10
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology Copyright 2000 by the American Psychological Association, Inc. 2000, Vol. 78, No. 3, 499-508 0022-3514/00/55.00 DOI: 10.1037//0022-3514.78.3.499 Talking Facilitates Cognitive-Emotional Processes of Adaptation to an Acute Stressor Stephen J. Lepore Brooklyn College of the City University of New York Jennifer D. Ragan University of Texas at Austin Scott Jones Carnegie Mellon University The authors examined the influence of talking and the social context of talking on cognitive-emotional processes of adjustment to stressors. Two hundred fifty-six undergraduates viewed a stressful stimulus and were then assigned to a no-talk control condition or 1 of 3 talk conditions: talk alone, talk to a validating confederate, or talk to an invalidating confederate. Two days later, they were reexposed to the stressor. Compared with individuals in the no-talk condition, those in the talk alone and validate conditions had a lower level of intrusive thoughts in the 2-day interim, and they had lower perceived stress when reexposed to the stressor. The effects of talking and validation on perceived stress appeared to be mediated by lowered intrusions. The benefits of talking were diluted when disclosures were invalidated. These findings suggest that talking about acute stressors can facilitate adjustment to stressors through cognitive resolution. A timely utterance gave that thought relief, and I again am strong ---William Wordsworth, "Ode: Intimations of Immortality from Recollections of Early Childhood" Poets, laypersons, and psychologists alike believe that there are benefits of talking about negative emotional experiences. There is a strong tendency for people to talk about aversive events (Rime, 1995), and disclosure of stressful experiences is the cornerstone of many psychotherapeutic interventions (Stiles, 1995). Empirical evidence suggests that "opening up" and expressing stress-related thoughts and feelings is associated with improved physical and mental health (Pennebaker, 1989; Pennebaker, 1993; Smyth, 1998). For instance, studies have indicated that talking and writing about stressors have been related to decreased distress (Donnelly & Murray, 1991; Lepore, 1997), fewer reports of illness and physician visits (Pennebaker & Beall, 1986; Pennebaker, Colder, & Sharp, 1990), better role and physical functioning (Kelley, Lumley, & Leisen, 1997; Pennebaker et al., 1990; Smyth, Stone, Stephen J. Lepore, Department of Psychology, Brooklyn College of the City University of New York; Jennifer D. Ragan, Department of Psychol- ogy, University of Texas at Austin; and Scott Jones, Department of Psychology, Carnegie Mellon University. We are extremely grateful for the competent research assistance of the following individuals: Tracy Bach, Lisa Don, Yuka Fujimori, Jodi Kurtz, Kelly McPhee, and Lina Shah. We also thank Joshua Smyth and David Eton for their insightful comments on an earlier version of this article. This work was partially supported by National Institutes of Health Grants MH54455 and CA68354. Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Stephen J. Lepore, Department of Psychology, Brooklyn College of the City Uni- versity of New York, 2900 Bedford Avenue, Brooklyn, New York 11210. Electronic mail may be sent to [email protected]. 499 Hurewitz, & Kaell, 1999; Spera, Buhrfeind, & Permebaker, 1994), enhanced immune system functioning (Esterling, Antoni, Fletcher, Margulies, & Schneiderman, 1994; Pennebaker & Kiecolt-Glaser, 1988; Petrie, Booth, Pennebaker, Davison, & Thomas, 1995), decreased autonomic arousal (Pennebaker, Hughes, & O'Heeron, 1987), and increased positive affect (Mendolia & Kleck, 1993). Despite the growing evidence linking expression to adjustment, we know little about the mechanisms that underlie this phenome- non. Recently, investigators have attempted to connect expression- based benefits to cognitive processes implicated in adjustment (Clark, 1993; Gold & Wegner, 1995; Greenberg, Wortman, & Stone, 1996; Lepore, 1997; Major & Gramzow, 1999; Pennebaker, 1993; Pennebaker, 1995; Tait & Silver, 1989). In several articles, Lepore and colleagues have argued that examining the effects of expression--or constraints on expression--on intrusive thought processes may elucidate how expression influences adjustment (Kliewer, Lepore, Oskin, & Johnson, 1998; Lepore, 1997; Lepore, in press; Lepore & Helgeson, 1998; Lepore, Silver, Wortman, & Wayment, 1996). Intrusive thoughts can prolong negative emo- tional and physiological responses to stressors. More importantly, they appear to be a marker of the extent to which individuals have cognitively integrated or resolved stressful experiences. In the present study, we test whether talking about stress-related thoughts and feelings facilitates adjustment through cognitive resolution processes (i.e., reducing the frequency of intrusive thoughts) and by desensitizing individuals to intrusive thoughts (i.e., reducing the impact of intrusive thoughts). In addition to exploring how talking facilitates adjustment to stressors, we examine whether social responses to talking alter adjustment processes. Disclosure of stressful life events to others appears to be a normative and important coping response (Rime, 1995; Silver & Wortman, 1980; Tait & Silver, 1989). However,

Talking Facilitates Cognitive-Emotional Processes of ... · Extremely negative emotional reactions to intrusive thoughts and other reminders of a stressor can impede cognitive integration

  • Upload
    others

  • View
    4

  • Download
    0

Embed Size (px)

Citation preview

Page 1: Talking Facilitates Cognitive-Emotional Processes of ... · Extremely negative emotional reactions to intrusive thoughts and other reminders of a stressor can impede cognitive integration

Journal of Personality and Social Psychology Copyright 2000 by the American Psychological Association, Inc. 2000, Vol. 78, No. 3, 499-508 0022-3514/00/55.00 DOI: 10.1037//0022-3514.78.3.499

Talking Facilitates Cognitive-Emotional Processes of Adaptation to an Acute Stressor

Stephen J. Lepore Brooklyn College of the City University of New York

Jennifer D. Ragan University of Texas at Austin

S co t t J o n e s Carnegie Mellon University

The authors examined the influence of talking and the social context of talking on cognitive-emotional processes of adjustment to stressors. Two hundred fifty-six undergraduates viewed a stressful stimulus and were then assigned to a no-talk control condition or 1 of 3 talk conditions: talk alone, talk to a validating confederate, or talk to an invalidating confederate. Two days later, they were reexposed to the stressor. Compared with individuals in the no-talk condition, those in the talk alone and validate conditions had a lower level of intrusive thoughts in the 2-day interim, and they had lower perceived stress when reexposed to the stressor. The effects of talking and validation on perceived stress appeared to be mediated by lowered intrusions. The benefits of talking were diluted when disclosures were invalidated. These findings suggest that talking about acute stressors can facilitate adjustment to stressors through cognitive resolution.

A timely utterance gave that thought relief, and I again am strong ---William Wordsworth, "Ode: Intimations of Immortality from

Recollections of Early Childhood"

Poets, laypersons, and psychologists alike believe that there are benefits of talking about negative emotional experiences. There is a strong tendency for people to talk about aversive events (Rime, 1995), and disclosure of stressful experiences is the cornerstone of many psychotherapeutic interventions (Stiles, 1995). Empirical evidence suggests that "opening up" and expressing stress-related thoughts and feelings is associated with improved physical and mental health (Pennebaker, 1989; Pennebaker, 1993; Smyth, 1998). For instance, studies have indicated that talking and writing about stressors have been related to decreased distress (Donnelly & Murray, 1991; Lepore, 1997), fewer reports of illness and physician visits (Pennebaker & Beall, 1986; Pennebaker, Colder, & Sharp, 1990), better role and physical functioning (Kelley, Lumley, & Leisen, 1997; Pennebaker et al., 1990; Smyth, Stone,

Stephen J. Lepore, Department of Psychology, Brooklyn College of the City University of New York; Jennifer D. Ragan, Department of Psychol- ogy, University of Texas at Austin; and Scott Jones, Department of Psychology, Carnegie Mellon University.

