30
Running title: Evidence for social rhythm ... / V 2.1 1 Running title: Evidence for social rhythm .... The definitive version of this article is published by Elsevier as: Meyer, T.D. & Maier, S. Is there evidence for social rhythm instability in people at risk for affective disorders?. Psychiatry Research 2006, 141(1), 103-114. doi:10.1016/j.psychres.2005.07.023 Is there evidence for social rhythm instability in people at risk for affective disorders? Thomas D. Meyer a * & Silke Maier a, b a Department of Clinical and Physiological Psychology; Eberhard Karls University; Psychological Institute; Christophstrasse 2; 72072 Tübingen, Germany b Universidade Federal do Rio Grande do Sul, Instituto de Psicologia, Porto Alegre, Brasil Published in ‘Psychiatry ResearchPlease keep in mind that there might be minor changes between this and the finally published version. *Corresponding Author. Tel.: +49 70 71 29 782 94; fax: +49 70 71 29 52 19; E-mail: [email protected]

Thomas D. Meyer a * & Silke Maier a, beprint.ncl.ac.uk/file_store/production/18578/B1CBA... · In Germany there is a long tradition to see the so-called Typus melancholicus as a premorbid

  • Upload
    others

  • View
    1

  • Download
    0

Embed Size (px)

Citation preview

Page 1: Thomas D. Meyer a * & Silke Maier a, beprint.ncl.ac.uk/file_store/production/18578/B1CBA... · In Germany there is a long tradition to see the so-called Typus melancholicus as a premorbid

Running title: Evidence for social rhythm ... / V 2.1 1

Running title: Evidence for social rhythm ....

The definitive version of this article is published by Elsevier as: Meyer, T.D. & Maier, S. Is there evidence for social rhythm instability in people at risk for affective disorders?. Psychiatry Research 2006, 141(1), 103-114. doi:10.1016/j.psychres.2005.07.023

Is there evidence for social rhythm instability in people at

risk for affective disorders?

Thomas D. Meyer a * & Silke Maier a, b

a Department of Clinical and Physiological Psychology; Eberhard Karls University; Psychological Institute; Christophstrasse 2; 72072 Tübingen, Germany

b Universidade Federal do Rio Grande do Sul, Instituto de Psicologia, Porto Alegre, Brasil

Published in ‘Psychiatry Research’

Please keep in mind that there might be minor changes between this and the

finally published version.

*Corresponding Author. Tel.: +49 70 71 29 782 94; fax: +49 70 71 29 52 19; E-mail:

[email protected]

Page 2: Thomas D. Meyer a * & Silke Maier a, beprint.ncl.ac.uk/file_store/production/18578/B1CBA... · In Germany there is a long tradition to see the so-called Typus melancholicus as a premorbid

Running title: Evidence for social rhythm ... / V 2.1 2

Abstract

Social rhythm disruptions are thought to be related to the etiology of affective symptoms.

‘Hypomanic personality’ and ‘rigidity’ are hypothesized to be risk factors for affective

disorders. We examined if people scoring high on such scales will demonstrate less stability

of social rhythms and sleep. Short-term prospective diary study with one group factor. Three

groups were selected from a non-university student sample: ‘Bipolar risk’ (scoring high on the

‘hypomanic personality scale’; n = 56); ‘Unipolar risk’ (scoring high on the ‘rigidity scale’; n

= 37); control group (scoring low on both scales; n = 48). The participants completed ratings

of their activities and sleep for 28 days. People at risk for bipolar disorders were showing

lower regularity of daily activities compared to controls. Their sleeping pattern was not

characterized by less but more variable hours of sleep. The unipolar risk group did not differ

from the control group at all. Despite some limitations there is partial evidence for social

rhythm and sleep irregularities in people putatively at risk for bipolar disorders. Further

research is, however, needed to replicate and extend these results.

Keywords: Hypomanic personality; rigidity; bipolar disorder; circadian rhythm

Page 3: Thomas D. Meyer a * & Silke Maier a, beprint.ncl.ac.uk/file_store/production/18578/B1CBA... · In Germany there is a long tradition to see the so-called Typus melancholicus as a premorbid

Running title: Evidence for social rhythm ... / V 2.1 3

1. Introduction

Following DSM IV- classifications, bipolar disorders are part of the affective disorders

(American Psychiatric Association, 1994). However, the discussion of whether a dimensional

approach or a categorical classification of affective disorders is more appropriate remains

controversial. As an alternative to the strict diagnostic categorization, several researchers

suggest a spectrum concept where affective symptoms are located on a continuum. If one

assumes one single spectrum of affective disorders encompassing bipolar and unipolar

conditions or better distinguishes between a unipolar and bipolar spectrum is still discussed

(e.g. Angst, 1978; Goodwin and Jamison, 1990; Akiskal, 2003; Angst et al., 2003a,b), Of

more relevance in our context ist that according to such a spectrum concept, manic as well as

depressive symptoms can differ in their intensities. Such a spectrum-model with its

dimensional point of view defines subsyndromal affective manifestations as part of the

affective symptom spectrum, opening up the field for research in high-risk paradigms.

There are multiple factors, which increase the probability of bipolar disorders. The

influence of genetic transmisson (Nurnberger and Gershon, 1992; McGuffin et al., 2003), the

effects of stressful live events and daily hassles (Healy, 1987; Wehr et al., 1987; Ehlers, 1988;

Johnson and Roberts, 1995) as well as temperaments and trait-like factors such as the “manic”

and “melancholic” types or attributional style (Tellenbach, 1961; Akiskal and Akiskal 1992;

von Zerssen, 1996; Alloy et al., 1999) can be brought together in the model of a “final-

common-pathway” (Akiskal and Mc Kinney, 1975). In this vulnerability-stress-model it is

argued that different risk factors or their combinations lead to the typical dysregulations in the

biochemic system and thereby cause affective symptoms.

From a theoretical as well as an empirical point of view there is reason to assume that

circadian rhythms play a central role in the origin and course of bipolar disorders. Research

focused on the sleep-wake cycle and could show that this circadian rhythm – primarily the

sleep-wake rhythm - plays a role in the origin and course of bipolar disorders. The sleep-

Page 4: Thomas D. Meyer a * & Silke Maier a, beprint.ncl.ac.uk/file_store/production/18578/B1CBA... · In Germany there is a long tradition to see the so-called Typus melancholicus as a premorbid

Running title: Evidence for social rhythm ... / V 2.1 4

wake cycle is clearly implicated in the pathophysiology of bipolar disorder by the fact that

sleep deprivation has antidepressant and manicogenic effects. (e.g. Wehr and Goodwin, 1983;

Ehlers et al., 1988; Healy & Williams, 1989; Wehr, 1990; Leibenluft et al. 1996; Malkoff-

Schwartz et al., 1998; Jones, 2001). Theoretical models emphasize that there might be an

interplay of circadian rhythm with the modern world, and that there is also a social rhythm in

addition to purely circadian and biological rhythms (e.g. Ehlers et al., 1988; Frank, 2005).

This means that factors such as a work schedule, regular activities (e.g. going to the gym,

having family dinners) are thought to influence and stabilize or destabilize biological rhythms,

especially if there is vulnerability for unstable rhythms. Ehlers et al. (1988) call such factors

that characterize our daily schedule and also influence biological rhythms ‘social zeitgebers’.

