16
UvA-DARE is a service provided by the library of the University of Amsterdam (https://dare.uva.nl) UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Selling ourselves short? How abbreviated measures of personality change the way we think about personality and politics Bakker, B.N.; Lelkes, Y. DOI 10.1086/698928 Publication date 2018 Document Version Final published version Published in The Journal of Politics Link to publication Citation for published version (APA): Bakker, B. N., & Lelkes, Y. (2018). Selling ourselves short? How abbreviated measures of personality change the way we think about personality and politics. The Journal of Politics, 80(4), 1311-1325. https://doi.org/10.1086/698928 General rights It is not permitted to download or to forward/distribute the text or part of it without the consent of the author(s) and/or copyright holder(s), other than for strictly personal, individual use, unless the work is under an open content license (like Creative Commons). Disclaimer/Complaints regulations If you believe that digital publication of certain material infringes any of your rights or (privacy) interests, please let the Library know, stating your reasons. In case of a legitimate complaint, the Library will make the material inaccessible and/or remove it from the website. Please Ask the Library: https://uba.uva.nl/en/contact, or a letter to: Library of the University of Amsterdam, Secretariat, Singel 425, 1012 WP Amsterdam, The Netherlands. You will be contacted as soon as possible. Download date:03 Apr 2021

Selling Ourselves Short? How Abbreviated Measures of Personality … · of Personality Change the Way We Think about Personality and Politics Bert N. Bakker, University of Amsterdam

  • Upload
    others

  • View
    4

  • Download
    0

Embed Size (px)

Citation preview

  • UvA-DARE is a service provided by the library of the University of Amsterdam (https://dare.uva.nl)

    UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository)

    Selling ourselves short? How abbreviated measures of personality change theway we think about personality and politics

    Bakker, B.N.; Lelkes, Y.DOI10.1086/698928Publication date2018Document VersionFinal published versionPublished inThe Journal of Politics

    Link to publication

    Citation for published version (APA):Bakker, B. N., & Lelkes, Y. (2018). Selling ourselves short? How abbreviated measures ofpersonality change the way we think about personality and politics. The Journal of Politics,80(4), 1311-1325. https://doi.org/10.1086/698928

    General rightsIt is not permitted to download or to forward/distribute the text or part of it without the consent of the author(s)and/or copyright holder(s), other than for strictly personal, individual use, unless the work is under an opencontent license (like Creative Commons).

    Disclaimer/Complaints regulationsIf you believe that digital publication of certain material infringes any of your rights or (privacy) interests, pleaselet the Library know, stating your reasons. In case of a legitimate complaint, the Library will make the materialinaccessible and/or remove it from the website. Please Ask the Library: https://uba.uva.nl/en/contact, or a letterto: Library of the University of Amsterdam, Secretariat, Singel 425, 1012 WP Amsterdam, The Netherlands. Youwill be contacted as soon as possible.

    Download date:03 Apr 2021

    https://doi.org/10.1086/698928https://dare.uva.nl/personal/pure/en/publications/selling-ourselves-short-how-abbreviated-measures-of-personality-change-the-way-we-think-about-personality-and-politics(6e18833a-f94f-4da7-b963-e1417cc6689c).htmlhttps://doi.org/10.1086/698928

  • Selling Ourselves Short? How Abbreviated Measuresof Personality Change the Way We Thinkabout Personality and Politics

    Bert N. Bakker, University of AmsterdamYphtach Lelkes, University of Pennsylvania

    Political scientists who study the interplay between personality and politics overwhelmingly rely on short personality

    scales. We explore whether the length of the employed personality scales affects the criterion validity of the scales. We

    show that need for cognition (NfC) increases reliance on party cues, but only when a longer measure is employed. Ad-

    ditionally, while NfC increases reliance on policy information, the effect is more than twice as large when a longer measure

    is used. Finally, Big Five personality traits that have been dismissed as irrelevant to political ideology yield stronger and more

    consistent associations when larger batteries are employed. We also show that using high Cronbach’s alpha and factor

    loadings as indicators of scale quality does not improve the criterion validity of brief measures. Hence, the measurement of

    personality conditions the conclusions we draw about the role of personality in politics.