We are extremely grateful for the competent research assistance of the following individuals: Tracy Bach, Lisa Don, Yuka Fujimori, Jodi Kurtz, Kelly McPhee, and Lina Shah. We also thank Joshua Smyth and David Eton for their insightful comments on an earlier version of this article. This work was partially supported by National Institutes of Health Grants MH54455 and CA68354.

Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Stephen J. Lepore, Department of Psychology, Brooklyn College of the City Uni- versity of New York, 2900 Bedford Avenue, Brooklyn, New York 11210. Electronic mail may be sent to [email protected].

499

Hurewitz, & Kaell, 1999; Spera, Buhrfeind, & Permebaker, 1994), enhanced immune system functioning (Esterling, Antoni, Fletcher, Margulies, & Schneiderman, 1994; Pennebaker & Kiecolt-Glaser, 1988; Petrie, Booth, Pennebaker, Davison, & Thomas, 1995), decreased autonomic arousal (Pennebaker, Hughes, & O'Heeron, 1987), and increased positive affect (Mendolia & Kleck, 1993).

Despite the growing evidence linking expression to adjustment, we know little about the mechanisms that underlie this phenome- non. Recently, investigators have attempted to connect expression- based benefits to cognitive processes implicated in adjustment (Clark, 1993; Gold & Wegner, 1995; Greenberg, Wortman, & Stone, 1996; Lepore, 1997; Major & Gramzow, 1999; Pennebaker, 1993; Pennebaker, 1995; Tait & Silver, 1989). In several articles, Lepore and colleagues have argued that examining the effects of expression--or constraints on expression--on intrusive thought processes may elucidate how expression influences adjustment (Kliewer, Lepore, Oskin, & Johnson, 1998; Lepore, 1997; Lepore, in press; Lepore & Helgeson, 1998; Lepore, Silver, Wortman, & Wayment, 1996). Intrusive thoughts can prolong negative emo- tional and physiological responses to stressors. More importantly, they appear to be a marker of the extent to which individuals have cognitively integrated or resolved stressful experiences. In the present study, we test whether talking about stress-related thoughts and feelings facilitates adjustment through cognitive resolution processes (i.e., reducing the frequency of intrusive thoughts) and by desensitizing individuals to intrusive thoughts (i.e., reducing the impact of intrusive thoughts).

In addition to exploring how talking facilitates adjustment to stressors, we examine whether social responses to talking alter adjustment processes. Disclosure of stressful life events to others appears to be a normative and important coping response (Rime, 1995; Silver & Wortman, 1980; Tait & Silver, 1989). However,

Page 2: Talking Facilitates Cognitive-Emotional Processes of ... · Extremely negative emotional reactions to intrusive thoughts and other reminders of a stressor can impede cognitive integration

500 LEPORE, RAGAN, AND JONES

disclosure is not always associated with positive psychological adjustment (see, e.g., Cutrona, 1986). Furthermore, disclosure seldom occurs in a social vacuum, and its effects may be contin- gent on the social context. We believe that adjustment to stressors is likely to be facilitated by disclosure in a supportive social context, but not in an unsupportive context. Indeed, numerous investigators have found that whereas supportive social networks tend to reduce distress in individuals under stress, unsupportive or critical social networks can actually increase distress (e.g., Abbey, Abramis, & Caplan, 1985; Holahan, Moos, Holahan, & Brennan, 1997; Lepore, 1992; Major et al., 1990; Major, Zubek, Cooper, Cozzarelli, & Richards, 1997; Manne, Taylor, Dougherty, & Ke- meny, 1997; Rook, 1984; Vinokur & van Ryn, 1993). In the present study, we test whether supportive and unsupportive re- sponses to disclosures about a stressor have differential effects on adjustment and on cognitive-processing variables implicated in adjustment.

Cognit ive Processing

Intrusive thoughts are repeated, unbidden memories, thoughts, and images of a stressor. They are commonplace among survivors of traumatic events, such as bereavement (McIntosh, Silver, & Wortman, 1993), incest (Silver, Boon, & Stones, 1983), disasters (Baum, Cohen, & Hall, 1993; Dougall, Craig, & Baum, 1999), cancer (Cordova et al., 1995; Lutgendorf, Antoni, Ironson, Schnei- derman, & Fletcher, 1997), and exposure to violence (Kliewer et al., 1998). Intrusive thoughts are a hallmark of clinical disorders, such as posttraumatic stress disorder (American Psychiatric Asso- ciation, 1994), but they also are a normal response to stressors in nonclinical populations and can be induced experimentally (Horowitz, 1986, 1993). In the aftermath of stressful events, fre- quent intrusive thoughts are associated with emotional, behavioral, and physiological disturbances (Baum et al., 1993; Lutgendorf et al., 1997; Silver et al., 1983; Tait & Silver, 1989), which suggests that they play a role in the development or maintenance of adjust- ment problems.

Cognitive-processing theorists hypothesize that stressors can cause emotional disturbance by challenging people's basic and often optimistic beliefs about the self and the world, such as a sense of control, self-worth, and coherence (Epstein, 1985, 1991; Horowitz, 1986; Janoff-Bulman, 1992; Marris, 1975; McCann & Pearlman, 1990; Parkes, 1975). Further, to the extent that people cannot resolve or cognitively integrate aspects of a stressful expe- rience into their mental models, the stressor will be maintained in active memory through intrusive thoughts (Horowitz, 1986). Pre- sumably, intrusive thoughts and associated adjustment problems will subside once individuals successfully integrate stressful infor- mation into their mental models. Cognitive integration may require repeated examination, contemplation, and evaluation of a stressful event and its implications. By engaging in these cognitive pro- cesses, individuals may be able to interpret stressors in personally meaningful terms, integrate threatening or confusing aspects of the experience into a coherent and nonthreatening conceptual frame- work, and reach a state of emotional acceptance.

Confronting stress-related intrusive thoughts and concerns also can facilitate emotional adjustment through desensitization, or

habituation, processes (Bootzin, 1997; Foa & Kozak, 1991; Lepore, 1997; Lepore, in press; Rachman, 1980). Creamer, Bur- gess, and Pattison (1992) have argued that exposure to stressful material, including intrusive thoughts, can be adaptive because it "allows stimulus-response connections to be weakened and prompts modification of the meaning associated with the incident" (p. 454). Thus, actively processing (i.e., contemplating, reevaluat- ing) stress-related thoughts and stimuli may facilitate emotional adjustment by extinguishing negative emotional responses or by creating more benign or neutral emotional associations with mem- ories or reminders of the stressor.

Extremely negative emotional reactions to intrusive thoughts and other reminders of a stressor can impede cognitive integration and habituation. When reexperiencing a stressor evokes strong emotional responses, individuals sometimes adopt avoidant coping styles. That is, they suppress thoughts of the stressor, inhibit themselves from talking about it, and divert their attention away from any reminders of the stressor. People also can cycle between engaging and disengaging in cognitive processing of stressful material to keep from becoming emotionally overwhelmed. In- deed, intrusive thoughts and avoidant thinking and behavior often go hand-in-hand (Horowitz, Wilner, & Alvarez, 1979). When avoidance is excessive, it might prevent deep processing, or op- portunities for cognitive restructuring, stressor reappraisal, and desensitization.

The precise role of avoidance in cognitive processing of stres- sots is not clear. Typically, avoidance is conceptualized as a defense mechanism used to contain intense negative affect asso- ciated with external or internal reminders of a stressor. It appears that after stressful events, symptoms of avoidance are less common than symptoms of intrusions (Graham-Bermann & Levendosky, 1998; Maercker & Schuetzwohl, 1997), but little else is known about the interrelationship between these two variables or their relative impact on adjustment to stressors. Some investigators have shown that both factors are associated with poorer adjustment (e.g., Brewin, Watson, McCarthy, Hyman, & Dayson, 1998; Davis, De-Nour, Shouval, & Melmed, 1998). However, others have shown that level of intrusions is a stronger predictor of distress than is avoidance (e.g., Baider & De-Nour, 1997; Kos- lowsky, Solomon, Bleich, & Laor, 1996), and still others have shown that level of intrusions is associated with distress, whereas avoidance is not (e.g., McFarlane, 1992). There is also some controversy over the temporal ordering of avoidance and intrusive thoughts. For some theorists, intrusions represent a breakdown in defense responses of avoidance, so intrusions are presumed to occur after avoidance (Horowitz, 1986; also see Gold & Wegner, 1995). For others, avoidance represents a mechanism of coping with aversive, intrusive thoughts about stressors, so intrusions are presumed to occur before avoidance (Creamer et al., 1992). It is possible that the truth lies in between and that intrusions and avoidance are reciprocally related.

