85
The Psychology of Thinking About the Past and Future Covering Recent Research on Immune Neglect and Affective Forecasting; the Planning Fallacy; and Counterfactual Thinking. This Presentation Includes Discussion Questions

The Psychology of Thinking About the Past and Future

Embed Size (px)

DESCRIPTION

Covering Recent Research on Immune Neglect and Affective Forecasting; the Planning Fallacy; and Counterfactual Thinking.

Citation preview

Page 1: The Psychology of Thinking About the Past and Future

The Psychology of Thinking About the Past and FutureCovering Recent Research on Immune Neglect and Affective Forecasting; the

Planning Fallacy; and Counterfactual Thinking.

This Presentation Includes Discussion Questions

Page 2: The Psychology of Thinking About the Past and Future

Kahneman and Tversky

Born: Haifa, Palestine

1937-1996

Born: Tel Aviv, Israel1934-

Their first jointly authored paper, "Belief in the Law of Small Numbers," was published in 1971

Page 3: The Psychology of Thinking About the Past and Future

“As I came closer to [the SS soldier], trying to walk fast, I noticed that he was looking at me intently. Then he beckoned me over,

picked me up, and hugged me. I was terrified that he would notice the star inside my sweater. He was speaking to me with great

emotion, in German. When he put me down, he opened his wallet, showed me a picture of a boy, and gave me some money. I went home more certain than ever that my mother was right: people

were endlessly complicated and interesting”

Daniel Kahneman on the roots of his interest in psychology:

Page 4: The Psychology of Thinking About the Past and Future

“When I was 18, I couldn’t understand why anyone would want to live past the age of 50.”

Daniel Kahneman at the age of 66:

Page 5: The Psychology of Thinking About the Past and Future

Immune Neglectand

Affective Forecasting

Page 6: The Psychology of Thinking About the Past and Future

Daniel GilbertHarvard University

Timothy D. WilsonUniv. of Virginia

et al.

Page 7: The Psychology of Thinking About the Past and Future

Got

his

law

licens

e at

W&M

First

Author

Mary Poppins

Austin Powers’s

Dad

James BondPerry Mason

Page 8: The Psychology of Thinking About the Past and Future

I might actually learn something by going down to the local community college and taking a course in writing. After all, as a high school dropout, not only had I never had a writing course, I wasn't a particularly good speller.So I got on the bus and I went down to the local community college. By the time I got down there, they said the course on writing was full. It had been a very long bus ride, so I said, "Alright, fine. What's open?" They looked and they told me, "Psychology." I thought, That's got something to do with crazy people; maybe I'll write a story about a crazy person some day. I told them to sign me up.

Page 9: The Psychology of Thinking About the Past and Future

http://tinyurl.com/2u7kmr5

Video:http://tinyurl.com/2u7kmr5

Page 10: The Psychology of Thinking About the Past and Future

People are less adept at estimating the duration of an affect.

Page 11: The Psychology of Thinking About the Past and Future

MisconstrualInaccurate Theories

Motivated DistortionsUndercorrection

FocalismImmune Neglect

Page 12: The Psychology of Thinking About the Past and Future
Page 13: The Psychology of Thinking About the Past and Future

Study 1: Looking Down Lonely Street

Study 2: Life After TenureStudy 3: The Politics of

Happiness

Page 14: The Psychology of Thinking About the Past and Future

Participant overestimated the duration of their negative affect

and they did this dramatically and consistently more than the duration of

their positive affect.

Page 15: The Psychology of Thinking About the Past and Future

Immune neglect may have been at work, but…

Page 16: The Psychology of Thinking About the Past and Future

Negative events may have been far from neutral.

Positives ones may have been close to neutral.

We know that losses are generally experienced as larger than gains, even when they are objectively

equal (Kahneman & Tversky)

Page 17: The Psychology of Thinking About the Past and Future

What if we have two conditions?

