10
The literacy abilities of 11 year-old students from socially disadvantaged backgrounds BETH MCINTOSH 1,2 , MARGARET TAYLOR 3 , SHARON CROSBIE 1,2 , ALISON HOLM 1,2 , & BARBARA DODD 1,2 1 Perinatal Research Centre, University of Queensland, Australia, 2 Royal Brisbane and Women’s Hospital, Queensland, Australia, and 3 Education Queensland, Queensland, Australia Abstract This study investigated the literacy skills of all children in Year 6 of a Queensland school in a socially disadvantaged area who were exposed to a ‘‘whole language’’ approach to the learning of written language. Standardized tests of non-word spelling and reading comprehension (including written sentence responses) revealed a large group of children with literacy difficulties. This group of 11-year-old children, and a group of matched controls, were further assessed on measures of phonological awareness and letter sound knowledge. The results indicated that the means of both groups were below those of the standardization populations, and that the children had particular difficulty writing short answers to questions. The group of children identified as performing poorly on the non-word spelling test was heterogeneous. Some had poor phonological awareness skills, some had limited sound-letter correspondence knowledge, some children had problems with both skills. The data demonstrated the need for assessment to identify the specific sub-skills with which individuals have difficulty, in order to allow for appropriate intervention rather than a generic phonological awareness intervention package for all children with literacy difficulties irrespective of age or social status. Keywords: Literacy, socio-economic background, phonological awareness, language comprehension, upper primary. Introduction The academic performance of children entering secondary schools depends upon their comprehen- sion of written text and their ability to produce well structured written language (Leahy & Dodd, 2002; Roberts, 2004). Poor literacy is associated with acade- mic failure, behaviour problems and reduced moti- vation that limits children’s ability to achieve their potential (High, LaGasse, Becker, Ahlgren, & Gardner, 2000). While the children reported to be most at risk for spoken and written language diffi- culties are those from socially deprived backgrounds (Bowey, 1995; Burt, Holm, & Dodd, 1999; Dodd & Carr, 2003; Duncan & Seymour, 2000; Locke, Ginsborg, & Peers, 2002; Raz & Bryant, 1990), Coomber (1997) contends that it is simplistic to equate poverty and illiteracy. Rather, explanations for literacy failure need to focus on the learning environment. One aspect of the literacy learning environment is teaching methodology. For example, research sug- gests that the whole language teaching approach adopted by many schools does not explicitly link speech sounds and letters so that children can acquire the alphabetic principle (e.g., van Kraayenoord & Paris, 1994). In addition, some children may receive little literacy learning experience outside school. Children from disadvantaged backgrounds are as- sumed to be at greater risk of not gaining adequate exposure to written text at home (Adams, 1994; Fisher, 1992; Moon & Wells, 1979). The study reported here examines the literacy abilities of all children, at the end of Year 6, who attend a school in a socially disadvantaged area, to establish the pre- valence and nature of any literacy difficulties. The links between social disadvantage, language and literacy skills A US Department of Education (2001) analysis indicated that 46% of children entering kindergarten came from family backgrounds with one or more factors that might affect their skills and knowledge. The risk factors included were living in a single- parent household, living in poverty, having a mother with low education, and coming from a non-English speaking background. The performance of children with one risk factor lagged behind that of their Correspondence: Barbara Dodd, Level 6 Ned Hanlon Building, Royal Brisbane and Women’s Hospital, Herston, QLD 4029, Australia. Tel: þ61 7 3636 4011. Fax: þ61 7 3636 1769. E-mail: [email protected] Advances in Speech–Language Pathology, June 2007; 9(2): 181 – 190 ISSN 1441-7049 print/ISSN 1742-9528 online ª The Speech Pathology Association of Australia Limited Published by Informa UK Ltd. DOI: 10.1080/14417040600970598 Int J Speech Lang Pathol Downloaded from informahealthcare.com by Mcgill University on 11/24/14 For personal use only.

The literacy abilities of 11 year-old students from socially disadvantaged backgrounds

  • Upload
    barbara

  • View
    214

  • Download
    2

Embed Size (px)

Citation preview

Page 1: The literacy abilities of 11 year-old students from socially disadvantaged backgrounds

The literacy abilities of 11 year-old students from sociallydisadvantaged backgrounds

BETH MCINTOSH1,2, MARGARET TAYLOR3, SHARON CROSBIE1,2,

ALISON HOLM1,2, & BARBARA DODD1,2

1Perinatal Research Centre, University of Queensland, Australia, 2Royal Brisbane and Women’s Hospital, Queensland,

Australia, and 3Education Queensland, Queensland, Australia

AbstractThis study investigated the literacy skills of all children in Year 6 of a Queensland school in a socially disadvantaged area whowere exposed to a ‘‘whole language’’ approach to the learning of written language. Standardized tests of non-word spellingand reading comprehension (including written sentence responses) revealed a large group of children with literacydifficulties. This group of 11-year-old children, and a group of matched controls, were further assessed on measures ofphonological awareness and letter sound knowledge. The results indicated that the means of both groups were below those ofthe standardization populations, and that the children had particular difficulty writing short answers to questions. The groupof children identified as performing poorly on the non-word spelling test was heterogeneous. Some had poor phonologicalawareness skills, some had limited sound-letter correspondence knowledge, some children had problems with both skills.The data demonstrated the need for assessment to identify the specific sub-skills with which individuals have difficulty, inorder to allow for appropriate intervention rather than a generic phonological awareness intervention package for all childrenwith literacy difficulties irrespective of age or social status.

Keywords: Literacy, socio-economic background, phonological awareness, language comprehension, upper primary.