In the present study, we do not attempt to disentangle the causal ordering of intrusions and avoidance, nor do we have firm predic- tions about the role of avoidance in adjustment. Instead, we aim to explore the impact of talking on both intrusions and avoidance and examine the relative contribution of these two cognitive processing variables on adjustment to a stressor.

Page 3: Talking Facilitates Cognitive-Emotional Processes of ... · Extremely negative emotional reactions to intrusive thoughts and other reminders of a stressor can impede cognitive integration

TALKING AND COGNITIVE--EMOTIONAL PROCESSES 501

Cogn i t i ve -Emot iona l Benefi ts of Express ion

There are at least two ways that expressive tasks might influence adjustment processes via intrusive thoughts (Lepore, 1997). These mechanisms are tied to the integration, or completion, hypothesis and the desensitization, or habituation, hypothesis described above.

From the completion perspective, expressive tasks help people to resolve stressful experiences. Expressive acts, such as talking or writing about stressors, help people to impose a cognitive structure on stressful experiences (Harber & Pennebaker, 1992; Pennebaker, 1989). By putting one's experience into language, individuals attempt to construct a coherent narrative, which can make a stres- sor understandable to them and to others (Clark, 1993; Meichen- baum & Fitzpatrick, 1993). The narrative becomes part of indi- viduals' cognitive representation of the experience, thereby broadening their perspective. Thus, expressive tasks can poten- tially change the content of trauma-related thoughts and memories. Supportive responses from social network members may further aid cognitive restructuring by suggesting ways of coping or posi- tively reframing a stressful situation. If expression facilitates cog- nitive restructuring, or resolution, then it should diminish the frequency of intrusive thoughts and associated adjustment problems.

From the desensitization perspective, expressive acts enable people to form new, nonthreatening associations with trauma- related stimuli. This is particularly so if the context of expression is nonthreatening or supportive. By changing the meaning, or appraisals, of trauma-related thoughts and memories, expressive tasks can diminish the emotional fallout of intrusive thoughts. This desensitization process would be evident if emotional expression attenuates the association between intrusive thoughts and adjust- ment problems.

There is empirical evidence that expression of stress-related thoughts and feelings influences both the frequency and emotional impact of intrusive thoughts. Lutgendorf and Antoni (1999) found that individuals who disclosed a traumatic experience to an exper- imenter showed decreases in intrusive thoughts over time, whereas individuals in an assessment-only control group had stable levels of intrusive thoughts. In two studies, Lepore and colleagues have observed significant positive correlations between social con- straints on expression of stress-related experiences and the fre- quency of intrusive thoughts (Kliewer et al., 1998; Lepore et al., 1996). These findings are consistent with the theory that being able to faithfully disclose stress-related thoughts and feelings to others can facilitate cognitive integration or resolution, whereas con- straints on disclosure can impede these processes.

Results of several other studies suggest that disclosure can moderate the relation between intrusive thoughts and emotional adjustment to stressors. Specifically, we have found that intrusive thoughts are more strongly associated with psychological distress in people who feel constrained in talking with network members about stressful experiences than in people who feel relatively unconstrained in talking (Kliewer et al., 1998; Lepore & Helgeson, 1998; Lepore et al., 1996). Major and Gramzow (1999) found that women who disclosed their feelings about an abortion to others were less distressed by intrusive thoughts than those who did not disclose. These findings are consistent with the theory that being able to talk freely about stress-related thoughts and feelings can

dampen negative emotional responses to intrusive thoughts, whereas constraints on disclosure can impede these processes.

Hypotheses

The present study extends previous research by testing both the completion and the desensitization hypotheses in an experiment and by examining the effects of talking about stressful experiences in diverse social contexts. We predicted that disclosing thoughts and feelings about a stressor would reduce negative psychological and physiological responses during reexposure to a stressor by facilitating cognitive resolution (i.e., reducing the frequency of intrusive thoughts) and desensitization (i.e., reducing the emo- tional and physiological impact of intrusive thoughts). If resolution processes are occurring, we should find evidence that talking reduces intrusive thoughts, which in turn reduces negative psycho- logical and physiological responses to a stressor. If desensitization processes are occurring, we should find evidence that talking reduces the association between intrusive thoughts and negative psychological and physiological responses to a stressor.

For exploratory purposes, we also examined the potential me- diating role of avoidance. We tested whether talking reduces avoidance of thoughts about a stressor, as well as the relation of avoidance to negative psychological and physiological responses.

Finally, we predicted that disclosing stress-related thoughts and feelings in a supportive social context would result in better adjustment than would disclosing in an unsupportive social con- text. On the basis of findings from previous studies (e.g., Major et al., 1990), we also expected that individuals who receive an unsupportive response to their disclosure might exhibit worse adjustment than their peers who do not disclose at all.

Method

O v e r v i e w

We used a randomized, four-group (no talk, talk alone, validate, inval- idate) repeated measures (Session 1, Session 2) design. In Session 1, participants came to the laboratory individually and watched a relaxing nature video, followed by a stressful slide and video presentation on the Nazi Holocaust. For 2 min after viewing the Holocaust stimuli, participants randomized to a talk alone condition remained alone while they disclosed their thoughts and feelings related to the Holocaust. Participants random- ized to a validate condition disclosed their thoughts and feelings on the Holocaust to a female confederate who acted as if she had a similar reaction. Participants randomized to an invalidate condition disclosed their thoughts and feelings on the Holocaust to a female confederate who acted as if she had a dissimilar reaction. Participants randomized to the no-talk control condition remained alone and did not disclose. In Session 2, 48 hr later, participants returned to the lab and were reexposed to the nature video and Holocaust stimuli. In Sessions 1 and 2, data were collected on physiological responses (blood pressure and pulse rate) and psychological responses (perceived stress and arousal) to the stimuli. In Session 2, participants completed measures related to cognitive processing (intrusive thoughts, avoidance).

Par t i c ipan t s

We recruited men (n = 128) and women (n = 128) through a college research participant pool and by advertisements on electronic bulletin boards. Participants were compensated with $10 and course credit. The average age of the participants was 19.41 years (SD = 1.42); 65% were

Page 4: Talking Facilitates Cognitive-Emotional Processes of ... · Extremely negative emotional reactions to intrusive thoughts and other reminders of a stressor can impede cognitive integration

502 LEPORE, RAGAN, ANDJONES

European American, 22% were Asian American, 4% were African Amer- ican, and 9% were of other ethnic origins.

Procedure

Session 1. On arrival at the laboratory, participants were told that the study was designed to examine college students' reactions to war. We then collected data on demographics and knowledge about the Nazi Holocaust. Next, participants were left alone throughout a 5-rain adaptation period, a 6-min relaxing nature video, and a 14-rain stress-inducing Holocaust slide and video presentation. Four blood-pressure and pulse-rate measures were taken during the nature video and 10 were taken during the Holocaust presentation. Participants then completed a subjective stress and arousal checklist.

Next, participants in the talk alone, validate, and invalidate conditions talked for 2 rain about their thoughts and feelings on the Holocaust presentation. Participants in the no-talk control condition were not given an opportunity to talk. Participants in the talk alone condition did not have an audience, other than the implied audience of the experimenter in the control room. Before participants in the validate and invalidate conditions dis- closed, the experimenter introduced them to a female confederate. She was described as another study participant who had seen the film 2 days earlier and was summoned back to share her reactions to war.