1. The negative affect is ameliorated

2. Not so much

Page 18: The Psychology of Thinking About the Past and Future

Hurting People in a Laboratoryand

Separating People with Negative Self-Views

(Studies 4-6)

Page 19: The Psychology of Thinking About the Past and Future

Study 4: The Hurting Machine (Computer vs. Human Personality

Feedback)

Study 5: Just Death (Make all participants feel bad;

Blameworthy vs. Blameless)

Study 6: Failure, Inc. (0 minutes + 10 minutes)

Page 20: The Psychology of Thinking About the Past and Future

Participants who didn’t expect negative feedback predicted they’d feel equally bad a few minutes after

receiving it from a fallible or infallible source. But they felt better in the

fallible source condition.

Page 21: The Psychology of Thinking About the Past and Future

Forecasters believed they’d be equally upset after reading both

stories, but experiencers who read the blameworthy story became less

upset than those who read the blameless story.

Page 22: The Psychology of Thinking About the Past and Future

Contrary to their predictions, participants felt they’d feel equally

bad after a job rejection on the basis of fair or unfair decisions, but this was not so, especially 10 minutes

afterwards.

Page 23: The Psychology of Thinking About the Past and Future

And yet we neglect the immune system because…

Page 24: The Psychology of Thinking About the Past and Future

First, events that are potentially averse are also potentially appetitive.

Second, forecasters may not look ahead because being aware of immunity might

paradoxically suppress immunity.

Third, if people were aware of how well immunity works, they might not be

motivated to take preventive action.

Page 25: The Psychology of Thinking About the Past and Future

More Research

Page 26: The Psychology of Thinking About the Past and Future

Individual Differences

Immune neglect in affective forecastingMichael Hoerger, Stuart W. Quir, Richard E. Lucas, Thomas H. CarrJournal of Research in Personality 43(2009), 91–94

Author studied coping strategies. Those reporting greater use of emotional processing coping strategies recovered more effectively from losses, but failed to foresee this when making predictions, leading to increased bias. This is the first study to document individual differences in immune neglect.

Page 27: The Psychology of Thinking About the Past and Future

Evidence for the hypothesized pattern of correlations between coping styles and the impact bias for football losses (Boerger, et al.)

Page 28: The Psychology of Thinking About the Past and Future

Belief-Transmission Model

"If a particular belief has some property that facilitates its own transmission, then that belief tends to be helped by an increasing number of minds...False beliefs, like bad genes, can and do become super-replicators...False beliefs that promote stable societies tend to propagate because people who hold these beliefs tend to live in stable societies, which provide the means by which false beliefs propagate.” – Daniel Gilbert, Stumbling Upon Happiness

Page 29: The Psychology of Thinking About the Past and Future

How to Make Better Predictions

"The experience of a randomly selected individual can sometimes provide a better basis for predicting your future experience than your own imagination can.” – Daniel Gilbert, Stumbling Upon Happiness

Page 30: The Psychology of Thinking About the Past and Future

But We All Like to Think We’re Unique

Page 31: The Psychology of Thinking About the Past and Future

(Or at least above average)

Page 32: The Psychology of Thinking About the Past and Future

Discussion Questions About Immune Neglect

Page 33: The Psychology of Thinking About the Past and Future

ConcernsIs asking participants how happy they are really a good measure of overall well being? What is another way they could have measured happiness that might have given a more accurate picture of how the people were really doing? Should they have measured other emotions as well? What about physiological measurements?

Although voters appear to have a more vested interest in the outcome of elections, does Gilbert et al.’s second study portray an ecologically valid situation in which most Americans would feel is personally relevant and would therefore feel the negative consequences as much as a breakup or being denied tenure? What about the death of a loved one? How are personal relevance and saliency important?

Page 34: The Psychology of Thinking About the Past and Future

General Happiness In psychology we often emphasize the importance that single events (such as the death of a family member or divorce) can have on an individual's life. The articles states on page 618, "Most people are reasonably happy most of the time, and most events do little to change that for long". Do you agree with this statement? Why or why not?

