Why Do Selfish Organisms Cooperate?

Embed Size (px)

Citation preview

  • 7/30/2019 Why Do Selfish Organisms Cooperate?

    1/6

    ANTH1014; ZFZY0; TA Name: Kathleen Bryson; Word Count: 1500

    Why do selfish organisms cooperate?

    A study conducted in 2011 by a team of scientists on 12 elephants at the Thai Elephant

    Conservation Center in Lampang, Thailand, revealed that elephants are highly cooperative.

    Working in pairs, in order for the animals to get a bucket of corn, they had to pull two ends of

    the same rope at the same time, so that a table with the corn slid within their reach. If one end of

    rope was pulled before or without the other, the whole rope slipped, leaving the elephants with

    nothing. Interestingly, they quickly learned to cooperate and none of them tried to cheat, that is,

    to try and reach for the corn on its own (Choi 2011). In intelligent mammals such as elephants,

    cooperation involves complex cognition tasks. But cooperation can also be found in species

    varying from bees to primates. Why should it have evolved in the first place, then?

    At first glance, a simplistic explanation such as Cooperation means mutual help,

    therefore benefits for both sides, so why not? may seem logical. Yet it ignores both the fact that

    there are also costs to helping another individual, and the fact that natural selection is unlikely to

    favour altruist genes. Therefore, cooperation must somehow aid selfishness (Boyd&Silk

    2006). I argue that the idea of altruism per se has to be replaced with that of pseudo-altruism,

    which is ultimately selfish. I first draw on a few intriguing anomalies such as altruistic suicide in

    bees or the occasions on which victorious deer do not kill their rivals, just to show how group-

    selectionist answers deal with them; then, I explain the most widely accepted theory about the

    mechanism that lies behind altruism, which is kin selection; after that, I summarize the points

    made by an opponent of kin selection; another aspect of pseudo-altruism as an evolutionary trait

  • 7/30/2019 Why Do Selfish Organisms Cooperate?

    2/6

    is how it can explain why humans evolved as social and moral beings so I look at moralistic

    aggression, gratitude, friendship etc.; in the end, I conclude that regardless of what mechanism

    is at the core of altruism, it is not favoured by natural selection, and therefore it is rare and

    maladaptive.

    Altruistic behaviour is a Darwinian paradox, as it benefits others at a personal cost to the

    individual. Mutualism is when this behaviour benefits the actor as well as the recipient. It is

    fragile, considering that slacking can be beneficial for the individuals, as found in coalitions

    amongst male baboons. Odd behaviours such as when bees are willing to die to defend their hive,

    or when deer who win a fight do not kill their rivals (Lorenz 1963), have been explained through

    group-selection, which states that the individual behaviour aids the preservation of a group,

    population or species (Wynne-Edwards 1962). Nevertheless, natural selection depends on

    genetic variability, which is greater within a group than between groups. Genes promoting

    selfishness would spread much faster than those promoting altruism.

    Then what is really going on? In order for natural selection to favour altruism, the

    benefits to the recipient have to outweigh the costs suffered by the actor. How could that be? If

    altruism is performed among individuals more likely to carry a gene for altruism, then that gene

    will increase in frequency. The probability of individuals sharing a gene for altruism is

    equivalent to asking what the average degree of relatedness between two individuals is. So an

    individual is 100% related to himself, 50% related to his parents and full-siblings, 25% related to

    his grandparents and half-siblings, 12.5% to his great-grandparents or cousins etc.

    Mathematically, it is easy to see how the average degree of relatedness multiplied by the benefits

    to the recipient have to outweigh the costs in order for an altruistic behaviour to result in

    increased fitness. This is why altruism is not expected to be found amongst non-related

  • 7/30/2019 Why Do Selfish Organisms Cooperate?

    3/6

    individuals (Hamilton 1964). Back to a group-selectionist view, groups containing altruistic

    individuals always out-compete those with no or fewer such individuals. But within a group,

    altruistic individuals are disadvantaged.

    Following this logic, it is reasonable that recognition mechanisms should have evolved so

    as to, on the one hand, enable individuals to distinguish their relatives from non-related

    individuals, on the other hand to enable them to maintain helpful social relationships. West et al.

    (2007) distinguish between genetic cues (smell) and environmental ones (shared environment or

    previous association), stating that the latter are more effective than the former, which would most

    likely reduce precisely the variation required for kin selection to occur. Another possible

    mechanism would be the green-beard effect (a term coined by Richard Dawkins in 1976), which

    states that a trait has to be perceptible, has to enable recognition of the same trait in others and a

    preferential treatment to those recognized. On the other hand, the importance of social

    relationships stems from how they aid cooperation. For instance, grooming cultivates social

    bonds between individuals useful to one another for coalitions, such as in male red howler

    monkeys. They live in groups consisting of two to four females and one or two males, which

    really makes mating a hard task. Males work together to gain access to breeding females by

    evicting male residents, and by mate guarding.

