47
Eastern Illinois University e Keep Masters eses Student eses & Publications 2000 e Effect of Predator Chemical Cues and Conspecific Alarm Signals upon Behavior in Early and Late Developmental Stages of American Toad (Bufo americanus) Tadpoles Carol Lynee Johnson Eastern Illinois University is research is a product of the graduate program in Biological Sciences at Eastern Illinois University. Find out more about the program. is is brought to you for free and open access by the Student eses & Publications at e Keep. It has been accepted for inclusion in Masters eses by an authorized administrator of e Keep. For more information, please contact [email protected]. Recommended Citation Johnson, Carol Lynee, "e Effect of Predator Chemical Cues and Conspecific Alarm Signals upon Behavior in Early and Late Developmental Stages of American Toad (Bufo americanus) Tadpoles" (2000). Masters eses. 1607. hps://thekeep.eiu.edu/theses/1607

The Effect of Predator Chemical Cues and Conspecific Alarm

  • Upload
    others

  • View
    5

  • Download
    0

Embed Size (px)

Citation preview

Page 1: The Effect of Predator Chemical Cues and Conspecific Alarm

Eastern Illinois UniversityThe Keep

Masters Theses Student Theses & Publications

2000

The Effect of Predator Chemical Cues andConspecific Alarm Signals upon Behavior in Earlyand Late Developmental Stages of American Toad(Bufo americanus) TadpolesCarol Lynette JohnsonEastern Illinois UniversityThis research is a product of the graduate program in Biological Sciences at Eastern Illinois University. Findout more about the program.

This is brought to you for free and open access by the Student Theses & Publications at The Keep. It has been accepted for inclusion in Masters Thesesby an authorized administrator of The Keep. For more information, please contact [email protected].

Recommended CitationJohnson, Carol Lynette, "The Effect of Predator Chemical Cues and Conspecific Alarm Signals upon Behavior in Early and LateDevelopmental Stages of American Toad (Bufo americanus) Tadpoles" (2000). Masters Theses. 1607.https://thekeep.eiu.edu/theses/1607

Page 2: The Effect of Predator Chemical Cues and Conspecific Alarm

THESIS/FIELD EXPERIENCE PAPER REPRODUCTION CERTIFICATE

TO: Graduate Degree Candidates (who have written formal theses)

SUBJECT: Permission to Reproduce Theses

The University Library is receiving a number of request from other institutions asking permission to reproduce dissertations for inclusion in their library holdings. Although no copyright laws are involved, we feel that professional courtesy demands that permission be obtained from the author before we allow these to be copied.

PLEASE SIGN ONE OF THE FOLLOWING STATEMENTS:

Booth Library of Eastern Illinois University has my permission to lend my thesis to a reputable college or university for the purpose of copying it for inclusion in that institution's library or research holdings.

Date

I respectfully request Booth library of Eastern Illinois University NOT allow my thesis to be reproduced because:

Author's Signature Date

rt>es1s4 form

Page 3: The Effect of Predator Chemical Cues and Conspecific Alarm

The Effect of Predator Chemical Cues and Conspecific Alarm Signals upon Beha,·ior in

Early and Late Developmental Stages of American toad (fu![Q americanus) Tadpoles.

(TITLE)

DATE

DATE

BY

Carol Lynette Johnson

/Ci 7 fi, -

THESIS

SUBMITTED IN PARTIAL FULFILLMENT OF THE REQUIREMENTS FOR THE DEGREE OF

Masters of Zoology

IN THE GRADUATE SCHOOL, EASTERN ILLINOIS UNIVERSITY CHARLESTON, ILLINOIS

2000

YEAR

I HEREBY RECOMMEND TH.A.T THIS THESIS BE ACCEPTED AS FULFILLING THIS PART OF THE GRADUATE DEGREE CITED ABOVE

Page 4: The Effect of Predator Chemical Cues and Conspecific Alarm

Table of Contents

Acknowledgments ........................................ . ...... . .. . ...... ii

Abstract ............ . .. ...... . .. .... . ............. . ......................... .. iii

Introduction .............................................. . .... . .. .... . ...... 1

Methods ..... . ....................... . ................ . ........ . .... .......... 5

Results .................. . .. .. .. . .. ... ... . ..................................... 10

Figures ....... . ... . .... . .. .................. . .................................. 13

Table 1 ..................... . .. . ... . .. ...... . .................................. 22

Discussion ... . ........ . .. .. ... ; ........................................ . ..... 23

Literature Cited ..................... . ...... . ............................... 28

Page 5: The Effect of Predator Chemical Cues and Conspecific Alarm

Acknowledgements

I first wish to express gratitude to my committee members: Dr. Kipp Kruse, Dr.

Robert Fischer, and Dr. Charles Pederson for their insight and support throughout the

duration of my work. I also would like to thank Dr. Jeff Laursen for his invaluable

direction in the collection and housing of my numerous tadpoles. Many thanks also to

family, friends, and Ted Watson for all their encouragement, companionship and never­

ending support throughout my time at Eastern.

All tadpoles and predatory beetles were collected by permit on the Fox Ridge

State Park, whose staff was extremely helpful; bluegill were collected from the Campus

Pond at Eastern. EIU's biology department provided laboratory space, equipment, and

some financial support. A grant from Eastern Illinois University was instrumental in

funding this thesis.

ii

Page 6: The Effect of Predator Chemical Cues and Conspecific Alarm

Abstract

The chemosensory capability and subsequent habitat choice of larval American

toad (Bufo americanus) tadpoles were quantified using choice of refuge with both early

and late developmental stages. Treatments were performed with two tadpole densities to

ascertain the effect of social aggregations upon behavior. Bluegill (1epomis

macrochirus) and predaceous diving beetle larvae (Dytiscus ~.)predators were used to

condition water. Conspecific tadpoles as well as Southern leopard frog (Rana

sphenocephala) tadpoles were used to prepare treatment extracts.

