39
BRAIN AND LANGUAGE 64, 83–121 (1998) ARTICLE NO. BL981958 Syllabic Constraints in the Phonological Errors of an Aphasic Patient Cristina Romani School of Psychology, University of Birmingham, Edgbaston, Birmingham B15 2TT, UK and Andrea Calabrese Department of Linguistics, University of Connecticut The Sonority Dispersion Principle (Clements, 1990) states that the sharper the rise in sonority between the beginning of the syllable and the nucleus, the better the syllable. So far evidence in favour of this principle has been derived mainly from the distributional properties of syllable types and, to a lesser extent, from language acquisition. The case of DB, presented in this study, provides strong evi- dence that the Sonority Dispersion Principle also applies to an explanation of aphasic errors and revives Jakobson’s idea that the same principles of complexity can ex- plain the distribution of syllables, language acquisition, and language loss ( Jakob- son, 1941, 1968). Although some evidence that sonority constraints aphasic errors has been presented before, this is the first study reporting systematic effects of sonority-based complexity in aphasia. 1998 Academic Press Phonological errors occur very commonly in aphasic patients. Studies en- dorsing traditional, clinical classifications of aphasia have reported that they occur in all major types of aphasia, Broca’s, Wernicke’s, and conduction, and with similar characteristics (e.g., Blumstein, 1978). In a cognitive frame- work, phonological errors can be attributed to different stages of a production model: they may arise because the correct entry in the phonological lexicon cannot be accessed (lexical access), because the right sequence of phonemes Preparation of this work was supported in part by INH Grant 23836 to Prof. Alfonso Cara- mazza and by a HFSPO (Human Frontier Science Program Organization) fellowship to Dr. Cristina Romani. We thank sincerely Gabriele Miceli who has provided access to the patient. We also thank Andrew Olson for helping with the analyses of the data and Alfonso Caramazza for making available some of the data collected on the patient. We finally thank Morris Halle for their comments and suggestions. Reprint requests should be addressed to Cristina Romani, Ph.D., at the School of Psychology, University of Birmingham, Edgbaston, Birmingham B15 2TT, UK. Fax: 44-21-414 4897. E-mail: [email protected]. 83 0093-934X/98 $25.00 Copyright 1998 by Academic Press All rights of reproduction in any form reserved.

Syllabic Constraints in the Phonological Errors of an ...€¦ · degree of complexity to the basic CV template. Following this principle, one can derive the following ranking of

  • Upload
    others

  • View
    0

  • Download
    0

Embed Size (px)

Citation preview

Page 1: Syllabic Constraints in the Phonological Errors of an ...€¦ · degree of complexity to the basic CV template. Following this principle, one can derive the following ranking of

BRAIN AND LANGUAGE 64, 83–121 (1998)ARTICLE NO. BL981958

Syllabic Constraints in the Phonological Errorsof an Aphasic Patient

Cristina Romani

School of Psychology, University of Birmingham, Edgbaston, Birmingham B15 2TT, UK

and

Andrea Calabrese

Department of Linguistics, University of Connecticut

The Sonority Dispersion Principle (Clements, 1990) states that the sharper therise in sonority between the beginning of the syllable and the nucleus, the betterthe syllable. So far evidence in favour of this principle has been derived mainlyfrom the distributional properties of syllable types and, to a lesser extent, fromlanguage acquisition. The case of DB, presented in this study, provides strong evi-dence that the Sonority Dispersion Principle also applies to an explanation of aphasicerrors and revives Jakobson’s idea that the same principles of complexity can ex-plain the distribution of syllables, language acquisition, and language loss ( Jakob-son, 1941, 1968). Although some evidence that sonority constraints aphasic errorshas been presented before, this is the first study reporting systematic effects ofsonority-based complexity in aphasia. 1998 Academic Press

Phonological errors occur very commonly in aphasic patients. Studies en-dorsing traditional, clinical classifications of aphasia have reported that theyoccur in all major types of aphasia, Broca’s, Wernicke’s, and conduction,and with similar characteristics (e.g., Blumstein, 1978). In a cognitive frame-work, phonological errors can be attributed to different stages of a productionmodel: they may arise because the correct entry in the phonological lexiconcannot be accessed (lexical access), because the right sequence of phonemes

Preparation of this work was supported in part by INH Grant 23836 to Prof. Alfonso Cara-mazza and by a HFSPO (Human Frontier Science Program Organization) fellowship to Dr.Cristina Romani. We thank sincerely Gabriele Miceli who has provided access to the patient.We also thank Andrew Olson for helping with the analyses of the data and Alfonso Caramazzafor making available some of the data collected on the patient. We finally thank Morris Hallefor their comments and suggestions. Reprint requests should be addressed to Cristina Romani,Ph.D., at the School of Psychology, University of Birmingham, Edgbaston, Birmingham B152TT, UK. Fax: 44-21-414 4897. E-mail: [email protected].

830093-934X/98 $25.00

Copyright 1998 by Academic PressAll rights of reproduction in any form reserved.

Page 2: Syllabic Constraints in the Phonological Errors of an ...€¦ · degree of complexity to the basic CV template. Following this principle, one can derive the following ranking of

84 ROMANI AND CALABRESE

cannot be accessed (phonological encoding), because information decays ina phonological output buffer, or, we suggest, because of an articulatory plan-ning deficit. If a sequence of phonemes is too complex to be produced, itmay be simplified by deleting, inserting, substituting, or transposing pho-nemes. The presence of phonological errors, therefore, by itself, is not diag-nostic of the locus of patient’s impairment. Not only could phonologicalerrors arise from different kinds of phonological impairment, but they couldalso arise because of an articulatory planning deficit.

This study analyzes in detail the phonological errors of a single aphasicpatient—DB. We use the descriptive term phonological errors without im-plying any commitment to the functional locus of damage. Our aim is notso much to establish the exact locus of the patient’s deficit, even if a precisehypothesis will be advanced, but, instead, to show the impact of linguisticconstraints on DB’s errors. This will enable both a better characterizationof those contexts that the patient finds most difficult—and that, therefore,are most likely to elicit errors—and a better characterization of the linguisticuniversals underlying the distribution of linguistic structures in the languagesof the world.

It has long been recognized that the ‘‘best’’ syllable is one made up bya single consonant in the onset and a vowel (CV). A complex onset (CCV),a coda (CVC), or the lack of an onset (V) can all be considered to add adegree of complexity to the basic CV template. Following this principle, onecan derive the following ranking of complexity for syllable templates (seeKaye & Lowenstamm, 1981; Clements & Keyser, 1983):

1. CV2. V, CCV, CVC3. VC, CCVC4. VCC.

The empirical evidence for a hierarchy of syllable templates stems mainlyfrom syllable distributions—a simpler syllable should occur more often inthe languages of the world and—from what are called implicational univer-sals—if a language allows a template of a given complexity, it should alsoallow the simpler templates. The errors made by aphasic patients, however,have also lent some support to such a hierarchy of syllable templates.

Studies that have looked at linguistic constraints on errors made by apha-sics were pioneered by Blumstein (1973, 1978), who established a numberof important generalizations confirmed by later studies (see Beland, 1990;Beland, Caplan, & Nespoulous, 1990; Beland, Paradis, & Bois, 1993; Buck-ingham, 1986; Ferreres, 1990; Nespoulous, Joanette, Beland, Caplan, & Le-cours, 1984). In particular, she noted that deletion and insertion errors areconstrained by syllabic context. Deletions occur mainly in complex onsets(e.g., /tre.no/ . /te.no/) or in coda (/sfor.tso/ . /sfo.tso/) (examples aretaken from DB) and very rarely involve a vowel or a consonant in simple

Page 3: Syllabic Constraints in the Phonological Errors of an ...€¦ · degree of complexity to the basic CV template. Following this principle, one can derive the following ranking of

PHONOLOGICAL ERRORS AND APHASIA 85

onsets. Insertions occur mainly at the beginning of a word starting with avowel (e.g., /A.pri.le/ . /pa.pri.le/) or to break the sequence of two vowels(a hiatus, e.g., /ne.o/ . /ne.no/). All these errors can be explained as simpli-fications of syllable structure: complex onsets, codas, onsetless syllables, andhiatuses are eliminated and replaced by simpler CV syllables.

DB’s deletion and insertion errors have characteristics similar to thosedescribed above and can also be explained in terms of a hierarchy of syllabletemplates. The main aim of this study, however, is to apply to DB’s errorsa finer-grained principle of syllabic complexity. Clements (1990), followingother phonologists like Vennemann (1988) and Basbøll (1977), has arguedthat a hierarchy of CV syllable templates is inadequate to capture the fullrange of linguistic facts. He has argued that syllables should be ranked notonly according to the structural properties of their components (e.g., whetherthey have an onset, whether or not the onset is complex, etc.), but also ac-cording to the nature of the segments in the syllabic components (e.g.,whether an obstruent or a fricative is in the onset). He has formulated aformal principle: the Sonority Dispersion Principle that by exploiting a prop-erty of segments—sonority—establishes the relative complexity of any syl-lable type, even within the same syllable template.

To apply the Sonority Dispersion Principle (henceforth SDP) to aphasiais interesting for a number of reasons. First of all, finding a correspondencebetween what is predicted by the SDP and the errors of an aphasic patientwould support the validity of this principle and would encourage perfor-mance models to take it into consideration. Second, it would provide aphasi-ology with a much finer metric of complexity which would allow a muchmore precise characterization of a patient’s speech. Finally, given the dys-arthric quality of DB’s speech, to find a correspondence between his errorsand the SDP would suggest that this principle has articulatory underpinnings.

The rest of the introduction is divided into three main sections that togetherprovide the theoretical apparatus necessary for our analyses of DB’s errors.First, we will introduce sonority and sonority-based notions of complexity.Second, we will discuss applications of the SDP to aphasia. Third and finally,we will briefly sketch a model of speech production and see which role theSDP may play in this model.

Syllabic Complexity

Sonority. It has long been noticed that not all combinations of soundsoccur as syllables in the languages of the world. For example, while combi-nations like /pla/, /pra/, /pan/, and /pya/ occur quite frequently, combina-tions such as /mta/, /npa/, and /lta/ occur very infrequently, if at all. Toaccount for these kinds of generalizations, Sievers (1901), Jespersen (1922),and others introduced the notion of sonority. Sonority has been defined eitherin terms of ease of perception (the higher the sonority, the higher the salience

Page 4: Syllabic Constraints in the Phonological Errors of an ...€¦ · degree of complexity to the basic CV template. Following this principle, one can derive the following ranking of

86 ROMANI AND CALABRESE

TABLE 1Example of a Sonority Scale for Italian Phonemes

Sonority level Classes of segments Segments

HighVowels a, e, o, i, uGlides y, w

rLiquids ,

lNasal m, nObstruents

Fricatives f, v, s, ʃAffricatives ts, tZ, tʃ, DVoiced stops d, b, g,

Low Unvoiced stops t, p, k

(Price, 1980)) or in terms of ease of articulation (the higher the sonority,the more open the vocal tract (Lindblom, 1983; Keating, 1983)). A formaldefinition of sonority as a single, underived property has proved elusive (seeClements, 1990). All sounds, however, can be ranked on a scale accordingto the degree in which they possess the property of ‘‘sonority’’ with onlyminimal disagreement. Table 1 represents an example of a sonority scale forItalian segments. Vowels are the most sonorous segments of all; unvoicedstops the least sonorous. Some of the distinctions represented in this scaleare more widely accepted than others. For example, not all the proposedsonority scales make a distinction between the liquids /r/ and /l/ or a distinc-tion between voiced and unvoiced stops. We have included these distinctionsboth on the basis of preexisting evidence (e.g., Steriade, 1982) and becausethey are supported by DB’s error pattern, as we will show.

The notion of sonority provides a way of defining the syllable. Accordingto the Sonority Sequencing Principle (Sievers, 1901; Jespersen, 1922) a syl-lable is a string of sounds organized in such a way that sonority rises fromthe beginning to a peak and then falls (vowels always correspond to syllabicpeaks since they have the highest sonority value). For example, the sequenceof sounds /t r a p/ makes a syllable since sonority increases steadily from/t/ to /r/ and from /r/ to /a/ and then drops. The sequence of sounds corre-sponding to /r t a p/, on the other hand, is not a legal syllable because thereis a drop in sonority between the beginning of the syllable and the peak. Thisexplains why sequences like /trya/ or /tya/ or /ta/ are acceptable syllables inmany languages while syllables like /ltka/ or /rka/ are found very rarely, ifat all.

Sonority-based hierarchies of complexity. The Sonority Sequencing Prin-ciple explains why certain syllables are acceptable while others are not. Itdoes not explain, however, why certain syllable types are far more common

Page 5: Syllabic Constraints in the Phonological Errors of an ...€¦ · degree of complexity to the basic CV template. Following this principle, one can derive the following ranking of

PHONOLOGICAL ERRORS AND APHASIA 87

than others. Harris (1983), for example, has noted that while /pra/ and /pla/are widely attested, /pna/ and /pma/ are not, even if they do not violate thesonority principle. To account for this fact Harris (1983) has proposed thateach language requires a minimal distance in sonority between the conso-nants that make up an onset. Spanish, like English, would accept onsets madeup by an obstruent and a liquid such as /pla/ and /tra/, since obstruents andliquids are separated by two main steps on the sonority scale. It would notaccept, instead, onsets made up by an obstruent and a nasal such as /pna/or /tma/ since obstruents and nasals are only one step apart on the sonorityscale.

