16
Whistleblowing Intentions of Lower-Level Employees: The Effect of Reporting Channel, Bystanders, and Wrongdoer Power Status Jingyu Gao Robert Greenberg Bernard Wong-On-Wing Received: 15 January 2013 / Accepted: 29 November 2013 / Published online: 12 February 2014 Ó Springer Science+Business Media Dordrecht 2014 Abstract It has been suggested that a reporting channel administered by a third-party may represent a stronger procedural safeguard of anonymity and avoids the appear- ance of impropriety. This study examines whistleblowing intentions among lower-tier employees, specifically exam- ines whether an externally-administered reporting channel increases whistleblowing intentions compared to an inter- nally-administered one. In contrast to the findings of an earlier study by Kaplan et al. (Audit J Pract Theory 28(2):273–288, 2009), our results suggest that whistle- blowing intentions are higher when the reporting channel is administered externally than when it is administered inter- nally. We also find that an externally-administered reporting channel mitigates the negative effect of bystanders on whistleblowing intentions. Implications are discussed. Keywords Whistleblowing Á Bystander effect Á Reporting channel Á Sarbanes-Oxley Introduction Each year, Time magazine selects a ‘‘Person of the Year’’—the individual whom it believes affected history the most during that year. In 2002, the ‘‘Person of the Year’’ consisted of three women who were whistleblowers within large organizations: Enron, the FBI, and WorldCom. The scandals following the reports of these whistleblowers provided impetus for the United States Congress to pass the Sarbanes-Oxley (SOX) Act (2002), which encourages whistleblowing and provides protection from retaliation. Since then, whistleblowing has continued to receive increased attention, particularly given evidence of its rel- ative importance in fraud detection compared to internal controls or internal auditing. For example, the 2008 Report to the Nation on Occupational Fraud and Abuse by the Association of Certified Fraud Examiners (ACFE 2008, p. 38) find that 46.2 % of frauds are initially detected by tips provided by employees while internal audit detects only 19.4 % and internal controls detect 23.3 %.Today, additional protection and incentives for whistleblowers are provided by the Dodd-Frank Wall Street Reform and Consumer Protection Act (2010). Although most frauds are perpetrated by upper man- agement or from within the accounting department (ACFE 2008), other employees within the organization may also have knowledge of the fraudulent activity. Moberly (2006, p. 1108) notes, for example, that in the financial miscon- duct cases at many companies, including Enron, ‘‘countless lower-level employees necessarily knew about, were exposed to, or were involved superficially in the wrong- doing and its concealment, but few disclosed it, either to company officials or to the public.’’ Apparently, a rela- tively large number of lower-level employees are exposed to fraud, but remain silent. We are thankful for the valuable comments of participants at the Waterloo 1st Biennial Symposium on Accounting Ethics, the 2012 ABO Meeting in Atlanta, and the 17th Annual Ethics Research Symposium in Washington, DC. J. Gao (&) Renmin University of China, Beijing, China e-mail: [email protected] R. Greenberg Á B. Wong-On-Wing (&) Washington State University, Pullman, WA, USA e-mail: [email protected] R. Greenberg e-mail: [email protected] 123 J Bus Ethics (2015) 126:85–99 DOI 10.1007/s10551-013-2008-4

Whistleblowing Intentions of LowerLevel Employees the Effect

Embed Size (px)

DESCRIPTION

dddddddd

Citation preview

  • Whistleblowing Intentions of Lower-Level Employees: The Effectof Reporting Channel, Bystanders, and Wrongdoer Power Status

    Jingyu Gao Robert Greenberg Bernard Wong-On-Wing

    Received: 15 January 2013 / Accepted: 29 November 2013 / Published online: 12 February 2014

    Springer Science+Business Media Dordrecht 2014

    Abstract It has been suggested that a reporting channel

    administered by a third-party may represent a stronger

    procedural safeguard of anonymity and avoids the appear-

    ance of impropriety. This study examines whistleblowing

    intentions among lower-tier employees, specifically exam-

    ines whether an externally-administered reporting channel

    increases whistleblowing intentions compared to an inter-

    nally-administered one. In contrast to the findings of an

    earlier study by Kaplan et al. (Audit J Pract Theory

    28(2):273288, 2009), our results suggest that whistle-

    blowing intentions are higher when the reporting channel is

    administered externally than when it is administered inter-

    nally. We also find that an externally-administered reporting

    channel mitigates the negative effect of bystanders on

    whistleblowing intentions. Implications are discussed.

    Keywords Whistleblowing Bystander effect Reporting channel Sarbanes-Oxley

    Introduction

    Each year, Time magazine selects a Person of the

    Yearthe individual whom it believes affected history

    the most during that year. In 2002, the Person of the

    Year consisted of three women who were whistleblowers

    within large organizations: Enron, the FBI, and WorldCom.

    The scandals following the reports of these whistleblowers

    provided impetus for the United States Congress to pass the

    Sarbanes-Oxley (SOX) Act (2002), which encourages

    whistleblowing and provides protection from retaliation.

    Since then, whistleblowing has continued to receive

    increased attention, particularly given evidence of its rel-

    ative importance in fraud detection compared to internal

    controls or internal auditing. For example, the 2008 Report

    to the Nation on Occupational Fraud and Abuse by the

    Association of Certified Fraud Examiners (ACFE 2008,

    p. 38) find that 46.2 % of frauds are initially detected by

    tips provided by employees while internal audit detects

    only 19.4 % and internal controls detect 23.3 %.Today,

    additional protection and incentives for whistleblowers are

    provided by the Dodd-Frank Wall Street Reform and

    Consumer Protection Act (2010).

    Although most frauds are perpetrated by upper man-

    agement or from within the accounting department (ACFE

    2008), other employees within the organization may also

    have knowledge of the fraudulent activity. Moberly (2006,

    p. 1108) notes, for example, that in the financial miscon-

    duct cases at many companies, including Enron, countless

    lower-level employees necessarily knew about, were

    exposed to, or were involved superficially in the wrong-

    doing and its concealment, but few disclosed it, either to

    company officials or to the public. Apparently, a rela-

    tively large number of lower-level employees are exposed

    to fraud, but remain silent.

    We are thankful for the valuable comments of participants at the

    Waterloo 1st Biennial Symposium on Accounting Ethics, the 2012

    ABO Meeting in Atlanta, and the 17th Annual Ethics Research

    Symposium in Washington, DC.

    J. Gao (&)Renmin University of China, Beijing, China

    e-mail: [email protected]

    R. Greenberg B. Wong-On-Wing (&)Washington State University, Pullman, WA, USA

    e-mail: [email protected]

    R. Greenberg

    e-mail: [email protected]

    123

    J Bus Ethics (2015) 126:8599

    DOI 10.1007/s10551-013-2008-4

  • Consistent with the above observation, Mesmer-Magnus

    and Viswesvaran (2005) find in their meta-analysis that

    younger employees or those at a lower job level are

    uncomfortable with reporting wrongdoing (particularly by

    those at higher levels in the organizational hierarchy).

    Those employees feel that they lack sufficient power to

    blow the whistle and effect change. In contrast, consistent

    with earlier studies, (e.g. Near and Miceli 1996; Miceli

    et al. 1991a, b), Mesmer-Magnus and Viswesvaran (2005)

    observe that employees of higher tenure and job level are

    more likely to blow the whistle. They suggest that orga-

    nizations should consider targeting lower-tier employees

    for specific training, including communicating how reports

    of wrongdoing might be made.

    In the present study, we examine whistleblowing inten-

    tions specifically among lower-tier employees. We investi-

    gate the extent to which an externally administered

    reporting channel increases whistleblowing intentions

    compared to an internally administered one. It has been

    suggested that a reporting channel administered by a third-

    party may represent a stronger procedural safeguard of

    anonymity and avoids the appearance of impropriety (ACFE

    2005; The Network 2006). Consequently, it should be

    associated with higher levels of whistleblowing than an

    internally administered reporting channel. Interestingly,

    however, a study by Kaplan et al. (2009) unexpectedly finds

    that reporting intention is greater when the reporting channel

    is administered internally than when administered exter-

    nally. Kaplan et al. (2009) speculate that their unexpected

    finding may be attributable to the preference of executives

    and upper management not to disclose fraud occurrences to

    outsiders because of their concern about the reputation. In

    the current study, we conjecture that to the extent that lower-

    level employees do not have the same perspective and

    incentive as upper-level executives, they may be less con-

    cerned about the companys reputation. Instead, they may

    focus on the anonymity of the reporting channel due to the

    concern about possible retaliation. As a result, the claimed

    benefit of an externally administered reporting channel may

    be observed among lower-level employees.