Introduction

The academic performance of children entering

secondary schools depends upon their comprehen-

sion of written text and their ability to produce well

structured written language (Leahy & Dodd, 2002;

Roberts, 2004). Poor literacy is associated with acade-

mic failure, behaviour problems and reduced moti-

vation that limits children’s ability to achieve

their potential (High, LaGasse, Becker, Ahlgren, &

Gardner, 2000). While the children reported to be

most at risk for spoken and written language diffi-

culties are those from socially deprived backgrounds

(Bowey, 1995; Burt, Holm, & Dodd, 1999; Dodd &

Carr, 2003; Duncan & Seymour, 2000; Locke,

Ginsborg, & Peers, 2002; Raz & Bryant, 1990),

Coomber (1997) contends that it is simplistic to equate

poverty and illiteracy. Rather, explanations for literacy

failure need to focus on the learning environment.

One aspect of the literacy learning environment is

teaching methodology. For example, research sug-

gests that the whole language teaching approach

adopted by many schools does not explicitly link

speech sounds and letters so that children can acquire

the alphabetic principle (e.g., van Kraayenoord &

Paris, 1994). In addition, some children may receive

little literacy learning experience outside school.

Children from disadvantaged backgrounds are as-

sumed to be at greater risk of not gaining adequate

exposure to written text at home (Adams, 1994;

Fisher, 1992; Moon & Wells, 1979). The study

reported here examines the literacy abilities of all

children, at the end of Year 6, who attend a school in

a socially disadvantaged area, to establish the pre-

valence and nature of any literacy difficulties.

The links between social disadvantage, language

and literacy skills

A US Department of Education (2001) analysis

indicated that 46% of children entering kindergarten

came from family backgrounds with one or more

factors that might affect their skills and knowledge.

The risk factors included were living in a single-

parent household, living in poverty, having a mother

with low education, and coming from a non-English

speaking background. The performance of children

with one risk factor lagged behind that of their

Correspondence: Barbara Dodd, Level 6 Ned Hanlon Building, Royal Brisbane and Women’s Hospital, Herston, QLD 4029, Australia. Tel: þ61 7 3636 4011.

Fax: þ61 7 3636 1769. E-mail: [email protected]

Advances in Speech–Language Pathology, June 2007; 9(2): 181 – 190

ISSN 1441-7049 print/ISSN 1742-9528 online ª The Speech Pathology Association of Australia Limited

Published by Informa UK Ltd.

DOI: 10.1080/14417040600970598

Int J

Spe

ech

Lan

g Pa

thol

Dow

nloa

ded

from

info

rmah

ealth

care

.com

by

Mcg

ill U

nive

rsity

on

11/2

4/14

For

pers

onal

use

onl

y.

Page 2: The literacy abilities of 11 year-old students from socially disadvantaged backgrounds

non-disadvantaged peers on assessments of both

reading and mathematics. Nearly half the children

with multiple risk factors performed in the lowest

quartile, typically not knowing letter names or being

aware of sound-letter associations. Similar results

have been reported for children across the primary

school years, for a range of language and literacy

skills, in both the USA and UK (Burt, Holm, &

Dodd, 1999; Duncan & Seymour, 2000; Hecht,

Burgess, Torgesen, Wagner, & Rashotte, 2000;

Locke et al., 2002). Locke et al. (2002) found that

the spoken language abilities of preschool children

reared in poverty were significantly below those of

the general population, despite the children’s cogni-

tive abilities being comparable. They concluded that

these children may be at risk for delayed develop-

ment of written language skills, which could in

turn affect overall academic progress. Duncan and

Seymour (2000) reported that children entering

school in areas of low socio-economic status (SES)

showed a delay in word recognition tasks. This

finding confirmed previous research indicating that

children from low SES backgrounds had delayed

phonological awareness and literacy development,

in comparison with high SES age-matched peers

(Bowey, 1995; Raz & Bryant, 1990).

Parents and family provide a framework that allows

the child to learn (Fisher, 1992). Preschool children

observe their caregivers’ functional and recreational

interaction with print. The home environment deter-

mines attitudes about literacy and may optimize or

minimize learning. Consequently, children begin

school with very different experiences and attitudes

about print (Moon & Wells, 1979). Some find literacy

work in the first year of school a clarification of what

they already know, while others struggle with minimal

knowledge of print and poor phonological awareness.

Adams (1994) argues that some children can therefore

be expected to learn more slowly and to experience

greater frustration. Statistics from the US Department

of Education (2001) indicate that at school entry,

children from disadvantaged backgrounds are more

likely to have poorer health, to be less socially adept and

to have low motivation to learn. From their first literacy

lesson, they are disadvantaged. The disadvantage

experienced during the first year of school can have

long-term educational implications. For example,

Jefferis, Power and Hertzman (2002) longitudinally

investigated the effect of social class and birthweight on

the cognitive trajectories and educational attainment

(including literacy) of nearly 11,000 children born in

1958. Participants were assessed at 7, 11, 16 and 33

years. The results showed a strong influence of social

class through to adulthood.

Literacy development and the links with phonological

awareness skills

The positive link between literacy development and

phonological awareness (PA) skills has been long

established (Bryant, MacLean, Bradley, &

Crossland, 1990; Hatcher & Hulme, 1999; Høien,

Lundberg, Stanovich, & Bjaalid, 1995; Stahl &

Murray, 1994). Both Høien et al. (1995) and

Hatcher and Hulme (1999) found that it was

possible to split phonological awareness skills into

independent factors—namely syllable level skills,

onset-rime skills and phoneme level skills—and that

phoneme manipulation skills best predicted the

acquisition of literacy. Duncan and Seymour

(2000) stated that letter-sound knowledge—a pho-

neme level task—is an important pre-requisite for

development through three stages of literacy acquisi-

tion: the logographic stage, the alphabetic stage and

the orthographic phase. They argued that the

logographic and alphabetic stages develop in tandem,

before leading on to the orthographic phase, with

letter-sound knowledge essential to the development

of the first two processes. In contrast, Frith (1985)

argued that letter-sound knowledge is not involved in

the logographic phase of reading, and only develops

during the alphabetic phase. Both agree that letter-

sound knowledge is essential for the development of

literacy acquisition, as is phonological awareness.

