12
Sensory Processing Disorder in Children Ages Birth–3 Years Born Prematurely: A Systematic Review Anita Witt Mitchell, Elizabeth M. Moore, Emily J. Roberts, Kristen W. Hachtel, Melissa S. Brown MeSH TERMS premature birth prevalence risk assessment sensation sensation disorders Anita Witt Mitchell, PhD, OTR, is Associate Professor, Occupational Therapy Department, University of Tennessee Health Science Center, Memphis; [email protected] Elizabeth M. Moore, MOT, OTR/L, is Occupational Therapist, Signature Healthcare at St. Francis, Memphis, TN. Emily J. Roberts, MOT, OTR/L, is Occupational Therapist, Regional One Health, Memphis, TN. Kristen W. Hachtel, MOT, OTR/L, is Occupational Therapist, First Choice Speech and Occupational Therapy, Hernando, MS. Melissa S. Brown, MOT, OTR/L, is Occupational Therapist, Methodist Healthcare South Hospital, Memphis, TN. At the time of the study, Elizabeth Moore, Emily Roberts, Kristen Hachtel, and Melissa Brown were Students, Department of Occupational Therapy, University of Tennessee Health Science Center, Memphis, TN. This systematic review of multidisciplinary literature synthesizes evidence of the prevalence and patterns of sensory processing disorder (SPD) in children ages birth–3 yr born preterm. Forty-five articles including physiological, behavioral, temperament, and SPD research met the inclusion criteria and provided 295 findings related to SPD—130 (44%) positive (evidence of SPD) and 165 (56%) negative (no evidence of SPD). The majority of findings related to sensory modulation disorder (SMD; 43% positive). The most prevalent subcategory of SMD was sensory overresponsivity (82% of findings positive). Evidence of sensory underresponsivity and sensory-seeking SMD, sensory discrimination disorder, and sensory-based motor disorder was limited. This study supports the education of neonatologists, pediatricians, and caregivers about the symptoms and potential consequences of SPD and helps justify the need for follow-up screening for SPD in children ages birth–3 yr born preterm. Research using measures based on sensory processing theory is needed. Mitchell, A. W., Moore, E. M., Roberts, E. J., Hachtel, K. W., & Brown, M. S. (2015). Sensory processing disorder in children ages birth–3 years born prematurely: A systematic review. American Journal of Occupational Therapy, 69, 6901220030. http://dx.doi.org/10.5014/ajot.2015.013755 I nfants born preterm may be at risk for difficulty processing sensory in- formation. Not only is their in utero neurosensory development cut short (Graven & Browne, 2008), but they may also experience intense stimuli in the neonatal intensive care unit (NICU) that could alter sensory system development and function (Nair, Gupta, & Jatana, 2003). Sensory processing dysfunction, in turn, could influence the development of play, social participation, education, and self-care occupations (Miller, Anzalone, Lane, Cermak, & Osten, 2007; Schaaf et al., 2010). Despite these problems, research is limited on the prevalence and patterns of dysfunction in infants and children born prematurely from the perspective of sensory integration theory. The aims of this review were to examine a variety of literature for evidence of sensory processing dysfunction in children ages birth–3 yr born prematurely and to identify the types of sensory processing deficits that are apparent. In 2007, Miller and colleagues proposed adoption of the new term sensory processing disorder (SPD) in place of the term sensory integration dysfunction, presenting a new taxonomy in diagnosing subtypes of SPD. This classification system was developed using focus groups and empirical evidence, with the ex- pectation of continued scholarly dialogue and research based on the taxonomy. This systematic review contributes to the discussion of the nosology and explores its validity for use with children ages birth–3 yr born preterm by examining the literature to determine whether the proposed patterns of SPD can be identified in this population. Such identification could help justify occupational therapy as- sessment, follow-up, and intervention for this population and specifically support an approach based on sensory integration theory. Our two research questions were The American Journal of Occupational Therapy 6901220030p1

Sensory Processing Disorder in Children Ages Birth-3 Years Born Prematurely a Systematic Review

Embed Size (px)

DESCRIPTION

SENSORY PROCESSING

Citation preview

  • Sensory Processing Disorder in Children Ages Birth3Years Born Prematurely: A Systematic Review

    Anita Witt Mitchell, Elizabeth M. Moore, Emily J. Roberts,

    Kristen W. Hachtel, Melissa S. Brown

    MeSH TERMS

    premature birth prevalence risk assessment sensation sensation disorders

    Anita Witt Mitchell, PhD, OTR, is Associate Professor,

    Occupational Therapy Department, University of

    Tennessee Health Science Center, Memphis;

    [email protected]

    Elizabeth M. Moore, MOT, OTR/L, is Occupational

    Therapist, Signature Healthcare at St. Francis, Memphis,

    TN.

    Emily J. Roberts, MOT, OTR/L, is Occupational

    Therapist, Regional One Health, Memphis, TN.

    Kristen W. Hachtel, MOT, OTR/L, is Occupational

    Therapist, First Choice Speech and Occupational Therapy,

    Hernando, MS.

    Melissa S. Brown, MOT, OTR/L, is Occupational

    Therapist, Methodist Healthcare South Hospital, Memphis,

    TN.

    At the time of the study, Elizabeth Moore, Emily Roberts,

    Kristen Hachtel, and Melissa Brown were Students,

    Department of Occupational Therapy, University of

    Tennessee Health Science Center, Memphis, TN.

    This systematic review of multidisciplinary literature synthesizes evidence of the prevalence and patterns of

    sensory processing disorder (SPD) in children ages birth3 yr born preterm. Forty-five articles including

    physiological, behavioral, temperament, and SPD research met the inclusion criteria and provided 295

    findings related to SPD130 (44%) positive (evidence of SPD) and 165 (56%) negative (no evidence of

    SPD). The majority of findings related to sensory modulation disorder (SMD; 43% positive). The most

    prevalent subcategory of SMD was sensory overresponsivity (82% of findings positive). Evidence of sensory

    underresponsivity and sensory-seeking SMD, sensory discrimination disorder, and sensory-based motor

    disorder was limited. This study supports the education of neonatologists, pediatricians, and caregivers about

    the symptoms and potential consequences of SPD and helps justify the need for follow-up screening for SPD

    in children ages birth3 yr born preterm. Research using measures based on sensory processing theory is

    needed.

    Mitchell, A. W., Moore, E. M., Roberts, E. J., Hachtel, K. W., & Brown, M. S. (2015). Sensory processing disorder in

    children ages birth3 years born prematurely: A systematic review. American Journal of Occupational Therapy, 69,

    6901220030. http://dx.doi.org/10.5014/ajot.2015.013755

    Infants born preterm may be at risk for difficulty processing sensory in-formation. Not only is their in utero neurosensory development cut short(Graven & Browne, 2008), but they may also experience intense stimuli in the

    neonatal intensive care unit (NICU) that could alter sensory system development

    and function (Nair, Gupta, & Jatana, 2003). Sensory processing dysfunction, in

    turn, could influence the development of play, social participation, education,

    and self-care occupations (Miller, Anzalone, Lane, Cermak, & Osten, 2007;

    Schaaf et al., 2010). Despite these problems, research is limited on the prevalence

    and patterns of dysfunction in infants and children born prematurely from the

    perspective of sensory integration theory. The aims of this review were to examine

    a variety of literature for evidence of sensory processing dysfunction in children

    ages birth3 yr born prematurely and to identify the types of sensory processing

    deficits that are apparent.

