Spatial memory by blind and sighted children

Embed Size (px)

Citation preview

  • 8/18/2019 Spatial memory by blind and sighted children.

    1/14

  • 8/18/2019 Spatial memory by blind and sighted children.

    2/14

    Br. J. Psychol.

     (1975), 66, 4,

     p p.

      449-459 4 4 0

    Printed in

      reat

      Britain

    SPATIAL MEMORY BY BLIND AND SIGHTED CHILDREN

     

    SUSANNA MILLAR

    Department of Experimental

      Psychology

    University of

      xford

    Non-verbal recall of haptically presented spatial positions by three age groups of blind and

    sighteil children was tested under conditions varying cueing, recall type and stimulus position

    in a within-subject design. Sighted stMuii was not only significant, but interacted significantly

    with recall typ e, and further w ith stimulus position, consistent with sequential hap tic by blind

    and quasi-simu ltaneous visuai processing by sighted children. Age was eignifleant, bu t its only

    significant interaction was a relatively small one with cueing conditions and stimulus position,

    suggesting that the oldest group, regardless of sightedness, used verbal strategies in pre-cued

    conditions. The findings support th e hypothesis th at visual and hap tic modalities of representa-

    tion have demonstrably different effects on processing and efficiency in spatial recall, but

    couiiterindicate the hypothesis tha t these relate differentially to age. Resu lts also suggest t ha t

    a combination of cue utilization and verbal strategies is a significant, but relatively minor, factor

    in improvements in spatial recall.

    The nature and function of imagery in memory for spatial position, the extent to

    which children use it, and its relation to other factors which affect memory are by

    no mean s clear. A t least thre e views are possible; Piag et Inhe lder  (1971,  1973)

    regard visual imagery as largely irrelevant, and stress the level of abstraction from

    actual action by the subject in the development of spatial as of other types of

    memory. This may mean either that modalities of representation have no differential

    effects and serve as 'props' for mental operations in the same way, or that action

    and movement with regard to objects and positions, and thus haptic factors, have

    special relations to memory functions, A second view is that visual imagery has

    specifically facilitating effects in spa tial mem ory. H utte nlo ch er Presson (1973)

    suggest th at children, like adults (A ttneave  Benson, 1969; Rock , 1974; Shephard

      ;

    Feng, 1972; Shephard  Metzler, 1971), visually image spatial layouts and mentally

    track positions on it. This suggests that visual imagery has a special role in spatial

    memory and possibly also in improvements with age, A third possible view is that

    spatial memory and development depend on verbal strategies.

    Theoi'etically, tbese views involve questions about 'memory modalities' or, more

    simply, whether differential mo dality effects can be dem onstra ted in spatial m emo ry,

    and, if so, how these relate to cognitive skills; for instance, the organization of

    material for recall, and the utilization of prior information; and to age. Memory for

    haptic (touch and movement) inputs, about which relatively little is kllow^l as yet,

    is of particular interest, because it provides a test case for the nature and effects of

    representations, but also because it is of practical importance in teaching the blind

    who need to rely on spatial memory far more than the sighted.

    The present study was therefore designed to test hypotheses arising from these

    differing views. Although visual imagery is frequently reported and suggested as an

    exp lana tion , it is more difficult t o find experimentally dem on strated differential

    effects on spatial memory in children. The methodological problems and difficulties

    of interpretation inherent in Piagetian and Paivio-type methods have been dis-

  • 8/18/2019 Spatial memory by blind and sighted children.

    3/14

  • 8/18/2019 Spatial memory by blind and sighted children.

    4/14

    Spatial memory  451

    delay (Gilson Baddeley , 1969) or with unfam iliar non-verba l m ater ial (Millar, 1974).

    In the author's current study on mental spatial rotation the sighted seemed to rely

    on visual strategies from the point of stoppage, while the blind were differentially

    penalized on the more complex directions and lost track already after two turns,

    consistent with sequential coding and the findingB that haptic memory is less

    subject to active strategies, except with familiar material. Thus, if the blind rely on

    haptic and movement rather than on verbal coding for spatial recall, a differential

    effect on the primacy position for the older blind should not be found, while an inter-

    action of recall type with sighted status would be predicted.