Clements (1990), Basbøll (1977), and Vennemann (1988), among others,have tried to extend the principle of Minimal Distance proposed by Harrisin order to rank not only onsets, but all types of syllabic configurations. Herewe will discuss Clements’ theory, which, in our opinion, most successfullyaccounts for the relative complexity of syllable configurations (for an alterna-tive theory of the same phenomena, see Cairns and Feinstein (1982)). Clem-ents’ SDP states that ‘‘The sonority profile of the optimal syllable type risesmaximally and steadily from the beginning to the peak, and falls minimallyfrom the peak to the end.’’

This principle, therefore, assumes that greater differences in sonority areto be preferred, not only between members of a complex onset, but alsobetween a consonant in the onset and the following vowel. Clements, more-over, hypothesizes that, whereas greater differences in sonority are preferredbetween segments in the onset and the nucleus, smaller differences in sonor-ity are preferred between the nucleus and the segments in coda. Thus, /ta/is better then /ra/ in the initial part of the syllable, since the rise in sonorityis steeper in /ta/ than in /ra/, but the opposite is true for the final part of asyllable. Here /ar/ is better than /at/ because a more gentle decline in sonorityis preferred. Maximization of the sonority slope in onset and minimizationof the sonority slope in coda create sonority ‘‘drops’’ between syllables thatmay be perceptually and, possibly, articulatorily useful (see Mehler, Dom-mergues, Fraunfelder, & Segui, 1981). All syllables can be ranked accordingto the SDP. Table 2 represents a hierarchy of Italian syllabic configurationsas derived from the SDP. Distinct hierarchies are derived for the initial andthe final part of the syllable and for simple and complex onsets. As is gener-ally accepted, CV onsets are considered simpler than CCV onsets.

The optimal syllables are those made up by an obstruent and a vowel (OV)as in /pa/, /ta/, or /ka/ since they have the maximum possible difference insonority between the onset and the nucleus. The worst syllable is one madeup by a liquid, a glide, and a vowel (LGV) since in the onset there is onlyone step difference in sonority between the liquid and the glide. Accordingto Clements, OLV syllables are to be preferred over OGV syllables, even ifsonority rises the same amount between the margin and the following peak,because equal differences in sonority are preferred over unequal sonority

Page 6: Syllabic Constraints in the Phonological Errors of an ...€¦ · degree of complexity to the basic CV template. Following this principle, one can derive the following ranking of

88 ROMANI AND CALABRESE

TABLE 2Hierarchy of Italian Syllable Configurations Based on the Sonority

Dispersion Principle Proposed by Clements (1990)

Syllable type Example

Simple onsets1. OV (obstruent-vowel) /ta/2. NV (nasal-vowel) /na/3. LV (liquid-vowel) /la/4. GV (glide-vowel) /ya/

Complex onsets1. OLV (obstruent-liquid-vowel) /tra/2. OGV (obstruent-glide-vowel) /tya/3. NGV (nasal-glide-vowel) /nya/4. LGV (liquid-glide-vowel) /rya/

Codas1. VG (vowel-glide) /ay/2. VL (vowel-liquid) /al/3. VN (vowel-nasal) /an/4. VO (vowel-obstruent) /as/

Note. 1 5 least complex; 4 5 most complex.

intervals. In an OLV syllable, sonority rises two steps between the obstruentand the liquid and another two steps between the liquid and the vowel. Ina OGV syllable, on the other hand, sonority rises three steps between theobstruent and the glide and only one step between the glide and the vowel.

Sonority and Aphasia

With the SDP, Clements provides a single, formal principle which aims toexplain the relative complexity of any syllable type. At present, the physicalcorrelates of sonority and of sonority-based hierarchies of complexity arenot completely clear. This, however, does not mean that there are none.Clements, in fact, states ‘‘Given the remarkable similarity among sonorityconstraints in different and widely separated languages, we might expect thatsonority could be related to one or more invariant physical or psychoacousticparameters’’ (Clements, 1990, p. 290). The relation between a phonologicaland a phonetic definition of sonority raises issues that have also been debatedfor other phonological concepts. Let us take for example phonological fea-tures. While a general attempt has been made to relate phonological featuresto articulatory configurations (e.g., Chomsky and Halle, 1968), these at-tempts have been only partially successful. The fact that, at present, somefeatures elude a precise phonetic definition, however, neither diminishes theirvalue in accounting for linguistic generalizations, nor implies that they haveno articulatory correlates.

Clements argues in favor of his hierarchy of syllabic configurations mainly

Page 7: Syllabic Constraints in the Phonological Errors of an ...€¦ · degree of complexity to the basic CV template. Following this principle, one can derive the following ranking of

PHONOLOGICAL ERRORS AND APHASIA 89

on the basis of cross-linguistic data. If a language includes a syllable typeof a given complexity, generally, it includes all syllable types of lower com-plexity. If the SDP has articulatory correlates, however, it should also beable to predict the errors made by patients with articulatory difficulties, aswell as the order in which syllables are acquired by children.

Previous studies. Although no study has yet provided systematic supportfor a metric of complexity based on sonority, the notion of sonority, and theSDP have already been applied to an explanation of aphasics’ errors (Belandet al., 1990; Buckingham, 1986; Christman, 1992, 1994). Buckingham(1986) first observed that aphasics avoid inserting or deleting a segment ifthis results in a syllabic configuration which is more complex in terms ofsonority. He also noted that aphasics have difficulty with hiatuses whichlack a contrast in sonority between the two adjacent segments (Buckingham,1990). Beland et al. (1990), have also noted that their patient, RL, deletesthe liquid and not the obstruent in OLV syllables. This maximizes the differ-ence in sonority between the margin and the nucleus and thus produces abetter syllable.

Other studies have attempted to apply the notion of sonority to aphasiain a more systematic way. Christman (1992) has shown that the neologisticproduction of jargonaphasic patients respects the SDP. Because this maysimply indicate a respect for phonotactic constraints, Christman (1994) hasanalyzed errors from the same populations in relation to their outcome interms of sonority. Considering all errors, 53% did not change sonority, 27%resulted in a better configuration, and 20% resulted in a worse configuration.These results are poor, but could well be attributed to the fact that jargon-aphasics are not an appropriate population to test the effects of the SDP.Their errors are likely to have a lexical locus and here syllabic simplificationsare not expected. Another study which examined the effects of sonority inaphasia is that of Bastiaanse, Gilbers, and Van der Linde (1994). They foundthat a group of four conduction aphasics made more substitution errors thatchanged sonority compared to a group of six Broca’s aphasics. There are,however, a number of problems with this study: (1) the difference betweenthe two groups was barely significant; (2) more substitutions that changedsonority in conduction aphasics could be attributed to a more severe impair-ment in this population (sonority changes often involve multiple featuretransformations); (3) finally and crucially, the authors failed to considerwhether the errors resulted in a better or worse syllabic configuration. Ifsonority has an articulatory basis, in fact, we may expect sonority substitu-tions to result in simplifications more often in Broca’s than in conductionaphasics.

The present study aims to address the limitations of these previous studiesby applying a sonority-based notion of syllabic complexity systematicallyto an analysis of all the phonological errors produced by an articulatorily

Page 8: Syllabic Constraints in the Phonological Errors of an ...€¦ · degree of complexity to the basic CV template. Following this principle, one can derive the following ranking of

90 ROMANI AND CALABRESE

disfluent patient. Our general predictions is that DB’s errors will show aclear trend toward producing syllabic configurations which are better (lesscomplex) than the target in terms of sonority dispersion.

A Model of Speech Production

We have discussed principles of syllabic complexity and their applicationto aphasia. We can ask now how these notions fit with a model of wordproduction. We will assume, as a frame of reference, a production modelsimilar to the one outlined by Levelt (see Levelt, 1992; Butterworth, 1992).We assume that, in the lexicon, phoneme order is represented by linking eachphoneme to a ‘‘place-holder’’ or ‘‘timing unit.’’ In autosegmental phonologythese units are represented either as a sequence of abstract Xs (see Goldsmith,1990; Kenstowicz, 1994) or as a sequence of consonants and vowels (Clem-ents & Keyser, 1983). For example,

(a) c a r p e t (b) c a r p e t| | | | | | | | | | | |x x x x x x C V C C V C.

Since syllabic structure is totally recoverable from a given sequence ofsegments, we assume that timing units in the lexicon are not linked to syllabicpositions. The lexical level, however, will have to provide some minimalinformation about stress since in many languages (Italian and English amongthem) stress can be an idiosyncratic property of a lexical representation (com-pare, in Italian /’ancora/ (anchor) vs. /an’cora/ (again)). We hypothesizethat segments are copied from the lexicon into a phonological output buffer(see Levelt, 1992; Buckingham, 1986). Here, phonemes are syllabified andother parameters relevant to production are set (loudness, register, intonation,etc.). We assume that: (1) a prosodic node in the lexicon controls the creationof a corresponding number of syllable nodes in the buffer; (2) segments areselected one by one and linked to syllable positions (onset, nucleus, coda);(3) syllable positions, in turn, are linked to syllable nodes.

We propose that the model sketched above should be augmented by refer-ence to the SDP which, in any given language, establishes the maximumallowed degree of complexity. Greek, for example, allows onsets made upof an obstruent and a nasal (e.g., /pn/), while English does not. If the degreeof complexity allowed by the language of the speaker is exceeded, as in thecase of a foreign or borrowed word, then, the syllabification of a segmentis blocked and the phonological representation is simplified though the dele-tion, substitution, or insertion of segments (see Calabrese, 1995). We assume,moreover, that the maximum allowed degree of complexity depends not onlyon the language of the speaker, but also on his articulatory capacities. In thecase of children or brain-damaged patients, the allowed degree of complexitycan, on occasions, be inferior to that tolerated by the language. In these cases,

Page 9: Syllabic Constraints in the Phonological Errors of an ...€¦ · degree of complexity to the basic CV template. Following this principle, one can derive the following ranking of

PHONOLOGICAL ERRORS AND APHASIA 91

we assume that the representation can be simplified through similar proce-dures.

Language specific syllabification procedures. While the SDP accounts fora great number of linguistic facts, there are some noticeable exceptions. Toaccount for them, phonologists have assumed that there are, besides coresyllabification procedures, language specific procedures that deal with thesyllabification of sounds which violate sonority principles (Steriade, 1982;Levin, 1985; Clements, 1990). We assume that in Italian there are three casesof this type: the syllabification of /s/ in onset, the syllabification of /s/ incoda, and the syllabification of geminates.

Like English, Italian has onsets made up by s1obstruents (e.g., /stanko/(tired); /skremare/ (to skim)). These onsets violate both the Sonority Se-quencing Principle and the SDP since sonority decreases instead of increas-ing after the /s/. Some linguists have assumed that in these cases, /s/ isadjoined to the onset by a special rule insensitive to the sonority of segments(see Harris (1983) and Steriade (1982), among others). Italian does not allowcomplex codas. The distribution of simple codas can be neatly accountedfor by assuming that the level of complexity allowed by the SDP excludesobstruents.1 There is evidence, however, that both /s/ (an obstruent) and thefirst member of a geminate obstruent are syllabified in coda positions.

The fact that after a vowel, /s/ is syllabified in coda and not in onset (e.g.,/as.ta/ and not /*a.sta/) is demonstrated by the fact that, for the purpose ofrules of stress assignment and other rules, the syllable preceding /s/ acts asa closed syllable, that is, as a syllable with a coda (for evidence from Englishsee Davis (1987); for evidence from Italian, see Davis (1990) and Chierchia(1986)2; for some experimental evidence, see Treiman, Gross, and Cwikiel-Glavin (1992)). We will assume that a single procedure, which is insensi-

1 This generalization holds only for the native Italian lexicon. There are a small set of loanwords from Latin, Greek, and other languages that contain obstruents in coda position, e.g.,/kaptare/, /kaptzioso/, /ektzema/, /afgano/, /Trotzkista/.

2 Davis (1990) has proposed that the distribution of the Italian masculine definite articles:‘‘il’’ and ‘‘lo’’ can be explained by assuming that ‘‘il’’ is used before a consonant in theonset, ‘‘lo’’ in all other cases. Words beginning with an /s/ cluster are exceptional since theytake ‘‘lo’’ and not ‘‘il’’ (e.g., lo stanzone, lo stimolo, lo smalto, etc.). This is consistent withthe hypothesis that these words do not start with a complex onset configuration. It is assumed,instead, that /s/ is attached as a coda to the vowel of the preceding syllable (e.g., lo smalto. los.mal.to). In these cases, therefore, ‘‘lo’’ would be followed by a consonant in the codaand not by a consonant in the onset. The phenomenon of syntactic doubling occurs when aword beginning with a single consonant is preceded by a word ending with a stressed vowel(e.g., palto’ pulito; citta’ triste). In these cases, the beginning consonant of the second wordis geminated to close the preceding syllable in compliance with stress assignment rules(pal.top.pu.li.to; cit.tat.tris.te). Chierchia (1986) has noted that the phenomenon of syntacticdoubling does not occur with words beginning with an /s/ cluster. This is consistent with thehypothesis that the /s/ is extrasyllabic and is incorporated as the coda of the preceding syllable,thus making syntactic doubling unnecessary (e.g., citta’ sporca . cit.tas.por.ca).

Page 10: Syllabic Constraints in the Phonological Errors of an ...€¦ · degree of complexity to the basic CV template. Following this principle, one can derive the following ranking of

92 ROMANI AND CALABRESE

tive to sonority constraints, syllabifies /s/ both in onset and coda: anunsyllabified /s/ is incorporated as the coda of an adjacent syllable to theleft, if available (e.g., /as.ta/), otherwise as the onset of an adjacent syllableto the right (e.g., /stan.ko/; /per.spi.ka.tʃe/). This allows us to maintain thegeneralization that, in Italian, the SDP excludes obstruents in coda.

