1
Copyright © 2008 by the Association for Computing Machinery, Inc. Permission to make digital or hard copies of part or all of this work for personal or classroom use is granted without fee provided that copies are not made or distributed for commercial advantage and that copies bear this notice and the full citation on the first page. Copyrights for components of this work owned by others than ACM must be honored. Abstracting with credit is permitted. To copy otherwise, to republish, to post on servers, or to redistribute to lists, requires prior specific permission and/or a fee. Request permissions from Permissions Dept, ACM Inc., fax +1 (212) 869-0481 or e-mail [email protected] . APGV 2008, Los Angeles, California, August 9–10, 2008. © 2008 ACM 978-1-59593-981-4/08/0008 $5.00 Tricking people into feeling like they are moving when they are not paying attention Laura C. Trut . oiu * , Stephan Streuber, Betty J. Mohler, J¨ org Schulte-Pelkum, and Heinrich H. B ¨ ulthoff Max Planck Institute for Biological Cybernetics Abstract Vection refers to illusion of self motion in stationary obervers usu- ally by means of moving visual stimuli [Fischer and Kornm¨ uller 1930]. Linear vection naturally occurs when seated in a train and observing another train on an adjacent track start moving. The very compelling but brief illusion happens as observers are not paying particular attention to the environment but are rather ”defocused” from the scene. We studied the effect of two visual attention tasks on the perception of linear vection. The results show a signicant decrease in vection onset time with an attention task. 1 Introduction Vection, or the illusion of self-motion can be useful in a wide range of aplications: educational (driving and ight simulators) and even entertainment (amusement park rides). Figure 1: Tunnel projected onto a large screen virtual environment. The VE setup consisted of an immersive large-screen display and a joystick (Saitek ST200). Four JVC D-ILA DLA-SX21S video projectors with a resolution of 1400 X 1050 pixels were used to display visual stimuli on the front, sides, and oor of the a custom made curved display. There are likely many high-level cognitive effects that inuence vection. In this paper we aim to investigate the role of attention on the perception of vection. One previous study showed that a non-attended ow pattern determines vection direction when two different optic ows are presented in the same depth plane [Ki- tazaki and Sato 2003]. In our study we used one optic ow pattern and a concurrent visual attention task. 2 Procedure and Method Eight paid volunteers participated in this experiment (balanced for gender). Participants sat on a chair at a distance of 3.5 meters from the curved projection screen which corresponds to a physical eld of view of about 220 degrees horizontal by 165 degrees vertical, as illustrated in Figure 1. The visual stimuli consisted of a fully en- closed tunnel, and a set of 4 targets varying in shape and color. Five * e-mail: [email protected] to eight of each of the four targets were placed directly in front of the observer in the same location on the bottom of the tunnel sepa- rated by a distance of 0-3 meters. Translational forward movements were presented through this tunnel at a speed of 10m/s and for 30 seconds. Participants were instructed to look straight ahead with- out following the moving targets. The experiment was programmed with Virtools 4.0. The within subject design consisted of a total of three conditions where participants were asked to provide two different measures of vection (vection onset and convincingness of vection). In two of the conditions an attention task was presented: either counting a spe- cic target (working memory condition) or pressing a button every time they saw a specic target (visual attention condition). In the control condition no attentional task was performed. Each partici- pant completed six blocks (2 repetitions of each condition). Every 3 blocks were preceded by 2 practice trials. The entire experiment took approximately 50 minutes to complete. 3 Results and Conclusions Average vection onset time for the control condition was 10.23 sec- onds, while for the two attentional task condition the onset times decreases to 8.25 seconds and 7.81 respectively (see Figure 2). For vection onset analysis we removed the 13% of the trials where vec- tion was not experienced . 10.23 8.25 7.81 0 2 4 6 8 10 12 Control Visual Aenon Working Memory Seconds Average Onset Time Control Visual Aenon Working Memory Figure 2: Average vection onset time for three different conditions of varying attention difculty for eight subjects. The results show a statistically signicant difference (a paired sam- ple t test) between the working memory and the control condi- tion indicating improved vection with an attentional task (t=2.426, p=0.046). Acknowledgements The authors wish to thank Prof. William B. Thompson, and Prof. John J. Rieser. References FISCHER, M. H., AND KORNM ¨ ULLER, A. E. 1930. Optokinetically induced motion perception and optokinetic nystagmus. Journal f¨ ur Psychologie und Neurologie, 273–308. KITAZAKI , M., AND SATO, T. 2003. Attentional modulation of self-motion percep- tion. Perception 32(4) 475 484. 190

Tricking people into feeling like they are moving when ...ltrutoiu/pdfs/APVG_2008_p190-trutoiu.pdfCopyright © 2008 by the Association for Computing Machinery, Inc. Permission to make

  • Upload
    others

  • View
    0

  • Download
    0

Embed Size (px)