    The study of personality and politics has—after lyingrelatively dormant for several decades—received re-newed interest from political scientists (e.g., Bakker,Rooduijn, and Schumacher 2016; Bullock 2011; Feldman andJohnston 2014; Gerber et al. 2010; Malka et al. 2014; Mondakand Halperin 2008). We now see more and more personalitymeasures being included in political science research. Yet spaceis scarce in omnibus surveys—such as the American NationalElection Studies or the British National Election Study—thatare the source for much of the personality and politics re-search. Furthermore, researchers often have limited resourcesat hand when designing their own studies. The trade-offfor limited space and resources is shorter measures. Whiletextbooks on measurement recommend long scales over shortscales (Cronbach 1949), political science research tends to ig-nore this advice (although see Achen 1975; Ansolabehere, Rod-den, and Snyder 2008).

    We turn our attention to personality traits, which are oftenmeasured using one or two items culled from fairly long and

    Bert N. Bakker ([email protected]) is an assistant professor at the University odelphia, PA 19122. Yphtach Lelkes ([email protected]) is an assistant professo

    Study 1 was reviewed and approved by the Institutional Review Board at theCentERdata (Tilburg University). CentERdata abides by the Dutch Protection of P(Directive 95/46/EC). Support for this research (study 1) was provided by the AData and supporting materials necessary to reproduce the numerical results in t/dataverse/jop). An online appendix with supplementary material is available at

    The Journal of Politics, volume 80, number 4. Published online August 22, 201q 2018 by the Southern Political Science Association. All rights reserved. 0022-

    This content downloaded from 145.018All use subject to University of Chicago Press Terms

    often multidimensional scales. As is well known, such shortforms are less reliable (Gosling, Rentfrow, and Swann 2003;Schmitt 1996) and maymeasure only some subdimension of atrait (Cronbach 1949; Smith, McCarthy, and Anderson 2000)leading to either regression dilution or overestimation of theassociation between a trait and a criterionmeasure (Credé et al.2012; Lord and Novick 1968; Smith et al. 2000). Despite theserisks, short personality measures continue to be a mainstay ofpersonality and politics research in political science.

    In this article, we assess the impact of the short measureson substantive conclusions. We focus on two central de-bates within the literature. The first touches on the role ofneed for cognition (NfC)—the tendency to enjoy thinking(Cacioppo and Petty 1982)—in moderating the reliance onparty cues and policy information (Bullock 2011; Kam 2005).The second addresses the association between the Big Fivepersonality traits and political ideology (Gerber et al. 2010;Mondak and Halperin 2008). In our studies, we show thatthe brief measures yield different results than the longer

    f Amsterdam and a visiting research fellow at Temple University, Philar at the University of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia, PA 19104.

    University of Amsterdam. The data for studies 2 and 3 were collected byersonal Data Act, which is consistent with and derived from European lawmsterdam School of Communication Research, University of Amsterdamhe article are available in the JOP Dataverse (https://dataverse.harvard.eduhttp://dx.doi.org/10.1086/698928.

    8. http://dx.doi.org/10.1086/6989283816/2018/8004-0014$10.00 1311

    .170.169 on February 25, 2019 05:11:20 AMand Conditions (http://www.journals.uchicago.edu/t-and-c).

    -

    .

  • 1312 / Selling Ourselves Short? Bert N. Bakker and Yphtach Lelkes

    measures. We also find that a slight increase in the numberof items we use yields outcomes consistent with the moreelaborate measures, which also implies that political scienceresearch need not turn to excessively long measures ofpersonality.