Finally, there are good reasons to believe that the first member of a gemi-nate is syllabified as the coda of the preceding syllable (e.g., /trat.to/ (tract);/top.pa/ (patch)). This evidence, again, comes mainly from stress assignmentrules which treat the syllable preceding the geminate as a closed syllable(see Kenstowicz, 1994; Goldsmith, 1990). This means, however, that in thecase of a geminate obstruent, an obstruent will be in coda in violation of thegeneralization proposed above. It has often been noted, however, that linkedconfigurations have idiosyncratic properties (e.g., Schein & Steriade, 1986).The presence of an obstruent geminate in coda, therefore, could be part ofa general relaxation of syllabic constraints for geminates (see Ito (1988); seealso Goldsmith (1990) and Clements (1990) for further discussion of theseissues).

In assessing whether DB’s pattern of errors conforms to the SDP, it wouldbe interesting to see whether it also conforms to its exceptions. If we findthat it does, this would strengthen the correspondence between DB’s errorsand the linguistic phenomena that, so far, have been the domain of explana-tion of the SDP. This will add to a demonstration that the same principlesof complexity which govern the distribution of syllable types within andbetween languages also apply to language loss in aphasia.

CASE REPORT

DB is an Italian patient in his early 40s with some college education. Hesuffered a stroke to his left hemisphere while scuba diving. A CT scanshowed an extensive left fronto-parietal lesion, both superficial and deep.He was tested at the Neuropsychological Service of the Catholic Universityof Rome for a period of several months. At the time that this study wascarried out, he was 6 years postonset and his condition remained stable dur-ing testing. The pattern of deletion errors made by DB has previously beendescribed in Calabrese and Romani (1991).

DB’s spontaneous speech is typical of a Broca’s aphasic. It is very limitedand effortful. It consists mainly of content words (function words are veryrarely produced) and there are frequent inflectional errors. Disfluencies canbe observed both between and within words and suggest articulatory diffi-culties. Phonemic paraphasias are frequent and are the focus of this paper.At least clinically, DB does not produce distorted or slurred phonemes, but,since his speech has not undergone a spectrographic analysis, we cannot ruleout the possibility that some of what are perceived as phonological errorsare, in fact, articulatory in nature.

Page 11: Syllabic Constraints in the Phonological Errors of an ...€¦ · degree of complexity to the basic CV template. Following this principle, one can derive the following ranking of

PHONOLOGICAL ERRORS AND APHASIA 93

DB shows no problems with voluntary movements involving the face andthe mouth (bucco-facial apraxia). He carried out correctly 10 out of 10 verbalcommands such as ‘‘blow a kiss’’ or ‘‘chatter your teeth.’’ The experimenterhad to demonstrate the required action for only one of them. He also per-formed correctly 10 out of 10 bucco-sequential movements (such as protrud-ing and spreading the lips or alternatingly touching the two corners of themouth with the tongue). Verbal agility, as measured by repetition of the word‘‘camomilla’’ in a unit of time, was within the lower normal range.

DB’s lack of bucco-facial apraxia, together with the presence of manysyllabic simplifications and with DB’s inconsistent production of phonemesdepending on context, exclude a peripheral impairment of motor implemen-tation (sometimes referred to as dysarthria or anarthria, e.g., Pierre Marie(1906)). They point, instead, to a more central deficit either in articulatoryprogramming or in the phonological representations that serve as input forarticulatory programming. If DB suffers from a deficit of articulatory pro-gramming, his difficulties are severe enough to affect the production of syl-labic configurations, but they are not severe enough to affect the productionof phonemes independently of context.

Comprehension

Clinically, DB appears able to follow a conversation and to carry out sim-ple commands. His comprehension of single words is good. In a task thatrequired him to match an auditorily presented noun to a picture presentedalong with phonological and semantic distractors, DB was 98% (39/40) cor-rect. On a written version of the same task, he was 90% (36/40) correct. Ona sentence–picture matching task with two alternatives including simple ac-tive and passive sentences, he was 77% correct (46/60). He was 85% correct(17/20) with the semantic distractors, 80% correct (16/20) with the morpho-logical distractors, and 65% correct (13/20) with the syntactic distractors.In a written version of the same task, he was 89% correct overall (40/45).He was 93% correct (14/15) with semantic distractors, 93% correct (14/15) with morphological distractors, and 80% correct (12/15) with syntacticdistractors.

Auditory Discrimination

DB was unimpaired on several tests of his phonological discriminationcapacity. He was given two same/different tasks involving pairs of nonsensesyllables (/trIn/, /drIn/, /crIn/, /grIn/, /prIn/, and /brIn/). In one task, thetwo syllables were presented auditorily one after the other and the patienthad to say ‘‘same’’ or ‘‘different.’’ In the second task, one syllable wasspoken and the other written on a card. In both tasks, the syllables of thenonmatching trials differed on either voicing, place of articulation, or both.In the first task, DB made 3/60 errors, in the second task he made 4/60

Page 12: Syllabic Constraints in the Phonological Errors of an ...€¦ · degree of complexity to the basic CV template. Following this principle, one can derive the following ranking of

94 ROMANI AND CALABRESE

errors. This performance is within the normal range (0–10 for a group of50 normal speakers, for both tasks).

A second same–different phonological discrimination task involved pairsof words presented auditorily with a brief pause between them. On half ofthe trials, the two words were the same; on the other half they were different.The purpose of this task was to rule out the possibility that at least a propor-tion of the errors produced by DB in repetition were due to an auditorydiscrimination impairment. The pairs for the nonmatching trials were, there-fore, constructed keeping in mind the speech errors produced by the patient.There were 430 matching pairs and 430 nonmatching pairs. Of the nonmatch-ing pairs, 180 pairs differed by a single phoneme in the root (100 on a vowel,and 80 on a consonant) and 250 pairs differed by a single phoneme in thesuffix (100 on a vowel and 150 on a consonant). Again, DB’s performancewas very good. Overall, he was 97% (836/860) correct. He was also 97%(417/430) correct on the nonmatching trials. All the errors were made whenthe two words differed by a consonant: four errors were made when thedifferent consonant was in the root and nine errors when the different conso-nant was in the suffix. Two normal controls matched to DB for age andeducation were 99 and 97% correct, respectively.

Phonological Short-Term Memory

DB showed signs of a reduced phonological STM. He repeated correctly76% (34/45) of single words, but only 60% (6/10) of two-word lists and0% (0/10) of three-word lists. With nonwords, he repeated correctly 83%single nonwords (15/18), 30% (3/10) of two-nonword lists, and 0% (0/10)of three-nonword lists. DB was also tested on probe recognition tasks. A listof either words or nonwords was read at the rate of about one per secondfollowed by a probe (word or nonword depending on the type of list). Thetask was to say whether or not the probe appeared in the preceding list. Allserial positions were probed. DB was 92% (22/24) correct with four-wordlists, 79% (19/42) correct with six-word lists, and 87% (21/24) correct witheight-word lists. With nonwords, he was 79% (19/24) correct with four-nonword lists and 75% (18/24) correct with six-nonword lists. In all condi-tions, DB’s performance is outside the normal range.

Reading and Writing

DB was asked to read and write to dictation words from a number of listsin order to assess effects of frequency, length, lexicality, concreteness, andgrammatical class. Different lists were used for reading and writing. In read-ing, DB showed a modest frequency effect, but no length effect. In writing,on the other hand, there was no effect of frequency, but a modest lengtheffect. Both reading and writing showed a lexicality effect, performance be-

Page 13: Syllabic Constraints in the Phonological Errors of an ...€¦ · degree of complexity to the basic CV template. Following this principle, one can derive the following ranking of

PHONOLOGICAL ERRORS AND APHASIA 95

ing better with words than with nonwords. Neither task showed a part ofspeech effect. In reading and writing, as well as in repetition (described be-low), the great majority of errors consisted of substitutions, deletions, inser-tions, and transpositions of single segments and the most common error wasthe substitution of a single phonological feature in a single phonologicalsegment.

EXPERIMENTAL INVESTIGATION

General Characteristics of the Errors

The general characteristics of DB’s speech errors were unaltered acrosslist types and to a lesser extent across different tasks. The same kinds oferrors were made in spontaneous speech, picture naming, and to a lesserdegree in reading and writing. All the analyses reported in this paper, how-ever, were carried out on speech errors produced during repetition.

In total, DB was given a very large corpus of words to repeat (7552 words).These words came from different lists. Some of them belonged to a morphol-ogy battery (Miceli & Capasso, 1985) that was administered to DB twice,once in 1987 and once in 1988. Others were devised to explore in moredetail the repetition deficit shown by DB and were administered in 1989.These latter lists had a higher proportion of words with difficult syllabicstructures (henceforth the ‘‘phonology lists’’). All of DB’s production wastape-recorded. DB’s responses were scored by one of the two authors andspot-checked by the other. Both authors, like the patient, are native speakersof Italian. Performance was very similar on the two presentations of themorphology lists, except for fewer deletions on the second presentation. Per-formance was also similar on the phonology lists except for more deletions.

The great majority of DB’s errors result in a phonologically related non-word and can be categorized as substitutions, deletions, insertions, and trans-positions of single phonemes. Table 3 shows overall the proportions of thedifferent kinds of errors made by DB. Among single- and two-phoneme er-rors, 264 resulted in a morphologically related word and 54 in a morphologi-cally unrelated word. The most parsimonious explanation of these errors isthat they are phonological errors like the others which result in another wordeither by chance or because lexical factors influence which phoneme is se-lected to replace an unavailable phoneme (see Romani, Semenza, and Grana’(1998b) for relevant evidence and discussion). The great majority of DB’smorphological errors were single vowel substitutions (226/264). Since allwords in Italian are inflected, a single vowel substitution at the end of theword has a very high chance of resulting in an inflectional error. At any rate,because it is possible that these errors have a morphological basis, they willbe excluded from our analysis of DB’s vowel substitutions.

Page 14: Syllabic Constraints in the Phonological Errors of an ...€¦ · degree of complexity to the basic CV template. Following this principle, one can derive the following ranking of

96 ROMANI AND CALABRESE

TABLE 3Proportions of Different Kinds of Errors Made by DB

N %

Single phonemeSubstitutions 812 47.0Deletions 291 16.1Insertions 98 5.4Transpositions 75 4.2

Two phonemesDeletion 1 substitution 44 2.4Insertion 1 substitution 7 0.4Insertion 1 deletion 4 0.2Transposition 1 substitution 5 0.3Transposition 1 deletion 2 0.1Deletion 1 deletion 2 0.1Substitution 1 substitution 47 2.6Insertion 1 insertion 3 0.2

Total (one or two phonemes) 1390 80Adjacent segments

Double substitutions 91 5.1Double deletions 29 1.6Double insertions 13 0.7Double transpositions 1 0.1

Multiple phonemes 126 7.0Complex morphological errors 37 2.1Fragments 39 2.2Omissions 2 0.1Total 1728 100

Other Effects on DB’s Errors

Lexical effects. On the first presentation of the Morphology Battery, DBshowed no part of speech effect: he repeated correctly 75% of nouns (113/150), 73% of adjectives (51/70), and 62% of verbs (31/50) (χ2 (2) 5 3.3;p 5 .18). He also did not show a strong lexicality effect. He repeated cor-rectly 78% of words (1778/2270) and 73% of nonwords (145/200) (cont.adj. χ2 (1) 5 3.3; p 5 .07). A significant effect, however, might be obtainedusing better controlled lists. A frequency effect also did not reach signifi-cance at this time. He repeated correctly 75% of high frequency words (113/150) and 66% of low frequency words (73/110) (cont. adj. χ2 (1) 5 2.1;p 5 .15; frequencies following Bortolini, Tavaglini, and Zampolli (1972)).A frequency effect did emerge, however, when we considered the wholecorpus of DB’s errors and looked at the frequency of words repeated cor-rectly and incorrectly. Overall the mean frequency of words repeated cor-rectly was 59.9, the mean frequency of words repeated incorrectly was 19.8(t 5 5.9; p , .0001; word frequencies calculated using the Bercellona Cor-pus (1994)—1,500,000 words—which incorporates Bortolini et al. (1972)).

Page 15: Syllabic Constraints in the Phonological Errors of an ...€¦ · degree of complexity to the basic CV template. Following this principle, one can derive the following ranking of

PHONOLOGICAL ERRORS AND APHASIA 97

Length. DB showed a quite clear length effect. Depending on the numberof phonemes in the word, his proportion of correctly repeated words was asfollows: 4–5 phonemes, 94% (143/152); 6–7 phonemes, 91% (607/669);8–9 phonemes, 77% (725/947); 10–11 phonemes, 62% (230/372); 12–14phonemes, 39% (226/66) (χ2 (4) 5 215; p , .0001). Considering only mor-phologically simple words with a CV–CV structure (N 5 248), the meanlength of words repeated correctly was 6.8 phonemes; the mean length ofwords repeated incorrectly was 8.5 phonemes.

Positional effects. We looked at DB’s rate of substitution and deletionerrors at the beginning, middle, and end of words.3 Including morphologicalerrors, DB made 133, 381, and 448 substitutions in initial, medial, and finalpositions, respectively. The same tendency is present with deletion errors.There were 57 deletions in initial position, 134 in medial position, and 153in final position. Excluding morphological errors, 197 substitutions weremade in word-final position (deletions never resulted in a morphologicallyrelated word). Thus, even excluding morphological errors, DB makes moreerrors at the end than at the beginning of a word.

