7
Two Alternative Job Stress Models and the Risk of Coronary Heart Disease Hans Bosma, PhD, Richard Peter, PhD, Johannes Siegrist, PhD, and Michael Marmot FFPHM Introduction ....t s-..h .. .d. ... ,: ; : :68 Amerian J of XP There is a cumulating body of evidence on the association between psychosocial hazards at work and the physical health of workers. The job stress model most fre- quently used is that of job strain.'3 This model postulates that job strain results from the joint effects of high job demands and low job control. Recent publications increasingly underscore the special impor- tance of low job control for a range of out- comes, including cardiovascular disease and sickness absence.7 Another recently devel- oped job stress model is that of effort- reward imbalance.) In this model, there is an explicit emphasis on individual attributes- .^.: that is, coping characteristics of high intrin- sic effort, as defined by the concept of "need for control."9 Extrinsic efforts, as defined by a high workload, are also specified. On the other side, this model also takes three differ- ent sources of rewards into account: money, esteem, and occupational status control (promotion prospects, job security). The focus is on a negative trade-off between experienced "costs" and "gains" at work rather than on specific job task characteris- tics, as in the job strain model. In a prospective study among German blue-collar men, poor promotion prospects and job insecurity (low rewards) in men having a high workload and a high need for control (high efforts) predicted new cardio- .... vascular events.'0 Thus, worksite-specific stressful experience has been linked with stressful experience related to broader socioeconomic influences of the labor mar- ket and income distribution. While the two job stress models clearly differ, there is also some overlap with respect to the dimen- sions of extrinsic effort (job demands) and esteem reward (social support at work)." Given both the promising results and the different analytical perspectives of the two job stress models, more information is greatly needed on how they compare in their association with coronary heart dis- ease, especially if the potentially overlap- ping dimensions (i.e., extrinsic effort and esteem reward) are excluded from the com- parative analysis. The longitudinal phase of the Whitehall II study of British civil ser- vants12 allowed us to examine whether cru- cial components of effort-reward imbalance and job strain are independently associated with new reports of coronary heart disease. Subjects and Methods Study Population The Whitehall IL study was set up to investigate the degree and causes of the social gradient in morbidity; to study work characteristics, social support, and addi- tional factors related to the gradient in mor- tality; and, importantly, to include women. In the study, a new cohort of civil servants was established between 1985 and 1988 (phase 1). All male and female civil ser- vants between 35 and 55 years of age in 20 London-based civil service departments were sent an introductory letter and screen- ing questionnaire and were offered a screening examination for cardiovascular Hans Bosma and Michael Marmot are with the International Centre for Health and Society, Department of Epidemiology and Public Health, University College London Medical School, Lon- don, England. Richard Peter and Johannes Siegrist are with the Institute of Medical Sociology, Uni- versity of Dusseldorf, Dusseldorf, Germany. Requests for reprints should be sent to Hans Bosma, PhD, Department of Epidemiology and Public Health, University College London Medical School, 1-19 Torrington P1, London, WC1E 6BT, England. This paper was accepted March 14, 1997. January 1998, Vol. 88, No. 1 .. .. ... ..., .: .: .: .. *;111...s * . a........ *I;: ..c...i ..ci... ..... a...., 01 :. .:. *:.

Two Alternative Job Stress Models And

Embed Size (px)

DESCRIPTION

psychology

Citation preview

  • Two Alternative Job Stress Models andthe Risk of Coronary Heart Disease

    Hans Bosma, PhD, Richard Peter, PhD, Johannes Siegrist, PhD,and Michael Marmot FFPHM

    Introduction

    ....ts-..h

    . . .d.... ,: ; ::68 Amerian J of X P

    There is a cumulating body of evidenceon the association between psychosocialhazards at work and the physical health ofworkers. The job stress model most fre-quently used is that of job strain.'3 Thismodel postulates that job strain results fromthe joint effects of high job demands andlow job control. Recent publicationsincreasingly underscore the special impor-tance of low job control for a range of out-comes, including cardiovascular disease andsickness absence.7 Another recently devel-oped job stress model is that of effort-reward imbalance.) In this model, there is anexplicit emphasis on individual attributes-

    .^.: that is, coping characteristics of high intrin-sic effort, as defined by the concept of "needfor control."9 Extrinsic efforts, as defined bya high workload, are also specified. On theother side, this model also takes three differ-ent sources of rewards into account: money,esteem, and occupational status control(promotion prospects, job security). Thefocus is on a negative trade-off betweenexperienced "costs" and "gains" at workrather than on specific job task characteris-tics, as in the job strain model.

    In a prospective study among Germanblue-collar men, poor promotion prospectsand job insecurity (low rewards) in menhaving a high workload and a high need forcontrol (high efforts) predicted new cardio-

    .... vascular events.'0 Thus, worksite-specificstressful experience has been linked withstressful experience related to broadersocioeconomic influences of the labor mar-ket and income distribution. While the twojob stress models clearly differ, there is alsosome overlap with respect to the dimen-sions of extrinsic effort (job demands) andesteem reward (social support at work)."

    Given both the promising results andthe different analytical perspectives of the

    two job stress models, more information isgreatly needed on how they compare intheir association with coronary heart dis-ease, especially if the potentially overlap-ping dimensions (i.e., extrinsic effort andesteem reward) are excluded from the com-parative analysis. The longitudinal phase ofthe Whitehall II study of British civil ser-vants12 allowed us to examine whether cru-cial components of effort-reward imbalanceand job strain are independently associatedwith new reports of coronary heart disease.

