59
St. Cloud State University theRepository at St. Cloud State Culminating Projects in Communication Sciences and Disorders Department of Communication Sciences and Disorders 5-2016 e Effect of Baby Sign on Early Language Development for "At-Risk" Populations Sarah S. Anderson St Cloud State University, [email protected] Follow this and additional works at: hps://repository.stcloudstate.edu/csd_etds is esis is brought to you for free and open access by the Department of Communication Sciences and Disorders at theRepository at St. Cloud State. It has been accepted for inclusion in Culminating Projects in Communication Sciences and Disorders by an authorized administrator of theRepository at St. Cloud State. For more information, please contact [email protected]. Recommended Citation Anderson, Sarah S., "e Effect of Baby Sign on Early Language Development for "At-Risk" Populations" (2016). Culminating Projects in Communication Sciences and Disorders. 3. hps://repository.stcloudstate.edu/csd_etds/3

The Effect of Baby Sign on Early Language Development for

  • Upload
    others

  • View
    1

  • Download
    0

Embed Size (px)

Citation preview

St. Cloud State UniversitytheRepository at St. Cloud StateCulminating Projects in Communication Sciencesand Disorders

Department of Communication Sciences andDisorders

5-2016

The Effect of Baby Sign on Early LanguageDevelopment for "At-Risk" PopulationsSarah S. AndersonSt Cloud State University, [email protected]

Follow this and additional works at: https://repository.stcloudstate.edu/csd_etds

This Thesis is brought to you for free and open access by the Department of Communication Sciences and Disorders at theRepository at St. CloudState. It has been accepted for inclusion in Culminating Projects in Communication Sciences and Disorders by an authorized administrator oftheRepository at St. Cloud State. For more information, please contact [email protected].

Recommended CitationAnderson, Sarah S., "The Effect of Baby Sign on Early Language Development for "At-Risk" Populations" (2016). Culminating Projectsin Communication Sciences and Disorders. 3.https://repository.stcloudstate.edu/csd_etds/3

Running header: EFFECT OF BABY SIGN ON LANGUAGE FOR AT-RISK POPULATIONS 1

The Effect of “Baby Sign” on Early Language Development for “At-Risk” Populations

By Sarah S. Anderson

A Thesis

Submitted to the Graduate Faculty of

St. Cloud State University

in Partial Fulfillment of the Requirements

for the Degree of

Master of Science

in Communication Sciences and Disorders

May 2016

Thesis Committee:

Dr. Sarah Smits-Bandstra, Chairperson

Dr. Rebecca Crowell

Dr. Amy Knopf

EFFECT OF BABY SIGN ON LANGUAGE FOR “AT-RISK” POPULATIONS 2

Abstract

The purpose of this case study was to examine the effect of exposure to symbolic gestures or

“Baby Sign” on the development of joint attention, receptive and expressive language of children

from low socioeconomic backgrounds, including culturally/linguistically diverse children aged 9

to 20 months. Two child-parent dyads participated in the program. Both were enrolled in the

local “Head Start” (Reach-Up) program. The MacArthur Bates, Preschool Language Scales-4

(PLS-4) and video-recorded interactions with the primary caregiver were used to assess the

effectiveness of Baby Sign parent-training to facilitate early language development. Results of

the Preschool Language Scales-4 and the MacArthur Bates identified improved both receptive

and expressive language in “at risk” (low socioeconomic and culturally diverse backgrounds)

infants exposed to Baby Sign. In addition, qualitative data collected by parent interview revealed

significant barriers to service delivery for early language facilitation, particularly for

culturally/linguistically diverse children from low socioeconomic backgrounds.

EFFECT OF BABY SIGN ON LANGUAGE FOR “AT-RISK” POPULATIONS 3

Acknowledgements

We would like to thank the staff at Reach-Up of St. Cloud for their collaborating during this

study, and undergraduate student, Julia McClung, for her assistance during data collection and

analysis. We would also like to thank St. Cloud State University Office of Sponsored Programs

Student Research Grant Award. Finally, we would like to thank our family and friends without

which this study could never had existed.

This study is dedicated to the child that lost his life during the process of this study.

EFFECT OF BABY SIGN ON LANGUAGE FOR “AT-RISK” POPULATIONS 4

Table of Contents

Chapter I: INTRODUCTION .........................................................................................................7

Chapter II: METHOD ....................................................................................................................17

Chapter III: RESULTS ..................................................................................................................22

Chapter IV: DISCUSSION ............................................................................................................28

Chapter V: CONCLUSION ...........................................................................................................36

References ......................................................................................................................................37

Appendixes ....................................................................................................................................46

Appendix A ................................................................................................................................47

Appendix B ................................................................................................................................48

Appendix C ................................................................................................................................50

Appendix D ................................................................................................................................54

EFFECT OF BABY SIGN ON LANGUAGE FOR “AT-RISK” POPULATIONS 5

List of Tables

Table 1 ...........................................................................................................................................23

Table 2 ...........................................................................................................................................24

Table 3 ...........................................................................................................................................25

Table 4 ...........................................................................................................................................26

Table 5 ...........................................................................................................................................27

Table 6 ...........................................................................................................................................32

EFFECT OF BABY SIGN ON LANGUAGE FOR “AT-RISK” POPULATIONS 6

List of Figures

Figure 1 ..........................................................................................................................................15

EFFECT OF BABY SIGN ON LANGUAGE FOR “AT-RISK” POPULATIONS 7

Chapter I: INTRODUCTION

Recently, research has been directed to improving the quality of early childhood care to

build and develop socio-linguistic foundations and promote overall learning (Levickis,

Girolametto, & Reilly, 2014). Early intervention, particularly before age 6, has been established

in literature for communication skills, because of a “critical period” of brain development at that

time (Uylings, 2006; Werker & Hensch, 2015). The irrefutable beneficial effects of early

intervention for communication skills has been demonstrated in auditory rehabilitation

(Campbell, Macsweeney, & Woll, 2014), autism (Webb, Jones, Kelly, & Dawson, 2014),

intellectual disability (Fujinaga, Shinagawa, Watanabe, Ogihara, Sasaki, & Hori, 2005), and

children “at risk” due to low socioeconomic status (Colmar, 2014).

A rising issue for early childhood centers developed for families from low socio-

economic status (SES) backgrounds is the culturally and linguistically diverse (CLD) families

whom they serve. For example, over 17% of children enrolled in the St Cloud “Reach-Up”

program are from CLD families. Of that percentage 58% have no receptive or expressive

English and the remaining 42% have only a “moderate” proficiency in English (Storm, personal

communication). Male infants and toddlers of this population are known to be at higher risk for

developing a language delay or disorder (Campbell, Dollaghan, Rockette, Paradise, Feldman,

Shriberg, & Kurs-Lasky, 2003; Letts, Edwards, Sinka, Schaefer, & Gibbons, 2013). Several

researchers have hypothesized this discrepancy may be because females develop at a more

accelerated rate than males.

Early childhood programs attempt to meet the needs of linguistically diverse families in a

functional way to promote a strong linguistic foundation. For example, when there is a

prominent language amongst the class of preschoolers that isn’t English, Head Start employs a

EFFECT OF BABY SIGN ON LANGUAGE FOR “AT-RISK” POPULATIONS 8

staff member that is a native speaker of that language to help promote the children’s first

language (L1). There is irrefutable evidence that promoting the development of the L1 in a

language learner helps to develop their second language (L2) (David, 2005; Murphy, 2014).

These programs would also benefit from an early intervention strategy to promote a strong

communication foundation within their infants and toddlers, no matter their first language.

The purpose of this research project is to investigate whether the implementation of Baby

Sign, is an effective early language facilitation program for low SES infants and toddlers from

CLD backgrounds.

Baby Sign History

The National Institute on Deafness and Other Communication Disorders (NIDCD) (2014)

defines American Sign Language (ASL) as a “complete, complex language that employs signs

made by moving the hands combined with facial expressions and postures of the body.”

According to NIDCD, ASL is primarily used by people in North America that associate

themselves with the Deaf1 and hard-of-hearing community. Within the past decade there has

been an increase of use of a simplified version of ASL with the targeted use as a means of early

communication with infants and toddlers (Nelson, White, & Grewe, 2012). There have been

several labels to refer to this method such as; deictic gesturing, symbolic gesture, sign training,

and Baby Sign. For the purposes of this research paper we will refer to this method as Baby

Sign.

Mueller, Sepulveda, and Rodriquez (2013) defined Baby Sign as facilitating

communication of normally developing hearing infants by early exposure to sign language.

Primarily, there is a use of functional everyday signs (i.e. cookie, water, more, all-done) that are

1 When the word “deaf” is capitalized (e.g. Deaf) it is a representation of someone who is deaf and associates

themselves with Deaf culture (i.e. other Deaf individuals, uses American Sign Language as a primary mode of

communication). It is also known as being “big D deaf.”

