paradox of emotions is that they are simultaneously in our control and out of our

control. “In our control” implies that emotions tend to follow particular patterns and are thus

amenable to prediction and regulation; “out of our control” suggests that they are idiosyncratic,

difficult-to-predict states. Experientially, this paradox is seen in the fact that strong feelings of

anger may elude our control, but even in a fury we rarely break our most precious objects

(Frijda, 1988). Our theorizing about emotion also illustrates this paradox. Emotions have been

conceived as interruptions (Mandler, 1985), as ineffable bodily states (James, 1884), and as

largely automatic responses out of our conscious control (Bargh & Chartrand, 1999; Damasio,

1994), yet emotions also follow predictable patterns, even “laws” (Frijda, 1988), and current

theories now focus on emotion regulation, emphasizing how commonplace emotion control is in

daily life (see Gross, 1998). It is my contention that this in-control / out-of-control paradox can

be fruitfully examined by conceiving of emotions as scripted responses. Emotions exhibit a

script-like structure. They are seen, experientially (by laypeople) and conceptually (by

researchers) as sequences of events based on an if-then goal-directed logic. At the same time,

social norms, individual differences, and differing contexts produce infinite variations in these

scripts. Thus, the existence of scripts suggests that control is possible, but variation sets limits on

that control.

This article examines emotional experience and expression from the perspective of script

theory. I present a model integrating a variety of script approaches as a multi-level model (see

Figure 1). The purpose of the model is to integrate various viewpoints, to accentuate connections

between disparate strands of literature rather than to add new strands. Script theory is useful in

this purpose because scripts reveal both the descriptive content of what “typically” happens

2

Page 3: Emotion regulation and scripts5

when emotions are felt and expressed, and they also offer clues into what ought to happen, the

normative content of what we expect to occur and what we regard as appropriate.

I will assert here that examination of emotion scripts is especially helpful in

understanding the nature of emotion in organizations. Organizations are boundedly rational

structures that constrain individuals’ experience and expression of emotion (Mumby & Putnam,

1992). In this context, many interactions have a scripted quality; for example, researchers have

analyzed performance appraisals (Gioia, Donnellon & Sims, 1989), selection interviews (Poole,

Gray & Gioia, 1990), and sales calls (Leigh & McGraw, 1989) as representing cognitive and

behavioral scripts. At the same time, the complexity of the variables involved—phenomena at

the individual, group, and organizational level—adds to the variation in scripts. Anger may not

be (and typically is not) expressed in the same way in two different organizations, in two

different groups, even with two different target individuals. However, as researchers begin to

refine their work in emotions and seek to demonstrate the utility of their theories to practicing

managers, they are drawn to identifying antecedents and outcomes of emotions. Script theory

offers a template against which to compare and contrast this complexity and variety. It applies

the logic of sequences of events to discovering how emotions might play out in organizational

situations.

As this Research Companion will attest, there are myriad ways of viewing and

researching emotion. Often, these varied approaches are set up as opposing dichotomies. The

“biological” and “cognitivist” perspectives are said to be “competing conceptualizations in the

literature” (Forgas, 1996: 278), while the “universalistic approach” (that there are basic

emotional responses characterizing all global cultures) is competing with the “cultural relativity

approach” (that cultures significantly shape the experience and expression of emotions), and this

competition is seen as a “major controversy” (Scherer & Wallbott, 1994, p. 310). One purpose of

3

Page 4: Emotion regulation and scripts5

this article is to show how these dichotomous views are interrelated, and in fact, can be thought

of different levels of emotion scripts rather than as competing explanations. As evidence for the

usefulness of scripts in integrating different levels of analysis, researchers have concluded that

scripts offer a way to reconcile the universalistic versus cross-cultural variation approaches to

understanding emotion meaning (Russell, 1991b; White, 2000).

I begin by defining how emotion scripts have been used in the extant emotions literature.

I then show how scripts have been evoked at a variety of levels: the biological level, the

cognitive level, the social level, the relational level, and the organizational level. I emphasize that

understanding emotion scripts at the organizational level depends on understanding scripts at the

preceding levels, and explore how the emotion script approach offers a methodology and

conceptual framework that can heighten our understanding of emotions in organizations.

Definitions: Scripts, Schemas, and Related Phenomena

Scripts are a type of knowledge structure; they are individuals’ structured ideas about

how thoughts, feelings and actions are carried out in particular situations. More formally,

schemas will be defined here as “organized representations of past behavior and experience that

function as theories about reality to guide a person in construing new experience” (Baldwin,

1992, p. 468). A cognitive script is a type of schema representing individuals’ ideas about the

appropriate sequences of events that occur in specific situations (Schank & Abelson, 1977;

Baldwin, 1992). Well-known examples include the “restaurant script,” depicting individuals’

ideas about the stereotypical order of events in ordering food in a restaurant (Schank & Abelson,

1977). Scripts for social situations are seen as characterized by 1) declarative or descriptive

knowledge that helps the perceiver describe what behavior tends to be followed by what

responses (“asking for the menu in a restaurant is typically followed by the person ordering

food”), and 2) procedural knowledge that offers a guide to the perceiver’s behavior (e.g., “If I

4

Page 5: Emotion regulation and scripts5

respond negatively to this person, they are likely to respond negatively back to me.” See

Baldwin, 1992; Cantor & Kihlstrom, 1985).

Scripts are goal driven. Scripts represent a temporally based hierarchical structure

consisting of “in-order-to” relationships between action elements (Lichtenstein & Brewer, 1980).

That is, an activity is performed in order to accomplish subsequent activity which is higher up in

the hierarchy. Selection of food on the menu, for example, is done in order to reach the goal of

eating in a restaurant. Effective performance appraisal interviews are structured as a specific

sequence of events as a way as to achieve the goal of providing useful feedback to an employee.

This structure implies that scripts are organized as goal-subgoal hierarchies, characteristic of

human goals in general, and add structure to both memory and behaviors (Austin & Vancouver,

1996; Lord & Kernan, 1987). In addition, scripts are adaptable; they are easily elaborated upon

to incorporate new experiences (Abelson, 1981; Bower, Black, & Turner, 1979), a phenomenon

called “tagging” (Lord & Kernan, 1987, p. 267). Having shared scripts—common

understandings of goal-directed behavior chains in well-known situations—is functional in that it

facilitates interactions and reduces ambiguity. Researchers suggest that in the organizational

context when employees share the same script this is beneficial because it “creates convergence

of knowledge and action, offering a strategy for reducing conceptual divergence among

individuals and teams confronted with the same situation” (Zohar & Luria, 2003, p. 841).

Emotion Scripts

This article will focus on a particular type of script, an emotion script, which refers to an

individual’s knowledge of emotion episodes and the prototypical sequence of events

characterizing particular emotions. As with cognitive scripts, emotion scripts contain both

descriptive elements (e.g., the ability of individuals to describe what causes feelings of anger and

5

Page 6: Emotion regulation and scripts5

what anger expressions look like) and normative elements (e.g., the ability of individuals to

identify contextual expectations and sanctions attached to anger expressions) (Fischer, 1991).

I will refer to the contents of an emotion script as a person’s specific ideas about what

occurs, for example, when one feels and expresses emotions such as anger or fear or surprise.

Abelson expresses this idea succinctly when he argues that, “A sizeable set of inferences can be

made from the knowledge that, say, ‘John is angry.’ A negative thing has happened to John; he

blames it on someone; he regards it as unjust; he is aroused, flushed, and prone to swear or lash

out; he may seek revenge on the instigator, and so on” (1981, p. 727). Emotion script theory

suggests that individuals’ knowledge structure for emotions is scriptlike; emotions are best

thought of as prototypical sequences of events that comprise an episode (see Fehr & Russell,

1984; Lazarus, 1991; Russell, 1991b; Shaver et al., 1987). An emotion episode is typically

comprised of four primary elements: 1) an antecedent or triggering event; 2) a physiological

reaction, and an awareness of “feeling” the emotional reaction; 3) expression or behavior or

effortful regulation of expression or behavior, and 4) an outcome, which may include the

individual’s own reaction to the episode as well as the reactions of others. I depict the general

contents of four typical emotion episodes in Table 1 (derived from Shaver et al., 1987). The

script concept is useful in that, when elicited, it helps to show how social reality is constructed,

and also indicates how “constructions of reality translate into social behavior through action

rules” (Abelson, 1981, p. 727).

