1
Poster crafted by Linda Pistolesi and Jennifer Griffith Lynn Liben [email protected] Department of Psychology, Pennsylvania State University Kim Kastens [email protected] Lamont-Doherty Earth Observatory of Columbia Universtiy Students’ Misconceptions About the Correspondences Between a Map and the Terrain Represented by the Map How can we tell what students understand and misunderstand about map reading? We have developed innovative field-based map skills assessment tasks. Types of errors fourth-graders make on the Flag-Sticker (Reality-to-Map) Task: Representational Correspondence Errors: Configurational Correspondence Errors: Imprecise Distance Determination: Rotational or Symmetry Problems: CHILD Dependent Variable PLACE ("reality") T A S K (map) REPRESENTATION Place Marker Task Map-to-Reality Task We gave each student four colored wooden disks and a map with four colored stickers on it. The student's task was to place the disks on the ground to show where each of the stickers was located on the map. Dependent Variable T A S K (map) REPRESENTATION CHILD PLACE ("reality") Flag Sticker Task Reality-to-Map Task We placed eight colored flags around campus and gave each student a paper map and eight colored stickers. The student's task was to place each sticker on the map to show where the corres- ponding-colored flag was located in the real world. We interpret these error types to indicate that the student has not fully understood the basic correspondence between symbols on the map and the categories of objects which the symbols are representing. We interpret these error types to indicate that the student has understood the represen- tational correspondence between symbols on the map and categories of object, but they have not successfully used the configuration of the features on the map to disambiguate between multiple occurrences of the same symbol on the map or to pinpoint the correct location within a large map symbol. This error type was common among students who performed poorly on a paper-and-pencil test of mental rotation ability. We interpret this error type to suggest that the student has not rotated the map (either physically or mentally) into alignment with the terrain. These students correctly narrowed down their sticker position to the correct region, but most of them failed to use either relative or absolute distance information to precisely pinpoint the sticker within a landmark-less path segment. We think that most students conceived of the yellow flag as being "in that half" or "at that end" of the path segment, and did not try for a more precise localization. Sticker Location Flag Location Sticker Location Flag Location We think that many students conceived of the blue flag as being "in the middle" of the path segment, whereas it was actually east of the center. Yellow Flag Blue Flag Number of Stickers in Bin Students that placed their sticker mid-way along the path segment. Students that placed their sticker along the western half of the path segment. Why is instructional technology so powerful for achieving this learning goal? Map reading is an example of a broader class of tasks of translating back and forth between a perceived aspect of reality and a paper representation of that aspect of reality. Other examples: Traditionally such "translation" skills are taught, painstakingly, by a human mentor who watches the learner try, and try again, and try again, and offers guidance after each try. (Image from Geography Education Standards Project (1994). Geography for Life: National Geography Standards 1994, National Geographic Research & Exploration.) Instructional technology can take over some of this effort by: 1) setting up situations where an aspect of reality needs to be compared with the representation, and 2) assessing the learner's tries and offering feedback. elevation = a cos (ωt - kx - α) from Barber (1969), Water Waves Musical notation Heard music Mathematical equation for wave elevation Ocean waves Paper Representation Perceived Aspect of Reality http://peabody.vanderbilt.edu/projects/funded/jasper/Jasperhome.html In the video adventure, "Rescue at Boone's Meadow," Emily must retrieve Jasper Woodbury and a wounded eagle from Boone's Meadow using an ultralight plane. Students analyze alternative ways in which Emily can carry out the rescue, and which way will be the fastest, based on information revealed during the video. By solving a series of largely mathematical problems, students formulate a rescue plan which takes into account fuel, travel time, payload, and landing area limitations. Classic Math Example: Jasper Woodbury Series Classic Music Example: Musical Maze In The Treehouse software, children ages 5-8 advance through the Musical Maze by correctly selecting the sign displaying the musical notation that corresponds to a heard snippet of music. Children can choose to guess the song being formed by the snippets, or continue to collect notes until the song is finished. The Treehouse software by Broderbund Software. Our curriculum analysis shows that most map skills curriculum materials do not require students to think about the real world; the tasks can be completed entirely within the frame of reference of the representation, i.e. the map. These types of representational correspondence tasks are useful, but not sufficient for students to learn to use maps for real world tasks. 0 10 20 30 40 50 60 70 Beginning Map Skills Comic-Strip Map Skills Geography Skills Learning About Maps Map Coloring Book Map Skills for Today Mapmaking with children Maps and Globes Maps and Navigation Mapworks Neighborhoods and Communities Reading Topographic Maps Teach with Topographic Maps Using maps in your community Around and about Exploring maps Exploring our world Finding your way Map Adventures Orienteering and map games Reading maps Number of lessons in category Production Comprehension Representational Correspondence Meta- Representational Why was a new curriculum approach needed? Child refers to a map: • What does the map key tell you? • What can you use the compass rose for? CHILD TASK PLACE ("reality") REPRESENTATION (map) Observable product or action Child refers to a sketch map of an imaginary neighborhood: • Who lives at the corner of Lion Street and Tiger Street? • Which streets does the railroad cross? • Which building is just south of Ann's house? CHILD REP #1 (map) REP #2 (words) TASK PLACE ("reality") Observable product or action Where are We? curriculum: Landmarks lesson Keeping Track of Where You’ve Been lesson Treasure Hunt field trip lesson CHILD TAS K PLACE ("reality") REPRESENTATION (map) Observable product or action Where are We? curriculum: Bird’s Eye View Mapping lesson Putting New Infomation on the Map lesson Lost! lesson Long Skinny Map field trip lesson Where was that Flag? field trip lesson Observable product or action PLACE ("reality") CHILD REPRESENTATION (map) T A S K Method for studying children's understanding of spatial representation of place (after Liben, 1997) Curriculum Example Curriculum Example Curriculum Example Curriculum Example Comprehension Methods Production Methods Representational Correspondences Methods with links to PLACE not exercised Meta-Representational Methods Where Are We? aims to help ele- mentary school children master the skill of "translating" between a portion of the real world and a spatial representation of that same terrain by setting up circumstances in which students must repeatedly make connections between a park map and a video filmed in the park. What does Where Are We? try to accomplish? The Where Are We? software and curriculum are available from Tom Snyder Productions: http://www.tomsnyder.com