The analyses presented in the rest of the paper will be carried out on DB’sphonological errors involving one or two phonemes. Since the unit of analy-ses will be individual errors, errors involving two phonemes will be consid-ered as two distinct errors. These analyses will involve looking at the propor-tion of DB’s errors on different syllabic configurations. Given the largenumber of words given to DB, the number of targets will be generally verylarge and independent factors, such as frequency and length, are likely tobalance out. More importantly, there is no evidence that in Italian (or in anyother language) the distribution of syllable types varies depending on wordfrequency or word length. There is also no evidence that the distribution ofsyllable types varies with word position if one excludes the very beginningand the very end of a word (e.g., Italian generally does not allow closedsyllables at the end of words). Thus, it is very unlikely that DB’s differenterror rates on different syllable types can be accounted for in terms of wordfrequency, length, or positional effects.

Syllabic Constraints on Deletion Errors

Onsets

Any theory of syllabic complexity predicts that deletion errors should oc-cur very rarely on simple onsets and should concentrate, instead, on complex

3 Syllable positions have been collapsed into initial, medial, and final positions as follows:

Two-syllable words 1i 2f

Three-syllable words 1i 2m 3f

Four-syllable words 1i 2–3m 4f

Five-syllable words 1–2i 3m 4–5f

Page 16: Syllabic Constraints in the Phonological Errors of an ...€¦ · degree of complexity to the basic CV template. Following this principle, one can derive the following ranking of

98 ROMANI AND CALABRESE

onsets and codas, thus reducing these more complex configurations to themost simple CV template. According to the SDP, however, syllables alsovary in complexity depending on the sonority relation among segments.Therefore, we should expect deletions to increase progressively, within bothsimple and complex onsets, according to the complexity ranking describedabove. For complex onsets, moreover, we should expect that errors shouldconcentrate on those segments whose deletion maximizes the difference insonority between the remaining consonant and the vowel since this wouldresult in a syllable with a lower complexity ranking (e.g., for OGV . OVand not . GV since OV is better than GV). Finally, if DB’s errors dependon a sonority-based notion of syllabic complexity, we should expect fewerrors with complex onsets of the type /s/-obstruent. There is general agree-ment, in fact, that these configurations are not part of the inventories of com-plex onsets of any language since they violate the fundamental principle thatsonority should increase from the margin of the syllable to the peak (e.g.,Cairns & Feinstein, 1982; Harris, 1983).

Results. DB deletes 0.01% (21/20111) consonants in simple onset com-pared to 9.7% (249/2560) consonants in complex onset (continuity adjustedχ2 5 1787; p , .0001). Table 4 reports DB’s percentage of deletions onsyllable types ranked according to the SDP. Deletion rates follow a sonority-based hierarchy of syllable types remarkably well for both simple and com-plex onsets. In simple onsets, the ranking is what is predicted by the SDP.The probability of obtaining this by chance is 1/24 (p , .05). In complexbisegmental onsets, there is only one exception to the SDP ranking: DBmakes fewer errors on fricative-/y/ onsets than on affricative-y onsets, whilethe SDP predicts the opposite. The correlation between the ranking predictedby the SDP and the one obtained in DB is very high and highly significant(Kendal tau coefficient 5 .93; z 5 3.2; p , .001). All individual differencesbetween major syllable types are also highly significant. Fewer deletions aremade on OLV than on OyV syllables (cont. adj. χ2 5 24; p , .0001), onOyV than on NyV syllables (cont. adj. χ2 5 6.9; p , .001), and on NyVthan on LyV syllables (cont. adj. χ2 5 15; p , .0001).4

4 In Table 4 we have included palatalizations errors. In a palatalization, a consonant followedby a glide is substituted by a single palatalized consonant (e.g., /attornyare/ . /attorar®are/).The inclusion of palatalization, therefore, is motivated by the fact that these errors involveboth the deletions and the substitution of a segment. At any rate, very similar results areobtained by excluding palatalization as shown below and the correlation between DB’s dele-tion rates and the ranking predicted by the SPD remains highly significant (Kendal tau coeffi-cient with ties 5 .65; z 5 2.28; p 5 .01):

Complex bisegmental onsets, excluding palatalization

1 OlV 1.7 2/1152 OrV 2.3 24/10463 stop-y-V 3.8 15/3914 affricate-y-V 5.8 18/311

Page 17: Syllabic Constraints in the Phonological Errors of an ...€¦ · degree of complexity to the basic CV template. Following this principle, one can derive the following ranking of

PHONOLOGICAL ERRORS AND APHASIA 99

Table 4 also shows which segments are deleted by DB in complex onsets.Excluding palatalizations in which both segments are affected, in an OLVsyllable, the segment deleted most often is the liquid (liquids 5 22; ob-struents 5 2); in an OyV syllable, it is the glide (glides 5 32; obstruents 52); in a NyV syllable, again it is the glide (glides 5 4; obstruents 5 0);finally, in a LyV, it is the liquid (liquids 5 120; glides 12). Thus, in threeof four cases DB deletes the segment of higher sonority and produces theleast marked syllable.5

Finally, it has to be noted that DB makes only one error in the case ofcomplex onsets involving /s/ (/skyu.meg.gi/ . /sku.meg.gi/). This is in spiteof 391 onsets of this type present in our corpus. The exception is strikinggiven DB’s tendency to simplify complex onsets in all other cases. We haveseen that phonological theory places these onsets outside the domain of appli-cation of all sonority principles (e.g., Harris, 1983). DB’s lack of errors con-firms their special status.

An alternative explanation? The majority of DB’s consonant deletionsinvolve segments of high sonority (238/301 5 79% of errors involve liquidsand glides). The sonority of the segments per se, however, cannot fully ac-count for the error pattern since error probability is strongly influenced bycontext. For example, 0.4% of /r/’s deletions (12/2695) occur in simple on-sets, but 2.2% occur in complex onsets (22/1046) (cont. adj. χ2 5 231;p , .0001). Equally, absolute differences in sonority, without reference tosyllabic context, cannot totally account for DB’s errors. We have compareddeletion rates on bisegmental clusters where the two consonants are tautosyl-labic (they belong to the same syllable; e.g., pa.dre) and where they areheterosyllabic (they belong to different syllables; e.g., pan.da). DB makessignificantly more deletions on tautosyllabic clusters (249/2560 5 9.7%)

5 fricative-y-V 5.5 9/1636 NyV 3.8 4/1047 lyV 17.3 7/528 ryV 36.9 127/344

5 One way of accounting for the different pattern on rG-onsets (deletions concentrate on/r/ which is less sonorous than the glide) is to hypothesize that there are two procedures ofsyllabic simplification. The first procedure simplifies onsets by deleting the most sonoroussegment. In this way the difference in sonority between the remaining consonant and the vowelis maximized and a better syllable is produced. The second procedure applies only when thereis little difference in sonority between the consonants and simplifies onsets by deleting thesegment that is further from the peak. Examples of this second procedure can be found in thehistory of languages. For example, in English, the phoneme /p/ has disappeared from wordslike ‘‘psychology’’ or ‘‘pneumonia.’’ /p/ could have been deleted in these cases because /p/and /s/ or /p/ and /n/ are close in sonority and /p/ is the segment further from the peak. InrGV syllables, DB would apply the second simplification procedure since the sonority of /r/is close to that of the following glide (for a theoretical foundation of simplification proceduressee Calabrese, (1995)).

Page 18: Syllabic Constraints in the Phonological Errors of an ...€¦ · degree of complexity to the basic CV template. Following this principle, one can derive the following ranking of

100 ROMANI AND CALABRESE

TABLE 4Percentages of Deletion Errors on Syllable Types Ranked from Least to Most Complex

According to Clements’ Hierarchy

% N err./

Errors N tar. Segments deleted

Simple onsets

1. OV 0.02 3/12909 t 5 1; f 5 1; e 5 1

2. NV 0.1 2/2703 n 5 2

3. LV 0.3 13/3973 r 5 12; l 5 1

4. yV 3.1 3/98 y 5 3

bisegmental onsets

1. OLV 2.1 24/1161

OlV 1.7 1/115 l 5 1; f 5 0

OrV 2.2 23/1046 r 5 21; f 5 2

2. OyV 7.0 61/865

stop 5.4 21/391 y 5 13; stop 5 2; palatalizations 5 6

affricate 10.3 32/311 y 5 19; affricate 5 0; palataliza-

tions 5 13

fricative 4.9 8/163 y 5 8; fricative 5 0

3. NyV 16.3 17/104 y 5 4; nasals 5 0; palatalizations 5 13

4. LyV 34.4 136/396

lyV 17.3 9/52 y 5 7; palatalizations 5 2

ryV 36.9 127/344 y 5 5; r 5 120

trisegmental onsets

5. OLyV 33.0 7/21 y 5 3; r 5 3; V 5 1

Onsets involving /w/a

1. wV 0 0/4 w 5 4

2. OwV 3.1 5/159 w 5 5

3. Labiovelar-w-V 1.1 3/269 w 5 3

4. Labiovelar-y-w-V 30.1 4/13 y 5 4

Codas

1 VG 5.0 18/334

Vy 3.8 6/156

Vw 6.7 12/178

2. VL. 0.4 4/1095

Vr 0.5 4/852

Vl 0.0 0/243

3. VN 0.2 4/1627

4 Vs. 0.0 0/717

VO (geminate) 0.6 14/2267

Note. 1 5 least complex; 4 5 most complex.a We report onsets involving the glide /w/ separately since there are reasons to believe that

in Italian /w/ is part of the nucleus and not of the onset of the syllable when it is in prevocalicposition (see Marotta, 1988).

Page 19: Syllabic Constraints in the Phonological Errors of an ...€¦ · degree of complexity to the basic CV template. Following this principle, one can derive the following ranking of

PHONOLOGICAL ERRORS AND APHASIA 101

than on heterosyllabic clusters (8/3439 5 .2%, all in coda position; cont.adj. χ2 5 327; p , .0001). One may object that the tautosyllabic sequencesare more susceptible to errors because they include more consonants withsimilar levels of sonority. To deal with this point, we have carried out asecond analysis restricted to clusters where /r/ and an obstruent are flankedby vowels. We have contrasted cases where /r/ and the obstruent are in thesame syllable with cases where they belong to different syllables (e.g.,a.tro.ce vs. ar.ca.no). As before, many more deletions occur in the case oftautosyllabic clusters than in the case of heterosyllabic clusters (V.OrV, 12/158 5 7.5%; Vr.OV, 3/578 5 .5%; Fisher exact test; p , .0001). Since thedifference in sonority between adjacent sounds is exactly the same in thesetwo conditions, these results confirm that DB has trouble with particularsyllabic configurations and not simply with sequences of sounds of similarsonority.

Codas

According to Clements’ SDP, sonority dispersion is preferred in the initialpart of the syllable up to the peak, but not in the final part of the syllable,from the peak on. Thus, unlike onsets, codas are less complex when theycontain sonorants (liquids and glides) and more complex when they containnasals and obstruents. Tables 2 and 4 show a possible ranking of Italiancodas. As already discussed, however, obstruent codas are possible in Italianonly in the case of /s/ or of a geminate and these are likely to be specialcases. It is unclear, moreover, whether, in Italian, a glide in coda is anydifferent than a full vowel. There is no distinction in terms of segmentalcontent between high vowels (/i/ and /u/) and the corresponding glides(/y/ and /w/). The main difference is stress and duration: glides are vowelsproduced in a reduced form. In Italian, nobody doubts the existence of glidesin onsets, but there are doubts as to whether they exist in codas since experi-mental evidence shows that the duration of high vocoids (/i/, /u/) is shortenedin onset, but not in coda (Marotta) (1988); see also Burani and Cafiero (1991)for consistent experimental results). To a certain extent, whether a high vo-coid is produced as a glide or as a vowel depends on personal style andspeech rate (a vowel may be pronounced as a glide in fast speech). GivenDB’s low speech rate, there are good reasons to consider VG sequences asno different than sequences of two vowels (hiatuses).

Results. DB makes 40/6040 5 .7% deletion errors in coda. This is signifi-cantly more errors than in simple onset (cont. adj. χ2 5 62; p , .0001),adding to the evidence that DB prefers simpler syllable types. Errors do notincrease according to the SDP. Given the discussion above, however, theonly segments clearly in codas are liquids and nasals. DB shows no signifi-cant difference between the two, but this could be due to the paucity of theerrors.

Page 20: Syllabic Constraints in the Phonological Errors of an ...€¦ · degree of complexity to the basic CV template. Following this principle, one can derive the following ranking of

102 ROMANI AND CALABRESE

Vowel Deletions and Syllable Contacts

Vowels constitute an integral part of a syllable. Their deletion, therefore,would generally result, not only in an increase of syllabic complexity, butalso in a violation of phonotactic constraints. There are two contexts, how-ever, where vowels can be deleted: in the case of a vowel sequence (a hiatus,e.g., po.e.si.a) or in the case of a vowel in word-initial position (e.g., a.mo.re).Sonority has been used to define not only complexity within syllables, butalso complexity of sequences which occur across a syllable boundary. Ac-cording to ‘‘The Syllable Contact Law,’’ a syllable contact is preferred tothe extent that the first segment outranks the second in sonority (e.g., ar.nais better than an.ra, because /r/ is more sonorous than /n/; see Hooper (1976),Murray and Vennemann (1983), and Vennemann (1988)). This is consistentwith the fact that sonorous consonants are preferred in coda: they create ahigh sonority contrast with the onset of the following syllable. If DB’s speechis affected by sonority-based principles of complexity, therefore, we shouldexpect a high rate of vowel deletions on vowel sequences (hiatuses) wherethere is no sonority difference at all between adjacent syllables.