Two further distinctions are in order. First, emotion scripts differ in the degree to which

there is agreement among individuals as to the specific contents of the script. When there is

substantial agreement about the antecedents and consequences for a particular emotion in a

particular setting, this is considered a strong script. A weak script is one exhibiting less

agreement on common antecedents and consequences (Abelson, 1981). For example,

6

Page 7: Emotion regulation and scripts5

individuals’ understanding of what happens when an employee expresses anger in a staff

meeting may be substantially shared: there may be substantial agreement that such expressions

are inappropriate and will elicit sanctions from the leader of the meeting. However, employees’

understanding of what happens when anxiety is expressed may be less elaborated; there may be

less common agreement on what the causes and consequences of this emotion expression are.

Second, emotion scripts vary to the degree to which they originate from idiosyncratic or

shared experiences (Fischer, 1991; Frijda & Mesquita, 1994). Individuals may have their own

emotion scripts developed on the basis of their own upbringing and family experiences. Other

scripts are widely shared based on cultural norms, for example the norm to feel sadness and cry

at funerals and feel happiness and smile at weddings (Hochschild, 1979). A person may use this

emotion script knowledge to their advantage. For example, an employee may be aware that in

professional roles the expression of extreme emotions is typically sanctioned (e.g., Gibson,

1997), but may have an individually developed script suggesting that expressions of extreme

emotions may, at times, generate the desired effect in others (see Pierce, 1995).

In line with the idiosyncratic approach, Tomkins (1979) developed a script theory

suggesting that individual personalities are made up of more or less salient scripts, driven by

emotions. He argued that individuals form scripts based on three criteria: 1) when they

experienced the most “intense and enduring affect” (1979, p. 223); 2) when affect changed

during an event suddenly (from positive to negative or the opposite); and 3) when sequences of

affect were repeated (e.g., an individual experiences a change from positive to negative affect

every time an event happens). While I acknowledge the existence of idiosyncratic scripts, the

emphasis in this article will be on the extent to which biological, cognitive, social, relational, and

organizational normative forces constrain and shape these idiosyncratic scripts.

7

Page 8: Emotion regulation and scripts5

A Multi-Level Model of Emotion Scripts

Given this basic idea, that emotions can be conceptualized as scripted sequences of

events, researchers have turned to the question, “Where do emotion scripts come from?” The

answer this chapter provides is that scripts emerge at multiple levels. These levels are depiected

in this model (from bottom to top) in terms of the relative effect of context and script specificity

(see Figure 1). The first level, the biological script, is considered the most basic and operates

primarily automatically and unconsciously (see LeDoux, 1996; Plutchik, 1980). Biological

scripts provide the basic map on which the succeeding layers operate. The second level, the

cognitive script, emphasizes the degree to which emotions arise from individuals’ appraisal of

specific situations. Cognitive scripts are more specific than biological scripts in that particular

antecedents (for example, the accomplishment of an important task) are predicted to lead to

specific emotions (for example, joy). They are not regarded as culturally specific, however;

cognitive scripts are assumed to operate intrapsychically to explain the connection between

cognitions and emotions. The third level, social scripts, suggest the degree to which emotions are

socially constructed and driven by power relationships and cultural norms (see Kemper, 1990;

Russell, 1991b; Scherer & Walbott, 1994). The fourth level, relational scripts, involve emotion

scripts enacted primarily in dyadic relationships (see Baldwin, 1992; Fehr et al., 1999; Fitness,

2000). The fifth level, organizational scripts, are characterized by substantial complexity

(involving multiple individual and group relationships; power and gender effects, among others),

and specificity: organizations are seen as providing relatively specific scripts for the feeling and

expression of emotions (see Fitness, 2000; Gibson, 1995, 1997; Hochschild, 1983).

This model is not meant to be comprehensive in the sense of including all possible levels

of scripts. Depending on one’s perspective, additional layers could be added and their listing re-

ordered. Rather, I illustrate this multi-level model as a way of providing a foundation for

8

Page 9: Emotion regulation and scripts5

understanding the focus of this chapter: the emergence of emotion scripts in organizations. It is

my assertion that we cannot understand the intricacies of scripted emotion experience and

expression in organizations without first understanding what drives and anticipates these scripts.

Biological Scripts

From this view, emotions are considered basic and hard wired, and our tendencies to act

are largely pre-programmed. This view has emotions driven by biology; they are primarily

adaptive responses to aid survival of species. While complex emotional responses exist and

cultural and social forces shape emotional responses, the biological view emphasizes that human

emotional responses, prior to the intervention of conscious cognition and cultural overlays, have

a basic quality that is largely universal: all humans respond to needs in their environment with

relatively similar emotional expressions representing relatively similar feelings (see Ekman,

1992, 1994; but see critiques in Russell, 1994; Wierzbicka, 1994).

In The Expression of the Emotions in Man and Animals Darwin (1872/1998) argued that

while the developing anatomy of a species could be explained as adaptive responses to an

organism’s environment, Darwin also realized that evolution applied not only to anatomy, but to

an animal’s mind and expressive behavior as well. Darwin viewed emotions, and specifically

their expression, as functional responses by animals to survive in their environment. Expressed

emotions acted as signals and as preparations for action, and communicated information to others

about intentions. Thus, there is an evolutionary connection between an animal baring hits teeth

and the snarl of a human being, the similarity in laughing expressions by monkeys and humans,

and the universal tendency for one’s hair to stand “on end” in conditions of anger and fear.

Darwin emphasized that many, but not all, emotional expressions are unlearned or innate.

He showed, for instance, that emotional expressions appear in very young children in the same

form as adults, before much opportunity for learning has occurred, and some expressions appear

9

Page 10: Emotion regulation and scripts5

in similar form in widely distinct races and groups of humans. Darwin’s contribution is the

notion that emotional expressions are largely universal, and thus have a biological basis, rather

than being culturally bound. Since emotions serve evolutionary functions, they must exist,

though modified, in observable patterns throughout the world.

The evolutionary view is supported by more recent lines of research. Ekman (1972, 1992)

drew on and extended Darwin’s ideas by showing that people from widely ranging cultures can

relatively accurately recognize emotion expressions for six basic emotions: surprise, happiness,

anger, fear, disgust, and sadness. Ekman argued that “there are distinctive movements of the

facial muscles for each of a number of primary affect states, and these are universal to mankind”

(Ekman & Friesen, 1969, p. 71). However, he also cautioned that while the movement of facial

muscles shows universal tendencies, the evoking stimuli, subjective feelings, emotional “display

rules” and the behavioral consequences “all can vary from one culture to another” (1969, p. 73).

His research, then, is largely consistent with a “dual-phase” model in which biological affects are

primary, and cultural or cognitive processes are a secondary, though critically important, overlay

(White, 2000, p. 32).

Plutchik (1980) in a “psychoevolutionary synthesis” argued that because all organisms

face “common survival problems,” including “finding food, avoiding predators and locating

mates” (1980, p. 130), emotions serve as behavioral patterns that help organisms adapt to these

problems by providing internal preparations for action as well as external behavior appropriate to

controlling the environment. Thus, anger successfully prepares the body by increasing the heart

rate and heightening attentiveness, and seeks to control environmental forces through facial

expression (e.g., snarling, hair raised) and action (aggressive approach) designed to elicit fear in

others. Viewing emotions from this evolutionary functional approach, Plutchik argues that there

are eight basic emotions (anger, fear, anticipation, surprise, acceptance, disgust, joy and sadness)

10

Page 11: Emotion regulation and scripts5

corresponding to the needs of any organism to respond to existential crises, including protection,

exploration, and reproduction.

Recent neurological research has provided some support for the evolutionary point of

view. Summarizing his own and other research examining fear centers in the brain, LeDoux

expressed his view as: “I believe that the basic building blocks of emotions are neural systems

that mediate behavioral interactions with the environment, particularly behaviors that take care

of fundamental problems of survival” (1996, p. 125). He also argued that different “basic”

emotions rely on unique centers and pathways in the brain rather than indicating an “emotional

center” for a variety of responses. He concluded that human brains are largely programmed by

evolution to respond in certain ways to significant situations, so there is a large dose of

automaticity in our emotional responses. Determining significance is a combination of

evolutionary history and our own memories of past experiences. While much of our initial

reactions are automatic, when we become conscious of this neural activity, we can be said to

“feel”—we can have the strong subjective reactions we think of as emotions. Emotions, then, are

“unconscious processes that can sometimes give rise to conscious content” (1996, p. 269).