EFFECT OF BABY SIGN ON LANGUAGE FOR “AT-RISK” POPULATIONS 9

produced by a primary caregiver during optimal exposure times (e.g. feeding time and play

time). This production is initially done with the expectation of not having the sign immediately

reproduced by the child, but introduced as a symbol for an action and/or word. Over time, and

repeated use of the symbol by the caregiver within context-rich interactions, the child can easily

employ these Baby Signs, either separate from or in conjunction with verbalizations as a means

of communication.

Minnesota Head Start Association

The Minnesota Head Start Association Inc. established in 1987. The Administration for

Children and Families is a federally operated program that was established nation-wide in 1991

to provide quality childcare for families from low SES backgrounds to support infant and toddler

development. This federally operated program was established to identify and overcome issues

impacting the development of children within low SES populations. One program they have

established is providing affordable quality childcare for people with limited financial means.

This is important due to the lack of resources available for children of low SES background and

the negative impact it can have on their development (Levine, & Zimmerman, 2010).

Reach-Up of Central Minnesota in conjunction with the Minnesota Head Start

Association (MHSA) was established in 1979. This program specifically targets at risk families

with infants and toddlers to provide and facilitate support that is fundamental to early socio-

linguistic development. According to the Glossary of Education Reform the term “at risk”

encompasses any student that is considered to have a higher probability of academic failure

(Great Schools Partnership, 2014). Children from low SES tend to have limited exposure to

language (i.e. books, magazines, verbal language input) effecting their language development

(Clearfield & Jedd, 2013).

EFFECT OF BABY SIGN ON LANGUAGE FOR “AT-RISK” POPULATIONS 10

The goal of Reach-Up is to facilitate the development in children across all areas (e.g.

motor, cognitive, language, social, behavior) to ensure a successful transition to school. Within

the infant and toddler curriculum there are opportunities for sensory experiences, literacy

exposure, cognitive decision making, fine and gross motor activities, and communicational

attempts (Minnesota Head Start Association Inc., 2014). This curriculum was specifically

designed to address the areas of common deficits found for infants and toddlers from CLD

and/or low SES families, in particular, language delays.

Implications of Low SES and CLD for Early Language Development

It has been noted that children from a low SES background are at risk for developing a

language delay (Clegg & Ginsborg, 2006). It is believed that, due to a lack of parental

education, in combination with minimal resources, children experience a language-impoverished

environment (Clegg & Ginsborg, 2006). Common resources and experiences not available to

CLD or low SES children include, but are not limited to, literacy materials such as picture books,

social interactions with other children within their peer groups, appropriate verbal parental

interactions, and motor and sensory stimuli such as age-appropriate toys.

Researchers Letts, Edwards, Sinka, Schaefer, and Gibbons (2013) described the marked

differences in language development in children from a low SES background compared to their

middle-high SES peers. They found characteristics of less developed phonological inventory and

early literacy skills (i.e. page turning, finding the title) in children from low SES background.

They reasoned that when the maternal figure’s education level is of high school equivalent or

lower, a child’s language is more likely to fall behind. They further argued that, “any negative

influence of low SES on early language development may then indirectly affect the child’s life-

long educational prospects.”

EFFECT OF BABY SIGN ON LANGUAGE FOR “AT-RISK” POPULATIONS 11

In agreement with this research, Downing and Ollila (1977) reported that children from

low SES typically have less exposure to speech, writing, and/or print until they enter school.

The authors stated that when children aren’t exposed to speech, writing, and/or print at home at a

young age they don’t develop necessary pre-literacy skills as a foundation for learning to read

and consequently fall behind.

In addition to low SES, it has been noted that children from CLD backgrounds are at risk

for language delays (Clifford, Rhodes, & Paxton, 2014). People of this population typically have

limited resources to support their dual language learning children, and it has been found that few

parents in this population are aware that programs exist to help facilitate dual language learning

(Woolfenden, 2014).

Bilingual language learning is considered a risk factor not specifically because of the

complexities of learning two spoken languages simultaneously, but the diverse conditions the

child may experience while this dual language learning is occurring (Kohnert, 2008). Researcher

Kathryn Kohnert (2008) describes potential risk factors such as: age when dual language input

first occurs (e.g. easier to acquire when exposed during infancy), the environments in which

language exposure is taking place (i.e. home, television, school), the ‘social prestige’ and

community support given for both languages (i.e. if one language is considered more useful), if

these languages share any commonalities (i.e. both are romantic languages), and the specific

purpose these languages serve (e.g. communication in public vs. home).

Typical language developmental milestones are met in multi-linguistic populations,

consistent with their monolingual peers, as long as there is a strong foundation in the L1 (Janet,

2009) and environmental support is evidenced (Bohman, Bedore, Peña, Mendez-Perez, &

Gillam, 2010). However, the problem lies when typical English Language Learners (ELL) are

EFFECT OF BABY SIGN ON LANGUAGE FOR “AT-RISK” POPULATIONS 12

not as proficient at English as their monolingual peers causing academic difficulties especially in

grades pre-k through second grade (Paradis, Emmerzael, & Duncan, 2010; Lindholm-Leary,

2014).

Typical Language Developmental Milestones

Language acquisition has a typical developmental pattern that corresponds to a child’s

age. Milestones of language acquisition can be broken down to three major areas: pre-linguistic

(birth to 18 months), emergent (18-36 months), and developing (2-5 years of age; Paul &

Norbury, 2012). For the purposes of this study, we will focus on the milestones that correspond

to the later pre-linguistic stage, and the early emergent stage.

From birth to 8 months infants evidence perlocutionary speech acts consisting of

vegetative sounds, cooing, laughing, quasi-resonant nuclei, vocal play, canonical and

reduplicated babbling (Chapman, 2000; Miller, 1981; Gordon-Brannan & Weiss, 2007). By the

time an infant reaches 11-18 months of age (locutionary speech acts) they should have the ability

to comprehend 3-50 words and should also be producing their first true words. When an

infant/toddler reaches approximately 24 months of age they are producing at least 50 words and

up to 300 words expressively (Chapman, 2000; Miller, 1981; Gordon-Brannan & Weiss, 2007).

When discussing the role gestures play in relation to language development, researchers

Goodwyn, Acredolo, and Brown (2000) discussed that “the onset of intentional communication

is signaled by a small set of gestures which essentially launch the child into purposefully

communicating with others.” Gestures used for the intentional communication, or deictic

gestures, begin to occur as a language milestone around 10 months of age (Bates, 1976; Bates,

Benigni, Bretherton, Camaioni, & Volterra, 1979; Goodwyn et al., 2000). With the onset of

‘deictic gesturing’ and the lack of fine motor control necessary for proficient verbal

EFFECT OF BABY SIGN ON LANGUAGE FOR “AT-RISK” POPULATIONS 13

communication, there are several months, in infancy, where intentional communication and

verbal ability are disjointed. With the complexities involved in spoken language development

there seems to be a supplementation of gesturing by infants/toddlers as a means to communicate

(Goodwyn et. al, 2000).

Importance of Symbolic Gesture

When a child points to an object a caregiver doesn’t disregard this action as one of little

importance. Just the opposite, the caregiver recognizes this as a communication attempt, and

goes through a trial and error series to understand what the child may be referring to. The use of

gesturing as a pre-linguistic and emergent means of communication is one that should not be

overlooked rather, substantiated as a way to promote overall communication in infants and

toddlers. Iverson and Goldin-Meadow (2005) reported

Gestures that children produce early in language development provide a way for them to

communicate information that they cannot yet express verbally, offering children a

technique for referring to objects before they have words for those objects (p. 367).

Bates, Thal, Whitesell, Fenson, and Oakes (1989) explain that by the age of 18 months

the majority of children have come to the realization of the symbolic meaning of words, that

words can be used to represent objects in the world. By promoting the use and understanding of

gestures a caregiver is facilitating information that aids in the acquisition of verbal

communication. Goodwyn et al. (2000) proposed that, just as crawling serves as a critical

stepping-stone to walking, gestures serve the same purpose for speaking.

Bates et al. (1979) viewed gestures as a variant of symbolic communication and that

when communication was attempted intentionally, utilizing ‘conventional signals,’ it is a

‘process that precedes, correlates with, and hence possibly contributes to the emergence’ of

EFFECT OF BABY SIGN ON LANGUAGE FOR “AT-RISK” POPULATIONS 14

language. Based on the evidence and research of the authors reviewed above, the main premise

of this paper is that promoting the use of symbolic gesture through utilizing Baby Sign will

facilitate a child’s intentional expressive attempt at communicating, and furthermore their spoken

vocabulary.