    Support for the hypothesis that modality of representation is a factor in develop-

    ment with age would be shown by appropriate age interactions. Rate of forgetting

    as such does not seem to be an important factor in memory development {Belmont 

    Butterfield, 1969). I t is of interes t, however, to re late th e role, if any , of the m oda lity

    of spatial representation to factors involving mental work in memory: the reorganiza-

    tion of material as in backward recall, and the utilization of prior information for

    encoding.

    This study therefore tested spatial recall by three age groups of blind and sighted

    children, varying cueing conditions, type of recall and stimulus position to test the

    hypotheses that (1) the modality of representation has differential effects on efficiency

    and on strategies of spatial recall; (2) this is a factor in improvements with age; and

    (3) improvements with age in spatial recall depend upon verbal strategies, organiza-

    tion, or cue utilization.

    METHOD

    A flve-way factorial design with age, sighted status, pre- and post-cueing, recall type, and

    stin]ulu8 position (repeated measures on the last three factors) was used for non-verbal recall of

    Iiaptically presented spatial positions as follows.

    Subjects

    There were 72 subjects: 36 congenitally or very early (20 mo nths or less) totally or severely

    Ijlind (minimal light, but no pattern perception) from two residential schools for the blind; and

     • S

      sighted children from two local authority schools within tlie catchment area, and similar in

    range of ability (low average to very superior) and socio-economic status of the parents. Tiie

    sighted were matchod to the blind on sex, age (within threo calendar months), forward digit

    span, and backward digit span (ascertained in the manner of Binet tests). IQ scores, although

    necessarily on different tests, available for the blind and some of the sighted, and ability ratings

    by teachers wore taken into account in matching.

    There were 12 sighted and 12 blind subjects in each of three age groups - oldest gi-oups:

    seven girls, five boys; mean age 10:4 (from 9:1 to 11:8); mean forward span, 6-0 (from 5 to 8);

    mean backward span, 3-2 (from 2 to 4); middle groups: four girls, eight boys; mean age, 8:2

    (from 7:0 to 8 :10 ); mean forward sp an, 5-4 (from 4 to 7 ); mean backw ard apan, 2-9 (from 0

    to 5); youngest groups: six girls, six boys; mean age, 6:11 (from 6:2 to 7:5); mean forward

    span, 5-0 (from 4 to 7); mean backward span, 2-2 (from 0 to 4).

      aterials

    The material consisted of a

     35

     x 4 cm slot cut in a 20-25 cm Perspex square to form a five-sided

    open-shaped sp atial plan w itli four right-angled tur ns . This was presented in one of four positions

    (north, south, east, west) in randomized order across conditions and trials for each subject.

    For presentation, the subject traced along the slot with a pencil from the start to prepared

  • 8/18/2019 Spatial memory by blind and sighted children.

    5/14

    452  SU S N N MI L L R

    the subject s haiid at tliis point. It was removed after contact and tlie subject continuod tracin

    the slot to the eud. The recall task was to trace the slot with the penoil, either

     fi-ora

     he start o

    from the end of the slot, to the position on the slot where tho stop had hoen encountered on th

    presentation trial. After recall the stop was replaced for the next presentation.

    Subjects usod only their preferred hand to hold the pencil. They were not permitted to palpa

    the display except by tracing tlirough the slot with tho pexxcil in the experimental condition

    For siglitcd subjects the display was presented in a curtained unit which prevented viewing, b

    permitted free hand and arm movements.

    Task

    The task was presented as a game of teacliing

     

    Bozo ~ a rubber animal fastened to the end

    the pencil  to remember the position of a traffic -light post, so that he would stop at that positio

    on the road next time.

    Tho starting point for eacli presentation was the start of the slot nearest to the subject an

    his preferred hand, in one of the four positions. Tliia point

     w£is

     called the subjects house . T

    end-point of the slot was called Bozo s kennel , and the slot was the road between the subjec

    house and Bozo s kennel . The stop formed by the stick and suction base was called the traffi

    light post .

    The subject was told that he always had to take Bozo on two journeys {by tracing the sl

    with the pencil to which Bozo was fastened) between his house and Bozo s kennel. The fir

    journey would always be from the subject s house (starting-point) to Bozo s kennel (en

    point), but they would be stopped by colliding with a traffic light-post somewhere on the roa

    I he post would then be removed so that they could go on to Bozo s kennel. The main point wa

    to remember the point on the road where they had felt the post. On the second joiu-ney th

    traffic-light post would not be there. Nevertheless, on the second journey Bozo would have

    stop dead at the point where the traffic-light post hatl been on the first journey ( to avoid traff

    liazards ). Presentation consisted in tlie first journey from house to kennel. The recall task w

    the second journey .