    Our article has important substantive implications. First,although we replicate the main findings of Kam (2005), un-like that article we find that NfC does moderate the relianceon party cues in a way that is consistent with theories of mo-tivated reasoning (Kahan 2013; Petersen et al. 2013); that is,those higher on NfC rely more on party cues. This findingcontradicts theories of bounded rationality (Popkin 1994),which suggests that a party cue is an easy heuristic for thoselow onNfC.We only reach these conclusions if we rely on thefull 18-item battery, while abbreviated batteries—such as atwo-item NfC measure developed by Bizer et al. (2000)—would lead us in line with Kam (2005) to conclude that NfCdoes notmoderate the reliance on party cues. Next, we turn tothe reliance on policy information. The majority of politicalscience studies have relied on an abbreviated two-item NfCmeasure and do not find evidence that those higher on NfCrely more on policy information (Berinsky and Kinder 2006;Holbrook 2006; Mérola and Hitt 2016; Rudolph 2011; Sokheyand McClurg 2012). We show that this conclusion is an ar-tifact of themeasure—using a full 18-item battery, NfC in factmoderates the reliance on policy information, as would bepredicted based the Elaboration Likelihood Model.

    In a third study, we show that, if we rely on a brief measureof the Big Five traits (i.e., 10-item Big Five Inventory [BFI];Rammstedt and John 2007), then we conclude that traits suchas agreeableness, extraversion, and conscientiousness are ir-relevant for politics, while other traits are weakly associatedwith political attitudes. Yet once we rely on a more elaboratebattery (i.e., the 50-item International Personality Item Poolbattery [IPIP]; Goldberg et al. 2006), many of the Big Five per-sonality traits are as highly correlated with the same politi-cal outcomes as openness, the trait perhaps most commonlyshown to be important for politics.

    THE CONSEQUENCES OF USING BRIEFPERSONALITY MEASURESBrief measures of personality offer several advantages overtheir longer counterparts. Short measures of personality arecheaper to administer, increase response rates (Edwards et al.2004), and decrease measurement error that may arise becauseof boredom and fatigue caused by completing a long person-ality battery (Burisch 1984). Since hundreds of questions oftenappear in a single wave of an omnibus survey, space comes at apremium, and brief measures allow scholars to study person-ality when they have limited space available in a survey. For-

    This content downloaded from 145.018All use subject to University of Chicago Press Terms

    tunately, some of the psychometric properties of brief person-ality measures are satisfactory. For the NfC, BFI (Rammstedtand John 2007), and other brief measures of personality, thetest-retest reliability (Gerber et al. 2013; Gosling et al. 2003;Rammstedt and John 2007) and the convergent validity—thatis, the degree to which the brief measure correlates with a lon-ger measure of the same construct—are acceptable (Donnellanet al. 2006; Rammstedt and John 2007).

    Other psychometric properties of brief personality bat-teries are more problematic. First, in all domains of research,it is well known that short batteries tend to be less reliablethan longer batteries (Lord and Novick 1968). The measure-ment error in the independent variables attenuates the rela-tionship with certain criterion (i.e., outcome) measures—a pro-cess called regression dilution.

    Second, personality research is particularly sensitive toshort batteries because of the so-called bandwidth-fidelitytrade-off (Cronbach and Gleser 1965). Bandwidth is theamount of complexity of information in a measure. Many per-sonality traits are fairly complex constructs and thus have ahigh bandwidth. Each of the Big Five traits (openness, con-scientiousness, extraversion, agreeableness, and neuroticism)consists—according to the Five FactorModel of personality—of six lower subdimensions. Conscientiousness, for instance,contains the subdimensions achievement striving, compe-tence, dutifulness, deliberation, self-discipline, and order(Costa, McCrae, and Dye 1991). Short batteries, such as theBFI and the Ten Item Personality Inventory (TIPI; Goslinget al. 2003) may only tap into a few of these subdimensions. Ifthe target measure we are interested in is associated with asubdimension that is missed by the BFI or TIPI, any corre-lation will be attenuated (Credé et al. 2012). Conversely, if atarget measure is only related to one specific facet of that traitbut not others, any correlation will be overestimated (Credéet al. 2012). Accordingly, there is the risk of a type M (mag-nitude) error (Gelman and Carlin 2014). A type S (sign) errormight even occur if our brief measure disproportionately tapsinto a subdimension that is differentially correlated with thecriterion measure than the broader trait.