Constructivist Theory

It has been theorized that one way children acquire linguistic knowledge is from exposure

to environmental input (Boyd & Goldberg, 2009; MacWhinney, 2004; Piaget, 2002; Ratner,

2013). This theory has several names but is most frequently called the Constructivist approach

(a.k.a. Interactionist or Empiricist approach). According to Owens (2012) a communication base

is established first, namely nonverbal communication (e.g. gestures), then a child is able to build

upon that framework to eventually communicate verbally. This building occurs over time with

environmental exposure (i.e. caregiver input). This theory is primarily attributed to Jean Piaget

and Lev Vygotsky who theorized that more information could be obtained through child play

(Pass, 2007; see figure 1).

EFFECT OF BABY SIGN ON LANGUAGE FOR “AT-RISK” POPULATIONS 15

Figure 1

Diagram of Piaget and Vygotsky’s Constructivist Theory

Information from Figure 1 obtained from: Pass, S. (2007). When Constructivists Jean Piaget

And Lev Vygotsky Were Pedagogical Collaborators: A Viewpoint From A Study Of

Their Communications. Journal Of Constructivist Psychology, 20(3), 277-282.

doi:10.1080/10720530701347944

Piaget (2002) explains that thought development is consecutive to a child’s language

development. It has been theorized, and found to be sound, that this development is

accomplished through social opportunities and language possibilities that are provided to the

child through adult models (Veneziano, 2013). The child is then able to acquire these models

through exposure, and eventually use in their verbal repertoire. It is anticipated that the results of

the current study will provide supportive evidence to support this theory of early language

development.

Previous Studies

Goodwyn, Acredolo, and Brown (2000) explored the effect of simple gestures with 11-

month old infants on early language development. Infants (n = 103) were randomly assigned into

Sign Training (ST), Verbal Training (VT) and Control conditions. Pre-intervention assessments

using the MacArthur Bates Communicative Developmental Inventory (Fenson et al., 2007) and

vocalization frequency during a caregiver/child 15-minute play session showed no significant

EFFECT OF BABY SIGN ON LANGUAGE FOR “AT-RISK” POPULATIONS 16

group differences. Caregivers in the ST group implemented symbolic gestures (e.g. fish, flower)

daily while VT caregivers targeted specific words (e.g. kitty, doggy). After 25 months it was

found that the ST experimental group significantly outperformed other groups in all areas of

language. Other studies have also found similar beneficial outcomes of implementing Baby Sign

during infancy on language development (Bates’ et al., 1989; Iverson & Goldin-Meadow, 2005;

Mueller’s et al., 2014).

Objective

Mueller and Sepulveda (2014) reported that the popularity of Baby Sign is increasing

despite a lack of “empirical” research supporting its use. Although there are many claims to the

benefits of incorporating Baby Sign as an alternative mode of communication for a child, there is

still a need for more research to substantiate these claims. In particular there is a paucity of

research regarding the effectiveness of early intervention strategies for low SES or CLD

populations (Kohnert, 2008).

Therefore, the primary objective of this research paper was to investigate whether the

implementation of Baby Sign is an effective early language facilitation program for low SES

infants and toddlers from CLD backgrounds.

Research Hypothesis

Based on the Constructionist Approach theoretical framework proposed by Boyd &

Goldberg (2009) it was predicted that infants, from “at-risk” populations, exposed to Baby Sign

during a critical period of linguistic development would demonstrate an improved receptive and

expressive vocabulary relative to matched controls.

EFFECT OF BABY SIGN ON LANGUAGE FOR “AT-RISK” POPULATIONS 17

Chapter II: METHOD

Research Design

Originally, the study was devised as a mixed experimental design with both within-group

and between-group comparisons. Interpretive statistics (ANOVA comparisons) were planned to

compare the results for parent-child dyads randomly assigned to experimental (Baby Sign) vs.

control conditions (fine motor skill training). Due to unforeseen difficulties with participant

recruitment and mortality, the study became a case study (see Discussion).

Screening Criteria

Infants participating in this program were screened by the Reach-Up staff with the Ages

& Stages Questionnaires and Ages & Stages Questionnaires: Social Emotional (Squires,

Twombly, Bricker, & Potter, 2009), as well as regular hearing/vision screenings. Participants had

ongoing developmental assessment by the Teaching Strategies Gold (Heroman, Burts, Berke &

Bickart, 2010) performed by Reach-Up staff.

Participants were excluded if they did not meet the age criteria specified and/or if the

parents did not agree to sign the participation waiver. Participants were not excluded from the

study if found to have previous use of Baby Sign, more than one language spoken in the home,

multiple (3 or more) ear infections prior to the study, or any language/developmental delays.

Participants

Participants were families participating in the Reach-Up Head Start of Minnesota

program. Participants self-selected by responding to flyers distributed by the director, teachers,

or home visit staff. Written informed consent was obtained and all participants were treated

according to ethical treatment of vulnerable human participant guidelines established by St

Cloud State University (Appendix A). Three parent-child dyads passed the screening criteria and

EFFECT OF BABY SIGN ON LANGUAGE FOR “AT-RISK” POPULATIONS 18

began the study. Data from one of the parent-child dyads was excluded because the dyad

withdrew from the study before completion.

Participant 1 (P1). P1 was a multi-cultural male of African American and Caucasian

decent, and had English spoken by both parents in the home. The mother reported to have

delivered P1 at full-term with no complications during pregnancy or birth. Prior to the study P1

had met all developmental milestones, had no significant health related problems, and had no

immediate (mother/father) family history of speech/language difficulties. P1 had previous

exposure to the following Baby Signs: milk, mom, dad, and ball.

Participant 2 (P2). P2 was a multi-cultural male of Caucasian and Hispanic

(Ecuadorian) decent, and had English spoken by the mother and English/Spanish spoken by the

father in the home. The mother reported to have delivered P2 at full-term with no complications

during pregnancy or birth. Prior to the study P2 had achieved all developmental milestones, had

no significant health related problems, and had no immediate (mother/father) family history of

speech/language difficulties. P2 had no previous exposure to Baby Signs.

Tasks and Procedures

Both the initial and post-training evaluations were completed by a graduate student in a

speech-language pathology program, and supervised by a licensed speech language pathologist

certified by the American Speech Language Hearing Association. Assessments were completed

in a small quiet room with both caregiver and the child present.

Baseline Measures. To assess the comparability of both participants at the beginning of

the study, both participants were compared on several demographic variables and baseline

language and communication measures similar to the study previously done by Goodwyn,

Acredolo and Brown (2000). Demographic variables included birth order, sex, and

EFFECT OF BABY SIGN ON LANGUAGE FOR “AT-RISK” POPULATIONS 19

maternal/paternal education (4 – pt scale: 1 = high school or less; 2 = some college; 3 = college

graduate; 4 = post-graduate work). The language measures at 10 months of age included:

participant history screening form (see Appendix B), maternal report of verbal and gestural

vocabulary (MacArthur Bates Communicative Developmental Inventory or CDI; Fenson et al.,

2007), the infants’ auditory comprehension and expressive communication from a standardized

assessment (Preschool Language Scale – 4th Ed. [PLS-4]; Zimmerman, Steiner & Pond, 2002),

and a measure of vocalization, joint attention, and communication attempt frequency during a

10-minute play session.

The reliability and validity of the standardized, norm-referenced measures are well

established (Mancilla-Martinez, Pan & Vagh, 2011; Qi & Marley, 2011). Previous studies

examining early language acquisition have employed both the PLS-4 (Volden et al., 2011) and

the CDI (Mancilla-Martinez, Pan & Vagh, 2011; Reilly et al., 2009) and found significant

differences on these assessment instruments after an intervention. No significant comprehensive

differences were found between P1 and P2. At the end of the initial assessment parents of P1 and

P2 scheduled their training session2.

Baby Sign Training. Parents completed an initial 30 minute seminar and, jointly with

the Primary Investigator (PI), designed a daily practice routine for implementation of the training

at home. The seminar included a video of the PI explaining the importance of early language

development and tips for implementing the Baby Sign (e.g. get down on the child’s level, be sure

to have the child’s attention, show child an item out of child’s reach, produce the sign with its

verbal counterpart simultaneously, wait for response from infant, produce sign/word again

assisting infant in production of sign, and then give infant item being withheld). After the video

2 P2 was assessed in the Spanish versions of the MacArthur Bates and PLS-4 for any discrepancies across languages.

No discrepancies were found between English and Spanish for P2.

EFFECT OF BABY SIGN ON LANGUAGE FOR “AT-RISK” POPULATIONS 20

the PI reiterated the process, and asked the parent to explain the process to verify understanding.

