26
The notion of derivations in linguistics: Syntax Howard Lasnik U. Of Maryland (MIT 1972) 50 years of Linguistics at MIT December 11, 2011

The notion of derivations in linguistics: Syntax - 50 years of

  • Upload
    others

  • View
    3

  • Download
    0

Embed Size (px)

Citation preview

Page 1: The notion of derivations in linguistics: Syntax - 50 years of
Page 2: The notion of derivations in linguistics: Syntax - 50 years of
Page 3: The notion of derivations in linguistics: Syntax - 50 years of
Page 4: The notion of derivations in linguistics: Syntax - 50 years of
Page 5: The notion of derivations in linguistics: Syntax - 50 years of
Page 6: The notion of derivations in linguistics: Syntax - 50 years of
Page 7: The notion of derivations in linguistics: Syntax - 50 years of
Page 8: The notion of derivations in linguistics: Syntax - 50 years of
Page 9: The notion of derivations in linguistics: Syntax - 50 years of
Page 10: The notion of derivations in linguistics: Syntax - 50 years of
Page 11: The notion of derivations in linguistics: Syntax - 50 years of
Page 12: The notion of derivations in linguistics: Syntax - 50 years of
Page 13: The notion of derivations in linguistics: Syntax - 50 years of

13

D‐Structure

S‐Structure

Given the full Aspects modification, this list of rules applies cyclically, first operating on the most deeply embedded clause, then the next most deeply embedded, and so on, working up the tree until they apply on the highest clause, the entire generalized P-marker

Thus, singulary transformations apply to constituent sentences as if 'before' they are embedded, and to matrix sentences as if 'after' embedding has taken place. "The ordering possibilities that are permitted by the theory of Transformation-markers but apparently never put to use are now excluded in principle." Chomsky (1965, p.135)

Page 14: The notion of derivations in linguistics: Syntax - 50 years of

In passing, I note that the cyclic principle was one of two major syntactic innovations of the mid 1960s borrowed from phonology.

The principle was first formulated in Chomsky et al. (1956), and applied in the phonological analysis of a variety of languages from the early 1960s on, most notably in SPE (Chomsky and Halle (1968)).

14

"... it is natural to suppose that in general the phonetic shapeof a complex unit ... will be determined by the inherent properties of its parts and the manner in which these parts are combined, and that similar rules will apply to units of different levels of complexity. These observations suggest a general principle for the application of rules of the phonological component, namely, what we will call the principle of the 'transformational cycle.'" [SPE p.15]

Page 15: The notion of derivations in linguistics: Syntax - 50 years of

15

Interestingly, it was almost three decades before it became clear that the absence of certain kinds of derivations was not a real argument against generalized transformations.

For it was not recursion in the base that excluded the unwanted derivations: It was the cyclic principle. And there are a variety of ways that a version of the cyclic principle could be grafted onto a theory with generalized transformations, as work of the last decade and a half has amply demonstrated.

Page 16: The notion of derivations in linguistics: Syntax - 50 years of

16

Derivations and interfaces

In LSLT the T-marker is the interface with semantic interpretation, and the final derived syntactic representation is the interface with morphophonemics.

In Aspects, we have:

Page 17: The notion of derivations in linguistics: Syntax - 50 years of

17

Already in Aspects, it was pointed out that some aspects of semantic interpretation (basically all except thematic relations) depend on derived structure. With the introduction of trace theory in the early 1970’s, D-structure was no longer crucial even for thematic relations.

That led to the ‘Extended Standard Theory’ and GB model:

Page 18: The notion of derivations in linguistics: Syntax - 50 years of

18

BUT, there had already been suggestions in the late 1960’s and very early 1970’s for links to semantic and phonological interpretation not just at levels but internal to the derivation.

Particularly important were the work of Bresnan (1971) wrt phonology (specifically sentence intonation) and Jackendoff (1969, 1972) wrt semantics (specifically anaphora).

For both, the derivation-internal points were end of cycle structures.