    Typt of recall

    Type of recall was either forward or bacltward.

      Forward

     recall:  the subject was instruct

    that the second joumoy between the house (starting-point) and tho kennel (end-point) wou

    start again at the same point

     —

     the subject s house . He would have to take Bozo along

    road from the house until he came to the point where he and Bozo had felt the trafSc-light po

    on the first journey. This meant that he would have to trace through the slot with tlie pencil

    which Bozo was fastened to that poiat and not any farther. The traffic post would no longer b

    there, but he had to make sure that Bozo stopped at exactly the same point on the road whe

    the traffic-light post had been on the first journey. Backtvard

     recall:

      the subject was instruc

    that the second journey between the house and the kemiel would start from the keixnel

    the end of the road. He would have to take Bozo back along the road (trace through the slo

    etc.) from the kennel (end-point of the slot) to the point on the road where they had felt th

    traffic-light post on the first journey. The traffic post would no longer be there, but he had t

    make sure that Bozo would stop at exactly that point.

    Recall was imniediato (approx. 3 sec.) after the end of presentation, and was signalled b

     same, from the house or back, from kennel , as appropriate for forward and backward reca

    To ensure a rapid start to the recall, the experimenter helped the subject to insert the top of th

    pencil at the start, or end of the slot, as appropriate,

     xt ing

      conditions

    Tliere were two cueing conditions, Tliey difFered solely on whether instructions for the type

    recall were given prior to presentation (first journey) or only prior to recall (second journey

    Pre cued conditions:  the subject was informed prior to a block of forward recall runs that rec

    (the second journey) would always start at the same point - his house - on the second journe

    as on the first. For the block of backward recall runs, the subject was informed prior to the bloc

    of runs that recall (second journeys) would always be back from the kennel (end-point of slot

  • 8/18/2019 Spatial memory by blind and sighted children.

    6/14

    Spatial memory

    453

    Fig. 1. The five-sided spatial display slot here shown in the W es t position. Inpu t d uring

    presentation is from tracing the slot from H (house) to K (kennel). Sj represents the first (near),

    S( represent the recent (far) stimulus position. Forward recall is from H to either   ̂or  Sg.  Back-

    ward recall is from K to either Sj or Sj.

    also sta rt from th is house or Rem em ber, the second journey will sta rt from the kennel at th e

    end ) .

     Presentation was then signalled immediately by st ar t . Post cued

     conditions

    the subject

    was told prior to the whole block of post-cued runs that the second journeys could either be

    forward from the house, starting at tlie same point as the first journey or backward from the

    kennel, starting back from tho end-point, but that he would not know before each of the first

    journeys w hether the second one would be from the house or from the kennel. He would, however,

    be told which of these it was to be, at the start of each of the second journeys by the signal

     sam e, from the house or baek, from kenne l . Presentation was signalled by s ta rt .

    Subjects had no difficulty in understanding the tas k, and thoroughly enjoyed th e ga m e .

    Stirmdus position

    Two positions on the d isplay, one near the st arting point (7-5 om) and one far from it (27 6 om ),

    were tested. Since tracing the display was sequential, the near stimulus represented an initial,

    and the far stimulus a recent, stimulus in relation to the touch and movement inputs of the

    display. Tlie display slot, sta rt ( house ) and end ( kenn el ) of presentation , and t he n ear (first)

    and far (recent) stimulus positions are shown in Fig. 1.

    Procedure

    Subjects were tested singly in a quiet room of their school. The game waa explained to the m ,

    and they received two demonstration trials (not counted), followed by 16 main trials (four in

    each of the eonditions: pre-cued forward, pre-cued backward, post-cued forward, post-cued

    backward). The order of cueing conditions, blocked in threes (post-cueing both recall types in

    counterbalanced order, pre-cued forward; pre-cued backward recall), recall type and stimulus

    position were counterbalanced across subjects.

  • 8/18/2019 Spatial memory by blind and sighted children.