    SHORT MEASURES WITH LOW BANDWIDTH:THE CASE OF THE NEED FOR COGNITIONAND MESSAGE PROCESSINGNfC captures individual differences in the tendency to en-joy thinking (Cacioppo and Petty 1982). It is possible thatNfC may either increase or decrease the reliance on partycues compared to the same information without party cues.In line with the expectation by Kam (2005), party cues offeran easy heuristic to those who would prefer not to thinkabout the implications about a policy. Therefore, we could

    .170.169 on February 25, 2019 05:11:20 AMand Conditions (http://www.journals.uchicago.edu/t-and-c).

  • 1. The article had received 287 citations by September 5, 2017 (GoogleScholar).

    2. Appendix A.1 (apps. A–C are available online) provides stimuli.3. Kam (2005) conducted her study in Michigan and focused on the

    House of Representatives in the state of Michigan. We conducted ourstudy across the United States, and accordingly the proposed ban on foodirradiation as is discussed in the US House of Representatives.

    Volume 80 Number 4 October 2018 / 1313

    expect that reliance on party cues is higher among thosewho are lower on NfC. However, those who are “the mostvulnerable to ideologically consistent bias” (Hatemi andMcDermott 2016, 342) tend to be cognitively reflective andpolitically sophisticated (Kahan 2013; Slothuus and DeVreese 2010), two traits that are positively correlated withthe NfC (Kahan 2013; Tidwell, Sadowski, and Pate 2000).Therefore, among those higher on NfC, party cues may alsotrigger motivated reasoning (Petersen et al. 2013) and leadpeople to conform to identity-consistent attitudes (Kahan2013; Slothuus and De Vreese 2010).

    Aside from party cues, the literature is fairly clear on theexpectation that NfC should increase the reliance on sub-stantive information. The Elaboration Likelihood Model (Pettyand Cacioppo 1986), which underpins much of the politicalpersuasion literature (Alvarez and Brehm 1995; Johnson andMartin 1998), argues that individual differences in NfC mod-erate the extent to which citizens’ political attitudes are in-fluenced by policy information. Those high on NfC tend bemotivated to understand and thoroughly process informa-tion that they receive and, therefore, should be more affectedby policy information compared to citizens who score low onNfC (Bullock 2011).

    When testing the effects of NfC on information processing,political scientists tend to rely on a two-item variant that wasoriginally developed for inclusion in the 2000 American Na-tional Election Studies (ANES; Bizer et al. 2000). The two-itemANES NfC measure is a highly shortened form of the 18-itemNfC scale (Cacioppo, Petty, and Kao 1984), which itself is a“short” form of the original 34-item scale (Cacioppo and Petty1982). Studies within political science that rely on the 2000ANES measure often fail to find evidence that NfC moderatesthe impact of party cues (Bullock 2011; Kam 2005) or policyinformation (Berinsky and Kinder 2006; Holbrook 2006;Mérola and Hitt 2016; Rudolph 2011; Sokhey and McClurg2012) on political attitudes. Bullock (2011, 513) suggests that“the accumulating non-findings about NfCmay well be drivenby measurement error.” Employing a somewhat larger (al-though not validated) six-item NfC battery, Bullock (2011)finds that NfC does moderate the effect of policy informationon policy attitudes. Yet NfC does not moderate the effect ofpolicy information when Bullock (2011) subsetted the six-itembattery to the two-item ANES battery. Importantly, Bullock(2011) does not compare the six-item measure to a validatedNfC battery, so we have to be cautious in overinterpretingthese results.

    We conduct two studies to determine whether the mea-surement of the NfC conditions the conclusions we reachabout the extent to which the NfC moderates citizens’ ten-dency to rely on party cues (study 1) and policy informa-

    This content downloaded from 145.018All use subject to University of Chicago Press Terms

    tion (study 2). We have formulated competing expectationsof the nature through which the NfC moderate the relianceon party cues, while we expect more reliance on policyinformation by persons high on NfC. Yet in both studies weexpect that the conclusion we draw about the importance ofthe NfC is conditional on the measurement of the NfC.