What are the implications of the biological approach for emotion script theory? First,

biological approaches provide support for the notion that emotions can be considered as

sequences of events beginning with sensing the environment for survival clues, reacting in

patterned physiological ways, and ending in behaviors or intended behaviors. Second, biological

approaches, by emphasizing the existence of relatively discrete “basic” emotions, suggest that

there are identifiable, and relatively strong emotion scripts surrounding a certain small number of

feeling states. The fact that researchers using the biological approach have not been able to agree

on the identity or number of basic emotions has been critiqued (see e.g., Russell, 1994). While

this lack of agreement hinders the development of universalistic scripts, the proposal of basic

11

Page 12: Emotion regulation and scripts5

emotions has provided a foundation to allow script researchers to explore families of scripts,

especially those for anger and fear (see Fehr & Russell, 1984).

What the biological view means for thinking about emotion scripts is that at its primary

level, our emotional responses are following biological scripts. Even researchers who emphasize

cultural differences note the importance of a biological “core”; for example, Russell (1991b, p.

437) states about emotional expression, “There is a core of emotional communication that has to

do with being human rather than with being a member of a particular culture.” As biological

scripts play out in real interactions, they are typically interrupted by consciousness and by willful

self regulation (Frijda, 1986). The level of regulation depends on the significance of the event

(how fearful one is, for example), and on the strength of the conscious scripts that are invoked to

alter the basic biological script. We examine these more conscious scripts next.

Cognitive Scripts

While the biological and evolutionary approaches emphasize the relative automaticity of

basic emotional responses, cognitive approaches emphasize the degree to which cognitions

impinge on nearly every aspect of feeling and expressing emotions. From a cognitive

perspective, how a person interprets or appraises a meaningful event and how emotions are

conceived as knowledge structures influence how different emotions are perceived, understood,

labeled and expressed (Fitness & Fletcher, 1993). Two research streams, one focused on

emotions as prototypes and one focused on cognitive appraisals of emotion, exemplify the

cognitive perspective on emotion scripts.

The prototype approach suggests that individuals conceive of emotions as “fuzzy sets” of

attributes. Emotions have been notoriously difficult for researchers to classically define because

there is not a set of conclusive necessary and sufficient features (such as would be true about the

category of even numbers, for example—see Shaver et al., 1987). Indeed, the difficulty

12

Page 13: Emotion regulation and scripts5

researchers have had in defining emotions (see Averill, 1983; Buck, 1990), combined with the

fact that laypeople have a strong intuitive sense of what emotions are and how they operate,

speaks to the applicability of the prototype approach (Fehr & Baldwin, 1996). According to this

approach, individuals categorize emotions based on whether they bear a resemblance to what

they think of as prototypical instances of emotion (see Rosch, 1975). Thus, just as “chair” is a

prototypical subcategory of “furniture,” “anger” and “fear” are considered by laypeople to be

prototypical subcategories of “emotion.” Shaver et al. (1987), found, for example, that when 135

emotion terms were subjected to hierarchical cluster analysis, five “basic” level emotion words

emerged: love, joy, anger, sadness and fear. They concluded that a large number of emotion

lexical terms could be tied to a small number of prototypical emotions. The variability in

emotion words tend to specify either the intensity of a basic emotion (i.e., rage being more

intense than annoyance; jubilation being more intense than satisfaction) or the antecedent

context in which the emotion arises (i.e., disappointment tends to be preceded by differing

antecedents than grief). Consistent with a prototype approach, these findings suggest a hierarchy

in which a range of emotion words (such as grief, annoyance, jubilation) are subordinate to a

basic level (love, joy, anger, sadness, fear) which is subordinate to a superordinate level

(emotions). The hierarchical structure of these prototypes has been supported in several studies

(see summary in Cropanzano et al., 2003).

In examining individuals’ knowledge structures of emotions, researchers further

discovered that these structures conceive of emotions as containing prototypical sequences of

events. That is, if asked, individuals not only provide good examples of what they think an

emotion is (e.g., “I felt really angry when my supervisor accused me of being late!”), they also

conceive of anger in terms of whether it fits a likely sequence of events (“When he accused me, I

felt tense and sweaty—I had the urge to yell at him, but managed to control it.”). As noted above,

13

Page 14: Emotion regulation and scripts5

individuals conceive of emotional feeling and expressions in terms of event sequences, or scripts.

As Fehr and Russel (1984) and Russell (1991a) depict these structures, emotions are categorized

depending on their prototypical features. These features are organized cognitions and “knowable

subevents: the causes, beliefs, feelings, physiological changes, desires, overt actions, and vocal

and facial expressions” of emotions. They are ordered “in a causal sequence, in much the same

way that actions are ordered in a playwright’s script” (Russell, 1991b, p. 442).

Similarly, Shaver et al. (1987) characterize laypeople’s emotion scripts as episodes

beginning with an interpretation of an event as good or bad, helpful or harmful, consistent or

inconsistent with a person’s motives (see also Roseman, 1984). Depending on whether a

situation is perceived as being motive consistent or inconsistent, the individual then assesses

whether action is necessary. Based on an individual’s appraisal of the event (Is this a threat to

me? Am I justified in taking action? Does this event make me feel good?) a pattern of possible

responses is initiated. These action responses (including action tendencies, cognitive biases, and

physiological patterns) are seen as arising fairly automatically. However, individuals tend to also

simultaneously engage in self-control efforts, which can be initiated at any point in the emotion

process and directed at any of the components (appraisal, physiological response, and emotion

expression (see Frijda, 1986; Gross, 1998).

Closely related to the prototype approach, the cognitive appraisal approach focuses on

one aspect of this prototypical sequence: how an individual’s appraisal of the situation leads to

specific emotional responses (Lazarus, 1991). These researchers argue that it is an individual’s

evaluation or interpretation of events, rather than the events per se, that determine whether an

emotion will be felt and which emotion it will be (Roseman, 1984). The particular emotion felt

by an individual depends on their appraisal of the situation based on several dimensions. For

example, Roseman (1984) identifies an individual’s appraisal of perceived power (weak versus

14

Page 15: Emotion regulation and scripts5

strong), the probability of an outcome (uncertain versus certain), and his or her motivation state

(seeking to avoid punishment versus seeking to attain reward), among others (Roseman, 1984).

For example, anger is seen as resulting from the absence of a reward or presence of a punishment

that is caused by other people when a positive outcome is deserved (see Roseman, Spindel, &

Jose, 1990). Cognitive appraisal theorists differ from biologically-oriented theorists in their

emphasis that emotions are not primarily hard-wired unthinking processes, but rather, based

primarily on cognitive interpretations (appraisals) of situational cues (Lazarus, 1991).

Social Scripts

The notion of universal, evolutionary bases of emotion have come under attack (Scherer

& Wallbott, 1994; White, 2000). Sociologists and anthropologists argue that culture is not simply

an overlay to biological and cognitive patterned responses; it is fully integrated and essential to

emotional experience and behavior. Social constructionist psychologists (e.g., Averill, 1982;

Gergen & Davis, 1984) contend that while emotions have physiological components, they are

largely a result of social processes, especially expectations and norms for how and when people

are expected to feel and express emotions (Parkinson, Fischer & Manstead, 2005). Geertz

(1973:81) concisely summarizes the point of social constructionists by arguing that “Not only

ideas, but emotions too, are cultural artifacts.” Hochschild (1979, p. 552) proposes a two step

process in the social experience of emotion, one in which factors in the structure of the situation

(such as how much power we have, or whether we are appreciated as part of a group) arouse

primary emotional responses (we are angered when a boss yells at us) that are then “managed”

by secondary acts. These secondary acts are cultural and organizational norms, described as

“feeling rules,” that stipulate how we ought to feel in given situations.

Social constructivists thus put relatively more importance in the effect of societal norms

on how we conceive of emotions rather than on biological responses. One such example is the

15

Page 16: Emotion regulation and scripts5

Japanese feeling and expression of amae. Amae means to presume upon another’s love or

indulge in another’s kindness; it is a sense of helplessness in which one is a passive love object

(see LeDoux,1996). While the script for amae is well known in Japan and considered an

essential part of the Japanese personality structure, there is essentially no strong equivalent for

this script in the western tradition, indeed no comparable word for it in European languages.