They argue that disruptions in the social rhythm cause a dysregulation of biological patterns

which in turn can cause affective symptoms in vulnerable individuals. Whether an irregular

social rhythm causes biological dysregulations itself or influences biological patterns through

influencing the sleep- wake- cycle which in turn causes a biological dysregulation, is yet

unclear. Using the “Social Rhythm Metric” (SRM; Monk et al., 1990) to quantify social daily

rhythms, an association between social rhythm disruptions and affective symptoms has been

demonstrated in several studies. Monk et al. (1991) has shown different patterns of social

rhythm for patients with unipolar depression demonstrating more intraindividual variability

compared to a healthy control group. Although the focus of theory of social zeitgebers (Ehlers

et al., 1988) was originally on depression, the relevance of disruptions of social and biological

rhythms is recently even more emphasized for the course of bipolar disorders (e.g. Frank et

al., 1999; Jones, 2001; Paykel, 2003).

In patients with bipolar disorders life events which cause a social rhythm disruption were

more frequently observed prior to (hypo-)manic episodes than prior to control periods without

affective symptoms (e.g. Malkoff-Schwartz et al., 1998, 2000). Similarly Ashman et al.

(1999) found a significantly reduced social rhythm in patients with rapid-cycling bipolar

Page 5: Thomas D. Meyer a * & Silke Maier a, beprint.ncl.ac.uk/file_store/production/18578/B1CBA... · In Germany there is a long tradition to see the so-called Typus melancholicus as a premorbid

Running title: Evidence for social rhythm ... / V 2.1 5

disorder. Therefore there is evidence that for individuals who received the diagnosis of an

affective disorder, social rhythm disruptions appear more frequently compared to healthy

individuals and might increase the risk for relapses. We do not know yet, however, if such

social rhythm disruptions precede the onset of affective disorders. Still there is no data

available confirming the hypotheses that disruptions in the social rhythm can be considered as

a causal risk factor for the occurrence of affective disorders. Nevertheless, the data indicates

an association between irregular social rhythms and affective symptoms and can be integrated

in a concept of reciprocal interaction where affective symptoms and social rhythm

irregularities might reinforce one another.

In a recent study, Chang et al. (2003) found a reduced social rhythm both in a high- risk

group of adolescents for developing bipolar disorders and in a sample of bipolar II patients

compared to a control group. Additionally, after a follow-up period of 20 weeks a reduced

social rhythm correlated with a higher probability for a hypomanic or a depressive episode.

Although this confirms their hypothesis, the SRM (Monk et al., 1990) was used

retrospectively to assess the social rhythm, i.e. asking the participants to describe their typical

rhythm for the month prior to the interview. The SRM is usually used to assess the daily times

for different activities and to empirically define the stability of the social rhythm.

Furthermore, Chang et al. (2003) did not actually investigate if changes in regularity (i.e.

social rhythm disruptions) – as hypothesized – influence symptom status but whether self-

reported social rhythm irregularities are associated with symptoms. Their results, however,

suggest that people who are considered within the bipolar spectrum (cyclothymia, bipolar II,

bipolar NOS) report fewer regular activities for the last month.

Using such as measure such as the SRM to retrospectively assess social rhythm stability

might lead to distorted results due to memory bias. Therefore we wanted to know what

happens if people at risk for affective disorders, especially bipolar disorders, complete the

SRM on a daily basis. The social zeitgeber model was originally proposed for depression,

Page 6: Thomas D. Meyer a * & Silke Maier a, beprint.ncl.ac.uk/file_store/production/18578/B1CBA... · In Germany there is a long tradition to see the so-called Typus melancholicus as a premorbid

Running title: Evidence for social rhythm ... / V 2.1 6

therefore it seemed important to consider the risk for unipolar and bipolar affective disorder.

To define vulnerability, we adopted a behavioral or psychometric high-risk approach (e.g.

Depue et al., 1989; Klein and Anderson, 1995; Meyer and Hautzinger, 2001). Using a

psychometric approach, the vulnerability is operationalized by scores on a test or

psychometric instrument (e.g. personality inventory) instead of being a first-degree relative of

a patient. Compared to the biological-genetic high-risk approach, the main advantages of the

behavioral or psychometric approach are that large samples can easily be screened and that

the risk status of the individual is not defined by the biological relationship to an index

patient. Since we do not know much yet about possible different pathways for familial and

non-familial forms of the disorder, the results of the psychometric approach are at least not

limited to the familial form.

In Germany there is a long tradition to see the so-called Typus melancholicus as a

premorbid personality style for depression (Tellenbach, 1961; von Zerssen, 1996). The Typus

melancholicus is characterized by obsessive-compulsive, perfectionistic and rigid traits with

the latter one being a central characteristic. Several studies showed that relatives of patients

with affective disorders have elevated scores on Rigidity (e.g. Maier et al., 1992, 1995 ; Lauer

et al., 1997), and these authors consider rigidity as a risk factor for affective disorders,

especially unipolar depression (e.g. Mundt et al., 1999). Instead of using attributional styles or

dysfunctional beliefs as others (e.g. Hammen and Goodman, 1990; Alloy et al., 1997, 1999),

we decided to use von Zerssen´s model (1996) as a guideline to select risk indicators and

therefore chose one of the core features of the Typus melancholius - the Rigidity Scale of the

Munich Personality Scale (von Zerssen et al., 1988) - as a risk indicator for unipolar

depression. Unfortunately the Rigidity Scale itself has not yet been tested in other studies

longitudinally if it does predict depression A concept that is related to rigidity –

perfectionism – has, however, been shown to predict depression (e.g. Hewitt et al., 1996;

Kenney-Benson and Pomerantz, 2005), In his model, von Zerssen also described a premorbid

Page 7: Thomas D. Meyer a * & Silke Maier a, beprint.ncl.ac.uk/file_store/production/18578/B1CBA... · In Germany there is a long tradition to see the so-called Typus melancholicus as a premorbid

Running title: Evidence for social rhythm ... / V 2.1 7

personality or temperament associated with mania, the Typus manicus. Characteristics of this

Typus manicus are high self confidence, unconventionality, and inconsistency. Sometimes

they are upbeat, gregarious, and very energetic people. We chose the “Hypomanic

Personality Scale” (HSP, Eckblad and Chapman, 1986) as an indictor for this Typus manicus

and therefore risk for bipolar disorders, because conceptually the HPS fits von Zerssen´s

description best, and there is growing evidence that the HPS is associated with affective

symptoms (e.g. Klein et al., 1996; Meyer, 2002a Meyer and Hautzinger, 2003; Blechert and

Meyer, 2004). People scoring high on the HPS also show similar aberrations in processing of

emotional information as do bipolar patients (e.g. French et al., 1996; Lyon et al., 1999), and

there is evidence for familial aggregation of HPS scores (Meyer and Hautzinger, 2001). Most

compelling, however, is that the HPS predicted bipolar disorders in a 13 year follow-up study

(Kwapil et al., 2000). Therefore it seems justified to use the HPS as a risk factor for

bipolarity.

One hypothesis was that there is an association between vulnerability for affective

disorders and irregularity of the social rhythm. People putatively at risk for bipolar disorders –

as defined by the HPS – were expected to show an unstable social rhythm over time,

reflecting the core vulnerability for bipolar disorders: the instability of circadian-biological

rhythms (e.g. Goodwin and Jamison, 1990). Individuals hypothesized to be at risk for unipolar

depression, however, were expected to have a very stable social rhythm due to their rigidity.

