24
11/5/2015 1 Emotional Development How Children Develop Chapter 10 Emotional Intelligence A set of abilities that contribute to competent social functioning: Being able to motivate oneself and persist in the face of frustration Control impulses and delay gratification Identify and understand one’s own and others’ feelings Regulate one’s moods Regulate the expression of emotion in social interactions Empathize with others’ emotions

Siegler Chapter 10: Emotional Developmentrakison/POCDclass17.pdf · 2015-11-05 · Mood (positive or negative) 8. ... knowledge is the recognition of different emotions in others

Embed Size (px)

Citation preview

11/5/2015

1

Emotional

Development

How Children Develop

Chapter 10

Emotional Intelligence

A set of abilities that contribute to

competent social functioning:

Being able to motivate oneself and

persist in the face of frustration

Control impulses and delay gratification

Identify and understand one’s own and

others’ feelings

Regulate one’s moods

Regulate the expression of emotion in

social interactions

Empathize with others’ emotions

11/5/2015

2

Emotional Intelligence

EQ is a better predictor than IQ

of how well people will do in life,

especially in their social lives

Emotion

Characterized by neural and

physiological responses, subjective

feelings, cognitions related to those

feelings, and the desire to take actions

Most psychologists share this general view:

but they disagree on importance of its key

components

11/5/2015

3

Perspectives

Research supports both perspectives to some degree,

and no one theory has emerged as definitive.

Discrete Emotions Theory

The Functionalist Approach

Discrete Emotions Theory

Argues that:

Emotions are innate

and are discrete

from one another

from very early in life

Each emotion is

packaged with a

specific and

distinctive set of

bodily and facial

reactions

11/5/2015

4

The Functionalist Approach

Maintains that emotions

are not discrete from

one another and vary

somewhat based on the

social environment

Emphasizes the role of

the environment in

emotional development

Proposes that the basic

function of emotions is

to promote action toward

achieving a goal

Characteristics of Some

Families of Emotions

11/5/2015

5

Happiness

Smiling is the first clear

sign of happiness that

infants express

Young infants smile from

their earliest days, but the

meaning of their smiles

appears to change with age

Social Smiles are directed

toward people and first emerge

as early as 6 to 7 weeks of age

Happiness

Around 3 or 4 months, infants

laugh as well as smile during

activities

At about 7 months, infants

start to smile primarily at

familiar people, rather than at

people in general

In 2nd year, children clown

around and enjoy making

others laugh

*

11/5/2015

6

Negative Emotions

The first negative emotion

that is discernible in infants is

generalized distress

By 2 months, facial

expressions of anger or

sadness can be differentiated

from distress/pain in some

contexts

By the second year of life, this

differentiation is no longer

difficult

Negative Emotions

Also display anger when:

stimulation removed

caregiver leaves

arms restrained

Why?

11/5/2015

7

Anger

Fear

The first clear signs of fear emerge at around 6 or 7 months, when unfamiliar people no longer provide comfort and pleasure similar to that provided by familiar people

The fear of strangers intensifies and lasts until about age 2; but there is variability

11/5/2015

8

Evidence of Fear

in Young Children

Separation Anxiety

Refers to feelings of distress that children,

especially infants and toddlers, experience

when they are separated, or expect to be

separated, from individuals to whom they

are attached

It is a salient and important type of fear

and distress that tends to increase from 8

to 13 or 15 months and then begins to

decline

This pattern is observed across many cultures

11/5/2015

9

Separation Anxiety

Self-Conscious Emotions

Feelings such as guilt, shame, embarrassment,

and pride that relate to our sense of self and

our consciousness of others’ reactions to us

Emerge during the second year of life

Around 15 to 24 months, children show

embarrassment when the center of attention

By 3 years, children’s pride is increasingly tied to

their level of performance

11/5/2015

10

Guilt and Shame

Guilt is associated with

empathy for others and involves

feelings of remorse and regret

and the desire to make amends

Shame does not seem to be

related to concern about others

Whether children experience

guilt or shame partly depends

on parental practices

Emotions in Middle Childhood

From early to middle childhood: acceptance by peers and achieving goals are important sources of happiness and pride

By the early school years, children’s perceptions of others’ motives and intentions are important in determining whether or not they will be angered

Children overall become less intense and less emotionally negative with age in the preschool and early school years

11/5/2015

11

Emotions in Adolescence

Adolescence is a time of

greater negative emotion

than middle childhood

A minority experience a

major increase in the

occurrence of negative

emotions, often in their

relations with their

parents

Depression

The rate of clinical depression, which

is 3% prior to adolescence, is 15% or

higher from age 15-18...

An addition 11% of U.S. youth experience

less serious symptoms of depression

Hispanic children report more symptoms

of depressions than do Euro- or African

Americans

Children with depression frequently

exhibit behavior problems

11/5/2015

12

Depression

Depression

Possible causes of depression include

genetic factors, maladaptive belief symptoms,

feelings of powerlessness, negative beliefs and

self-perceptions, and the lack of social skills

Family factors also

contribute to depression

Antidepressant drugs are most common treatment

11/5/2015

13

Regulation

of Emotion

The Development of

Emotional Regulation

The Relation of Emotional

Regulation to Social Competence

and Adjustment

Patterns in

Developing Self-Regulation

Transition from Regulation

by Others to Self-Regulation

Use of Cognitive Strategies

to Control Negative Emotions

Ability to Select Strategies

Appropriate for the Situation

11/5/2015

14

Emotional Self-Regulation

The process of initiating, inhibiting, or modulating

internal feeling states, emotion-related physiological

processes, and emotion-related cognitions or

behaviors in the service of accomplishing one’s goals

Its emergence in childhood is a long, slow process

Transition to Self-Regulation

In the first months of life,

parents

help infants regulate their

emotional arousal by

controlling their exposure to

stimulating events

6 months: infants reduce

distress by averting gaze and

self-soothing

Between 1 and 2 year: infants

increasingly divert their

attention to non-distressing

objects or people

11/5/2015

15

Transition to Self-Regulation

Over the course of the early years, children

become more likely to rely on themselves rather than

their parents

Children’s improving self-regulation is due at least in

part to the increasing maturation of the neurological

system

They are also influenced by increases in adults’