    In 2007, Miller and colleagues proposed adoption of the new term sensoryprocessing disorder (SPD) in place of the term sensory integration dysfunction,presenting a new taxonomy in diagnosing subtypes of SPD. This classification

    system was developed using focus groups and empirical evidence, with the ex-

    pectation of continued scholarly dialogue and research based on the taxonomy.

    This systematic review contributes to the discussion of the nosology and explores

    its validity for use with children ages birth3 yr born preterm by examining the

    literature to determine whether the proposed patterns of SPD can be identified in

    this population. Such identification could help justify occupational therapy as-

    sessment, follow-up, and intervention for this population and specifically support

    an approach based on sensory integration theory. Our two research questions were

    The American Journal of Occupational Therapy 6901220030p1

  • (1) What does evidence in the literature suggest about the

    prevalence of SPD in children ages birth3 yr born pre-

    term? and (2) What types of SPD are evident in the liter-

    ature describing these children?

    Background Literature

    All motor, behavior, emotional, and attention responses

    are a result of how the brain processes sensory information.

    Sensory information serves as an important foundation for

    adaptive behaviors such as self-control, learning, and the

    ability to organize (Schaaf et al., 2010). Sensory integration

    theory explains how sensations are processed and organized

    to facilitate adaptive behaviors.

    A. Jean Ayres developed the concepts of sensory in-

    tegration theory in 1963 to help explain learning problems

    and behaviors in children (Miller et al., 2007). Ayres (1989)

    defined sensory integration as the neurological process thatorganizes sensations from ones body and from the envi-

    ronment and makes it possible to use the body effectively in

    the environment (p. 22). She described sensory integration

    dysfunction as impaired sensory processing resulting in

    functional problems.

    On the basis of patterns observed in children di-

    agnosed with sensory integration dysfunction, the term

    sensory processing disorder was proposed to increase di-agnostic precision. This nomenclature change was recom-

    mended because of the paradigm shift that has occurred as

    sensory integration theory has evolved. SPD is defined as thebrains inability to organize sensory input for appropriate

    use, and it may be associated with learning, developmental,

    and emotional disabilities (Miller et al., 2007).

    According to the new nomenclature, SPD consists

    of three categories: sensory modulation disorder (SMD),

    sensory discrimination disorder (SDD), and sensory-based

    motor disorder (SBMD). Sensory modulation is the abilityof the central nervous system to effectively alter neural

    messages from sensory input; it allows a person to focus

    on some stimuli and ignore others, therefore responding

    appropriately to the demands of a particular situation.

    In SMD, a mismatch occurs between the demands of the

    environment and a persons emotional and attentional

    responses.

    SMD has three subtypes: sensory overresponsivity

    (SOR), sensory underresponsivity (SUR), and sensory

    seeking (SS). SOR is characterized by faster and more in-tense responses to sensory input from one or more sensory

    systems. These responses may appear uninhibited, aversive,

    or defensive and occur for a longer duration than expected.

    People with SUR often demonstrate a lack of response tosensory input. This inability to detect and respond to sen-

    sory input results in low arousal levels, causing a person to

    seem sluggish, passive, and apathetic. SUR is often associ-

    ated with other SPDs, particularly SDD and dyspraxia

    (discussed later in this article). People who demonstrate SS

    behaviors seek out an abnormal type or amount of sensory

    input. Their insatiable desire for sensory input can lead to

    extreme behaviors that are disruptive, socially unacceptable,

    or unsafe. SMD can involve any sensory system and more

    than one sensory system (Miller et al., 2007).

    SDD is characterized by the inability to interpret

    qualities of sensory stimuli, such as the location of

    stimuli or similarities and differences between stimuli.

    Some people have difficulty identifying stimuli. People

    with SDD can present with motor, learning, or language

    disorders depending on the sensory systems affected

    (Miller et al., 2007).

    SBMD results in postural instability or deficits in vol-

    untary movement caused by problems processing sensory

    input in one or more sensory systems (Miller et al., 2007).

    The two subtypes of SBMD are postural disorder (PD) and

    dyspraxia. PD occurs when sensory processing deficits result

    in poor postural stability. Dyspraxia is defined as an im-

    paired ability to conceive of, plan, sequence, or execute novel

    actions (Miller et al., 2007, p. 138). People with dyspraxia

    may have difficulty generating ideas for action, move

    awkwardly and in a disorganized manner, demonstrate

    inaccurate timing and sequencing of movement, and fail

    to anticipate the movement needed in a particular sit-

    uation. These three categories of SPD are not mutually

    exclusive; a person may demonstrate one or more SPD.

    Prematurity

    Premature birth is birth before the 37th week of gestation.Premature delivery occurs in 13% of all pregnancies

    (Rais-Bahrami & Short, 2013). A child can also be classified

    as premature on the basis of low, very low, or extremely

    low birthweight. Low birthweight is defined as

  • interfere with the sensory development when exposed to

    stimuli (p. 170).

    Many babies who are born prematurely are admitted

    to the NICU. Stimuli in the NICU are both unusual and

    much more intense than stimuli experienced by infants in

    utero (Als & McAnulty, 2011). Bright lights may deter

    them from opening their eyes and taking in the environ-

    ment around them. Babies in the NICU are also subjected

    to necessary aversive procedures such as needle sticks and

    nasogastric intubation. Handling of the infant can lead to

    behavioral stress, and touch may be stressful rather than

    soothing (Nair et al., 2003). Exposure to high-pitched

    sounds can cause physiological changes such as tachycardia,

    tachypnea, apnea, oxygen desaturation, sudden increases in

    blood pressure, the release of stress hormones, and disturbed

    sleep (Graven & Browne, 2008; Nair et al., 2003). Rapid-

    eye-movement sleep deprivation in itself could alter the

    development of sensory systems (Graven & Browne, 2008).

    Research Based on a Sensory Processing Perspective

    Only four published studies have examined sensory pro-

    cessing in infants and young children born preterm from

    a sensory integration theoretical basis (i.e., Bart, Shayevits,

    Gabis, & Morag, 2011; Case-Smith, Butcher, & Reed,

    1998; Wickremasinghe et al., 2013; Wiener, Long, De-

    Gangi, & Battaile, 1996), with three of the four including

    a comparison group of children born at term (i.e., Bart

    et al., 2011; Case-Smith et al., 1998; Wiener et al., 1996).

    Two of the studies were published before the SPD nosology

    was proposed (i.e., Case-Smith et al., 1998; Wiener et al.,

    1996), and their findings were somewhat contradictory.