The two skills allow children to decode words they

have never read before, and to invent spellings for

words (Gillon & Dodd, 2005). Share (1995) argued

that early literacy activities at home (e.g., shared

reading with parents and writing shopping lists),

combined with instruction at school, provide specific

information about phoneme-grapheme relationships

that promotes ‘‘self-teaching’’ in reading.

Literacy development and the links with language

comprehension

The role of reading comprehension is receiving

increasing research attention, since some children

who fail to acquire adequate literacy have phonolo-

gical awareness skills within normal limits (Catts,

Adlof, & Ellis Weismer, 2006). Bishop and Adams

(1990) concluded that although expressive phonolo-

gical skills play a part in the development of literacy

skills, ‘‘the importance of phonological processing

may have been previously overstated’’ (p. 1046).

Their findings indicated that it is language abilities

that exert major influence on reading progress.

Recent studies suggest that reading comprehension

is correlated with spoken language comprehension

(Catts et al., 2006). Some children, despite having

adequate phonological awareness skills, fail to

acquire literacy because they have poor comprehen-

sion, which seems to be due to semantic difficulties

in both spoken and written modalities (Nation &

Snowling, 2000).

Hogan, Catts and Little (2005) reported that while

phonological awareness in kindergarten predicts

word reading 2 years later, it looses its predictive

power once children are older. Current research

therefore suggests that there is a need to identify

182 B. McIntosh et al.

Int J

Spe

ech

Lan

g Pa

thol

Dow

nloa

ded

from

info

rmah

ealth

care

.com

by

Mcg

ill U

nive

rsity

on

11/2

4/14

For

pers

onal

use

onl

y.

Page 3: The literacy abilities of 11 year-old students from socially disadvantaged backgrounds

children who are failing to acquire literacy in terms of

their sub-skills. Some children are poor decoders

because they have difficulty with phonological

processing (Hogan et al., 2005). Other children

may have poor comprehension of written text despite

intact phonological processing skills. Catts et al.

(2006) therefore suggested that the best way to

classify poor readers was not on reading comprehen-

sion alone but on children’s strengths and weak-

nesses in word recognition and spoken language

comprehension.

Teaching approach and the development

of literacy

Although many primary school teachers use an

eclectic approach to literacy instruction, teaching

reading has been characterized as following one of

two basic approaches. Current teaching in Australia

predominantly follows a ‘‘whole language’’ approach

where children are immersed in print to gain

exposure to a wide variety of relevant and appro-

priate texts (National Inquiry into the Teaching of

Literacy, 2006). They are expected to abstract the

alphabetic principle without explicit teaching that

involves breaking down words into syllables and

phonemes. In contrast, the phonics approach teaches

children sound-letter correspondences that allow

them to read unknown regularly spelled words

because they have learned the reasonably systematic

relationship between spoken and written words.

Supporters of the phonics approach argue that whole

language is associated with a high level of failure to

acquire literacy skills that allow children to access the

curriculum and particularly disadvantages children

from socially deprived backgrounds (Hempenstall,

2002). The literacy levels of Australian school-

children, however, are reported to be high. The

2000 Program for International Student Assessment

(PISA) evaluated 15 year-old children’s ability to

understand texts in order to achieve goals, develop

knowledge and use literacy effectively in society.

Lockan, Greenwood and Cresswell (2001) reported

that Australian children performed better than those

from many countries in Europe, the Americas and

Asia. Finland was the only country whose children

performed better.

The research reviewed raises some questions. To

what extent are children from a socially deprived

background disadvantaged in the acquisition of

literacy in comparison to their peers in non-socially

disadvantaged areas? Are children who have poor

literacy levels a homogeneous group in that they all

lack understanding of the alphabetic principle, an

essential skill for developing adequate literacy? The

final question addresses an important practical issue

in assessing large numbers of children by class

teachers. Most literacy research involves extensive

individual testing that would be inappropriate for

classroom teachers because it is time consuming and

expensive. Is there a cost-effective way of screening

whole classes to identify those children in need of

intervention before they commence secondary

school? The study reported here investigated these

questions. It was hypothesized that:

1. Children, at the end of Year 6, attending a

primary school in an area of high social

disadvantage in Queensland, would per-

form less well than the standardization sample

for a reading assessment and a spelling

assessment, both normed on Queensland

populations.

2. Children performing more than 1 standard

deviation below the mean of the standardiza-

tion samples on all tests would be a hetero-

geneous group in terms of phonological

awareness, letter-sound matching and quali-

tative analyses of reading and writing.

3. The whole-class Non-word Spelling assess-

ment used would prove reliable and valid in

identifying children with literacy difficulties.

Method

Participants

The study assessed all children (n¼ 112) enrolled in

Year 6 at a school in a socially disadvantaged area in

Queensland using measures taken from subtests of

assessments standardized on children from the same

state, and commonly used by clinicians or teachers.

Children were assessed by their teachers and

experienced paediatric speech and language patholo-

gists. The school was located in an area that had

been identified by Education Queensland as being

an area of low socio-economic status (SES). The

SES of all Queensland schools is based on census

data collected by the Australian Bureau of Statistics

and school principals are informed of their catch-

ment area’s school status. Students were prima-

rily instructed in literacy using whole language.

This was determined by teacher report and at

an in-service session for teachers taught by the

researchers.

The 112 children were in their fourth term of their

6th year in school. There were 53 (47.3%) boys and

59 (52.7%) girls, aged between 10 years 11 months

and 12 years 1 month, the mean age of the group

being 11 years 5 months (SD 3.6 months), with

English being the first language of all the children in

the study. Nine of the children included in the study,

who were receiving mainstream education, had been

ascertained under Education Queensland guidelines

as autistic spectrum disorder. Students ascertained

under the category of intellectual impairment, who

were enrolled in the school’s special education unit,

were excluded. None of the children were currently

receiving intervention for a specific speech or

language difficulty.