    In the present research, we first replicate the study of the

    effect of reporting channel administration on whistle-

    blowing intentions and test the generalizability of the

    unexpected finding by Kaplan et al. (2009) among lower-

    tier employees. We specifically seek to establish the extent

    to which whistleblowing intention among those employees

    is higher (or lower as in Kaplan et al. 2009) when the

    reporting channel is administered externally than when it is

    administered internally. We then extend the research by

    examining the degree to which an externally administered

    reporting channel can alleviate the negative effect of two

    contextual factors on whistleblowing intentions: the pre-

    sence of bystanders and high wrongdoer power status.

    The bystander effect, documented by Latane and Darley

    (1968), suggests that the presence of other people will

    inhibit ones intention to help. In the current context, it

    means that compared to the situation in which a single

    individual observes some wrongdoing, when more than one

    person witnesses the wrongdoing, each individual will be

    less likely to blow the whistle because according to the

    diffusion of responsibility, perceived individual responsi-

    bility is decreased.

    Wrongdoer power status can also reduce whistleblowing

    intentions. Earlier studies (e.g., Cortina and Magley 2003;

    Lee et al. 2004; Rehg et al. 2008) have shown that retali-

    ation is more likely from individuals whose power status is

    high. Given the higher likelihood of retaliation, whistle-

    blowing intention can be expected to be lower when the

    wrongdoing is perpetrated by high-level employees than

    when it is performed by lower-level employees.

    Participants in our study were undergraduate business

    students from a state university in the US. They had limited

    work experience and served as surrogate lower-tier

    employees. To observe the influence of the three factors of

    interest, we manipulated reporting channel administrator

    (internal vs. external), presence of bystanders (absence vs.

    presence of other bystanders), and wrongdoers power

    status (supervisor vs. entry-level employee) between

    subjects.

    The results of our experiment suggest that lower-level

    employees reporting intention is higher when the reporting

    channel is administered externally by a third-party than

    when it is administered internally. Moreover, the externally

    administered reporting channel moderates the bystander

    effect. In particular, when the reporting channel is admin-

    istered internally, the reporting intention of a potential

    whistleblower is significantly lower when there are other

    bystanders compared to when there are no other bystand-

    ers. In contrast, whistleblowing intentions do not differ

    significantly between the two conditions when the report-

    ing channel is administered externally by a third-party. We

    also find that lower-level employees whistleblowing

    intention is significantly lower when the wrongdoer is a

    supervisor than when s/he is a coworker. This effect is,

    however, not significantly reduced by the use of an exter-

    nally administered reporting channel.

    Our research contributes to the whistleblowing literature

    in several ways. First, our study provides evidence that

    reporting channels administered externally may be more

    effective than those administered internally in promoting

    whistleblowing among lower-tier employees. Our study

    thus complements that of Kaplan et al. (2009), who find

    opposite results using participants in a management role.

    Together, the results of the two studies provide evidence

    consistent with the notion that lower-tier employees may

    not be as concerned as upper management about disclosing

    86 J. Gao et al.

    123

  • fraud occurrences to outsiders. This presents an interesting

    dilemma. The issue is, however, in practice; companies can

    balance the need to detect fraud and the importance of

    protecting the companys reputation. Second, we find a

    bystander effect that negatively impacts whistleblowing

    intentions. Moreover, results of our path analysis provide

    evidence consistent with the theory of diffusion of

    responsibility. This outcome is opposite of the findings by

    Robinson et al. (2012). We conjecture that possible

    explanations for this inconsistency may be differences in

    the design, participants and data collection procedures

    between the two studies. Third, we find that the negative

    effect of bystanders is moderated by the use of an exter-

    nally administered reporting channel. This result provides

    further evidence of the difference in effectiveness between

    internally and externally administered reporting channels

    in promoting whistleblowing.

    The remainder of this paper is organized as follows. The

    next section provides background information and devel-

    ops the hypotheses. The third section describes the research

    method and the fourth section reports the results of the

    study. The implications of the findings as well as the

    limitations of the current study are discussed in the final

    section.

    Background and Hypotheses

    Whistleblowing

    Since the scandals at Enron, the FBI, Worldcom, and other

    organizations, whistleblowing has received growing

    attention from the public and academia. Research has

    examined both determinants of individuals whistleblowing

    as well as other aspects of the phenomenon, such as steps in

    making whistleblowing decisions (Vandekerckhove and

    Lewis 2012; Lewis and Kender 2010), the correlation

    between whistleblowing intention and actual behavior

    (Mesmer-Magnus and Viswesvaran 2005) and perceived or

    actual retaliation following whistleblowing (e.g. Mesmer-

    Magnus and Viswesvaran 2005; Rehg et al. 2008).

    A significant amount of research has focused on factors

    that impact individuals intention to blow the whistle. It has

    been documented that people in an organization are more

    likely to report unethical or illegal issues if they have better

    job performance reviews, better education, superior posi-

    tions compared to the wrongdoer, and higher scores in

    moral judgment tests (e.g. Brabeck 1984; Chiu 2003; Mi-

    celi and Near 1984; Near and Miceli 1996; Sims and

    Keenan 1998). Potential whistleblowers demographic

    information, such as gender, may also play a role in the

    decision making process. For example, Kaplan et al. (2008)

    indicate that when an anonymous reporting channel is

    available, males and females differ in their judgments

    regarding the reduction in personal costs of an anonymous

    reporting channel compared to a non-anonymous reporting

    channel.

    The effects of contextual and organizational attributes

    on company employees willingness to blow the whistle

    have also been studied (e.g. Keenan 1995; King 1999;

    Kaptein 2011). For example, perceived organizational

    justice (Seifert et al. 2010), the supervisors support (Sims

    and Keenan 1998), the organizations propensity to

    encourage whistleblowing (Mesmer-Magnus and Vis-

    wesvaran 2005) and public industry type (Perry and Wise

    1990) are strongly and positively related to employees

    intention to report fraud. Furthermore, employees are more

    willing to blow the whistle when it is encouraged by the

    general organizational climate. An ethical organization

    culture consists of many aspects, including integrating

    ethics in decision making processes, establishing a com-

    pensation system that rewards ethical behavior, and setting

    a tone that allows employees to question obedience of

    authority (Rothwell and Baldwin 2006; Trevino et al.

    1999). Not surprisingly, whistleblowers tend to report

    externally when the organizational context seems unfa-

    vorable (Miceli and Near 1992). Blowing the whistle

    externally has also been found to be positively associated

    with the supervisors support, as well as informal and

    formal company policies that encourage external whistle-

    blowing (Sims and Keenan 1998). Additionally, compared

    to internal whistleblowers, those who report to an external

    recipient tend to provide more convincing evidence with

    which to support their allegations of wrongdoing (Dworkin

    and Baucus 1998).

    In the present study, we posit that relative to an inter-

    nally administered reporting channel, one that is externally

    administered can enhance whistleblowing among lower-

    level employees. We also hypothesize that an externally

    administered reporting channel can alleviate the negative

    effect on whistleblowing intentions of two contextual fac-

    tors: the presence of bystanders and high wrongdoer power

    status. Below we develop our hypotheses.

    Reporting Channel

    After observing that someone is engaged in wrongdoing,

    employees may report it to the wrongdoers supervisor or

    to someone who has the authority to investigate, detect, or

    terminate the wrongdoing. However, under this normal

    face-to-face reporting, perceived personal cost will be

    higher compared to when the reporting is anonymous

    (Ayers and Kaplan 2005). Consequently, face-to-face

    reporting of wrongdoing can be expected to be low relative

    to anonymous reporting. Surveys of employees provide

    evidence of this. For example, The Network (2010) found

    Whistleblowing Intentions of Lower-Level Employees 87

    123

  • that in 2009, 71 % of the participants did not directly notify

    management of an observed questionable issue. Similarly,

    the Ethics Resource Center (2007) found that 42 % of

    employees who observed misconduct remained silent.