Five signs, unique to each child, were selected by the parent and the PI jointly for each parent-

child dyad to practice. Signs selected were words/signs that were not previously produced by the

child, but signaled a commonly occurring message in the child’s daily routine (e.g. all-done,

more, milk). Parents were given a book of common Baby Signs, which included their five signs

as well as others (Briant, 2004). Each target Baby Sign was highlighted within the book for easy

referral. Parents were asked to demonstrate production of all of the five signs, and also

demonstrated the implementation of one sign to check for understanding of process. When

completed, the PI explained the parent log (see Appendix C) and established with parents the

optimal times of day and frequency of implementing each sign (i.e. feeding time, play time, 3-5

times daily per sign were suggested). Parents and the PI then established the most convenient

day/time to call for bi-weekly phone interviews (i.e. during infant nap time).

Qualitative and Quantitative Phone Interviews. Parents were contacted bi-weekly

during the 14-week training period to ensure they were maintaining the training protocol.

During this conversation a list of specific questions were asked (see Appendix D). The first

interview was preformed one week post-training. The interview collected information about

parent feelings and perceptions of the training program, as well as frequency and implementation

of Baby Signs.

Post-Training Evaluation. At the conclusion of the 14-week training period both

participants completed a 60-minute post-training evaluation. The post-training evaluation

included re-administration of the PLS-4, the CDI, and a 10-minute video-recorded free play

session with the caregiver.

EFFECT OF BABY SIGN ON LANGUAGE FOR “AT-RISK” POPULATIONS 21

Dependent Variables. Dependent variables of performance on the task included scores

on the CDI and PLS-4 as well as the number of vocal attempts, sign attempts, joint attention, and

total communication attempts within the 10-minute play sample.

Speech samples were video and audio recorded, and all types of communication attempts

were analyzed off-line3 by the PI. The PI counted the total number of each type of attempt for

each minute of interaction. A second rater (BR), blind to the conditions of the study, also

analyzed the recordings in one-minute intervals. Percentage agreement was calculated between

the two raters for 25% of the samples to determine inter-rater reliability and to minimize

investigator bias.

3 Not in real time.

EFFECT OF BABY SIGN ON LANGUAGE FOR “AT-RISK” POPULATIONS 22

Chapter III: RESULTS

A total of 3 parent-child dyad participants were recruited to participate in this study. Due

to attrition, 1 participant dyad left the study, the remaining 2 dyads continued the study in its

entirety. Both participants were initially screened at 9-months 24-days old.

Screening Results

Based upon previously discussed criteria during the screening process (see Methods), it

was deemed that both participants were appropriate subjects for this study. The following data

was collected through: parent interview, 10-minute play sample, CDI, and the PLS-4. After

initial qualitative data was collected, ongoing data was collected through bi-weekly parent phone

calls. P1 conducted re-assessments 14-weeks post-intervention using the same assessments from

the initial assessment. Due to scheduling complications, P2 was re-assessed 16-weeks post-

intervention.

Pre-Post Percentiles for CDI

Analysis was conducted using the results acquired through the use of the CDI. The CDI

assesses the child’s current abilities through the use of Early Words (e.g. Phrases Understood,

Words Understood, and Words Produced), and Actions and Gestures (e.g. Total Gestures). The

results are then represented through a percentile rank when compared to other same-aged peers.

Both participants’ results from pre-intervention and post-intervention are depicted below (see

Table 1).

EFFECT OF BABY SIGN ON LANGUAGE FOR “AT-RISK” POPULATIONS 23

Table 1

Pre-Post Intervention Results for CDI

Participant Category of Language Pre-

Intervention

Percentile

Post-Intervention

Percentile

Change

(+/-)

P1 Part I: Early

Words

Phrases

Understood

35% 55% +20%

Words

Understood

60% 65% +5%

Words

Produced

65% 80% +15%

Part II:

Actions and

Gestures

Total

Gestures

65% 85% +20%

P2 Part I: Early

Words

Phrases

Understood

50% 75% +25%

Words

Understood

65% 45% -25%

Words

Produced

55% 55% No

change

Part II:

Actions and

Gestures

Total

Gestures

40% 45% +5%

Both participants showed a percentile rank increase between 20-25% in Phrases

understood, and 5-20% in Total Gestures used when analyzed with the CDI.

Pre-Post Standardized Scores and Percentiles for PLS-4

Analysis was conducted using the results acquired through the use of the PLS-4. The

PLS-4 assesses the child’s current abilities in subtests entitled Auditory Comprehension,

Expressive Communication, and Total Language Scores. The results are then analyzed and

represented through standardized scores (SS) and percentile ranks when compared to other same-

aged peers. Both participants’ results from pre-intervention and post-intervention are depicted

below (see Table 2).

EFFECT OF BABY SIGN ON LANGUAGE FOR “AT-RISK” POPULATIONS 24

Table 2

Pre-Post Intervention Results for the PLS-4

Participant Category of

Language

Pre-

Intervention

SS

Post-

Intervention

SS

Pre-

Intervention

Percentile

Rank

Post-

Intervention

Percentile

Rank

P1 Auditory

Comprehension

93 117 32 87

Expressive

Communication

98 120 45 91

Total Language

Score

100 121 50 92

P2 Auditory

Comprehension

103 109 58 73

Expressive

Communication

97 113 42 99

Total Language

Score

100 113 50 81

Both participants showed an increase in standard score on receptive (Auditory

Comprehension) language (P1 increased 24 points and P2 increased 6 points), Expressive

Language/Communication (P1 increased 28 points and P2 increased 16 points), and Total

Language Scores (P1 increased 21 points and P2 increased 13).

Pre-Post Analysis of Video-Recorded Interaction Samples

Analysis was conducted using the number of vocal attempts, sign attempts, joint

attention, and total communication attempts within the 10-minute play video recorded samples.

A rater (BR), blind to the study, was trained in the video recording analysis (e.g. vocal attempts,

sign attempts, and joint attention). BR’s results were then compared to the PI’s analysis results.

This comparison is depicted below (see Table 3).

EFFECT OF BABY SIGN ON LANGUAGE FOR “AT-RISK” POPULATIONS 25

Table 3

Results of 10-minute Communication Sample

Video Rater

Vocal

Attempts

Rater

Consist-

ency

Sign

Attempts

# of Joint

Attention

Occur-

rences > 2

seconds

Rater

Consist-

ency

# of

conversational

turns > 2

P1V1

Julia

(Blind

Rater) 52 96.29% 0 14 85.70% 2

P1V1 Sarah 54 96.29% 0 12 85.70% 3

P1V2

Julia

(Blind

Rater) 18 94.40% 0 13 94.40% 3

P1V2 Sarah 17 94.40% 0 13 94.40% 2

P2V1 Sarah 5 N/A 0 10 N/A 0

P2V2 Sarah 15 N/A 1 (reach) 10 N/A 0

Neither of the participants showed change in joint attention, sign attempts, or number of

conversational turns greater than two. P1 showed a decrease in vocal attempts from

approximately 53 to approximately 18, whereas, P2 showed an increase of vocal attempts from 5

to 15. However, we do not consider these vocal attempts to be significant. Rather they match

appropriately to participants’ activity choice during the 10- minute segment (e.g. P1 Video 1

pulling items from a box/bin and handing them to parent, P1 Video 2 walking items from one

room to the next, P2 Video 1 book reading with parent, P2 Video 2 removing items from a

box/bin and showing them to parent).

Interview Results

Scheduled interviews were initially set up to take place bi-weekly to help reinforce the

continued use of the targeted intervention, identify barriers, and monitor progress made. It was

found during each interview that both participants reported the intervention as going “good.” It

was also found that both participants would recommend this form of intervention to other

EFFECT OF BABY SIGN ON LANGUAGE FOR “AT-RISK” POPULATIONS 26

parents. P2 reported this intervention as being “an easy way to communicate with your child

before they can talk…very cool to have that communication already.” Both participants reported

the results of the intervention being “exciting” to watch, and beneficial to their child’s ability to

receive and express communicatively. Additionally, both participants found that intervention

was most efficient during feeding time and it provided more opportunities for models of the

signs. Lastly, both participants referred to the positive benefits of explaining how their child was

communicating with others (i.e. signing to friends and family). P1 reported that her family was

“really impressed by him being able to sign.”

P1. P1 participated in 5/7 phone interview opportunities. Due to several barriers (i.e.

participant’s phone disconnected, unavailable to answer the phone, unable to return phone call)

two phone interviews did not take place. A portion of the results collected during the interviews

are reported in the table below (see Table 4).

Table 4

Results of P1 Phone Interviews

Date Avg. #of times

signs were

modeled

Ideal time to

model signs

Avg. # of signs

attempted by

child

Signs produced with or

without vocalization?

6-17 3-5x per day Feeding/play 1 Without

7-1 3-5x per day Feeding/play 3 Without

7-27 3-5x per day Feeding 4 With

8-12 2-3x per week Feeding 4 With

9-3 2-3x per week Feeding 5 With

P1 showed a notable increase in the average number of signs attempted and production of

signs with vocalization. P1 also showed a decrease in the number of models provided overtime

(3-5x per day to 2-3x per week).