    Study 1: Need for cognition and party cuesIn study 1, we test whether the NfC conditions the relianceon party cues (Bullock 2011; Kam 2005). Specifically, wecan assess whether those high on the NfC rely more (Kahan2013) or less on party cues (Kam 2005). In order to testthese competing expectations, we replicate one of the mostinfluential papers that assesses the extent to which the re-liance on party cues is moderated by the NfC conducted byKam (2005).1 Participants were randomly exposed to ashort newspaper article introducing a proposal to ban foodirradiation—a low-salience political issue in the UnitedStates. In the first condition, Democrats supported the pol-icy, and Republicans opposed it. In the second condition,Republicans supported the policy, while Democrats opposedit. In the control condition no party cues were mentioned,while all other information remained constant.2 We imple-mented the design directly in line with Kam (2005).3 SurveySampling International (SSI) fielded the experiment in theUnited States on their online panel between July 4 and July 6,2016. In exchange for participation, SSI compensates pan-elists with points that can be exchanged for various rewards.In total, 883 respondents were randomly assigned to par-ticipate in the cue-taking experiment.

    Following Kam (2005), we measured support for the banon food irradiation on a five-point Likert scale ranging from“strongly agree” (1) to “strongly disagree” (5). In order to de-crease measurement error in the dependent variable, we in-cluded two additional items, namely, “The costs of food irra-diation outweigh the benefits,” which was scored on a scaleranging from “strongly agree” (1) through “strongly disagree”(5), and “All things considered, food irradiation is a goodthing,” scored on a scale ranging from “food irradiation is bad”(1) through “food irradiation is good” (5). We created a ad-ditive scale ranging from (0) “oppose a ban on food irradia-tion” to (1) “support a ban on food irradiation” (M p 0:54,SD p 0:20, a p 0:54).

    .170.169 on February 25, 2019 05:11:20 AMand Conditions (http://www.journals.uchicago.edu/t-and-c).

  • 1314 / Selling Ourselves Short? Bert N. Bakker and Yphtach Lelkes

    The NfC was measured with the 18-item battery (Ca-cioppo et al. 1984) using items such as “I prefer complex tosimple problems,” which respondents answered on a Likert-type scale from “extremely characteristic” (1) to “extremelyuncharacteristic” (5). We compute the average score of themeasures (on the original five-point scale) and then, for thesake of comparability, rescale results to lie between 0 and 1 bysubtracting the minimum score (1) and then dividing by themaximum minus the minimum (6). The 18-item NfC batterycan be subsetted to the two-item measure that is included intheANES 2000 (Bizer et al. 2000) as well as the six-itembatteryemployed by Bullock (2011). These NfC batteries—as well asother personality inventories in this article—were rescaled torange from 0 to 1 like we did with the 18-item battery. Ap-pendix A.2 provides descriptive statistics and psychometricproperties of the scales. Randomization checks indicated thatNfC as well as a set of observed background characteristicswere balanced across the treatments (app. A.3).

    Treatment status was indicated by a set of dummy vari-ables indicating whether the participants read that the party

    This content downloaded from 145.018All use subject to University of Chicago Press Terms

    they identify with proposed to ban food irradiation (in-party cue) or opposed the ban (out-party cue). A control con-dition omitted mention of the party label (no party cue).

    While this study replicates the main effect findings ofKam (2005) that citizens rely on party cues (app. A.4), weare interested in the question whether the NfC moderatesthe reliance on the party cues. To investigate when the re-liance on party cues is moderated by the NfC, we estimatedthree ordinary least squares (OLS) regression models. Weset the out-party cue as the reference category and inter-acted the NfC with the in-party cue and no party cue. Fig-ure 1 presents the results of the regression models and plotsthe marginal effect of the in-party cue on support for ban-ning food irradiation over the range of the NfC. Note thathere and throughout the manuscript we report unstandard-ized coefficients; standardized coefficients appear in the ap-pendix.