    More than half indicated that they did so because they

    would have had to report to the person involved or because

    they were not aware of the availability of an anonymous

    reporting channel. These findings indicate that an anony-

    mous reporting channel may be able to effectively detect

    fraud or even potentially prevent fraud.

    The SOX Act of (2002) requires the audit committee of

    companies to establish whistleblowing procedures whereby

    employees can anonymously submit issues of concern

    regarding questionable accounting or auditing. However,

    there is no detailed guideline about how to administer an

    anonymous reporting channel. Within publicly-traded

    companies, the SEC allows audit committees broad dis-

    cretion regarding policies and procedures for anonymous

    reporting channels. Therefore, the effectiveness and con-

    fidentiality of an anonymous reporting channel may vary

    depending on how it is administered.

    Corporations can operate their anonymous reporting

    channels in-house within their Department of Internal

    Audit or Department of Human Resource (Kaplan et al.

    2009). In an investigation of ethics programs in Fortune

    500 service and industrial firms, Weaver et al. (1999) find

    that Human Resources (HR) is ultimately responsible for

    ethics/compliance management in 28 % of responding

    firms, while legal and audit staff are taking a more prom-

    inent role (Weaver and Trevino 2001). Apparently, the role

    of HR in companies ethics management is relatively minor

    (Weaver and Trevino 2001). On the other hand, the

    Department of Internal Audit has a unique role within the

    organization, with close relationship to both senior man-

    agement and the audit committee. Thus it is a natural outlet

    for potential whistleblowers (PricewaterhouseCoopers

    2004; Read and Rama 2003). However, from the employ-

    ees perspective, the internal audit is not a preferred

    reporting channel, especially when social confrontation

    occurs (Kaplan et al. 2010).

    Companies can also utilize a reporting system main-

    tained by an independent third-party provider which is

    considered to provide stronger procedural safeguards

    including high confidentiality, trained professionals, and an

    effective investigation process afterward (Kaplan et al.

    2009). According to Trevino et al. (1999), a number of US

    companies outsource their telephone hotlines to outside

    consultants or security companies in distant cities. Fifteen

    percent of members of the Ethics Officers Association

    (EOA) outsource their reporting system. Another nineteen

    percent have a hybrid (internal/outsourced) system. Pro-

    fessional organizations (e.g. ACFE 2005; The Network

    2006) indicate that a hotline administered by a third party

    represents a strong procedural safeguard and provides

    many benefits. One important benefit is that an independent

    third-party can provide greater protection of anonymity,

    especially for smaller companies. The reason is that in

    those small-size organizations, everyone knows each other

    and thus, it is particularly difficult to guarantee confiden-

    tiality and anonymity. Furthermore, outsourcing the

    reporting hotline can not only be less expensive for com-

    panies, but it can also insure that issues are treated pro-

    fessionally and quickly.

    Based on the above, compared to an internally admin-

    istered reporting channel, one that is administered exter-

    nally should be perceived to be more reliable in protecting

    the anonymity of individuals who disclose fraudulent

    behavior. This in turn should reduce individuals perceived

    personal cost of whistleblowing including that associated

    with the risk of retaliation. As a result, whistleblowing

    should be more likely when the reporting channel is

    administered externally than when administered internally.

    Thus, our first hypothesis is stated as follows:

    H1 After observing a fraudulent act, employees inten-

    tion to blow the whistle will be greater when the com-

    panys anonymous reporting hotline is administered

    externally by a third-party than when it is administered

    internally by the company.

    Bystander Effect

    The bystander effect (Latane and Darley 1968, 1970)

    occurs when bystanders are less likely to intervene because

    other people are present. In the current context, the

    bystander effect suggests that an individual would be less

    likely to blow the whistle when s/he is aware that others

    have witnessed a wrongdoing. For example, Dozier and

    Miceli (1985) and Graham (1986) find that individuals are

    less likely to blow the whistle when many organizational

    members could have observed the wrongdoing. Similarly,

    Miceli et al. (1991a, b) note that internal auditors are more

    likely to blow the whistle through an external reporting

    channel when there are few observers. As described below,

    we posit that the bystander effect will influence whistle-

    blowing intention via individuals perception of personal

    responsibility and perceived personal cost in the form of

    openness to blame and evaluation apprehension.

    Diffusion of responsibility has been used to explain the

    bystander effect phenomenon. Responsibility is typi-

    cally defined in terms of openness to blame. Discussion of

    the decision process associated with aiding has often

    focused on the costs (and rewards) of either helping or not

    helping (e.g. Latane and Darley 1970; Piliavin and Piliavin

    1972). When one is alone, one is the sole object of blame

    from others if the emergency results in a tragedy. Thus,

    88 J. Gao et al.

    123

    Windows UserHighlight

  • perceived personal responsibility and the cost to oneself of

    failing to intervene may be high. When many are present,

    however, the potential for blame is more diffused. Conse-

    quently, perceived personal responsibility and the cost of

    failing to intervene is less for any given individual. When

    perceived personal responsibility is lower and noninter-

    vention has less cost, intervention is less likely to occur.

    The bystander effect phenomenon can also be explained

    in terms of cost in the form of evaluation apprehension. If

    bystanders believe that others are aware of their presence,

    the bystanders may be apprehensive regarding others

    expectations and evaluations, and thus seek to optimize

    these evaluations. Latane and Darley (1970) have called

    this process audience inhibition, whereas Schwartz and

    Gottlieb (1976) have adopted the label evaluation appre-

    hension, because they reason that concern with others

    evaluations may enhance as well as inhibit helping. When

    wrongdoing is observed by a single individual, s/he is less

    likely to be apprehensive about others evaluations and

    expectations, since no one else is aware of her/his action. In

    contrast, concern about others evaluation and reaction

    would be high if other bystanders are present.

    In summary, in the case of offering help to victims in an

    emergency situation, the responsibility and cost for non-

    intervention would be higher when the observer is alone

    than when there are several onlookers, since in the latter

    case, the responsibility and cost for nonintervention are

    shared. In contrast, negative consequences (e.g., getting the

    cold shoulder from the boss and co-workers, retaliation) to

    whistleblowers may arise from their intervention instead of

    their non-intervention. Moreover, the responsibility and

    cost for intervention would be higher when there are sev-

    eral witnesses than when the whistleblower is the only one

    to know about the wrongdoing. Thus, consistent with the

    bystander effect, an employee is more likely to blow the

    whistle when there are no bystanders than when others are

    present. Interestingly, a recent study by Robinson et al.

    (2012) finds the opposite of the bystander effect. We re-

    examine and predict the bystander effect using a homo-

    geneous student participant sample and a between-subject

    design, instead of participants from diverse backgrounds

    and a within-subject design as employed in that study.

    Anonymity vis-a-vis the other bystander has been found

    to affect helping, apparently by reducing evaluation appre-

    hension (Schwartz and Gottlieb 1980). As noted earlier, in

    the current context, the potential whistleblower is likely to

    perceive personal blame to be higher when others have

    witnessed the wrongdoing than when the whistleblower is

    alone to have witnessed it. Thus, s/he will be less likely to

    blow the whistle when others are present. We posit that the

    benefit of an external reporting channel on whistleblowing

    intentions (predicted in H1) will be more significant when

    others are present than when the whistleblower is alone to

    observe the wrongdoing. This is because when personal

    blame is perceived to be high due to the presence of others,

    providing a better guarantee of anonymity through an

    externally administered reporting channel can significantly

    lower perceived personal cost including the likelihood of

    retaliation. The effect on whistleblowing intentions should

    similarly be significant. In contrast, when the whistleblower

    is the only observer of the wrongdoing, the increased

    assurance of anonymity afforded by an externally adminis-

    tered reporting channel is expected to have a less significant

    effect on perceived personal cost and thus, whistleblowing

    intentions. Accordingly, we predict the following:

    H2 The positive effect of an externally administered

    reporting channel on employees whistleblowing intention

    will be more significant when wrongdoing is observed in

    the presence of bystanders than when it is observed in the

    absence of bystanders.

    Wrongdoer Power Status

    The wrongdoers power status can also influence whistle-

    blowing intention via perceived seriousness and personal

    cost. It is often measured in terms of the wrongdoers

    position in the organization, which reflects bases of power

    or the dependence of the organization on the individual

    (Miceli et al. 2008). Perceived seriousness can thus be

    inferred from the power status of the wrongdoer, which

    may indicate information about the amount and/or the

    extent of the damages or losses associated with the

    wrongdoing. We propose that one way in which wrong-

    doers power status may influence whistleblowing intention

    is through the perceived seriousness of the wrongdoing.