    Subjects and MethodsStudy Population

    The Whitehall IL study was set up toinvestigate the degree and causes of thesocial gradient in morbidity; to study workcharacteristics, social support, and addi-tional factors related to the gradient in mor-tality; and, importantly, to include women.In the study, a new cohort of civil servantswas established between 1985 and 1988(phase 1). All male and female civil ser-vants between 35 and 55 years of age in 20London-based civil service departmentswere sent an introductory letter and screen-ing questionnaire and were offered ascreening examination for cardiovascular

    Hans Bosma and Michael Marmot are with theInternational Centre for Health and Society,Department of Epidemiology and Public Health,University College London Medical School, Lon-don, England. Richard Peter and Johannes Siegristare with the Institute of Medical Sociology, Uni-versity of Dusseldorf, Dusseldorf, Germany.

    Requests for reprints should be sent to HansBosma, PhD, Department of Epidemiology andPublic Health, University College London MedicalSchool, 1-19 Torrington P1, London, WC1E 6BT,England.

    This paper was accepted March 14, 1997.

    January 1998, Vol. 88, No. 1

    .. .. ...

    ...,.: .: .:

    ..

    *;111...s* . a........

    *I;:..c...i..ci...

    ..... a....,

    01:.

    .:.

    *:.

  • Job Stress and Coronary Heart Disease

    diseases. The response rate was 73% andprobably would have been higher, hadabout 4% of the civil servants on the listsprovided by the civil service not movedbefore the study and thus become ineligiblefor inclusion. In total, 10 308 civil servantswere examined: 6895 men (67%) and 3413women (33%). The participants wereapproached again in 1989/1990 (phase 2:postal questionnaire) and in 1991/1993(phase 3: postal questionnaire and screeningexamination). The participation rates atthese two phases were 79% and 83%,respectively; 7372 subjects (72%) partici-pated in all three phases and 9302 subjects(90%) participated in either phase 2 orphase 3. The length of follow-up was 5.3years on average, with a range of 3.7 to 7.6years. Full details of the screening examina-tions are reported elsewhere. 12,13

    Coronary Heart Disease

    Three indicators of coronary heart dis-ease were analyzed: angina pectoris,doctor-diagnosed ischemia, or either ofthese outcomes. Angina pectoris was meas-ured by the Rose questionnaire and definedas pain located over the sternum or in boththe left chest and left arm that is precipi-tated by exertion, causes the person to stop,and goes away in 10 minutes or less.'4 Doc-tor-diagnosed ischemia depended onwhether the subject reported that a generalpractitioner or hospital doctor ever sus-pected or confirmed a heart attack or anginapectoris.

    The outcomes were assessed at allthree phases. Excluding 230 men and 144women with any coronary heart disease atphase 1, there were 239 men and 174women reporting any new coronary heartdisease outcome at phases 2 and 3.

    Effort-Reward Imbalance

    As there was no original measurementof effort-reward imbalance at phase 1,proxy measures (available from theauthors) had to be constructed for the cru-cial components of the model. In the origi-nal measurement, adverse personal charac-teristics of high need for control (highintrinsic effort) were assessed using a well-tested scale that contains 29 items.9 Theseitems measure characteristics such as com-petitiveness, latent hostility, a high need forapproval, and an excessive work commit-ment. On the basis of the highest face valid-ity compared with the original "need forcontrol" items, 10 items were selected fromthe Framingham type A questionnaire'5 andthe Cook-Medley hostility questionnaire'6

    for assessment at phase 1. The dimensional-ity of these 10 items was confirmed withfactor analysis. The three identifiedscales-competitiveness (3 items), work-related overcommitment (4 items), and hos-tility (3 items)-were considered validproxy measures of the original "need forcontrol" scale.

    On the reward side, information wasavailable on core aspects of occupationalstatus control, especially on perceived poorpromotion prospects (1 item) and-on objec-tively determined restricted occupationalmobility ("poor promotion prospects" and"blocked career"). A blocked career wasdetermined by a lower than expected currentemployment grade level. The expectationwas based on the mean current grade levelfor the different grade levels in which sub-jects started their careers. Job insecurity, afurther important aspect of occupational sta-tus control, was not assessed at phase 1because most civil servants were not (yet)threatened by unemployment (see "Discus-sion'). Information on financial rewards wasnot available. Thus, the effort-reward imbal-ance in our study reflected a mismatchbetween personal characteristics (highefforts) and occupational career characteris-tics (low rewards).

    Each effort scale was constructed bysummning the scores on the individulal itemsand dichotomizing the resulting score(O = bottom two tertiles and 1= highest ter-tile). The highest tertile indicated highefforts. Poor promotion prospects were alsodichotomized using the most adverse tertileto indicate low rewards. Blocked career wasalready a dichotomous variable (lowrewards). The indicators were used to createthe effort-reward imbalance indicator, whichhad three categories: 1= neither high effortsnor low rewards; 2 = either high efforts orlow rewards; and 3 = both high efforts andlow rewards. This indicator reflects the theo-retically postulated cumulative effect of highefforts (adverse personal characteristics) andlow rewards (adverse occupational career).