Tadpole density (n= IO and n=20) had no significant effect upon the percentage of

tadpoles seeking cover in any treatment. The percentage of toad tadpoles of early

developmental stages seeking cover was significantly higher when exposed to both early

and late staged conspecific alarm signals as compared to the control, however, the

treatment responses were statistically indistinguishable from one another. Similarly, the

presence of beetle larvae in test chambers elicited a strong fright reaction by tadpoles

when compared to controls. Tadpoles responded more strongly to the presence of beetle

larvae than to chemical cues oflarval beetles. Likewise, the addition of bluegill cues

elicited a significant antipredator response from toad tadpoles.

Late developmental stage toad tadpoles showed no significant increases in the

percentage seeking cover in any treatment; these results are attributable to the high

percentage of larvae seeking cover in control trials. My results suggest that tadpoles may

display antipredator tactics that are stage-specific and geared toward differing suites of

predators.

111

Page 7: The Effect of Predator Chemical Cues and Conspecific Alarm

Introduction

Predation is a constant threat throughout the life of an amphibian; therefore

behavioral responses appropriate to the developmental stage may be shaped by natural

selection. The antipredator responses of amphibian larvae have been well studied;

tadpoles are known to respond to predators in a variety of ways ranging from altering life

history strategies (Skelly and Werner 1990, Werner and Anholt 1993) to various

behavioral responses (Petranka et al.1987, Skelly 1994). Behavioral antipredator tactics

include altering activity levels (Lawler 1989, Anholt et al. 1996), seeking refuge (Babbitt

and Tanner 1997), and swarming (Watt et al. 1997). A distinct evolutionary advantage

would exist for tadpole species able to detect predators before contact occurs, thereby

increasing the escape interval as well as the possibility of avoiding a predation episode

entirely. Several species of larval amphibians, including toads (Bufonidae ), use indirect

signals (Petranka and Hayes 1998) to detect and respond to substances associated with

predators including diet and waste metabolites (Hews 1988, Lawler 1989, Wilson and

Lefcort 1993, Laurila et al. 1997, Petranka and Hayes 1998), as well as alarm signals

issuing from the skin of damaged conspecifics (Hews and Blaustein 1985, Petranka and

Hayes 1998, Kiesecker et al. 1999).

Larvae of the American toad, (Bufo americanus) are capable of further

antipredator tactics. They exhibit aposematic coloration which alerts predators to their

toxic and noxious qualities caused by a chemical called bufotoxin, which is only

produced late in the larval period (Brodie et al. 1978) beginning at developmental stage

41 Gosner (1960). This production ofbufotoxin by granular glands in the skin just prior

Page 8: The Effect of Predator Chemical Cues and Conspecific Alarm

to the onset of metamorphosis causes toad tadpoles to be unpalatable or even

occasionally toxic (Brodie at al. 1978) to several species of predators, including some

insect predators (e.g., dytiscids) and fish (Brodie et al. 1978, Formanowicz and Brodie

1982),. In earlier developmental stages, however, tadpoles may be fully palatable

(Brodie et al. 1978, Semlitsch and Gavasso 1992) and consequently much more

susceptible to predation.

Toad tadpoles form complex aggregations (Beiswenger 1975, Rodel and

Linsenmair 1997, Watt et al 1997), in which members both recognize and preferentially

associate with siblings (Waldman and Adler 1979, Waldman 1982). This schooling

behavior bas several potential benefits to members, including enhanced predator

vigilance and food location (Beiswenger 1975), along with increased survival due to the

dilution effect (Watt et al. 1997). These groups may also lend protection to palatable

individuals. Used in conjunction with aposematism and kin preference, swarming is

instrumental in increasing inclusive fitness of swarm members (Brodie and Formanowicz

1981, Brodie and Formanowicz 1987) by facilitating avoidance of unpalatable larvae by

predators. Social aggregations are known to be comprised of mainly early stage tadpoles,

whereas later stage tadpoles (Gosner 1960 stage 41 or later) have been demonstrated to

be much less social (Beiswenger 1975).

Bufonids also will modify activity levels, avoid areas containing predators (Skelly

and Werner 1990, Anholt et al. 1996), and utilize cover in escaping predators (Babbitt

and Jordan 1996, Babbitt and Tanner 1997). These tactics, while effective against some

predators, are extremely costly in terms of growth (Kupferberg 1997). Therefore,

tadpoles need to adaptively balance the risk of predation they experience against the

2

Page 9: The Effect of Predator Chemical Cues and Conspecific Alarm

percentage of time spent foraging. At the time of metamorphic climax, tadpoles may be

exceptionally vulnerable to predators (Arnold and Wassersug 1978, Crump 1984) as

limbs develop and the tail is reabsorbed which has been shown to severely decrease

swimming velocity beginning at Gosner (1960) developmental stage 42 (Huey 1980).

Because tadpoles cease feeding at stage 42 (Beisenger 1975) they may have no need to

engage in foraging, and therefore may appropriate more time to the avoidance of

predators. Given the differing physiologies, nutitive needs, and swimming capabilities, it

seems possible that toad tadpoles may exhibit antipredator tactics that are stage-specific.

To further explore the role of chemo-sensory capability in mediating behavior

choice in early and late developmental stage American toad tadpoles, several questions

were posed and then translated into laboratory experiments. First, what is the extent of

chemo-sensory capabilities in both early and late stage tadpoles? Second, how do these

chemo-sensory capabilities affect behavior of early and late stage tadpoles?