The validating confederate nodded, maintained mutual eye contact, and smiled approvingly while the participant disclosed. When it was her turn to disclose, she identified and agreed with several thoughts and feelings that were expressed by the participant (e.g., "In terms of what I felt, it was mostly what you said, like feeling upset and disturbed by the images"). She also shared some scripted responses (e.g., "I was surprised by how graphic some of the slides were"). The invalidating confederate maintained a neutral countenance and avoided eye contact while the participant dis- closed. When it was her turn to disclose, she identified and disagreed with several thoughts and feelings expressed by the participant (e.g., "In terms of what I felt, it wasn' t like what you said. I didn't really feel upset and disturbed by the images"), and shared some scripted responses (e.g., "I expected that this is what I would see, so I wasn' t surprised by how graphic some of the slides were"). Multiple confederates were trained and used in both the validate and invalidate roles.

Before the experimenter dismissed participants, they were asked to confn'm that they would not talk to anyone about the study before returning for Session 2, 48 hr later.

Session 2. Participants first completed a questionnaire that measured the frequency with which they had intrusive thoughts about the Holocaust and avoided thinking about the Holocaust in the 2-day interim. Next, the experimenter repeated the procedures from Session 1 to assess psycholog- ical and physiological responses to the Holocaust stimuli. Participants were also asked whether they had talked to anyone about the experiment. At the end of Session 2, participants in the validate and invalidate conditions completed questionnaires designed to test the confederate manipulation. The experimenter debriefed participants and again attempted to ascertain whether they had talked about the experiment.

Measures

Manipulation checks. We developed two questionnaires to assess whether the validation and invalidation manipulation worked as intended. One consisted of 4 items that assessed the degree to which the participant felt that the confederate's thoughts and feelings about the Holocaust were similar or dissimilar. These items were rated on a 5-point scale ranging from 1 (We completely disagreed) to 5 (We completely agreed) and had good reliability (Cronbach's ct = .94). The other scale consisted of 21 items that the participant used to rate, on a 6-point scale ranging from 0 (strongly disagree) to 5 (strongly agree), the confederate's interpersonal qualities and knowledge of the Holocaust. A principal-components analysis

revealed four unique factors, which we labeled Friendliness (e.g., friendly, warm), Empathy (e.g., accepting, compassionate), Appeal (e.g., interesting, boring), and Knowledge (e.g., informed, uninformed). All factors had acceptable reliability (Cronbach's alphas ranged from .76 to .95).

Physiological stress responses. Cardiovascular responses (blood pres- sure and pulse rate) were measured from the participant's nondominant ann using a DinaMap XL (Model 9300; Johnson & Johnson Medical Inc., Tampa, FL) vital signs monitor. The monitor was operated from a control room adjacent to the laboratory. Participants' readings taken during the nature video were used to calculate an average resting response, and the readings taken during the Holocaust presentation were used to calculate an average stress response.

Perceived stress and arousal. We used the Stress/Arousal Adjective Checklist (SACL; King, Burrows, & Stanley, 1983) to examine the psy- chological impact of the Holocaust presentation. The SACL comprises two 10-item subscales: Perceived Stress (e.g., uneasy, distressed; a = .89) and Perceived Arousal (e.g., alert, aroused; a = .73). Items were rated on a 4-point scale ranging from 0 (definitely no) to 3 (definitely yes). King and colleagues have confirmed the factorial independence and validity of the two subscales.

Cognitive processing. We used a 9-item scale to assess the frequency of intrusive thoughts and avoidance of thoughts and feelings about the Holocaust in the intersession period. All items were rated on a 6-point scale ranging from 0 (not at all) to 5 (very often). We used five high-loading items from the Intrusive Thought subscale of the Impact of Events Scale (IES; Horowitz et al., 1979) to assess intrusive thoughts (e.g., "had thoughts about the Holocaust or the Holocaust images when you didn't mean to"). We used four high-loading items from the Avoidance subscale of the IES to assess level of avoidance of thoughts and feelings concerning the Holocaust (e.g., "tried not to think about the Holocaust"). The intru- sions and avoidance scales had adequate reliability (as = .83 and .82, respectively). Although these variables were highly intercorrelated (r = .76, p < .001), we chose to analyze them separately for theoretical reasons.

R e s u l t s

Manipula t ion Checks and Gender Di f ferences

All confederate rat ing scales were ana lyzed us ing t tests (vali-

date vs. invalidate). Rela t ive to the inval idat ing confederates , the

val idat ing confedera tes were rated as more similar , fr iendly, em-

pathic, appeal ing, and knowledgeab le (all p s < .001; see Table 1).

These f indings indicate that the manipu la t ion was successful .

However , the group di f ferences in ra t ings o f confederate appeal

and knowledge sugges t that par t ic ipants m igh t have d iscounted the

Table 1

Mean Ratings of Similarity, Friendliness, Empathy, Appeal, and Knowledge of Validating (n = 64) Versus Invalidating (n = 64) Confederates

Validating Invalidating

Variable M SD M SD t(126)

Similarity a 4.49 0.50 2.25 0.74 20.24*** Friendliness b 4.48 0.50 3.01 0.68 13.91"** Empathy b 4.12 0.59 2.32 0.66 16.39"** Appeal b 3.98 0.83 3.35 0.71 4.64*** Knowledge b 3.56 0.74 2.94 0.87 4.21"**

a Scale ranged from 1 (completely disagree) to 5 (completely agree). b Scale ranged from 0 (strongly disagree) to 5 (strongly agree). *** p < .001.

Page 5: Talking Facilitates Cognitive-Emotional Processes of ... · Extremely negative emotional reactions to intrusive thoughts and other reminders of a stressor can impede cognitive integration

TALKING AND COGNITIVE,--EMOTIONAL PROCESSES 503

credibility of the invalidating confederate. Further, 10 of the par- ticipants who were invalidated and only 1 of the participants who was validated mentioned being suspicious about the confederate after they were debriefed. This difference was significant, X2(1, N = 256) = 11.40, p < .001; however, removing suspicious participants did not alter the substantive results reported below.

There were significant differences between men and women on many variables. The t-test analyses (male vs. female) were per- formed on the means of all self-report and cardiovascular vari- ables. Relative to men, women reported more stress and arousal after the Holocaust presentation (ps < .05), as well as more frequent intrusions and avoidance (ps < .001). There were no gender differences in pulse rate or diastolic blood pressure, but men had higher systolic blood pressure than did women at rest and during the tasks (all ps < .01). There were no significant gender differences in the ratings of the confederate in the validate and invalidate conditions, nor were there any Gender × Condition interactive effects on any outcomes. Thus, gender was not factored into subsequent analyses.

Forty-five percent of the participants reported that they talked about the study between Sessions 1 and 2. The majority (92%) indicated that they had only mentioned the study in passing (e.g., told a roommate they were going to an experiment). Talking outside of the laboratory did not confound the experiment because it was not associated with condition and did not moderate the relation between condition and the outcomes.

Cognitive Processing

We used a one-way analysis of variance (ANOVA) to test whether the talk manipulation had direct effects on the cognitive processing variables on day two. As shown in Table 2, there was an overall effect of condition on intrusive thoughts and avoidance. A Newman-Keuls post hoc test (a = .05) was used to identify all significant differences between means. Participants in the no-talk condition had a significantly higher level of intrusive thoughts than did participants in both the talk alone and validate conditions. Participants in the no-talk condition had a significantly higher level of avoidance than did participants in the talk alone condition. No other groups differed significantly from one another in level of intrusions or avoidance.

Psychological and Physiological Responses to Reexposure to the Holocaust Stimuli

Repeated measures ANOVAs were used to test whether the talking manipulation influenced psychological and physiological

outcomes. Measurement day, or period, was the within- participants variable and talking condition was the between- participants variable. In these analyses, we expected no differences between groups on Day 1 because talking was manipulated after these variables were measured. However, we did expect an effect of the talking manipulation on Day 2. Thus, we were predicting Condition × Period interactions.