    Case-Smith and colleagues found little evidence of SPD

    except for tactile overresponsivity. Wiener and colleagues,

    however, found evidence of vestibularproprioceptive

    overresponsivity and some evidence of PD (i.e., 1 of 3

    findings was positive). Somewhat consistent with Case-

    Smith and colleagues, Wiener and colleagues also reported

    some evidence of tactile overresponsivity (i.e., 3 of 6 find-

    ings were positive). Although both studies tested children in

    the age range of 718 mo, they used different instruments

    to measure sensory processing (i.e., the Test of Sensory

    Functions in Infants [TSFI; DeGangi & Greenspan, 1989]

    in Wiener et al. and the Sensory Rating Scale [SRS; Provost

    & Oetter, 1993] in Case-Smith et al.). In both studies,

    limitations of the instruments were identified as factors that

    may have influenced the results.

    In a more recent study, Bart and colleagues (2011)

    used both the Infant/Toddler Sensory Profile (Dunn,

    2002) and the TSFI to assess sensory modulation of in-

    fants born from 3435 6/7 wk gestational age. Similar to

    Wiener and colleagues (1996), they found some evidence

    of SMD in the tactile (i.e., 2 of 3 findings were positive)

    and vestibularproprioceptive systems (i.e., 1 of 2 findings

    was positive). These positive findings tended to be in the

    direction of SOR. In addition, Bart and colleagues found

    evidence of SMD in the auditory system (i.e., 1 of 1 finding

    was positive) and of dyspraxia (i.e., 1 of 1 finding was

    positive).

    Wickremasinghe et al. (2013) found atypical overall

    scores on the Sensory Profile (either the Infant/Toddler

    Sensory Profile or the Sensory Profile [Dunn, 1999],

    depending on childs age) in 39% of a group of children

    ages 18 yr born preterm. Consistent with Bart and

    colleagues (2011), auditory, tactile, and vestibular deficits

    were most common, with relatively few atypical Sensory

    Profile scores on the oral sensory processing and visual

    processing sections. Wickremasinghe and colleagues also

    found that a larger-than-expected proportion of children

    born preterm had atypical scores in each of the four

    quadrants of the Sensory Profile (i.e., low registration,

    sensory seeking, sensory sensitivity, and sensory avoiding);

    however, unlike the other three studies, the largest per-

    centage of participants (24%) in Wickremasinghe and

    colleagues study demonstrated low registration, which is

    consistent with SUR rather than SOR. In the other three

    quadrants, 10%11% of participants had atypical scores.

    This systematic review could add to the literature in

    several ways. Compilation of evidence from multiple

    studies may provide a more reliable representation of the

    sensory processing characteristics of infants and young

    children born preterm than evidence from a single study

    with a relatively small number of participants. Findings

    from the studies discussed previously show some similar

    trends but also inconsistencies.

    Further, Bart and colleagues (2011) reported a dif-

    ference in their findings based on the instrument used to

    gather the data (the TSFI vs. the Infant/Toddler Sensory

    Profile). Scores on the TSFI reflect ratings of observed

    behaviors, whereas scores on the Infant/Toddler Sensory

    Profile are derived from parent responses to a question-

    naire. Consideration of a variety of types of evidence

    from multiple disciplines, including behavioral obser-

    vations, parent reports, and physiological data, may allow

    for a broader picture of the prevalence and patterns of

    SPD in infants and young children born preterm.

    This review could lend further support for NICU

    adaptations such as reducing sound and light and mod-

    ifying touch stimuli. Implementation of these adaptations

    may help prevent SPD. Moreover, if preterm infants are

    at risk for developing SPD, pediatricians need to be ed-

    ucated to recognize the disorders symptoms and provide

    The American Journal of Occupational Therapy 6901220030p3

  • appropriate referrals for occupational therapy intervention.

    If children born prematurely were regularly screened for

    SPD, early intervention could be implemented to address

    dysfunction and potentially prevent future disability, which

    has both quality-of-life and financial implications.

    Method

    Sources

    We independently searched multidisciplinary literature

    using electronic databases, including CINAHL, PsycINFO,

    Medline, Scopus, and PubMed, from 1990 to present. A

    medical research librarian also assisted in developing the search

    strategy and conducting the search. In addition, we examined

    references cited by articles included in the review for potential

    articles. Key search terms, used in a variety of combinations,

    included sensory processing disorder, sensory processing dys-function, sensory integration, sensory underresponsiveness, sen-sory overresponsiveness, sensory seeking, visual processing, tactileprocessing, auditory processing, proprioception, vestibularprocessing, premature, preterm, low birth weight, regulatorydisorders, developmental coordination disorder, temperament,evoked potentials, and 03 yr.

    Inclusion Criteria

    The review included peer-reviewed research articles published

    in English since 1990 that provided evidence of SPD in

    children ages birth3 yr born preterm or with low birth-

    weight, or both. Studies compared children born preterm

    either with a control group of children born full term or

    with normative data. Articles that focused on sensory im-

    pairments (e.g., deafness or blindness) or major motor im-

    pairment such as cerebral palsy were excluded. Data from

    presentations, conference proceedings, non-peer-reviewed

    research literature, dissertations, and theses were also ex-

    cluded. To reach consensus, the five authors discussed

    whether articles met the inclusion criteria.

    Data Extraction and Analysis

    We composed an evidence table of all articles, summarizing

    participant characteristics (chronological age, gestational age,

    and birthweight), outcomemeasures used, findings related to

    SPD, level of evidence, and study limitations. We used SPD

    categories and their descriptions from Miller and colleagues

    (2007) and descriptions and explanations of outcome

    measures provided in each study to categorize data on the

    basis of SPD type and sensory system involved. We con-

    sulted additional literature describing the study instruments

    when necessary. For 23 articles, we used group discussion

    and consensus to classify the findings according to SPD

    type. The first author (Mitchell) classified the remaining 22

    articles. The number of positive (evidence of SPD) and

    negative (evidence of typical sensory processing) findings

    in each category were tallied, and total percentages were

    calculated.

    Results

    Initially, the searches found 81 articles, 45 of which met

    inclusion criteria. Most (91%) of these 45 articles pro-

    vided Level II evidence (nonrandomized two-group

    design), whereas 9% provided Level III evidence (non-

    randomized one-group design). Randomized controlled

    trials were not expected because of the nature of the research

    questions.

    The total number of preterm study participants was

    2,584, with an age range of 1 day to 3 yr, 1 mo; gestational

    age from 22 to 37wk; and birthweight from 450 to 2,865 g.

    Mean values cannot be reported because some articles

    lacked necessary information. Study instruments included

    physiological measures, temperament questionnaires (In-

    tensity, Approach/Withdrawal, and Threshold subtests),

    and behavioral measures (Table 1). Only 3 studies included

    measures based on theories of sensory processing.

    Within the 45 studies, 295 findings related to SPD.