Experimental treatments were performed using chemical cues of conspecifics,

heterospecifics, and two predators to condition water. Treatments were performed with

two densities of tadpoles (n=IO, n=20) to determine how and if social aggregations

affected behavior, and choice of refuge was used to quantify behavior. Predaceous diving

beetle larvae (Dytiscus §R.) were chosen because they are known to be a highly efficient

predator of toad tadpoles, and have been found to kill and eat more toad tadpoles of all

sizes than other predators (Brodie and Formanowicz (1983). Diving beetle larvae have

also been shown to be primarily non-visual, relying instead upon tactile and chemical

cues before initiating a strike (Formanowicz 1987). In contrast, bluegill sunfish, Lepomis

macrochirus, are visually oriented predators that only occasionally are syntopic with toad

3

Page 10: The Effect of Predator Chemical Cues and Conspecific Alarm

tadpoles. Toad tadpoles are distasteful to bluegill (Voris and Bacon 1966), although

naive predators may still engulf tadpoles. Furthermore, tadpoles are known to

behaviorally respond to these fish (Lawler 1989), perhaps due to their fragility.

4

Page 11: The Effect of Predator Chemical Cues and Conspecific Alarm

Methods

Collections

American toad (Bufo americanus) tadpoles, Southern leopard frog (Rana

sphenocephala) tadpoles, and predaceous diving beetle larvae (Dytiscus fil2.) were dip­

netted from an ephemeral pond in Coles County, Illinois, beginning early May 2000.

Tadpoles were separated by species and housed in aerated plastic aquaria (39x25xl0.5

cm) filled to a depth of 10 cm with tap water treated using a commercial aquarium water

conditioner, and were housed in environmental chambers kept at 22°C, with 14:10

photoperiod (which approximates the natural photoperiod during this time of the year);

water changes were performed every second day. Aquaria were wrapped in black plastic

to reduce startling tadpoles by human presence. Tadpoles were maintained on a diet of

commercial fish pellets fed ad libi:rnm; plastic plants were provided for refuge. Tadpoles

were kept in the laboratory for at least 48 hours prior to testing to allow for acclimation.

Beetle larvae were housed individually in I .SL plastic containers filled with conditioned

water to a depth of 4 cm and containing plastic plants, with their diet consisting of

backswimmers (notonectids). Bluegill sunfish (Lepomis macrochirus) were seined from

a permanent pond on the Eastern Illinois University campus (Coles Co., IL) and were

housed in aerated 40L aquaria. Fish were fed bloodworms and guppies; sufficient live

food was provided to ensure continuous access to prey. All predators were kept at room

temperature near windows under natural photoperiod conditions; chambers were cleaned

every 48 hours. Predators also were held at least 48 hours in the laboratory before use to

ensure no residual chemicals from prey captured in the wild remained in their digestive

5

Page 12: The Effect of Predator Chemical Cues and Conspecific Alarm

track. Following conclusion of the study, tadpoles and predators were released at the site

of capture.

Experiments

The experimental protocol was consistent for all treatments tested. Test chambers

were of the same dimensions as holding chambers, and filled with conditioned tap water

to a depth of 4 cm. All experiments were conducted under fluorescent lighting at room

temperature. Forty percent ( 40%) of each chamber contained plant cover as one

contiguous clump, consisting of 10-12 stalks approximately 20 cm long of plastic Elodea

and Anacharis plants. In each experimental chamber, cover was randomly placed in

either the north or south direction of each arena. Toad tadpoles were netted from aquaria

and staged according to Gosner (1960). For experiments in Part I, tadpole stages 25-27

were used and designated as early stage. In Part II, tadpole stages 41-42 were used and

designated as late stage. Toad tadpoles were then introduced into test arenas and allowed

10 minutes for acclimation. Each experiment lasted 15 minutes, during which period the

number of tadpoles under cover was recorded immediately and then every 3 minutes

thereafter (repeated measure). Each of the experimental treatments was performed with

tadpole densities of 10 and 20. For each density 5 replicates were performed; no tadpoles

were used twice (excluding those in experiment #7-see below) so all data are

independent.

Additions of chemical and control stimuli were poured into the center oftest arenas

during each experimental replicate. To prepare extract, tadpoles of similar size and stage

were humanely killed then homogenized for approximately 15 seconds using a blender

6

Page 13: The Effect of Predator Chemical Cues and Conspecific Alarm

and 150 ml of deionized water. From this mixture, 10 ml of liquid were collected and

slowly poured into the center of the arena. Fresh extract was used for each trial to

prevent potential degradation of chemical cues, and a total of 12 beetle larvae and 10

bluegill were used in both preparing extracts as well as in certain trials (see below). For

treatments requiring addition of an extract to the test chamber, a control was established

as follows. Five minutes into the acclimation period, 10 ml of conditioned tap water was

poured into the center of the arena, which controlled for the tactile disturbance effect of

adding liquids. At the conclusion of each experiment, arenas were thoroughly washed,

rinsed, and refilled with conditioned water.

Part I: Treatments

To determine how early stage toad tadpoles respond to conspecific alarm signals,

extracts of early and late stage tadpoles were prepared and added to test aquaria. To

determine the response of early stage tadpoles to a heterospecific, an extract ofR

sphenocephala tadpoles was prepared and added to test aquaria.

Tadpole response to diving beetle larvae was ascertained using two methods. First,

chemical cues were collected from a chamber holding a diving beetle larva and added to

the test chamber. Prior to use, beetle larvae were transferred and held individually for a

minimum of 5 hours in a clean chamber with 100 ml of conditioned water to prevent

introduction of conflicting chemical cues from prey. From a chamber containing an

individual beetle larva, 10 ml of water were collected and carefully added to the center of

the test arena. Secondly, to approximate natural conditions more closely, a single, live

beetle larva was placed in an enclosure in each test arena. Enclosures consisted of

styrofoam cups that were thoroughly washed before use and then discarded after each

7

Page 14: The Effect of Predator Chemical Cues and Conspecific Alarm

trial. A small washed rock was placed in the bottom of each cup to prevent it from

floating. Each cup was pierced 60-70 times with a sterile wooden toothpick to allow for

water flow. Enclosures were introduced prior to the introduction of tadpoles for

acclimation, and randomly placed in one of 3 locations approximately 5 cm from the

edge of the arena opposite the side containing plants. A control for this treatment was

established using empty enclosures.