    Because the hostility questionnairewas included in the questionnaire at a laterstage of the first phase, 3790 subjects (37%)had no hostility score. However, these sub-jects did not differ from the other group intheir risk of newly reported coronary heartdisease. Furthermore, using an imputationmethod did not result in substantially differ-ent odds ratios (ORs) for the effort-rewardimbalance indicator.

    Job Strain

    At all three phases, the subjects weregiven a questionnaire asking them about the

    central components of the job strain model:high job demands, low job control, and lowwork support. The items, which were basedon the job strain questionnaire,217, 8 can befound elsewhere.5 Furthermore, jobs wereexternally assessed at phase 1: in 18 of 20departments, 140 well-informed personnelmanagers provided information on workpace ("How often does the job involveworking very fast?") and on the level ofcontrol ("How often does the job permitcomplete discretion and independence indetermining how, and when, the work is tobe done?"). Detailed information wasobtained on individual jobs because 5766different jobs were rated and 8838 subjectsoccupied them. Both self-reported andexternally assessed work characteristicswere grouped into tertiles.

    Job strain was modeled by assigningsubjects who simultaneously scored abovethe median of job demands and below themedian ofjob control to the group with jobstrain. All others were assigned to the "nojob strain" category. In an alternative strat-egy, interaction terms of job demands andjob control were introduced into a logisticregression model with the main compo-nents. This was done separately for the cat-egorical and continuous work scales.

    Statistical Analysis

    Logistic regression analyses were usedto determine whether effort-reward imbal-ance and job strain at phase 1 were relatedto new reports of coronary heart diseaseduring follow-up (phases 2 and 3). In allanalyses, coronary heart disease cases atphase 1 were excluded and age and lengthof follow-up were controlled for. In a sub-sequent analysis, effort-reward imbalanceand job strain were controlled for eachother. This model was successively adjustedfor employment grade level, negative affec-tivity, and classical coronary risk factors,including smoking (never, stopped, orsmoked I to 10, 11 to 20, or 21 or more cig-arettes per day), cholesterol (mmol/L),hypertension according to diastolic(> 95 mm Hg) or systolic (> 160 mm Hg)blood pressure or drug treatment for hyper-tension, and body mass index (kg/M2),which were all assessed at phase 1. Nega-tive affectivity, which is the disposition torespond negatively to questionnaires andwhich may inflate correlations betweenself-reported work characteristics and self-reported disease,'9-22 was measured withthe negative affect subscale of the affectbalance scale.23 24 Ordinal variables, such asemployment grade level, were representedby dummy indicators in the analyses.

    American Journal of Public Health 69January 1998, Vol. 88, No. I

  • Bosma et al.

    Results

    Table 1 shows that women reportedhigh effort and low reward conditions moreoften than men (48% and 41%, respec-tively). Moreover, men and women in loweremployment grades reported effort-rewardimbalance more often than subjects inhigher grades. This was mainly owing to ahigher prevalence of hostility, a blockedcareer, and poor promotion prospects in thelower grades; work-related overcommitmentand competitiveness were more prevalent inthe higher grades. High job strain was alsosomewhat more prevalent among womenthan among men (self-reported job strain:17% and 15%, respectively).

    The lowest prevalence ofjob strain wasfound in the highest employment grades.

    This was primarily owing to the strong asso-ciation between high job control and highemployment grade level. In the highestgrade level, 60% reported high job controlcompared with 5% in the lowest grade. Jobdemands were also highest in the highergrades, reported by 45% compared with10% in the lowest grade level. Similarresults were found with the external assess-ments.

    Table 2 shows that effort-reward imbal-ance is associated with elevated risks of sub-sequent coronary heart disease. The risk ofcoronary heart disease for men and womenwho have both high efforts and low rewardsis about three times as high as that for sub-jects with low efforts and high rewards(ORs varied from 2.59 to 3.63). Job strainwas not consistently related to new coronary

    heart disease reports; only the associationbetween self-reported job strain and anycoronary heart disease outcome in men wasstatistically significant (OR = 1.45). Theinteraction terms of job demands and jobcontrol were not statistically significant andnot consistently in the expected direction;this is primarily because (strong) adverseeffects of high job demands were absent.Nor did low work support affect the coro-nary heart disease outcomes. These negativefindings did not change when additionalinformation from phase 2 was used. Lowjob control was, however, consistentlyrelated to new coronary heart diseasereports. Because low job control, as meas-ured on two occasions (phases 1 and 2), wasearlier found to have cumulative effects onnew coronary heart disease at phase 3,5 sub-sequent analyses will use the mean ofphases 1 and 2 job control to predict newcoronary heart disease outcomes at phase 3.(Coronary heart disease cases at phases 1and 2 were excluded in these analyses.)

    The interaction terms between sex andeffort-reward imbalance or job control werenot statistically significant, so all furtheranalyses were based on the total sample andsex was controlled for in each logisticregression model. Table 3 shows that botheffort-reward imbalance and low job con-trol have strong and significant associationswith the coronary heart disease outcomes inthe total sample. Odds ratios of any coro-nary heart disease outcome were 3.14, 2.04,and 1.57 for effort-reward imbalance (highefforts and low rewards), self-reported lowjob control, and extemally assessed low jobcontrol, respectively. The associationbetween job control and doctor-diagnosedischemia was somewhat smaller.