On perceived stress, there was a significant main effect of period, F(I , 252) = 63.66,p < .001, and a significant Condition × Period interaction, F(3, 252) = 2.69, p < .05. As shown in Table 3, there was a significant effect of condition on perceived stress on Day 2, F(3, 252) = 2.88, p < .05, but not on Day 1. Newman-Keuls tests were used to compare all pairs of means on Day 2. Participants in the no-talk condition had a significantly higher level of perceived stress than did participants in both the talk alone and validate conditions. No other groups differed sig- nificantly from one another in level of perceived stress. On per- ceived arousal, there was a significant effect of period, F(1, 252) = 22.92, p < .001, but no main effect of condition or Condition × Period interactive effect. Participants reported less perceived arousal on Day 2 than they did on Day 1.

There were no significant main or interactive effects of period and condition on blood pressure or pulse rate. Thus, participants' physiological response to the stimulus did not change as a function of the reexposure or talking condition.

Mediation Analyses

We used a multiple regression approach to test the possibility that intrusive thoughts mediated the effects of condition on per- ceived stress. Recall that participants in the no-talk group had a higher level of perceived stress and a higher level of intrusive thoughts than did participants in the talk alone and validate groups, whereas those in the invalidate group fell somewhere in between. Thus, we were interested in whether intrusive thoughts accounted for the differences in perceived stress over time in the talk condi- tions relative to the no-talk condition.

Following the recommendations of Baron and Kenny (1986; also Kenny, Kashy, & Bolger, 1998), we used regression analyses to establish if the following conditions for mediation were met: (a) The predictor (condition) is associated with the outcome (change in perceived stress from Day 1 to Day 2); (b) the predictor is associated with the mediator (intrusions); (c) the mediator is as- sociated with the outcome, controlling for the predictor; and (d) the predictor has zero association with the outcome after controlling for the mediator. If all four steps were met, then the data would be

Table 2 Mean Level of Intrusions and Avoidance as a Function of Talking Condition (n = 256)

No talk Talk Validate Invalidate

Variable M SD M SD M SD M SD F(3, 252)

Intrusive thoughts 1.80 a 1.06 1.23 b .89 1.38 b .95 1.47a.b .98 3.89** Avoidance 1.83 a 1.22 1.23 b 1.05 1.44a. b 1.13 1.56a. b 1.11 3.21"

Note. For each dependent variable, pairs of means sharing a lettered subscript are not significantly (p < .05) different from one another. Scales ranged from 0 (not at all) to 5 (very often). * p < . 0 5 . * * p < . 0 1 .

Page 6: Talking Facilitates Cognitive-Emotional Processes of ... · Extremely negative emotional reactions to intrusive thoughts and other reminders of a stressor can impede cognitive integration

504 LEPORE, RAGAN, AND JONES

Table 3 Mean Level of Perceived Arousal and Perceived Stress After Exposure to Holocaust Stimuli as a Function of Talking Condition and Exposure Period (n = 256)

No talk Talk Validate Invalidate

Variable M SD M SD M SD M SD F(3, 252)

Arousal a Day 1 1.08 0.46 1.18 0.48 1.13 0.47 1.12 0.48 0.49 Arousal Day 2 0.95 0.51 0.98 0.51 0.95 0.49 1.01 0.48 0.12 Stress a Day 1 2.08 0.59 1.98 0.67 1.96 0.65 2.03 0.53 0.45 Stress Day 2 1.98 0.72 1.67 0.74 1.64 0.77 1.78 0.61 2.88*

a Scale ranged from 0 (definitely no) to 3 (definitely yes). * p < .05.

consistent with the hypothesis that intrusions completely mediate the relation between condition and changes in perceived stress. If just the first three steps were met, then the data would be consistent with the hypothesis that intrusions partially mediate the effects of condition on changes in perceived stress. We used the modified Sobel (1982) test discussed in Kenny et al. (1998) to evaluate the null hypothesis that the indirect effect of condition on perceived stress via intrusions equals zero.

In the regression analyses, the dependent measure was change in perceived stress (Day 2 - Day 1). Lower scores indicate lower perceived stress on Day 2 than on Day 1. Condition was dummy coded to examine the effects of interest. We created two dummy variables: The first contrasted the effects of talk alone (coded 1) against no talk (0); the second contrasted validate (1) against no talk (0). For exploratory purposes, we also created a dummy code to contrast invalidate (1) and no talk (0). However, preliminary analyses of this variable revealed no effects of invalidation on intrusions or change in perceived stress. Therefore, we did not run further mediation analyses of the invalidate condition.

All criteria for mediation were satisfied in the regression anal- yses. First, the talk alone condition was associated with declines in perceived stress (B = - . 2 1 , p < .05), and the validate condition was associated with declines in perceived stress (B = - . 22 , p < .05). Second, the talk alone condition was associated with a lower level of intrusions (B = - . 5 7 , p < .001), and the validate condi- tion was associated with a lower level of intrusions (B = - . 42 , p < .05). Third, independent of the effect of talk alone, level of intrusions was associated with higher perceived stress (B = .10, p < .05). Similarly, independent of the effect of validate, level of intrusions was associated with higher perceived stress (B = .12, p < .01). Finally, after controlling for level of intrusions, the talk alone condition was no longer associated with changes in per- ceived stress (B = - . 1 5 , p > .05), and the validate condition was no longer associated with changes in perceived stress (B = - . 17, p > .05). The modified Sobel test revealed that the reduction in perceived stress due to lower intrusions in the talk alone versus the no-talk group was significant (Z = 2.06, p < .05). Similarly, the reduction in perceived stress due to lower intrusions in the vali- dated versus the no-talk group was significant (Z = 1.97, p < .05). These results are consistent with the hypothesis that intrusive thoughts fully mediated the effects of talk alone and validate on declines in perceived stress.

For exploratory purposes, we also evaluated whether avoidance could account for the condition effects on changes in perceived

stress. These analyses were lilnited to testing whether avoidance accounted for the effects of the talk alone condition on changes in perceived stress, because only the talk alone group had a lower level of avoidance than the no-talk group. Preliminary analyses revealed that avoidance was not associated with changes in per- ceived stress after controlling for the condition effect. Therefore, it is not possible that avoidance mediated the effects of condition on perceived stress.

Moderat ion Analyses

We used regression analyses to examine whether the talking condition moderated the association between Holocaust-related intrusive thoughts and emotional and physiological responses to the Holocaust stimulus. These analyses addressed the question of whether talking alone or in particular social contexts reduces the impact of intrusive thoughts on emotional and physiological out- comes. We followed the procedures outlined by Aiken and West (1991) for testing interactions between categorical and continuous variables. The psychological outcomes were changes in perceived stress and perceived arousal from Day 1 to Day 2. The physiolog- ical outcomes were changes in blood pressure and pulse rate response from Day l to Day 2. Results revealed no significant interactive effects of condition and intrusive thoughts on any of the outcomes.

D i s c u s s i o n

While many investigators have shown that expressive tasks, such as writing or talking, facilitate adjustment to stressors, rela- tively few have identified processes that mediate or moderate these effects. The first aim of this study was to identify cognit ive- emotional processes that mediate the effects of talking on adjust- ment to a stressor. Our central hypothesis was that talking would enhance adjustment by facilitating cognitive integration (comple- tion hypothesis) or habituation (desensitization hypothesis). Re- sults were largely consistent with the completion hypothesis. In comparison with participants who did not talk about the stressor, those who talked alone or to a supportive, validating confederate had fewer residual intrusive thoughts about the stressor and re- ported lower subjective stress when reexposed to the stressor. Results from the mediation analyses were consistent with the hypothesis that reductions in intrusive thoughts explained the

Page 7: Talking Facilitates Cognitive-Emotional Processes of ... · Extremely negative emotional reactions to intrusive thoughts and other reminders of a stressor can impede cognitive integration

TALKING AND COGNITIVE-EMOTIONAL PROCESSES 505

reductions in subjective stress in participants who talked alone or with a validating confederate.