What to expect in the case of vowels in word-beginning position is moreunclear. On the one hand, single vowel syllables are clearly marked com-pared to CV syllables. While all languages allow CV sequences (and, in fact,some allow only CV sequences), single vowel syllables are not allowed bymany languages. In fact, for perceptual and articulatory reasons, it is difficultto conceive of a language that would consist only of vowels. However, theprohibition against single vowel syllables often applies only word-internally,in the context of a hiatus (V.V), but not word-initially (see Guerssel, 1986;and Dell & Elmedlaui, 1985, for examples and discussion). In word-initialposition, a segment does not compete for sonority with any preceding seg-ment. This may actually make high sonority segments in general, and vowelsin particular, preferred in this position.

Results. DB makes 26 vowel deletions, 44 if we consider glides in codato be full vowels as argued above (e.g., /a.u.ste.ro/ and not /aw.ste.ro/). Withtwo exceptions, DB never deletes a vowel in a syllable with an onset. Ninety-one percent (40/44) of DB’s vowel deletions occur in the context of a hiatus(e.g., /ri.or.di.na/ . /ror.di .na/) and only two vowel deletions occur inword-initial position. This is in spite of the large number of vowel-initialwords in our corpus (N 5 1763) and in Italian in general. A difference be-tween vowel deletions in hiatus and in word-initial position is highly signifi-cant even when we control for word position by considering only hiatuseswhich occur at the beginning of the word (between first and second syllable).DB makes 4.7% (14/344) deletions in the context of a hiatus and 0.1%(2/1763) deletions in word-initial position (cont.adj. χ2 5 55; p , .0001).DB’s insertions show the same trend: in 35 cases, a consonant is insertedbetween two vowels to eliminate a hiatus (e.g., /bal.la.i/ . /bal.la.ri/), while

Page 21: Syllabic Constraints in the Phonological Errors of an ...€¦ · degree of complexity to the basic CV template. Following this principle, one can derive the following ranking of

PHONOLOGICAL ERRORS AND APHASIA 103

in only one case is a consonant inserted before a vowel in word-initial posi-tion (/un.De.re/ . /pun.De.re/). These results show that DB has a problemwith single vowel syllables only when they occur in the context of a hiatus.Thus, they support the hypothesis that hiatuses are difficult because of thenull difference in sonority between adjacent vowels and not because theyinvolve a single vowel syllable. In word-initial position, in fact, single vowelsyllables are unproblematic.

Deletion rates on different vowels will be discussed later on together withthe substitution errors.

Conclusions

DB’s deletion errors in onset clearly support the existence of hierarchiesof syllable types derived from the SDP. Not only are more errors made incomplex than in simple onsets, but, within both simple and complex onsets,deletion rates increase progressively the more the segments are close in so-nority to one another. The only complex onsets on which DB makes almostno errors are those involving /s/-O which are outside the SDP. DB’s voweldeletions are also influenced by sonority contrasts: they occur when twovowels are in sequence, but not in word-initial position. Deletions in codado not conform to the SDP, but coda positions offer few contrasts and thereare few data points. This issue will be raised again in the General Discussion.

Syllabic Constraints on Substitution Errors

The largest category of errors made by DB is substitutions. It is important,therefore, to establish whether substitutions, like deletions, are conditionedby the SDP. DB makes 962 substitutions overall. The majority (642/962 567%) are on consonants. Error rates are respectively 2.0% (642/30776) forconsonants and 1.2% (320/26998) for vowels (cont. adj. χ2 5 327; p ,.0001). Consonants are replaced with consonants and vowels with vowels(with only three exceptions). Seventy-eight percent (503/642) of consonantsubstitutions involve a single phonological feature. These characteristicshave been noted before in the errors of aphasics (see Blumstein, 1973, 1978).Moreover, in our corpus, glides are occasionally replaced by liquids (N 56), and vice versa (N 5 4) (e.g., /miʎʎa.ya/ (thousands) . /miʎʎa.ra/;/di.lu.vyo/ (storm) . /di.yu.vyo/). This shows that the syllabic structure ofthe target constrains the nature of the substitution.

Consonant Substitutions

Nature of substitutions. The SDP states that the greater the sonority disper-sion in the onset the better the syllable, while the opposite holds for codas.According to the SDP, therefore, we should expect consonant substitutionsin simple onsets to decrease sonority since this maximizes the differencewith the following vowel. The same trend should be expected in complex

Page 22: Syllabic Constraints in the Phonological Errors of an ...€¦ · degree of complexity to the basic CV template. Following this principle, one can derive the following ranking of

104 ROMANI AND CALABRESE

TABLE 5Outcome of DB’s Consonant Substitutions in Terms Syllabic Complexity

Outcome % of Substitutions N

Simple onsetsDecrease sonority 1 68 334/490Increase sonority 2 22 109/490Neutral 5 10 47/490

Complex onsetsDecrease Sonority 1 41 37/90Increase Sonority 2 19 17/90Neutral 5 40 36/90

CodasDecrease Sonority 2 47 8/17Increase Sonority 1 47 8/17Neutral 5 6 1/17

Note. Plusses and minuses indicate a positive or a negative outcome interms of the SDP.

onsets, provided that the error occurs on the most peripheral consonant, sincethis again maximizes sonority dispersion. This trend, however, should notbe present in codas.

Relative rate of substitutions. Blumstein (1978) has reported that, unlikedeletions, substitutions occur mostly on single onsets. However, she has con-sidered absolute numbers and not error rates. We would expect substitutionsto occur in both contexts, since they can increase the ‘‘goodness’’ of a sylla-ble in either cases. The relative error rate is more difficult to predict. Gener-ally, we would expect more errors in complex onsets. However, DB alreadymakes many deletions in this context which may keep the number of substitu-tions down. Finally, Italian does not allow complex codas and, for simplecodas we have argued that it allows only liquids and nasals. Therefore, fewsubstitutions should be made on codas since liquids and nasals are alreadyhigh-sonority segments which are optimal in this position.

Results. DB makes about the same rate of substitution errors in simpleonsets (490/20115 5 2.4%) and complex onsets (90/2560 5 3.5%), butsignificantly fewer substitutions in codas (17/5706 5 0.3%; cont. adj. χ2 5101 and 87; p , .001) (geminate substitutions are not included since theirsyllabic position is ambiguous). The proportion of errors resulting in compli-cations versus simplifications of syllabic structures according to SDP arereported in Table 5. It is to be noted that all of DB’s substitutions in complexonsets involve the external consonants (glides and liquids in complex onsetsare eliminated through deletion).

As predicted by the SDP, DB makes significantly more errors that decreasethan increase sonority in both simple onsets (χ2 5 61; p , .001) and complexonsets (χ2 5 3.9; p 5 .05). While substitutions decrease sonority in onsets,

Page 23: Syllabic Constraints in the Phonological Errors of an ...€¦ · degree of complexity to the basic CV template. Following this principle, one can derive the following ranking of

PHONOLOGICAL ERRORS AND APHASIA 105

they do not in codas. This last result is also consistent with what is predictedby SDP, but it should be taken with caution because of the small numberof the errors. The fact that high sonority segments are preferred in coda,however, is also supported by the fact that DB makes significantly moresubstitutions on these segments when in onset than in coda. He makes 2.2%(88/3973) liquid substitutions in onset versus 0.6% (7/1095) liquid substitu-tions in coda (Fisher exact test; p , .001). Equally, he makes 1.4% (40/2703) nasal substitutions in onsets versus .3% (5/1627) nasal substitutionsin coda (Fisher exact test; p , .001).

It has to be noted that many of DB’s errors which decrease sonority inonset are devoicing errors (DB makes 222 devoicing errors versus 41 voicingerrors). Since these errors are often made by patients with articulatory diffi-culties (e.g., Blumstein, Cooper, Goodglass, Statlender, & Gottlieb, 1980),one could argue that while DB’s pattern is a consequence of articulatorydifficulties, it is not related to contrasts in sonority. DB’s onset substitutions,however, show the same simplification pattern even when one excludes er-rors involving 1/2 voicing: 63% (148) of the errors result in a less sonoroussegment and only 37% (87) in a more sonorous segment (χ2 5 8.0; p ,.005).

Finally, one should note that DB makes no substitutions of /s/ in coda(0/717), paralleling the lack of errors in the case of deletions. We can offerthe same explanation, namely, that /s/ in coda is syllabified by a special rulewhich makes no reference to the SDP.

Syllabic versus Phonological Markedness

Previous studies have reported that substitution rates are determined bythe markedness or frequency of the segment (markedness and frequency aretwo largely overlapping concepts). Some studies have shown that aphasicpatients tend to replace more marked (complex) segments with less markedsegments (Jakobson, 1941; Blumstein, 1973; Klich, Ireland, & Weidner,1979). One study has shown that more substitutions occur on segments thatare less frequent in the language (Ferreres, 1990). An important question toaddress, therefore, is whether syllabic complexity, segmental complexity, orboth play a role in determining DB’s substitution errors.

Segmental markedness. It is commonly assumed not only that combina-tions of segments (syllables) differ in complexity, but also that segments inisolation have an intrinsic complexity (Kean, 1981; Basbøll, 1981; Maddie-son, 1984; Eckman, Moravcsik, & Wirth, 1986; Calabrese, 1995). The crite-ria used by linguistics to establish segmental complexity are quite similarto those used to establish syllabic complexity, that is, frequency within andbetween languages, age of acquisition, and, most importantly, implicationaluniversals: if a marked segment (or class of segments) is present in a lan-guage, the corresponding unmarked segment (or class of segments) should

Page 24: Syllabic Constraints in the Phonological Errors of an ...€¦ · degree of complexity to the basic CV template. Following this principle, one can derive the following ranking of

106 ROMANI AND CALABRESE

also be present (see, Greenberg, 1978). As was the case for syllabic complex-ity, one can imagine that the least marked segments are those which areperceptually/articulatorily easier.

Contrary to what happens with syllables, however, segments cannot beorganized into a hierarchy of complexity. Phonemes are seen by phonologistsas bundles of binary features. Markedness is established in terms of whichof the binary values of a feature creates a less marked combination in thecontext of the other features in the bundle. Thus, to establish the absolutemarkedness of segments is not possible. It is possible to establish only therelative markedness of two features given the same contexts. Contrary towhat happens for syllables, moreover, there is not a single principle like theSDP which defines segmental markedness, and empirical criteria are some-times in disagreement with one another. For example, /r/ is usually acquiredlate in language development and after /l/ (e.g., Jakobson, 1941, 1968;Smith, 1973). However, /r/ does not seem to be less widely distributed acrosslanguages than /l/ (Greenberg, 1978). For the sake of clarity, a listing of thefeatures which we consider marked in Italian, with their relative contexts,is provided in Appendix A.

Results. Substitutions involving multiple features which went in differentdirections were scored as ambiguous (N 5 15). Three neutral errors werealso excluded. Leaving aside voicing/devoicing errors, 196 (55%) of substi-tutions result in a less marked segment and 163 (45%) result in a moremarked segment (χ2 5 1.5; ns). This provides some evidence that syllabiccomplexity is more important than segmental markedness in constrainingDB’s errors. It is to be noted that, in most cases, the outcome of substitutionscoincide in terms of segmental and syllabic markedness. Most substitutionsoccur in simple onsets where less sonorous segments are not only lessmarked in terms of syllabic complexity, but they are also generally lessmarked in terms of segmental complexity. Two other considerations,however, also suggest that syllabic markedness is a more important factorthan segmental markedness for DB. Segmental markedness predicts the samerate of substitutions in onset and in coda while syllabic markedness predictsdifferent error rates since segments that are optimal in onset are not favoredin coda and vice versa. We have already seen that DB makes more substitu-tions on liquids and nasals in onset than in coda. A second considerationinvolves the palatalization errors that have been included in Table 4. Theseerrors involve both a deletion and a substitution since the first consonantof a complex onset and the following glide are substituted by a singlepalatalized consonant (e.g., /stat.tsyo.ne/ . /stat.tʃone/, /at.tor.nya.re/ ./at.tor.®a.re/). These ‘‘merging’’ substitutions produce a segment which ismore marked than the target. Thus, when in conflict, the need for a simplersyllabic structure wins over the need for a simpler segmental configuration.Notably, DB makes 30 errors of this type, while there are only four casesof the opposite kind of error (e.g., /proʃ.ʃut.to/ . /pros.syut.to/).

Page 25: Syllabic Constraints in the Phonological Errors of an ...€¦ · degree of complexity to the basic CV template. Following this principle, one can derive the following ranking of

PHONOLOGICAL ERRORS AND APHASIA 107

TABLE 6Percentages of Onset Substitutions That Decrease or Increase

Sonority Depending on Heterosyllabic Contexts

Sonority

Decreases IncreasesNo change

Context N % N % N

# C 13 31 29 69 12V. C 163 70 69 30 46L. C 2 40 3 60 7N. C 174 88 24 12 1s. C 17 85 3 15 10

Syllable Contacts

We have reported that deletions reduce not only the complexity of syllabletypes but also the complexity of syllable contacts. Do substitutions have thesame effect? We have seen that the best contact is one where the differencein sonority is the greatest. The need for decreasing the sonority of a conso-nant in onset, therefore, should depend, in part, on the preceding segment.This need should be minimal if there is no preceding segment at all, as inword-initial position, since in this case there is no sonority competition, itshould be greatest in the case where the preceding segment is a consonant,and it should be intermediate in the case where the preceding segment is avowel.