Social constructivists use examples such as these to show that emotions are typically culturally

determined rather than essentially hard-wired.

From an emotion script approach, I argue that while evolutionary psychologists provide

the neurological and biological “rules” that govern emotional feeling and expression, sociologists

such as Hochschild provide the social rules that shape and guide these basic physiological

responses (see also Kemper, 1990). There is a layer of biological responses that form the

foundational script for emotional response. Overlaid on that script is a more refined social script

that provides the connection between these basic responses and the needs and expectations of

social situations.

Russell (1991b) uses a script theory of emotions to reconcile the universalistic and

cultural relativity approaches. He argues that those cultures which have languages containing

fewer emotion categories have more general emotion scripts. These scripts have fewer specific

features and cover a broad range of phenomena (we have termed these “weak” scripts above).

Cultures with languages with many emotion categories have more specific scripts—each script

“would have more features and cover a narrower range of phenomena” (1991b, p. 443). In this

way, scripts vary to the degree they are universal or specific, depending on the culture. Within

the script, antecedents of particular emotions will also vary from universal to specific, as will

action tendencies (Frijda, 1986), facial or vocal expressions (Ekman, 1972), and physiological

16

Page 17: Emotion regulation and scripts5

changes (Ekman, Levenson & Friesen, 1983). But the nature of emotions-as-scripts exists across

cultures.

Relational Scripts

The previous work cited has primarily been at the neurological and intrapsychic level—

emphasizing a focal person’s thoughts, physiological changes, and reactions. However, most

emotions are felt in response to and in relation with other people, and thus emotion scripts should

include an interactional or relational component (Fehr et al., 1999; Parkinson et al., 2005). The

approach of researchers employing relational scripts is that, based on past experience,

individuals develop cognitive structures representing their expectations around how their actions

are likely to lead to reactions by another person (Baldwin, 1992). In terms of emotion, this

approach holds that we learn over time how other people are likely to react to our expressions of

particular emotions. If I have learned that expressing my anger to my partner increases the

chances that he or she will react with defensiveness and avoidance, for example, this experience

pattern will affect my current expectations around what expressing anger means and others’

likely responses, shaping the patterns of my new relationships (see Baldwin, 1992).

Work in the area of relational scripts has focused on determining whether there are

normatively held interpersonal scripts for emotional expression, and then examining the specific

contents of those scripts. Gergen and Gergen (1988) cite a series of studies in which they gave

participants a scenario in which an emotion was expressed, then provided a series of possible

responses. For example, they had participants read a scenario about a young married couple. In

the first scene, the husband mildly criticized the wife’s cooking. The participants then rated a

range of behavioral options that the wife could take in response (from embracing and kissing to

physically striking). Following their choice of an option, the participant then read that the wife

had escalated the hostility—she had responded by criticizing her husband. The story is again

17

Page 18: Emotion regulation and scripts5

interrupted, and participants are asked to rate the husband’s probable reactions, along with their

desirability and advisability. Through this methodology Gergen and Gergen (1988) found

predictable patterns of escalation based on whether primarily aggressive or conciliatory tactics

were used in early stages of anger expression.

Fitners and Fletcher (1993) examined love, hate, anger and jealousy in marital

relationships. They first examined whether respondents, in outlining their experiences of these

emotions, showed evidence of prototypical knowledge structures. They found, using profile

analysis, that respondents cited cohesive elements for each emotion, allowing researchers to

construct summary prototypes. In second and third studies they also showed that by presenting

prototypical emotion elements, respondents could differentiate and identify specific emotions

based on the nature of the event and the appraisals offered by protagonists. The more information

provided in the vignette (the more complete the script), the more accurate was their identification

of the emotion.

Anger has been the most common focal emotion in studies of emotion scripts in

relationships; this is not surprising, given its prototypicality ratings (Fehr & Russell, 1984;

Shaver et al., 1987). Fehr et al., (1999) studied anger in close heterosexual relationships. Rather

than having respondents generate their own experiences of anger episodes, they provided

respondents with basic elements of an anger script and explored whether common patterns

emerged. They were particularly interested in whether there would be gender differences in the

understanding and implementation of anger scripts. Based on previous research and pilot testing,

they presented respondents five causes of anger (e.g., betrayal of trust, negligence, unwarranted

criticism—each with specific examples), six possible anger reactions they could anticipate

engaging in (e.g., avoid, aggress directly, talk it over/compromise), and responses they would

anticipate from their partner (e.g., avoid, deny responsibility, mock or minimize). Analyzing

18

Page 19: Emotion regulation and scripts5

these responses, they found that betrayal of trust was the most anger-provoking elicitor in these

close relationships, and that respondents anticipated that they and their partners would react to an

anger-provoking situation by talking things over rather than expressing aggression (similar to

previous research; see Averill, 1982). They also discovered gender differences: women found the

events to be more anger provoking overall, and were more likely to say they would express hurt

feelings and behave aggressively, if necessary. These responses arose more frequently in

instances in which there was negligence (e.g., forgetting a birthday, or personal criticism).

An important finding of this study, however, was that while men and women held similar

anger scripts in some situations (e.g., when an angered person chooses to express anger in a

positive way), under other conditions men’s and women’s anger scripts were different.

Specifically, when an angered person chose to react in a negative way, such as aggressing

directly, women were more likely than men to expect that their partner would deny

responsibility; men were more likely to expect that their partner would express hurt feelings,

avoid them or reject them. This study, then, showed both that individuals hold similar scripts for

the expression and reaction to anger, but that other variables such as gender can shape the

content of the script and script selection.

Fehr and Harasymchuk (2005) found that emotion scripts differed in the context of

relationships between friends versus romantic partners. They found that people’s emotional

reactions were based on the responses they expected from a romantic partner or friend when they

expressed dissatisfaction. Specifically, they found that when a romantic partner expressed

dissatisfaction and received a response of neglect (a passive, destructive response) they

responded in a much more intense and negative way than when a friend responded to

dissatisfaction with neglect. Their study showed that the same event had different meanings in

19

Page 20: Emotion regulation and scripts5

the context of different relationships, and produced different types of emotional behavior (see

Whitesell & Harter, 1996).

Overall, these studies of emotion scripts in close relationships provide substantial support

for the idea that interpersonal expectations for emotional expression can be empirically

examined, and the findings suggest that relational scripts for emotions are cognitively

represented as if-then contingencies between self and other (Fehr & Harasymchuk, 2005). This

work adds further complexity, however, by emphasizing that individual differences such as

gender shape the expectations and contents of emotion scripts.

Organizational Scripts

The notion that emotions may best be represented as scriptlike phenomena has special

relevance to the organizational context, which constrains and organizes human behavior, often

through patterned sequences, such as rituals and routines (Lord & Kernan, 1987). Cognitive

researchers have applied script concepts to organizational behavior, arguing that scripts perform

two functions: to serve as guides to appropriate behavior; and to provide a means for making

sense of the behavior of others (Gioia & Poole, 1984). From a cognitive schema approach,

organizations themselves can be seen as “systems of shared knowledge and meaning composed

of repertoires of schemas that guide comprehension and action” (Poole et al., 1989, p. 272).

Schemas provide a system for individuals to aid in understanding the onrush of organizational

decisions, behaviors, and interactions.

In applying cognitive schema models to organizations, however, observers argue that

emotions are often missing from the picture. Organizations are portrayed as shared systems of

meaning exemplified in routines and tacit assumptions, and scripts are portrayed as behavioral

and cognitive structures. For example, an analysis of the script for employee performance

appraisals (Gioia et al., 1989) contains little reference to likely emotional responses.

20

Page 21: Emotion regulation and scripts5

Sociologists of emotion, however, argue that organizations, as situations in which vertical power

relations and horizontal group cohesion play a large part, are situations likely to generate strong

emotional responses (see Collins, 1981; Gibson, 1997; Kemper, 1978). Collins (1981) argues

that organizations can be seen as “marketplaces” of emotional and cultural resources, where

resources are compared through conversational rituals and loyalties and power are negotiated.

Organizational participants “monitor what each is feeling toward the other and especially toward

those in authority” (Collins, 1981: 994).

Three studies have extended the idea of emotion scripts into the sphere of organizations.

What we find, in comparing these script analyses to the previous levels we have examined, is an

increasing level of complexity. Biological scripts indicate the degree to which particular

emotions fulfill discrete functions and exhibit unique action sequences. The prototype and

cognitive appraisal approaches more specifically identify these action sequences and focus on

antecedents and appraisals as determining the shape and structure of the emotion script.