A series of experiments were designed to test tadpole response to bluegill using

chemical cues. Ten bluegill were kept for 5 hours in a clean chamber with 1 L of

conditioned tap water prior to use. From this holding chamber, 10 ml of water were

collected and introduced into the test chamber 5 minutes into the acclimation period.

Following use in the prior experiment, the response of each tadpole group to fish

chemical cue conditioning was determined. Each tadpole group was removed from the

test chamber and housed separately in l .5L containers for 48 hours. Every 8 hours, 50 ml

of water from fish aquaria were added. After conditioning the tadpoles to fish stimuli in

this manner, they were then retested for response to fish cues. Controls were performed

in which tadpoles were housed in identical groups and conditioned using conditioned tap

water additions every 8 hours.

Part II: Treatments

Treatments in Part II were performed using protocols similar to those in Part I.

Controls were established as in Part I using late stage tadpoles, and predators were

randomly assigned to replicates.

Late stage tadpoles were exposed to additions of tadpole extract prepared using early

stage tadpoles and late stage tadpoles. Similarly, tadpoles were exposed to predator cues

8

Page 15: The Effect of Predator Chemical Cues and Conspecific Alarm

using bluegill cues collected from a lL aquarium housing 10 bluegill, and 10 ml water

samples taken from a larval diving beetle chamber.

Analysis

Data was converted to percentage of tadpoles under cover, then transformed using an

arcsine transformation (Sokal and Rohlf 1981 ). An analysis of variance was used to

compare means, with time as a repeated measure. Computer software (NWA ST ATP AK

1986) was used, with an alpha value set~ priori at 0.05. Figures are composed of

untransformed data, in the form of percentages of tadpoles seeking cover, with means +

one standard error shown.

9

Page 16: The Effect of Predator Chemical Cues and Conspecific Alarm

Results

A three-way ANOVA, with main effects (tadpole density and treatment) was

calculated with one repeated measure (time). Results of these analyses revealed that

tadpole density did not contribute a significant amount of variance to the statistical model

in any experiment. Consequently, density was pooled in further analyses, resulting in a

two-way ANOVA with one repeated measure (time), and treatment effect.

Part I: Early Stage

The mean percentage of toad tadpoles seeking cover varied significantly (F=5.12,

P=0.0047) among treatments (Fig. 1). Significantly higher mean percentages of tadpoles

exposed to early and late stage extracts were found seeking cover (mean=47.1±8.2%,

mean=46.8±6.7%, respectively) as compared to tadpoles exposed to control treatments

(mean=21 .6±4.8%), however, the treatment means were statistically indistinguishable

from one another. Similarly, the mean percentages of tadpoles in the control

(mean=21.58±4.81 %) and R. sphenocephala treatments (mean=28.5±7.47%) were not

different. Over time, the percent of tadpoles in these treatments seeking cover increased

significantly (F=8.35, P<0.0001--Fig. 2) as compared to controls.

The mean percentage of toad tadpoles seeking cover varied among Dytiscus ~·

treatments. Tadpoles exposed to caged beetle larvae had a mean percent under cover

(53.95±2.52%) that was significantly higher than the mean percent (43.49±3.70%)

observed for tadpoles exposed to an empty cage (F=9.85, P=0.005--Fig 3). Trials with

beetle larvae cues alone also differed significantly (F=7.24, P=0.014), with the mean

percent of tadpoles in control water conditions under cover being 34.97±3.91 %, while the

10

Page 17: The Effect of Predator Chemical Cues and Conspecific Alarm

mean percent of tadpoles seeking cover during exposure to beetle larval cues was

45.52±2.97% (Fig 4). The two control treatments utilized (empty predator cage and

water additions), did not differ significantly from one another (F=3.0l , P>0.05). Of these

two sources of beetle larvae cues, caged beetles had a significantly greater effect on cover

usage (F=13.52, P=0.0017) than did the addition of water from beetle larvae chambers.

Treatments involving bluegill stimuli varied in the percentages of tadpoles

seeking cover (Fig. 5). A significant difference (F=8.97, P=0.007) was found between

the mean percentages of tadpoles seeking cover in water controls (mean=2 l .58±4.81 % )

and fish cue treatments (mean=38.42±3.51%-- Fig 5). A significant difference (F=22.87,

P=0.0001) also resulted between tadpoles conditioned to bluegill ,cues

(mean=25.92±2.85%), and tadpoles treated with conditioned tap water

(mean=42.42±2.98%--Fig 6). Surprisingly, more tadpoles were found seeking cover in

the conditioning control than in the fish treatment. Of the two control treatments used,

tadpoles assigned to the conditioning regime were found under cover a significantly

higher percentage of time (F=l 7.05, P=0.0006) than tadpoles exposed to water additions

(mean = 50.3±3.0%, mean= 36.0±3.8%, respectively). Initially, 47.5% of tadpoles

responded to the addition of bluegill cues by seeking cover, whereas after conditioning,

tadpoles significantly reduced use of cover to 39.7%. The percentage of tadpoles seeking

cover increased significantly (F=4.15, P=0.0013) with time following a 48 hour

conditioning period to bluegill cues (Fig 7).

Part II: Late Stage

There were no significant differences among treatment means (Fig 8) involving

late stage toad tadpoles (F=l.11 , P=0.363). During exposure to extracts of both early and

11

Page 18: The Effect of Predator Chemical Cues and Conspecific Alarm

late stage tadpoles, use of cover varied significantly (F=3.89, P=0.0025) over time (Fig

9). Analysis of controls for early versus late stage tadpoles revealed that a greater

percentage of late stage tadpoles sought cover (F= l2.47, P=0.002). The results of

pairwise analysis of variance in early and late stage tadpoles are shown (Table 1).

12

Page 19: The Effect of Predator Chemical Cues and Conspecific Alarm

Fig I. Effects of water controls, early [Bufo(e)] stages (25-27) Bufo americanus, late

[Bufo(l)] stages (41-42) B. americanus tadpole extract, and early stages Rana

spenocephala (Rana) extract upon the percentage of early stage B . americanus tadpoles

seeking cover. Means + one standard error are shown.