    Of these, 130 (44%) were positive and 165 (56%) were

    negative. These findings were categorized according to

    sensory system affected, SPD, and SPD subtype (Table 2).

    The majority of findings related to SMD. Because of the

    nature of the assessment tools used in the studies, many of

    these findings could not be classified according to SMD

    subtype. For example, neurobehavioral and temperament

    Table 1. Sensory Processing Measures Used by SystematicReview Studies

    Type of Sensory Processing MeasureNo. of SystematicReview Studies

    Physiological measures (e.g., cardiac or respiratoryresponses, event-related potentials, reaction time,salivary cortisol)

    17

    Temperament questionnaires and ratings 12

    Neurobehavioral assessments (e.g., Assessment ofPreterm Infants Behavior, Einstein NeonatalNeurobehavioral Assessment Scale)

    9

    Observations of behavioral reactivity (e.g., facialexpressions, gross body movements)

    8

    Standardized sensory processing instruments (e.g.,Infant/Toddler Sensory Profile, Test of SensoryFunctions in Infants)

    3

    Standardized motor scales (e.g., Neurosensory MotorDevelopment Assessment)

    2

    Study-designed parent reports of sensoryresponsiveness

    2

    Motor planning tasks 2

    6901220030p4 January/February 2015, Volume 69, Number 1

  • Table 2. Sensory Processing Disorder and Typical Sensory Processing Findings According to Sensory System Affected and SensoryProcessing Disorder Subtype

    Sensory SystemAffected

    Sensory ProcessingDisorder Subtype

    Level ofEvidence Authors

    No. of SensoryProcessing

    Disorder Findings

    No. of TypicalSensory Processing

    Findings

    Sensory Modulation Disorder

    Auditory Unclassified SMD II Bart, Shayevits, Gabis, & Morag (2011) 1 0

    II Case-Smith, Butcher, & Reed (1998) 0 1

    II Coles, Bard, Platzman, & Lynch (1999) 0 3

    II Majnemer, Rosenblatt, & Riley (1993) 3 15

    SOR II Fellman et al. (2004) 2 0

    Nonspecific Unclassified SMD II Brown, Doyle, Bear, & Inder (2006) 1 0

    II Coles et al. (1999) 0 2

    II DiPietro, Porges, & Uhly (1992) 0 5

    II Gennaro, Tulman, & Fawcett (1990) 0 2

    II Lester, Boukydis, & LaGasse (1996) 2 2

    II Haley, Grunau, Oberlander, & Weinberg (2008) 6 0

    II Hara Mitsuishi, & Yamaguchi (1990) 0 3

    III Hughes, Shults, McGrath, & Medoff-Cooper (2002) 0 9

    II Ishaik et al. (2000) 0 2

    II Jeng, Yau, & Teng (1998) 1 0

    II Langkamp, Kim, & Pascoe (1998) 0 6

    II Langkamp & Pascoe (2001) 0 4

    II Majnemer, Brownstein, Kadanoff, & Shevell (1992) 1 0

    II Newman et al. (1997) 0 1

    II Oberklaid, Sewell, Sanson, & Prior (1991) 0 8

    II Oberlander et al. (2000) 2 2

    II Parslow et al. (2002) 0 4

    II Sajaniemi, Salokorpi, & von Wendt (1998) 0 1

    II Scher, Steppe, & Banks (1996) 0 6

    III Weiss, St. John-Seed, & Wilson (2004) 0 2

    II Wolf et al. (2002) 26 14

    SOR II Brown et al. (2006) 1 1

    II Coles et al. (1999) 1 0

    II Duffy, Als, & McAnulty (1990) 6 0

    II Gennaro et al. (1990) 4 0

    II Huppi et al. (1996) 4 2

    II Ishaik et al. (2000) 1 0

    II Jeng et al. (1998) 1 2

    II Langkamp & Pascoe (2001) 3 0

    II Sajaniemi et al. (1997) 1 0

    II Wolf et al. (2002) 10 3

    SUR II Brown et al. (2006) 1 1

    II Jeng et al. (1998) 2 4

    II Oberklaid et al. (1991) 1 0

    II Sajanienemi et al. (1998) 1 0

    III Weiss et al. (2004) 1 0

    Tactile Unclassified SMD II Bart et al. (2011) 1 1

    II Grunau et al. (2010) 0 3

    II Hellerud & Storm (2002) 0 2

    II Riese (1998) 0 2

    II Slater et al. (2010) 0 1

    II Wiener, Long, DeGangi, & Battaile (1996) 0 1

    SOR II Abdulkader (2008) 3 0

    II Bart et al. (2011) 1 0

    II Case-Smith et al. (1998) 1 0

    II Hellerud & Storm (2002) 3 0

    (Continued )

    The American Journal of Occupational Therapy 6901220030p5

  • assessments often did not describe the direction of differences

    in sensory responses (greater vs. lesser responsivity); therefore,

    these findings were considered unclassified SMD. Negative

    findings in this category were not considered negative find-

    ings for each SMD subtype because this conclusion would

    result in overrepresentation of negative findings. For all three

    categories (unclassified SMD, SOR, and SUR) in total, 43%

    of findings were positive and 57% were negative. Numbers

    and total percentages of positive and negative findings in

    different sensory systems for each SMD category were

    tallied and calculated (Table 3). Of the SMD findings that

    could be classified, the majority were positive for SOR.

    Very few findings of SUR were identified, and no findings

    were classified as SS.

    Again, because of the assessment tools used in the

    studies (e.g., temperament questionnaires, neurobehavioral

    assessments), a majority (64%) of the SMD findings could

    not be classified according to sensory system. These findings

    Table 2. Sensory Processing Disorder and Typical Sensory Processing Findings According to Sensory System Affected and SensoryProcessing Disorder Subtype (cont. )

