1
15. Choudhry NK, Fletcher RH, Soumerai SB. Systematic review: the relationship between clinical experience and quality of health care. Ann Intern Med 2005;142: 260-73. 16. Van Sickle K, Ritter EM, Baghai M, Goldenberg AE, Huang IP, Gallagher AG, et al. Prospective, randomized, double-blind trial of curriculum-based training for intra- corporeal suturing and knot tying. J Am Coll Surg 2008; 207:560-8. 17. Seymour NE, Gallagher AG, Roman SA, O’Brien MK, Ban- sal VK, Andersen DK, et al. Virtual reality training improves operating room performance: results of a randomized, double-blinded study. Ann Surg 2002;236:458-63; discussion 63-4. 18. Ahlberg G, Enochsson L, Gallagher AG, Hedman L, Hogman C, McClusky DA III, et al. Proficiency-based virtual reality training significantly reduces the error rate for residents dur- ing their first 10 laparoscopic cholecystectomies. Am J Surg 2007;193:797-804. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.surg.2012.06.030 You canÕt beat experience, but you can cheat it To the Editors: We thank Smith and colleagues for their positive comments regarding our study. We certainly endorse the development of training curricula that are based upon a sound evidence base and an understanding of the processes that underpin skill acquisition. Indeed, we have been working with clinicians for a number of years to apply knowledge derived primarily from sports psy- chology and human movement science to the acquisi- tion and performance of laparoscopic skills. 1-4 Although the apprenticeship model may have served surgical train- ing well, there is increasing recognition that both a good understanding of skill acquisition processes 5 and an appreciation of the influence of stress on technical performance 6 are important when designing effective training. We also agree with the authors that ‘‘throwing more money’’ at the problem of training (eg, high- fidelity, virtual-reality simulators) is not the only answer---especially if there is no integrated, evidence- based strategy to guide their adoption. We note that the authors raised concerns about our use of the phrase cheating experience. Our use of this lin- guistic ‘‘eye candy’’ may or may not trivialize the impor- tance of the findings, but given that we are now engaged in a positive dialogue about skill acquisition for surgery, the eye candy appears to have served its purpose: atten- tion has been drawn to the article and to the field of inquiry in general. The title is more than just provoca- tive, however, because our research shows that trainees can achieve proficient levels of performance without necessarily completing a criteria-based number of repeti- tions as part of a rigid training curriculum. Instead, they can expedite or ‘‘cheat’’ the learning curve to achieve proficiency more quickly. Specifically, trainees in our study did not waste time acquiring an expert-like visuo- motor gaze strategy by gaining experience from trial and error over many trials (as most experts do), but instead were helped to apply an expert-like visuomotor gaze strategy from the outset of learning---effectively cheating experience. Clearly, training in technical skills is only a small part of all that constitutes surgical training, so we acknowl- edge the limitations of gaze training. However, reaching proficiency earlier in the learning of basic technical skills affords the trainee more time to develop complex (and arguably more important) judgment and decision-making skills. Indeed, research in skill acquisition has shown that complex decision making is better during motor performance when the motor skills have been acquired implicitly, with few demands on working memory. 7,8 Such gains may be particularly im- portant in the current medical education climate, where legislation (eg, the European Working Time Directive) limits the time available for training. Making the best use of this time by augmenting the learning process is one way to ‘‘cheat’’ experience. Samuel J. Vine, PhD Rich S. W. Masters, DPhil John S. McGrath, MD Elizabeth Bright, MD Mark R. Wilson, PhD College of Life and Environmental Sciences University of Exeter St Lukes Campus Heavitree Road Exeter EX1 2LU, UK E-mail: [email protected] References 1. Masters RS, Lo CY, Maxwell JP, Patil NG. Implicit motor learning in surgery: implications for multi-tasking. Surgery 2008;143:140-5. 2. Zhu FF, Poolton JM, Wilson MR, Maxwell JP, Masters RSW. Implicit motor learning promotes neural efficiency during laparoscopy. Surg Endosc 2011;25:2950-5. 3. Wilson MR, Vine SJ, Bright E, Masters RSW, Defriend D, McGrath JS. Gaze training enhances laparoscopic technical skill acquisition and multi-tasking performance: a random- ized controlled study. Surg Endosc 2011;25:3731-9. 4. Vine SJ, Masters RSW, McGrath JS, Bright E, Wilson RM. Cheating experience: guiding novices to adopt the gaze strategies of experts expedites technical laparoscopic skill learning. Surgery 2012;152:32-40. 5. Gallagher AG, OÕSullivan GC. Fundamentals of surgical sim- ulation: principles and practices. London: Springer Verlag; 2011. 6. Arora S, Tierney T, Sevdalis N, Aggarwal R, Nestel D, Wo- loshynowych M, et al. The Imperial Stress Assessment Tool (ISAT): a feasible, reliable and valid approach to measur- ing stress in the operating room. World J Surg 2010;34: 1756-63. 7. Masters RSW, Poolton JM, Maxwell JP, Raab M. Implicit motor learning and complex decision making in time con- strained environments. J Motor Behav 2008;40:71-9. 8. Poolton JM, Masters RSW, Maxwell JP. The influence of anal- ogy learning on decision making in table tennis: evidence from behavioural data. Psychol Sport Exerc 2006;7:677-88. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.surg.2012.10.002 Surgery February 2013 300 Letters to the Editors

You can't beat experience, but you can cheat it

  • Upload
    mark-r

  • View
    217

  • Download
    0

Embed Size (px)

Citation preview

Page 1: You can't beat experience, but you can cheat it

SurgeryFebruary 2013

300 Letters to the Editors

15. Choudhry NK, Fletcher RH, Soumerai SB. Systematicreview: the relationship between clinical experienceand quality of health care. Ann Intern Med 2005;142:260-73.