Students’ Misconceptions About the Correspondences Between a … › ~kastens › talks_posters › ... · 2011-12-28 · PLACE ("reality") Flag Sticker Task Reality-to-Map Task

  • Upload
    others

  • View
    2

  • Download
    0

Embed Size (px)

Citation preview

Page 1: Students’ Misconceptions About the Correspondences Between a … › ~kastens › talks_posters › ... · 2011-12-28 · PLACE ("reality") Flag Sticker Task Reality-to-Map Task

Poster crafted by Linda Pistolesi and Jennifer Griffith

Lynn Liben [email protected] of Psychology, Pennsylvania State University

Kim Kastens [email protected] Earth Observatory of Columbia Universtiy

Students’ Misconceptions About the Correspondences Between a Map and the Terrain Represented by the Map How can we tell what students understand and misunderstand about map reading?

We have developed innovativefield-based map skills assessment tasks.

Types of errors fourth-graders make on the Flag-Sticker (Reality-to-Map) Task:

Representational Correspondence Errors: Configurational Correspondence Errors:

Imprecise Distance Determination:

Rotational or Symmetry Problems:

CHILD

TASK

DependentVariablePLACE ("reality")

TASK(map)

REPRESENTATION

Place Marker Task

Map-to-Reality TaskWe gave each student four coloredwooden disks and a map with four colored stickers on it. The student'stask was to place the disks on theground to show where each of thestickers was located on the map.

DependentVariable

TASK

(map)REPRESENTATION

CHILD

PLACE ("reality")

Flag Sticker Task

Reality-to-Map Task

We placed eight colored flags aroundcampus and gave each student a papermap and eight colored stickers. Thestudent's task was to place each stickeron the map to show where the corres-ponding-colored flag was located inthe real world.

We interpret these error types to indicate that the student has not fullyunderstood the basic correspondence between symbols on the map andthe categories of objects which the symbols are representing.

We interpret these error types to indicate that the student has understood the represen-tational correspondence between symbols on the map and categories of object, but theyhave not successfully used the configuration of the features on the map to disambiguatebetween multiple occurrences of the same symbol on the map or to pinpoint the correctlocation within a large map symbol.

This error type was common among students whoperformed poorly on a paper-and-pencil test of mentalrotation ability. We interpret this error type to suggestthat the student has not rotated the map (either physicallyor mentally) into alignment with the terrain.

These students correctly narrowed down their stickerposition to the correct region, but most of them failedto use either relative or absolute distance informationto precisely pinpoint the sticker within a landmark-lesspath segment.

We think that most students conceived of the yellow flagas being "in that half" or "at that end" of the path segment,and did not try for a more precise localization.

Sticker Location

Flag Location

Sticker Location

Flag Location

We think that many students conceived of the blue flagas being "in the middle" of the path segment, whereas itwas actually east of the center.