Citation preview

Page 1: Tricking people into feeling like they are moving when ...ltrutoiu/pdfs/APVG_2008_p190-trutoiu.pdfCopyright © 2008 by the Association for Computing Machinery, Inc. Permission to make

Copyright © 2008 by the Association for Computing Machinery, Inc. Permission to make digital or hard copies of part or all of this work for personal or classroom use is granted without fee provided that copies are not made or distributed for commercial advantage and that copies bear this notice and the full citation on the first page. Copyrights for components of this work owned by others than ACM must be honored. Abstracting with credit is permitted. To copy otherwise, to republish, to post on servers, or to redistribute to lists, requires prior specific permission and/or a fee. Request permissions from Permissions Dept, ACM Inc., fax +1 (212) 869-0481 or e-mail [email protected]. APGV 2008, Los Angeles, California, August 9–10, 2008. © 2008 ACM 978-1-59593-981-4/08/0008 $5.00

Tricking people into feeling like they are movingwhen they are not paying attention

Laura C. Trut.oiu!, Stephan Streuber, Betty J. Mohler, Jorg Schulte-Pelkum, and Heinrich H. BulthoffMax Planck Institute for Biological Cybernetics

AbstractVection refers to illusion of self motion in stationary obervers usu-ally by means of moving visual stimuli [Fischer and Kornmuller1930]. Linear vection naturally occurs when seated in a train andobserving another train on an adjacent track start moving. The verycompelling but brief illusion happens as observers are not payingparticular attention to the environment but are rather ”defocused”from the scene. We studied the effect of two visual attention taskson the perception of linear vection. The results show a significantdecrease in vection onset time with an attention task.

1 IntroductionVection, or the illusion of self-motion can be useful in a wide rangeof aplications: educational (driving and flight simulators) and evenentertainment (amusement park rides).

Figure 1: Tunnel projected onto a large screen virtual environment. The VE setupconsisted of an immersive large-screen display and a joystick (Saitek ST200). FourJVC D-ILA DLA-SX21S video projectors with a resolution of 1400 X 1050 pixelswere used to display visual stimuli on the front, sides, and floor of the a custom madecurved display.

There are likely many high-level cognitive effects that influencevection. In this paper we aim to investigate the role of attentionon the perception of vection. One previous study showed that anon-attended flow pattern determines vection direction when twodifferent optic flows are presented in the same depth plane [Ki-tazaki and Sato 2003]. In our study we used one optic flow patternand a concurrent visual attention task.2 Procedure and MethodEight paid volunteers participated in this experiment (balanced forgender). Participants sat on a chair at a distance of 3.5 meters fromthe curved projection screen which corresponds to a physical fieldof view of about 220 degrees horizontal by 165 degrees vertical, asillustrated in Figure 1. The visual stimuli consisted of a fully en-closed tunnel, and a set of 4 targets varying in shape and color. Five

!e-mail: [email protected]

to eight of each of the four targets were placed directly in front ofthe observer in the same location on the bottom of the tunnel sepa-rated by a distance of 0-3 meters. Translational forward movementswere presented through this tunnel at a speed of 10m/s and for 30seconds. Participants were instructed to look straight ahead with-out following the moving targets. The experiment was programmedwith Virtools 4.0.The within subject design consisted of a total of three conditionswhere participants were asked to provide two different measures ofvection (vection onset and convincingness of vection). In two of theconditions an attention task was presented: either counting a spe-cific target (working memory condition) or pressing a button everytime they saw a specific target (visual attention condition). In thecontrol condition no attentional task was performed. Each partici-pant completed six blocks (2 repetitions of each condition). Every3 blocks were preceded by 2 practice trials. The entire experimenttook approximately 50 minutes to complete.

3 Results and ConclusionsAverage vection onset time for the control condition was 10.23 sec-onds, while for the two attentional task condition the onset timesdecreases to 8.25 seconds and 7.81 respectively (see Figure 2). Forvection onset analysis we removed the 13% of the trials where vec-tion was not experienced .

10.238.25 7.81

0

2

4

6

8

10

12

Control Visual A!en"on Working Memory

Seconds Average Onset Time

Control

Visual A!en"on

Working Memory

Figure 2: Average vection onset time for three different conditions of varying attentiondifficulty for eight subjects.

The results show a statistically significant difference (a paired sam-ple t test) between the working memory and the control condi-tion indicating improved vection with an attentional task (t=2.426,p=0.046).

AcknowledgementsThe authors wish to thank Prof. William B. Thompson, and Prof.John J. Rieser.

ReferencesFISCHER, M. H., AND KORNMULLER, A. E. 1930. Optokinetically induced motionperception and optokinetic nystagmus. Journal fur Psychologie und Neurologie,273–308.

KITAZAKI, M., AND SATO, T. 2003. Attentional modulation of self-motion percep-tion. Perception 32(4) 475 484.

190