Literacy and social disadvantage 183

Int J

Spe

ech

Lan

g Pa

thol

Dow

nloa

ded

from

info

rmah

ealth

care

.com

by

Mcg

ill U

nive

rsity

on

11/2

4/14

For

pers

onal

use

onl

y.

Page 4: The literacy abilities of 11 year-old students from socially disadvantaged backgrounds

Procedure and materials

The reading and spelling whole class assessments

were administered by the Support Teacher Learning

Difficulties and the Deputy Principal in order to

maintain consistency of presentation. The assess-

ments’ manuals indicate that they were standardized

on children representing all socio-economic back-

grounds.

Reading assessment. All children were assessed in

whole class groups. Their reading was assessed using

the Test of Reading for Inferential Comprehension-TRIC

(Murphy, 2003) since this assessment was recently

standardized on Queensland children. The TRIC

comprises of five factual texts (one for each year level

from Year 3 to Year 7) with an accompanying

response sheet. These expository texts are devel-

opmentally ordered. Students are required to read

the text and then answer seven multiple choice

questions and complete four written responses. They

are able to refer back to the text throughout the test.

Secret Weapon was the text for Year 6. Students were

given a score of one for a correct response and zero

for an incorrect response. Correct responses for both

multiple choice and written responses were tallied

and scores were compared to the mean of 8.2

(standard deviation 1.6) as per the manual. At this

point, any child whose score was more than one

standard deviation below the mean was referred for

further assessment to the school’s leaning support

teacher.

Non-word spelling. Children’s spelling was assessed

by the Non-word Spelling test of the Queensland

University Inventory of Literacy (QUIL) (Dodd,

Oerlemans, MacCormack, & Holm, 1996). This

assessment has been standardized on Queensland

children and requires the application of phonological

awareness skills as well as knowledge of letter-sound

correspondence and spelling rules. Words from the

non-word Spelling test were given to each class

group by the support teacher learning difficulties, for

consistency of presentation.

A non-word spelling was correct if the student had

written a word that represented the pronunciation on

the tape. A score of one was given for a correct

response and zero for an incorrect response. The

total number of correct responses was added to give a

raw score and this was converted to a standard score

using the table for Year 6 students in the QUIL

manual. Standard scores were then used to deter-

mine if further testing was required.

Those children who fell more than one standard

deviation below the mean on the Non-word Spelling

task were subsequently assessed individually on four

PA subtests from the QUIL plus a letter-sound

knowledge task. Thirty-five students performed

below the set criterion and were individually assessed

at school by a speech-language pathologist, support

teacher learning difficulties or teacher aide. Another

35 children (the control group) who performed

within normal limits on the non-word spelling test,

matched for class and gender, were also assessed.

The phonological awareness tasks were:

. Syllable Segmentation. In this task students

were required to analyse words into their

component syllable structure and count the

number of syllables (e.g., How many parts in

candlestick? can/dle/stick).

. Spoken Rhyme Recognition. In this task the

students were required to listen to and make

judgements about the phonological similarity

of word pairs (e.g., Do shell and bell rhyme?).

. Phoneme Segmentation. In this task the

students were required to identify and count

the individual sounds (up to five) in words

presented orally (e.g., the word stamp has five

sounds).

. Phoneme Manipulation. In this task the

students were required to identify a given

sound within a word, remove the sound from

the word and then say the new word (e.g., belt

without the /t/ sounds like bell).

. The letter recognition task is one used regularly

by teachers in schools. Twenty-four conso-

nants, six single vowels and 22 vowel diagraphs

were presented individually to each student.

Students were required to say the sound that

corresponded with that letter.

Reliability

Twelve children were reassessed in a group on the

reading comprehension (TRIC) and Non-word

Spelling assessment 2 months after initial assess-

ment. The results showed a high correlation between

the raw scores at the first and second assessments

for both tasks (TRIC correlation¼ 0.821, p¼ 0.001;

non-word spelling correlation¼ 0.910, p5 0.001).

Repeated measures t-tests showed no significant

difference between the two assessments (TRIC

t(11)¼ 1.318, p¼ 0.214; non-word spelling t(11)¼0.220, p¼ 0.830).

Statistical analysis

The data were analysed in three ways. The first set

of analyses compared the study sample against the

standardization samples, while a qualitative analysis

explored the type of errors made on the non-word

spelling test. The second analysis compared good

and poor spellers (as determined by performance on

the non-word spelling test) on a range of tasks that

measure abilities thought to underlie literacy using a

multivariate analysis of variance. The third set of

analyses evaluated the non-word spelling test as a

screener for literacy difficulties, using a regression

analysis to determine the number of children with

184 B. McIntosh et al.

Int J

Spe

ech

Lan

g Pa

thol

Dow

nloa

ded

from

info

rmah

ealth

care

.com

by

Mcg

ill U

nive

rsity

on

11/2

4/14

For

pers

onal

use

onl

y.

Page 5: The literacy abilities of 11 year-old students from socially disadvantaged backgrounds

literacy difficulties correctly identified by the non-

word spelling test.

Results

Comparison of study population and norms

The mean overall score achieved on the TRIC by the

Year 6 group was 6.4 (SD 2.0), as compared to the

standardization population’s mean score of 8.2 (SD

1.6). A one sample t-test, comparing the standar-

dized data and results of the children participating

in the study was significant (t(df 111)¼ 9.352, p50.001), indicating poorer performance by the study

sample. Figure 1 suggests that the children’s group

performance does not reflect the normal curve. This

was confirmed by a nonparametric test (one-sample

Kolmogorov-Smirnov Test¼ 1.581, p5 0.02). The

group’s performance on the TRIC was positively

skewed, suggesting that while many children per-

formed close to the standardized population mean, a

subgroup performed poorly, pulling down the study

sample’s mean score.