    70 American Joumnal of Public Health

    TABLE 1-Number and Percentage of Men and Women Reporting Effort-Reward Imbalance and Job Strain at Phase 1, by Employment GradeLevel"

    Effort-Reward Job Strain: Job Strain: ExternalNumber Imbalance (%) Self-Report (%) Assessment(%)

    Men 6895 41.2 14.7 11.9Grade level

    High 2647 33.3 10.5 7.5Intermediate 3607 44.4 17.6 13.4Low 641 55.6 15.8 20.3

    Women 3413 48.1 17.2 18.8Grade level

    High 381 36.9 12.4 9.2Intermediate 1336 37.5 20.4 15.7Low 1696 60.8 15.7 23.4

    aEmployment grades were grouped into three levels: unified grades 1-7 (administrators inWhitehall I), executive officers, and clerical and office support grades. Professionalgrades were classified with the equivalent administrative or executive grade.

    TABLE 2-Odds Ratios (ORs)8 and 95% Confidence Intervals (Cis) of New Coronary Heart Disease Reports, by Effort-RewardImbalance and Job Strain at Phase I

    Men WomenAny Coronary Any Coronary

    Angina Diagnosed Heart Disease Angina Diagnosed Heart DiseasePectoris Ischemia Outcome Pectoris lschemia Outcome

    OR (95% Cl) OR (95% Cl) OR (95% Cl) OR (95% Cl) OR (95% Cl) OR (95% Cl)Effort-reward imbalanceLow efforts and high rewards 1.00 1.00 1.00 1.00 1.00 1.00High efforts or low rewards 2.13 (0.97, 4.70) 2.13 (0.75, 6.03) 2.12 (1.05, 4.27) 2.08 (0.63, 6.84) 1.45 (0.18, 11.6) 2.41 (0.74, 7.91)High efforts and low rewards 2.59 (1.17, 5.73) 3.63 (1.30,10.2) 2.98 (1.48, 5.99) 3.14 (0.96,10.3) 3.10 (0.40, 23.8) 3.59 (1.10, 11.7)

    Number (events) 3751 (129) 3910 (97) 3724 (178) 1589 (110) 1687 (30) 1588 (125)Job strain (self-reported) 1.40 (0.93, 2.10) 1.16 (0.70,1.94) 1.45 (1.03, 2.06) 1.01 (0.65,1.58) 1.89 (0.90, 3.99) 1.14 (0.76,1.72)Number (events) 4817 (168) 5027 (118) 4784 (227) 2116 (141) 2247 (35) 2116 (160)Job strain (external assessment) 0.91 (0.53,1.57) 1.18 (0.65, 2.14) 1.03 (0.66,1.61) 1.27(0.81,1.98) 0.97 (0.40, 2.39) 1.22 (0.80,1.86)Number (events) 4169 (149) 4351 (106) 4143 (204) 1839 (122) 1958 (32) 1841 (140)aAdjusted for age and length of period between phases 1 and 3; coronary heart disease cases at phase 1 were excluded; new coronary heartdisease reports at phases 2 or 3 were the outcome.

    January 1998, Vol. 88, No. I

  • Job Stress and Coronary Heart Disease

    Subjects with low job control reportedeffort-reward imbalance conditions moreoften than subjects with high job control(51% and 36%, respectively). Despite thisassociation, both characteristics were par-tially independently associated with newreports of any coronary heart disease out-come (Table 4). The odds ratios only mar-ginally decreased when job control andeffort-reward imbalance were simultane-ously adjusted for (model 2). Additionaladjustments for employment grade level,negative affectivity, and coronary risk fac-tors only marginally affected the odds ratiosfor the work characteristics. When gradewas controlled for, the odds ratios for self-reported low job control increased strik-ingly (from 2.04 to 2.44). This was causednot by any unexpected direction of theunderlying associations but by the combi-nation of a relatively small number ofevents (115) with strong associationsbetween low job control, low grade, andeffort-reward imbalance. In the fullyadjusted model, subjects with high effort-low reward conditions had more than twicethe risk of any new coronary heart diseaseoutcome compared with their counterpartswithout such conditions (OR = 2.15). Lowjob control had independent effects on newcoronary heart disease reports because theodds ratios in the fully adjusted model were2.38 and 1.56 for self-reported low job con-

    trol and extemally assessed low job control,respectively.

    DiscussionThe findings in the Whitehall II study

    further support the predictive validity ofcomponents of two alternative job stressmodels-the effort-reward imbalancemodel and the job strain model-for coro-nary heart disease morbidity. In the lattermodel, only low job control was related tonew reports of coronary heart disease.Hence, subjects experiencing high effortand low reward conditions and subjectswith low job control had higher risks ofnew coronary heart disease than their coun-terparts in less adverse psychosocial workenvironments.

    Effort-Reward Imbalance

    Subjects experiencing a mismatchbetween their personal characteristics andcharacteristics of their occupational careerhad strongly elevated risks of subsequentcoronary heart disease. More specifically,competitive, hostile, and overcommittedsubjects experiencing poor promotionprospects and blocked careers had the high-est risks. The association between thiseffort-reward imbalance indicator and the

    coronary heart disease outcomes was pres-ent after adjustment for employment gradelevel, negative affectivity, and coronary riskfactors and was not significantly different inmen and women. These findings corroboratethe results found in male German blue-collar and middle-management popu-lations.8 In a previous paper based on theWhitehall II study, Ferrie and colleaguesfound adverse health effects from anticipa-tion ofjob loss orjob change.25 Their resultsmay be interpreted as providing further evi-dence for the importance of "status control"(job insecurity, poor promotion prospects) inthe effort-reward imbalance model.