Social rhythm disruptions are seen as risk factors for the occurrence of depressive symptoms

(e.g. Ehlers et al., 1988), but at baseline it is assumed that rigidity is associated with a very

stable daily rhythm. In a premorbid status a stable social rhythm might protect people with

high rigidity against depression. Due to their high rigidity, however, they display a very low

level of flexibility in their life-styles and life-rhythms. According to von Zerrsen (1996) they

might be at risk not to be able to deal with rhythm disruptions in a healthy, adaptive way. In

the long run their risk of developing affective symptoms increases by their non-adaptive

Page 8: Thomas D. Meyer a * & Silke Maier a, beprint.ncl.ac.uk/file_store/production/18578/B1CBA... · In Germany there is a long tradition to see the so-called Typus melancholicus as a premorbid

Running title: Evidence for social rhythm ... / V 2.1 8

(rigid) need for rhythmicity, and any kind of dysregulations may lead to strong affective

reactions. Participants who are neither at risk for bipolar disorders nor at risk for depression

were thought to have an intermediate position between the two high-risk groups concerning

the stability of social rhythm. The rational for this was that their social rhythm is on one hand

more flexible and responsive to situational requirements, but returns quickly to their

individual (stable) baseline. In addition to this main hypothesis we also tested if the notion is

true that people with a hypomanic-hyperthymic temperament habitually sleep less than others

(Eckblad and Chapman, 1986; Akiskal, 1996). Although they might report this as a trait for

themselves (which is considered highly socially desirable, implying being more productive

and energetic), nobody has ever tested if this reflects reality if one looks at sleep reports on a

daily basis. Therefore using a diary we want to see if they truly sleep less than control

subjects. In viewing an instability of biological-circadian rhythms as the core vulnerability for

bipolar disorders (e.g. Goodwin and Jamison, 1990) we predict, however, a greater

intraindividual variability of sleep for the bipolar risk group compared to others.

2. Methods

2.1 Participants

In 1998/1999 6000 students who either still attended college or high-school or went to

vocational schools receiving a specific training for a job (e.g. cosmetology, mail services,

banking, etc.) completed a questionnaire package including the German version of the

Hypomanic Personality Scale (HPS; Eckblad and Chapman, 1986; Meyer, 2002b; Meyer and

Hautzinger, 2003) and the subscale Rigidity of the Munich Personality Test (MPT, von

Zerssen et al., 1988). The study was approved by the ethical committee of the German

Psychological Association (DGPs), and subjects provided written consent prior to enrolling

the study. The questionnaires were completed in the classrooms. The original sample

consisted of 6000 students. Non-native German students were excluded (see Chmielewski et

Page 9: Thomas D. Meyer a * & Silke Maier a, beprint.ncl.ac.uk/file_store/production/18578/B1CBA... · In Germany there is a long tradition to see the so-called Typus melancholicus as a premorbid

Running title: Evidence for social rhythm ... / V 2.1 9

al., 1995). Additionally, we measured an Infrequency score (Chapman et al., 1976) by which

inadequate response patterns can be identified. A score higher than 3 indicates unusual and

untypical responses and identifies those who tend to distort their answers. Individuals were

excluded who scored 3 or higher. Those whose score on the Lie scale of the EPI was 5 or

higher (Eysenck and Eysenck, 1964) were also excluded from the study.

Then we selected three groups that corresponded to different vulnerability status for

affective disorders based on their HPS and Rigidity scores: (1) Group with vulnerability for

bipolar disorders (bipolar): Students whose scores were in the upper 10% of the score

distribution of the original cohort in the HPS; (2) Group with vulnerability for unipolar

depression (unipolar): Students whose score were in the upper 10% of the score distribution

of the original cohort in the Rigidity Scale, (3) Control group (control): Subjects whose scores

did not exceed M + 1/2 SD in neither the HPS nor the Rigidity Scale . The control group was

matched for sex and age to the two risk groups as to limit sample size (for details about the

study: Krumm-Merabet and Meyer, 2003; Meyer and Keller, 2003; Blechert and Meyer,

2005; Meyer and Krumm-Merabet, 2005).

Persons who met the criteria for one of the three groups were invited to an interview (n =

357). Two-hundred-forty-seven (69.2 %) participated. There was no evidence for selective

attrition concerning risk status, χ 2 (2, n = 357) = 2.20, n.s., or age, t (355) = 0.78, n.s., but

women were more willing to further participate (74.3 % [159/214]) than men (61.5 %

[88/143]), χ 2 (1, n = 357) = 6.55, P = 0.01. Furthermore, all participants were asked to take

part in a prospective study for four weeks completing a diary every day. This diary contained

the Social Rhythm Metric (see below). To be included in the analyses, the participants had to

provide us with continuous data from the diary, i.e. completing at least 21 days. Ninety-nine

female and 41 male participants (n = 141) agreed and completed the diary continuously every

day (predominantly female with 70.7 %). The mean age was 18.18 (SD = 2.14; range 15-23).

Thirteen students (9.2 %) had a high level of education (13 years of education, “Abitur”),the

Page 10: Thomas D. Meyer a * & Silke Maier a, beprint.ncl.ac.uk/file_store/production/18578/B1CBA... · In Germany there is a long tradition to see the so-called Typus melancholicus as a premorbid

Running title: Evidence for social rhythm ... / V 2.1 10

majority with 54.6 % (n = 77) of students attended school for 10 years, representing an

intermediate level of education (“Mittlere Reife”), and 10.6 % % (n = 15) of the students had

either a low level of education (9 years, “Hauptschule”). Additional 9.9 % (n = 14) were

attending vocational schools and 14.2 % (n = 20) had not yet completed school at the time of

the study . Two persons did not provide this kind of information (1.4 %)

The final group sizes were therefore (1) a group with vulnerability for bipolar disorders

(bipolar): n = 56 (39.7 %), (2) a group with vulnerability for unipolar depression (unipolar): n

= 37 (26.2 %), and (3) a control group (control): n = 48 (34 %) (Table 1).

2.2 Materials

2.2.1 Hypomanic Personality Scale (HPS; Eckblad and Chapman, 1986):

The HSP contains 48 items and proved to be reliable and sufficiently stable (e.g. Eckblad

and Chapman, 1986; Klein et al., 1996). The German version achieved an internal consistency

of .87 to .89 (Cronbach´s Alpha) and a retest-correlation of r tt = 0.77 (n = 74; interval: 7

months) (Meyer, 2002b; Meyer et al., 2000). Different studies have shown its validity for the

bipolar spectrum (e.g. French et al., 1996; Kwapil et al., 2000; Meyer, 2002b; Meyer and

Hautzinger, 2003).

2.2.2 MPT- “Rigidity- Scale”( von Zerssen, et al., 1988):

The Rigidity- Scale is one of the six scales of the Munich Personality Inventory (von

Zerssen, 1977). It contains eight items and was validated with a Cronbach´s α between 0.68

and 0.84. After a 12-month test-retest interval, it reached a retest-correlation of r tt = 0.70.

Different studies showed its association to unipolar depression (e.g. Maier et al., 1992;

Heerlein et al. 1996; Mundt et al., 1999).

2.2.3 Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV Axis I disorders (SCID, First et al., 1996).

Participants were interviewed using the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM IV (SCID,

First et al., 1996; German version: Wittchen et al., 1997) to assess symptoms of affective

Page 11: Thomas D. Meyer a * & Silke Maier a, beprint.ncl.ac.uk/file_store/production/18578/B1CBA... · In Germany there is a long tradition to see the so-called Typus melancholicus as a premorbid

Running title: Evidence for social rhythm ... / V 2.1 11

disorders. This interview was chosen on the basis of its accepted and widespread international

use. Interviews were conducted by graduate psychology students trained extensively for this

purpose. Specifically, this training consisted of two full weekends and additional supervised

training interviews. Furthermore, all interviews were videotaped to permit a consensus

decision for all diagnoses.