The picture then would look like:

Page 19: The notion of derivations in linguistics: Syntax - 50 years of

19

D‐Structure

S‐Structure

(Some) semantic interpretation

(Some) semantic and phonological interpretation(Some) semantic and phonological interpretation(Some) semantic and phonological interpretation(Some) semantic and phonological interpretation(Some) semantic and phonological interpretation

(Some) semantic and phonological interpretation

Page 20: The notion of derivations in linguistics: Syntax - 50 years of

20

Going further back in time, recall the Aspects arguments against GTs.

The one based on non-occurring derivations turned out to miss the mark, as it was the cyclic principle, not recursion in the base that blocked those derivations.

The simplicity argument took for granted that PS rules exist, and that the Aspects modification thus reduced the class of basic operations from 3 to 2 (just singularyTs and PS rules).

But if there are no PS rules (as suggested by McCawley and again by Lasnikand Kupin) …

Page 21: The notion of derivations in linguistics: Syntax - 50 years of

21

An early, or middle-aged, Minimalist model of structure building where everything is done by transformations.

GTs don’t just merge clausal structures with clausal structures, but even lexical items with lexical items. The derivation begins with a selection of lexical items (the ‘numeration’), which are then combined; these combinations are then combined with other lexical items or other combinations, and so on.

Meanwhile, interspersed with these instances of ‘external merge’ we also have ‘internal merge’, the classic singulary movement transformation. All theseoperations are constrained by some version of cyclicity (for example, alwaysmerge at the ‘root’ of the ‘tree’).

Eventually (the point of ‘spellout’), the derivation branches to PF and LF.

Page 22: The notion of derivations in linguistics: Syntax - 50 years of

22

Numeration

Spellout

PF LF

Page 23: The notion of derivations in linguistics: Syntax - 50 years of

23

There are, though, a number of reasons for thinking that Bresnan and Jackendoffwere fundamentally correct about the derivational nature of the interface relations. Among many examples, I could mention Barss (1986) on ‘reconstruction’ into intermediate psoitions; McCoskey (1991) and Torrego (1984) on intermediate footprints of wh-movement; Uriagereka (1999) on deriving certain island constraints; Fox and Pesetsky (2003) on forcing successive cyclic movement; and Merchant (2001), based on Perlmutter (1971), on distinctions between representational and derivational constraints.

Grafting Bresnan’s and Jackendoff’s ideas ideas onto the early minimalist model, we wind up with a ‘Mutiple Spellout’ (Uriagereka (1999)) or Derivation by Phase (Chomsky (2001)) picture.

Page 24: The notion of derivations in linguistics: Syntax - 50 years of

24

Numeration

End of derivation

Semantic and phonological interpretationSemantic and phonological interpretationSemantic and phonological interpretationSemantic and phonological interpretationSemantic and phonological interpretation

Page 25: The notion of derivations in linguistics: Syntax - 50 years of

25

What I find fascinating about all these developments is that the same ideas (and sometimes even arguments) keep coming back in slightly revised forms, and combining in slightly different ways. Does this mean these ideas must be right? Or is it just a poverty of imagination situation? I hope I’ll last long enough to find out. Maybe at the 100th anniversary?

Page 26: The notion of derivations in linguistics: Syntax - 50 years of

B a rs s , A n d re w . 1 9 8 6 . C h a in s a n d a n a p h o r i c d e p e n d e n c e : O n r e c o n s tr u c tio n a n d itsim p lic a tio n s . D o c to ra l d is s er t a tio n , M IT , C a m b r id g e , M a ss .

B re s n a n , J o a n W . 1 9 7 1 . S e n te n ce s tr es s a n d s y n ta c tic tr a n s fo rm a tio n s . L a n g u a g e 4 7 : 2 5 7 -2 8 1 . C h o m sk y , N o a m . 1 9 5 5 . T h e lo g ic a l stru c tu re o f lin g u istic th e o ry . M s . H a rv a rd U n iv e r s it y ,

C a m b r id g e , M a ss . a n d M IT , C a m b r id g e, M a s s. , . [R e v is ed 1 9 5 6 v e r s io n p u b lis h e d in p a r t b yP le n u m , N ew Y o rk , 1 9 7 5 ; U n iv er s ity o f C h ic a g o P res s , C h ica g o , 1 9 8 5 ] .