Inspection of the two sections of the TRIC

revealed that children performed better on the

multiple choice questions (mean score 5.4 (1.4) out

of 7) than in the section requiring them to write a

sentence (mean score of 1.0 (1.01) out of 4). These

data suggest that children from disadvantaged social

background performed poorly when required to

write answers. For example, in answer to the

question (examples include children’s errors):

‘‘Can the boomerang be used more than once?’’

one child wrote ‘‘yes the boomerang can be used to

cut done fruit from trees. And to catch animals’’. In

answer to the question ‘‘What do you think is the

aborigines opinion of the boomerang?’’ a student

wrote ‘‘it is there main wepon and it is the funnest

and deadliest’’. Another question ‘‘Which type of

boomerang do you think would be used to catch a

wallaby?’’ elicited the written response ‘‘The retern

bommerang because if it reterns and it has blood

on it it tells you that you have hit the animal you

sore’’.

The mean overall raw score on the QUIL’s non-

word spelling test for the Year 6 class was 12.9 (5.0)

out of a possible 24. The normative population’s

mean score was 16 (4.0). A one sample t-test was

significant (t(df 111)¼ 6.534, p5 0.001). Figure 2

suggests that the class showed a normal distribution

and this was confirmed by a nonparametric test (one-

sample Kolmogorov-Smirnov Test¼ 0.699, p¼0.712, NS).

To investigate the nature of the errors being made

in the non-word spelling test, the children’s spelling

errors were scored in three additional ways. In one

analysis the children’s ability to represent the correct

number of syllables in words was assessed. Examples

of errors are: stipdiken for strimperdiction (omits one

syllable), clonderlee for clondly (adds a syllable),

damhop for dramplehoffer (omits two syllables) were

each counted as one error. The mean correct score

for syllable representation was 22.9 (1.9) out of 24

words, indicating that the children were, in general,

representing the number of syllables in words

reasonably accurately. The second analysis found

that the number of consonants represented correctly

was 92.7% (SD 6.5), suggesting that the children’s

ability to represent consonants as graphemes was

very good. A similar count of vowels showed that the

number of vowels represented plausibly to be 83.1%

(SD 11.4). A paired t-test showed that children

performed better on consonants than vowels

(t(111)¼ 14.255, p5 0.001). That is, children’s pri-

mary difficulty when inventing spellings for non-

words appeared to be in selecting plausible spelling

choices to represent vowels.

Comparison of abilities assumed to underlie literacy

Two groups of children, selected according to their

performance on the non-word spelling test, were

compared on a range of measures. Thirty-five children

who scored less than a standard score of 7 (raw score of

10 or less) were compared with a group of 35 children

who had a standard score of 7 or above (raw score of

11 or more). A multivariate analysis of variance

explored the abilities associated with good and poor

non-word spelling. There was a significant difference

in the performance of the groups (F(11, 58)¼ 10.702,

p5 0.001), indicating poorer performance across

tasks by children who performed poorly on the

Non-word Spelling task. Table I lists the variables

included in the analyses. Given that the groups

were selected in terms of their performance on the

Non-word Spelling test, it is not surprising that

all measures derived from their performance on

that assessment differed significantly between the

groups.Figure 1. Performance on the TRIC (n¼112) with fitted normal

curve.

Literacy and social disadvantage 185

Int J

Spe

ech

Lan

g Pa

thol

Dow

nloa

ded

from

info

rmah

ealth

care

.com

by

Mcg

ill U

nive

rsity

on

11/2

4/14

For

pers

onal

use

onl

y.

Page 6: The literacy abilities of 11 year-old students from socially disadvantaged backgrounds

Evaluation of whole class non-word

spelling assessment

A stepwise discriminant analysis was also carried out

on data from these 70 children in their two groups:

one group who scored within normal limits on the

non-word spelling task and one group that per-

formed below the normal range. Discriminant

analysis was used to assess whether group member-

ship, determined by the non-word spelling test,

predicted performance on other measures of literacy.

The dependent variable was, then, grouping and the

independent variables were the QUIL’s syllable

segmentation, rhyme recognition phoneme segmen-

tation and phoneme manipulation subtests as well

as the TRIC and letter knowledge assessments.

After two steps, only two variables were left in the

analyses: rhyme recognition and phoneme manip-

ulation (Wilks’ lambda 0.748, F (2,67)¼ 11.270,

p5 0.001). The scores from these two tasks form

the canonical discriminant function that reflects the

greatest difference between the two groups. Table II

presents the classification results. Overall, the dis-

criminant analysis (based on the QUIL, TRIC and

letter recognition scores) classified 74.3% of cases in

Figure 2. Performance on the non-word spelling test (n¼112) with fitted normal curve.

Table I. Comparison of groups of good and poor non-word spellers, from the same class, on spelling, phonological awareness and reading

measures.

Mean Correct (SD)

Variable F(1,70) Significance Poor Good

Non-word Spelling Measures

Accuracy 109.60 p50.001 7.14 (2.8) 16.03 (4.2)

Syllable representation 17.52 p50.001 21.49 (2.8) 23.54 (0.95)

Per cent vowels correct 21.81 p50.001 72.4 (13.1) 85.83 (10.9)

Per cent consonants correct 33.18 p50.001 86.08 (7.1) 95.38 (6.4)

Phonological Awareness

Syllable segmentation 0.76 p¼0.387 NS 9.57 (2.6) 10.06 (2.01)

Rhyme recognition 5.74 p50.025 10.71 (1.6) 11.43 (0.8)

Phoneme segmentation 0.57 p¼0.455 NS 7.17 (3.1) 7.69 (2.6)

Phoneme manipulation 16.94 p50.001 7.14 (1.9) 8.7 (1.2)

Text

TRIC 2.16 p¼0.15 NS 5.89 (1.9) 6.63 (2.3)

Letter Knowledge 8.83 p50.01 39.69 (4.7) 43.29 (5.4)

*Consonants 3.650 p¼ 0.06 NS 22.2 (1.7) 22.9 (1.4)

*Vowels 3.263 p¼ 0.08 NS 4.06 (1.1) 4.57 (1.3)

*Digraphs 6.421 p50.025 13.43 (3.8) 15.8 (4.1)

*Additional one way ANOVAs investigated the components of letter knowledge scores.