2.2.4 Social Rhythm Metric (SRM; Monk et al., 1990):

The Social Rhythm Metric quantifies the stability of an individual´s daily routine. It lists

17 activities which have been identified as central in everyday life (Rehm, 1978; Kanner et

al., 1981) and leaves space for two more optional activities for the individual. The SRM is

meant to be completed every day, writing down the exact time of the beginning of an activity.

A short version of the SRM was proposed by Monk et al. (2002) where only five activities

(get out of bed, first communication, start work, have dinner, go to bed) are considered. Using

the SRM-5 short form, we investigated these five central activities. As two of the five

activities are related to sleep-wake-patterns (“get out of bed” and “go to bed”) this short

version of the SRM emphasizes the sleep-wake-cycle as a major anchor in the daily social

rhythm. The score of the SRM rises with the regularity of daily activities. For this purpose

one calculates the average time for each activity and counts how often these took place within

a certain predetermined time frame, i.e. average time +/- 45min. Then one divides these hits

through the number of activities performed more than three times a week (see for details:

Monk et al., 1990). The SRM has a moderate to high retest-reliability. Monk et al. (1990)

reports a highly significant correlation (rho= 0.44; P< 0.001) between week 1 and week 2.

The SRM has been described as a valid instrument in different studies (e.g. Monk et al. 1990;

Frank et al. 1994, 1997, 1999; Chang et al., 2003).

2.3 Procedure

Page 12: Thomas D. Meyer a * & Silke Maier a, beprint.ncl.ac.uk/file_store/production/18578/B1CBA... · In Germany there is a long tradition to see the so-called Typus melancholicus as a premorbid

Running title: Evidence for social rhythm ... / V 2.1 12

Following the procedure used by Alloy et al. (1997) and by Lovejoy and Steuerwald

(1997), participants were asked to complete a diary every day. The diary consisted of 28

sheets, each for one day. Each sheet included the SRM (e.g. time going to bed, falling asleep,

starting work, doing sport). In order to obtain the same stability and consistency, each

participant was instructed thoroughly about how to fill out the sheets daily and to send them

back daily. For this purpose they all received 28 pre-paid envelopes. If the sheet for a specific

day was not returned to us within 2 days (except weekends), the subjects were called and

asked if they have forgotten to fill out (leading to a drop out) or if there was a mailing

problem. Subjects were paid 80 DM (about 40 US $) for their participation.

3. Results

3.1 Dropout analyses and distribution of affective disorders

Comparing the ones who continuously completed the diary (n = 141) with those that did

not, there was no difference regarding risk status, χ 2 (2, n = 247) = 2.74, n.s., or age, t (245) =

-0.54, n.s.. However, more women (100/159) than men (41/88) provided us with complete

data from the diaries, χ 2 (1, n = 247) = 6.15, P = 0.01. Completion of the diary was not

associated with the presence or absence of affective disorders, χ 2 (1, n = 247) = 2.04, n.s..

In Table 1 the distribution of bipolar disorder and major depression disorder is displayed

for the three groups. Although bipolar disorders were only diagnosed in the two risk groups,

with 5.4 % in the bipolar risk group and 2.7 % in the unipolar risk group, there were no

significant group differences in this sample of adolescents and young adults, χ 2 (2, n = 141) =

2.69, n.s.. Episodes of major depression were found in all three groups but did also not differ

between groups, χ 2 (2, n = 141) = 1.53, n.s..

3.2 Social rhythm: means

Comparing all three groups concerning the Social Rhythm Score (SRS), a one-way

ANOVA resulted in a significant effect, F (2, 141) = 4.61, P = 0.01. Running planned simple

Page 13: Thomas D. Meyer a * & Silke Maier a, beprint.ncl.ac.uk/file_store/production/18578/B1CBA... · In Germany there is a long tradition to see the so-called Typus melancholicus as a premorbid

Running title: Evidence for social rhythm ... / V 2.1 13

contrasts comparing the risk groups with the control group, the unipolar risk group did not

differ significantly from the control group (P = 0.18) but the bipolar risk group did

significantly differ from the control group (P = 0.01) indicating a lower regularity of daily

activities in people hypothesized to be at risk for bipolar disorders (see Table 2).

To see if these results are due to people suffering from affective disorders, we also ran the

analysis for the sample excluding the participants with a bipolar disorder (n = 4) and those

with a major depression disorder (n = 18). The ANOVA did not approach significance any

more, but the same trend was still seen, F (2, 119) = 2.55, P = 0.08.

3.3 Social rhythm: stability

To test the stability of the SRS over time, we calculated the retest-correlation of SRS for

each week with the following weeks. Table 3 shows the retest-correlations for the sample as a

whole. The range of the test-retest correlation is 0.20 to 0.41, all being significant at least at P

< 0.05. There is therefore some indication for stability of the SRS score over time but the

correlations are fairly low.

We also calculated the retest correlations separate for all three groups (see Table 3). In the

control group we find more significant retest correlations than in the unipolar and bipolar risk

group. Testing for possible significant differences in stability of the SRS over time, we used

Fisher’s Z transformation for the averaged retest correlations for all weeks. However, none of

the comparisons reached significance, i.e. despite different levels of social rhythm regularity

the three groups were similar regarding the stability of the SRS scores (z < 0.3, all n.s.)

3.4 Sleep duration

Hypomanic temperament is thought to be associated with less sleep but we did not find the

expected differences between groups concerning means of sleep duration using the diary, F

(2, 138) = 0.10, n.s. Regardless of risk status the average duration of sleep was comparable

over four weeks, and this was also the case if we excluded people suffering from major

Page 14: Thomas D. Meyer a * & Silke Maier a, beprint.ncl.ac.uk/file_store/production/18578/B1CBA... · In Germany there is a long tradition to see the so-called Typus melancholicus as a premorbid

Running title: Evidence for social rhythm ... / V 2.1 14

affective disorders, F (2, 116) = 0.17, n.s.. Table 2 displays the means and standard deviations

for the total sample.

Additionally, we assumed that the bipolar risk group shows a less stable sleep pattern

compared to both the unipolar risk group and the control group. To test this prediction, we

calculated the intraindividual standard deviations in sleep duration over the four weeks and

averaged them for the three groups (Table 2). A one-way ANOVA indicated a highly

significant effect, F (2, 134) = 6.19; P < 0.003. Planned contrasts revealed - as expected - that

the unipolar risk group did not differ from controls (P = 0.98), but the bipolar risk group

showed significantly more variability in their sleep pattern (P < 0.01). This result remained

significant even after excluding all participants with bipolar and unipolar affective disorders,

F (2, 114) = 5.53, P < 0.005.

4. Discussion

We examined whether there is evidence of social rhythm instabilities in people at risk for

affective disorders. We actually found evidence for reduced social rhythm scores measured by

the Social Rhythm Metric (SRM; Monk et al, 1990) for participants of the bipolar risk group

compared to the control group. This was not the case for people from the unipolar risk group,

defined by high rigidity scores. When excluding participants who already were diagnosed

with bipolar or unipolar disorders, this effect is no longer significant but a trend remains that

people with high HYP scores show reduced social rhythms. Correlations of the Social

Rhythm Score between the four weeks become all but one significant. However, correlations

are fairly low (0.20 ≥ r ≤ 0.41). Looking at the sleeping pattern we found in contrast to

descriptions of the hypomanic temperament (e.g. Eckblad and Chapman, 1986; Akiskal,

1996) that people with high HYP scores did not sleep less averaged over 28 days compared

with others. However, although sleeping the same amount on average, their daily sleep pattern

Page 15: Thomas D. Meyer a * & Silke Maier a, beprint.ncl.ac.uk/file_store/production/18578/B1CBA... · In Germany there is a long tradition to see the so-called Typus melancholicus as a premorbid

Running title: Evidence for social rhythm ... / V 2.1 15

shows much more fluctuations over time than the one of controls or people hypothesized at

risk for unipolar depression.