    In addition, previous studies (see Cortina and Magley

    2003; Lee et al. 2004; Rehg et al. 2008) find that the more

    powerful the wrongdoer, or the more dependent the orga-

    nization is on the wrongdoer, the more retaliation will be

    directed toward the whistleblower, ceteris paribus. Since

    retaliation is an important component of personal cost, the

    potential whistleblower is likely to perceive personal cost

    to be higher when the power status of the wrongdoer is

    high than when it is low. Because of the perceived higher

    cost, we propose that a potential whistleblower will be

    more concerned about anonymity, and thus, be less likely

    to blow the whistle when the power status of the wrongdoer

    is high than when it is low. We further posit that the benefit

    of an external reporting channel on whistleblowing inten-

    tions (predicted in H1) will be more significant when the

    wrongdoers power status is high than when it is low. This

    is because when the personal cost is perceived to be high

    due to the high power status of the wrongdoer, providing a

    better guarantee of anonymity through an externally

    administered reporting channel can significantly lower the

    Whistleblowing Intentions of Lower-Level Employees 89

    123

  • likelihood of retaliation and the associated cost. The effect

    on whistleblowing intentions should similarly be signifi-

    cant. In contrast, when power status is low, the perceived

    personal cost is already relatively low given that the like-

    lihood of retaliation is low, compared to when power status

    is high. The increased assurance of anonymity afforded by

    an externally administered reporting channel is expected to

    have a less significant effect on the likelihood of retaliation

    and thus, whistleblowing intentions. Accordingly, we pre-

    dict the following:

    H3 The positive effect of an externally administered

    reporting channel on employees whistleblowing intention

    will be more significant when the power status of a

    potential wrongdoer is high than when it is low.

    Methods

    Design

    We employed a 2 (reporting channel) 9 2 (bystander) 9 2

    (power status) 9 2 (case) design, fully crossing the

    reporting channel administration (internal vs. external), the

    number of bystanders (no other bystander vs. two other

    bystanders) and the wrongdoers power status (supervisor

    vs. entry-level employee) between subjects. Moreover,

    since each participant evaluated two cases (fictitious sup-

    plier case and qualification fee case), case is a within-

    subject factor.

    Participants

    A total of 369 business students at a US public university

    participated in the study. They served as surrogate low-tier

    employees. Their average age was 20.5 years (s.d. = 2.72)

    and 57 % were male. Over 75 % of the participants

    reported having work experience. The average work

    experience was 3.5 years (s.d. = 2.23). These demo-

    graphic factors do not differ significantly (p [ 0.10) acrossexperimental groups.

    Task

    Participants were provided with information about a

    hypothetical company, High Energy Corporation (hereafter

    HEC). We included excerpts from its Code of Ethics to

    communicate the availability of an anonymous reporting

    channel and enable the manipulation of the channel

    administration factor. Every participant then read two

    independent case scenarios, which each described an

    employee faced with a whistleblowing decision after

    becoming aware of a fraudulent act. In the first case (case

    A: fictitious supplier), the fraudster approves payment of a

    fake invoice for a fictitious supplier. In the second case

    (case B: qualification fee), the fraudulent employee

    requires payment of a fee to his personal account for a

    supplier to qualify for a bid. Within each case, the number

    of bystanders and the power status of the wrongdoer were

    manipulated (see Independent Variables section). After

    reading each case, participants were asked to indicate the

    probability that the would-be whistleblower would call the

    hotline to report the wrongdoing. At the end of the task,

    participants responded to manipulation check questions

    and provided demographic information.

    Independent Variables

    Reporting Channel Administration

    In the internal administration condition, participants were

    told that HECs reporting hotline is operated by the com-

    panys Department of Internal Audit. Participants in the

    external administration condition were told that the

    reporting hotline is operated by an independent third party

    named Compliance Expert Co., Ltd., that is located in New

    York, NY, USA. Other details regarding the reporting

    hotlines (e.g., availability 24 h a day, 7 days a week) were

    identical across both conditions.

    Presence of Bystanders

    We manipulated the presence versus absence of bystanders

    (other than the potential whistleblower) in each of the two

    cases as follows. In the fictitious supplier case, the would-

    be whistleblower becomes aware of the wrongdoing when

    one late evening at the office, he overhears a phone con-

    versation (about the fake invoice) between the fraudster

    and his accomplice. In the bystander present condition, the

    potential whistleblower also hears two coworkers talking

    about the wrongdoing after the fraudster leaves. In the

    bystander absent condition, the two coworkers are not

    mentioned.

    In the qualification fee case, the would-be whistleblower

    learns about the fraud when he goes to the copy room and

    finds a printed copy of the fraudsters letter to the pro-

    spective supplier requesting the prohibited fee. In the

    bystander present condition, the potential whistleblower

    sees two other employees reading the letter when they go to

    the copy room. In the bystander absent condition, the other

    two employees are not mentioned.

    Wrongdoer Power Status

    The manipulation of wrongdoer power status was effected

    in the two cases as follows. In the high power status

    90 J. Gao et al.

    123

  • condition, the wrongdoer was described as the observers

    supervisor in case A, and as a purchasing manager in case

    B. In the low power status condition, the wrongdoer was

    described as an entry-level employee in both case A and

    case B.

    Dependent Variable

    The primary dependent variable is the whistleblowing

    intention using the available hotline. Participants were

    asked to assess the probability (from 0 to 100 %) that the

    potential whistleblower would call the hotline to report the

    wrongdoing. Previous research (e.g. Cohen et al. 1998,

    2001; Chung and Monroe 2003) indicates that social

    desirability bias is more likely when behavior intention

    items are phrased in the first person than when phrased in

    the third person. Therefore, we phrased the question in the

    third person and asked participants to indicate the target

    persons intention to report the potential wrongdoing.

    Results

    Manipulation and Other Checks

    We first verified whether the study participants perceived

    our three manipulations as intended. To that end, they were

    asked in the last section of the research instrument:

    (i) whether the hotline was operated by the companys

    Department of Internal Audit or by an independent third-

    party company; (ii) whether the would-be whistleblower

    was the only employee aware of the wrongdoing; and (iii)

    whether the presumed wrongdoer was an entry-level

    employee or a supervisor.

    Of the 369 participants, 96 (26 %) failed at least one

    manipulation check: 52 participants failed the manipulation

    check for Reporting Channel, 38 failed the bystander

    manipulation check, and 47 failed the manipulation check

    for wrongdoers power status. Note that some participants

    failed more than one manipulation check. Data from the

    remaining 273 participants are used for testing the

    hypotheses.

    For each of the two reporting channel administration,

    participants were also asked to indicate using a 7-point

    likert scale (1 = Not at all confident, 7 = Extremely

    Confident), how confident they were that their anonymity

    would be guaranteed if they report wrongdoing. Consistent

    with our expectation, the reporting channel administered by

    an independent third party (5.47) was perceived to better

    protect a reporters anonymity than that maintained by the

    companys Department of Internal Audit (4.18) (t = 8.70,

    p = 0.000, 2-tailed).

    Tests of Hypotheses

    To test our hypotheses, a 2 (reporting channel) 9 2

    (bystander) 9 2 (power status) 9 2 (case) ANOVA was

    performed with whistleblowing intention as the dependent

    variable.1 Case is a within-subject factor. The results are

    presented in Table 1.

    H1 Our first hypothesis predicts that an individuals

    intention to report a fraudulent act using an anonymous

    reporting hotline will be greater if it is administered by a

    third-party than if it is maintained by the companys own

    Department of Internal Audit. Table 1 shows a significant

    (F (1, 271) = 7.5, p = 0.007) reporting Channel main

    effect on whistleblowing intention. As shown in Table 2,

    whistleblowing intention is higher under the externally

    administered reporting channel (58.4, s.d. = 17.3) than

    under the internally administered reporting channel (52.2,

    s.d. = 19.2). These results are consistent with H1.