Research on close relationships introduces at least two new variables to these existing scripts: the

reactions of a target and the critical variable of gender. In organizational contexts, a range of

additional variables must be considered, including hierarchical status and power relationships,

multiple interactants (i.e., group emotion scripts), and organizational culture.

In the first study, Fischer (1991) interviewed 56 employees reflecting on anger and fear

episodes in both “public” (organizational) and “private” spheres. In constructing scripts based on

her interviews, she examined respondents’ appraisal of the event, their action tendencies, the

perceived intensity and duration of emotional experience, the emotion words used to describe the

episode, their actual behavior, and whether they consciously tried to regulate their emotion. In

examining anger, Fischer noted that individuals tend to have a “general anger script” similar to

the prototype identified by Shaver et al., (1987). Individuals then refine this general script by

21

Page 22: Emotion regulation and scripts5

adding specific elements depending on the context (in this case, public versus private settings),

where these differing contexts are likely to evoke different expectations, and thus, different

scripts.

She found that the primary difference between anger scripts in private and public

situations is how one appraises the expected reactions of others. In private situations respondents

“do not wish to hurt others,” but they want to “show commitment to others by expressing their

anger” (1991: 151). In public situations, however, “one is far more concerned with how others

will evaluate one’s anger, so anger seems primarily to be used as a device to maintain or improve

one’s position” (1991: 151). She found a few gender differences in terms of likely antecedents of

anger: men were more likely to mention unjust reproaches in private situations and more likely

to refer to the negative behavior or others in public situations. Women more often got angry

because of rule violation in private situations and because they got “passed over” in public

situations.

Overall, Fischer found support for the idea of a “general anger script” driving

respondents’ knowledge structures. There were wide differences in the types of antecedents

cited, however, making anger scripts specific to particular public and private settings more

difficult to compare. An example of how anger scripts became more contingent on context is in

the expression of anger. Overall, respondents regarded expressing anger as desirable. It was

considered to promote a healthy relationship in intimate settings, and it was necessary to show

one’s commitment in professional settings. At the same time, there were limits to this script:

respondents noted that if anger was expressed uncontrollably, negative consequences tended to

result.

Gibson (1995, 1997) applied Plutchik’s (1980) evolutionary model as a way of

understanding scripts for eight emotions in organizations: anger, fear, sadness, disgust, surprise,

22

Page 23: Emotion regulation and scripts5

joy, acceptance and anticipation. He constructed scripts by coding and categorizing respondents’

recollections of an emotional experience at work (n = 143 MBA students) into antecedents,

agents (who were the instigator of emotion), whether the emotion was expressed and to whom,

and the perceived consequences of emotional expression or non-expression. He found that there

was substantial agreement by respondents on scripts for particular emotions, and analyses of

variance indicated differing feeling and expression patterns across the different emotions.

Qualitatively, Gibson (1995) found, for example, that fear episodes revolved around a general

theme of uncertainty, especially about one’s actions (see Table 2). In this script, 53% of episodes

were explained by the top three categories, which included the failure of the respondent to carry

out a task appropriately (27% of antecedents), threats to survival, either personal or career (13%)

and threats to the organization itself (e.g., to merge or be bought out, 13%). Fear tended to be

caused by individuals superior to the respondent (43%) and tended not to be expressed. Anger

episodes, similar to previous findings (e.g., de Rivera, 1977; Russell, 1991a) were characterized

by a theme of perceived injustice. Criticism of the respondent characterized 16% of these

episodes, and another 16% surrounded instances when respondents’ suggestions or comments

were ignored by others. At the organizational level, respondents were angered by the company

acting in an unjust way (e.g., by laying off workers—16% of the response). Agents of anger were

primarily superiors (39%) or the company itself (22%), and were often expressed: respondents

expressed their anger to the agent in 53% of the episodes.

Gibson (1997) reported exploratory findings indicating that status of the agent made a

difference, noting that when superiors were the agent of emotions, there was less likelihood of

emotion expression than if peers or subordinates were agents. Interestingly, this occurred for

both positive (aggregating joy, acceptance and anticipation) and negative (aggregating anger,

fear, sadness, and disgust) emotions. In terms of gender, though the small sample size made his

23

Page 24: Emotion regulation and scripts5

findings speculative, Gibson found that women both felt and (for some emotions) expressed their

emotions to a greater degree than did men. Women, he proposed, typically had to engage in more

regulation of emotion, since they their emotions significantly more strongly than did men, yet

expressed them at about the same level.

Gibson concluded that these emotion scripts indicate that there is a small number of

emotions that are considered appropriate to express in organizations, primarily “approach”

emotions such as anger and acceptance, while many emotions—primarily those indicating

avoidance or vulnerability—are rarely expressed, such as fear, sadness, and joy. He argued that

this kind of limitation in emotion scripts could have implications for organizational decision

making and interpersonal processes. If employees’ full range of emotions are not allowed to be

expressed in organizational settings, for example, group decision-making in organizations may

be limited by a constricted set of data.

Fitness (2000) examined anger scripts in the workplace, using a sample of 175 episodes.

She explored script differences that depended on the focal person’s hierarchical status in the

organization, whether the anger was directed to a supervisor (80 respondents), to a co-worker (57

respondents), or to a subordinate (38 respondents). She elicited scripts through an interview

schedule that asked respondents to “remember a time when you felt really angry with someone at

work,” and then to describe the antecedents to their anger, how they thought and felt at the time,

how they behaved, and whether they thought the incident had been successfully resolved. As

with previous studies, there was substantial agreement over prototypical anger-eliciting events.

For example, 44% involved “being directly and unjustly treated by another.” Other prominent

antecedents included immoral behavior (23%) and job incompetence (15%). Importantly, Fitness

also found differences in antecedents depending on who was perceiving the anger; for example,

69 of the superior-instigated incidents involved directly unjust treatment, while only 28% of co-

24

Page 25: Emotion regulation and scripts5

worker and 16% of subordinate-instigated offences were considered to be unjust. For co-workers

who instigated anger, the primary event involved morally reprehensible behaviors, such as

laziness or dishonesty; for subordinates who instigated anger, the primary antecedent was job

incompetence.

In terms of behavior, Fitness found expected differences in whether anger was expressed

depending on status. Only 45% of respondents angered by superiors immediately confronted

them during the course of feeling anger, compared with 58% of respondents angered by co-

workers, and 71% of respondents angered by subordinates.

Fitness’ study demonstrates the importance of studying context in order to outline and

understand emotion scripts. She identified two distinct anger scripts, depending on power. That

is, high power respondents were likely to be angered by different eliciting events, likely to

express their anger to a greater degree than low power respondents, and were more likely to

think that the anger incident had been successfully resolved. She also noted that she discovered

no gender differences in this setting: rather, in the organizational context the variable of power

appeared to overwhelm gender in affecting emotion scripts.

While there are few studies specifically examining organizational emotion scripts,

numerous other studies have implications for a script approach, though they might not

specifically use the terms of script theory. For example, Sutton (1991) found that respondents in

a bill collection agency were well aware of specific norms around how to express emotions to

debtors they wanted to collect money from. There were norms, for example, to express neutrality

to angry debtors and norms to by more easy-going (at first) with distressed debtors. These

normative instructions are clearly indicative of an emotional script for these transactional dyads.

Moreover, in the negotiations literature, studies now examine how emotional expressions by

negotiators affect their targets, and vice versa (Van Kleef, DeDreu & Manstead, 2004).

25

Page 26: Emotion regulation and scripts5

A particularly important direction for emotion script research is the recognition that

knowledge of emotion scripts may allow participants to express their emotions strategically,

knowing that they are likely to elicit a particular response. Clark, Pataki and Carver (1996) argue

that because people share assumptions about the script (its structure, antecedents and

consequences), people can “learn to present emotions to others to accomplish specifiable social

goals” (1996, p. 248). Indeed, negotiations researchers are finding that negotiators who

strategically display particular emotions are able to affect the outcome of the negotiation

(Kopelman, Rosette & Thompson, 2006) and qualitative studies of professionals—such as

lawyers—show frequent use of strategic emotions (Pierce, 1995). As Forgas notes, this emphasis

on the strategic nature of scripts suggests that “affect is not merely a private experience, but at

the same time is a public event” (1996, p. 282).