EFFECT OF BABY SIGN ON LANGUAGE FOR “AT-RISK” POPULATIONS 27

P2. P2 was able to participate in 6/7 phone interview opportunities. Due to several

barriers (i.e. unavailable to answer the phone, unable to return phone call) one phone interview

did not take place. Quantitative results collected during the interviews are reported in the table

below (see Table 5).

Table 5

Results of P2 Phone Interviews

Date Avg. #of times

signs were

modeled

Ideal time to

model signs

Avg. # of signs

attempted by

child

Signs produced with or

without vocalization?

7-17 3-5x per day Feeding 1 Without

7-31 3-5x per day Feeding 2 Without

8-16 1-3x per day Feeding 4 With

8-31 3-5x per day Feeding 3 With

9-18 2-3x per week Feeding 4 With

10-2 1-3x per day Feeding 4 With

P2 showed a substantial increase in the average number of signs attempted and

production of signs with vocalization. P2 also showed a decrease in the number of models

provided overtime (3-5x per day to 1-3x per day).

While working with culturally and linguistically diverse populations, several barriers

were experienced or reported during phone interviews that hindered compliance with the

research protocol. Some of these barriers experienced during this study included: life-

threatening domestic violence, inability to maintain/follow through with intervention,

insufficient time, mental health issues, insufficient safety, and/or insufficient housing.

EFFECT OF BABY SIGN ON LANGUAGE FOR “AT-RISK” POPULATIONS 28

Chapter IV: DISCUSSION

The results of the present study support the research hypothesis that infants, from “at-

risk” populations, exposed to Baby Sign during a critical period of linguistic development would

demonstrate an improved receptive and expressive vocabulary relative to matched controls. In

addition, these results also support the constructivist theory suggesting that one way children

acquire linguistic knowledge is from exposure to environmental input (Boyd & Goldberg, 2009;

MacWhinney, 2004; Piaget, 2002; Ratner, 2013).

Research Design

Due to unforeseen difficulties with participant recruitment and mortality the study

became a case study. Some of the strengths of conducting a case study include: getting a more

thorough understanding of the participant’s emotional and environmental needs instead of just

the notation of exhibited behaviors, as well as the allowance for creativity and innovative therapy

approaches. In addition, case studies can be used as a basis for theory in clinical application, and

they allow for direction in further research (Portney & Watkins, 2009).

However, as with any study design, case studies have several drawbacks as well. Some

drawbacks of utilizing a case study include difficulty justifying that the outcomes of the study

were solely produced from the experimental variable, and not from an outside variable. For

example, a case study design does not allow control for improvement due to maturation.

However, utilization of the norm-referenced PLS-4 assessment test allowed comparison of

participants with same-age peers to, at least partially address this confound. With larger studies

it is easier to determine the effectiveness of treatment based on the magnitude of participant

outcomes. When the participant size is smaller there is more to be justified when providing

EFFECT OF BABY SIGN ON LANGUAGE FOR “AT-RISK” POPULATIONS 29

supportive evidence for the experimental variable outcome (i.e. listing all possible variables and

how they impact the study results; Portney & Watkins, 2009).

Initially, we had anticipated having a larger subject size (6-12 subjects), but had

challenges recruiting participants within our criteria and experienced attrition. Had we known

prior to our study the participant size would result in two parent-dyads we would have

implemented a single-subject experimental research design with multiple baselines across

subjects because of the strengths of this design. Within a multiple baseline design, by providing

and removing treatment for a length of time, researchers have an opportunity to observe the

effects that therapy provides. In addition, this type of research design provides the ability to rule

out any other subsequent, non-therapy behaviors that may be affecting the outcome (Morgan &

Morgan, 2009).

CDI

Support for the research hypothesis was evidenced by the notable increase in receptive

and expressive language outcomes captured by the CDI. Results of the CDI Early Words showed

an increase of percentile rank for both participants (P1, 20%; P2, 25%) in Phrases Understood,

and an increase of percentile rank for Total Gestures when compared to same-aged peers (P1,

20%; P2, 5%). These results mirror those found in previous research (Goodwyn, Acredolo, &

Brown, 2000; Suanda & Namy, 2013).

However, it should be noted that there was a decrease in the Words Understood

percentile rank for P2 (25%) on the CDI, which was not consistent with the results of previous

research (Suanda & Namy, 2013).

This difference could be accounted for in several ways. For example, this effect may be

due to the parents’ habitual use of phrases with the determiner/pronoun “that” with their child

EFFECT OF BABY SIGN ON LANGUAGE FOR “AT-RISK” POPULATIONS 30

versus specific words (i.e. “bring me that” vs. “bring me the ball”). Parent 2 reported in

interviews that she was, at times, unsure if her child knew the specific words, but she was

confident in knowing that her child understood phrases clearly at the post-intervention

assessment. The results for single words understood may not be representative to P2’s skill level

at the time. A clinician-administered vocabulary test/screening may have been useful as a

supplement to parent reported treatment outcome measures and may have captured different

results for this area of development.

PLS-4

Results of the PLS-4 expressive and receptive language showed a substantial increase in

standard scores by both participants on Auditory Comprehension (P1 increased 24 points and P2

increased 6 points), Expressive Communication (P1 increased 28 points and P2 increased 16

points), and Total Language Scores (P1 increased 21 points and P2 increased 13). Language

improvements found on standardized tests were also found by the information collected during

continued bi-weekly phone interviews. Results from these two data sources support the research

hypothesis and constructivist theory. These results are similar to those found by researchers

Goodwyn, Acredolo, and Brown (2000). For example, these researchers also found that there

was a significant increase in receptive and expressive language when compared to both of their

control groups (verbal training and non-intervention).

Video-Recorded Interactions

Results of the 10-minute video showed that both participants had no change in joint

attention, sign attempts, or number of conversational turns greater than two. P1 showed a

decrease in vocal attempts from approximately 53 to approximately 18, whereas, P2 showed an

increase of vocal attempts from 5 to 15. Vocal attempts captured in one 10-minute video pre and

EFFECT OF BABY SIGN ON LANGUAGE FOR “AT-RISK” POPULATIONS 31

post-treatment did not appear to reliably reflect participants learning or the experimental effect.

Rather, communication attempt frequency tended to correspond to participants’ activity choice

during the 10- minute segment (e.g. P1V1 pulling items from a box/bin and handing them to

parent, P1V2 walking items from one room to the next, P2V1 book reading with parent, P2V2

removing items from a box/bin and showing them to parent).

The finding that short videos were not optimal measures for detecting experimental

effects is not unexpected. Researchers Goodwyn, Acredolo, and Brown (2000) also indicated

that the information collected during their video play segments was insightful, but did not

indicate any significant change for participants in the earliest age group. They concluded,

however, that more reliable data and significant changes in communication behavior during

infant video-recorded play was for subjects 24-months of age or older. This finding suggests that

parent interview and report may serve as more valid and reliable measures of communication

change of over time for children younger than two. This finding also suggests that a range of

different measures, including clinician-administered standardized tests, parent report, and video

are necessary to get a true picture of child communication changes.

Interview Results

The research hypothesis was also supported by the information collected during bi-

weekly phone interviews. These results were similar to those found by researchers Goodwyn et

al., 2000). Goodwyn et al. (2000) reported that subjects in their “sign training” group out

preformed their same aged subjects in their control group in receptive, expressive, and non-

verbal communication (p. 94-96).

Results of the bi-weekly phone interview showed a noteworthy increase in the average

number of signs attempted and production of signs with vocalization with both P1 and P2. Both

EFFECT OF BABY SIGN ON LANGUAGE FOR “AT-RISK” POPULATIONS 32

participants also showed a decrease in the number of models provided over time. This indicated

that the number of models needed for the participants to successfully engrain the newly acquired

signs decreased as the signs became more habitual and generalized. Results indicated that the

ideal modeling time for both participants over time was during feeding. This could be due to the

direct one-on-one attention received by the participants, and their parents’ ability to hold the

participants’ attention for a length of time. This finding indicated that future interventions

planned for children of this age in the home may consider implementing/training strategies

around regular home routines such as feeding.