    Low Job Control

    Low job control also increased therisks of coronary heart disease. However,neither high job demands nor low socialsupport nor the interactions between workcharacteristics (job strain) were related tothe coronary heart disease outcomes. It ispossible that specific characteristics of oursample of white-collar workers contributedto the negative findings for high jobdemands and high job strain. High jobdemands were more common in the higheremployment grade levels and were posi-tively associated with high job control,resulting in a relatively small number ofhigh strain jobs. Our finding corresponds tothat in the review by Schnall and col-leagues, in which they concluded that 17 of25 studies that examined main effects foundsignificant associations between job controland cardiovascular outcome, whereas only8 of 23 studies found significant associa-tions with job demands.3

    The importance of (job) control is fur-ther elaborated upon by several investiga-tors.2-28 More details on the associationbetween self-reported and externallyassessed low job control and coronary heartdisease can be found in another Whitehall Hpaper.5 Other reports based on the White-hall II study have shown the wider predic-tive validity of low job control as it relatesto sickness absence,7 fibrinogen,29 and psy-chiatric disorder.24 These results underscorethe view expressed by Johnson and col-leagues that in the job strain model, it iscontrol over the work process rather thanhigh job demands or job strain that increas-ingly emerges as the main critical compo-nent of a healthy work environment.6

    Alternative Job Stress Models

    To our knowledge, this is the firstreport that compares components of theeffort-reward imbalance model and the job

    American Journal of Public Health 71

    TABLE 3-Odds Ratios (ORs)a and 95% Confidence Intervals (Cis) of NewCoronary Heart Disease Reports by Effort-Reward Imbalance atPhase 1, Self-Reported Job Control (Mean Phases I and 2) andExternally Assessed Job Control at Phase I In the Total Sample

    Angina Diagnosed Any Coronary HeartPectoris Ischemia Disease Outcome

    OR (95% CI) OR (95% Cl) OR (95% Cl)Effort-reward imbalancebLow efforts and high rewards 1.00 1.00 1.00High efforts or low rewards 2.06 (1.07, 3.98) 2.00 (0.79, 5.06) 2.17 (1.19, 3.95)High efforts and low rewards 2.78 (1.44, 5.37) 3.55 (1.42,8.90) 3.14 (1.72, 5.71)

    Number (events) 5340 (239) 5597 (127) 5312 (303)Self-reported job contror

    High job control 1.00 1.00 1.00Intermediate job control 1.36 (0.83, 2.23) 1.39 (0.79, 2.45) 1.61 (1.04, 2.48)Low job control 2.09 (1.29,3.37) 1 49 (0.81, 2.74) 2.04 (1.32, 3.16)

    Number (events) 6565 (132) 6982 (73) 6489 (163)Extemally assessed job controlb

    High job control 1.00 1.00 1.00Intermediate job control 1.28 (0.91, 1.81) 1.08 (0.68,1.71) 1.26 (0.92,1.71)Low job control 1.47 (1.77, 2.02) 1.38 (0.74, 2.09) 1.57 (1.17, 2.08)

    Number (events) 6003 (271) 6303 (137) 5979(343)

    aAdjusted for age, sex, and length of period between phases 1 and 3.bCoronary heart disease cases at phase 1 were excluded; new coronary heart diseasereports at phase 2 or phase 3 were the outcome.

    cMean phases 1 and 2 job control; coronary heart disease cases at phases 1 and 2 wereexcluded; new coronary heart disease reports at phase 3 were the outcome.

    January 1998, Vol. 88, No. I

  • Bosma et al.

    strain model. Effort-reward imbalance andlow job control were independently relatedto the coronary heart disease outcomes.When these were controlled for one anotherand for other potential confounders, theodds ratio for effort-reward imbalance was2.15 whereas those for low job control were2.38 and 1.56 for self-reported and exter-nally assessed job control, respectively.This suggests that the further refinement ofjob stress theories may benefit from inte-grating theories on control-related personalattributes and theories on actual controlover environmental factors, such as dailytasks (job control) and occupational career(status control). The cumulative adversehealth impact of low job control and effort-reward imbalance indicates that both jobstress factors provide supplementary infor-mation on relevant stressors in the psy-chosocial work environment.

    Conceptual Overlap between JobStress Models

    Possible conceptual overlap betweenthe effort-reward imbalance model andother models needs to be explored in furtherstudies. First, job demands and work sup-port from the job strain model closely

    resemble extrinsic efforts and esteemrewards from the effort-reward imbalancemodel. However, the effort-reward imbal-ance model attaches much importance tothe perception and appraisal of adversework conditions, whereas the job strainmodel focuses attention primarily on the"objective" psychosocial work environment(we used both self-reported and objectivemeasures ofjob control).2

    Second, there might be overlap withhostility, which on its own has been shownto be strongly related to future coronaryheart disease.30 Although the odds ratios ofour imbalance indicator did not change sub-stantially when the total 38-item hostilityscale was controlled for, the theoretical andempirical contribution of hostility to effort-reward imbalance is worth some furtherelaboration. Third, it has also been sug-gested that the adverse personal characteris-tics (e.g., competitiveness or hostility) inthe effort-reward imbalance model could bethe result of low job control and high jobdemands. If this were the case, controllingeffort-reward imbalance for job controlshould have resulted in decreased oddsratios of the former. However, the oddsratio for high efforts and low rewardshardly decreased (from 2.68 to 2.54). More-

    over, in a 6.5-year prospective study onblue-collar workers, an impressive degreeof stability over time was reported for the"need for control" measures.9'10 Fourth,proponents of the job strain model haveoften referred to the need to expand the jobstrain model by including information onjob insecurity (macro-level decision lati-tude).2 In further studies, the links betweenlow job control (job strain model) and lowoccupational status control (effort-rewardimbalance model) should be elaboratedupon and tested more vigorously.