expectations of children and to age-related

improvement in the ability to inhibit motor behavior

Individual Differences

in Emotion and its

Regulation

Temperament

11/5/2015

16

Temperament

The constitutionally based

individual differences in

emotional, motor, and

attentional reactivity and self-

regulation that demonstrate

consistency across situations,

as well as relative stability over

time

Differences in the various

aspects of children’s emotional

reactivity that emerge early in

life are labeled as dimensions

of temperament

141 children followed from infancy to adulthood)

Infants were rated on 9 personality dimensions:

1. Activity level

2. Rhythmicity

3. Approach/withdrawal

4. Adaptability

5. Emotional reactivity

6. Responsiveness to stimuli

7. Mood (positive or negative)

8. Distractibility

9. Attention span

New York Longitudinal Study

by Thomas and Chess

11/5/2015

17

Easy 40% Regular routines, cheerful, adapts

easily

Difficult 10% Irregular routines, dislikes new

experiences, reacts negatively

Slow-to-

warm

up

15% Inactive, mid to low-key reactions,

negative in mood, adapts slowly.

35% fit no category: a mixture of these.

Difficult pattern: 70% developed behavior problems by

school age (only 18% of easy children did).

Slow-to-warm-up: few problems in early years, but some

later in school when need to respond actively/quickly.

Infant Temperament

11/5/2015

18

Infant Temperament

In contrast to Thomas and Chess’s approach,

many contemporary psychologists believe that

it is important to

Assess positive and negative emotion as separate

components of temperament

Differentiate among types of negative emotionality

Assess different types of regulatory capacity

Recent research suggests that infant

temperament is captured by six dimensions

Fearful distress, irritable distress, attention span and

persistence, activity level, positive affect, and

rhythmicity

Examples of Items in Mary

Rothbart’s Temperament Scales

11/5/2015

19

Examples of Items in Mary

Rothbart’s Temperament Scales

The Stability of Temperament

Children who as infants showed

behavioral inhibition with novel

stimuli also showed elevated

levels of fear in novel situations

at age 2 and elevated levels of

social inhibition at age 4 ½

It is important to note, however,

that some aspects of

temperament tend to be more

stable than others

*

11/5/2015

20

Temperament and

Social Adjustment

Different problems with adjustment

are associated with different

temperaments

Children’s adjustment depends on how

their temperament fits with the demands

and expectations of the social

environment:

a concept described as goodness of fit

Moreover, the child’s temperament and

the parents’ socialization efforts seem to

affect each other over time

Children’s Emotional

Development

in the Family

Quality of the Child’s

Relationships with Parents

Parental Socialization of

Emotional Responding

11/5/2015

21

Personality

Refers to the pattern of

behavioral and emotional

propensities, beliefs and

interests, and intellectual

capacities that

characterize an individual

Has its roots in

temperament but is

shaped by interactions

with the social and

physical world

Socialization

How individuals, through others, develop the skills and

ways of thinking and feeling, as well as standards and

values, that allow them to adapt to their group and live

with other people

Parents, teachers, and

other adults are important

socializers for children,

(other children,

the media, and social

institutions too)

11/5/2015

22

Parental Emotions

The emotions to which children are exposed

may affect their level of distress and arousal

The consistent and open expression of positive

emotion in the home is associated with positive

outcomes

When negative emotions are predominant,

children have low

levels of social competence

and to express negative

emotions themselves

Parental Reactions to Emotion

Parents who respond to their

children’s sadness and anxiety by

dismissing or criticizing their feelings

communicate to their children that

their feelings are not valid

In turn, their children tend to be less

emotionally and socially competent

than children whose parents are

emotionally supportive

11/5/2015

23

Children’s

Understanding

of Emotion

Identifying the Emotions of Others

Understanding the Causes of Emotion

Children’s Understanding of

Real and False Emotions

Understanding Simultaneous

and Ambivalent Emotions

Identifying the

Emotions of Others

The first step in the development of emotional knowledge is the recognition of different emotions in others

By 4 to 7 months, infants can distinguish certain emotional expressions, such as happiness and surprise

At 8 to 12 months, children demonstrate social referencing, the use of a parent’s facial, gestural, or vocal cues to decide how to deal with novel, ambiguous, or possibly threatening situations

By age 3, children in laboratory studies demonstrate a rudimentary ability to label a fairly narrow range of emotional expression

11/5/2015

24

Identifying the

Emotions of Others

Young children are best at labeling happiness,

with the ability to distinguish among different

negative emotions gradually appearing the late

preschool and early school years

Most children cannot label more complex

emotions until early- to mid-elementary school

The ability to discriminate and label different

emotions helps children respond appropriately

to their own and others’ emotions