    According to Trivers (1985), kin selection explains parents-offspring rivalry: selection

    favours mothers who provide less than their infants desire, and infants that demand more than

    their mothers are willing to give. Not everybody agrees. Goldschmidt (2005), focusing on the

    mother-child relationship and cooperation rather than on couples or social groups, developed the

    theory of affect hunger (the craving for attention and affection as means of manipulating other

  • 7/30/2019 Why Do Selfish Organisms Cooperate?

    4/6

    people to gain access to various resources) as a neuro-anthropological alternative to kin

    selection. He has no problem with arguing that people are selfish, but only with arguing that

    altruism is the right way to be selfish. He challenges kin selection stating that there is hardly any

    consideration of neural mechanisms involved in the process it all sums up to the assumption

    that the gene causes altruistic behaviour if it is favourable to its transmission. Moreover, altruism

    should be even more selective after all, human beings often behave altruistically towards non-

    related individuals! Acknowledging the kin-selectionist theory which explains indiscriminate

    altruism through the fact that our ancestors were generally in the exclusive presence of their kin,

    he does not find this explanation reasonable for altruism extended to other species. Another

    interesting point he makes is that kin selection takes for granted the assumption that organic

    variation easily produces adaptations suitable to any environment, and that all traits are

    necessarily adaptive, when in fact selection acts upon a whole organism and given that traits

    are often genetically connected, selection might act on another trait produced by the same gene.

    Despite the fact that the two views mentioned above clash when it comes to explaining

    how cooperation evolved, both of them acknowledge the inexistence of pure altruism as an

    adaptive behavioural trait. I would argue that when we rush to save somebody in urgent need, we

    act instinctively without considering any beneficial outcome to our inclusive fitness. Even so,

    this proves the existence of pure altruism, but not that it would occur on a daily basis or be

    favoured by natural selection.

    To outline wider socio-psychological implications of our adaptations towards

    cooperation, I now turn to an article by Trivers (1985) which explains the evolution of sociality

    and morality as beneficial for cooperation. He states that our tendency to form relationships and

  • 7/30/2019 Why Do Selfish Organisms Cooperate?

    5/6

    to act altruistically towards friends and towards the people we like goes both ways: the more we

    like them, the more altruistically we behave towards them and the more altruistic they are, the

    more we like them. I believe that our social complexities confirm this view: a lot of people

    would rather help their very close friends rather than an estranged relative. Regarding moralistic

    aggression, always preceded by a sense of fairness, it is yet another mechanism ensuring the

    strength and durability of social relationships, and the punishment and ostracizing of cheaters.

    Even gratitude, sympathy, guilt and reparative altruism have evolved in order to preserve and

    repair social bonds. This is especially important in the human species, where systems of multi-

    party altruism may operate simultaneously, so that an individual does not gain benefits from the

    individual aided, but from a third-party individual.

    I have argued that cooperation, especially altruism, is an apparent Darwinian paradox, for

    it costs the actor who could be better off not cooperating at all. Group selection is not a viable

    explanation, as it would destroy the very variability that laid its foundation. While kin selection

    is most widely acknowledged, complementary views still need to be considered, such as

    Goldschmidts affect hunger theory. As a concluding remark, I state that pure altruism is not

    favoured by natural selection and, in this sense, can be pictured as a maladaptive behaviour.

    However, pseudo-altruism and other types of cooperation are favoured, and understanding their

    further influences on our sociality and morality may help us reassess just how good and selfless

    we think we are.

  • 7/30/2019 Why Do Selfish Organisms Cooperate?

    6/6

    References

    BOYD, R. AND SILK, J. B. 2006. How Humans Evolved. University of California, Los

    Angeles.

    CHOI, C. Q. 2011. Elephants cooperate, proving how smart they really are.

    http://www.livescience.com/13108-elephants-cooperate-intelligent-behavior.html

    DAWKINS, R. 1976. The Selfish Gene. Oxford University Press.

    GOLDSCHMIDT, W. 2005. The Bridge to Humanity. How Affect Hunger Trumps the Selfish

    Gene. Oxford University Press.

    HAMILTON, W. D. 1996. The Evolution of Social Behaviour. Journal of Theoretical

    Biology.

    LORENZ, K. 1963. On aggression. Harcourt and Brace Company.

    TRIVERS, R. 1985. Social Evolution. Menlo Park: Benjamin Cummings.

    WEST, S. A.; GRIFFIN, A. S.; GARDNER, A. 2007. Evolutionary explanations for

    cooperation. Current Biology 17: R661-R672.

    WYNNE-EDWARDS, V. 1962. Animal Dispersion in Relation to Social Behaviour.

    Edinburgh: Oliver & Boyd.

    http://www.livescience.com/13108-elephants-cooperate-intelligent-behavior.htmlhttp://www.livescience.com/13108-elephants-cooperate-intelligent-behavior.html