C h o m sk y , N o a m . 1 9 6 5 . A s p e c ts o f t h e th e o r y o f s y n ta x . C a m b r id g e , M a s s. : M IT p re ss . C h o m sk y , N o a m . 2 0 0 1 . D e r iv a tio n b y p h a s e. In K e n H a le : A l ife in la n g u a g e , ed . M ic h a e l

K e n s to w ic z, 1 -5 2 . C a m b r id g e , M a ss . : M IT P res s. C h o m sk y , N o a m a n d M o r r is H a l le . 1 9 6 8 . T h e s o u n d p a tte r n o f E n g lis h . N ew Y o rk : H a rp e r a n d

R o w . C h o m sk y , N o a m , M o r r is H a lle a n d F red L u k o ff . 1 9 5 6 . O n a c ce n t a n d ju n c tu re in E n g lis h . In F o r

R o m a n J a k o b s o n , e d . M . H a lle , H . L u n t, a n d H . M a c L ea n , 6 5 -8 0 . T h e H a g u e : M o u to n &C o .

F illm o re , C h a r les J . 1 9 6 3 . T h e p o s itio n o f e m b ed d in g tr a n s fo rm a t io n s in a g ra m m a r . W o r d 1 9 : 2 0 8 -2 3 1 .

F o x , D a n n y a n d D a v id P es e tsk y . 2 0 0 3 . C y c lic lin e a r iz a tio n a n d th e t y p o lo g y o f m o v e m e n t. M s.M IT , C a m b r id g e, M a s s.

J a c k en d o ff, R a y . 1 9 6 9 . S o m e r u le s o f s e m a n tic in te r p r e ta tio n fo r E n g lis h . D o cto ra l d is se r ta tio n ,M IT , C a m b r id g e, M a s s.

J a c k en d o ff, R a y . 1 9 7 2 . S e m a n tic in te r p r e ta tio n in g e n e r a t iv e g r a m m a r . C a m b r id g e , M a ss . : M ITP res s.

L a s n ik , H o w a rd a n d J o se p h J . K u p in . 1 9 7 7 . A re s tr ic t iv e th e o ry o f t r a n s fo rm a tio n a l g ra m m a r .T h e o r e t ic a l L in g u is t ic s 4 : 1 7 3 -1 9 6 . [R e p r in ted in E ss a y s o n r e str ic tiv e n e ss a n d le a rn a b lity ,H o w a rd L a s n ik , 1 7 -4 1 . D o rd re c h t: K lu w e r , 1 9 9 0 ]

M cC a w le y , J a m e s . 1 9 6 8 . C o n ce rn in g th e b a se c o m p o n e n t o f a tr a n s fo rm a tio n a l g ra m m a r .F o u n d a t io n s o f L a n g u a g e 4 : 2 4 3 -2 6 9 .

M cC lo sk e y , J a m e s . 1 9 9 1 . R e s u m p tiv e p ro n o u n s , A '-b in d in g , a n d le v e ls o f r ep res e n ta tio n in I r is h . InS y n ta x a n d S e m a n t ic s 2 3 : T h e S y n ta x o f th e M o d e r n C e ltic L a n g u a g e s , ed . R a n d a llH e n d r ic k , 1 9 9 -2 4 8 . N ew Y o rk : A c a d e m ic P res s.

M erc h a n t, J a so n . 2 0 0 1 . T h e s y n ta x o f s ile n c e : S lu ic in g , is la n d s , a n d t h e th e o r y o f e llip s i s . O x fo rd :O x fo rd U n iv e rs ity P re ss .

P er lm u tte r , D a v id . 1 9 7 1 . D e e p a n d s u r fa ce c o n s tr a in ts in s y n ta x . N e w Y o rk : H o lt, R in e h a r t a n dW in s to n .

T o r re g o , E s th e r . 1 9 8 4 . O n in v e r s io n in S p a n is h a n d s o m e o f its e ffe c ts. L in g u is tic In q u ir y 1 5 : 1 0 3 -1 2 9 .

U r ia g e rek a , J u a n . 1 9 9 9 . M u lt ip le s p e ll- o u t. In W o r k in g m in im a lis m , ed . S a m u e l D a v id E p s te in a n dN o rb e r t H o rn s te in , 2 5 1 -2 8 2 . C a m b r id g e , M a s s. : M IT P res s.