    7/14

    454

      S U S A NN A M I L L A R

    RESULTS AND DISCUSSION

    Errors were counted in mm deviations from  the  stimulus position. Mean absolu

    errors,

      shown

      in

      Table

      1,

     were subjected

      to a

      five-way  ANOVA  with

      age

     {younges

    middle, oldest), sighted sta tus blind, sighted), cueing conditions pre -po st),

      type

     

    recall forward, backward)  and  stimulus position near, far) as variable s repeate

    measures

     on the

      last three factors).

    Table 1.

     Mean

      bsolute errors

      mm)

     for

     blind

      nd

     sighted children under

    different cueing, rec ll  nd stimulus position conditions

    Pre-cueing Po«t-cueing

    stimulus position .. .

    Mean age

    Blind 6:11

    8:2

    10:4

    Heaa

    Sighted 6:11

    8:2

    10:4

    Mean

    Forward recall

    Near

    69

    58

    51

    59

    86

    110

    31

    76

    F a r

    70

    67

    40

    5 9

    52

    51

    21

    4 1

    Backward recall

    Near

    100

    119

    90

    103

    78

    57

    44

    60

    P ar

    59

    37

    42

    46

    41

    40

    21

    34

    Forward recall

    Near

    97

    62

    85

    81

    101

    47

    60

    7 3

    F a r

    58

    76

    54

    63

    70

    40

    22

    44

    Backward

    Near

    103

    119

    96

    106

    66

    41

    41

    4 9

    recall

    F a r

    55

    54

    54

    54

    69

    62

    11

    47

    8 I

    7

    6

    50

     

    Blind

    Sighted

    Forward Backward

    Type of recall

  • 8/18/2019 Spatial memory by blind and sighted children.

    8/14

    Spatial memory

    465

    U O r

     

    90

    8

    7

    6

    50

    4

    Forward recall

    blind

    Backward recall

    blind

    Forward recall

       ̂ • sighted

    • Backward recall

    sighted

    far

    Stimulus position

    Fig. 3. Mean absolute errors (mm) by blind and sighted children for near and

    far stimuli in forward and backward recall.

    Sighted status was a significant effect  {F =  12-04; d.f. = 1, 66 ; P 0-001) and

    interacted significantly with recall type  {F =  10-0; d.f. = 1, 66 ;  P 0-005), sup-

    porting th e first hypothesis. The interactio n, shown in Fig. 2, shows no t only tha t the

    blind prod uced larger errors, bu t th at t he m ain difference was obviously on back w ard

    recall, Avith significantly

      {P

    0-01) worse backward than forward recall for the blind,

    while for the sighted the relation was reversed. This supports the assumption that

    the blind used sequential strategies and the sighted did not.

    The relation of sighted status to recall type waa further elucidated by the fact that

    this interacted also with stimulus position  {F =  11-40; d.f. = 1, 66;  P 0-005).

    Stim ulus position was indeed a highly significant m ain effect { =

      69 97;d.f.

      = 1,66;

    P

    0*0001). This meant that the near (primacy) position was very much more

    difficult. The in teraction of sighted sta tus , recall type a nd stimulus position

     is

     graphed

    in

     Fig.

     3. This shows th at for the b lind the difference between recall of th e two po sitions

    was clearly greater for backward recall, with greatly increased errors on the near

  • 8/18/2019 Spatial memory by blind and sighted children.

    9/14

    456 SusAJ^NA

      M I L L A R

    recall from stimulus presentation to recall point were greatest for the near positio

    and least for the far position for backward recall, and between these for forward recal

    this implies that time and distance determined errors by the blind. Tliis sugges

    a sequential haptio rather than a verbal rehearsal strategy. For the sighted, th

    pic ture is very different. The rece nt position was unaffected by recall t> pe, b

    responses for the near (primacy) position were actually easier in backward tha

    forward recall, i.e. in the opposite direction from w hat would be predicted with verb

    strat-cgies. Reorganization of sequential verbally encoded positions on the displa

    would produce larger rather than smaller error for backward recall, and verb

    rehearsa l would no t penalize responses to th e near position in forward recall. A com

    pletely simultaneous strategy could not have been used either. The results sugge

    that one sighted visually evoked or envisaged the display from tbe end-point (

    recall) regardless of cueing conditions, rather than during presentation. This wou

    penalize responses more on the near position in forward recall. A similar strategj^

    reconstruc ting fi om the po int of stoppag e w as used by the sighted in the au tho r

    current study on mental rotation of a haptically presented line.