Results. Table 6 reports the proportion of substitutions which decreaseversus increase sonority depending on the preceding segment. As predicted,the proportion of errors that decrease sonority is higher when the segmentis preceded by a consonant than when it is preceded by a vowel (86% vs.70%; χ2 5 17.7; p , .0001). The same tendency is not found with liquids,but this is not surprising given the small number of errors (it is unclear,however, why DB makes so few errors in this condition). Also, as predicted,the proportion of errors that decrease sonority is higher when the segmentis preceded by a vowel than when it is in word-initial position (70% vs. 31%;χ2 5 23.9; p , .0001). Here, in fact, there is a reverse of the tendency withmore errors which increase rather than decrease sonority. Although this lastresult is not a direct prediction of the SDP, it is consistent with it and withour previous observation that DB makes very few errors on vowels in word-initial position. As argued, it is plausible that high sonority segments arefavored in this position because they provide a better contrast between si-lence and speech.

Page 26: Syllabic Constraints in the Phonological Errors of an ...€¦ · degree of complexity to the basic CV template. Following this principle, one can derive the following ranking of

108 ROMANI AND CALABRESE

TABLE 7DB’s Rates of Substitution and Deletion Errors on Different Vowels

Target Substitutions Deletions Total

Type N N % N % N %

a 7626 9 0.1 1 0.01 10 0.1e 6784 36 0.5 13 0.2 49 0.7o 6067 18 0.3 5 0.1 28 0.4i 5266 18 0.3 13 0.2 31 0.6u 1255 13 1.0 12 1.0 25 1.9

Note. Vowels are ranked from the most to the least sonorous according to Goldsmith’s(1990) sonority ranking.

Conclusions

Sonority plays a crucial role in determining DB’s consonant substitutionsas it did for his deletions. In agreement with the SDP, most substitutions inonset, but not in coda, reduce sonority. As we found for deletions, moreover,there is evidence that the errors are motivated more by the complexity ofthe syllabic contexts in terms of sonority than by the complexity of the seg-ments per se. Finally, as for deletions, the nature of substitutions is alsoaffected by context, with a tendency to increase the contrast in sonority withthe preceding segment.

Vowel Errors (Substitutions and Deletions)

Clements (1990) does not discuss vowel sonority. Moreover, given theiroverall high sonority, differences in sonority among vowels are likely to beless strong than differences among consonants. According to Chomsky andHalle (1968), for example, /a/ is the only vowel that can be distinguishedfrom the others in terms of sonority and markedness: it is both the mostsonorous and the least marked (see also Calabrese, 1995). However, if, fol-lowing Clements, differences in sonority are preferred for the first part ofthe syllable (onset plus nucleus), but not for the second part of the syllable(nucleus plus coda), then high sonority vowels should be generally preferred.A high sonority nucleus, in fact, maximizes the difference in sonority withthe preceding consonant.

Results. DB makes only 94 morphologically unrelated vowel substitutions.If we look at the number of vowel–vowel substitutions which increase ordecrease sonority there is no overall difference (41 vs. 51 errors), but thisis probably due to lack of sonority differences between intermediate vowels.Table 7 shows deletion and substitution errors on vowels ranked for sonority.DB makes the least number of errors on /a/—which is both the most sono-rous and the least marked vowel (all differences with other vowels are sig-

Page 27: Syllabic Constraints in the Phonological Errors of an ...€¦ · degree of complexity to the basic CV template. Following this principle, one can derive the following ranking of

PHONOLOGICAL ERRORS AND APHASIA 109

nificant; χ2 5 90 2 12; p , .001). On the other hand, he makes the mosterrors on /u/—which is the least sonorous vowel (all differences with othervowels are significant; χ2 5 90 2 17; p , .001). All other differences arenot significant. Thus, at the extreme ends of the scale, DB’s vowel errors areconsistent with his general tendency to maximize the difference in sonoritybetween a consonant in the onset and the following vowel.

Syllabic Constraints on Transposition Errors

It is difficult to make predictions for transpositions in terms of the SDPsince it is difficult to use these errors to simplify syllable structure. Becauseof this, we expect transpositions not to be very common among patients witharticulatory difficulties (see, Canter, Trost, & Burns, 1985). It is possible,however, that transpositions may concentrate on words with difficult syllabicconfigurations, thus still revealing a difficulty with these structures.

We considered an error to be a transposition if either a segment was movedto a different position in the word (e.g., /benyamino/ . /byenamimo/) ortwo segments exchanged positions (e.g., /serale/ . /selare/). DB makesvery few errors of this kind. Only 5.4% (82/1504) of DB’s single phonemeerrors are transpositions. Of these errors, 93% (76/82) do not change theoverall complexity of the word, 7% (6/82) result in a simpler structure, and0% result in a more complex structure. A good portion of transpositions dochange structure, but they do not result in simplifications because a complexstructure (a complex onset, a coda or a hiatus) is moved from one positionin the word to another or because a complex structure is substituted withanother complex structure. Although they are very few, these last errors sug-gest that hiatuses are harder for DB than complex onsets: 10 transpositionseliminated hiatuses and created complex onsets (/pi.ro.et.ta/ . /pyo.ret.ta/),while only 5 went in the opposite direction (e.g., /gwer.rye.ro/ ./gwer.re.i.ro/). They also suggest that complex onsets are harder than codas:5 transpositions eliminated a complex onset and created a coda (e.g.,/re.klu.ta/ . /re.kul.ta/), but none went the opposite direction. These resultsare consistent with previous observations showing that DB performs betterwith codas than with complex onsets and that he has a special difficulty withhiatuses.

Another important feature of transpositions is that, just as for deletions,their majority involve liquids or glides (56/82 5 68%). Most transpositions,moreover (60/82 5 72%), occur on words containing one or more of thesyllabic structures that DB finds most difficult: complex onsets (N 5 36)and hiatuses (N 5 24). Therefore, transpositions still reveal a difficulty withcomplex syllabic structures in terms of sonority. It is possible that while DBis struggling to produce a complex syllabic configuration, his representationof order becomes less salient in a phonological output buffer. DB’s notedphonological short-term memory impairment may contribute to this degrada-tion of information.

Page 28: Syllabic Constraints in the Phonological Errors of an ...€¦ · degree of complexity to the basic CV template. Following this principle, one can derive the following ranking of

110 ROMANI AND CALABRESE

Syllabic Constraints on Insertion Errors

Some of the insertion errors made by DB, as well as by other aphasics,are puzzling because they unduly complicate the syllabic structure of thetarget word. Overall, DB makes 115 insertion errors. Forty percent (46/115)of these errors result in simplifications of syllabic structure, but 60% donot.

Interestingly, as we found for transpositions, most of DB’s insertions (85/115 5 74%), involve liquids and glides. A possibility, therefore, is that atleast a portion of these errors should also be interpreted as perseverationsand anticipations. This interpretation is supported by the fact that 71% (49/69) of insertions that complicate syllable structure and 56% (26/46) of inser-tions that simplify syllable structure involve a segment that occurs eitherearlier or later in the word. Like for transpositions, moreover, most of DB’sinsertion errors occur on words which contain one or more of the syllabicstructures that he finds most difficult: 82/115 5 72% of insertions occur onwords containing a complex onset or a hiatus.

Beland and Favreau (1991) reported that aphasic patients generally usedentals as epenthetic segments. They interpret this phenomenon as evidencefor Underspecification Theory (e.g., Archangeli, 1988), which assumes thatunderlying phonological representations consist of ‘‘underspecified’’ featurematrices. Dentals would be used more than velars, labials, and nonanteriorcoronals because the coronal place of articulation is the default value foran underspecified consonant position. This interpretation, however, cannotaccount for DB’s insertions. Although liquids and glides are dentals, ac-cording to Underspecification Theory, the consonant inserted most com-monly should be /t/ which has an empty feature matrix (all its features areunmarked). DB, however, never inserts /t/ (and he inserts /d/ only once).

While a portion of DB’s errors can be interpreted as transpositions, notall can. Moreover, some of the features of DB’s symmetric pattern of deletionof liquids and glides and insertion of the same segments remain in need ofan explanation. Liquids and glides are the only segments that can be insertedbetween a consonant and a vowel, and this may explain why these segmentsare both deleted and most commonly inserted (see Favreau, Nespoulous, andLecours (1990) for a report of this phenomenon in four fluent aphasics). DB,however, also inserts liquids and glides almost exclusively to break a hiatus(28/35 5 87%; /r/ 5 17, /l/ 5 6, /y/ 5 5). Here insertion of an obstruentconsonant would create a simpler syllable. This phenomenon remains to beexplained. One possibility is that he inserts a consonant which would de-crease the difficulty of the sequences but still maintain a ‘‘high sonorityprofile.’’ Another possibility, perhaps, is that liquids and glides becomehighly activated during DB’s struggle to produce them (even if this struggle

Page 29: Syllabic Constraints in the Phonological Errors of an ...€¦ · degree of complexity to the basic CV template. Following this principle, one can derive the following ranking of

PHONOLOGICAL ERRORS AND APHASIA 111

is not always successful). Thus, when he has to insert a phoneme, liquidsand glides may be among the most activated segments.6

GENERAL DISCUSSION

Our study has presented substantial evidence that the errors of DB, a pa-tient with articulatory difficulties, are influenced by sonority contrasts be-tween adjacent segments. In particular, we have presented evidence that hiserrors conform to Clements’ SDP. We have also seen, however, that not allof the error characteristics conform to this principle. We will start our discus-sion by reviewing these discrepancies.

The most conspicuous discrepancy with the SDP is the fact that DB doesnot show complexity effects for codas and generally makes very few errorson codas. One explanation for this pattern is that DB has problems withonsets, but not codas. This, however, is quite unlikely given his general ten-dency to simplify syllable structure. A more likely explanation has to dowith the characteristics of the Italian language. Italian does not allow com-plex codas, and there are reasons to believe that it only allows the simplestof the simple codas: those made by a liquid or a nasal. There is, therefore,little scope for DB’s errors to simplify codas and little scope to assess ahierarchy of complexity for codas. Further studies are needed to disentanglethese possibilities and gather evidence for the relative complexity of codas.It would be particularly useful to consider patients speaking languages hav-ing both complex codas like English and French and/or a variety of simplecodas (see Beland et al., 1990; and Ferreres, 1990 for some preliminaryevidence).

Other, more minor features of DB’s errors do not fit with predictions basedon the SDP. We have seen that, on ryV syllables, /r/ was generally deletedinstead of /y/. We also expected more substitutions that decreased sonorityin the case of a consonant preceded by a liquid. Generally, we have seenthat not all of DB’s substitutions resulted in simplifications. Some actuallyresulted in complications. Finally, we have also seen that a good portion ofinsertions resulted in complications. Clearly, it is possible that other princi-ples, besides sonority, operate in constraining DB’s output. An example al-ready discussed is segmental markedness. DB’s errors are affected more by

6 It is to be noted that phenomena similar to DB’s symmetric deletions and insertions ofglides occur in the history of languages (see Hooper, 1976; Piggott & Singh, 1985; Venne-mann, 1972). McCarthy (1991), for example, has suggested that an alternation between /r/deletions and insertions in Eastern Massachusetts English is not casual. He suggests that theuse of /r/ as an epenthetic segment to break a hiatus between two words (e.g., ‘‘he put thetuna away’’ . ‘‘he put the tuna[r] away’’), derives from a previous rule which deletes /r/in coda (e.g, ‘‘the spar seems to be broken’’ . ‘‘the spa seems to be broken’’). This rulemay have established an alternation in the grammar between presence and absence of /r/which was exploited by a new rule which required the insertion of a consonant.

Page 30: Syllabic Constraints in the Phonological Errors of an ...€¦ · degree of complexity to the basic CV template. Following this principle, one can derive the following ranking of

112 ROMANI AND CALABRESE

syllabic complexity than by segmental complexity since error rates on thesame segments (both deletions and substitutions) change depending on syl-labic position. We cannot exclude, however, that phonological markednessalso plays a role and, indeed, we would expect this to be the case. Althougha patient with mild articulatory difficulties like DB may have problemsmainly with difficult sequences of segments, problems with individual seg-ments may be revealed when segments are particularly complex. One clearexample may be DB’s substitution of /s/ for /ʃ/ (where /s/ is less markedthan /ʃ/, but not less sonorous). There were 19 of these substitutions in ourcorpus and none in the opposite direction. Another factor which may alsoplay a role in DB’s speech is the place of articulation of contiguous conso-nants (see Beland et al., 1990).

We do not claim to have provided an exhaustive account of all the factorsaffecting DB’s speech, but to have shown that syllabic complexity and sonor-ity play an important role in explaining his performance. This is clearlyshown in Table 8 which presents a summary of all of DB’s errors involvingone or two phonemes (deletions, substitutions, insertions, and transpositions)according to whether they complicate or simplify syllable structure. We con-sidered a simplification any error which eliminated a complex onset, a coda,or a hiatus. Conversely, we considered a complication any error which cre-ated one of these structures. We also considered substitutions which de-creased sonority in onset or increased it in coda to be simplifications, whilesubstitutions with the opposite outcome were considered to be complications(examples for each type of simplification or complication error are reportedin Appendix B). Errors which did not simplify or complicate syllable struc-ture were excluded from this analysis (N 5 513). They involved vowel sub-stitutions, consonant substitutions that did not change sonority, transposi-tions that did not change syllable structure, errors either creating oreliminating a single vowel syllable in word-initial position (there were onlythree errors like this), and errors that both created and eliminated a complexstructure.