    Sensory SystemAffected

    Sensory ProcessingDisorder Subtype

    Level ofEvidence Authors

    No. of SensoryProcessing

    Disorder Findings

    No. of TypicalSensory Processing

    Findings

    II Slater et al. (2010) 1 0

    III Weiss & Wilson (2006) 0 1

    II Wiener et al. (1996) 2 3

    SUR II Grunau, Whitfield, & Petrie (1994) 1 0

    VestibularProprioceptive Unclassified SMD II Bart et al. (2011) 0 1

    II Case-Smith et al. (1998) 0 1

    SOR II Bart et al. (2011) 1 0

    II Wiener et al. (1996) 3 0

    SUR II Bougle et al. (1990) 1 0

    Visual Unclassified SMD II Bart et al. (2011) 0 1

    II Case-Smith et al. (1998) 0 1

    II Coles et al. (1999) 0 2

    II Grunau et al. (2004) 0 3

    II Majnemer et al. (1993) 2 15

    II Mirabella, Kjaer, Norcia, Good, & Madan (2006) 0 1

    SOR II Coles et al. (1999) 1 0

    II Grunau et al. (2004) 1 0

    II Mirabella et al. (2006) 2 0

    Sensory Discrimination Disorder

    Auditory II Fellman et al. (2004) 5 1

    II Hemgren & Persson (2007) 0 1

    Proprioceptive II Hemgren & Persson (2007) 0 1

    Tactile II Hemgren & Persson (2007) 0 1

    II Nevalainen et al. (2008) 1 2

    II Tombini et al. (2009) 1 0

    Visual II Hemgren & Persson (2007) 0 1

    II Oliveira, Costa, de Souza, & Ventura (2004) 0 4

    II Rose, Feldman, Jankowski, & Caro (2002) 1 2

    Sensory-Based Motor Disorder

    Unclassified SBMD II Hemgren & Persson (2007) 0 1

    PD II Newman et al. (1997) 1

    II Wiener et al. (1996) 1 2

    II Wolf et al. (2002) 2 0

    III Yeo & Chan (2005) 4 0

    Dyspraxia II Bart et al. (2011) 1 0

    II DiPietro et al. (1992) 1 0

    II Sun, Mohay, & OCallaghan (2009) 3 1

    II Wiener et al. (1996) 0 3

    Note. PD 5 postural disorder; SBMD 5 sensory-based motor disorder; SMD 5 sensory modulation disorder; SOR 5 sensory overresponsivity; SUR 5 sensoryunderresponsivity.

    6901220030p6 January/February 2015, Volume 69, Number 1

  • were therefore labeled unspecific. Of those that could be

    categorized by sensory system, the largest number of

    findings related to the tactile (30) and visual (29) systems.

    Whereas 79% of the visual system findings were negative

    for SPD, more than half of the tactile system findings

    were positive. All but 2 of the positive tactile system

    findings reflected SOR.

    There were far fewer findings of SDD and SBMD than

    of SMD. Evidence of SDDwas found in 6 studies (see Table

    2). Most of the findings related to the auditory, visual, and

    tactile systems. Whereas the majority of findings for visual

    discrimination were negative, there were more positive than

    negative findings for auditory discrimination. For tactile

    discrimination, there were close to equal numbers of positive

    and negative findings (see Table 3).

    Evidence of SBMD was found in 8 articles (see Table

    2). One finding could not be classified by subtype and

    therefore was considered unclassified SBMD. This finding

    was negative. For PD, there were 7 positive and 3 negative

    findings, and for dyspraxia, there were 5 positive and 4

    negative findings.

    Findings from research articles that used the three most

    common types of instrumentsphysiological measures,

    temperament questionnaires, and neurobehavioral assess-

    ments (see Table 1)were examined separately. Findings

    from the physiological measures and neurobehavioral as-

    sessments mirrored the overall findings; however, the large

    majority (46 of 57, or 81%) of findings from temperament

    questionnaires were negative. Of the 11 positive findings,

    9 suggested SOR and 2 suggested SUR.

    In summary, of the 295 findings from the 45 articles

    included in the review, 44% were positive and 56% were

    negative for SPD. The majority of findings related to SMD,

    with 43% of those being positive. The most prevalent

    subcategory of SMD was SOR, and the majority of these

    findings were positive. Evidence of SUR and SS, SDD, and

    SBMD was limited.

    Discussion

    Although the results of this systematic review appear

    mixed, a substantive proportion of the findings suggests

    children ages birth3 yr born preterm may be at risk for

    sensory processing dysfunctions such as those defined by

    Miller et al. (2007). Evidence of SMD, specifically SOR,

    was most prominent, which seems logical considering the

    immaturity of newborns sensory systems and the po-

    tentially overwhelming and aversive stimuli to which they

    may be exposed in the NICU. These results are also

    consistent with studies by Case-Smith and colleagues

    (1998), Wiener and colleagues (1996), and Bart and

    colleagues (2011), who based their research on a sensory

    integration theoretical perspective and reported SOR in

    infants and young children born preterm.

    The preponderance of SOR findings in this review is

    in contrast to Wickremasinghe and colleagues (2013)

    study, which found a greater incidence of SUR in chil-

    dren ages 18 yr born preterm. Several explanations are

    possible for this difference. For example, this review included

    studies published as early as 1990; therefore, some of the

    participants in the early studies may have been exposed to

    NICUs that had not yet implemented developmental care

    and environmental modification. Changes in NICU envi-

    ronments over the years may have resulted in differences in

    sensory processing outcomes for infants and young children

    born prematurely. However, a more recent study by Bart

    and colleagues (2011) found evidence of SOR in children

    born preterm, which is consistent with this review.

    Another potential explanation for the difference in

    Wickremasinghe and colleagues (2013) findings could in-

    volve the outcome measures used. This review included

    a variety of measuresincluding evoked potentials, behav-

    ioral observations, and temperament questionnaires com-

    pleted by parentswhereas Wickremasinghe and colleagues

    used the Sensory Profile, which involves only parent

    Table 3. Number of Positive (1) and Negative () Findings of Sensory Modulation Disorder and Sensory Discrimination Disorder inDifferent Sensory Systems

    Sensory System

    Sensory Modulation Disorder

    Unclassified SMD SOR SUR

    SensoryDiscrimination

    Disorder

    1 1 1 1

    Auditory 4 19 2 0 0 0 5 2

    Tactile 1 10 14 4 1 0 2 3

    Vestibularproprioceptive 0 2 4 0 1 0 0 1

    Visual 2 23 4 0 0 0 1 7

    Unspecific 39 73 32 8 6 5 0 0

    Total, n 46 127 56 12 8 5 8 13

    Total, % 27 73 82 18 62 38 38 62

    Note. SDD 5 sensory discrimination disorder; SMD 5 sensory modulation disorder; SOR 5 sensory overresponsivity; SUR 5 sensory underresponsivity.

    The American Journal of Occupational Therapy 6901220030p7

  • responses to a questionnaire. Some parents inWickremasinghe

    and colleagues study may not have recognized behaviors in-

    dicating SOR. For example, some children who are over-

    responsive may shut down as a way of coping with their

    overresponsivity (Schaaf et al., 2010). Therefore, what may

    have appeared to a parent to be underresponsivity may

    actually have been overresponsivity. This reviews inclusion

    of studies that used physiological measures and behavioral

    observations may have provided a clearer picture of sensory

    processing than a single study using one type of outcome

    measure. Finally, this review focused on children ages birth

    3 yr, whereas Wickremasinghe and colleagues participants

    ranged in age from 1 to 8 yr. Wickremasinghe and col-

    leagues reported a trend toward a greater incidence of

    atypical Sensory Profile scores in older children and sug-

    gested SPD may become more evident with age.

    The SS pattern was not found in the literature in-

    cluded in this review, except for a mention in 1 study. On

    the basis of analysis of two SRS items, Case-Smith and

    colleagues (1998) reported that SS behaviors were com-

    mon in children born preterm; however, they considered

    these to be age-appropriate behaviors. This lack of SS

    findings may also be related to the tools used in the

    studies. For example, Wickremasinghe and colleagues

    (2013) study used the Sensory Profile, which does iden-

    tify the SS pattern, and found that a statistically larger

    proportion of participants than expected (11%) demon-

    strated SS behaviors. Further research using standardized

    measures that identify the SS pattern is needed before

    drawing conclusions about its prevalence in children ages

    birth3 yr born preterm.