    Age   {F =  8 -23; d.f. = 2, 66; P < O-OOl) was significant, b u t did no t significantl

    interact with sighted s tatus in any combination, cou nterindi eating th e hypothes

    that modality effects were a determining factor in improvements with age.

    The only significant interaction of age \vith other factors was with a combinatio

    of cueing conditions and stimulus position  {F =  3-25; d.f. = 2, 66; P < 0-05

    graphed in Fig. 4. This shows that pre-cueing significantly   P

  • 8/18/2019 Spatial memory by blind and sighted children.

    10/14

     p ti l

     m emory

      7

     

    I-

    9

    7

    o 60

    SO

    4

    3

    N^

     Youngest group

    post-cued

     ^ «

     Middle group post-cued

    Youngest group

    pre-cued

      Middle group pre-cued

    ' N _ \ ^ Oldest group

    ^. post-cued

    - '* Oldest group

    pre-cued

    far

    Stimulus position

    Fig. 4. Mean absolute errors (mm) for the youngest {mean age, 6:11), middle (mean age, 8:2)

    and oldest {mean age, 10:4) groups for near and far stimuli in pre- and post-euod coiiditions.

    point, possibly because they mainly recoded visually at recall rather than during

    presen tation. The findings supported th e hypothesis th a t m odalities of represe ntation

    differentially affect efficiency and processing in spatial recall.

    The findings also supported the view that visual imagery has a special role in

    spatial recall by children. In so far as Piaget's theory implies the opposite, or suggests

    th at visual imagery provides 'p ro ps ' in the same w ay as other types of represe ntation,

    this was counterindicated. The absence of any interactions between sighted status

    and age, also counterindicated the implication in Piaget's theory that haptic and

    movement representations have a special role in age changes. At the same time, this

    finding does support his view that visual imagery as such does not relate to age. It

    suggests indeed that age changes were independent of visual and haptic modality-

    specific processing.

    The fact that recall type did not relate to age may explain this. For visual -

    simultaneous or evoked imagery - reorganization and therefore worse backw ard

    recall and age changes in this \vith increased use of active organizational strategies

  • 8/18/2019 Spatial memory by blind and sighted children.

    11/14

    4 5 8 SUSAJ^NA MlLLAH

    wa rd recall was predicted for sequential hap tic processing an d w as found for th e blin

    The fact th a t tliis did no t in terac t w ith age either

     is

     consistent w ith previous findi

    that haptic memory is less subject to active strategies unless the material is ove

    learned (Gilson Badde ley, 1969; Millar, 1974, 1975a,

      b

    Sullivan  Turvey, 197

    A reasonable explanation of the absence of relations between modality effects an

    age is therefore th a t visual ima gery and processing does not requ ire, and ha ptic repr

    sentations and processing do not readily lend themselves to, active reorganization

    Finally, the fact that the only variables which interacted significantly wilh ag

    were a combination of cueing conditions and stimulus position which implied th

    utilization of prior information elicited verbal strategies by both blind and sighte

    subjects in th e oldest gro up , m ust be considered. Since th e effect, a ltho ugh significan

    was relatively sm all, and ne ither cueing conditions nor stimu lus position alone relate

    to age, this suggests (i) that age changes depended on combinations of factors rath

    than on any single cognitive or verbal one, and (ii) that cue utilization and verb

    coding played a significant, but relatively small part in age changes in spati

    memory.

    The practical implications for the blhid are twofold. First, the deliberate use

    verbal labelling and rehearsal, particularly at the time spatial positions are fir

    encountered, seems to aid recall of the more difficult positions, but probably requii'

    specific teaching and encouragement in younger children who do not seem to do th

    spontaneously. Secondly, since the effect was small and the blind apparently main

    relied on memory for touch and movement, repeated actual familiarization wi

    spatial layouts which allows over-leaniing of touch and movement cues is probab

    essential.

    In summary, the findings suggest a model of spatial memory in children whic

    includes modahties of representation as important factors, determining type

    processing, but not changes with age. It was argued that this may be because visu

    an d ha ptic, or modality-specific represe ntations and the typ e of processing these elic

    do not easily lend them selves to active controls. The finding.s suggest further th

    such a model may need to include changes to a combination of verbal strategies an

    utilization of prior information for encoding as a significant, but relatively min

    factor in age changes in spatial memory. The main improvements in spatial memor

    with age seemed to take place within the modalities of representation without bein

    determined by the differences between them.