186 B. McIntosh et al.

Int J

Spe

ech

Lan

g Pa

thol

Dow

nloa

ded

from

info

rmah

ealth

care

.com

by

Mcg

ill U

nive

rsity

on

11/2

4/14

For

pers

onal

use

onl

y.

Page 7: The literacy abilities of 11 year-old students from socially disadvantaged backgrounds

the same way as the groupings based on non-word

spelling performance. The table shows that 18 out of

70 children were classified differently: 12 children

had poor non-word spelling but adequate phonolo-

gical awareness skills; and six children had good

non-word spelling ability but poor phonological

awareness skills.

A qualitative analysis of the raw data of those

18 children was carried out to account for the

mismatch in categorization. Twelve children were

‘‘false positives’’ i.e., they scored poorly on the non-

word spelling test but within normal limits on the

phonological awareness tasks. A comparison of these

false positive participants and 12 matched children

who performed well on both non-word spelling and

phonological awareness indicated that there was a

significant difference in their vowel recognition

scores on the letter recognition test, with a trend

for poor digraph recognition (see Table III).

A comparison of spelling patterns used by children

who were false negatives (good non-word spelling

and poor phonological awareness) and children who

scored poorly on both sets of measures suggested

that the ‘‘false negatives’’ may have relied on visual

memory to do the task. They seemed more likely to

include real words in their invented spellings (e.g.,

doorf for dorf, beartelephone for beartelfome, shocktiple

for soptiple, shuts for suts). In contrast the children

with both poor non-word spelling and phonological

awareness were less likely to include real words

making errors (e.g., droph for dorf, wobe for wump,

elimeb for elinarm, drphofer for dramplehoffer). This

observation should be treated cautiously because

only six participants were involved in each group.

Discussion

The non-word spelling and reading comprehension

abilities of all children (n¼ 112) in Year 6 who

attended a school in a socio-economically disadvan-

taged area, were assessed on standardized measures

administered to whole classes. The results indicated

that the group means for both reading and spelling

were below that of the standardization populations.

Children revealed to have difficulties (n¼ 35) were

given follow-up assessments of phonological aware-

ness and letter sound knowledge. A group of controls

(n¼ 35) who were performing within normal limits

and were matched for class and sex was also

assessed. The group of children identified as

performing poorly on the non-word spelling test

was heterogeneous. Some had poor phonological

awareness skills, some had limited sound-letter

correspondence knowledge and some children had

problems with both skills. The non-word spelling test

was a cost-time efficient assessment method that

resulted in few false-negative identifications (i.e.,

children who were classified as not having a difficulty

even though further testing indicated phonological

awareness difficulties).

Comparison of study population and norms

On the TRIC (Murphy, 2003), the children’s mean

score was significantly poorer than the standardiza-

tion mean. While they scored well on the multiple

choice questions, as a group the children performed

poorly in the section requiring them to write a

sentence in answer to a specific question. This

finding cannot be attributed to question difficulty.

Standardization of the TRIC, using Rasch metho-

dology, showed that questions in the multiple choice

section were not necessarily easier than short answer

questions. There are a number of other possible

explanations for the findings. The task may have

been unfamiliar and the children unsure of the task

requirements. They may have had difficulty under-

standing the demands of the question. For example

some students failed to completely answer questions

that had two parts (e.g., ‘‘What do you think is the

Aborigine’s opinion of the boomerang? Give a reason

for your answer’’).

Alternatively, the children may have been dis-

advantaged by their language skills as suggested by

Catts et al. (2006). For example, one question asked

the children to find the sentence that did not belong

in the text. Children performed particularly poorly

on this question, indicating a lack of comprehension

of the text read. Lack of comprehension cannot be

attributed to poor phonological processing ability

because there was no significant difference on the

TRIC between good and poor non-word spellers. If

it were a decoding difficulty that accounted for the

TRIC performance, then a difference would be

expected.

Table II. Classification results from discriminant analysis.

Predicted Group Membership

Poor Good

Original Groups

Count

Poor 23 12

Good 6 29

Percentage

Poor 65.7 34.3

Good 17.1 82.9

Table III. False positives: letter recognition.

False

Positives

(n¼ 12)

Matched

Controls

(n¼12)

F(1,23)

Significance

Consonant

recognition

22.5 23.33 3.09 p¼ 0.093 NS

Vowel recognition 3.75 4.83 7.78 p¼0.011

Digraph

recognition

15.92 18.1 3.74 p¼ 0.066 Trend

Literacy and social disadvantage 187

Int J

Spe

ech

Lan

g Pa

thol

Dow

nloa

ded

from

info

rmah

ealth

care

.com

by

Mcg

ill U

nive

rsity

on

11/2

4/14

For

pers

onal

use

onl

y.

Page 8: The literacy abilities of 11 year-old students from socially disadvantaged backgrounds

Inferencing ability may explain the results better

than either the decoding of written language or

children’s comprehension of what was required to

answer the questions. The TRIC actively explores

children’s search for meaning. In school, children

require inferencing skills to become independent

learners. Inferencing skills depend on the ability to

use general knowledge and contextual information to

draw assumptions. Some of the children’s answers

simply paraphrased what they had read. This may

indicate that they understood the question but didn’t

know how to answer it.

Performance on the non-word spelling subtest of

the QUIL was significantly poorer than that of the

standardization sample. Qualitative analysis sug-

gested adequate representation of syllables and

consonants but considerably more difficulty with

vowels. Children’s primary difficulty when inventing

spelling for non-words appeared to be in selecting

appropriate representations of vowels. This difficulty

is particularly important in written English since

vowels in English carry a heavy functional load.