    Evidence related to SDD and SBMD was also scarce.

    These patterns were difficult to identify in the current

    review because of limits in the number and variety of per-

    ceptual andmotor skills demonstrated by children ages birth

    3 yr and tested by the tools for children in this age range.

    Thus, the outcome measures used often provided limited

    findings that could be classified as SDD or SBMD. Even if

    evidence of mild motor disorders was present, it was often

    difficult to determine whether they were sensory-based dis-

    orders because the instruments used in the studies usually

    did not measure tactile, vestibular, or proprioceptive pro-

    cessing. Again, research using measures consistent with the

    SPD nosology is needed to clarify the incidence of these

    patterns in children ages birth3 yr born preterm.

    This review revealed a difference between findings of

    studies that used temperament measures and findings of

    studies that used other types of instruments. Although

    DeSantis, Harkins, Tronick, Kaplan, and Beeghly (2011)

    found evidence to support the idea that sensory processing

    assessments and temperament questionnaires may describe

    the same behaviors using different theoretical perspectives,

    in this review few positive findings were based on the

    temperament measures. Although this difference may sim-

    ply reflect a lack of SPD in infants and young children born

    preterm, it contrasts with the evidence from studies using

    other sensory processing measures. However, the difference

    may relate to the type of SPD most associated with scores

    on temperament questionnaires.

    In a study of children with autism spectrum disorder,

    Brock et al. (2012) found evidence of commonalities

    between five of the nine dimensions of a temperament

    questionnaire and measures of sensory processing. Three

    of the temperament dimensions related to SUR, and two

    related to SMD in general. The researchers found no

    statistically significant effects between SOR or SS and the

    temperament measure. This may explain the difference be-

    tween the findings of studies using temperament measures

    and findings of studies using other types of measures. That is,

    temperament assessments tend to reflect SUR rather than

    SOR or SS and infants born preterm are more prone to

    SOR. Interestingly, in this review, of the few positive findings

    based on temperament questionnaires, most suggested SOR,

    which is consistent with the overall findings of the review.

    Limitations

    The exclusion of unpublished literature and non-English

    studies may have been a limitation of this review because their

    inclusion could have changed the proportions of positive and

    negative findings. In addition, few of the instruments used in

    the included studies were designed to directly assess any of the

    SPD categories defined by Miller et al. (2007). Thus, these

    results were based on inferences drawn from a variety of

    types of data, described previously. To address this challenge,

    at least two of the authors examined and appraised findings

    from a subset of 23 of the 45 studies. All five authors then

    agreed on the classification of the findings from these 23

    studies to ensure the credibility of the classification of the

    remaining articles. Finally, many of the studies tested rela-

    tively small convenience samples, with limited power to

    detect differences between groups. Despite these limi-

    tations, the substantive number of positive SPD findings

    (particularly in the SMD category) has implications for

    occupational therapy practice with children ages birth

    3 yr born preterm.

    Recommendations for Future Research

    Further research examining the prevalence of SPD in

    infants born prematurely would be beneficial. This re-

    search should be based on a sensory processing theoretical

    6901220030p8 January/February 2015, Volume 69, Number 1

  • perspective and use instruments that allow detection of

    SPD patterns. Multisite studies with large samples of

    participants and examiners blinded to term or preterm

    status would also increase the reliability of the findings.

    Expansion of the review to include preschool- and

    school-age children may provide an opportunity to detect

    SPD patterns difficult to identify in children ages birth

    3 yr. Wickremasinghe and colleagues (2013) noted a trend

    toward increasing frequency of atypical Sensory Profile

    scores with increasing age; therefore, they argued, SPD may

    increase in severity or become more evident as children age.

    Implications for OccupationalTherapy Practice

    The results of this review have the following implications

    for occupational therapy practice:

    Children ages birth3 yr born preterm should bescreened for SPD, particularly SOR.

    Parents whose infants are born preterm and spendtime in the NICU may benefit from education about

    SPD, including signs and symptoms to be alert for and

    techniques and approaches to incorporate at home.

    Findings of this study could be used to support theneed for environmental modification in the NICU.

    Education of pediatricians about the risk of SPD inchildren ages birth3 yr born preterm is needed to

    promote follow-up and early intervention services for

    young children whose subtle dysfunctions may other-

    wise be overlooked.

    Conclusion

    The results of this study could help justify the need for

    children born preterm to be referred for post-NICU

    follow-up to assess sensory processing. Infants who are

    born before their sensory systems can fully develop in

    utero and are exposed to atypical, overwhelming, and

    aversive stimuli in the NICU appear to be more at risk for

    SPD, particularly SOR. Occupational therapy can play an

    important role in intervention with this population. Oc-

    cupational therapy practitioners can help identify the risks in

    the NICU and make recommendations for modifications,

    recognize the signs and symptoms of SPD, and provide

    treatment of SPD for children born preterm. s

    Acknowledgments

    The authors thank Brenda Green, medical research li-

    brarian at the University of Tennessee Health Science

    Center, for her assistance.

    ReferencespAbdulkader, H. M., Freer, Y., Garry, E. M., Fleetwood-

    Walker, S. M., & McIntosh, N. (2008). Prematurityand neonatal noxious events exert lasting effects on infantpain behavior. Early Human Development, 84, 351355.http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.earlhumdev.2007.09.018

    Als, H., &McAnulty, G. B. (2011). The Newborn IndividualizedDevelopmental Care and Assessment Program (NIDCAP)with Kangaroo Mother Care (KMC): Comprehensive carefor preterm infants. Current Womens Health Reviews, 7,288301. http://dx.doi.org/10.2174/157340411796355216

    Ayres, A. J. (1989). The Sensory Integration and Praxis Tests.Los Angeles: Western Psychological Services.

    pBart, O., Shayevits, S., Gabis, L. V., & Morag, I. (2011).Prediction of participation and sensory modulation oflate preterm infants at 12 months: A prospective study.Research in Developmental Disabilities, 32, 27322738.http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.ridd.2011.05.037

    pBougle, D., Denise, P., Yaseen, H., Tranier, S., Voirin, J.,Pottier, M., & Venezia, R. (1990). Maturation of periph-eral nerves in preterm infants: Motor and proprioceptivenerve conduction. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neuro-physiology, 75, 118121. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0013-4694(90)90159-H

    Brock, M. E., Freuler, A., Baranek, G. T., Watson, L. R., Poe,M. D., & Sabatino, A. (2012). Temperament and sensoryfeatures of children with autism. Journal of Autism andDevelopmental Disorders, 42, 22712284. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10803-012-1472-5

    pBrown, N. C., Doyle, L. W., Bear, M. J., & Inder, T. E.(2006). Alterations in neurobehavior at term reflect differingperinatal exposures in very preterm infants. Pediatrics, 118,24612471. http://dx.doi.org/10.1542/peds.2006-0880

    pCase-Smith, J., Butcher, L., & Reed, D. (1998). Parents re-port of sensory responsiveness and temperament in pre-term infants. American Journal of Occupational Therapy,52, 547555. http://dx.doi.org/10.5014/ajot.52.7.547

    pColes, C. D., Bard, K. A., Platzman, K. A., & Lynch, M. L.(1999). Attentional response at eight weeks in prenatallydrug-exposed and preterm infants. Neurotoxicology andTeratology, 21, 527537. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0892-0362(99)00023-9

    DeGangi, G. A., & Greenspan, S. I. (1989). Test of SensoryFunctions in Infants. Torrance, CA: Western PsychologicalServices.