The results of Table 8 confirm what has already been suggested by ouranalyses of each error type. DB’s errors show a clear tendency to eliminatecomplex syllabic structures: overall, three times more errors result in simpli-fications than in complications. It is also clear that the structures which aremore problematic for DB are hiatuses and complex onsets: 11 times moreerrors eliminate a hiatus than create one, and 4 times more errors eliminatea complex onset than create one.

The quantitative overview provided by Table 8 is strengthened by ourobservations on individual error types:

(a) An important feature of DB’s error pattern which is not captured byTable 8 is that deletion rates increase steadily with increasing complexityaccording to the SDP: progressively more errors are made in onsets withdecreasing sonority dispersion.

Page 31: Syllabic Constraints in the Phonological Errors of an ...€¦ · degree of complexity to the basic CV template. Following this principle, one can derive the following ranking of

PHONOLOGICAL ERRORS AND APHASIA 113

TABLE 8Summary of DB’s Simple Phonological Errors According to Their Syllabic Outcome

(Simplifications vs. Complications)

Simplifications N err. % tar. Complications N err. % tar. N tar.

Eliminate CO Create COCons deletions 226 Cons del in SO 1Vowel inser. 8 Cons insertions 50Palatalizations 33 Vowel deletion 3Transpositions 1

Simplify CO Complicate COSubstitutions 37 Substitutions 17Total CO 305 11.9 Total CO 71 2.8 2560

Simplify SO Complicate SOSubstitutions 334 Substitutions 109Total SO 334 1.7 Total SO 109 0.5 20111

Eliminate hiatus Creates hiatusVowel deletions 36 Cons deletions 7Cons. deletions 8 Vowel insertions 1Palatalizations 5Cons. insertions 35Transpositions 5Total hiatus 89 10.1 Total hiatus 8 0.8 877

Eliminate C Create CDeletions 11 Cons insertions 17Geminate deletions 14 Geminate insertions 15Vowel insertions 2

Simplify C Complicate CSubstitutions 8 Substitutions 8Total coda 35 0.6 Total coda 40 0.7 5706

Grand total 763 Grand total 228

Note. SO, simple onset; CO, complex onset; err 5 errors; tar 5 targets.

(b) Although most of transpositions and insertions are not syllabic simpli-fications they still occur on words containing those syllabic structures whichDB finds most difficult: words containing complex onsets and hiatuses.

(c) Among vowels, DB makes the least number of deletion and substitu-tion errors on /a/, which is the most sonorous vowel, and the least numberof errors on /u/, which is the least sonorous vowel.

(d) Finally, we have noted that DB makes no errors on complex onsetsmade by /s/1obstruent. These onsets, however, are a general exception toa sonority-based definition of ‘‘syllable,’’ since /s/ is less sonorous thanthe following obstruent. To explain this exceptional pattern, some linguistssuggest that /s/ is not really part of the syllable, but it is a ‘‘premargin’’(e.g., Cairns & Feinstein, 1982), others, as we have discussed, suggest that/s/ is adjoined to the syllable by a special rule which is not sensitive to so-nority constraints (Harris, 1983; Steriade, 1982). Whatever, ultimately, is theexplanation, it is interesting that DB treats /s/1obstruent onsets differently,

Page 32: Syllabic Constraints in the Phonological Errors of an ...€¦ · degree of complexity to the basic CV template. Following this principle, one can derive the following ranking of

114 ROMANI AND CALABRESE

so that the linguistic phenomena described by phonological theories andDB’s errors are in close correspondence even with regard to exceptions.

These analyses provide good evidence that DB’s errors are influenced notonly by syllabic complexity, but also by a sonority-based notion of syllabiccomplexity (as shown by deletion rates, nature of substitutions, and difficul-ties with hiatuses). Each of the characteristics described above showing aninfluence of sonority on DB’s errors, by itself, could occur for independentreasons. Taken together, however, they provide a strong pattern. This is espe-cially true since syllabic simplifications are not a necessary characteristic ofphonological errors. In a forthcoming paper (Romani, Semenza, & Grana’,1997b), we describe a patient who, superficially, makes phonological errorsvery similar to DB’s, but who makes, overall, about the same number oferrors which result in complications and simplifications of syllabic structure.Future studies will provide further evidence for which of the characteristicswe have described in DB cooccur and which (if any) dissociate in patientswith similar production problems. This will help us to gauge evidence forand against the SDP and ultimately to refine and revise this principle. Webelieve, however, that, in order to make progress in this area and judgewhether linguistic principles are an important factor constraining aphasicerrors, future studies will have to carry out linguistic analyses at the levelof detail of those exemplified by the present study.

Locus of Impairment

Given the characteristics of DB’s errors described above where could weplace the locus of his deficit? We have seen that DB has no problems withphonological discriminations in input tasks, while his speech is articulatorilyeffortful. These characteristics place his problem on the output side of thephonological system. While DB’s speech is articulatorily effortful, however,he does not produce slurred phonemes. Moreover, his errors are clearly in-fluenced by linguistic factors, especially syllabic complexity. These charac-teristics rule out a peripheral articulatory problem and suggest a more centraldeficit of articulatory programming. To see how this problem may result inerrors with the characteristics shown by DB, consider a production modelwith the following four stages:

(1) Phonological lexicon. Lexical representations make available, for eachword, a number of phonemes, their order, as well as prosodic and syllabicinformation.

(2) Phonological output buffer. The phonological segments activated inthe lexicon are copied into an output buffer where they are syllabified andwhere other parameters relevant to the idiosyncratic production of a wordare set (e.g., emotional pitch, volume, etc.).

(3) Articulatory planning. Using information in this buffer, articulatoryplanning transforms a set of abstract phonological features into a series ofmuscle commands.

Page 33: Syllabic Constraints in the Phonological Errors of an ...€¦ · degree of complexity to the basic CV template. Following this principle, one can derive the following ranking of

PHONOLOGICAL ERRORS AND APHASIA 115

(4) Articulatory implementation. The commands are carried out by theproper effectors.

Given the production model outlined above, our hypothesis is that, in DB,a difficulty in computing articulatory programs leads to simplifications ofthe abstract phonological representation that is the immediate antecedent ofthese programs. In other words, a deficit at the third level (articulatory plan-ning) leads to simplifications at the second level (phonological representa-tions in an output buffer). A similar mechanism has been invoked to explainwhy semantic errors (arising at the semantic level) could be caused by adeficit in a following component (the phonological output lexicon; Cara-mazza & Hillis, 1990). If DB finds an articulatory program too complex tocompute, the phonological representations held in the buffer will be simpli-fied to achieve a degree of complexity lower than normally allowed by Ital-ian. To accomplish the simplification, segments will be deleted, substituted,or inserted.

If our hypothesis of DB’s locus of impairment is correct, this implies thatthe dimension of complexity formalized by the SDP can be interpreted interms of articulatory complexity (and possibly perceptual complexity). Inother words, generally, syllabic configurations where adjacent segmentsare close in sonority would be more difficult to articulate than configura-tions where there is a contrast in sonority and the stronger the contrast thebetter the configuration. That the SDP has articulatory underpinnings isquite likely from a theoretical point of view. Our language system has devel-oped for the purpose of efficient communication and it is reasonable that themost successful syllable types, those with a wider distribution within andbetween languages, are those that are both easier to perceive and easier toarticulate.

We have assumed that DB’s phonological errors are the result of an articu-latory planning deficit, consistent with the hesitant quality of his speech.Another possibility, however, is that they are the result of a more centralphonological deficit. For example, DB’s deficit may involve the algorithmthat syllabifies phonological representations. This, however, does not explainwhy DB would syllabify phonological representations using a lower degreeof complexity than normally accepted by Italian. It seems to us that an articu-latory impairment provides a deeper level of explanation. Another possibilityis that DB’s phonological errors are caused by a faster than normal decayin a phonological buffer. This explanation is consistent with DB’s reducedspan. However, while we believe that decay from the buffer may play a moredirect role in causing his transposition and insertion errors, it is not clearwhy decay should generally result in syllabic simplifications. It would beinteresting to see whether other patients where phonological errors are asso-ciated with a limited digit span, but not with articulatory difficulties, makethe same kind of syllabic simplifications as those reported for DB.

Finally, one may wonder whether frequency may provide an alternative

Page 34: Syllabic Constraints in the Phonological Errors of an ...€¦ · degree of complexity to the basic CV template. Following this principle, one can derive the following ranking of

116 ROMANI AND CALABRESE

explanation of DB’s data: more errors may be made on syllable types whichare less frequent in the language. Frequency and the SDP, however, are bestdescribed not as alternatives, but as different levels of explanation. Fre-quency distributions, and possibly DB’s errors, are the data that the SDPtries to explain; the SDP provides the explanation in terms of a single algo-rithm based on differences on a sonority scale. It is possible, however, thatdifferences in sonority and familiarity with a given syllabic configurationmay both contribute to articulatory fluency. According to the SDP, if a lan-guage has syllables at a given level of complexity, it must also have all thesyllables at lower levels of complexity. As a consequence of this, acrosslanguages, simple syllables have a wider distribution than more complexones. Within a language, however, the predictions made by a frequency ac-count and by the SDP may not be totally overlapping. Future studies maywant to examine the relative influence of familiarity and the SDP when thesetwo principles are in relative contrast.

Differences with Other Interpretations

We have assumed that DB’s articulatory difficulty results in phonologicalsimplifications. The concept of simplification or repair strategies has beenused by others to explain the errors made by aphasic patients (e.g., Belandet al., 1993; Nespoulous, 1991). Repair strategies would be used by a patientto deal with a phonological representation that cannot be processed in a nor-mal fashion. Beland et al. (1993), for example, state ‘‘A neurological diseasecan impair the functioning of the phonological component. The phonologicalparameter settings are thus affected: parameters that are normally positivelyset are momentarily or permanently negatively set by aphasic patients, re-sulting in an increase of negative constraints in comparison with normalsubjects.’’ In this case, the patient may simplify the phonological representa-tion to comply with the new constraints.

We endorse a similar view. However, we disagree with the Beland et al.claim (1993) that repair strategies are the factor determining all phonologicalerrors. We have suggested that syllabic markedness has a strong influence onall the types of errors made by DB. However, we have invoked the conceptof repair strategies to explain only a subset of the errors (substitutions anddeletions). We have argued, instead, that most of DB’s transpositions andinsertions occur when segments are selected in the wrong order while he isstruggling to produce a difficult articulatory program. Moreover, Beland etal. claim that the same constraints should hold to explain the phonologicalerrors made by all aphasic patients. By contrast, we want to stress thatwhether there is an effect of syllabic markedness should depend on the func-tional damage responsible for the errors. Phonological errors could be theconsequence of very different cognitive impairments. For example, theycould also occur because of damaged lexical representations, or because of

Page 35: Syllabic Constraints in the Phonological Errors of an ...€¦ · degree of complexity to the basic CV template. Following this principle, one can derive the following ranking of

PHONOLOGICAL ERRORS AND APHASIA 117

a damaged phonological buffer. These other impairments may have no con-sequence for the ability to produce difficult syllabic configurations. Consis-tent with this hypothesis, Favreau et al. (1990) have reported no effect ofsyllabic markedness in four fluent aphasics and Nespoulous et al. (1984)have reported that Broca’s aphasics make more errors simplifying complexclusters than conduction aphasics.

Conclusions

Our study shows how an analysis of aphasic errors benefits from the intro-duction of detailed linguistic principles and how linguistic principles, in turn,can be tested and developed through an analysis of the errors made by anaphasic patient. It has been recognized for a long time that segment combina-tions that contrast more sharply in sonority are easier to produce. Only re-cently, however, has this general principle been elaborated to the point ofderiving hierarchies of syllabic configurations and of syllable contacts (e.g.,Clements, 1990; Vennemann, 1988). Overall, DB’s results provide good sup-port for these hierarchies and, more generally, for the notion that the sameprinciples of complexity can be used to account for the distribution of sylla-ble types (and syllable contacts) in the languages of the world and to predictthe characteristics of errors made by patients with damage to the appropriatecomponent of the production system.

APPENDIX A

Following is a listing of marked/unmarked Italian phonological featuresin their relative contexts. Feature specifications that create marked segmentsare in italics. The corresponding unmarked sounds can be obtained by chang-ing the value of these features.

Marked Unmarked

a. [2sonorant, 1continuant] fricatives (s,f,v,ʃ) stops (p, t, k, d, b, g)b. [2sonorant, 1voice] voiced (b,d,g,v,D) unvoiced (p,t,k,f,tʃ)c. [2continuant, Labial] labials (p,b)

. dentals (t,d)d. [2continuant, Dorsal] velars (k,g)e. [1consonantal, 1sonorant]/ l,r,m,n,ʎ,® p,t,k,b,d,g,s,ʃ,f,v

[ 2distributed]f. [1consonantal, 1distributed] ts,dz,tʃ,D p,t,k,b,d,g,s,ʃ,f,v,l,r,m,n,ʎ,®g. [1sonorant, 2nasal] r,l,ʎ m,n,®h. [1sonorant, 2lateral]/[ ,2nasal] r li. [2continuant, 2anterior]/ [ ,1distributed] tʃ,D ts,tzl. [1continuant, 2anterior] ʃ sm. [1lateral, 2anterior] ʎ ln. [1nasal, 2anterior] ® m, no. [1continuant, labial] f,v s

Page 36: Syllabic Constraints in the Phonological Errors of an ...€¦ · degree of complexity to the basic CV template. Following this principle, one can derive the following ranking of

118 ROMANI AND CALABRESE

APPENDIX B

Following are examples of DB’s errors which have been considered sim-plifications or complications of syllable structure in Table 7.