    Further Considerations

    Despite our promising findings, somefurther considerations need to be taken intoaccount. First, given the known variability inangina reporting,31 which has been repli-cated in our data set, using new reports ofangina pectoris as an indicator of incidentcoronary pathology may be problematic.One might speculate that the underlyingcondition has not altered but that the ten-dency to report it has changed. A new reportof a doctor diagnosis is likely to be a betterindicator of new disease, although other fac-tors may also influence both recall of diag-nosis or access to medical care. Preliminary

    72 American Joumal of Public Health

    TABLE 4-Odds Ratios (ORs) and 95% Confidence Intervals (Cis) of Any New Coronary Heart Disease Outcome by Effort-Reward Imbalance at Phase 1, Self-Reported Job Control (Mean Phases I and 2), and Externally Assessed JobControl at Phase 1 in the Total Sample, Adjusted for Age, Sex, and Length of Period between Phases I and 3(Model 1), Additionally Adjusted for Other Work Characteristic (Model 2), and Model 2 Adjusted for EmploymentGrade Level, Negative Affectivity, and Coronary Risk Factors.

    Model 2 Separately Adjusted forModel la Model 2b Employment Negative Coronary Fully

    OR (95% CI) OR (95% CI) Grade Level Affectivity Risk Factors AdjustedEffort-reward imbalancecLow efforts and high rewards 1.00 1.00 1.00 1.00 1.00 1.00High efforts or low rewards 1.93 (1.05, 3.55) 1.88 (1.02, 3.45) 1.87 (1.01, 3.44) 1.70 (0.92, 3.13) 1.93 (1.05, 3.57) 1.77 (0.95, 3.28)High efforts and low rewards 2.68 (1.46,4.91) 2.54 (1.38, 4.67) 2.52 (1.36, 4.65) 2.12 (1.15, 3.91) 2.56 (1.39, 4.72) 2.15 (1.15, 4.01)

    N = 4393 (251 events)Self-reported job controld

    High job control 1.00 1.00 1.00 1.00 1.00 1.00Intermediate job control 2.05 (1.22, 3.44) 2.02 (1.20, 3.39) 2.08 (1.22, 3.53) 1.96 (1.17, 3.37) 2.05 (1.22, 3.44) 2.08 (1.22, 3.55)Low job control 2.15 (1.26, 3.67) 2.04 (1.19, 3.49) 2.44 (1.37, 4.38) 1.94 (1.13, 3.34) 1.97 (1.15, 3.40) 2.38 (1.32, 4.29)

    N = 4702 (115 events)Externally assessed job controPc

    High job control 1.00 1.00 1.00 1.00 1.00 1.00Intermediate job control 1.53 (1.07, 2.14) 1.52 (1.06, 2.17) 1.52 (1.06, 2.19) 1.51 (1.06, 2.17) 1.52 (1.06, 2.18) 1.53 (1.06, 2.20)Low job control 1.72 (1.23, 2.02) 1.63 (1.16, 2.28) 1.61 (1.11, 2.33) 1.62 (1.16, 2.27) 1.53 (1.09, 2.15) 1.56 (1.08, 2.27)

    N = 4393 (251 events)

    Note. Subjects with missing values on any of the variables were excluded from the separate analyses.aAdjusted for age, sex, and length of period between phases 1 and 3.bModel 1 additionally adjusted for other work characteristics. Effort-reward imbalance was adjusted for extemally assessed job control(findings were similar when mean self-reported job control was adjusted for); mean self-reported job control was adjusted for effort-rewardimbalance; externally assessed job control was adjusted for effort-reward imbalance.

    cCoronary heart disease cases at phase 1 were excluded; new coronary heart disease reports at phase 2 or phase 3 were the outcome.dMean phases I and 2 job control; coronary heart disease cases at phases 1 and 2 were excluded; new coronary heart disease reports atphase 3 were the outcome.

    January 1998, Vol. 88, No. I

  • Job Stress and Coronary Heart Disease

    results show that 87% of subjects reporting amyocardial infarction at phase 3 had docu-mented coronary heart disease. Furthermore,the classic coronary risk factors were relatedto both doctor-diagnosed ischemia andangina pectoris, suggesting that the end-points do reflect coronary heart disease, notjust reporting bias. Despite the likely differ-ent levels of sensitivity and specificity ofboth endpoints, effort-reward imbalance andlow job control show consistent associationswith both angina and doctor-diagnosedischemia. This supports an etiologicalhypothesis. Future analyses will examine theeffects of effort-reward imbalance and lowjob control on fatal and non-fatal myocardialinfarction and biological mechanisms.