    DeSantis, A., Harkins, D., Tronick, E., Kaplan, E., & Beeghly,M. (2011). Exploring an integrative model of infant behavior:What is the relationship among temperament, sensory pro-cessing, and neurobehavioral measures? Infant Behavior andDevelopment, 34, 280292. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.infbeh.2011.01.003

    pDiPietro, J. A., Porges, S. W., & Uhly, B. (1992). Reactivityand developmental competence in preterm and full-terminfants. Developmental Psychology, 28, 831841.

    pDuffy, F. H., Als, H., & McAnulty, G. B. (1990). Behavioraland electrophysiological evidence for gestational age effects

    * Indicates studies that were systematically reviewed for this article.

    The American Journal of Occupational Therapy 6901220030p9

  • in healthy preterm and full-term infants studied two weeks

    after expected due date. Child Development, 61, 271286.Dunn, W. (1999). Sensory Profile. San Antonio, TX: Psycho-

    logical Corporation.Dunn, W. (2002). The Infant/Toddler Sensory Profile. San

    Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation.pFellman, V., Kushnerenko, E., Mikkola, K., Ceponiene, R.,

    Leipala, J., & Naatanen, R. (2004). Atypical auditory

    event-related potentials in preterm infants during the first

    year of life: A possible sign of cognitive dysfunction. Pe-diatric Research, 56, 291297. http://dx.doi.org/10.1203/01.PDR.0000132750.97066.B9

    pGennaro, S., Tulman, L., & Fawcett, J. (1990). Tempera-

    ment in preterm and full-term infants at three and six

    months of age. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 36, 201215.Graven, S. N., & Browne, J. V. (2008). Sensory development

    in the fetus, neonate, and infant: Introduction and over-

    view. Neonate and Infant Nursing Reviews, 8, 169172.http://dx.doi.org/10.1053/j.nainr.2008.10.007

    pGrunau, R. E., Tu, M. T., Whitfield, M. F., Oberlander,

    T. F., Weinberg, J., Yu, W., . . . Scheifele, D. (2010).

    Cortisol, behavior, and heart rate reactivity to immuniza-

    tion pain at 4 months corrected age in infants born very

    preterm. Clinical Journal of Pain, 26, 698704. http://dx.doi.org/0.1097/AJP.0b013e3181e5bb00

    pGrunau, R. E., Weinberg, J., & Whitfield, M. F. (2004).

    Neonatal procedural pain and preterm infant cortisol re-

    sponse to novelty at 8 months. Pediatrics, 114, e77e84.http://dx.doi.org/10.1542/peds.114.1.e77

    pGrunau, R. E., Whitfield, M. F., & Petrie, J. H. (1994). Pain

    sensitivity and temperament in extremely low-birth-weight

    premature toddlers and preterm and full-term controls.

    Pain, 58, 341346.pHaley, D. W., Grunau, R. E., Oberlander, T. F., & Weinberg,

    J. (2008). Contingency learning and reactivity in preterm

    and full-term infants at 3 months. Infancy, 13, 570595.http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/15250000802458682

    pHara, H., Mitsuishi, C., & Yamaguchi, K. (1990). Temper-

    ament of very low-birthweight infants. Journal of MentalHealth, 36, 7984.

    pHellerud, B. C., & Storm, H. (2002). Skin conductance and

    behaviour during sensory stimulation of preterm and term

    infants. Early Human Development, 70, 3546. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0378-3782(02)00070-1

    pHemgren, E., & Persson, K. (2007). Associations of motor

    co-ordination and attention with motorperceptual devel-

    opment in 3-year-old preterm and full-term children who

    needed neonatal intensive care. Child: Care, Health andDevelopment, 33, 1121. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1365-2214.2006.00625.x

    pHughes, M. B., Shults, J., McGrath, J., & Medoff-Cooper, B.

    (2002). Temperament characteristics of premature infants

    in the first year of life. Journal of Developmental and Be-havioral Pediatrics, 23, 430435. http://dx.doi.org/10.1097/00004703-200212000-00006

    pHuppi, P. S., Schuknecht, B., Boesch, C., Bossi, E., Felblinger,

    J., Fusch, C., & Herschkowitz, N. (1996). Structural and

    neurobehavioral delay in postnatal brain development of

    preterm infants. Pediatric Research, 39, 895901. http://dx.doi.org/10.1203/00006450-199605000-00026

    pIshaik, G., Asztalos, E., Perlman, K., Newton, S., Frisk, V., &

    Rovet, J. (2000). Hypothyroxinemia of prematurity and

    infant neurodevelopment: A pilot study. Journal of Devel-opmental and Behavioral Pediatrics, 21, 172179.

    pJeng, S. F., Yau, K. I. T., & Teng, R. J. (1998). Neurobeha-

    vioral development at term in very low-birthweight infants

    and normal term infants in Taiwan. Early Human Devel-opment, 51, 235245. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0378-3782(98)00035-8

    pLangkamp, D. L., Kim, Y., & Pascoe, J. M. (1998). Temper-

    ament of pre-term infants at 4 months of age: Maternal

    ratings and perceptions. Developmental and BehavioralPediatrics, 19, 391396. http://dx.doi.org/10.1097/00004703-199812000-00001

    pLangkamp, D. L., & Pascoe, J. M. (2001). Temperament of

    pre-term infants at 9 months of age. Ambulatory ChildHealth, 7, 203212. http://dx.doi.org/10.1046/j.1467-0658.2001.00131.x

    pLester, B. M., Boukydis, C. E. Z., & LaGasse, L. (1996).

    Cardiorespiratory reactivity during the Brazelton scale in

    term and preterm infants. Journal of Pediatric Psychology,21, 771784. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/jpepsy/21.6.771

    pMajnemer, A., Brownstein, A., Kadanoff, R., & Shevell, M. I.

    (1992). A comparison of neurobehavioral performance of

    healthy term and low-risk preterm infants at term. Devel-opmental Medicine and Child Neurology, 34, 417424.http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-8749.1992.tb11454.x

    pMajnemer, A., Rosenblatt, B., & Riley, P. S. (1993). Influ-

    ence of gestational age, birthweight, and asphyxia on neo-

    natal neurobehavioral performance. Pediatric Neurology, 9,181186.