    H2 Our second hypothesis predicts an interaction between

    reporting channel administration and the number of

    bystanders on whistleblowing intentions. As shown in

    Table 1, the predicted interaction between the two factors is

    significant (F (1,271) = 3.688, p = 0.056). To further

    investigate this finding and understand the nature of this

    interaction, we perform simple effects tests. The results are

    shown in Table 2. In the no bystander situation, the differ-

    ence in means of whistleblowing intention between internal

    reporting channel administration (54.9, s.d. = 16.5) and

    external reporting channel administration (57.5, s.d. = 17.6)

    is not statistically significant (t = -0.086, p = 0.196, one-

    tailed). However, in the bystander situation the mean of

    whistleblowing intention under the externally administered

    reporting channel (59.4, s.d. = 16.8) is significantly higher

    (t = -2.96, p = 0.0018, one-tailed) than that under the

    internally administered reporting channel (49.8, s.d. =

    21.2). This pattern of results is consistent with H2.

    H3 Our third hypothesis predicts an interaction between

    reporting channel administration and the power status of the

    wrongdoer on whistleblowing intentions. As shown in

    Table 1, the interaction between reporting channel admin-

    istration and wrongdoer power status is not significant

    (p = 0.91). The mean difference in whistleblowing intention

    between high and low wrongdoer power status is not sig-

    nificantly different between internal (6.99) and external

    1 We replicated the ANOVA omitting the wrongdoer power

    status 9 bystander interaction and the three-way interaction as these

    interactions are not hypothesized. The resulting hypothesis tests and

    significance levels are nearly identical to those in Table 1. In addition,

    we performed an ANCOVA with gender as a covariate. Gender is

    omitted from the analysis as it is not statistically significant.

    Whistleblowing Intentions of Lower-Level Employees 91

    123

  • (6.28) reporting channel administration. These results do not

    support H3.

    Supplemental Analyses

    Interaction of Bystander and Case

    Results of the test of within-subjects effects in Table 1

    indicate a significant interaction between case and

    bystander (F (1,269) = 9.76, p = 0.002). After conduct-

    ing simple effects tests (results are shown in Table 3),

    we find that the bystander effect on whistleblowing

    intention is significant in the qualification fee case (mean

    differenceno-bystanderbystander = 6.6, t = 2.33, p = 0.0102,

    one-tailed). Interestingly, the opposite effect is obtained in the

    fictitious supplier case (mean differenceno-bystanderbystander =

    -3.3, t = -1.29, p = 0.0978, one-tailed). We speculate on

    this unexpected finding in the Discussion section.

    Test of Mediation

    To provide insights into how our three manipulated factors

    (reporting channel administration, bystander, and wrong-

    doers power status) influence reporting intentions, we test

    the effect of three mediating variables theorized by Schultz

    et al. (1993). Their theoretical model depicts whistle-

    blowing intentions of individuals as a function of three

    assessments: perceived seriousness of questionable act,

    perceived personal cost of reporting, and perceived per-

    sonal responsibility for reporting.

    For each case scenario, our research instrument inclu-

    ded three questions, one each related to participants per-

    ceptions of the seriousness of the observed fraudulent act,

    the personal cost of reporting the fraud, and the perceived

    responsibility for reporting it. We, respectively, asked

    participants the extent to which they agreed that the

    potential whistleblower is likely (i) to judge that the fraud

    is totally unacceptable; (ii) to be extremely concerned that

    if he reports the potential wrongdoing, the fraudster might

    find out and try to harm him; and (iii) to count on someone

    else to report the potential wrongdoing. All responses were

    recorded on a 7-point likert scale (7 = completely agree,

    1 = completely disagree). To be consistent with the cod-

    ing of the seriousness and personal cost measures,

    responses to the responsibility measure (third item) was

    reverse-coded.

    To test the effect of the theorized mediators, we con-

    ducted path analysis using Mplus. Because Case is a sig-

    nificant factor based on the ANOVA results shown

    previously, we conduct separate path analyses for the fic-

    titious supplier case and the qualification fee case. The

    results of the analyses are presented in Fig. 1 and Table 4.

    In each case, the model fits the data well (fictitious supplier

    case: v2 = 12.80, CFI = 0.94, RMSEA = 0.06, SRMR =0.04; qualification fee case: v2 = 17.98, CFI = 0.94,RMSEA = 0.08, SRMR = 0.05). The mediating roles of

    perceived responsibility and perceived personal cost of

    reporting can be observed. Specifically, the presence of

    bystanders decreases whistleblowing intention through

    reduced perceived personal responsibility. Similarly, a

    Table 1 ANOVA for the effectof reporting channel

    administration, bystander, and

    wrongdoer power status on

    whistleblowing intention

    a Fictitious supplier case (case

    A) and qualification fee case

    (case B)

    Source SS df MS F p value

    Within-subjects effects

    Casea 4,768 1 4,768 15.27 0.000

    Case 9 reporting channel administration 30.98 1 30.98 0.099 0.753

    Case 9 wrongdoer power status 46.82 1 46.82 0.15 0.699

    Case 9 bystander 3048.9 1 3048.9 9.76 0.002

    Case 9 reporting channel administration 9 wrongdoer power

    status

    105.7 1 105.7 0.338 0.561

    Case 9 reporting channel administration 9 bystander 22.39 1 22.39 0.072 0.789

    Case 9 wrongdoer power status 9 bystander 32.1 1 32.1 0.103 0.749

    Case 9 wrongdoer power status 9 bystander 9 reporting

    channel administration

    30.9 1 30.9 0.099 0.753

    Between-subjects effects

    Reporting channel administration 4772.7 1 4772.7 7.5 0.007

    Wrongdoer power status 6073.1 1 6073.1 9.52 0.002

    Bystander 553.1 1 553.1 0.867 0.353

    Reporting channel administration 9 wrongdoer power status 8.38 1 8.38 0.013 0.909

    Reporting channel administration 9 bystander 2353.4 1 2353.4 3.688 0.056

    Wrongdoer power status 9 bystander 825 1 825 1.293 0.257

    Wrongdoer power status 9 bystander 9 reporting channel

    administration

    606.6 1 606.6 0.950 0.330

    92 J. Gao et al.

    123

  • powerful wrongdoer decreases employees whistleblowing

    intentions through increased perceived personal cost. Fol-

    lowing Preacher and Hayes (2008), multiple mediators

    analysis was performed for each case (see Tables 5, 6) to

    provide further evidence of mediation.2 The results are

    presented below.

    Bystander Effect

    We expected that the bystander effect would decrease

    participants whistleblowing intention through increased

    perceived personal cost and decreased perceived personal

    responsibility. In the fictitious supplier case (case A, see

    panel A in Table 5), we find that the total indirect effect is

    statistically significant (Z = -4.303, p \ 0.000), althougha significant total indirect effect is not a prerequisite for

    investigating specific indirect effects. Assuming normality,

    the results suggest that the separate indirect effect of per-

    ceived personal responsibility (Z = -4.422, p \ 0.000) issignificant. The normal theory tests for indirect effects

    compute the standard errors using the delta method, which

    assumes that the estimates of the indirect effect are nor-

    mally distributed. To relax this assumption, bootstrap test

    Table 2 Effect of reporting channel administration and bystander on whistleblowing intention

    Panel A: cell means

    Reporting channel administration Total

    Department of Internal Audit Third party

    Bystander

    No bystander 54.9 (16.5) N = 66 57.5 (17.6) N = 67 56.2 (17.0) N = 133

    Bystander 49.8 (21.2) N = 70 59.4 (16.8) N = 70 54.6 (19.7) N = 140

    Total 52.2 (19.2) N = 136 58.4 (17.3) N = 137

    Panel B: planned comparisons

    df t p value (one-tailed)

    Effect of reporting channel administration in no bystander situation 131 -0.86 0.1960

    Effect of reporting channel administration in bystander situation 138 -2.96 0.0018

    Effect of bystander in Department of Internal Audit reporting channel administration 134 1.57 0.0595

    Effect of bystander in a third party reporting channel administration 135 -0.65 0.2599

    Panel C: figure of means (interaction effect of reporting channel administration and bystander)

    Whistleblowing intention is equal to the average of whistleblowing intention in two cases

    2 Advantages to specifying and testing a single multiple mediation

    model in lieu of separate simple mediation models are: (1) it is

    possible to determine the significance of a specific mediator,

    conditional on the presence of other mediators in the model; (2) it

    is possible to include multiple mediators in one model to allow the

    test of competing theories; and (3) in a single multiple mediation

    model, the likelihood of parameter bias due to omitted variables is

    reduced (Preacher and Hayes 2008).