Yellow FlagBlue Flag

Num

ber o

f Stic

kers

in B

in

Students that placed their sticker mid-way along the path segment.

Students that placed their sticker alongthe western half of the path segment.

Why is instructional technology so powerful for achieving this learning goal?

Map reading is an example of a broader class of tasks of translating back and forth between a perceivedaspect of reality and a paper representation of that aspect of reality.

Other examples:

Traditionally such "translation" skills are taught, painstakingly, by ahuman mentor who watches the learner try, and try again, and try again,and offers guidance after each try.(Image from Geography Education Standards Project (1994). Geography for Life: National Geography Standards 1994, National GeographicResearch & Exploration.)

Instructional technology can take over some of this effort by: 1) setting up situations where an aspect of reality needs to be compared with the representation, and2) assessing the learner's tries and offering feedback.

elevation = a cos (ωt - kx - α)from Barber (1969), Water Waves

Musical notationHeard music

Mathematical equationfor wave elevation

Ocean waves

Paper Representation Perceived Aspect of Reality

http://peabody.vanderbilt.edu/projects/funded/jasper/Jasperhome.html

In the video adventure, "Rescue at Boone's Meadow,"Emily must retrieve Jasper Woodbury and a woundedeagle from Boone's Meadow using an ultralight plane.

Students analyze alternative ways in which Emilycan carry out the rescue, and which way will be thefastest, based on information revealed during thevideo.

By solving a series of largely mathematical problems,students formulate a rescue plan which takes intoaccount fuel, travel time, payload, and landing arealimitations.

Classic Math Example: Jasper Woodbury SeriesClassic Music Example: Musical MazeIn The Treehouse software, children ages 5-8 advance through theMusical Maze by correctly selecting the sign displaying the musicalnotation that corresponds to a heard snippet of music. Children canchoose to guess the song being formed by the snippets, or continueto collect notes until the song is finished.

The Treehouse software by Broderbund Software.

Our curriculum analysis shows that most map skills curriculum materials donot require students to think about the real world; the tasks can be completedentirely within the frame of reference of the representation, i.e. the map.

These types ofrepresentationalcorrespondencetasks are useful,but not sufficientfor students tolearn to use mapsfor real worldtasks.

0 10 20 30 40 50 60 70

Beginning Map Skills

Comic-Strip Map Skills

Geography Skills

Learning About Maps

Map Coloring Book

Map Skills for Today

Mapmaking with children

Maps and Globes

Maps and Navigation

Mapworks

Neighborhoods and Communities

Reading Topographic Maps

Teach with Topographic Maps

Using maps in your community

Around and about

Exploring maps

Exploring our world

Finding your way

Map Adventures

Orienteering and map games

Reading maps

Number of lessons in category

Production

Comprehension

RepresentationalCorrespondence

Meta-Representational

Why was a new curriculum approach needed?

Child refers to a map:• What does the map key tell you?• What can you use the compassrose for?

CHILDTASK

REPRESENATION(map)

PLACE ("reality")

REPRESENTATION(map)

Observableproduct or

action

Child refers to a sketch map of animaginary neighborhood:• Who lives at the corner of LionStreet and Tiger Street?• Which streets does the railroadcross?• Which building is just south ofAnn's house?

CHILD

REP #1(map)

REP #2(words)

TASK

PLACE ("reality")Observableproduct or

action

Where are We? curriculum:• Landmarks lesson• Keeping Track of Where You’ve Been

lesson• Treasure Hunt field trip lesson

CHILD

TASK

PLACE ("reality")

REPRESENATION(map)

REPRESENTATION(map)

Observableproduct or

action

Where are We? curriculum:• Bird’s Eye View Mapping lesson• Putting New Infomation on the Map lesson• Lost! lesson• Long Skinny Map field trip lesson• Where was that Flag? field trip lesson

Observableproduct or

action

PLACE ("reality")

CHILD

REPRESENTATION(map)

TASK

Method for studying children's understanding of spatial representation of place (after Liben, 1997)

Curriculum Example

Curriculum Example

Curriculum Example Curriculum Example

Comprehension MethodsProduction Methods

Representational Correspondences Methodswith links to PLACE not exercised

Meta-Representational Methods

Where Are We? aims to help ele-mentary school children masterthe skill of "translating" betweena portion of the real world and aspatial representation of that sameterrain by setting up circumstancesin which students must repeatedlymake connections between a parkmap and a video filmed in the park.

What does Where Are We? try to accomplish?

The Where Are We? softwareand curriculum are availablefrom Tom Snyder Productions:

http://www.tomsnyder.com