    Second, small numbers did not permitextensive analyses of the associationsbetween employment grade level, job con-trol, effort-reward imbalance, and coronaryheart disease. Because the interactionbetween grade and any of the job stress fac-tors was never statistically significant, wewould not expect different effects of workcharacteristics among grades. The absenceof "confounding by" or "interaction with"employment grade level suggests that theassociation between work and coronaryheart disease does not depend on employ-ment grade level. A related issue is theextent to which work contributes to theinverse association between employmentgrade level and coronary heart disease inWhitehall II. This is further investigated inanother paper, which concludes that muchof the inverse social gradient in coronaryheart disease incidence in Whitehall II canbe attributed to differences in the psychoso-cial work environment.32

    Third, some further methodologicalissues need to be addressed. Although thethree-category effort-reward imbalanceindicator reflects a mismatch betweenefforts and rewards, there was no significant(multiplicative) interaction term betweenour proxy measures of efforts and rewardsin the logistic regression analysis. Thisshould be further elaborated upon usingoriginal effort and reward measures. Thehigh number of subjects whose effort-reward imbalance scores were not avail-able, mainly owing to a delayed inclusionof the hostility scale in the phase 1 ques-tionnaire, probably did not bias the resultsbecause these subjects did not differ fromthe other group in their risk of newlyreported coronary heart disease. Moreover.using an imputation method did not resultin substantially different odds ratios for theeffort-reward imbalance indicator. Giventhat individuals with effort-reward imbal-ance, low job control, angina pectoris, and

    doctor-diagnosed ischemia at phase 1 hadsomewhat lower participation rates atphases 2 and 3, it is likely that the impact ofeffort-reward imbalance and low job con-trol on newly reported coronary heart dis-ease is somewhat underestimated in theseanalyses. Controlling for whether individu-als had left the civil service did not affectthe results. Theoretically, infonnation biasmay have caused overestimated odds ratiosin our analyses because a complaining atti-tude regarding work and health (negativeaffectivity) may have resulted in negativereports about job control, effort-rewardimbalance, and coronary heart disease.33-36However, because baseline cases wereexcluded in a longitudinal setting and nega-tive affect balance was controlled for, it isunlikely that negative affectivity biased theresults.1922

    ConclusionLow job control and high cost/low

    gain conditions influence the developmentof heart disease among men and womenworking in British government offices. Thefinding that competitive, overcommitted,and hostile subjects with a less successfuloccupational career and low job controlhave higher risks of coronary heart diseaseunderscores the advantage of a job stressmodel combining personal and environ-mental factors. To our knowledge, this isthe first report showing independent effectson coronary heart disease of components oftwo alternative job stress models: the effort-reward imbalance model and the job strainmodel (ob control). FZ

    AcknowledgmentsThe work presented in this paper was supported bygrants from the Medical Research Council, BritishHeart Foundation, National Heart Lung and BloodInstitute (2 RO1 HL36310), Agency for HealthCare Policy Research (5 ROI HS06516), Healthand Safety Executive, the Institute for Work andHealth, Toronto, Ontario, and the John D. andCatherine T. MacArthur Foundation Research Net-work on Successful Midlife Development.Professor Marmot is supported by a MedicalResearch Council research professorship. DrBosma is supported by grants from the EU BIO-MED network "Socio-Economic Variations inCardiovascular Disease in Europe: The Impact ofthe Work Environment (Heart at Work)."

    We thank all participating civil servicedepartments and their welfare and personnel offi-cers; the Civil Service Occupational Health Ser-vice and their directors, Dr Elizabeth McCloy, DrGeorge Somre, and Dr Adrian Semmence; and allparticipating civil servants.

    References1. Karasek RA. Job demands, job decision lati-

    tude, and mental strain: implications for jobredesign. Admin Sci Q. 1979;24:285-308.

    2. Karasek RA, Theorell T. Healthy Work:Stress, Productivity, and the Reconstructionof Working Life. New York, NY: BasicBooks; 1990.

    3. Schnall PL, Landsbergis PA, Baker D. Jobstrain and cardiovascular disease. Ann RevPublic Health. 1994;15:381-41 1.

    4. Alterman T, Shekelle RB, Vernon SW, BurauKD. Decision latitude, psychologic demand,job strain, and coronary heart disease in theWestem Electric Study. Am J Epidemiol.1994; 139:620-627.

    5. Bosma H, Marmot MG, Hemingway H,Nicholson AC, Brunner E, Stansfeld SA. Lowjob control and the risk of coronary heart dis-ease in the Whitehall II (prospective cohort)study. BMJ.1997;314:558-564.

    6. Johnson JV, Stewart W, Hall EM, Fredlund P,Theorell T. Long-term psychosocial workenvironment and cardiovascular mortalityamong Swedish men. Am J Public Health.1996;86:324-331.

    7. North FM, Syme SL, Feeney A, Shipley M,Marmot M. Psychosocial work environmentand sickness absence among British civil ser-vants: the Whitehall II study. Am J PublicHealth. 1996;86:332-340.

    8. Siegrist J. Adverse health effects of higheffort/low reward conditions. J Occup HealthPsychol. 1996;1 :27-41.

    9. Matschinger H, Siegrist J, Siegrist K,Dittmann K. Type A as a coping career:towards a conceptual and methodologicalredefinition. In: Schmidt H, Dembroski TM,Blimchen G, eds. Biological and Psychologi-cal Factors in Cardiovascular Disease. Hei-delberg, Germany: Springer; 1986:104-126.