    The research was aupported by a grant from the Social Scionoe Reaoarch Council. Tho auth

    wislies to thank tlie staff and the ctiildren of the seliools wlio took part in the experiment f

    their unfailing help and cooperation.

  • 8/18/2019 Spatial memory by blind and sighted children.

    12/14

    Spatial memory

      459

    R E F E R E N C E S

     R. C. S H I F P R I N ,  R .  M .  (1968). Human memory: a proposed Bystem and its control

    processes. In K. W. Spence J. T. Spence (eds.). The Psychology of Learning and Motivation,

    vol. 2. New York: Academic Press.

      F . B E N S O N ,  B . (1969). Spatial coding of tac tua l stimulation.  J. exp. Psychol.  81,

    216-222.

     G. (1971). An experimental study of visual imagination and memory.  Cogn. Psychol. 2,

    290-299.

      Y . M .  BuTTERFiELD, E. C. (1969). The relations of ahort-tenn memory to develop-

    ment and intelligence. In L. P. Lipsi tt H. W. Reese (eds.),  Advances in Child Development

    and Behaviour.  London: Academic Press.

      H . A . (1967). The effect of labels in short term memory for colours with nursery

    school children.  Psychon. Sci.  7, 149-150.

      C, J . H E T J V E L ,  K . V, (1974). Development of geometric imagery in five- to eight-

    year olds.  Genet. Psychol. Monogr.  89, 89-143.

     L . R . (1967). The suppression of visualizing hy reading.

     Q. J. exp. Psychol.

      19, 289-299.

     R . (1971). Thechronology of the development of covort speech in children. Dev. Psychol.

    5 ,

      398-405.

    GiLsoN, E. Q. B A D D E L E Y ,  A. D.  (1969). Tactile short-term memory.  Q. J. exp. Psychol.  21

    180-184.

      B . O ' C O N N O R ,  N . (1971). Spatial coding in normal, autistic and blind children.

    Percept, niot. Skills  33, 127-132.

    NLOCnEB, J. P R E S S O N ,  C. C. (1973), Mental rotation and the perspective problem.

    Cogn. PsycJwl.  4, 277-299.

      S. (1974). Tactile short-term memoi-y by blind and sighted children.  Br. J. Psychol.  65,

    2 5 3 - 2 6 3 .

    M I L L A R , S. (1975a). EfFects of phonological and tactual similarity on serial object recall by blind

    and sighted children.  Cortex  (in press).

    MiLLiiR, S. (19756). Effects of tactual and phonological similarity on the recall of Braille letters

    by blind children.  Br. J. Psychol.  66,  1 9 3 - 2 0 1 .

    X E I S S E E ,  V. K E R R ,  N . (1973). Spatial and mnemonic proper ties of visual images.  Cogn.

    Psychol.  5, 138-150.

    O ' C O N N O R ,

      N . H E R M E L I N , B .

      (1972). Seeing and hearing in spa.ce and t ime . Percept. Paychophys.

     

    46-48.

    PiAGET, J. I N H B L D E R ,  B . (1971). Mental Imagery in the Child London: Routledge Keg€in

    Paul.

    PiAQET, J.  I N H E L D E R ,  B . (1973). Memory and IrUelligence. London: Rontledge Kegan Paul.

    R O C K ,  I. (1974). Tlie perception of disoriented figures.  Sci. Amer.  230 78-85.

    S H E P H A R D ,  R . N . F E N G ,  C. (1972). A chronometric study of paperfolding.

      Cogn. Psychol.

      3,

    2 2 8 - 2 4 3 .

    SHErHARD, R. N.

    M E T Z L E R ,

      J . (1971). Mental rotat ion of three-dimensional objects.  Science,

    N.Y.  171 701-703.

    StJLLiv.,iN, E. V. TcTRVEY, M. T. (1972). Sliort-term retention of tac tile stimulation.  Q. J. exp.

    Psychol.  24,

      2 5 3 - 2 6 1 .

    {Manuscript received 3 October 1974; revised manuscript

    received  10 February  1975)

  • 8/18/2019 Spatial memory by blind and sighted children.

    13/14

  • 8/18/2019 Spatial memory by blind and sighted children.

    14/14