Implications and Conclusions

This article outlines a multi-level model of emotion scripts. It provides a way of

conceptualizing scripts that helps to integrate widely divergent approaches to emotion. Scripts

are both observed sequences of events and they are understandings about how sequences of

events tend to occur. On one hand, this combination of descriptive and normative elements

accounts for their explanatory versatility across a range of widely divergent research landscapes.

On the other hand, this eclecticism may have also impeded further study into scripts. By

addressing a range of approaches, the script approach offers a metaphor and a methodology for

studying sequences of behaviors, but its very applicability and consequent lack of specificity

may also be its undoing. Compared to general cognitive appraisal approaches (e.g., Lazarus,

1991; Scherer, 2001) or sociological/normative approaches (e.g., Kemper, 1990), the

development of script research has been less well developed.

26

Page 27: Emotion regulation and scripts5

The fundamental outlines of prototypical emotion sequences are well developed and well

supported (see Fehr & Russell, 1984; Shaver et al., 1987). But the development of scripts in

differing contexts is much less well developed. Studies of anger predominate in script theory (see

Gibson, 1995, 1997 for an exception), while scripts for other emotions (other than fear) have

received far less attention. The issue, for the future of emotion script research, will be to develop

scripts at a level of detail that can help in understanding organizational problems while not being

so specific that they are only applicable to one context (see Fitness, 2000 for one illustration of

such meso-level scripts).

Future Directions in Emotion Script Research

Studies of additional emotion scripts. In parallel with emotions research more

generally, emotion script research needs to expand its focus from anger and fear to other critical

discrete emotions (see Gibson, 1997). While anger and fear offer a cohesive prototypical view,

they also offer only one slice of organizational life. For example, while most studies of emotion

have focused on negative emotions and moods, in fact, linkages to organizational outcomes such

as individual and group achievement, decision-making effectiveness, and creativity tend to be far

more compelling for positive emotions (Lyubomirsky, King & Diener, 2005). While emotion

scripts have been relatively well-articulated for anger and fear, we know much less about

happiness and liking/acceptance. Studies of strategic displays of emotional expression (e.g.,

Clark et al., 1996) indicate that displaying happiness (and suppressing anger and sadness) is

related to ingratiation behavior and increasing the liking of a target, both phenomena of interest

to organizational researchers (Jones & Pittman, 1982). More refined scripts for organizational

envy (Cohen-Charash & Mueller, in press), sadness, and shame/guilt (Poulson, 2000) would also

be in line with current research inquiry.

27

Page 28: Emotion regulation and scripts5

Individual differences and scripts. A continuing avenue of research will be to discover

under what conditions individual differences shape the execution of scripts. Because they are

focused on sequences of events, studying scripts can uncover findings about contingencies that

would not be revealed in correlational work or work focused on intrapsychic, context-free

environments. For example, Fehr and Baldwin (1996) point out that the commonsense

understanding that women are more likely to cry in response to anger may not be the whole

story. Rather, what their findings indicate is that women may not be more likely to respond with

crying and hurt feelings whenever angry, but rather, they “are more likely to experience being

angered in situations when hurt feelings are a key element of the anger experienced (e.g., the

betrayal of trust)” (1996, p. 240). That discovery was only possible by researchers examining the

antecedents of anger, since different kinds of people may be more or less sensitive to different

kinds of instigators, may have different kinds of experience with them, may have different styles

in terms of emotion regulation, etc. Thus, script methodologies may be especially well suited to

discovering different contingencies related to individual differences, gender being a prominent,

but not the sole, example.

Degree of Script Convergence. A primary approach to determining whether

organizational participants share a script is to measure the degree to which participants cite a

particular element in their narrative of an episode. For example, Fitness (2000) found that 69%

of anger episodes in her sample were caused by superiors who unjustly treated their subordinates

(see also Gibson, 1997; Fischer, 1991). While these proportional approaches provide good

overall support for the level of agreement in terms of the existence of common scripts, more

specific and accurate measures need to be developed. Studies of cognitive scripts, for example,

have used videotaped interactions and more elaborate qualitative methods to assess the degree of

cohesiveness in organizational scripts (see Gioia, Donnellon, & Sims, 1989; Poole, Gray &

28

Page 29: Emotion regulation and scripts5

Gioia, 1990). Advancing methods in sequence analysis (see Abbott, 1990), and reliability ratings

(Forrest & Abbott, 1990) will provide important means of gathering these data. Measuring the

degree of convergence would represent a significant advance in understanding emotion scripts.

One application of this research would be to examine the effect of diverse versus homogenous

scripts on organizational behavior and performance. For example, Barsade et al. (2000) showed

that similarities in affective disposition in top management teams led to increased performance in

top management teams. Future research should examine organizational members’ emotional

scripts to determine whether similarity in scripts also contributes to team effectiveness, and

under what conditions.

Scripts as Methodology. I have argued that scripts offer both descriptive and normative

material for analysis. The script approach is particularly applicable to emotions, since laypeople

tend to think of emotions in terms of prototypical sequences of events (see Shaver et al., 1987).

Following this line of reasoning, in addition to identifying scripts and assessing their

convergence, script data is very useful for identifying and understanding organizational emotion

norms and culture. Recently, scholars have called for more research on the nature of

organizational cultural norms for emotion expression (see, e.g., Barsade et al., 2003). However,

gathering data on norms (without directly observing behavior), is often difficult. Having

respondents outline their perceived scripts for emotional expression may be one means to

illustrate organizational norms.

For example, Van Maanen and Kunda (1989) identified emotion norms stipulating that

employees at Disneyland express positive emotions while cloaking their dissatisfaction, and

participants in a high technology company express passion around the firm’s products. While

these authors determined these norms through participant observation, an alternative method

would have been to interview participants on the structure of emotion scripts in their

29

Page 30: Emotion regulation and scripts5

organization. While identifying generalized norms may be difficult for participants, experiential

learning with scripts suggests that they may more readily generate episodes of emotional

expression (Gibson, 2006) that can be useful in determining normative scripts. Similarly, while it

may be difficult for employees to discuss risky issues such as gender and power in their

organization, having them discuss emotion scripts may indirectly lead to these issues (see

Fitness, 2000). A caution here, of course, is one of social desirability: there may be a tendency

on the part of employees to provide “acceptable and warrantable public explanations” for their

behavior, rather than a faithful recollection of events (Forgas, 1996, p. 284). Forgas argues that a

wider variety of methods, including experimentation approaches (see, e.g., Clark et al., 1996)

would help to address this concern. New approaches in negotiation research (see Kopelman et

al., 2006; Van Kleef et al., 2004) apply similar methods focusing on specific emotions (e.g.,

happiness and anger) to determine more specific antecedents and consequences.

Conclusion

The explosion of emotions work in the organizational context has advanced the field in

many ways: as this research companion demonstrates, advances in the definition of emotions, its

specificity and methodologies have stripped away some of the mystery and “conceptual and

definitional chaos” that once characterized emotions research (see Buck, 1990, p. 330). I am

recommending emotion script theory as one advance that deserves more attention. While scripts

have been invoked in emotions research almost as long as we have examined emotions

themselves, work using this approach has advanced unevenly. Scripts provide clues to the basic,

biological nature of emotions, and they allow us to examine how additional levels of normative

structures inherent to relationships and organizations are laid over this basic foundation. They

provide vital clues to how we live out our emotional experiences in organizations.