Implications for Future Studies

Data acquired through phone interviews, and entering the home, also suggested several

barriers to the study when working with at-risk populations (e.g. phone being disconnected, not

always modeling signs daily, life threatening domestic violence, insufficient time). These results

had important implications for future studies to consider when working with these populations

(see Table 6)

Table 6

Implications for Future Studies

Concerns While Entering

the Home

Loss and Mandated Reporting Barriers

Primary Investigator safety Child safety Domestic violence

Disarray and disorder in the home

(e.g. food remnants and clothing on

the floor)

Mandated Reporting of abuse and

neglect

Housing relocation

Aggressive behavior from family

members

Loss of life Phone disconnection

EFFECT OF BABY SIGN ON LANGUAGE FOR “AT-RISK” POPULATIONS 33

Disconnected Phone. In one instance, the PI attempted to call P1 several times over a 2-

week period where the P1’s phone services were disconnected. This instance affected the ability

to collect data effectively (both qualitative and quantitative). In addition, this experience

appeared to initially cause some embarrassment for P1’s mother the next time the PI was able to

make contact. The PI expressed to her that “these things happen,” which reassured the mother,

but it could be something that could negatively impact future studies (i.e. attrition).

Concerns While Entering the Home. In another instance, the PI and an assistant went

to a participant’s home. This home was overwhelmingly in disarray. It was difficult for the PI

the assistant to find a place to sit due to the abundance of toys, clothing, food remnants, and

garbage that littered the floor. This did not seem to upset the children and/or parents, but it did

make it difficult for the PI and the assistant to assess, train, and collect data affectively. It was

difficult to ensure standardized/regulated assessment conditions. The participant would play

with toy items that were not brought by the PI/assistant, and the participant would put food items

into their mouth that had been sitting on the floor for an undetermined amount of time. The

PI/assistant would assist the parent often by removing the food items, and asking if they could

move some toys/clothes to an area out-of-reach. However, future researchers should be aware of

these potentials issues, and devise a plan in case they run into similar issues.

There were moments of concern for the investigators when the PI and assistant entered a

participant’s home. The PI followed the protocol of calling someone, to let them know where

they were going, how long they would be gone, and that they would call when the PI was out of

the home. This was done for the safety of the PI and assistant. The participants lived in areas

that were known to have frequent crime, and safety was important to consider. Additionally,

several participants’ mothers had significant others with a history of aggressive behavior. Future

EFFECT OF BABY SIGN ON LANGUAGE FOR “AT-RISK” POPULATIONS 34

studies should consider a similar protocol for investigators’ safety, and have a system in place for

immediate assistance if in danger (i.e. texting an outside source to call the police).

There were moments of concern for the participants when the PI entered the home. One

of these concerns was due to the older siblings being physically aggressive towards the

participants (i.e. pushing, pulling, hitting, throwing toys). There were several instances where

the parent needed to interfere on the participant’s behalf. These situations were potentially fatal,

and were not always swiftly remedied by the observing parent. Lastly, one participant’s parent

(not involved in the study) had a known history of aggression and drug use. Even though this

parent was not a part of the household at the time, it is important to consider in case that parent

should return. It should be recommended that researchers be prepared for situations in which

mandated reporting are necessary.

Loss and Mandated Reporting. Researchers should also be prepared for the loss of a

participant. Due to the risk of domestic abuse and heightened stress in these populations it is

important for researchers to know that there is a chance were their participant could be

physically harmed and/or die. During the course of this study one potential participant lost their

life due to domestic abuse. This event happened 3 days prior to the PI going into the infant’s

home. Throughout the study the PI had grown to know the participant and their family well.

The news of the death was devastating.

It is important for future researchers to consider the possibility of domestic abuse, infant

neglect, and harm done to the investigators. The safety of the research team and participants

should not be taken lightly, and the investigators should know where to go if they should need to

seek other professional help (i.e. police, counselor, social work professionals).

EFFECT OF BABY SIGN ON LANGUAGE FOR “AT-RISK” POPULATIONS 35

Barriers. Additional qualitative results found during the phone interview, and

throughout the study, indicated several barriers while working culturally and linguistically

diverse populations. These barriers made it difficult and/or impossible for participants to access

early intervention services during this study. While some experienced a majority of the barriers

listed, all experienced at least three: life threatening domestic violence, participant resident

relocation, financial difficulty, child care, transportation, inability to maintain/follow through

with intervention, insufficient time, mental health issues, insufficient safety, and/or insufficient

housing. These barriers were significant findings to consider for any further research conducted

with similar populations.

Continued Research. The results found by this study have multiple implications, and

provide numerous areas of continued research. It is recommended that more research be

conducted in this area with more participants, longer duration, and additional measures.

Additional research may include a team approach including other professions such as social

work, physical and occupational therapy, and/or applied behavior analysts. In addition, with

longer duration, it is recommended that literacy should be another targeted area to be included in

a longitudinal study.

EFFECT OF BABY SIGN ON LANGUAGE FOR “AT-RISK” POPULATIONS 36

Chapter V: CONCLUSION

In summary, the results of this study support the research hypothesis that implementation

of Baby Sign as a form of early language development intervention for “at-risk” populations can

be beneficial to the infants/toddlers. More and more, early childhood center programs are

developed for families from low SES and CLD populations. Within these populations early

intervention is crucial for optimal language development. Results of this study further

substantiate the advantages found in the implementation of Baby Sign as a form of early

intervention for receptive and expressive language in infants of “at-risk” populations. However,

it is recommended that further, and more extensive, research be conducted to study the long-term

benefits of such an intervention within this population.

EFFECT OF BABY SIGN ON LANGUAGE FOR “AT-RISK” POPULATIONS 37

References

Acredolo, L. P., & Goodwyn, S. W. (1985). Symbolic gesturing in language development: A

case study. Human Development, 28, 40±49.

Acredolo, L. P., & Goodwyn, S. W. (1988). Symbolic gesturing in normal infants. Child

Development, 59, 450±466.

Bates, E. (1976). Language and context: The acquisition of pragmatics. New York, NY:

Academic Press.

Bates, E., Benigni, L., Bretherton, I., Camaioni, L., & Volterra, V. (1979). The emergence of

symbols: Cognition and communication in infancy. New York: Academic Press.

Bates, E., Bretherton, I., Shore, C., & McNew, S. (1983). Names, gestures, and objects:

Symbolization in infancy and aphasia. In K. Nelson (Ed), Children’s language: Volume

IV (pp. 59-124). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

Bates, E., Thal, D., Whitesell, K., Fenson, L., & Oakes, L. (1989). Integrating Language and

Gesture in Infancy. Developmental Psychology, 25(6), 1004-1019.

Briant, Monta Z. (2004). Baby Sign Language Basics: Early Communication for Hearing Babies

and Toddlers. Carlsbad, CA: Hay House Inc.

Bohman, T. M., Bedore, L. M., Peña, E. D., Mendez-Perez, A., & Gillam, R. B. (2010). What

you hear and what you say: language performance in Spanish-English

bilinguals. International Journal Of Bilingual Education & Bilingualism,13 (3), 325-344.

doi:10.1080/13670050903342019

Boyd, J. K., & Goldberg, A. E. (2009). Input Effects Within a Constructionist

Framework. Modern Language Journal, 93(3), 418-429. doi:10.1111/j.1540-

4781.2009.00899.x

EFFECT OF BABY SIGN ON LANGUAGE FOR “AT-RISK” POPULATIONS 38

Campbell, R., Macsweeney, M., & Woll, B. (2014). Cochlear implantation (CI) for prelingual

deafness: The relevance of studies of brain organization and the role of first language

acquisition in considering outcome success. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 8(OCT).

doi:10.3389/fnhum.2014.00834

Carpenter, M. & Liebal, K. Joint attention, communication, and knowing together in infancy. In

A. Seeman (ed). Joint attention: New Developments (in press).

Clearfield, M. W., & Jedd, K. E. (2013). The Effects of Socio-Economic Status on Infant

Attention. Infant & Child Development, 22(1), 53-67. doi:10.1002/icd.1770

Clegg, J., & Ginsborg, J. (2006). Language and social disadvantage: Theory into practice.

Hoboken, NJ, US: John Wiley & Sons Inc.

Clifford, V., Rhodes, A., & Paxton, G. (2014). Learning difficulties or learning English

difficulties? Additional language acquisition: An update for paediatricians. Journal of

Peadiatrics & Child Health, 50(3), 175-181. doi:10.1111/jpc.12396

Colmar, S. H. (2014). A Parent-Based Book-Reading Intervention for Disadvantaged Children

with Language Difficulties. Child Language Teaching and Therapy, 30(1), 79-90.

David, J. (2005). Head Start Embraces Language Diversity. Young Children, 60(6), 40-43.

Doherty-Sneddon, G. (2008). The great Baby Signing debate. The Psychologist, 21(4), 300-303.

Dowshen, S. A., Izenberg, N., & Bass, E. R. (2002). The Kidshealth Guide for Parents:

Pregnancy to Age 5. Chicago: Contemporary Books.

Edwards, C., & Willis, L. (2000). Integrating Visual and Verbal Literacies in the Early

Childhood Classroom. Early Childhood Education Journal, 27(4), 259.

EFFECT OF BABY SIGN ON LANGUAGE FOR “AT-RISK” POPULATIONS 39

Fenson, L., Marchman, V. A., Thal, D. J., Dale, P. S., Reznick, J. S., & Bates, E. (2007).