Before discussing the other results, it seems important to address the issue why the stability

of social rhythm seems fairly low in our sample. Looking at retest-reliability for week 1 and

week 2 Monk et al. (1990) reported a retest-correlation of r= 0.44. All participants were

intensively instructed how to complete the SRM. One reason for the lower stability might be

that our subjects were asked to send back the forms daily. It seems that Monk et al. (1990)

collected the booklets containing the SRM only at the end of the two-week period.

Furthermore our sample consisted only of adolescents and young adults. Although this is

highly speculative it seems plausible to assume that they tend to have a lower regularity in

their life-styles than adults.These factors might explain that the retest-correlations of the SRM

scores are lower than might have been expected.

The background for our study was that it is assumed that depression and bipolar disorders

are related to social rhythm disruptions. Caused by stress or other conditions (e.g. divorce,

birth of a child, traveling over time zones) such disruptions in social rhythms are thought to

lead to circadian rhythm disruptions. The latter ones cause physical symptoms that interact

with biological and/or psychological vulnerabilities. The interaction of these factors is

hypothesized to lead to affective symptoms in people who are at risk (e.g. Ehlers et al. 1988;

Monk et al., 1990; Frank et al. 1994). There is evidence that circadian rhythm disruptions,

mostly defined by changes in sleep or the SRM, are associated with depression and mania

(e.g. Wehr et al., 1982; Wehr, 1990; Brown et al., 1996; Malkoff-Schwartz et al., 1998, 2000;

Bukhari et al., 2003; Chang et al., 2003).

All studies focused so far on patients diagnosed with either unipolar or bipolar affective

disorders. Theoretically social rhythm disruptions are always seen as an independent factor

interacting with the vulnerability. Chang et al. (2003) examined if social rhythm regularity is

different in people diagnosed with cyclothymia and bipolar II disorder compared to controls

Page 16: Thomas D. Meyer a * & Silke Maier a, beprint.ncl.ac.uk/file_store/production/18578/B1CBA... · In Germany there is a long tradition to see the so-called Typus melancholicus as a premorbid

Running title: Evidence for social rhythm ... / V 2.1 16

and found evidence for this assumption. Furthermore low social rhythm regularity predicted

occurrence of hypomanic and major depressive episodes. Our data does not allow predictions

of onset of affective episodes because we were not able to follow-up the participants long

enough. Nevertheless assessing social rhythm and sleep propectively over four weeks on a

daily basis the results show that social rhythm irregularity and instability of sleeping pattern is

only pronounced in people putatively at risk for bipolar disorders and not in people

hypothesized at risk for unipolar depression.

Overall the results support the available evidence for the Hypomanic Personality scale as

an indicator for vulnerability of bipolar disorders (e.g. French et al., 1996; Kwapil et al. 2000;

Meyer, 2001; Meyer and Hautzinger, 2003; Blechert and Meyer, 2004). Looking at our results

one might question, however, the accuracy of criteria for or descriptions of the hyperthymic-

hypomanic temperament assuming “habitual less sleep” (e.g. Eckblad and Chapman, 1986;

Akiskal, 1996). It is not clear if this notion of less need for sleep or actually less sleep is a

recall bias by people characterized by a hypomanic temperament when asked for their

sleeping pattern or if this idea is more derived by the clinical picture of hypomania. Our data

suggest that these people scoring high on the HYP scale are better characterized by an

unstable sleeping pattern resulting in switching between hyposomnia and hypersomnia. This

is consistent with the idea that hyperthymia is part or special form of cyclothymia (e.g.

Akisskal, 1996) and that the core vulnerability for bipolar disorders is instability of biological

rhythms (e.g. Goodwin and Jamison, 1990; Frank, 2005). Another consideration focuses on

the fact that the groups also might differ in their sleep efficiency. Sleeping time is measured

by the time between the two activities “go to bed” and “get out of bed”. Differentiating

between actual sleep and time spent in bed one can argue that groups do not actually differ in

the amount of time spent in bed, but in the amount of sleep. Our study does not help resolve

this question. Better controlled studies in sleep laboratories are needed to clarify such issues.

Page 17: Thomas D. Meyer a * & Silke Maier a, beprint.ncl.ac.uk/file_store/production/18578/B1CBA... · In Germany there is a long tradition to see the so-called Typus melancholicus as a premorbid

Running title: Evidence for social rhythm ... / V 2.1 17

Looking at risk for depression, people scoring high on Rigidity (von Zerssen et al., 1988)

did not seem to show any evidence for deviations in social rhythms and sleep. On one hand

there is evidence for an association between melancholia and rigidity (e.g. Maier et al., 1992;

Lauer et al., 1997; Mundt et al., 1999), but on the other the only existing longitudinal study

stems from our own laboratory. We did not find evidence that rigidity is predicting depression

over a two-year period (Blechert and Meyer, 2005). Further studies are needed to resolve the

question if rigidity is a valid indicator of risk for depression at all. Related concepts, however,

seem to predict depression (e.g. Hewitt et al., 1996).

Before drawing final conclusions some limitations of the study should be mentioned: First,

we had a selective dropout of male participants so that the majority of our sample was female.

Chang et al. (2003) reported that women endorsed significantly more activities as ‘regular’

than males, but they did not assess the Social Rhythm Metric on a daily basis so comparisons

are difficult. On a theoretical level there seems, however, to be no reason to assume that social

rhythm disruptions themselves have differential effects for women and men when it comes to

affective disorders. Second, although we had a prospective design this only covered 28 days.

Our primary goal was to assess the stability of sleep and social rhythm and not the occurrence

of affective episodes. However, this means that we cannot say anything about the relationship

between social rhythm disruptions and the onset of mania or depression. One might also raise

the question if the group differences are due to any potential confounding variables (e.g.

season, life events). The subjects started at different points of time completing the diary (over

a 9 month period) and we did actually not control for such variables. Nevertheless we think

hat a lot of potential confounding variables are randomly distributed over the groups.

However, it seems reasonable to assume that people at risk experience more (perhaps even

self-inflicted) stress, so it could be that group differences (e.g. unstable sleep pattern) are

indeed reflecting different stress levels. Last but not least – and related to the former

limitation - it has to be emphasized that we cannot disentangle the question if social rhythm

Page 18: Thomas D. Meyer a * & Silke Maier a, beprint.ncl.ac.uk/file_store/production/18578/B1CBA... · In Germany there is a long tradition to see the so-called Typus melancholicus as a premorbid

Running title: Evidence for social rhythm ... / V 2.1 18

disruptions or changes in sleep are of more concern because sleep parameters were also used

to estimate the social rhythm More research is definitely needed to see if social rhythm

disruptions themselves lead to changes in symptoms or even onset of episodes in people at

risk for affective disorders or if these are only mediated by changes in sleep.