16. Van Sickle K, Ritter EM, Baghai M, Goldenberg AE,Huang IP, Gallagher AG, et al. Prospective, randomized,double-blind trial of curriculum-based training for intra-corporeal suturing and knot tying. J Am Coll Surg 2008;207:560-8.

17. Seymour NE, Gallagher AG, Roman SA, O’Brien MK, Ban-sal VK, Andersen DK, et al. Virtual reality training improvesoperating room performance: results of a randomized,double-blinded study. Ann Surg 2002;236:458-63; discussion63-4.

18. AhlbergG, EnochssonL, Gallagher AG,HedmanL,HogmanC, McClusky DA III, et al. Proficiency-based virtual realitytraining significantly reduces the error rate for residents dur-ing their first 10 laparoscopic cholecystectomies. Am J Surg2007;193:797-804.

http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.surg.2012.06.030

You can�t beat experience, but you can cheat it

To the Editors:We thank Smith and colleagues for their positive

comments regarding our study. We certainly endorse thedevelopment of training curricula that are based upon asound evidence base and an understanding of theprocesses that underpin skill acquisition. Indeed, wehave been working with clinicians for a number of yearsto apply knowledge derived primarily from sports psy-chology and human movement science to the acquisi-tion and performance of laparoscopic skills.1-4 Althoughthe apprenticeship model may have served surgical train-ing well, there is increasing recognition that both a goodunderstanding of skill acquisition processes5 and anappreciation of the influence of stress on technicalperformance6 are important when designing effectivetraining. We also agree with the authors that ‘‘throwingmore money’’ at the problem of training (eg, high-fidelity, virtual-reality simulators) is not the onlyanswer---especially if there is no integrated, evidence-based strategy to guide their adoption.

We note that the authors raised concerns about ouruse of the phrase cheating experience. Our use of this lin-guistic ‘‘eye candy’’ may or may not trivialize the impor-tance of the findings, but given that we are now engagedin a positive dialogue about skill acquisition for surgery,the eye candy appears to have served its purpose: atten-tion has been drawn to the article and to the field ofinquiry in general. The title is more than just provoca-tive, however, because our research shows that traineescan achieve proficient levels of performance withoutnecessarily completing a criteria-based number of repeti-tions as part of a rigid training curriculum. Instead, theycan expedite or ‘‘cheat’’ the learning curve to achieveproficiency more quickly. Specifically, trainees in ourstudy did not waste time acquiring an expert-like visuo-motor gaze strategy by gaining experience from trialand error over many trials (as most experts do), butinstead were helped to apply an expert-like visuomotor

gaze strategy from the outset of learning---effectivelycheating experience.

Clearly, training in technical skills is only a small partof all that constitutes surgical training, so we acknowl-edge the limitations of gaze training. However, reachingproficiency earlier in the learning of basic technical skillsaffords the trainee more time to develop complex(and arguably more important) judgment anddecision-making skills. Indeed, research in skillacquisition has shown that complex decision making isbetter during motor performance when the motor skillshave been acquired implicitly, with few demands onworking memory.7,8 Such gains may be particularly im-portant in the current medical education climate, wherelegislation (eg, the European Working Time Directive)limits the time available for training. Making the bestuse of this time by augmenting the learning process isone way to ‘‘cheat’’ experience.

Samuel J. Vine, PhDRich S. W. Masters, DPhil

John S. McGrath, MDElizabeth Bright, MDMark R. Wilson, PhD

College of Life and Environmental SciencesUniversity of ExeterSt Lukes CampusHeavitree Road

Exeter EX1 2LU, UKE-mail: [email protected]

References

1. Masters RS, Lo CY, Maxwell JP, Patil NG. Implicit motorlearning in surgery: implications for multi-tasking. Surgery2008;143:140-5.

2. Zhu FF, Poolton JM, Wilson MR, Maxwell JP, Masters RSW.Implicit motor learning promotes neural efficiency duringlaparoscopy. Surg Endosc 2011;25:2950-5.

3. Wilson MR, Vine SJ, Bright E, Masters RSW, Defriend D,McGrath JS. Gaze training enhances laparoscopic technicalskill acquisition and multi-tasking performance: a random-ized controlled study. Surg Endosc 2011;25:3731-9.

4. Vine SJ, Masters RSW, McGrath JS, Bright E, Wilson RM.Cheating experience: guiding novices to adopt the gazestrategies of experts expedites technical laparoscopic skilllearning. Surgery 2012;152:32-40.

5. Gallagher AG, O�Sullivan GC. Fundamentals of surgical sim-ulation: principles and practices. London: Springer Verlag;2011.

6. Arora S, Tierney T, Sevdalis N, Aggarwal R, Nestel D, Wo-loshynowych M, et al. The Imperial Stress Assessment Tool(ISAT): a feasible, reliable and valid approach to measur-ing stress in the operating room. World J Surg 2010;34:1756-63.

7. Masters RSW, Poolton JM, Maxwell JP, Raab M. Implicitmotor learning and complex decision making in time con-strained environments. J Motor Behav 2008;40:71-9.

8. Poolton JM, Masters RSW, Maxwell JP. The influence of anal-ogy learning on decision making in table tennis: evidencefrom behavioural data. Psychol Sport Exerc 2006;7:677-88.

http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.surg.2012.10.002