    In line with Kam (2005), we find that when using thetwo-item ANES measure (fig. 1, left-hand column), the NfCdoes not moderate the effect of the party cue on the policy

    Figure 1. Study 1: need for cognition and party cues. Marginal effects of the in-party cue compared to the out-party cue on support for food irradiation, with

    95% confidence intervals (see app. A.5 for tables with results).

    .170.169 on February 25, 2019 05:11:20 AMand Conditions (http://www.journals.uchicago.edu/t-and-c).

  • 4. Bullock (2011, 503), for instance, suggested that the low Cronbach’salpha of the two-item ANES measure explains the poor performance ofthe short battery.

    Volume 80 Number 4 October 2018 / 1315

    attitude (b p 0:07, SE p 0:08). NfC does moderate the re-liance on party cues when we use an 18-item NfC measure(fig. 1, right-hand column; b p 0:26*, SE p 0:13). In linewithmotivated reasoning, we find that citizens that score highon theNfC relymore on the in-party cue compared to the out-party cue (Kahan 2013; Slothuus and De Vreese 2010). Thesix-item measure does not significantly moderate the relianceon party cues (b p 0:18, SE p 0:12). However, the effect ismore than twice as large when using the two-item measure.The results do not change once we run structural equationmodels (app. A.6), rely on the single-item dependent variableas employed by Kam (2005; app. A.7), or employ the slightlydifferent operationalization of the treatment indicators foundin appendix A.8.

    Our results might be due to the idiosyncrasies of the two-item ANES measure. Perhaps another brief NfC inventorywould result in estimates more consistent with a larger NfCbattery. To estimate whether the effect size is a function of theparticular items or the number of items included in the battery,we turn to a second analysis. Using the 18 items, we generatedall possible combinations for scales of different lengths. Thisresulted, for instance, in 153 two-item measures, 816 three-itemmeasures, 3,060 four-itemmeasures, 8,586 five-itemmea-sures, 18,564 six-item measures, and so on. For each of these262,143 instantiations of NfC, we then calculated the interac-

    This content downloaded from 145.018All use subject to University of Chicago Press Terms

    tion effects between receiving the in-party cue or out-party cueand NfC. Figure 2 plots the distribution of the point estimatesof these measures sorted by the number of items used to gen-erate the trait. Thex-axis indicates thenumber of itemsused tomake a particular trait. The median point estimate is plottedas the thick horizontal in each box plot. The results show thata randomly generated two-item scale yields estimates of aninteraction effect that is roughly 2.5 times smaller comparedto the 18-item scale. The median point estimate of randomlychosen scales does not increase linearly with the number ofitems in a scale, however. The median point estimate of a ran-domly chosen four-item scale is three-fourths the size of the18-item scale, by nine items, the median coefficient estimatethat is roughly 90% of the 18-itemNfC estimate. Study 1 showsthat the ANES measure seems to yield estimates that aresmaller than roughly 70% of any other two-item measure.

    Could short measures be saved if we just more carefullyconstruct them? For low-bandwidth scales, one suggestionis to make sure that the Cronbach’s alpha is at least “sat-isfactory” (greater than .7).4 We show, that Cronbach’s al-pha increases with the number of items, as expected, but

    Figure 2. Study 1: Relationship between interaction effect size and the number of items used to create need for cognition (NfC). Coefficient of the interaction

    effect between NfC and the policy cues and each possible combination of the NfC. Distribution of the point estimates of these measures sorted by the

    number of items used to generate the NfC.

    .170.169 on February 25, 2019 05:11:20 AMand Conditions (http://www.journals.uchicago.edu/t-and-c).

  • 1316 / Selling Ourselves Short? Bert N. Bakker and Yphtach Lelkes

    when we compare scales that contain the same number ofitems, there is no association between the Cronbach’s alphaof a scale and the extent to which estimates are closer to the18-item NfC measure (app. A.9).