13

Page 20: The Effect of Predator Chemical Cues and Conspecific Alarm

... G.>

50 > 0 u ... 40

G.> mg c ::> 30 Ul G.> -0 a. 20 mg «S I-

~ 10

Control Bufo (e) Bufo (I) Rana

Treatment

Page 21: The Effect of Predator Chemical Cues and Conspecific Alarm

Fig 2. The effects of time upon the percentage of early stages (25-27) Bufo americanus

tadpoles seeking cover in treatments: water controls (CTRL), extracts of early stages B.

americanus [B(e)], late stages (41-42) B. americanus [B(l)], and early stages Southern

leopard frog (Rana). Means + one standard error are shown.

14

Page 22: The Effect of Predator Chemical Cues and Conspecific Alarm

70

... 60 Q)

> 0 () 50 ... Q)

"'C c:

::> 40 rn Q) -0 c. 30 "'C ca I-~ 0 20

--- ........... / /

I ........... / · . . ...........

I ...........

/ ..... · · ........... ....:.../

/ -.......

/ ,,,,.....,.. ..............

/

0 Min 3 Min 6 Min 9 Min 12 Min 15 Min

Time (minutes)

- CTRL B(e)

- - B(I) - Rana

Page 23: The Effect of Predator Chemical Cues and Conspecific Alarm

Fig 3. Effects of controls with empty predator cages (C. cage) and enclosed predaceous

diving beetle larvae (Dytiscus) upon the percentage of early stage (25-27) Bufo

americanus tadpoles seeking cover. Means+ one standard error are shown.

15

Page 24: The Effect of Predator Chemical Cues and Conspecific Alarm

60

... 50 Q)

> 0

(.) ... 40 Q) "'O c

::::> en 30 Q) -0 Q.

"'O 20 co ..... ~ 10

C. Cage Dytiscus

Treatment

Page 25: The Effect of Predator Chemical Cues and Conspecific Alarm

Fig 4. The effects of water controls [Ctrl (w)] and diving beetle larvae (Dytiscus m.)

chemical cues [Dyts(w)] upon the percentage of early stages (25-27) Bufo americanus

tadpoles seeking cover. Means + one standard error are shown.

16

Page 26: The Effect of Predator Chemical Cues and Conspecific Alarm

50

45

... 40 Cl)

> 0 35 0 ... Cl) 30 "C c

:::> 25 en Cl)

20 -0 Q. "C 15 ra I-~ 0

10

5

0 Ctrl (w) Oyts (w)

Treatment

Page 27: The Effect of Predator Chemical Cues and Conspecific Alarm

Fig 5. Effects of water controls (Control) and Lepomis rnacrochirus chemical cues (Fish

I) upon the percentage of early stages (25-27) Bufo americanus tadpoles seeking cover.

Means + one standard error are shown.

17

Page 28: The Effect of Predator Chemical Cues and Conspecific Alarm

50

45 ... 40 Cl)

> 0 35 0 ... Cl) 30 "CS c

:::> 25 ,,, Cl)

20 -0 c.

"CS 15 ca ~

';le. 10

5

0 Control Fish I

Treatment

Page 29: The Effect of Predator Chemical Cues and Conspecific Alarm

Fig 6. Percentage of early stages (25-27) Bufo americanus tadpoles seeking cover

following 48 hour conditioning treatments to controls [Ctrl(c)], and Lepomis macrochirus

stimuli [Fish(c)]. Means+ one standard error are shown.

18

Page 30: The Effect of Predator Chemical Cues and Conspecific Alarm

60

... 50 Cl) .r

> 0

CJ 40 ... Cl)

"'C c

::> 30 0 Cl) -0 c. 20 "'C ca ~

~ 0 10

Ctrl (c) Fish (c)

Treatment

Page 31: The Effect of Predator Chemical Cues and Conspecific Alarm

r

Fig 7. Effects of time upon the percentage of late stages (41-42) Bufo americanus

tadpoles seeking cover following a 48 hour conditioning period to water controls

[Ctrl(c)], and Lej>omis macrochirus stimuli [Fish(c)]. Means+ one standard error are

shown.

19

Page 32: The Effect of Predator Chemical Cues and Conspecific Alarm

.. CD > 0 0 .. CD ,, c

:::> fl) CD -0 Q. ,, ca I-~ 0

55

50

45

40

35

30

25

20

. . .

..

0 Min. 3 Min. 6 Min. 9 Min. 12 Min. 15 Min.

Time (minutes)

- Ctrt (c) . • . Fish (c)

Page 33: The Effect of Predator Chemical Cues and Conspecific Alarm

Fig 8. Effects of various treatments upon the percentage of late stages (41-42) Bufo

americanus tadpoles seeking cover. Means+ one standard error are shown.

20

Page 34: The Effect of Predator Chemical Cues and Conspecific Alarm

70 ... G> 60 > 0 0 ... 50 G>

"C c: ::> 40 ,,, G> - 30 0 Q.

"C ca 20 I-~ 0

10

0 Control Bufo (e) Bufo (I) Dyl Cues Fish

Treatment

Page 35: The Effect of Predator Chemical Cues and Conspecific Alarm

Fig 9. Effects of time upon the percentage of late stages (41-42) Bufo americanus

tadpoles seeking cover during exposure to controls, extract of early stages (25-27) B.

americanus tadpoles, and extract of late stages B. americanus tadpoles.

21

Page 36: The Effect of Predator Chemical Cues and Conspecific Alarm

60

.... G> > 0 50 0 .... G>

"C c

:::> 40 ti) G> -0 c.

"C 30 ca I-~ 0

20

. .

.· /9 ·--....._.....-...

. . . . . .

.

. .