The mediating effects of intrusive thoughts are consistent with the theory that disclosing stress-related thoughts can help people to make sense of stressful events or achieve cognitive and emotional resolution (Meichenbaum & Fitzpatrick, 1993; Pennebaker, 1995). According to the completion hypothesis, the decline in intrusions associated with talking is a result of successful cognitive integra- tion of stress-related information. However, intrusive thoughts are just indirect markers of cognitive processing. Changes in the frequency of intrusions reveal little about the quality of individu- als' thoughts. Future research should be directed at examining how talking influences qualitative aspects of stress-related thought pat- terns. For instance, does disclosure in a benign social context stimulate assimilation processes (i.e., reinterpretation of external events) or accommodation processes (i.e., changes in basic beliefs about a stimulus, worldviews, or self-views), as suggested by some theorists? Future investigations might address these questions by in-depth, qualitative, and quantitative analyses of individuals' de- scriptions of traumatic experiences (e.g., appraisals of threat and harm), self-perceptions (e.g., self-esteem, self-efficacy), and be- liefs about the world (e.g., controllability of negative outcomes, trust in others) before and after disclosing in different social contexts.

Contrary to our expectations, there was no evidence of desen- sitization to intrusive thoughts. This is inconsistent with some previous correlational and experimental studies on disclosure and adjustment. For instance, in correlational studies investigators have found that being able to talk about stressors with supportive others reduces the emotional impact of intrusive thoughts (e.g., Lepore & Helgeson, 1998; Lepore et al., 1996; Major & Gramzow, 1999). In an experimental study, we found that individuals who wrote their deepest thoughts and feelings about an impending graduate school entrance examination were less distressed by exam-related intrusive thoughts than individuals in a control group who wrote about trivial topics (Lepore, 1997). It is possible that disclosure in the present study resulted in cognitive resolution rather than desensitization because the stressor was relatively minor and short-lived. Unlike the present study, those studies in which desensitization effects were found involved populations facing highly personal and ongoing stressors (e.g., abortion, be- reavement, cancer). In situations involving emotionally significant stressors, disclosure may have palliative effects, thus reducing the emotional impact of intrusive thoughts, but may not have sufficient power to fully eradicate intrusive thoughts. In contrast, when stressors are not so emotionally significant and are limited by time, disclosure may be powerful enough to eradicate intrusive thoughts.

The null effect of the manipulation on arousal also was contrary to our expectations. We attribute this to a poor choice of arousal measures. In previous studies, investigators found null effects of disclosure on cardiovascular measures, such as heart rate (e.g., Mendolia & Kleck, 1993; Pennebaker et al., 1987), but significant effects on skin conductance levels (Pennebaker et al., 1987). Citing work by Fowles (1980), Pennebaker, Barger, and Tiebout (1989) have argued that skin conductance level is a more sensitive and appropriate indicator of the autonomic arousal processes affected by disclosure than is cardiovascular response. It appears that skin conductance activity is linked to behavioral inhibition, whereas cardiovascular activity is linked to behavioral activation (Obrist,

1981). In the present study, the video stimulus did not require participants to engage in active coping behaviors, so behavioral activation was limited. However, it is possible that inhibition was increased in those participants who were prohibited from express- ing their thoughts and feelings about the stimulus, or who were censured for their thoughts and feelings. In these conditions, we might have observed elevations in autonomic arousal using a measure of skin conductance, even though participants in these conditions did not exhibit elevations on the cardiovascular measures.

A second major aim of this study was to describe how social responses to talking about stressful events influences adjustment processes. We hypothesized that unsupportive, or invalidating, responses to participants' talking about a stressor would under- mine the benefits of talking. The result partially supported this prediction. Participants in the invalidate condition appeared to be neither better nor worse than those in the other three conditions on measures of cognitive processing and psychological stress.

One interpretation of these findings is that invalidation diluted the benefits of talking (i.e., the effects of talking were not as strong in the invalidate condition as in the talk alone and validate condi- tion). This interpretation is consistent with the theory that talking is most beneficial in a supportive context. Another interpretation is that some participants benefited from the invalidation manipula- tion, whereas others did not. Hypothetically, invalidation could reduce perceived stress by changing the way individuals think about a stressor. Most participants who talked about the Holocaust stimuli had negative responses, stating that it was upsetting, hor- rific, and disturbing. In the invalidating condition, confederates provided an alternative and less distressing perspective, which might have been sufficient to shift some participants' perspectives on the situation. Of course, not all participants accepted the posi- tion of the invalidating confederate. As shown in Table 1, the invalidating confederate was rated as less interesting and knowl- edgeable about the Holocaust than was the validating confederate.

It is interesting that participants in the invalidate condition were not worse off than those in the no-talk condition on measures of cognitive processing and perceived stress. Major et al. (1990) found that women who felt less than fully supported when they told a significant other about their abortion were more distressed than women who kept their abortion a secret. It is possible that invalidating responses are most damaging when they originate from significant others, such as family members and friends. In the present study, the confederate was a peer, but one who did not have an emotionally significant bond with the participant. And, as noted above, when the confederate challenged how the participant responded to the Holocaust stimuli, oftentimes the participant simply discounted or discredited the confederate.

A final and more exploratory aim of this study was to examine the role of avoidance in cognitive-emotional processes of adjust- ment. Although avoidance and intrusions were highly correlated, they did not play the same role in adjustment. Unlike intrusions, avoidance was unrelated to changes in perceived stress, and did not mediate the effects of talking on perceived stress. These findings are consistent with those from a study on the effects of HIV on emotional distress in HIV-infected women (Moneyham et al., 1997). In that study, the relation between HIV-related stressors and emotional distress appeared to be mediated by intrusive thoughts, but avoidance was unrelated to distress and did not mediate the

Page 8: Talking Facilitates Cognitive-Emotional Processes of ... · Extremely negative emotional reactions to intrusive thoughts and other reminders of a stressor can impede cognitive integration

506 LEPORE, RAGAN, AND JONES

relation between HIV-related stressors and distress. Of course, other investigators have found negative associations between avoidance and emotional distress. Because of these mixed find- ings, much more research is needed to clarify the role of avoidance in cognitive-emotional processes of adjustment to stressors.

Although many of the present findings were consistent with our expectations, we should note that the ecological validity of the study is somewhat limited. It is not clear to what extent the study informs us about real-world stress and coping processes. As noted above, the stressor in this experiment probably did not have high personal significance for many of the participants. The stimulus is stressful to the extent that it elicits sympathy and concern for another's suffering, but it does not directly threaten participants. Further, the stressor in the present study was very brief. It is possible that emotional expression may influence adjustment to personal and ongoing stressors in a different manner than we observed.

A related problem of ecological validity stems from the brief and scripted interaction between the research participants and the confederates. The disclosure process in this social context is likely to be a pale version of what happens in the real world among friends discussing a stressful experience. On the one hand, it is remarkable that we achieved such robust effects of disclosure and validation on adjustment given the artificiality and brevity of the manipulation. On the other hand, we must ask whether similar results would be observed if the disclosure were longer, repeated, or made to significant others. Validation might be highly beneficial when it is from a significant other, but, as suggested by the present findings, validation from a stranger may not confer benefits be- yond those reaped from simply talking to nobody in particular (i.e., ventilating). Invalidation did not have strong effects on outcomes in the experiment. However, invalidation from a significant other might be distressing or it might reduce distress by causing victims to adopt a different perspective on their situation.

In summary, the present findings further our understanding of how and under what conditions expression of stress-related thoughts and feelings facilitates adjustment to stressors. More generally, this line of research points to potential mechanisms to explain the benefits of emotional support, "talk therapies," and psychosocial support groups, as well as the costs of failed support and social constraints on disclosure. It appears that sharing one's thoughts and feelings about an acute stressor shortly after it occurs can reduce the frequency of distressing, intrusive thoughts about the stressor and reduce psychological stress when reexposed to the stressor. However, when individuals' disclosures about stressors are invalidated, the benefits of talking may be diluted. This is important to consider in group therapy situations, where it is sometimes difficult to control how members will react to one another's disclosures. It is also important to consider when advis- ing friends or family members on how to be most supportive to individuals in crisis. Indeed, sometimes it may be necessary for victims to avoid people who are critical of how they cope with life stressors.