    Another common practice when forming a short mea-sure is to select items that load higher in a confirmatoryfactor analysis. This was the method used to develop thetwo-item NfC ANES measure (Bizer et al. 2000). Yet wefind no association between the extent to which items loadhigh on a brief NfC measure and closeness to the estimateof the 18-item measure (app. A.10). This suggests that re-lying solely on Cronbach’s alpha or high factor item load-ings does not result in brief measures of personality with ahigh criterion validity. Instead, study 1 suggests that de-creasing measurement error by increasing the number ofitems is the best strategy.

    Study 2: Need for cognition and policy informationIn study 2, we test whether NfC moderates the effect ofpolicy cues on policy attitudes. The survey experiment—designed by a separate team (Coffé 2013)—was a 2 (policycue: center-right vs. radical-right message) # 2 (ideologicalcue: no ideological cue vs. ideological cue) # 2 (sex: malevs. female politician)# 2 (speech: aggressive vs. nuancedman-ner of speech) experimental design. Participants were ran-domly assigned to be shown a center-right message or radical-right message on a professionally edited campaign video. Forinstance, discussing the issue of immigration, the center-rightmessage was, “We believe that the influx of underprivileged,lowly educated immigrants must stop. Instead, we shouldopen our doors only to higher educated, promising immi-grants,” while the radical-right message was, “We demand acomplete cessation of immigration of people from Islamiccountries.”5 Each treatment lasted approximately two min-utes. Our interest lies in the extent to which participants relyon policy information (center-right vs. radical-rightmessage).

    The experiment was run on the Longitudinal InternetStudies for the Social Sciences (LISS) panel, a true proba-bility sample of Dutch households drawn from the officialpopulation registry (Scherpenzeel and Das 2010) and ad-ministered by CentERdata (Tilburg University). LISS panelmembers fill out one survey each month and get reimbursed€15 per hour. We used three waves of the LISS panel. BetweenOctober 1 and October 30, 2012, 6,434 LISS panel memberswere invited to participate in a survey that contained a cue-taking experiment, and 5,179 panel members completed thesurvey (80.5% response rate). In total 455 (i.e., 8.77%) par-ticipants indicated that they could not hear or could not see

    5. Appendix B.1 provides complete treatments.

    This content downloaded from 145.018All use subject to University of Chicago Press Terms

    the videos in the experiment. These participants were ex-cluded from further analyses.6

    The dependent variable measured the extent to whichcitizens agree with the party position, namely, “How muchdo you agree with the party position on migration by thecandidate?” which was scored on scale ranging from “com-pletely disagree” (1) through “completely agree” (5). We re-coded the scale to range from 0 (completely disagree) to1 (completely agree).

    NfC was measured as part of the Personality and Valuesmodule of the LISS panel that is fielded to LISS panelmembers in May of each year; to increase our sample size,we include NfC from May 2011 (74% response rate) andMay 2012 (79.3% response rate). By merging these waves,we had a measure of the NfC for almost all participants(N p 4; 544). The 18-item NfC battery can be subsetted tothe ANES two-item measure (Bizer et al. 2000) as well asthe six-item battery employed by Bullock (2011). Appen-dix B.2 provides the item wording of the NfC, descriptivestatistics, and psychometric properties of the three NfCmeasures. Randomization checks indicate that the threeNfC batteries are randomly distributed across the differenttreatments (app. B.3).

    Figure 3 presents the results of the regression models andplots the marginal effect of the radical-right message onsupport for the proposed migration policy over the range ofthe NfC. We control for the other conditions in the experi-ment and the interaction between the NfC and these con-ditions. Using the two-itemANESmeasure, we find that NfCmoderates the effect of the policy cue on policy attitudes(b p 20:08, SE p 0:04). That is, the agreement with theradical-right policy decreases compared to the right-wingpolicy as NfC goes up (see left-hand panel of fig. 3). How-ever, when running a similar model using the 18-item NfCbattery, a significant interaction effect that is more thantwice the size of the ANES-based estimate of the interactioneffect emerges (b p 20:17, SE p 0:06). Specifically, the dif-ference in the agreement between the right-wing and radical-right messages increases as NfC goes up (see right-hand panelof fig. 3). Subsetting our 18-item measure to the six-itemmeasure employed by Bullock (2011) yields a result that isslightly stronger than the two-item measure (b p 20:12,SE p 0:05), as can be seen in themiddle panel of figure 3. Theresults for the six-item and 18-item NfC do not change oncewe directly account for measurement error (app. B.5). Whenwe analyze the effect among liberals and conservatives sepa-