0 Min. 3 Min. 6 Min. 9 Min. 12 Min. 15 Min.

Time (minutes) -Ctrl ... Bufo (e)

- - Bufo (I)

Page 37: The Effect of Predator Chemical Cues and Conspecific Alarm

Eb

Early ext

Lb

Early ext

Eb

Late ext

Lb

Late ext

Eb

Fish cue

Lb

Fish cue

Eb

Dyt. cue

Lb

Oyt. cue

Table 1. Results of pairwise ANOV A's of early (E) stages (25-27) American toad, Bufo americanus, tadpoles (b) compared to late stages (41-42) toad tadpoles.

0 3 6 9 12 15 St.

Min Min. Min. Min. Min. Min. Mean Error F cal

34 45 35 51 56 53 46 5.22

0.721

37 32 49 39 45 34 41 2.61

40 35 52 53 48 55 47 4.37

22 39 41 40 42 47 39 3.16 2.256

31 31 35 37 48 43 34 3.51 0.636

24 34 42 37 32 37 34 3.98

38 31 34 36 39 30 35 2.98 0.633

42 34 29 42 37 38 37 3.21

P ca1

0.401

0.150

0.426

0.437

Page 38: The Effect of Predator Chemical Cues and Conspecific Alarm

Discussion

Lefcort (1998) found that group size affected behavior in larvae ofBufo terrestris

under laboratory conditions. Similarly, Graves et al. (1993) with B. cognatus, and Hokit

and Blaustein (1995) found further that larger groups of Rana cascadae tadpoles moved

significantly more than did smaller groups in response to predator cues. Group size may

be an especially important mediator of behavior in bufonids because of their complex

aggregative behaviors (Beiswenger 1975, Hokit and Blaustein 1995). In contrast to these

studies, however, I found that group size did not have any significant effect upon cover

use by B. americanus tadpoles regardless of the developmental stage. The possibility

exists that the numbers of tadpoles I used were too small to have a significant impact

upon tadpole behavior, although Rodel and Linsenmair (1997), using Phrynornantis

rnicrops, found density effects beginning with a sample size of n= lO tadpoles; group

sizes of 10 and larger preferred open waters. My findings do however support the results

of Hews and Blaustein (1985) who found that alarm substances tended to break up

swarms of tadpoles; it seems possible that this effect may also inhibit swarm formation in

the presence of predatory cues. Waldman and Adler (1979), Hokit and Blaustein (1995),

and Watt et al (1997), demonstrated that tadpoles are more likely to form aggregations

when with siblings than when with non-siblings. Since I collected tadpoles randomly it

was likely that the coefficient of relatedness among them was low, therefore inhibiting

the formation of swarms. After collection, tadpoles were reared with conspecifics, which

has also been demonstrated to affect the intensity with which tadpoles respond to

predator cues (Bridges and Gutzke 1997).

23

Page 39: The Effect of Predator Chemical Cues and Conspecific Alarm

Bufonids are known to recognize conspecifics through release of alarm signals

during predatory events (Hrbacek 1950, Adams and Claeson 1998), and to exhibit

avoidance tactics (Petranka 1989) upon detection of those signals. This early warning

system allows tadpoles to modify their behavior in advance of an encounter with a

predator and has been shown to significantly reduce predator success (Hews 1988). I

found that early stage toad tadpoles sought cover in signilicantly higher percentages

when exposed to extracts of both early and late stage conspecifics, which is congruent

with previous studies of other Bufonids (Petranka 1989, Petranka and Hayes 1998).

Although both cue types elicited an increase in the percentage of tadpoles seeking cover,

no apparent differences existed between extracts of early or late stages. This may be an

adaptive generalized antipredator response of early stage tadpoles to all stages of

conspecific alarm signals. These results suggest that no alterations to alarm signals occur

prior to metamorphosis, however, Belden et al. (2000) found an apparent alteration in

signals between larvae and juvenile Bufo boreas.

The rising percentages of tadpoles seeking cover over the 15 minute time period

in several of the experiments might be explained by slow diffusion rates of cues through

the test chamber. However, chemical cues are known to rapidly degrade, as

demonstrated by Petranka (1989), who showed that B. americanus tadpoles failed to

respond to alarm cues after 8 minutes in open water, thereby imposing a limit to

experimental duration.

Though they are often syntopic, American toad tadpoles exhibited no signilicant

response to R. sphenocephala larvae extract. This could be evolutionarily advantageous

because the two species are subject to different predators at least during the latter

24

Page 40: The Effect of Predator Chemical Cues and Conspecific Alarm

development stages, a possibility attributable to the great size of R. sphenocephala larvae.

Rana sphenocephala larvae attain a much greater size than toad tadpoles, and owing to

the fact that many tadpole predators are size-dependant (e.g. diving beetle larvae-­

Formanowicz 1986, Babbitt and Tanner 1998), ignoring alarm signals from

heterospecifics would be adaptive if no inherent threat existed.

Exposure to beetle larvae in enclosures as well as chemical cues from water taken

from larval beetle chambers resulted in increased use of available cover by toad tadpoles

as compared to controls. These treatments were designed to simulate two separate

situations in nature. By adding water, a situation existed similar to when a diving beetle

larva inhabits and then vacates an area; the cues slowly dissipate and degrade.

Interestingly, the number of tadpoles seeking cover with beetle larvae maintained in

enclosures was approximately 16% greater than when just chemical cues of larval beetles

were added. This may have been a response to the heightened risk of predation posed

when an actual predator was present. Anuran larvae are known to perceive and respond

differentially to varying levels of predation (Horat and Semlitsch 1994, Anholt et al.

1996, Laurila et al. 1997). It is possible that cues emanating from the caged larvae were

more potent than the chemical cues present in the water sample, or perhaps the tadpoles

may have sensed movement within the cage and responded with increased cover use.