Refe rences

Abbey, A., Abramis, D. J., & Caplan, R. D. (1985). Effects of different sources of social support and social conflict on emotional well-being. Basic and Applied Social Psychology, 6, 111-129.

Aiken, L. S., & West, S. G. (1991). Multiple regression: Testing and interpreting interactions. Newbury Park, CA: Sage.

American Psychiatric Association. (1994). Diagnostic and statistical man- ual of mental disorders (4th ed.). Washington, DC: Author.

Baider, L., & De-Nour, A. K. (1997). Psychological distress and intrusive thoughts in cancer patients. Journal of Nervous & Mental Disease, 185, 346 -348.

Baron, R. M., & Kenny, D. A. (1986). The moderator-mediator variable distinction in statistical social psychological research: Conceptual, stra- tegic, and statistical considerations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 51, 1173-1182.

Baum, A., Cohen, L., & Hall, M. (1993). Control and intrusive memories as possible determinants of chronic stress. Psychosomatic Medicine, 55, 274 -286.

Bootzin, R. R. (1997). Examining the theory and clinical utility of writing about emotional experiences. Psychological Science, 8, 167-169.

Brewin, C. R., Watson, M., McCarthy, S., Hyman, P., & Dayson, D. (1998). Memory processes and the course of anxiety and depression in cancer patients. Psychological Medicine, 28, 219-224.

Clark, L. F. (1993). Stress and the cognitive-conversational benefits of social interaction. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 12, 25-55.

Creamer, M., Burgess, P., & Pattison, P. (1992). Reaction to trauma: A cognitive processing model. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 101, 452-459.

Cordova, M. J., Andrykowski, M. A., Kenady, D. E., McGrath, P. C., Sloan, D. A., & Redd, W. H. (1995). Frequency and correlates of posttranmatic-stress-disorder-like symptoms after treatment for breast cancer. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 63, 981-986.

Cutrona, C. E. (1986). Behavioral manifestations of social support: A microanalytic investigation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychol- ogy, 51, 201-208.

Davis, H., De-Nour, A. K., Shouval, D., & Melmed, R. N. (1998). Psy- chological distress inpatients with chronic, nonalcoholic, uncomplicated liver disease. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 44, 547-554.

Donnelly, D. A., & Murray, E. J. (1991). Cognitive and emotional changes in written essays and therapy interviews. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 10, 334-350.

Dougall, A. L., Craig, K. J., & Baum, A. (1999). Assessment of charac- teristics of intrusive thoughts and their impact on distress among victims of traumatic events. Psychosomatic Medicine, 61, 38-48.

Epstein, S. (1985). The implications of cognitive-experiential self-theory for research in social psychology and personality. Journal for the Theory of Social Behaviour, 15, 283-310.

Epstein, S. (1991). The self-concept, the traumatic neurosis, and the struc- ture of personality. In D. Ozer, J. J. M. Healy, & A. J. Stewart (Eds.), Perspectives on personality (pp. 163-185). London: Jessica Kingsley.

Esteding, B. A., Antoni, M. H., Fletcher, M. A., Margulies, S., & Schnei- derman, N. (1994). Emotional disclosure through writing or speaking modulates latent Epstein-Barr virus antibody titers. Journal of Consult- ing and Clinical Psychology, 62, 130-140.

Foa, E. B., & Kozak, M. J. (1991). Emotional processing: Theory, research and clinical implications for anxiety disorders. In J. D. Safran & L. S. Greenberg (Eds.), Emotion, Psychotherapy & Change (pp. 21-49). New York: Guilford Press.

Fowles, D. C. (1980). The three arousal model: Implications of Gray's two-factor theory for heart rate, electrodermal activity, and psychopathy. Psychophysiology, 17, 87-104.

Gold, D. B., & Wegner, D. M. (1995). Origins of ruminative thought: Trauma, incompleteness, nondisclosure, and suppression. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 25, 1245-1261.

Graham-Bermann, S. A., & Levendosky, A. A. (1998). Traumatic stress symptoms in children of battered women. Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 13, 111-128.

Greenberg, M. A., Wortman, C. B., & Stone, A. A. (1996). Emotional

Page 9: Talking Facilitates Cognitive-Emotional Processes of ... · Extremely negative emotional reactions to intrusive thoughts and other reminders of a stressor can impede cognitive integration

TALKING AND COGNITIVE-EMOTIONAL PROCESSES 507

expression and physical health: Revising traumatic memories or foster- ing self-regulation? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 71, 588-602.

Harber, K. D., & Pennebaker, J. (1992). Overcoming traumatic memories. In S. A. Christianson (Ed.), The handbook of emotion and memory (pp. 359-387). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

Holahan, C. J., Moos, R. H., Holahan, C. K., & Brennan, P. L. (1997). Social context, coping strategies, and depressive symptoms: An ex- panded model with cardiac patients. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 72, 918-928.

Horowitz, M. J. (1986). Stress response syndromes (2nd ed.). New York: Jason Aronson.

Horowitz, M. J. (1993). Stress-response syndromes: A review of posttrau- matic stress and adjustment disorders. In J. P. Wilson & B. Raphael (Eds.), International handbook of traumatic stress syndromes (pp. 4 9 - 60). New York: Plenum.

Horowitz, M. J., Wilner, N., & Alvarez, W. (1979). The Impacts of Events Scale: A measure of subjective stress. Psychosomatic Medicine, 41, 209-218.

Janoff-Bulman, R. (1992). Shattered assumptions: Toward a new psychol- ogy of trauma. New York: Free Press.

Kelley, J. E., Lumley, M. A., & Leisen, J. C. (1997). Health effects of emotional disclosure in rheumatoid arthritis patients. Health Psychol- ogy, 16, 331-340.

Kenny, D. A., Kashy, D. A., & Bolger, N. (1998). Data analysis in social psychology. In D. T. Gilbert & S. T. Fiske (Eds.), The handbook of social psychology: Vol. 2 (4th ed., pp. 233-265). Boston: McGraw-Hill.

King, M. G., Burrows, G. D., & Stanley, G. V. (1983). Measurement of stress and arousal: Validation of the stress/arousal adjective checklist. British Journal of Psychology, 74, 473-479.

Kliewer, W. L., Lepore, S. J., Oskin, D., & Johnson, P. D. (1998). The role of social and cognitive processes in children's adjustment to community violence. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 66, 199-209.

Koslowsky, M., Solomon, Z., Bleich, A., & Laor, N. (1996). Effects of stressful stimuli: A comparison between two time periods. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 52, 279-283.

Lepore, S. J. (1992). Social conflict, social support, and psychological distress: Evidence of cross-domain buffering effects. Journal of Person- ality and Social Psychology, 63, 857-867.

Lepore, S. J. (1997). Expressive writing moderates the relation between intrusive thoughts and depressive symptoms. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 73, 1030-1037.

Lepore, S. J. (in press). A social-cognitive processing model of emotional adjustment to cancer. In A. Baum & B. Andersen (Eds.), Psychosocial interventions for cancer. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.

Lepore, S. J., & Helgeson, V. (1998). Social constraints moderate the relation between intrusive thoughts and mental health in prostate cancer survivors. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 17, 89-106.

Lepore, S. J., Silver, R. C., Wortrnan, C. B., & Wayment, H. A. (1996). Social constraints, intrusive thoughts, and depressive symptoms among bereaved mothers. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 70, 271-282.

Lutgendorf, S. K., & Antoni, M. H. (1999). Emotional and cognitive processing in a trauma disclosure paradigm. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 23, 423-440.

Lutgendorf, S. K., Antoni, M. H., Ironson, G., Schneiderman, N., & Fletcher, M. A. (1997). Cognitive processing style, affective status, and immune function following HIV seropositivity notification. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 21, 157-184.