    10. Siegrist J, Peter R, Junge A, Cremer P, SeidelD. Low status control, high effort at work andischemic heart disease: prospective evidencefrom blue-collar men. Soc Sci Med. 1990;3 1:1127-1134.

    11. Kasl SV. The influence of the work environ-ment on cardiovascular health: a historical,conceptual, and methodological perspective. JOccup Health Psychol. 1996; 1:42-56.

    12. Marmot MG, Davey Smith G, Stansfeld S, etal. Health inequalities among British civil ser-vants: the Whitehall II study. Lancet.1991;337: 1387-1393.

    13. Beksinska M, Yea L, Brunner E. Whitehall IIStudy. Operations Manual for ScreeningExamination 1991-93 (Phase s3). London,England: University College London, Depart-ment of Epidemiology and Public Health;1995.

    14. Rose G, Reid DD, Hamilton PS, McCartneyP, Jarrett RJ. Myocardial ischaemia risk fac-tors and death from coronary heart disease.Lancet. 1977;i:105-109.

    15. Haynes SG, Levine S, Scotch N,.Feinleib M,Kannel WB. The relationship of psychosocialfactors to coronary heart disease in the Fram-ingham study, I: methods and risk factors. AmJEpidemiol. 1978;107:362-383.

    16. Cook WW, Medley DM. Proposed hostilityand pharisaic-virtue scales for the MMPI. JAppl Psychol. 1954;38:414-418.

    January 1998, Vol. 88, No. I American Journal of Public Health 73

  • Bosma et al.

    17. Karasek RA, Baker D, Marxer F, Ahlbom A,Theorell T. Job decision latitude, jobdemands, and cardiovascular disease: aprospective study of Swedish men. Am J Pub-lic Health. 1981 ;71 :694-705.

    18. Quinn RP, Staines GL. The 1977 Quality ofEmployment Survey. Ann Arbor, Mich: Sur-vey Research Center, Institute for SocialResearch, University of Michigan; 1979.

    19. Brief AP, Burke MJ, George JM, RobinsonBS, Webster J. Should negative affectivityremain an unmeasured variable in the study ofjob stress? JAppl Psychol. 1988;73: 193-198.

    20. Chen PY, Spector PK. Negative affectivity asthe underlying cause of correlations betweenstressors and strains. J Appl Psychol. 1991;76:398-407.

    21. McCrea G. Controlling neuroticism in themeasurement of stress. Stress Med. 1990;6:237-241.

    22. Spector PE. A consideration of the validity andmeaning of self-report measures of job condi-tions. Int Rev Ind Org Psychol. 1992; 123-151.

    23. Bradburn NM. The Structure ofPsychologicalWellbeing. Chicago, Ill: Aldine; 1969.

    24. Stansfeld S, North FM, White I, Marnot MG.Work characteristics and psychiatric disorder

    in civil servants in London. JEpidemiol Com-munity Health. 1995;49:48-53.

    25. Ferrie JE, Shipley MJ, Marmot MG, StansfeldS, Davey Smith G. Health effects of anticipa-tion of job change and non-employment: lon-gitudinal data from the Whitehall II study.BMJ. 1995:311:1264-1269.

    26. deJonge J. Job Autonomy, Well-Being, andHealth. Maastricht, The Netherlands: Datawyseb.v. Universitaire Pers Maastricht; 1996. Mas-ter's thesis.

    27. Sauter SL, Hurrell JJ, Cooper CL, eds. JobControl and Worker Health. New York, NY:John Wiley & Sons; 1989.

    28. Steptoe A, Appels A, eds. Stress, PersonalControl and Health. New York, NY: JohnWiley & Sons, 1989.

    29. Brunner E, Davey Smith G, Marmot M, Can-ner R, Beksinska M, O'Brien J. Childhoodsocial circumstances and psychosocial andbehavioural factors as determinants of plasmafibrinogen. Lancet. 1996;347: 1008-1013.

    30. Miller TQ, Smith TW, Turner CW, GuijarroML, Hallet AJ. A meta-analytic review ofresearch on hostility and physical health.Psychol Bull. 1996;1 19:322-348.

    31. Rose G. Variability of angina. Some implica-tions for epidemiology. Br J Prev Soc Med.1968;22:12-15.

    32. Marmot M, Bosma H, Hemingway H, Brun-ner E, Stansfield S. Contribution of job con-trol and other risk factors to social variationsin coronary heart disease incidence. Lancet.1997;350:235-239.

    33. Frese M. Stress at work and psychosomaticcomplaints: a causal interpretation. J ApplPsychol. 1985;70:314-328.

    34. Frese M, Zapf D. Methodological issues in thestudy of work stress: objective vs subjectivemeasurement of work stress and the question oflongitudinal studies. In: Cooper CL, Payne R,eds. Causes, Coping and Consequences ofStress at Work New York, NY: John Wiley &Sons, 1988:375-411.

    35. Kasl SV. The challenge of studying the dis-ease effects of stressful work conditions. Am JPublic Health. 1981;71:682-684.

    36. Kasl SV. Methodologies in stress and health:past difficulties, present dilemmas, future direc-tions. In: Kasl SV, Cooper CL, eds. Stress andHealth: Issues in Research Methodology. NewYork, NY: John Wiley & Sons, 1987:307-318.

    74 American Joumal of Public Health January 1998, Vol. 88, No. I