Use of chemical cues from bluegill resulted in higher numbers of tadpoles seeking

cover as compared to the control. While bufonids have been shown to be unpalatable to

fish (Voris and Bacon 1966, Kruse and Stone 1984, Lefcort 1998), they may still respond

to fish cues if they operate under a generalized antipredator defense system. Lawler

(1989) demonstrated that refuge use increased by tadpoles in response to the presence of

25

Page 41: The Effect of Predator Chemical Cues and Conspecific Alarm

fish; furthermore, Sernlitsch and Gavasso (1992) also showed that tadpoles decreased

swimming time in the presence of fish (but see Kats et al. 1988). This strategy may be

adaptive for small, vulnerable larvae that may be injured or killed by naive predators

even if they were to be expelled, uneaten (Brodie et al. 1978). It is interesting to note

that following a 48 hour conditioning period the percentage of tadpoles seeking cover

decreased; this effect has been previously shown (Semlitsch and Ryer 1992). Tadpoles

are also known to respond to novel stimuli (Manteifel 1995), and in a geographic range

where toad tadpoles rarely encounter fis~ this is also a plausible explanation. During the

conditioning period, tadpoles either became habituated to the novel stimulus, or, through

lack of predatory attempts, failed to respond to fish cues. Quite surprisingly and

inexplicably, tadpoles that were "conditioned" with conditioned tap water sought cover at

a rate similar to tadpoles assigned to the fish cue treatment. It is unlikely that this

resulted from the simple addition of liquid because tadpoles conditioned with fish water

actually decreased cover usage.

In Part II, late stage tadpoles showed no significant responses to predators or

conspecific alarm signals as compared to the controls. It was found, however, that in

treatments involving use of early and late stage B. americanus extracts, the percentages of

tadpoles seeking cover increased by 8.8% over time, however, this was statistically

insignificant. Late developmental stage toad tadpoles in control trials were found seeking

cover in significantly higher percentages when compared to control trials with early

developmental stage toad tadpoles. This increased use of cover by late stage tadpoles

could explain the lack of statistical significance in further analyses of early versus late

developmental stage tadpoles. Perhaps this increased use of cover is a generalized

26

Page 42: The Effect of Predator Chemical Cues and Conspecific Alarm

response to predators by tadpoles at a highly vulnerable time in life. At Gosner (1960)

stage 42, tadpoles cease feeding (Beiswenger 1975), and production ofbufotoxin surges

(Brodie et al. 1978, Formanowicz and Brodie 1982), which may then become the primary

antipredator defense mechanism in this toad species.

Behavioral disparities could likely exist between early and late developmental

stage toad tadpoles due to a variety of factors such as differing suites of predators,

habitat, escape tactics, biochemistry, and chemical detection capability. Studies focusing

on these behavioral changes between juveniles and pre-metamorphic individuals are few,

but see (Bridges and Gutzke 1997, Belden et al. 2000). In this study, American toad

tadpoles responded adaptively to conspecific alarm signals and predator cues, at least in

early stages of development. However, this was not found to be the case with tadpoles

nearing metamorphosis. This study is one of the first to demonstrate that toad tadpoles

approaching metamorphic climax exhibit behaviors that are different than that of tadpoles

in earlier developmental stages.

27

Page 43: The Effect of Predator Chemical Cues and Conspecific Alarm

Literature Cited

Adams M. and S. Claeson. 1998. Field response of tadpoles to conspecific and

heterospecific alarm. Ethology 104:955-961.

Anholt B., D. Skelly, and E. Werner. 1996. Factors modifying antipredator behavior in

larval toads. Herpetologica 52:301-313.

Arnold S. and R. Wassersug. 1978. Differential predation on metamorphic anurans by

Garter snake (Thamnophis): social behavior as a possible defense. Ecology 59:367-

373.

Babbitt K. and G. Tanner. 1998. Effects of cover and predator size on survival and

development of Rana utricularia tadpoles. Oecologia 114:258-262.

Babbitt K. and G. Tanner. 1997. Effects of cover and predator identity on predation

of Hyla squirella tadpoles. J. Herp. 31: 128-130.

Babbitt K. and F. Jordan. 1996. Predation on Bufo terrestris tadpoles: effects of cover

and predator identity. Copeia 1996:485-488.

Beiswenger R. 1975. Structure and function in aggregations of tadpoles of the

American toad, Bufo americanus. Herpetologica 31 :222-233.

Belden L., E. Wildy, A. Hate~ and A. Blaustein. 2000. Juvenile western toads, Bufo

boreas, avoid chemical cues of snakes fed juvenile, but not larval, conspecifics.

Anim. Behav. 59:871-875.

Bridges C. and W. Gutzke. 1997. Effects of environmental history, sibship, and age

on predator-avoidance responses of tadpoles. Can. J. Zool. 75:87-93.

28

Page 44: The Effect of Predator Chemical Cues and Conspecific Alarm

Brodie E. Jr., D. Formanowicz Jr. , and E. Brodie III. 1978. The development of

noxiousness ofBufo americanus tadpoles to aquatic insect predators. Herpetologica

34:302-306.

Brodie E. Jr. and D. Formanowicz Jr. 1981. Larvae of the predaceous diving beetle

Dytiscus verticalis acquire an avoidance response to skin secretions of the newt

Notophthalrnus viridescens. Herpetologica 37: 172-176.

Brodie E. Jr. and D. Formanowicz Jr. 1983. Prey size preference of predators:

differential vulnerability of larval anurans. Herpetologica 39:67-75.

Brodie E. Jr., and D. Formanowicz Jr. 1987. Antipredator mechanisms of larval anurans:

protection of palatable individuals. Herpetologica 43:369-373.

Crump M. 1984. Ontogenetic changes in vulnerability to predation in tadpoles of

Hyla pseudopurna. Herpetologica 40:265-271.

Formanowicz D. Jr. and E. Brodie Jr. 1982. Relative palatabilities of members of a

larval amphibian community. Copeia 1982:91-97.

Formanowicz D. Jr. 1986. Anuran tadpole/aquatic insect predator-prey interactions:

tadpole size and predator capture success. Herpetologica 42:367-373.