Maercker, A., & Schuetzwohl, M. (1997). Long-term effects of political imprisonment: A group comparison study. Social Psychiatry and Psy- chiatric Epidemiology, 32, 435-442.

Major, B., Cozzarelli, C., Sciacchitano, A. M., Cooper, M. L., Testa, M.,

& Mueller, P. M. (1990). Perceived social support, self-efficacy, and adjustment to abortion. Journal of Personality and Social Psychol- ogy, 59, 452-463.

Major, B., & Gramzow, R. H. (1999). Abortion as stigma: Cognitive and emotional implications of concealment. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 77, 735-745.

Major, B., Zubek, J. M., Cooper, M. L., Cozzarelli, C., & Richards, C. (1997). Mixed messages: Implications of social conflict and social support within close relationships for adjustment to a stressful event. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 72, 1349-1363.

Manne, S. L., Taylor, K. L., Dougherty, J., & Kemeny, N. (1997). Sup- portive and negative responses in the partner relationship: Their associ- ation with psychological adjustment among individuals with cancer. Journal of Behavioral Medicine, 20, 101-125.

Marris, P. (1975). Loss and change. Garden City, NY: Anchor/Doubleday. McCann, I. L., & Pearlman, L. A. (1990). Psychological trauma and the

adult survivor: Theory, therapy, and transformation. New York: Brun- ner/Mazel.

McFarlane, A. C. (1992). Avoidance and intrusion in posttraumatic stress disorder. The Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 180, 439-445.

Mcintosh, D. N., Silver, R. C., & Wortman, C. B. (1993). Religion's role in adjustment to a negative life event: Coping with the loss of a child. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 65, 812- 821.

Meichenbaum, D., & Fitzpatrick, D. (1993). A constructivist narrative perspective on stress and coping: Stress inoculation applications. In L. Goldberger & S. Breznitz (Eds.), Handbook of stress: Theoretical and clinical aspects (2nd ed., pp. 706-723). New York: Free Press.

Mendolia, M., & Kleck, R. E. (1993). Effects of talking about a stressful event on arousal: Does what we talk about make a difference? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 64, 283-292.

Moneyham, L., Seals, B., Sowell, R., Hennessy, M., Demi, A., & Brake, S. (1997). The impact of HIV on emotional distress of infected women: Cognitive appraisal and coping as mediators. Scholarly Inquiry for Nursing Practice, 11, 125-145.

Obrist, P. A. (1981). Cardiovascular psychophysiology: A perspective. New York: Plenum Press.

Parkes, C. M. (1975). What becomes of redundant world models? A contribution to the study of adaptation to change. British Journal of Medical Psychology, 48, 131-137.

Pennebaker, J. W. (1989). Confession, inhibition, and disease. In L. Berkowitz (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 22, pp. 211-244). Orlando, FL: Academic Press.

Pennebaker, J. W. (1993). Putting stress into words: Health, linguistic, and therapeutic implications. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 31, 539- 548.

Pennebaker, J. W. (1995). Emotion, disclosure, and health. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.

Pennebaker, J. W., Barger, S. D., & Tiebout, J. (1989). Disclosure of traumas and health among Holocaust survivors. Psychosomatic Medi- cine, 51, 577-589.

Pennebaker, J. W., & BeaU, S. K. (1986). Confronting a traumatic event: Toward an understanding of inhibition and diseases. Journal of Abnor- mal Psychology, 95, 274-281.

Pennebaker, J. W., Colder, M., & Sharp, L. K. (1990). Accelerating the coping process. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 58, 528-537.

Pennebaker, J. W., Hughes, C. F., & O'Heeron, R. C. (1987). The psy- chophysiology of confession: Linking inhibitory and psychosomatic processes. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 52, 781-793.

Pennebaker, J. W., & Kiecolt-Glaser, R. (1988). Disclosure of traumas and immune function: Health implications for psychotherapy. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 56, 239-245.

Petrie, K. J., Booth, R. J., Pennebaker, J. W., Davison, K. P., & Thomas, M. G. (1995). Disclosure of trauma and immune response to a hepatitis

Page 10: Talking Facilitates Cognitive-Emotional Processes of ... · Extremely negative emotional reactions to intrusive thoughts and other reminders of a stressor can impede cognitive integration

508 LEPORE, RAGAN, AND JONES

B vaccination program. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychol- ogy, 63, 787-792.

Rachman, S. (1980). Emotional processing. Behaviour Research and Ther- apy, 18, 51-60.

Rime, B. (1995). Mental rumination, social sharing, and the recovery from emotional exposure. In J. W. Pennebaker (Ed.), Emotion, disclosure, & health (pp. 271-292). Washington, DC: American Psychological Asso- ciation.

Rook, K. S. (1984). The negative side of social interaction: Impact on psychological well-being. Journal of Personality and Social Psychol- ogy, 46, 1097-1108.

Silver, R. L., Boon, C., & Stones, M. H. (1983). Searching for meaning in misfortune: Making sense of incest. Journal of Social Issues, 39, 81- 102.

Silver, R. L., & Wortman, C. B. (1980). Coping with undesirable life events. In J. Garber & M. E. P. Seligman (Eds.), Human helplessness (pp. 279-340). New York: Academic Press.

Smyth, J. M. (1998). Written emotional expression: Effect sizes, outcome types, and moderating variables. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 66, 174-184.

Smyth, J. M., Stone, A. A., Hurewitz, A., & Kaell, A. (1999). Effects of writing about stressful experiences on symptom reduction in patients

with asthma or rheumatoid arthritis. Journal of the American Medical Association, 281, 1304-1329.

Sobel, M. E. (1982). Asymptomatic confidence intervals for indirect ef- fects in structural equation models. In S. Leinhart (Ed.), Sociological methodology (pp. 290-312). San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.

Spera, S. P., Buhrfeind, E. D., & Pennebaker, J. W. (1994). Expressive writing and coping with job loss. Academy of Management Journal, 37, 722-733.

Stiles, W. B. (1995). Disclosure as a speech act: Is it psychotherapeutic to disclose? In J. W. Pennebaker (Ed.), Emotion, disclosure, and health (pp. 271-292). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.

Tait, R., & Silver, R. C. (1989). Coming to terms with major negative life events. In J. S. Uleman & J. A. Bargh (Eds.), Unintended thought (pp. 351-382). New York: Guilford Press.

Vinokur, A. D., & van Ryn, M. (1993). Social support and undermining in close relationships: Their independent effects on the mental health of unemployed persons. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 65, 350-359.

Received August 31, 1998 Revision received September 23, 1999

Accepted September 28, 1999 •

Low Publication Prices for APA Members and Affiliates

Keeping you up-to-date. All A P A Fel lows, Members , Associates, and Student Affi l iates r e c e i v e m a s part o f their annua l dues - - subsc r i p t i ons to the American Psychologist and APA Monitor. High School Teacher and In terna t ional Affi l iates receive subscr ipt ions to the APA Monitor, and they may subscr ibe to the American Psychologist at a s ignif icant ly reduced rate. In addit ion, all Member s and Student Affi l iates are el igible for savings of up to 60% (plus a jou rna l credit) on all other A P A journals , as well as s ignif icant discounts on subscr ipt ions f rom cooperat ing societies and publishers (e.g., the Amer ican Associa t ion for Counse l ing and Deve lopment , Academic Press, and H u m a n Sciences Press).

Essential resources . A P A members and affiliates receive special rates for purchases of A P A books, inc lud ing the Publication Manual o f the American Psychological Association, and on dozens of new topical books each year.

Other benefits of membership. Membersh ip in A P A also provides el igibil i ty for compet i t ive insurance plans, con t inu ing educat ion programs, reduced A P A conven t ion fees, and special ty divis ions.

More information. Write to Amer ican Psychological Associat ion, Membersh ip Services, 750 First Street, NE, Washing ton , DC 20002-4242.