MacArthur-Bates Communicative Development Inventories (2nd ed.). Baltimore:

Brookes.

Fujinaga, T., Shinagawa, R., Watanabe, C., Ogihara, M., Sasaki, T., & Hori, A. (2005). Early

Therapeutic Intervention for Down Syndrome Children, and Mothers' Attitudes toward

Childrearing. Japanese Journal of Developmental Psychology, 16(1), 81-91.

Goodwyn, S. W., & Acredolo, L. P. (1993). Symbolic gesture versus word: Is there a modality

advantage for onset of symbol use?. Child Development, 64(3), 688-701.

doi:10.1111/1467-8624.ep9308115009

Goodwyn, S. W., Acredolo, L. P., & Brown, C. A. (2000). Impact of Symbolic Gesturing on

Early Language Development. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 24(2), 81-103.

Gordon-Brannan, M. E., & Weiss, C. E. (2007). Clinical management of articulatory and

phonologic disorders. Philadelphia: Lippincott Williams & Wilkins, c2007.

Great Schools Partnership. (2014). The Glossary of Education Reform: For Journalists, Parents,

and Community Members. Retrieved from: http://edglossary.org/at-risk/.

Gunnar, M. R., & Donahue, M. (1980). Sex Differences in Social Responsiveness between Six

Months and Twelve Months. Child Development, 51(1), 262-265. doi:10.1111/1467-

8624.ep12325484

Heroman, D.C., Burts, K., Berke, T.S., Bickart. (2010) Teaching Strategies for Early Childhood.

GOLD. Baltimore: Paul H. Brookes Publishing.

Holmes, K.M., & Holmes, D. W. (1980). Signed and spoken language development in a hearing

child of hearing parents. Sign Language Studies, 28, 239-254.

EFFECT OF BABY SIGN ON LANGUAGE FOR “AT-RISK” POPULATIONS 40

Iverson, J. M., & Goldin-Meadow, S. (2005). Gesture Paves the Way for Language

Development. Psychological Science (Wiley-Blackwell), 16(5), 367-371.

doi:10.1111/j.0956-7976.2005.01542.x

Janet F., W. (2009). Bilingual beginnings to learning words. Philosophical Transactions of the

Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 364(1536), 3649-3663.

Kohnert, K. (2008). Language disorders in bilingual children and adults. San Diego, CA: Plural

Pub., c2008.

Levickis, P., Girolametto, L., & Reilly, S. (2014). Maternal behaviors promoting language

acquisition in slow-to-talk toddlers: prospective community-based study. Journal of

Developmental & Behavioral Pediatrics, (4). 274.

Levine, Phillip B. & Zimmerman, David J. (2010). Targeting Investments in Children: Fighting

Poverty When Resources Are Limited. University of Chicago Press.

doi:10.7208/chicago/9780226475837.001.0001

Letts, C., Edwards, S., Sinka, I., Schaefer, B., & Gibbons, W. (2013). Socio‐economic status and

language acquisition: Children's performance on the new Reynell Developmental

Language Scales. International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders, 48(2),

131-143.

Lindholm-Leary, K. (2014). Bilingual and Biliteracy Skills in Young Spanish-Speaking Low-

SES Children: Impact of Instructional Language and Primary Language

Proficiency. International Journal of Bilingual Education and Bilingualism, 17(2), 144-

159.

EFFECT OF BABY SIGN ON LANGUAGE FOR “AT-RISK” POPULATIONS 41

Mancilla-Martinez, J., Pan, B. A., & Vagh, S. B. (2011). Assessing the Productive Vocabulary of

Spanish-English Bilingual Toddlers from Low-Income Families. Applied

Psycholinguistics, 32(2), 333-357.

MacWhinney, B. (2004). Emergent Language. In K. p. Trott, S. p. Dobbinson, P. p. Griffiths

(Eds.), The Child Language Reader (pp. 259-276). London, England: Routledge.

Morales, M., Mundy, P., Crowson, M. M., Neal, A., & Delgado, C. F. (2005). Individual

differences in infant attention skills, joint attention, and emotion regulation behaviour.

International Journal of Behavioral Development, 29(3), 259-263.

doi:10.1080/01650250444000432

Morgan, David L., & Morgan, Robin K. (2009). Single-Case Research Methods: for the

Behavioral and Health Sciences (pp. 127-134). Thousand Oakes, California: SAGE

Publications Inc.

Mueller, V., & Sepulveda, A. (2014). Parental perception of a Baby Sign workshop on stress and

parent–child interaction. Early Child Development & Care, 184(3), 450-468.

doi:10.1080/03004430.2013.797899

Mueller, V., Sepulveda, A., & Rodriguez, S. (2014). The effects of Baby Sign training on child

development. Early Child Development & Care, 184(8), 1178-1191.

doi:10.1080/03004430.2013.854780

Murphy, A. F. (2014). The Effect of Dual-Language and Transitional-Bilingual Education

Instructional Models on Spanish Proficiency for English Language Learners. Bilingual

Research Journal, 37(2), 182-194. doi:10.1080/15235882.2014.934971

EFFECT OF BABY SIGN ON LANGUAGE FOR “AT-RISK” POPULATIONS 42

Nelson, L. H., White, K. R., & Grewe, J. (2012). Evidence for Website Claims about the

Benefits of Teaching Sign Language to Infants and Toddlers with Normal Hearing. Infant

& Child Development, 21(5), 474-502. doi:10.1002/icd.1748

Normand, M. P., Machado, M. A., Hustyi, K. M., & Morley, A. J. (2011). Infant Sign Training

and Functional Analysis. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 44(2), 305-314.

Owens, R. J. (2012). Language development: an introduction. Boston: Pearson, c2012.

Paradis, J., Emmerzael, K., & Duncan, T. S. (2010). Assessment of English language learners:

Using parent report on first language development. Journal of Communication

Disorders, 43(Clinics in Communication Disorders: Assessment and intervention of

bilingual children with SLI), 474-497. doi:10.1016/j.jcomdis.2010.01.002

Pass, S. (2007). When Constructivists Jean Piaget And Lev Vygotsky Were Pedagogical

Collaborators: A Viewpoint From A Study Of Their Communications. Journal of

Constructivist Psychology, 20(3), 277-282. doi:10.1080/10720530701347944

Piaget, J. (2002). The language and thought of the child. London; New York: Routledge, 2002,

c1959.

Leslie Portney, D. F., & Mary Watkins, D. M. (2009). Foundations Of Clinical Research.

Pearson Health Science.

Qi, C. H., & Marley, S. C. (2011). Validity Study of the Preschool Language Scale-4 With

English-Speaking Hispanic and European American Children in Head Start Programs.

Topics in Early Childhood Special Education, 31(2), 89-98. doi:

10.1177/0271121410391108

EFFECT OF BABY SIGN ON LANGUAGE FOR “AT-RISK” POPULATIONS 43

Ratner, N. (2013). Why talk with children matters: Clinical implications of infant- and child-

directed speech research. Seminars in Speech and Language, 34(4), 203-214.

doi:10.1055/s-0033-1353449

Reilly, S., Bavin, E., Bretherton, L., Conway, L., Eadie, P., Cini, E., & ... Wake, M. (2009). The

Early Language in Victoria Study (ELVS): a prospective, longitudinal study of

communication skills and expressive vocabulary development at 8, 12 and 24 months.

International Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 11(5), 344-357.

Paul, R. & Norbury, C. (2012). LANGUAGE DISORDERS FROM INFANCY THROUGH

ADOLESCENCE. Elsevier Health Sciences.

Ruff, Holly A.; & Rothbart, Mary K. (1996). Attention in Early development: Themes and

variations. New York: Oxford University Press.

Suanda, S. H., & Namy, L. L. (2013). The Organization of Words and Symbolic Gestures in 18-

Month-Olds' Lexicons: Evidence from a Disambiguation Task. Infancy, 18(2), 276-288.

Tayloe Jr., D. T. (2009). What you need to know about your baby's development

milestones. Baby Talk, 74(3), 54. (MOTOR TASKS REF)

Tomasello, M., Carpenter, M., & Liszkowski, U. (2007). A new look at infant pointing. Child

Development, 78, 705-722.

Tomasello, M., & Farrar, J. (1986). Joint attention and early language. Child Development, 57,

1454-1463.

Uylings, H. M. (2006). Development of the Human Cortex and the Concept of "Critical" or

"Sensitive" Periods.Language Learning, 56(1), 59-90.