Formanowicz D. Jr. 1987. Foraging tactic ofDytiscus verticalis larvae (Coleoptera:

Dytiscidae): prey detectio~ reactive distance and predator size. J. Kansas

Ent. Soc. 60:92-99.

Gosner K. 1960. A simplified table for staging anuran embryos and larvae with notes

on identification. Herpetologica 16: 183-190.

Graves B., C. Summers, and K. Olmstead. 1993. Sensory mediation of aggregation

among postmetarnorphic Bufo cognatus. J. Herp. 27:315-319.

29

Page 45: The Effect of Predator Chemical Cues and Conspecific Alarm

Hews D. and A. BJaustein. 1985. An investigation of the alarm response in Bufo boreas

and Rana cascadae tadpoles. Behav. Neural Biol. 43:47-57.

Hews D. 1988. A.Jann response in larval western toads, Bufo boreas: release of larval

chemicals by a natural predator and its effect on predator capture efficiency. Anim.

Behav. 36:125-133.

Hokit D. and A. Blaustein. 1995. Predator avoidance and alarm-response behavior in

kin-discriminating tadpoles (Rana cascadae). Ethology 101 :280-290.

Horat P, and R. Semlitsch. 1994. Effects of predation risk and hunger on the

behaviour of two species of tadpoles. Behav. Ecol. Socio. 34:393-401 .

Hrbacek J. 1950. On the flight reaction of tadpoles of the common toad caused by

chemical substances. Experientia 6: I 00-101 .

Huey R. 1980. Sprint velocity of tadpoles (Bufo boreas) through metamorphosis.

Copeia 1980:537-540.

Kats L, J. Petranka, and A. Sih. 1988. Antipredator defenses and the persistence of

ampluoian larvae with fishes. Ecology 69: 1865-1870.

Kiesecker J. , D. Chivers, A. Marco, C. Quilchanos, M. Anderson, and A. Blaustein.

1999. Identification of a disturbance signal in larval red-legged frogs, Rana aurora.

Anim. Behav. 57:1295-1300.

Kruse K. and B. Stone. 1984. Large-mouth bass (Micropterus salmoides) learn to

avoid feeding on toad (Bufo) tadpoles. Anim. Behav. 32: 1035-1039.

Kupferberg S. 1997. The role oflarval diet in anuran metamorphosis. Amer. Zool.

37: 146-159.

30

Page 46: The Effect of Predator Chemical Cues and Conspecific Alarm

Laurila A., J. Kujasalo, and E. Ranta. 1997. Different antipredator behaviour in two

anuran tadpoles: effects of predator diet. Bebav. Ecol. and Socio. 40:329-336.

Lawler S. 1989. Behavioral responses to predators and predation risk in four species of

larval anurans. Arum. Behav. 38:1039-1047.

Lefcort H. 1996. Adaptive, chemically mediated fright response in tadpoles of the

Southern frog, Rana utricularia. Copeia 1996:455-459.

Lefcort H. 1998. Chemically mediated fright response in Southern toad CBufo terrestris)

tadpoles. Copeia 1998:445-450.

Manteifel Y. 1995. Chemically-mediated avoidance of predators by Rana temporaria

tadpoles. J. Herp. 29:461-463.

NWA STATPAK. 1986. Version4.l. Northwest Analytical, Inc. Portland, Oregon.

Petranka J. , L. Kats, and A. Sib. 1987. Predator-prey interaction among fish and larval

amphibians: use of chemical cues to detect predatory fish. Anim. Behav. 35:420-425.

Petranka J. 1989. Response of toad tadpoles to conflicting chemical stimuli: predator

avoidance versus "optimal" foraging. Herpetologica 45: 283-292.

Petranka J. and L. Hayes. 1998. Chemically mediated avoidance of a predatory odonate

(Anax junius) by American toad (Bufo americanus) and wood frog~ ~lvatica)

tadpoles. Behav. Ecol. and Socio. 42:263-271.

Rodel M. and K Linsenmair. 1997. Predator-induced swarms in the tadpoles of an

African savanna frog, Phrynomantis microps. Ethology 103:902-914.

31

Page 47: The Effect of Predator Chemical Cues and Conspecific Alarm

Semlitsch R. and S. Gavasso. 1992. Behavioral responses ofBufo bufo and Bufo

calamita tadpoles to chemical cues of vertebrate and invertebrate predators. Ethol.

Ecol. and Evol. 4:165-173.

Scmlitsch R. and H. Reyer. 1992. Modification of anti-predator behaviour in tadpoles

by environmental conditioning. Ecology 61 :353-360.

Skelly D. and E. Werner. 1990. Behavioral and life-rustorical responses oflarval

American toads to an odonate predator. Ecology 71 :2313-2322.

Skelly D. 1994. Activity level and the susceptibility of anuran larvae to predation.

Anim. Behav. 47:465-468.

Sokal R. and F. Rohlf. 1981. Biometry (2nd ed.) W. H. Freeman and Co., San Francisco.

859 pp.

Voris H. and J. Bacon. 1966. Differential predation on tadpoles. Copeia 1966:594-598.

Waldman B. and K. Adler. 1979. Toad tadpoles associate preferentially with siblings.

Nature 282:611-613.

Waldman B. 1982. Sibling association among schooling toad tadpoles: field evidence

and implications. Anim. Behav. 30:700-713.

Watt P., S. Nottingham, and S. Young. 1997. Toad tadpole aggregation behaviour:

evidence for a predator avoidance function. Anim. Behav. 54:865-872.

Werner E. and B. Anholt. 1993. Ecological consequences of the trade-off between

growth and mortality rates mediated by foraging activity. Arn. Nat. 142:242-272.

Wilson D. and H. Lefcort. 1993. The effect of predator diet on the alann response of

red-legged frog, Rana aurora, tadpoles. Anim. Behav. 46:1017-1019.

32