    Whistleblowing Intentions of Lower-Level Employees 93

    123

  • results are also reported. They indicate that only Perceived

    Personal Responsibility is a mediator, as its 95 % confi-

    dence interval does not contain zero (-9.150 to -3.718).

    However, perceived personal cost does not significantly

    mediate the influence of bystander on participants whis-

    tleblowing intention.

    Similarly, in the qualification fee case (case B), as shown

    in panel A in Table 6, the total indirect effect is statistically

    significant (Z = -4.454, p \ 0.000). Of the two proposedmediators, only Perceived Personal Responsibility is statis-

    tically significant (Z = -5.130, p \ 0.000). The resultsfrom the bootstrapping test demonstrate the same pattern. In

    particular, perceived personal responsibility is a significant

    mediator, as its 95 % confidence interval does not contain

    zero (-11.631 to -5.147). In summary, we find that per-

    ceived personal cost does not significantly mediate the effect

    of bystander on participants whistleblowing intention, but

    the mediation role of perceived personal responsibility is

    supported.

    Wrongdoer Power Status

    We expected that wrongdoer power status would influence

    participants whistleblowing intention through perceived

    personal cost and perceived seriousness. In the fictitious

    supplier case (case A), as shown in panel B in Table 5, the

    total indirect effect is statistically significant (Z = -1.946,

    p \ 0.052). The results also suggest that the separateindirect effect of perceived personal cost (Z = -2.167,

    p \ 0.030) is significant. Bootstrapping tests indicate thatonly perceived personal cost is a significant mediator, as its

    95 % confidence interval does not contain zero (-3.583 to

    -0.434).

    Similarly, in the qualification fee case (case B, see panel

    B in Table 6), the total indirect effect is statistically sig-

    nificant (Z = -2.011, p \ 0.044). Among the two expec-ted mediators, only perceived personal cost is statistically

    significant (Z = -2.700, p \ 0.007). Again, the resultsfrom the bootstrapping test indicate that perceived personal

    Table 3 Effect of bystander and case on whistleblowing intention

    Panel A: cell means

    Bystander Total

    No bystander Bystander

    Case

    Fictitious supplier case (case A) 56.7 (20.1) N = 133 60 (21.8) N = l40 58.4 (21.0) N = 273

    Qualification fee case (case B) 55.7 (22.4) N = 133 49.1 (16.8) N = l40 52.3 (23.5) N = 273

    Total 56.2 (17.0) N = 266 54.6 (19.7) N = 280

    Panel B: planned comparisons

    df t p value (one-tailed)

    Effect of bystander in fictitious supplier case (case A) 271 -1.29 0.0978

    Effect of bystander in qualification fee case (case B) 271 2.33 0.0102

    Panel C: figure of means (interaction effect of bystander and case)

    94 J. Gao et al.

    123

  • cost is a significant mediator, as its 95 % confidence

    interval does not contain zero (-4.224 to -0.877).

    In summary, we find that perceived seriousness does not

    significantly mediate the effect of Wrongdoer Power Status

    on participants whistleblowing intention. This is consis-

    tent with previous findings of (Kaplan and Whitecotton

    2001) and those of Schultz et al. (1993) among their US

    participants. However, our results provide evidence of the

    mediating role of perceived personal cost on the link

    between wrongdoer power status and participants whis-

    tleblowing intention.

    Reporting Channel Administration

    We next examine whether perceived personal cost medi-

    ates the effect of reporting channel administration on

    participants whistleblowing intention. In the fictitious

    supplier case (i.e. case A), perceived personal cost is not

    statistically significant (Z = -0.390, p = 0.697, see panel

    C in Table 5). The results from the bootstrapping test also

    indicate that perceived personal cost is not a significant

    mediator, as its 95 % confidence interval contains zero

    (-1.645 to 0.709). Therefore the mediation role of per-

    ceived personal cost is not supported in the fictitious sup-

    plier case (i.e. case A).

    The same procedure was repeated in the qualification fee

    case (i.e. case B). The results are similar to that of the

    fictitious supplier case (see panel C in Table 6). Specifi-

    cally, the indirect effect of perceived personal cost is not

    significant (p = 0.646). In addition, its 95 % confidence

    interval contains zero (-1.455 to 0.558). Thus, perceived

    personal cost does not significantly mediate the effect of

    reporting channel administration on participants whistle-

    blowing intention.

    Discussion

    As a result of recent corporate scandals, regulatory agen-

    cies, and the public have become more concerned about

    organizational wrongdoing and ways to encourage whis-

    tleblowing. This study examines how the reporting channel

    can influence whistleblowing intentions specifically among

    lower-tier employees. Since audit committees have wide

    discretion on how to administer their anonymous reporting

    channel, investigating the effect of different reporting

    channel administrations on employees whistleblowing

    0.151** (0.389**)

    0.073 (0.21**)-0.144** (-0.199**)

    0.026 (0.024)

    -0.013 (-0.027)

    -0.144** (-0.07)

    0.018 (-0.099*)0.19** (-0.001)

    0.326** (0.336**)

    -0.427** (-0.424**)

    0.143** (0.097**)

    0.179** (0.228**)

    Bystander

    Wrongdoer Power Status

    Reporting Channel

    Administration

    Perceived Personal Responsibility

    Perceived Seriousness

    Perceived Personal Cost

    WhistleblowingIntention

    Fig. 1 Results of path analyses. Fictitious supplier case (qualificationfee case). **Coefficients are significant at p \ .01; *coefficients aresignificant at p \ .05. Coding of variables Reporting channel

    administration: 1 = internal audit department, 2 = external third-

    party; bystander: 1 = no bystander, 2 = two other bystanders;

    wrongdoer power status: 1 = entry level employee, 2 = supervisor

    Table 4 Fit statistics for path analysis

    Case v2 p CFI(0.90.95)

    RMSEA

    (B0.08)

    SRMR

    (B0.06)

    Fictitious supplier

    case (case A)

    12.80 0.05 0.94 0.06 0.04

    Qualification fee

    case (case B)

    17.98 0.01 0.94 0.08 0.05

    Whistleblowing Intentions of Lower-Level Employees 95

    123

  • intentions is particularly important from the perspective of

    practice and academia.

    Our results suggest that lower-level employees whis-

    tleblowing intention is higher when the reporting channel is

    administered externally by a third-party instead of inter-

    nally. This is consistent with expectations that a reporting

    channel administered by a third-party may represent a

    stronger procedural safeguard of anonymity and avoids the

    appearance of impropriety than one administered internally

    (ACFE 2005; The Network 2006).

    Our study also finds a bystander effect that negatively

    influences whistleblowing intentions in the qualification

    fee case. In contrast, the presence of bystanders seems to

    have a positive impact on whistleblowing intention in the

    fictitious supplier case. We surmise that a likely explana-

    tion for this unexpected finding is an important difference

    in our manipulation of the presence of bystanders across

    the two cases. In the qualification fee case, because the

    bystanders see the would-be whistleblower leave the copy

    Table 5 Mediation analysis in fictitious supplier case (case A)

    A. The effect of bystander on whistleblowing intention

    a. Normal theory tests for indirect effects

    Effect SE Z p

    Total -6.275 1.458 -4.303 0.000

    Perceived personal cost -0.177 0.393 -0.451 0.652

    Perceived personal responsibility -6.097 1.379 -4.422 0.000

    b. Bias corrected and accelerated confidence intervals

    Lower Upper

    Total -9.506 -3.583

    Perceived personal cost -1.153 0.542

    Perceived personal responsibility -9.150 -3.718

    B. The effect of wrongdoer power status on whistleblowing intention

    a. Normal theory tests for indirect effects

    Effect SE Z p

    Total -1.486 0.764 -1.946 0.052

    Perceived personal cost -1.393 0.643 -2.167 0.030

    Perceived seriousness -0.093 0.447 -0.208 0.835

    b. Bias corrected and accelerated confidence intervals

    Lower Upper

    Total -3.384 -0.074

    Perceived personal cost -3.583 -0.434

    Perceived seriousness -1.240 0.837

    C. The effect of reporting channel administration on whistleblowing

    intention

    a. Normal theory tests for indirect effects

    Effect SE Z p

    Total -0.201 0.516 -0.390 0.697

    Perceived personal cost -0.201 0.516 -0.390 0.697

    b. Bias corrected and accelerated confidence intervals

    Lower Upper

    Total -1.645 0.709

    Perceived personal cost -1.645 0.709

    Table 6 Mediation analysis in qualification fee case (case B)