    Miller, L. J., Anzalone, M. E., Lane, S. J., Cermak, S. A., &

    Osten, E. T. (2007). Concept evolution in sensory inte-

    gration: A proposed nosology for diagnosis. AmericanJournal of Occupational Therapy, 61, 135140. http://dx.doi.org/10.5014/ajot.61.2.135

    pMirabella, G., Kjaer, P. K., Norcia, A. M., Good, W. V., &

    Madan, A. (2006). Visual development in very low birth

    weight infants. Pediatric Research, 60, 435439. http://dx.doi.org/10.1203/01.pdr.0000238249.44088.2c

    Nair, M. N. G., Gupta, G., & Jatana, S. K. (2003). NICU

    environment: Can we be ignorant? Medical Journal ArmedForces India, 59, 9395. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0377-1237(03)80046-1

    pNevalainen, P., Pihko, E., Metsaranta, M., Andersson, S.,

    Autti, T., & Lauronen, L. (2008). Does very premature

    birth affect the functioning of the somatosensory cortex?

    A magnetoencephalography study. International Journal ofPsychophysiology, 68, 8593. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2007.10.014

    pNewman, D. G., OCallaghan, M. J., Harvey, J. M., Tudehope,

    D. I., Gray, P. H., Burns, Y. R., & Mohay, H. A. (1997).

    Characteristics at four months follow-up of infants born

    small for gestational age: A controlled study. Early HumanDevelopment, 49, 169181. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0378-3782(97)01870-7

    6901220030p10 January/February 2015, Volume 69, Number 1

  • pOberklaid, F., Sewell, J., Sanson, A., & Prior, M. (1991).Temperament and behavior of preterm infants: A six-yearfollow-up. Pediatrics, 87, 854861.

    pOberlander, T. F., Grunau, R. E., Whitfield, M. F., Fitzgerald,C., Pitfield, S., & Saul, J. P. (2000). Biobehavioral painresponses in former extremely low birth weight infants atfour months corrected age. Pediatrics, 105, e6.

    pOliveira, A. G. F., Costa, M. F., de Souza, J. M., & Ventura,D. F. (2004). Contrast sensitivity threshold measured bysweep-visual evoked potential in term and preterm infantsat 3 and 10 months of age. Brazilian Journal of Medicaland Biological Research, 37, 13891396. http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/S0100-879X2004000900014

    pParslow, P. M., Horne, R. S. C., Ferens, D., Bandopadhayay,P., Mitchell, K., Watts, A. M., & Adamson, T. M. (2002).Temperament ratings do not predict arousability in normalinfants and infants at increased risk of sudden infant deathsyndrome. Journal of Developmental and Behavioral Pediat-rics, 23, 365370. http://dx.doi.org/10.1097/00004703-200210000-00011

    Provost, B., & Oetter, P. (1993). The Sensory Rating Scale forInfants and Young Children: Development and reliability.Physical and Occupational Therapy in Pediatrics, 13,1535. http://dx.doi.org/10.1300/J006v13n04_02

    Rais-Bahrami, K., & Short, B. L. (2013). Premature and small-for-dates infants. In M. L. Batshaw, N. J. Roizen, & G. R.Lotrecchiano (Eds.), Children with disabilities (7th ed., pp.87104). Baltimore: Brookes.

    pRiese, M. L. (1998). Patterns of reactivity to an aversive tactilestimulus in fullterm and preterm infants. Merrill-PalmerQuarterly, 44, 97113.

    pRose, S. A., Feldman, J. F., Jankowski, J. J., & Caro, D. M.(2002). A longitudinal study of visual expectation andreaction time in the first year of life. Child Development,73, 4761. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/1467-8624.00391

    pSajaniemi, N., Salokorpi, T., & von Wendt, L. (1998). Tem-perament profiles and their role in neurodevelopmentalassessed preterm children at two years of age. EuropeanChild and Adolescent Psychiatry, 7, 145152. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s007870050060

    Schaaf, R. C., Schoen, S. A., Smith Roley, S., Lane, S. J., Koomar,J., & May-Benson, T. A. (2010). A frame of reference forsensory integration. In P. Kramer & J. Hinojosa (Eds.),Frames of reference for pediatric occupational therapy (3rd ed.,pp. 99186). Baltimore: Lippincott Williams & Wilkins.

    pScher, M. S., Steppe, D. A., & Banks, D. L. (1996). Pre-diction of lower developmental performances of healthy

    neonates by neonatal EEG-sleep measures. PediatricNeurology, 14, 137144. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0887-8994(96)00013-6

    pSlater, R., Fabrizi, L., Worley, A., Meek, J., Boyd, S., &Fitzgerald, M. (2010). Premature infants display increasednoxious-evoked neuronal activity in the brain comparedto healthy age-matched term-born infants. Neuroimaging,52, 583589. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.04.253

    pSun, J., Mohay, H., & OCallaghan, M. (2009). A compar-ison of executive function in very preterm and term infantsat 8 months corrected age. Early Human Development, 85,225230. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.earlhumdev.2008.10.005

    pTombini, M., Pasqualetti, P., Rizzo, C., Zappasodi, F.,Dinatale, A., Seminara, M., . . . Agostino, R. (2009). Extra-uterine maturation of somatosensory pathways in preterminfants: A somatosensory evoked potential study. ClinicalNeurophysiology, 120, 783789. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.clinph.2008.12.032

    pWeiss, S. J., St. John-Seed, M. S., & Wilson, P. (2004). Thetemperament of pre-term, low birth weight infants andits potential biological substrates. Research in Nursingand Health, 27, 392402. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/nur.20038

    pWeiss, S. J., & Wilson, P. (2006). Origins of tactile vulner-ability in high-risk infants. Advances in Neonatal Care, 6,2536. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.adnc.2005.11.006

    Wickremasinghe, A. C., Rogers, E. E., Johnson, B. C., Shen,A., Barkovich, A. J., & Marco, E. J. (2013). Children bornprematurely have atypical Sensory Profiles. Journal ofPerinatology, 33, 631635. http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/jp.2013.12

    pWiener, A. S., Long, T., DeGangi, G., & Battaile, B. (1996).Sensory processing of infants born prematurely or withregulatory disorders. Physical and Occupational Ther-apy in Pediatrics, 16, 117. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/J006v16n04_01

    pWolf, M. J., Koldewijn, K., Beelen, A., Smit, B., Hedlund,R., & de Groot, I. J. (2002). Neurobehavioral and de-velopmental profile of very low birthweight preterminfants in early infancy. Acta Paediatrica, 91, 930938.http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1651-2227.2002.tb02858.x

    pYeo, C. L., & Chan, C. (2005). Motor development of verylow birthweight infants with chronic lung diseaseAcomparative study. Annals of the Academy of Medicine,34, 411416.

    The American Journal of Occupational Therapy 6901220030p11

  • Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited withoutpermission.