Despite these limitations our conclusion is that that we found evidence for reduced social

rhythms in young people hypothesized to be at risk for bipolar disorders, referring to daily

routines as well as to sleep. Since instability of the social rhythm and sleep are seen as

potential risk factors for the onset and occurrence of mania and depression, further studies are

needed to examine the interplay of vulnerability, social rhythm (disruptions) and symptoms in

people at risk for affective disorders. Of special relevance would be to investigate if social

rhythm disruptions are mediated by sleep disruptions

Page 19: Thomas D. Meyer a * & Silke Maier a, beprint.ncl.ac.uk/file_store/production/18578/B1CBA... · In Germany there is a long tradition to see the so-called Typus melancholicus as a premorbid

Running title: Evidence for social rhythm ... / V 2.1 19

Acknowledgements:

This research was supported by a grant from the Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft to T. D.

Meyer (DFG Me 1681/7-1). We would like to thank our team of graduate students and

research assistants for their assistance.

Page 20: Thomas D. Meyer a * & Silke Maier a, beprint.ncl.ac.uk/file_store/production/18578/B1CBA... · In Germany there is a long tradition to see the so-called Typus melancholicus as a premorbid

Running title: Evidence for social rhythm ... / V 2.1 20

5. References

Akiskal, H.S., 1996. The prevalent clinical spectrum of bipolar disorders: Beyond the

DSM-IV. Journal of Clinical Psychopharmacology 16 (Suppl.1), 4S-14S.

Akiskal, H.S., 2003. Validating ‘hard’ and ‘soft’ phenotypes within the bipolar spectrum:

continuity or discontinutiy? Journal of Affective Disorders 73, 1-5.

Akiskal, H. S., Akiskal K.,1992. Cyclothymic, hyperthymic and depressive temperaments

as subafective variants of mood disorders. In: A. Tasman & M. B. Riba (Eds.), Review of

Psychiatry- Vol. 11. American Psychiatric Press, Washington, D. C., pp. 43- 62.

Akiskal, H. S., Mc Kinney, W. T., 1975. Overview of recent research in depression.

Archives of General Psychiatry 32, 285- 295.

Alloy, L. B., Just, N., Panzarella, C., 1997. Attributional Style, Daily Life events and

Hopelessness Depression: subtype Validation by Prospective Variability and Specificity of

Symptoms. Cognitive Therapy and Research 21, 321- 344.

Alloy, L.B., Reilly-Harrington, N., Fresco, D.M., Whitehouse, W.G., Zechmeister, J.S.,

1999. Cognitive styles and life events in subsyndromal unipolar and bipolar disorders:

Stability and prospective prediction of depressive and hypomanic mood swings. Journal of

Cognitive Psychotherapy: An International Quarterly 13, 21-40.

American Psychiatric Association, 1994. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental

Disorders. 4th ed. American Psychiatry Association, Washington D. C.

Angst, J., 1978. The course of affective disorders: II Typology of bipolar manic-

depressive illness. Archiv für Psychatrie und Nervenkrankheiten 226, 65- 73.

Angst, J., Gamma, A., Benazzi, F., Ajdacic, V., Eich, D., Rössler, W., 2003a. Toward a re-

definition of subthreshold bipolarity: epidemiology and proposed criteria for bipolar II, minor

bipolar disorders and hypomania. Journal of Affective Disorders 73, 133-146.

Angst, J., Gamma, A., Endrass, J., 2003b. Risk factors for the bipolar and depression

spectra. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica 108, 15-19.

Page 21: Thomas D. Meyer a * & Silke Maier a, beprint.ncl.ac.uk/file_store/production/18578/B1CBA... · In Germany there is a long tradition to see the so-called Typus melancholicus as a premorbid

Running title: Evidence for social rhythm ... / V 2.1 21

Ashman, S. B., Monk, T. H., Kupfer, D. J., Clark, C. H., Myers, F. S., Frank, E.,

Leibenluft, E., 1999. Relationship between social rhythms and mood in patients with rapid

cycling bipolar disorder. Psychiatry Research 86, 1-8.

Blechert, J., Meyer, T.D., 2005. Are the scales Hypomanic Personality, Impulsive

Nonconformity and Rigidity predictors of bipolar symptoms? British Journal of Clinical

Psychology 44, 15-27.

. Brown, L.F., Reynolds III, C.F., Monk, T.H:, Prigerson, H.G., Dew, M.A., Houck, P.R.,

Mazamdar, S., Buysse, D.J., Hoch, C.C., Kupfer, D. J., 1996. Social rhythm stability

following late-life spousal bereavement: associations with depression and sleep impairment.

Psychiatry Research 62, 161-169.

Bukhari, L., Smaverdy, A., Amand, A., Mayeda, A., Lawson, W., Nurnberger Jr, J.J.,

2003. The effect of circadian rhythm disruption on mood in a subgroup of patients with

bipolar affective disorder. Poster presented at the 5th International Conference on Bipolar

Disorders, Pittsburgh, PA, June 12-14.

Chang G.H., Alloy, L. B., Abramson, L. Y., 2003. Eaxmining social rhythm regularity to

predict affective episodes in bipolar spectrum individuals. Bipolar Disorders 5 (Suppl. 1), 39-

40.

Chapman, L. J., Chapman, J. P., Raulin, M. L., 1976. Scales for physical and social

anhedonia. Journal of Abnormal Psychology 85, 374- 382.

Chmielewski, P.M., Fernandes, L.O.L., Yee, C.M., Miller, G.A., 1995. Ethnicity and

gender in scales of psychosis proneness and mood disorders. Journal of Abnormal Psychology

106, 464-470.

Depue, R. A., Krauss, S., Spoont, M. R., Arbisi, P., 1989. General Behaviour Inventory

identification of unipolar and bipolar affective conditions in a nonclinical university

population. Journal of Abnormal Psychology 98, 117- 126.

Page 22: Thomas D. Meyer a * & Silke Maier a, beprint.ncl.ac.uk/file_store/production/18578/B1CBA... · In Germany there is a long tradition to see the so-called Typus melancholicus as a premorbid

Running title: Evidence for social rhythm ... / V 2.1 22

Eckblad, M., Chapman, L. J., 1986. Development and validation of a scale for hypomanic

personality. Journal of Abnormal Psychology 3, 214- 222.

Ehlers, C. Frank, E., Kupfer, D. J., 1988. Social Zeitgebers and biological rhythms.

Archives of General Psychiatry 45, 948- 952.

Eysenck, H.J., Eysenck, S.B.G., 1964. Manual of the Eysenck Personality Inventory.

University of London Press, London. [German: Eggert, D., 1983. Eysenck-Persönlichkeits-

Inventar (EPI). Hogrefe, Göttingen]

First, M.B., Gibbon, M., Spitzer, R.L., Williams, J.B.W., 1996. Users guide if the

structured clinical Interview for DSM IV Axis I disorders [German version: Wittchen, H.U.,

Zaudig, M., Fydrich, T., 1997. Strukturiertes Klinisches Interview für DSM IV. Hogrefe,

Göttingen].

Frank, E., 2005. Treating Bipolar Disorder. A Clinician's Guide to Interpersonal and Social

Rhythm Therapy. New York: Guilford Press.

Frank, E., Hlastala, S., Ritenour, A., Houck, P., Tu, X. M., Monk, T. H., 1997. Inducing

lifestyle regularity in recovering bipolar disorder patients: results from the maintenance

therapies in bipolar disorder protocol. Biological Psychiatry 47, 1165- 1173.

Frank, E., Kupfer, D. J., Ehlers, C. L., Monk, T. H., Cornes, C., Carter, S., Frankel, D.,

1994. Interpersonal and social rhythm therapy for bipolar disorder: Integrating interpersonal

and behavioural approaches. Behaviour Therapist 17, 143- 149.

Frank, E., Swartz, H. A., Mallinger, A. G., Thase, M. E., Weaver, E. V., Kupfer, D. J., 1999.