30

Page 31: Emotion regulation and scripts5

Biological Script: Emotions primarily serving evolutionary survival functions

Social Script: Emotions created and shaped by the structure of the social situation and cultural norms

Cognitive Script: Emotions shaped by intrapsychic appraisals of situations and prototypical ways of responding

Relational Script: Emotions shaped by interactions with significant others and their reactions

Organizational Script: Emotions shaped by structure (delineation of groups, hierarchy), power, and gender

FIGURE 1Emotion Scripts: A Multi-Level Model Level

Individual, Group, Organization

Dyadic

Individually internalized social and cultural norms

Intrapsychic (within individual)

Neurological

Specificity

More Specific

More General

31

Page 32: Emotion regulation and scripts5

Table 1: Generic Emotion Scripts(adapted from Shaver et al., 1987)

Emotions Described by Respondents

Script Elements Joy Anger Love Fear

Antecedents A desirable outcome; getting what was wanted (68%)1

Task success, achievement (54%)

Receiving esteem, respect, praise (33%)

Judgment that the situation is illegitimate, wrong, unfair (78%)

Real or threatened physical or psychological pain (57%)

Violation of an expectation; things not working out as planned (54%)

Having spent a lot of time together, having shared special experiences (33%)

P finds O attractive (Physically and/or psychologically) (28%)

O offers/provides something that P wants, needs, likes (22%)

Threat of harm or death (68%)

Being in a novel, unfamiliar situation (43%)

Threat of social rejection (28%)

Being alone (walking alone, etc.) (28%)

Behavioral Responses

Smiling (72%)

Communicating the good feeling to others (or trying to) (40%)

Positive outlook; seeing only the bright side (40%)

Verbally attacking the cause of anger (69%)

Loud voice, yelling, screaming, shouting (59%)

Thinking “I’m right, everyone else is wrong” (38%)

Feeling happy, joyful, exuberant, etc. (52%)

Smiling (44%)

Feeling warm, trusting, secure, etc. (43%)

Feeling nervous, jittery, jumpy (48%)

Picturing a disastrous conclusion to events in progress (42%)

Talking less, being speechless (31%)

Self-Control Procedures

Suppressing the anger; trying not to show or express it (20%)

Redefining the situation (11%)

Acting unafraid, hiding the fear from others (23%)

Comforting oneself, telling oneself everything is all right, trying to keep calm (22%)

32

Page 33: Emotion regulation and scripts5

1Percentages indicate the percentage of 120 subjects mentioning that feature. Subjects could identify multiple categories; thus these percentages do not sum to 100%.

33

Page 34: Emotion regulation and scripts5

Table 1: Organizational Emotion Scripts(adapted from Gibson, 1997)

Emotions Described by Respondents

Script Elements Joy Anger Liking Fear

Antecedents Theme: Personal SuccessJob or Project completed (47%)*Respondent receives recognition or promotion (24%)Job or Project beginning (12%)

82% explained by top 3 categories

Theme: InjusticeCriticism of respondent (16%)Suggestions ignored by Agent (16%)Company initiates layoffs (16%)

48% explained by top 3 categories

Theme: BondingCamaraderie in groups (53%)Positive relationship with a particular other (40%)Respondent receives recognition or promotion (7%)

100% explained by top 3 categories

Theme: UncertaintyFailure by Self (27%)Threats external to the organization (13%)Lack of corporate support for respondent (13%)

53% explained by top 3 categories

Agents Work itself (38%)Superiors (25%)

Superiors (39%)Company (22%)

Team/peers (67%) Superiors (43%)Self (21%)External agents (13%)

Expression/ Behavior

Expressed to agent (19%)Did not express (81%)

Expressed to agent (53%)Did not express (47%)

Expressed to agent (60%)Did not express(40%)

Expressed to agent (20%)Did not express (80%)

Consequences None listed (47%)Bonding with group or peers (24%)

Nothing; no one cared (21%)Outcome favorable (21%)Outcome unfavorable (16%)

Bonding with group or peers (47%)Positive feedback from agent (27%)

Respondent receives sympathy, emotional support from others (33%)Nothing; no one cared (27%)

1Percentages indicate the percent of respondents mentioning each script element. Sample size for Joy was n = 16, Anger, n = 19, Liking, n = 15, Fear, n = 15.

34

Page 35: Emotion regulation and scripts5

References

Abbott, A. (1990). A primer on sequence methods. Organization Science, 1, 375-392.

Abelson, R. P. (1981). Psychological status of the script concept. American Psychologist, 36, 715-729.

Austin, J. T., & Vancouver, J. B. (1996). Goal constructs in psychology: Structure, process, and content. Psychological Bulletin, 120, 338-375.

Averill, J. R. (1982). Anger and aggression: An essay on emotion. New York: Springer-Verlag.

Averill, J. R. (1983). Studies on anger and aggression: Implications for theories of emotion. American Psychologist, 38, 1145-1160.

Baldwin, M. W. (1992). Relational schemas and the processing of social information. Psychological Bulletin, 12, 461-484.

Bargh, J. A., & Chartrand, T. L. (1999). The unbearable automaticity of being. American Psychologist, 54, 462-479.

Barsade, S., Brief, A., & Spataro, S. (2003). The affective revolution in organizational behavior: The emergence of a paradigm. In J. Greenberg (Ed.), Organizational Behavior: The State of the Science, pp. 3-52. London: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Publishers.

Barsade, S. G., Ward, A. J., Turner, J .D .F., & Sonnenfeld, J. A. (2000). To your heart’s content: A model of affective diversity in top management teams. Administrative Science Quarterly, 45, 802-836.

Bower, G., Black, J., & Turner, T. (1979). Scripts in text comprehension and memory. Cognitive Psychology, 11, 177-220.

Buck, R. (1990). Mood and emotion: A comparison of five contemporary views. Psychological Inquiry, 1, 330-336.

Cantor, N., & Kihlstrom, J. F. (1985). Social intelligence: The cognitive basis of personality. Review of Personality and Social Psychology, 6, 15-33.

Clark, M. S., Pataki, S. P., & Carver, V. H. (1996). Some thoughts and findings on self-presentation of emotions in relationships. In G. J. O. Fletcher & J. Fitness (Eds.), Knowledge structures in close relationships: A social psychological approach, pp. 247-274. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

35

Page 36: Emotion regulation and scripts5

Cohen-Charash, Y. & Mueller, J. S. In press. Does perceived unfairness exacerbate or mitigate interpersonal counterproductive work behaviors related to envy? Journal of Applied Psychology.

Collins, R. (1981). On the microfoundations of macrosociology. American Journal of Sociology, 86, 984-1014.

Cropanzano, R., Weiss, H. M., Hale, J. M. S., & Reb, J. (2003). The structure of affect: Reconsidering the relationship between negative and positive affectivity. Journal of Management, 29, 831-857.

Damasio, A. R. (1994). Descartes’ error: Emotion, reason, and the human brain. New York: Avon.

Darwin, C. (1872/1998). The expression of the emotions in man and animals. New York: Oxford University Press.

de Rivera, J. (1977). A structural theory of emotions. New York: International Universities Press.

Ekman, P. (1972). Universal and cultural differences in facial expressions of emotions. In J. K. Cole (Ed.), Nebraska symposium on motivation 1971 (pp. 207-283). Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press.

Ekman, P. (1992). An argument for basic emotions. Cognition and Emotion, 6, 169-200.

Ekman, P. (1994). Strong evidence for universals in facial expressions: A reply to Russell’s mistaken critique. Psychological Bulletin, 115, 268-287.

Ekman, P., & Friesen, W. V. (1969). The repertoire of nonverbal behavior: Categories, origins, usage, and coding. Semiotica, 1, 49-98.

Ekman, P., Levenson, R. W., & Friesen, W. V. 1983. Autonomic nervous system activity distinguishes among emotions. Science, 221, 1208-1210.

Fehr, B., & Baldwin, M. (1996). Prototype and script analyses of laypeople’s knowledge of anger. In G. J. O. Fletcher & J. Fitness (Eds.), Knowledge structures in close relationships: A social psychological approach, pp. 219-246. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

Fehr, B., Baldwin, M., Collins, L., Patterson, S., & Benditt, R. (1999). Anger in close relationships: An interpersonal script analysis. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 25, 299-312.

Fehr, B., & Harasymchuk, C. (2005). The experience of emotion in close relationships: Toward an integration of the emotion-in-relationships and interpersonal script models. Personal Relationships, 12, 181-196.

Fehr, B., & Russell, J. A. (1984). Concept of emotion viewed from a prototype perspective. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 113, 464-486.

36

Page 37: Emotion regulation and scripts5

Fischer, A. (1991). Emotion scripts: A study of the social and cognitive aspects of emotion. Leiden, NL: DSWO-Press.

Fitness, J. (2000). Anger in the workplace: An emotion script approach to anger episodes between workers and their superiors, co-workers and subordinates. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 21, 147-162.

Fitness, J., & Fletcher, G. J. O. (1993). Love, hate, anger, and jealousy in close relationships: A prototype and cognitive appraisal analysis. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 65, 942-958.

Forgas, J. P. (1996). The role of emotion scripts and transient moods in relationships: Structural and functional perspectives. In G. J. O. Fletcher & J. Fitness (Eds.), Knowledge structures in close relationships: A social psychological approach, pp. 275-298. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

Forrest, J., & Abbott, A. (1990). The optimal matching method for studying anthropological sequence data: An introduction and reliability analysis. Journal of Quantitative Anthropology, 2, 151-170.