Many words in English are discriminated only by

their vowel, e.g., pit, pat, pot, pet, putt, pert, part, peat,

port, pout. Children’s difficulties may stem from the

inconsistent way spoken vowel sounds are repre-

sented in written English. Vowel symbols are

ambiguous in that a particular grapheme or digraph

can represent a different sound according to the

lexical item (e.g., moth, mother, motive; deaf, deal; ball,

ballet, balm) and the same vowel can be represented

in a number of different ways (e.g., paw, pore; pour,

poor). Teaching reading and spelling is consequently

difficult and it seems unlikely that either ‘‘whole

language’’ or ‘‘phonics’’ approaches can solve this

inherent difficulty. Complex spelling rules need to be

abstracted, analogies drawn and specific word

spellings learned by rote (e.g., yacht, unique). That

is, learning to spell words requires integration of both

phonological and orthographic (visual) information.

Comparison of abilities assumed to underlie literacy

Two groups of children, selected according to their

performance on the non-word spelling test, were

compared on a range of measures. Children who

scored below normal limits on the non-word spelling

assessment also performed less well on measures of

rhyme recognition, phoneme manipulation and recog-

nition of vowel digraphs. What is surprising is that the

two groups did not differ on measures of syllable or

phonological segmentation. While the results sug-

gested that poor spellers were good at representing

the number of syllables in words, they scored poorly

on phoneme representation despite their phoneme

segmentation ability. This finding provides evidence

against the idea that poorer spellers are unable to

identify the constituent phonemes of words. It would

seem, however, that they have difficulty assigning

plausible graphemes to phonemes.

Although the sound-letter task showed that the

poor spellers had more difficulty when recognizing

vowel digraphs, their ability to recognize consonants

and simple vowels did not differ from controls.

Inspection of their non-word spelling errors clearly

showed that not all their errors could be explained in

terms of vowel digraph difficulties such as syllable

deletion, cluster reduction and incorrect consonant

digraphs. Taken together, these two findings suggest

that the children may have difficulty applying their

phonological awareness to the task of spelling non-

words. The task requires children to hear the

non-word, correctly discriminate the constituent

phonemes, store the word in short term (phonolo-

gical working) memory, apply phoneme grapheme

correspondence rules and/or knowledge of analogy

and then write the word. Poor performance might,

then, reflect a range of impairments including

hearing loss, auditory processing impairment, mem-

ory constraints, phoneme-grapheme/analogy con-

straints or difficulty writing. The task is a screening

tool, not a differential diagnosis of literacy disability.

Nevertheless, it provides a means of identifying

difficulty with a skill that teachers and students rely

on once children reach secondary school—the ability

to write down unfamiliar words.

Evaluation of the whole class non-word spelling

assessment

Non-word spelling does not, then, simply reflect

phonological awareness skills. It assesses a range of

abilities and is, perhaps, an accurate reflection of

children’s literacy functioning in the classroom.

Nevertheless, a stepwise discriminant analysis of

other literacy measures classified 74.3% of cases in

the same way as the groupings based on non-word

spelling performance. This finding reflects the strong

relationship between non-word and phonological

awareness.

While false positive identifications are acceptable,

the six children who were false negatives are of

concern because they might be overlooked for

intervention. Inspection of these children’s standard

scores on the non-word spelling task revealed all six

children had a standard score of 7 (i.e., the lowest

score that placed them in the normal range).

Children scoring a standard score of 7 should,

perhaps, be considered ‘‘borderline’’ cases that

should be included in further testing to ensure that

no child with literacy difficulties is overlooked.

Interpretation

The study reported had three aims. The first was to

investigate whether children from socially deprived

areas, nearing the end of primary school, had lower

levels of literacy than their more advantaged peers.

Their group means were significantly lower for both

reading and spelling. Just under one-third of children

188 B. McIntosh et al.

Int J

Spe

ech

Lan

g Pa

thol

Dow

nloa

ded

from

info

rmah

ealth

care

.com

by

Mcg

ill U

nive

rsity

on

11/2

4/14

For

pers

onal

use

onl

y.

Page 9: The literacy abilities of 11 year-old students from socially disadvantaged backgrounds

were performing below the normal range for non-

word spelling and just over one-third were below the

normal range on the reading assessment. Given the

social and economic consequences attached to low

literacy, these figures are of concern.

The second aim was to investigate the nature of

the underlying difficulties associated with literacy

performance below the normal range on the non-

word spelling test. These results are of particular

interest given the current debate concerning ap-

proaches to literacy instruction. It was surprising that

the children’s ability to segment words into consti-

tuent syllables and phonemes did not differ from

controls, nor did their ability to recognize consonants

and simple vowels. In general, their difficulties

seemed to lie in applying that knowledge (phoneme

manipulation and awareness of rhyme and conse-

quently the ability to use analogy) and in their grasp

of the relationship between speech sounds and vowel

digraphs. It is important to recognize, however, that

the children were not a homogeneous group. Some

children were poor at one set of skills but had

adequate performance on the other, while other

children performed poorly on both sets of skills.

Intervention should be planned to suit the needs of

groups of children with similar difficulties.

The third aim of the investigation was to evaluate

the usefulness of the QUIL’s non-word spelling test

as a screening tool for literacy difficulties. It is a

‘‘whole class’’ assessment, and takes little time to

administer. The results of the discriminant analysis

suggest that it is a valid and reliable first step in the

identification of children with literacy difficulties.

References

Adams, M. (1994). Beginning to read: Thinking and learning about

print. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Bishop, D., & Adams, C. (1990). A prospective study of the

relationship between language impairment, phonological dis-

orders and reading retardation. Journal of Child Psychology &

Psychiatry, 31, 1027 – 1050.

Bowey, J. A. (1995). Socioeconomic status differences in pre-

school phonological sensitivity and first-grade reading achieve-

ment. Journal of Educational Psychology, 87, 476 – 487.

Bryant, P. E., MacLean, M., Bradley, L., & Crossland, J. (1990).

Rhyme and alliteration, phoneme detection and learning to

read. Developmental Psychology, 2, 429 – 438.