Adjunctive psychotherapy for bipolar disorder: effects of changing treatment modality. Journal of

Abnormal Psychology 108, 579- 587.

French, C. C., Richards, A., Scholfield, E. J. C., 1996. Hypomania, anxiety, and the

emotional Stroop. British Journal of Clinical Psychology 35, 617-626.

Goodwin, F. K., Jamison, K. R., 1990. Manic- depressive illness. Oxford University Press,

New York, NY.

Page 23: Thomas D. Meyer a * & Silke Maier a, beprint.ncl.ac.uk/file_store/production/18578/B1CBA... · In Germany there is a long tradition to see the so-called Typus melancholicus as a premorbid

Running title: Evidence for social rhythm ... / V 2.1 23

Hammen, C., Goodman Brown, T., 1990. Self-schemas and vulnerability to specific life

stress in children at risk for depression. Cognitive Therapy and Research 14, 215-227.

Healy, D., 1987. Rhythm and blues. Neurochemical, neuropharmacological and

neuropsychological implications of a hypothesis of circadian rhythm dysfunction in affective

disorders. Psychopharmacology 93, 271- 285.

Healy, D., Williams, J.M.G., 1989. Moods, misattributions and mania: An interaction of

biological and psychological factors in the pathogenesis of mania. Psychiatric Developments

1, 49-70.

Heerlein, A., Santander, J., Richter, P., 1996. Premorbid personality aspects in mood and

schizophrenic disorders. Comprehensive Psychiatry 37, 430-434.

Hewitt, P.L., Flett, G.L., Ediger, E., 1996. Perfectionism and depression: longitudinal

assessment of a specific vulnerability hypothesis. Journal of Abnormal Psychology 105, 276-

280.

Johnson, S. L., Roberts, J. E., 1995. Life events and Bipolar Disorder: Implication From

Biological Theories. Psychological Bulletin 117, 434- 449.

Jones, S.H., 2001. Circadian rhythms, multilevel models of emotion and bipolar disorder -

An initial step towards integration? Clinical Psychology Review 21, 1193-1209

Kanner, A., Coyne, J. C., Schaefer, C., 1981. Comparison of two models of stress

management: Daily hassles and uplifts versus major life events. Journal of Behavioral

Medicine 4, 1-6.

Kenney-Benson, G.A., Pomerantz, E.M., 2005. The role of mother’s use of control in

chidrenm’s perfectionism: implications for the development of children’s depressive

symptoms. Journal of Personality 73, 23-46.

Klein, D. N., Anderson, R.L., 1995. The behavioral high-risk paradigm in the mood

disorders. In G. A. Miller (Eds.), The behavioral high-risk paradigm in psychopathology.

Springer, New York, pp. 199-221

Page 24: Thomas D. Meyer a * & Silke Maier a, beprint.ncl.ac.uk/file_store/production/18578/B1CBA... · In Germany there is a long tradition to see the so-called Typus melancholicus as a premorbid

Running title: Evidence for social rhythm ... / V 2.1 24

Klein, D. N., Lewinsohn, P. M., Seeley, J. R., 1996. Hypomanic personality traits in a

community sample of adolescents. Journal of Affective Disorders 38, 135- 143.

Krumm-Merabet, C., Meyer, T.D., 2005. Leisure activities, alcohol and nicotine

consumption in people putatively at risk for bipolar disorders. Personality and Individual

Differences 38, 701-712.

Kwapil, T. R., Miller, M. B., Zinser, M. C., Chapman, L. J., Chapman, J., Eckblad, M.,

2000. A longitudinal Study of High Scorers on the Hypomanic Personality Scale. Journal of

Abnormal Psychology 109, 222- 226.

Lauer, C. J., Bronisch, T., Kainz, M., Schreiber, W., Holsboer, F., Krieg, J.- C., 1997. Pre-

morbid psychometric profile of subjects at high familial risk for affective disorders.

Psychological Medicine 27, 355- 463.

Leibenluft, E., Albert, P. S., Rosenthal, N. E., Wehr, T. A., 1996. Relationship between

sleep and mood in patients with rapid cycling bipolar disorder. Psychiatry Research 63, 161-

168.

Lovejoy, C. M., Steuerwald, B. D., 1997. Subsyndromal Unipolar and Bipolar Disorders

II: Comparisons on Daily Stress Levels. Cognitive Therapy and Research 21, 607- 618.

Lyon, H.M., Startup, M., Bentall, R.P., 1999. Social cognition and the manic defense:

Attributions, selective attention, and self-schema in bipolar affective disorder. Journal of

Abnormal Psychology 108, 273-282.

Maier, W., Lichtermann, D., Minges, J., Heun, R., 1992. Personality traits in subjects at

risk for unipolar major depression: A family study perspective. Journal of Affective Disorders

24, 153-164.

Maier, W., Lichtermann, D., Minges, J., Heun, R., 1995. Personality disorders and

personality variations in relatives of patients with bipolar affective disorders. Journal of

Affective Disoders 53, 173- 181.

Page 25: Thomas D. Meyer a * & Silke Maier a, beprint.ncl.ac.uk/file_store/production/18578/B1CBA... · In Germany there is a long tradition to see the so-called Typus melancholicus as a premorbid

Running title: Evidence for social rhythm ... / V 2.1 25

Malkoff-Schwartz, S., Frank, E., Anderson, B. P., Hlastala, S. A., Luther, J. F., Sherrill, J.

T., Houck, P. R., Kupfer, D. J., 2000. Social rhythm disruption and stressful life-events in the

onset of bipolar and unipolar episodes. Psychological Medicine 30, 1005- 1016.

Malkoff-Schwartz, S., Frank, E., Anderson, B., Sherrill, J. T., Siegel, L., Patterson, D.,

Kupfer, D. J., 1998. Stressful life events and social rhythm disruption in the onset of manic

and depressive bipolar episode. Archives of General Psychiatry 55, 702- 707.

Mc Guffin, P., Fruhling, R., Andrew, M., Sham, P., Katz, R., Cardno, A., 2003. The

Heritability of Bipolar Affective Disorder and the Genetic Relationship to Unipolar

Depression. Archives of General Psychiatry 60, 497- 502.

Meyer, T. D., 2002a. The Hypomanic Personality Scale, the Big Five and their relationship

to depression and mania. Personality and Individual Differences 32, 659- 660.

Meyer, T.D., 2002b. Correlates of the Hypomanic Personality Scale: Results from a family

sample. Personality and Individual Differences 32, 3-14.

Meyer, T. D., Drüke, B., Hautzinger, M., 2000. Hypomane Persönlichkeit-

Psychometrische Evaluation und erste Ergebnisse zur Validität der deutschen Version der

Chapman- Skala. Zeitschrift für Klinische Psychologie und Psychotherapie 29, 35- 42.

Meyer, T. D., Hautzinger, M., 2001. Hypomanic Personality, Social Anhedonia and

Impulsive Nonconformity: Evidence for familial aggregation? Journal of Personality

Disorders 15, 281-299.

Meyer, T. D., Hautzinger, M., 2003. Screening for bipolar disorders using the Hypomanic

Personality Scale. Journal of Affective Disorders 75, 149- 154.

Meyer, T.D., Keller, F., 2003. Is there evidence for a latent class called “hypomanic

temperament”? Journal of Affective Disorders 75, 259-267.

Meyer, T.D., Krumm-Merabet, C., 2003. Academic performance and expectations for the

future in relation to a vulnerability marker for bipolar disorders: the hypomanic temperament.

Personality and Individual Differences 35, 785-796.