Simplifications Example Complications Example

Eliminate CO Create COCons del. tye.pido.te.pido Cons del coordi.namento. coor.dya..Vowel ins. frut.two.so. frut.ti.wo.so Cons ins. pasto.ya.pasto.ryaPalatal. comu.nyo.ne.comu.®o.ne Vowel del. poste.ryo.re. pos.tryo.reTranspos. bib.blyo.grafia.bib.bi.lo . . .

Simplify CO Compl. COSubstitutions gri.dare.kri.tare Substitutions sacerdo.tsyo.sacerdo.ryorio

Insertions fruttare.sfruttareSimplify SO Compl. SO

Substitutions protesta.re.protesta.ve Substitutions ar.ti.gyano. ar.tʃi.gyanoEliminate Hiatus Creates hiatus

Vowel del. curvili.ne.o.curvili.no Cons deletions auste.ro.auste.oCons. del. li.uto.yu.to Vowel inser. a.yutare.a.e.yutarePalatalizations errone.o . erro.®oCons. inser. orma.i. orma.liTranspositions mutu.are.mutu.rale

Eliminate C Create CDeletions or.ganismo.o.ganismo Cons insertions eko.no.mia.eko.nen.miaGeminate del. av.viato.a.viato Geminate inser. ca.noro.can.nolo

Simplify C Complicate CSubstitutions con.fermo.cor.fermo Substitutions cimen.ta.sti.tʃimes.ta.sti

REFERENCES

Archangeli, D. B. 1988. Aspects of underspecification theory. Phonology, 5(2), 183–292.

Barcellona Corpus 1994. Instituto di Linguistica Computazionale del CNR di Pisa, unpub-lished manuscript.

Basbøll, H. 1977. The structure of the syllable and a proposed hierarchy of distinctive features.In W. Dressler and O. Pfeiffer (Eds.), Phonologica. Innsbruck: Institute fur Sprach-wissenschaft, der Universitat Innsbruck. Pp. 143–148.

Basbøll, H. 1981. Remarks on distinctive features and markedness in generative phonology.In A. Belletti, L. Brandi, and L. Rizzi (Eds.), Theory of markedness in generative gram-mar. Pisa: Scuola Normale Superiore. Pp. 25–64.

Bastiaanse, R., Gilbers, D., & van der Linde, K. 1994. Sonority substitutions in Broca’s andconduction aphasia. Journal of Neurolinguistics, 8(4), 247–255.

Beland, R. 1990. Vowel epenthesis in aphasia: Evidence for coplanar representation in nonlin-ear phonology. In J.-L. Nespoulous and P. Villiard (Eds.), Morphology, phonology andaphasia. New York: Springer-Verlag. Pp. 235–252.

Beland, R., & Favreau, Y. 1991. On the special status of coronals in aphasia. In C. Paradisand J. F. Prunet (Eds.), The special status of coronals: Internal and external evidence.San Diego: Academic Press. Pp. 201–221.

Beland, R., Caplan, D., & Nespoulous, J.-L. 1990. The role of abstract phonological representa-tions in word production: Evidence from phonemic paraphasias. Journal of Neurolinguis-tics, 5(2/3), 125–164.

Page 37: Syllabic Constraints in the Phonological Errors of an ...€¦ · degree of complexity to the basic CV template. Following this principle, one can derive the following ranking of

PHONOLOGICAL ERRORS AND APHASIA 119

Beland, R., Paradis, C., & Bois, M. 1993. Constraints and repairs in aphasic speech: A groupstudy. Canadian Journal of Linguistics, 38(2), 279–302.

Blumstein, D. E. 1973. A phonological investigation of aphasic speech. The Hague Mouton.

Blumstein, S. E. 1978. Segment structure and the syllable in aphasia. In A. Bell and J. B.Hooper (Eds.), Syllables and segments. Holland: North-Holland. Pp. 189–200.

Blumstein, S. E., Cooper, W. E., Goodglass, H., Statlender, S., & Gottlieb, J. 1980. Productiondeficits in aphasia: A voice-onset time analysis. Brain and Language, 9, 153–170.

Bortolini, U., Tavaglini, C., & Zampolli, A. 1972. Lessico di frequenza della lingua italianacontemporanea. Milano: Garzanti.

Buckingham, H. W. 1986. The scan-copier mechanisms and the positional level of languageproduction: Evidence from phonemic paraphasia. Cognitive Science, 10, 195–217.

Buckingham, H. W. 1990. Principle of sonority, doublet creation, and the checkoff monitor.In J.-L. Nespoulous and P. Villiard (Eds.), Morphology, phonology and aphasia. NewYork: Springer-Verlag. Pp. 193–205.

Burani, C., & Cafiero, R. 1991. The role of subsyllabic structure in lexical access to printedwords. Psychological Research, 53(1), 42–52.

Butterworth, B. 1992. Disorders of phonological encoding. Cognition, 42, 261–286.

Cairns, C. E., & Feinstein, M. H. 1982. Markedness and the theory of syllabic structure.Linguistic Inquiry, 13(2), 193–225.

Calabrese, A. 1995. A constraint based theory of phonological markedness and simplificationprocedures. Linguistic Inquiry, 26(3), 373–463.

Calabrese, A., & Romani, C. 1991. Syllable structure in aphasia: A case study. In P. Bertinetto,M. Kenstowiz, and M. Loporcaro (Eds.), Certamen phonologicum II. Torino: Rosen-berg & Sellier.

Canter, G., J., Trost, J. E., & Burns, M. 1985. Contrasting speech patterns in apraxia of speechand phonemic paraphasia. Brain and Language, 24, 204–222.

Caramazza, A., & Hillis, A. E. 1990. Where do semantic errors come from? Cortex, 26(1),95–122.

Chierchia, G. 1986. Length, syllabification and the phonology cycle in Italian. Journal ofItalian Linguistics, 8, 5–34.

Chomsky, N., & Halle, M. 1968. The sound pattern of English. Cambridge, MA: The MITPress.

Christman, S. S. 1992. Uncovering phonological regularity in neologisms: Contributions ofsonority theory. Clinical Linguistics and Phonetics, 6(3), 219–247.

Christman, S. S. 1994. Target-related neologism formation in jargonaphasia. Brain and Lan-guage, 46, 109–128.

Clements, G. N. 1990. The role of the sonority cycle in core syllabification. In J. Kingstonand M. Beckmann (Eds.), Papers in laboratory phonology 1. Cambridge: CambridgeUniv. Press.

Clements, G. N., & Keyser, S. J. 1983. CV Phonology: A generative theory of the syllable.Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.

Davis, S. 1987. Some observations concerning English stress and phonotactics using a compu-terized lexicon. Research on Speech Perception, 13, 225–239.

Davis, S. 1990. Italian onset structure and the distribution of il and lo. Linguistics, 28, 43–55.

Dell, F., & Elmedlaui, M. 1986. Syllabic consonants and syllabification in Imdlawn TashlhiytBerber. Journal of African Languages and Linguistics, 7, 105–130.

Page 38: Syllabic Constraints in the Phonological Errors of an ...€¦ · degree of complexity to the basic CV template. Following this principle, one can derive the following ranking of

120 ROMANI AND CALABRESE

Eckman, F., Moravcsik, E., & Wirth, J. (Eds.) (1986). Markedness. Milwaukee, WI: The Uni-versity of Wisconsin.

Favreau, Y., Nespoulous, J.-L., & Lecours, A. 1990. Syllable structure and lexical frequencyeffects in the phonemic errors of four aphasics. Journal of Neurolinguistics, 5(2/3), 165–187.

Ferreres, A. R. 1990. Phonematic alterations in anarthric and Broca’s aphasic patients speakingArgentine Spanish. Journal of Neurolinguistics, 5(2/3), 189–213.

Goldsmith, J. 1990. Autosegmental Phonology. New York: Garland Press.

Greenberg, J. 1978. Universals of human language: Phonology. Standford, CA: StandfordUniversity Press. Vol. 2.

Guerssel, M. 1986. Glides in Berber and sillabicity. Linguistic Inquiry, 17, 1–12.

Harris, J. 1983. Syllable structure and stress in Spanish: A non linear analysis. LinguisticInquiry Monograph 8. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Hooper, J. B. 1976. An introduction to natural generative phonology. New York: AccademicPress.

Ito, M. 1988. Syllable theory in prosodic phonology. New York: Garland Press.

Jakobson, R. 1941. Kindersprache, aphasie und allegemeine lautgesetze. In Jakobson (1962),328–401. Selected writings I, The Hague: Mouton.

Jakobson, R. 1968. Child language, aphasia and phonological universals. Paris: Mouton-TheHague.

Jespersen, O. 1904. Lehrbuch der Phonetik. Leipzig/Berlin: Teubner.

Jespersen, O. 1922. Language, its nature and origin. New York: Holt.

Kaye, J., & Lowenstamm, J. 1981. Syllable structure and markedness theory. In A. Belletti,L. Brandi, and L. Rizzi (Eds.), Theory of markedness in Generative Grammar. Pisa:Pacini Editore. Pp. 287–315.

Kean, M. L. 1981. On the theory of markedness: Some general considerations and a case inpoint. In A. Belletti, L. Brandi, and L. Rizzi (Eds.), Theory of markedness in generativegrammar. Pisa: Scuola Normale Superiore.

Keating, P. 1983. Comments on the jaw and syllable structure. Journal of Phonetics, 11, 401–406.

Kenstowicz, M. 1994. Phonology in generative grammar. London: Blackwell.Klich, R. J., Ireland, J. V., & Weidner, W. E. 1979. Articulatory and phonological aspects of

consonant substitutions in apraxia of speech. Cortex, 15, 451–470.

Levelt, W. J. 1992. Accessing words in speech production: Stages, processes and representa-tions. Cognition, 42, 1–22.

Levin, J. 1985. A metrical theory of syllabicity. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, MIT, Cam-bridge, MA.

Lindblom, B. 1983. Economy of speech gestures. In P. F. Mac Neilage (Ed.), The productionof speech. New York: Springer-Verlag. Pp. 217–245.

Maddieson, I. 1984. Patterns of sounds. Cambridge: Cambridge Univ. Press.

Marie, P. 1906. La troisiemme circonvolution frontale gauche ne joue aucun role special dansla foncion du langage. Semeotica Medica, 26, 241–247.

Marotta, G. 1988. The Italian diphtongs and the autosegmental framework. In P. M. Berti-netto & M. Loporcaro (Eds.), Certamen Phonologicum, papers from Cortona PhonologyMeeting. Torino: Rosenberg and Sellier.

McCarthy, J. 1991. Synchronic rule inversion. In L. A. Sutton, C. Johnson, & R. Shields,(Eds.), Proceedings of the 17th Annual Meeting of the Berkeley Linguistic Society. Berke-ley: Berkely Linguistic Society.

Page 39: Syllabic Constraints in the Phonological Errors of an ...€¦ · degree of complexity to the basic CV template. Following this principle, one can derive the following ranking of

PHONOLOGICAL ERRORS AND APHASIA 121

Mehler, J., Dommergues, I. Y., Frauenfelder, U., & Segui, J. 1981. The syllable’s role inspeech segmentation. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 20, 298–305.

Miceli, G., & Capasso, R. 1985. Batteria della morfologia. Neupsychological Service of theCatholic University of Rome. Unpublished testing material.

Murray, R. W., & Vennemann, T. 1983. Sound change and syllable structure in Germanicphonology. Language, 59, 514–528.

Nespoulous, J.-L. 1991. Segmental errors in aphasia as (psycho)linguistic evidence of thenature of phonological representations. Paper presented at the IX European Workshopon Cognitive Neuropsychology, Bressanone, Italy.

Nespoulous, J.-L., Joanette, Y., Beland, R., Caplan, D., & Lecours, A. 1984. Phonologicaldisturbances in aphasia: Is there a ‘‘markedness effect’’ in aphasic phonetic errors? InF. C. Rose (Ed.), Advances in neurology: Progress in aphasiology. Vol. 42. London:Raven Press.

Piggott, G., & Singh, R. 1985. The phonology of epenthetic segments. Canadian Journal ofLinguistics, 30(4), 415–451.

Price, P. J. 1980. Sonority and syllabicity: Acoustic correlates of perception. Phonetica, 37,327–343.

Romani, C., Semenza, C., & Grana’, A. 1998a. Contrasting patterns of morphological errorsin two aphasic patients. Manuscript in preparation.

Romani, C., Semenza, C., & Grana’, A. 1998b. Patterns of phonological errors as a functionof locus of impairment: Phonological vs. articulatory. Manuscript in preparation.

Schein, B., & Steriade, D. 1986. On geminates. Linguistic Inquiry, 1, 691–744.

Sievers, E. 1901. Grundzuge der phonetic. Leipzig: Breitkopf und Hartel.Smith, N. V. 1973. The acquisition of phonology: A case study. Cambridge: Cambridge Univ.

Press.

Steriade, D. 1982. Greek prosodies and the nature of syllabification. Unpublished doctoraldissertation, MIT, Cambridge, MA.

Treiman, R., Gross, J., and Cwikiel-Glavin, A. 1992. The syllabification of /s/ clusters inEnglish. Journal of Phonetics, 20, 383–402.

Vennemann, T. 1972. Rule inversion. Lingua, 29, 209–242.Vennemann, T. 1988. Preference laws for syllable structure and the explanation of sound

change. Berlin: Mouton.