Page 35: The Psychology of Thinking About the Past and Future

Individual DifferencesAdolescence is characterized by higher impulsivity and more frequent emotional highs and lows than adulthood. Would teens be more susceptible to durability bias than adults and be more likely to overpredict the extent of their affective responses to future events?

On p. 636 the authors propose people’s inability to correctly predict their feelings after a negative event but not positive events may be because negative events are consistently avoided but positive events are not. Would this imply that people with high openness to new experiences—being less avoidant—are more accurate predictors than people with low openness?

Page 36: The Psychology of Thinking About the Past and Future

Risky BehaviorWhat does the immune neglect article tell us about risky behavior? Are people less likely to engage in behaviors with a potential for failure, simply because they expect the negative affect to last longer than it actually will? How can we inform people and train them to know this? Would they be more likely to undertake entrepreneurial behaviors with potentially big gains on one hand and big failures on the other if they know that failure would not be as problematic as it seems initially?

Page 37: The Psychology of Thinking About the Past and Future

Planning Fallacy

Page 38: The Psychology of Thinking About the Past and Future

Dale Griffin and Michael Ross

Based on Buehler’s doctoral dissertation

Roger BuehlerSimon Fraser University, now at Wilfrid Laurier University

Univ. of Waterloo

Page 39: The Psychology of Thinking About the Past and Future
Page 40: The Psychology of Thinking About the Past and Future
Page 41: The Psychology of Thinking About the Past and Future

People Underestimate Their Task Completion Times

Page 42: The Psychology of Thinking About the Past and Future

Although aware that previous predictions were overly optimistic,

people believe their current forecasts are realistic.

Page 43: The Psychology of Thinking About the Past and Future

Study 1: Optimistic Time Estimates

Study 2: More Typical Task + Measure of Confidence

Study 3: Adding a Think-Aloud Procedure and Making Failure

Salient

Page 44: The Psychology of Thinking About the Past and Future

Study 4: Recall Condition vs. Recall-Relevant Condition

Study 5: Social Prediction

Page 45: The Psychology of Thinking About the Past and Future

In each study fewer than 50% of participants finished their tasks in

their predicted time.

Participants recall of the past is biased and its relevance

diminished.

But in an observer role, participants were overly pessimistic.

Page 46: The Psychology of Thinking About the Past and Future

Research continues on:1. The role of motivation, and

2. The consequence of optimistic estimates

Page 47: The Psychology of Thinking About the Past and Future

More Research

Page 48: The Psychology of Thinking About the Past and Future

Peetz, J., Buehler, R., & Wilson, A. E. (2010). Planning for the near and distant future: How does temporal distance affect task completion predictions? Journal of Experimental Social Psychology,

46, 709-720 . In contexts that elicited a focus on planning, individuals predicted earlier completion times for close than distant projects. In

contexts that prompted a focus on obstacles, individuals predicted later completion times for close than distant projects.

Buehler, R., Peetz, J., & Griffin, D. (2010). Finishing on time: When do predictions influence completion times? Organizational Behaviour and Human Decison Processes, 111, 23-32.

Manipulated predictions affected completion times of closed tasks, defined as tasks carried out within a single, continuous session but not of open tasks, defined as tasks requiring multiple work sessions. This implies that task completion predictions help to initiate

action, but their impact diminishes over the course of extensive, multi-stage projects.

Peetz, J., & Buehler, R. (2009). Is there a budget fallacy? The role of saving goals in the prediction of personal spending. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 35, 1579-1591.

First, participants tended to underestimate their future spending. They predicted spending substantially less money in the coming week than they actually spent or than they remembered spending in the previous week. Second, the prediction bias stemmed from

people’s savings goals—defined as the general desire to save money or minimize future spending—at the time of prediction.