Page 26: Thomas D. Meyer a * & Silke Maier a, beprint.ncl.ac.uk/file_store/production/18578/B1CBA... · In Germany there is a long tradition to see the so-called Typus melancholicus as a premorbid

Running title: Evidence for social rhythm ... / V 2.1 26

Monk, T. H., Flaherty, J. F., Frank, E., Hoskinson, K., Kupfer, D. J., 1990. The social

rhythm metric. An instrument to quantify the daily rhythms of life. Journal of Nervous and

Mental Disease 178, 120- 126.

Monk, T. H., Kupfer, D. J., Frank, E., Ritenour, A. M., 1991. The social rhythm metric

(SRM): Measuring daily social rhythm over 12 weeks. Psychiatry Research 36, 195- 207.

Monk, T. H., Frank, E., Potts, J. M., Kupfer, D. J., 2002. A simple way to measure lifestyle

regularity. Journal of Sleep Research 11, 183- 190.

Mundt, Ch., Backenstrass, M., Kronmüller, Fiedler, P., Kraus, A., Stangbellini, G., 1999.

Personality and endogeneous/major depression: An empirical approach to Typus

melancholicus. Psychopathology 30, 130-139.

Nurnberger, J. I., Gershon, E. S., 1992. Genetics. In E. S. Psykel (Eds.): Handbook of

affective disorders. Edinburgh, Churchill- Livingstone, pp. 131- 148.

Paykel, E.S., 2003. Life events and affective disorders. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica 108

(Suppl. 408), 61-66.

Rehm, L. P., 1978. Mood, pleasent events, and unpleasant events. Two pilot studies.

Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology 46, 854- 859.

Tellenbach, H., 1961. Melancholie (4th ed. 1983). Springer Verlag, Berlin, Heidelberg.

Wehr, T. A., Goodwin, F. K., Wirz- Justice, A., Breitmeier, J., Craig, C., 1982. 48- hour

sleep-wake cycles in manic-depressive illness: Naturalistic observations and sleep-

deprivation experiments. Archives of General Psychiatry 39, 559- 565.

Wehr, T. A., Goodwin, F. K., 1983. Biological rhythms in manic-depressive illness. In:

Wehr, T.A., Goodwin, F. K. (Eds.): Circadian Rhythms in Psychiatry. The Boxwood Press,

Pacific Grove, CA, pp. 129- 184.

Wehr, T. A., Sack, D. A., Rosenthal, N. E., 1987. Sleep reduction as a final common

pathway in the genesis of mania. American Journal of Psychiatry 144, 201- 204.

Page 27: Thomas D. Meyer a * & Silke Maier a, beprint.ncl.ac.uk/file_store/production/18578/B1CBA... · In Germany there is a long tradition to see the so-called Typus melancholicus as a premorbid

Running title: Evidence for social rhythm ... / V 2.1 27

Wehr, T. A., 1990. Effects of sleep and wakefulness in depression and mania. In:

Montplaisir, J., Godbout, R. (Eds.). Sleep and Biological Rhythms. Oxford University Press,

London.

Zerssen, D. von, Pfister, H., Koeller, D.- M., 1988. The Munich Personality Test (MPT)- a

short questionnaire for self- rating and relatives´ rating of personality traits: Formal

properties and clinical potential. European Archives of Psychiatry and Neurological Science

238, 73- 93.

Zerssen, D., von, 1977. Premorbid personality and affective psychoses. In: Burrows, G. D.

(Eds.), Handbook of Studies on Depression. Excerpta Medica, Amsterdam, pp. 79- 103.

Zerssen, D., von, 1996. „Melancholic“ and „manic“ types of personality as premorbid

structures in affective disorders. In C. Mundt, M.J. Goldstein, K. Hahlweg & P. Fiedler

(Eds.), Interpersonal factors in the origin and course of affective disorders, Gaskell, London,

pp. 65- 85.

Page 28: Thomas D. Meyer a * & Silke Maier a, beprint.ncl.ac.uk/file_store/production/18578/B1CBA... · In Germany there is a long tradition to see the so-called Typus melancholicus as a premorbid

Running title: Evidence for social rhythm ... / V 2.1 28

Table 1:

Sample description and Distribution of affective disorders depending on risk group status

Control

Unipolar Risk Group

Bipolar Risk

Group

N = 48

N= 37

N = 56

N women (%)

34 (70.8 %)

27 (73.0 %)

39 (69.6 %)

Affective Disorders

Bipolar

0

1

3

Major Depression

8

4

6

Mean (SD) Mean (SD) Mean (SD)

Age 18.33 2.10 18.32 2.11 17.95 2.20 HPS score 16.19 4.55 15.81 4.88 33.78 3.91

Rigidity score 1.85 1.13 5.45 0.57 3.13 2.19

Notes. Unipolar Risk = High scores on the Rigidity Scale; Bipolar Risk = High scores on the

HPS; HPS = Hypomanic Personality Scale (Eckblad & Chapman, 1986); Rigidity = Rigidity

scale from the MPI (Zerssen et al., 1988). Diagnoses were assessed with SKID I (First et al.,

1995).

Page 29: Thomas D. Meyer a * & Silke Maier a, beprint.ncl.ac.uk/file_store/production/18578/B1CBA... · In Germany there is a long tradition to see the so-called Typus melancholicus as a premorbid

Running title: Evidence for social rhythm ... / V 2.1 29

Table 2:

Means and Standard Deviation of Social Rhythm Score, intraindividual Stability of Social

Rhythm Score over four weeks, Sleep Duration and Stability of Sleep over four weeks

Control Unipolar Risk

Group Bipolar Risk

Group N = 48 N = 37 N = 56

Mean (SD) Mean (SD) Mean (SD)

SRS

F (2, 141)= 4.61, p= 0.01

2.47 0.81 2.71 0.94 2. 19 0.75

Sleep Duration

F (2, 138)= 0.01, n.s.

469.04 47.56 468.60 54.33 473.10 62.92

Stability of Sleep

F (2, 134)= 5.53, p< 0.005

88.57 30.39 89.13 27.47 145.43 142.58

Notes. Unipolar Risk = High scores on the Rigidity Scale; Bipolar Risk = High scores on the

HPS; HPS = Hypomanic Personality Scale.

SRS= Score of the Social Rhythm Metric (Monk et al., 1990); Sleep Duration= minutes of

sleep per night; Stability of Sleep= Intraindividual Variation of the sleeping duration over

four weeks

Page 30: Thomas D. Meyer a * & Silke Maier a, beprint.ncl.ac.uk/file_store/production/18578/B1CBA... · In Germany there is a long tradition to see the so-called Typus melancholicus as a premorbid

Running title: Evidence for social rhythm ... / V 2.1 30

Table 3: Retest- Correlations of the Social Rhythm Scores for each week with the following

Notes: There were no significant differences in retest-correlations between groups if one averaged the coefficients over the weeks (all z < 0.3, n.s.); Mean correlation were obtained through Fisher’s Z transformation. * P < 0.05 ** P < 0.01

Total

(N= 141)

Control (N = 48)

Unipolar Risk Group (N= 37)

Bipolar Risk Group

(N= 56)

week 1 * week2

0.17*

0.04 0.25

0.23

week 1 * week 3

0.30**

0.13 0.41**

0.15

week 1 * week 4

0.32**

0.30* 0.31

0.25

week 2 * week3

0.24**

0.20 0.26

0.14

week 2 * week 4

0.27**

0.37** 0.14

0.23

week 3 * week 4

0.43**

0.37** 0.36*

0.40**

Estimated Mean

Correlation

0.29

0.25

0.29

0.23