    A. The effect of bystander on whistleblowing intention

    a. Normal theory tests for indirect effects

    Effect SE Z p

    Total -7.642 1.716 -4.454 0.000

    Perceived personal cost 0.630 0.446 1.413 0.158

    Perceived personal responsibility -8.272 1.613 -5.130 0.000

    b. Bias corrected and accelerated confidence intervals

    Lower Upper

    Total -11.362 -4.127

    Perceived personal cost -0.021 1.857

    Perceived personal responsibility -11.631 -5.147

    B. The effect of wrongdoer power status on whistleblowing intention

    a. Normal theory tests for indirect effects

    Effect SE Z p

    Total -2.845 1.415 -2.011 0.044

    Perceived personal cost -2.273 0.843 -2.700 0.007

    Perceived seriousness -0.572 1.272 -0.450 0.653

    b. Bias corrected and accelerated confidence intervals

    Lower Upper

    Total -5.804 -0.371

    Perceived personal cost -4.224 -0.877

    Perceived seriousness -3.309 1.847

    C. The effect of reporting channel administration on whistleblowing

    intention

    a. Normal theory tests for indirect effects

    Effect SE Z p

    Total -0.190 0.415 -0.459 0.646

    Perceived personal cost -0.190 0.415 -0.459 0.646

    b. Bias corrected and accelerated confidence intervals

    Lower Upper

    Total -1.455 0.558

    Perceived personal cost -1.455 0.558

    96 J. Gao et al.

    123

  • room, he knows that they are aware of his knowledge about

    the fraudsters scheme. In contrast, in the fictitious supplier

    case, the potential whistleblower is completely anonymous

    and knows that the bystanders who overheard the fraudster

    are not aware of his knowledge of the fraud. Given this

    important difference in our manipulation of the bystander

    present condition, the apparent inconsistent findings can be

    explained by the concept of evaluation apprehension

    (Schwartz and Gottlieb 1976). According to this notion,

    would-be whistleblowers are concerned with others eval-

    uations. In the qualification fee case, the potential whistle-

    blower is not anonymous and would be concerned about the

    other bystanders evaluation of him if he reports the fraud.

    In the fictitious supplier case, however, the potential whis-

    tleblower is completely anonymous and does not have to be

    concerned about others evaluation. Moreover, identifying

    him as the whistleblower may be more difficult since there

    are at least two other bystanders who observed the fraud.

    The results also suggest that when the reporting channel

    is administered internally, the reporting intention of a

    potential whistleblower is significantly lower when there

    are other bystanders compared to when there are no other

    bystanders. In contrast, whistleblowing intentions do not

    differ significantly between the two conditions when the

    reporting channel is administered externally by a third-

    party. This provides further evidence of the difference in

    effectiveness between internally and externally adminis-

    tered reporting channels in promoting whistleblowing.

    In addition, we find that an individuals whistleblowing

    intention is significantly lower when the wrongdoer is a

    supervisor than when s/he is a coworker. This effect is,

    however, not significantly reduced by the use of an exter-

    nally administered reporting channel.

    The overall results of our path analyses are consistent

    with Schultz et al. (1993) model which depicts individuals

    intention of reporting questionable acts as a function of

    three assessments (i.e. perceived seriousness of question-

    able acts, perceived personal responsibility, and perceived

    personal cost). More importantly, our results indicate that

    presence of bystander and power status of wrongdoer

    impact employees whistleblowing intention through per-

    ceived personal responsibility and assessments of personal

    cost respectively.

    Before discussing the implications of our findings, we

    note some limitations. First, we do not specifically measure

    the extent of diffusion of responsibility, evaluation appre-

    hension, threat of retaliation, and anonymity. Thus, we are

    not able to directly assess the actual process by which

    whistleblowing intentions are formed. Second, our manip-

    ulation of the bystander effect across the two cases differs in

    anonymity in observing the wrongdoing. As a result, the

    influence of this aspect of anonymity and that of the

    bystander effect appear to be confounded in the second case.

    Third, our dependent measure is whistleblowing intentions,

    which are not always predictive of behavior. Fourth, our

    participants are business students. Whether the results can be

    generalized to other groups of individuals is unknown.

    Our research has implications for both practice and

    future research. With respect to practice, our results toge-

    ther with those of Kaplan et al. (2009) present an inter-

    esting dilemma. While our findings suggest that an

    externally administered reporting channel may promote

    whistleblowing among lower-tier employees, upper-level

    management may prefer an internally administered channel

    to protect the reputation of the company. The issue is,

    however, in practice; companies can balance the need to

    detect fraud and the importance of protecting the com-

    panys reputation.

    With respect to research, our study findings suggest

    several areas for future investigation. One possible exten-

    sion is the simultaneous examination of the external

    reporting channels high confidentiality, its use of trained

    professionals and its effective investigation process after-

    ward, which are all posited to have a positive influence on

    whistleblowing intentions (Kaplan et al. 2009). It may be

    the case, for instance, that employees perception of the

    quality of the professionals employed at the external

    reporting agency, along with their assessment of the quality

    of the post-report investigation procedures, are just as

    important as their belief in the confidentiality of reporting.

    A further interesting question relates to the reporting

    channel medium. Trevino et al. (1999) examine outsourced

    telephone hotlines. Future studies can examine whether the

    reporting mediumtelephone, email, web-based, etc.

    influences whistleblowing intentions.

    The results of our mediation tests also suggest that not

    all expected mediators are significant. Future research can

    investigate alternative processes by which the antecedent

    factors impact whistleblowing intentions.

    Finally, the apparent inconsistent findings between our

    two cases indicate that distinguishing anonymity in

    observing the wrongdoing from anonymity in reporting the

    wrongdoing is important. In particular, our results suggest

    that anonymity in observing the wrongdoing can impact the

    extent of evaluation apprehension which may be an

    important factor in influencing whistleblowing. Future

    research can be specifically designed to further examine

    this possibility.

    References

    Association of Certified Fraud Examiners (ACFE). (2005). Detecting

    and deterring fraud using hotlines. Continuing-education course .

    Austin, TX: ACFE.

    Whistleblowing Intentions of Lower-Level Employees 97

    123

  • Association of Certified Fraud Examiners (ACFE). (2008). Report to

    the nation on occupational fraud and abuse . Austin, TX: ACFE.

    Ayers, S., & Kaplan, S. E. (2005). Wrongdoing by consultants: An

    examination of employees reporting intentions. Journal of

    Business Ethics, 57, 121137.

    Brabeck, M. (1984). Ethical characteristics of whistle blowers.

    Journal of Research in Personality, 18, 4153.

    Chiu, R. K. (2003). Ethical judgment and whistleblowing intention:

    Examining the moderating role of locus of control. Journal of

    Business Ethics, 43, 6574.

    Chung, J., & Monroe, G. (2003). Exploring social desirability bias.

    Journal of Business Ethics, 44, 291302.

    Cohen, J., Pant, L., & Sharp, D. (1998). The effect of gender and

    academic discipline diversity on the ethical evaluations, ethical

    intentions and ethical orientation of potential public accounting

    recruits. Accounting Horizons, 12(3), 250270.

    Cohen, J., Pant, L., & Sharp, D. (2001). An examination of

    differences in ethical decision-making between Canadian busi-

    ness students and accounting professionals. Journal of Business

    Ethics, 30(4), 319336.

    Cortina, L. M., & Magley, V. J. (2003). Raising voice, risking

    retaliation: Events following interpersonal mistreatment in the

    workplace. Journal of Occupational Health Psychology, 8(4),

    247265.

    Dodd-Frank Wall Street Reform and Consumer Protection Act, Pub.

    Law No. 111-203, 124 Stat. 1376, 1612 (July 21, 2010).

    Dozier, J. B., & Miceli, M. P. (1985). Potential predictors of whistle-

    blowing: A prosocial behavior perspective. Academy of Man-

    agement Review, 10(4), 823836.

    Dworkin, T. M., & Baucus, M. S. (1998). Internal vs. external

    whistleblowers: A comparison of whistleblowering processes.

    Journal of Business Ethics, 17(12), 12811298.

    Ethics Resource Center. (2007). National Business Ethics Survey: An

    inside view of private sector ethics. Arlington, VA: Ethics

    Resource Center.