Planning Your

Dissertation

Planning How to Spend

Your Meager TA Salary.

Good luck with that.

Page 49: The Psychology of Thinking About the Past and Future

Discussion Questions About the Planning Fallacy

Page 50: The Psychology of Thinking About the Past and Future

Other Factors in Estimating TimeWhat is the role of distracting events? When do they exert influence and when not? Maybe people are accurate about deadlines because they are less likely to cave in to distraction.

Page 51: The Psychology of Thinking About the Past and Future

Correlates and Individual Differences Would the temporal proximity of the deadline affect the accuracy of people’s completion time predictions? Would people be more or less likely to make optimistic predictions if their deadline was 6 months away (as opposed to a week away)? Outside an experimental setting, are people more likely to be accurate in their predictions if a deadline is sooner rather than significantly later (and perhaps seemingly more personally relevant)?

When participants recalled concrete past experiences, absolute accuracy did not improve, such that some participants were overly pessimistic and some overly optimistic. What personality traits might covary with these differences? What could differentiate the foolish mistake-makers from their more accurate counterparts?

Would chronic procrastinators be more or less prone to the planning fallacy than those who don’t tend to procrastinate much?

Page 52: The Psychology of Thinking About the Past and Future

CounterfactualThinking

Page 53: The Psychology of Thinking About the Past and Future

Constructing alternative versions of the

Past

Page 54: The Psychology of Thinking About the Past and Future
Page 55: The Psychology of Thinking About the Past and Future

Neal J. Roese

Psychology Marketing

Northwestern University

Page 56: The Psychology of Thinking About the Past and Future
Page 57: The Psychology of Thinking About the Past and Future

Constructing alternative versions of the

Past

Page 58: The Psychology of Thinking About the Past and Future

Both Beneficial and Aversive

Page 59: The Psychology of Thinking About the Past and Future

Upward Counterfactuals

Downward Counterfactuals

Better than reality

Even worse

Page 60: The Psychology of Thinking About the Past and Future

Related to Social Comparison Theory (Festinger)

Motives that are relevant to comparison include self-enhancement, perceptions of relative standing, maintenance of a positive self-evaluation, closure, components of attributes and the avoidance of closure (Kruglanski & Mayseless, 1990; Suls, Martin, & Wheeler, 2002).

Upward: Affect-enhancing tendency in which people note their similarity to “higher” people. Or when people need motivation for improvement.

Downward: Defensive tendency in which people look at “lower” people in order to feel better off.

Page 61: The Psychology of Thinking About the Past and Future

NegativeAffect

Activation

Consistent withApproach-Avoidance

principles

ClosenessAnd

More efficient to focus on

Minor Finetuning

Page 62: The Psychology of Thinking About the Past and Future

Normswhich are

Content Determinants

deviated from

Page 63: The Psychology of Thinking About the Past and Future

Exceptional Outcomes

Come fromExceptional Antecedents

And Normal Outcomesfrom Normal

Page 64: The Psychology of Thinking About the Past and Future

People mutate

Actions after successBut Inactions after Failures

Page 65: The Psychology of Thinking About the Past and Future

People focus on

Actions in the Short TermBut Inactions in the Long Term

Page 66: The Psychology of Thinking About the Past and Future

People focus on

The ControllableRather than the Uncontrollable

Page 67: The Psychology of Thinking About the Past and Future

Because…

Page 68: The Psychology of Thinking About the Past and Future
Page 69: The Psychology of Thinking About the Past and Future

Contrast EffectsOccur when a standard is juxtaposed

Page 70: The Psychology of Thinking About the Past and Future

Contrast Effects

Affect

Greater monetary compensation to the victim’s family when he had nearly made

it to safety

than when he died

75 miles

away

Judgment

Page 71: The Psychology of Thinking About the Past and Future

Causal-Inference EffectsLink antecendent to outcomes

within the counterfactual

Page 72: The Psychology of Thinking About the Past and Future

Causal-Inference Effects

Affect

Judgments of Blame

Judgments of Hindsight

Expectancies and Intentions

Page 73: The Psychology of Thinking About the Past and Future

Implications

Functional (mostly)

Tied With Coping and Adjustment

Maximized in Controllable Situations

Page 74: The Psychology of Thinking About the Past and Future

For MoreLook at research by

Shelley E. Taylor, UCLADan McAdams, Northwestern Univ.