Burt, L., Holm, A., & Dodd, B. J. (1999). Phonological awareness

skills of 4-year-old British children: an assessment and

developmental data. International Journal of Language and

Communication Disorders, 34, 311 – 335.

Catts, H., Adlof, S., & Ellis Weismer, S. (2006). Language deficits

in poor comprehenders: A case for the simple view of reading.

Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 49, 278 –

294.

Coomber, B. (1997). Literacy, poverty and schooling: Working

against deficit equations. English in Australia, 119(20), 1 – 16.

Dodd, B., & Carr, A. (2003). Young children’s letter-sound

knowledge. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools,

34, 128 – 137.

Dodd, B., Oerlemans, M., MacCormack, M., & Holm, A. (1996).

The Queensland University inventory of literacy. Brisbane:

University of Queensland.

Duncan, L. G., & Seymour, P. H. K. (2000). Socio-economic

differences in foundation level literacy. British Journal of

Psychology, 91, 145 – 166.

Fisher, R. (1992). Early literacy and the teacher. New York: Hodder

& Stoughton.

Frith, U. (1985). Beneath the surface of developmental dyslexia.

In K. Patterson, J. C. Marshall, & M. Coltheart (Eds.), Surface

dyslexia: Neuropsychological and cognitive studies in phonological

reading (pp. 301 – 330). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

Gillon, G., & Dodd, B. (2005). Understanding the relationship

between speech-language impairment and literacy difficulties:

The central role of phonology. In B. Dodd (Ed.), Differential

diagnosis of speech disorder (pp. 289 – 304). London: Whurr.

Hatcher, P. J., & Hulme, C. (1999). Phonemes, rhymes and

intelligence as predictors of children’s responsiveness to

remedial reading instruction: Evidence from a longitudinal

intervention study. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 72,

130 – 153.

Hecht, S., Burgess, S., Torgesen, J., Wagner, R., & Rashotte, C.

(2000). Explaining social class differences in growth of reading

skills from beginning kindergarten through fourth-grade. The

role of phonological awareness, rate of access, and print

knowledge. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal,

12, 99 – 127.

Hempenstall, K. (2002). Phonological processing and phonics:

Towards an understanding of their relationship to each other

and to reading development. Australian Journal of Learning

Disabilities, 7, 4 – 29.

High, P. C., LaGasse, L., Becker, S., Ahlgren, I., & Gardner, A.

(2000). Literacy promotion in primary care pediatrics: Can we

make a difference? Pediatrics, 105, 927 – 934.

Hogan, T. P., Catts, H. W., & Little, T. D. (2005). The

relationship between phonological awareness and reading:

Implications for the assessment of phonological awareness.

Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools, 36, 285 –

293.

Høien, T., Lundberg, I., Stanovich, K. E., & Bjaalid, I. (1995).

Components of phonological awareness. Reading and Writing:

An Interdisciplinary Journal, 7, 171 – 188.

Jefferis, B., Power, C., & Hertzman, C. (2002). Birthweight,

childhood socioeconomic environment and cognitive develop-

ment in the 1958 British birth cohort study. British Medical

Journal, 325, 305 – 307.

Leahy, M. M., & Dodd, B. (2002). Why should secondary schools

come second? RCSLT Bulletin, May, 11 – 13.

Lockan, J., Greenwood, L., & Cresswell, J. (2001). How literate

are Australia’s students? The PISA 2000 survey of students

reading, mathematical and scientific literacy skills. Camberwell:

ACER.

Locke, A., Ginsborg, J., & Peers, I. (2002). Development and

disadvantage: Implications for the early years and beyond.

International Journal of Language and Communication Disorders,

37, 3 – 15.

Moon, C., & Wells, G. (1979). The influence of home on learning

to read. Journal of Research in Reading, 2, 53 – 62.

Murphy, J. (2003). The development of a reading test for

inferential comprehension. Unpublished PhD thesis, Univer-

sity of Queensland.

Nation, K., & Snowling, M. J. (2000). Factors influencing

syntactic awareness skills in normal readers and poor compre-

henders. Applied Psycholinguistics, 21, 229 – 241.

National Inquiry into the Teaching of Literacy (2006). Accessed

24 April 2006, available at: http://www.dest.gov.au/sectors/

school_education/policy_initiatives_reviews/key_issues/literacy_

numeracy/national_inquiry/.

Raz, I. S., & Bryant, P. (1990). Social background, phonological

awareness and children’s reading. British Journal of Develop-

mental Psychology, 8, 209 – 225.

Roberts, J. (2004). Jury out on literacy row. The Australian, 22

November.

Share, D. (1995). Phonological recoding and self-teaching: Sine

qua non of reading acquisition. Cognition, 55, 151 – 218.

Literacy and social disadvantage 189

Int J

Spe

ech

Lan

g Pa

thol

Dow

nloa

ded

from

info

rmah

ealth

care

.com

by

Mcg

ill U

nive

rsity

on

11/2

4/14

For

pers

onal

use

onl

y.

Page 10: The literacy abilities of 11 year-old students from socially disadvantaged backgrounds

Stahl, S. A., & Murray, B. A. (1994). Defining phonological

awareness and its relationship to early reading. Journal of

Educational Psychology, 86, 221 – 234.

United States Department of Education, National Center for

Education Statistics (2001). Entering kindergarten: A portrait of

American children when they begin school: Findings from the

condition of education 2000. Accessed 3 November 2002,

available at: http:/nces.ed.gov.

van Kraayenoord, C. E., & Paris, S. G. (1994). The literacy

challenge: Strategies for early intervention for literacy and

learning for Australian children. The Reading Teacher, 48,

218 – 228.

190 B. McIntosh et al.

Int J

Spe

ech

Lan

g Pa

thol

Dow

nloa

ded

from

info

rmah

ealth

care

.com

by

Mcg

ill U

nive

rsity

on

11/2

4/14

For

pers

onal

use

onl

y.