EFFECT OF BABY SIGN ON LANGUAGE FOR “AT-RISK” POPULATIONS 44

Vallotton, C. D. (2012). Infant signs as intervention? Promoting symbolic gestures for preverbal

children in low-income families supports responsive parent–child relationships. Early

Childhood Research Quarterly, 27(3), 401-415. doi:10.1016/j.ecresq.2012.01.003

Veneziano, E. (2013). A cognitive-pragmatic model for the change from single-word to

multiword speech: A constructivist approach. Journal of Pragmatics, 56133-150.

doi:10.1016/j.pragma.2013.03.013

Volden, J., Smith, I. M., Szatmari, P., Bryson, S., Fombonne, E., Mirenda, P., & ... Thompson,

A. (2011). Using the Preschool Language Scale, Fourth Edition to Characterize Language

in Preschoolers With Autism Spectrum Disorders. American Journal of Speech-Language

Pathology, 20(3), 200-208. doi:10.1044/1058-0360(2011/10-0035)

Wagner, S. M., Nusbaum, H., & Goldin-Meadow, S. (2004). Probing the Mental Representation

of Gesture: Is Handwaving Spatial?. Journal of Memory and Language, 50(4), 395-407.

doi:10.1016/j.jml.2004.01.002

Webb, S. J., Jones, E. H., Kelly, J., & Dawson, G. (2014). The motivation for very early

intervention for infants at high risk for autism spectrum disorders. International Journal

of Speech-Language Pathology, 16(1), 36-42. doi:10.3109/17549507.2013.861018

Werker, J. F., & Hensch, T. K. (2015). Critical Periods in Speech Perception: New

Directions. Annual Review of Psychology, 66(6), 173-196. doi:10.1146/annurev-psych-

010814-015104

Woolfenden, S., Posada, N., Krchnakova, R., Crawford, J., Gilbert, J., Jursik, B., & ... Kemp, L.

(2014). Equitable access to developmental surveillance and early intervention -

understanding the barriers for children from culturally and linguistically diverse (CALD)

EFFECT OF BABY SIGN ON LANGUAGE FOR “AT-RISK” POPULATIONS 45

backgrounds. Health Expectations: An International Journal of Public Participation in

Health Care And Health Policy, doi:10.1111/hex.12318

Zimmerman, I.L., Steiner, V.G., & Pond, R.E. (2002). Preschool Language Scale (4th-edition).

US: Pearson Corp Inc.

EFFECT OF BABY SIGN ON LANGUAGE FOR “AT-RISK” POPULATIONS 46

Appendixes

EFFECT OF BABY SIGN ON LANGUAGE FOR “AT-RISK” POPULATIONS 47

Appendix A

EFFECT OF BABY SIGN ON LANGUAGE FOR “AT-RISK” POPULATIONS 48

Appendix B

Early Development Research Screening Form

Investigators: Sarah Anderson, M.S. Candidate, St Cloud State University

Julia McClung, B.S. Candidate, St Cloud State University

Sarah Smits-Bandstra, Assistant Professor, St Cloud State

University

Participant Screening Form to be filled out with Primary Caregiver

Date: __________________ Name of Child: _________________ Child’s Date of Birth: ____________ Name of Caregiver: __________________________ Gender: Male Female Email: _______________________

(optional) Telephone: Home: ______________ Address: ______________________________________________________ Current Profession: _____________ Highest Education Level:_________

Medical/Developmental History

1. Did your child have difficulty learning to hold their head up, sit up, make sounds,

reach and grab, crawl, or eat? Yes No If yes, please describe________________________________________

2. Do you have any concerns about your child’s hearing or vision?

If yes, please describe________________________________________

3. Is your child seeing a doctor regularly or taking any medication(s) for a disease or

disorder? Yes No If yes, please list them________________________________________

EFFECT OF BABY SIGN ON LANGUAGE FOR “AT-RISK” POPULATIONS 49

4. Has your child ever had seizures, epilepsy or fits? Yes No

5. Has your child even been in the hospital for a serious injury or illness (car accident,

head injury, meningitis, high fever)? Yes No If yes, please describe __________________________________________

6. To the best of your knowledge, did you or the child’s father ever have difficulty

learning to speak or learn in school as a child? Yes No If yes, please describe__________________________________________

7. . Is English your native language? Yes No

If not, what is/are your native language/s ___________________________

8. What languages are spoken to the child? 9. What language do you plan to use for the training?

10. Do you know any type of sign language? Yes No If yes, please describe__________________________________________

11. Do you use sign language with your child? Yes ٱ No ٱ

If yes, please describe__________________________________________

12. Do you or another person work with the child to improve their hand strength and

skill? Yes No If yes, please describe__________________________________________

EFFECT OF BABY SIGN ON LANGUAGE FOR “AT-RISK” POPULATIONS 50

Appendix C

Baby Sign Log

Days

Models

While

Eating/Feedi

ng

Response From

Child

Models

During Play

Response

From Child

WE

EK

1

Monday

Tuesday

Wednesda

y

Thursday

Friday

Saturday

Sunday

WE

EK

2

Monday

Tuesday

Wednesda

y

Thursday

Friday

Saturday

Sunday

WE

EK

3

Monday

Tuesday

Wednesda

y

Thursday

Friday

Saturday

Sunday

WE

EK

4

Monday

Tuesday

Wednesda

y

Thursday

Friday

EFFECT OF BABY SIGN ON LANGUAGE FOR “AT-RISK” POPULATIONS 51

Saturday

Sunday

EFFECT OF BABY SIGN ON LANGUAGE FOR “AT-RISK” POPULATIONS 52

Days

Models

While

Eating/Feedi

ng

Response From

Child

Models

During Play

Response

From Child W

EE

K 5

Monday

Tuesday

Wednesda

y

Thursday

Friday

Saturday

Sunday

WE

EK

6

Monday

Tuesday

Wednesda

y

Thursday

Friday

Saturday

Sunday

WE

EK

7

Monday

Tuesday

Wednesda

y

Thursday

Friday

Saturday

Sunday

WE

EK

8

Monday

Tuesday

Wednesda

y

Thursday

Friday

Saturday

Sunday

EFFECT OF BABY SIGN ON LANGUAGE FOR “AT-RISK” POPULATIONS 53

EFFECT OF BABY SIGN ON LANGUAGE FOR “AT-RISK” POPULATIONS 54

Days

Models

While

Eating/Feedi

ng

Response From

Child

Models

During Play

Response

From Child W

EE

K 9

Monday

Tuesday

Wednesda

y

Thursday

Friday

Saturday

Sunday

WE

EK

10

Monday

Tuesday

Wednesda

y

Thursday

Friday

Saturday

Sunday

WE

EK

11

Monday

Tuesday

Wednesda

y

Thursday

Friday

Saturday

Sunday

WE

EK

12

Monday

Tuesday

Wednesda

y

Thursday

Friday

Saturday

Sunday

EFFECT OF BABY SIGN ON LANGUAGE FOR “AT-RISK” POPULATIONS 55

EFFECT OF BABY SIGN ON LANGUAGE FOR “AT-RISK” POPULATIONS 56

Days

Models

While

Eating/Feedi

ng

Response From

Child

Models

During Play

Response

From Child W

EE

K 1

3

Monday

Tuesday

Wednesda

y

Thursday

Friday

Saturday

Sunday

WE

EK

14

Monday

Tuesday

Wednesda

y

Thursday

Friday

Saturday

Sunday

WE

EK

15

Monday

Tuesday

Wednesda

y

Thursday

Friday

Saturday

Sunday

WE

EK

16

Monday

Tuesday

Wednesda

y

Thursday

Friday

Saturday

Sunday

EFFECT OF BABY SIGN ON LANGUAGE FOR “AT-RISK” POPULATIONS 57

EFFECT OF BABY SIGN ON LANGUAGE FOR “AT-RISK” POPULATIONS 58

Appendix D

Phone Interview Template for Baby Sign Group

1. How have things been going (open-ended)?

2. How many times per day would you say you model each sign?

a. < 3 times per day (how often per week)

b. 3 – 5 times per day

c. 6 – 9 times per day

d. 10 times or more per day

3. When do you model each sign?

a. During feeding

b. During play

c. During both feeding and play

d. Other (explain)

4. Has your child made any attempts to communicate with Baby Sign?

a. If yes, continue with questions

b. If no, skip to question 11

5. Which sign/s has your child used?

6. Did they use the sign after it was modeled or spontaneously?

7. Were they using the sign appropriately to the context (i.e. the sign MORE when

asking for more of something)?

8. Was/were the sign/s used with vocalization or without vocalization?

9. Where were you when your child used the sign/s (i.e. at home)?

10. How did you respond to their use of Baby Sign?

11. How have you been feeling about the Baby Sign program?

12. How do you feel about the number of signs you are training with?

a. Too many (explain)

b. Too few (explain)

c. Just right

13. How do you feel about the functionality of the signs (i.e. do they fit with the needs

of your child)?

14. How do you feel about using Baby Sign with your baby?

15. Would you recommend using Baby Sign to other parents (explain)?