Laura King, Univ. of Missouri

Page 75: The Psychology of Thinking About the Past and Future

But Wait…

Page 76: The Psychology of Thinking About the Past and Future

Another Roese StudyFrom 2010

Page 77: The Psychology of Thinking About the Past and Future

Four experiments explored whether 2 uniquely human characteristics—counterfactual thinking (imagining

alternatives to the past) and the fundamental drive to create meaning in life—are causally related….

Counterfactual thinking heightens the meaningfulness of key life experiences…. Through counterfactual

reflection, the upsides to reality are identified, a belief in fate emerges, and ultimately more meaning is derived

from important life events.

From what might have been to what must have been: Counterfactual thinking creates meaning.

(2010)

Kray, Tetlock and Roese

Page 78: The Psychology of Thinking About the Past and Future

Discussion QuestionsAbout Counterfactuals

Page 79: The Psychology of Thinking About the Past and Future

Concerns Is the fact that “the issue of the everyday accuracy of causal judgments per se has not been satisfactorily addressed in attributional research in general” a cause for major concern considering that counterfactuals rely on accurate causal judgments?

Page 80: The Psychology of Thinking About the Past and Future

CorrelatesHow might locus of control relate to counterfactual thinking? Are people who believe that they have no control over their fate less likely to engage in counterfactual thinking?

The author states that certain situational factors (fatal near misses) would be likely to evoke downward counterfactuals (alternative is worse than actuality). What individual differences could correlate with downward counterfactual thinking or counterfactual thinking in general?

What belief systems may be worth considering?And Belief Systems

And Other Individual

Differences

Locus of Control

Page 81: The Psychology of Thinking About the Past and Future

The article mentions "outcome closeness", where people are more likely to engage in counterfactual thinking when they perceive themselves as having missed a goal by a small margin. Does this match with your own experiences? Are there any advantages to focusing on far misses instead?

The benefits depend upon how accurate the content is. “If I would have studied more for the test, I could have done better” is more accurate than “If the teacher was not out to get me, I would have done better”. How often do people form accurate counterfactuals, and what influences accuracy?

Accuracy

Far Misses

Advantages and Costs

Page 82: The Psychology of Thinking About the Past and Future

Counterfactual thinking regarding traumatic events is one of the hardest cognitive processes to correct in individuals who suffer from PTSD. How might this tendency to hang onto counterfactual beliefs be affected by a belief in a just world or the desire to make inferences for future action?

So is there an appropriate level of counterfactual thinking that will lead to benefits? Would a person who excessively ruminates about what could have been miss the beneficial lesson to be learned or try to overcorrect in a way still causes failure? With regard to depression, how would an individual break the positive feedback loop between negative affect and counterfactual thinking?

Depression

PTSD

Page 83: The Psychology of Thinking About the Past and Future

ConditionsThe Roese review of literature regarding counterfactuals summarizes counterfactuals usage without exploring limitations that would otherwise restrict counterfactual thinking. How could cognitive load and attention impact the generation of counterfactuals?

Page 84: The Psychology of Thinking About the Past and Future

SimilaritiesWhat are the differences and similarities between counterfactual thinking and ruminating over past experiences?

Page 85: The Psychology of Thinking About the Past and Future

Predictability, in large part, is what turns play into work. Like a path that is fully visible, known outcomes engender tiredness at the outset, so game quickly becomes chore.

–Elizabeth Farrelly, Blubberland: The Dangers of Happiness