Frijda, N. H. (1986). The emotions. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.

Frijda, N. H. (1988). The laws of emotion. American Psychologist, 43, 349-358.

Frijda, N. H., & Mesquita, B. (1994). The social roles and functions of emotions. In S. Kitayama & H. R. Markus (Eds.), Emotion and culture: Empirical studies of mutual influence, pp. 51-88. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.

Geertz, C. 1973. The interpretation of cultures. New York: Basic Books.

Gergen, K. K, & Davis, K. (1984). The social construction of the person. New York: Springer.

Gergen, K. K., & Gergen, M. M. (1988). Narrative and the self as relationship. In L. Berkowitz (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology, (Vol. 21, pp. 17-56). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.

Gibson, D. E. (1995). Emotional Scripts and Organization Change. In F. Massarik (Ed.), Advances in Organization Development, Vol. 3, pp. 32-62. Norwood, NJ: Ablex.

Gibson, D. E. (1997). The struggle for reason: The sociology of emotions in organizations. In R. J. Erickson & B. Cuthbertson-Johnson (Eds.) Social Perspectives on Emotion, Vol. 4, pp. 211-256. Greenwich, CT: JAI Press.

Gibson, D. E. (2006). Emotional episodes at work: An experiential exercise in feeling and expressing emotions. Journal of Management Education, 30, pp. 477-500.

37

Page 38: Emotion regulation and scripts5

Gioia, D. A., Donnellon, A., & Sims, H. P., Jr. (1989). Communication and cognition in appraisal: A tale of two paradigms. Organization Studies, 10, 503-530.

Gioia, D. A., & Poole, P. P. (1984). Scripts in organizational behavior. Academy of Management Review, 9, 449-459.

Gioia, D. A., & Sims, H. P. (1986). Cognition-behavior connections: Attribution and verbal behavior in leader-subordinate interactions. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 37, 197-229.

Gross, J. J. 1998. Antecedent and response-focused emotion regulation: Divergent consequences for experience, expression, and physiology. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74(1): 224-237.

Hochschild, A. (1979). Emotion work, feeling rules, and social structure. American Journal of Sociology, 85, 551-575.

Hochschild, A. (1983). The managed heart: Commercialization of human feeling. Berkeley: University of California Press.

James, W. (1884). What is an emotion? Mind, 9, 188-205.

Jones, E. E., & Pittman, T. S. (1982). Toward a general theory of strategic self-presentation. In J. Suls (Ed.), Psychological perspectives on the self, Vol. 1, pp. 231-262. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

Kemper, T. D. 1978. A social interactional theory of emotions. New York: Wiley.

Kemper, T. D. (1990). Social relations and emotions: A structural approach. In T. D. Kemper (Ed.), Research agendas in the sociology of emotions, pp. 207-237. Albany, NY: State University of New York Press.

Kopelman, S., Rosette, A. S., & Thompson, L. (2006). The three faces of Eve: Strategic displays of positive, negative and neutral emotions in negotiations. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 99, 81-101.

Lazarus, R. S. (1991). Emotion and adaptation. New York: Oxford University Press.

LeDoux, J. (1996). The emotional brain: The mysterious underpinnings of emotional life. New York: Simon & Schuster.

Leigh, T. W., & McGraw, P. F. (1989). Mapping the procedural knowledge of industrial sales personnel: A script-theoretic investigation. Journal of Marketing, 53, 16-34.

Lichtenstein, E. H., & Brewer, W. F. (1980). Memory for goal-directed events. Cognitive Psychology, 12, 412-445.

38

Page 39: Emotion regulation and scripts5

Lord, R. G., & Kernan, M. C. (1987). Scripts as determinants of purposeful behavior in organizations. Academy of Management Review, 12, 265-277.

Lyubomirsky, S., King, L., & Diener, E. (2005). The benefits of frequent positive affect: Does happiness lead to success? Psychological Bulletin, 131, 803-855.

Mandler, G. (1985). Cognitive psychology: An essay in cognitive science. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

Mumby, D. K., & Putnam, L. L. (1992). The politics of emotion: A feminist reading of bounded rationality. Academy of Management Review, 17, 465-486.

Parkinson, B., Fischer, A. H., & Manstead, A. S. R. (2005). Emotion in social relations: Cultural, group, and interpersonal processes. New York: Psychology Press.

Pierce, J. L. (1995). Gender trials: Emotional lives in contemporary law firms. Berkeley, CA: University of California Press.

Plutchik, R. (1980). Emotion: A psychoevolutionary synthesis. New York: Harper & Row.

Poole, P. P., Gioia, D. A., & Gray, B. (1989). Influence modes, schema change, and organizational transformation. The Journal of Applied Behavioral Science, 25, 271-289.

Poole, P. P., Gray, B., & Gioia, D. A. (1990). Organizational script development through interactive accommodation. Group & Organization Studies, 15, 212-232.

Poulson, C. F. (2000). Shame and work. In N. M. Ashkanasy, C. E. Hartel, & W. J. Zerbe (Eds.), Emotion in the workplace: Research, theory, and practice, pp. 250-271. Westport, CT: Quorum.

Roseman, I. J. (1984). Cognitive determinants of emotions: A structural theory. In P. Shaver (Ed.), Review of personality and social psychology, Vol. 5, pp. 11-36. Beverly Hills: Sage.

Roseman, I. J. (1991). Appraisal determinants of discrete emotions. Cognition and Emotion, 5, 161-200.

Roseman, I. J., Spindel, M. S., & Jose, P. E. (1990). Appraisals of emotion-eliciting events: Testing a theory of discrete emotions. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 59, 899-915.

Rosch, E. (1975). Cognitive representations of semantic categories. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 104, 192-233.

Russell, J. A. (1991a). In defense of a prototype approach to emotion concepts. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 60, 37-47.

39

Page 40: Emotion regulation and scripts5

Russell, J. A. (1991b). Culture and the categorization of emotions. Psychological Bulletin, 110, 426-450.

Russell, J. A. (1994). Is there universal recognition of emotion from facial expression? A review of cross-cultural studies. Psychological Bulletin, 115, 102-141.

Schank, R. C., & Abelson, R. F. (1977). Scripts, plans, goals, and understanding. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

Scherer, K. R. (2001). Appraisal considered as a process of multilevel sequential checking. In K. R. Scherer, A. Schorr, & T. Johnstone (Eds.), Appraisal processes in emotion: Theory, methods, research, pp. 92-120. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.

Scherer, K. R., & Wallbott, H. G. (1994). Evidence for universality and cultural variation of differential emotion response patterning. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 66, 310-328.

Shaver, P., Schwartz, J., Kirson, D., & O’Connor, (1987). Emotion knowledge: Further exploration of a prototype approach. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 52, 1061-1086.

Sutton, R.I. 1991. Maintaining norms about expressed emotions: The case of bill collectors. Administrative Science Quarterly, 36: 245-268.

Tomkins, S. (1979). Script theory: Differential magnification of affects. In R. A. Dienstbier (Ed.) Nebraska symposium on motivation 1978, Human Emotion, (pp. 201-236). Lincoln, NB: University of Nebraska Press.

Van Kleef, G. A., De Dreu, C. K. W., & Manstead, A. S. R. (2004). The interpersonal effects of anger and happiness in negotiations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 86, 57-76.

Van Maanen, J., & Kunda, G. (1989). ‘Real feelings’: Emotional expression and organizational culture. B. M. Staw & L. L. Cummings (Eds.), Research in organizational behavior, Vol. 11, pp. 43-103. Greenwich, CT: JAI.

White, G. (2000). Representing emotional meaning: Category, metaphor, schema, discourse. In M. Lewis & J. M. Haviland-Jones (Eds.), Handbook of emotions, 2nd ed., pp. 30-44. New York: Guilford.

Whitesell, N. R., & Harter, S. (1996). The interpersonal context of emotion: Anger with close friends and classmates. Child Development, 67, 1345-1359.

Wierzbicka, A. (1994). Emotion, language, and cultural scripts. In S. Kitayama & H. R. Markus (Eds.), Emotion and culture: Empirical studies of mutual influence, pp. 133-196. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.

40

Page 41: Emotion regulation and scripts5

Zohar, D., & Luria, G. (2003). Organizational meta-scripts as a source of high reliability: The case of an army armored brigade. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 24, 837-859.

41