    Graham, J. W. (1986). Principled organizational dissent: A theoretical

    essay. Research in Organizational Behavior, 8, 152.

    Kaplan, S. E., Pany, K., Samuels, J. A., & Zhang, J. (2008). An

    examination of the association between gender and reporting

    intentions for fraudulent financial reporting. Journal of Business

    Ethics, 87, 1530.

    Kaplan, S. E., Pany, K., Samuels, J. A., & Zhang, J. (2009). An

    examination of the effects of procedural safeguards on intentions

    to anonymously report fraud. Auditing: A Journal of Practice

    and Theory, 28(2), 273288.

    Kaplan, S. E., Pope, K. R., & Samuels, J. A. (2010). The effect of

    social confrontation on individuals intentions to internally

    report fraud. Behavioral Research in Accounting, 22(2), 5167.

    Kaplan, S. E., & Whitecotton, S. M. (2001). An examination of

    auditors reporting intentions when another auditor is offered

    client employment. Auditing: A Journal of Practice and Theory

    (Spring), 20(1), 4564.

    Kaptein, M. (2011). From inaction to external whistleblowing: The

    influence of the ethical culture of organizations on employee

    responses to observed wrongdoing. Journal of Business Ethics,

    98, 513530.

    Keenan, J. P. (1995). Whistleblowing and the first-level manager:

    Determinants of feeling obliged to blow the whistle. Journal of

    Social Behavior and Personality, 10(3), 571584.

    King, G. (1999). The implications of an organizations structure on

    whistleblowing. Journal of Business Ethics, 20, 315326.

    Latane, D., & Darley, J. M. (1968). Bystander intervention in

    emergencies: Diffusion of responsibility. Journal of Personality

    and Social Psychology, 8(4), 377383.

    Latane, D., & Darley, J. M. (1970). The unresponsive bystander: Why

    doesnt he help me?. New York, NY: Appleton-Century-Crofts.

    Lee, J. Y., Heilmann, S. G., & Near, J. P. (2004). Blowing the whistle

    on sexual harassment: Test of a model of predictors and

    outcomes. Human Relations, 57(3), 297322.

    Lewis, D., & Kender, M. (2010). A survey of whistleblowing/

    confidential reporting procedures in the top 250 FTSE firms.

    London: SAI Global.

    Mesmer-Magnus, J., & Viswesvaran, C. (2005). Whistleblowing in

    organizations: An examination of correlates of whistleblowing

    intentions, actions and retaliation. Journal of Business Ethics,

    62, 277297.

    Miceli, M. P., Dozier, J. B., & Near, J. P. (1991a). Blowing the

    whistle on data fudging: A controlled field experiment. Journal

    of Applied Social Psychology, 21(4), 271295.

    Miceli, M. P., & Near, J. P. (1984). The relationships among beliefs,

    organizational position, and whistle-blowing status: A discrimi-

    nant analysis. Academy of Management Journal, 27(4), 687705.

    Miceli, M. P., & Near, J. P. (1992). Blowing the whistle: The

    organizational and legal implications for companies and

    employees. New York: Lexington.

    Miceli, M. P., Near, J. P., & Dworkin, T. M. (2008). Whistle-blowing

    in organizations. New York: Taylor and Francis Group, LLC.

    ISBN: 978-0-8058-5989-8.

    Miceli, M. P., Near, J. P., & Schwenk, C. R. (1991b). Who blows the

    whistle and why? Industrial and Labor Relations Review, 45(1),

    113130.

    Moberly, R. E. (2006). Sarbanes-Oxleys structural model to encour-

    age corporate whistleblowers. Brigham Young University Law

    Review, 2006(5), 11071180.

    Near, J. P., & Miceli, M. P. (1996). Whistle-blowing: Myth and

    reality. Journal of Management, 22(3), 507525.

    Perry, J. L., & Wise, L. R. (1990). The motivational bases of public

    service. Public Administration Review, 50(3), 367373.

    Piliavin, J. A., & Piliavin, I. M. (1972). Effect of blood on reactions to a

    victim. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 23, 353361.

    Preacher, K. J., & Hayes, A. F. (2008). Asymptotic and resampling

    strategies for assessing and comparing indirect effects in multiple

    mediator models. Behavior Research Methods, 40, 879891.

    PricewaterhouseCoopers. (2004). The emerging role of internal audit

    in mitigating fraud and reputation risks. New York, NY:

    PricewaterhouseCoopers.

    Read, W. J., & Rama, D. V. (2003). Whistle-blowing to internal

    auditors. Managerial Auditing Journal, 18, 354362.

    Rehg, M. T., Near, J. P., Miceli, M. P., & Van Scotter, J. R. (2008).

    Antecedents and outcomes of retaliation against whistleblowers:

    Gender difference and power relationships. Organization Sci-

    ence, 19(2), 221240.

    Robinson, S. N., Robertson, J. C., & Curtis, M. B. (2012). The effects

    of contextual and wrongdoing attributes on organizational

    employees whistleblowing intentions following fraud. Journal

    of Business Ethics, 106, 213227.

    Rothwell, G. R., & Baldwin, J. N. (2006). Ethical climates and

    contextual predictors of whistle-blowing. Review of Public

    Personnel Administration, 26(3), 216244.

    Sarbanes-Oxley Act (Corporate and Auditing Accountability,

    Responsibility, and Transparency Act of 2002). (2002). PL

    107-204, 116 Stat 745.

    Schultz, J. J., Jr, Johnson, D. A., Morris, D., & Dyrnes, S. (1993). An

    investigation of the reporting of questionable acts in an

    international setting. Journal of Accounting Research (Supple-

    ment), 31, 75103.

    Schwartz, S. H., & Gottlieb, A. (1976). Bystander reactions to a

    violent theft: Crime in Jerusalem. Journal of Personality and

    Social Psychology, 34(6), 11881199.

    Schwartz, S. H., & Gottlieb, A. (1980). Bystander anonymity and

    reactions to emergencies. Journal of Personality and Social

    Psychology, 39(3), 418430.

    98 J. Gao et al.

    123

  • Seifert, D. L., Sweeney, J. T., Joireman, J., & Thornton, J. M. (2010).

    The influence of organizational justice on accountant whistle-

    blowing. Accounting, Organizations & Society, 15, 707717.

    Sims, R. L., & Keenan, J. P. (1998). Predictors of external

    whistleblowing: Organizational and intrapersonal variables.

    Journal of Business Ethics, 17, 411421.

    The Network, Inc. (The Network). (2006, 2010). Corporate Gover-

    nance and Compliance Hotline Benchmarking Report. Norcross,

    GA: The Network.

    Trevino, L. K., Weaver, G. R., Gibson, D. G., & Toffler, B. L. (1999).

    Managing ethics and legal compliance: What works and what

    hurts. California Management Review, 41(2), 131151.

    Vandekerckhove, W., & Lewis, D. (2012). The content of whistle-

    blowing procedures: A critical review of recent official guide-

    lines. Journal of Business Ethics, 108, 253264.

    Weaver, G. R., & Trevino, L. K. (2001). The role of human resources

    in ethics/compliance management: A fairness perspective.

    Human Resource Management Review, 11, 113134.

    Weaver, G. R., Trevino, L. K., & Cochran, P. L. (1999). Corporate

    ethics programs as control systems: Influences of executive

    commitment and environmental factors. Academy of Manage-

    ment Journal, 42, 4157.

    Whistleblowing Intentions of Lower-Level Employees 99

    123

  • Copyright of Journal of Business Ethics is the property of Springer Science & Business MediaB.V. and its content may not be copied or emailed to multiple sites or posted to a listservwithout the copyright holder's express written permission. However, users may print,download, or email articles for individual use.

    Whistleblowing Intentions of Lower-Level Employees: The Effect of Reporting Channel, Bystanders, and Wrongdoer Power StatusAbstractIntroductionBackground and HypothesesWhistleblowingReporting ChannelBystander EffectWrongdoer Power Status

    MethodsDesignParticipantsTaskIndependent VariablesReporting Channel AdministrationPresence of BystandersWrongdoer Power Status

    Dependent Variable

    ResultsManipulation and Other ChecksTests of HypothesesSupplemental AnalysesInteraction of Bystander and CaseTest of Mediation

    Bystander EffectWrongdoer Power StatusReporting Channel Administration

    DiscussionReferences