    6. The tendency to not be able to hear or see the treatment is ran-domly distributed across the conditions (app. B.3).

    .170.169 on February 25, 2019 05:11:20 AMand Conditions (http://www.journals.uchicago.edu/t-and-c).

  • Volume 80 Number 4 October 2018 / 1317

    rately, the NfC # message interaction is never significantamong liberals, but among conservatives, the results aresimilar to those reported in figure 3 (app. B.6). This is notsurprising given that themessage was either center-right (andappealing to conservatives) or far-right.

    Next, we estimate whether the effect size is a function ofthe particular items or the number of items included in thebattery (fig. 4). As in study 1, these results clearly show theimpact of scale length on the size of the effect. A one-itemscale yields estimates of an interaction effect that is roughlya third of the 18-item scale. The median point estimate ofrandomly chosen scales does not increase linearly with thenumber of scales, however. The median point estimate of arandomly chosen four-item scale is three-fourths the size ofthe 18-item scale, by 9 items, the median coefficient esti-mate that is roughly 90% of the 18-item NfC estimate. TheANES measure seems to yield estimates that are smallerthan roughly 70% of any other two-item measure, indi-cating that the ANES measure is a particularly poor alter-native to the full NfC measure.

    This content downloaded from 145.018All use subject to University of Chicago Press Terms

    As in study 1, we find that high Cronbach’s alpha (app. B.7)or factor loadings (app. B.8) do not lead to measures of per-sonality with a high criterion validity. Instead, study 2 re-affirms that decreasing measurement error by increasing thenumber of items in a battery is the best strategy.

    The NfC is a low-bandwidth measure, and therefore dif-ferences in the criterion validity between the three NfC mea-sures are most likely due to regression dilution, and our errorswere of magnitude (i.e., type M; Gelman and Carlin 2014).Some personality traits—such as the Big Five traits—aremuchmore complex and have a high bandwidth. What are theconsequences of using short measures for these constructs?

    SHORT MEASURES WITH HIGH BANDWIDTH:THE CASE OF THE ASSOCIATION BETWEENPERSONALITY AND POLITICAL IDEOLOGYTraditionally, the Big Five traits—openness, conscientious-ness, extraversion, agreeableness, and neuroticism—are high-bandwidth constructs that were assessed with up to 240 itemscales using convenience samples of university students. How-

    Figure 3. Need for cognition and policy information. Marginal effects of the radical-right message compared to the right-wing message, with 95% confidence

    intervals (app. B.4 provides tables with results).

    .170.169 on February 25, 2019 05:11:20 AMand Conditions (http://www.journals.uchicago.edu/t-and-c).

  • 7. Overestimation—just like underestimation—can occur when rely-ing on brief measures of personality (Credé et al. 2012).

    8. Mondak et al. (2010, app. A) partly address the issue by creating10 possible two-item measures out of the five-item measure of each trait.Yet it is not possible to assess whether their results comport with under-estimation or overestimation of the association because they group bothunderestimation and overestimation in one category.

    1318 / Selling Ourselves Short? Bert N. Bakker and Yphtach Lelkes

    ever, more and more political scientists rely on short mea-sures of the Big Five personality traits. Ten-item personalityinventories such as the BFI and TIPI are now included in theGeneral Social Survey, the International Social Survey Pro-gramme, World Values Survey, the Cooperative Congres-sional Election Study, the ANES, the AmericasBarometer,and the British National Election Studies. The items for a shortmeasure of each Big Five trait are selected so that the shortmeasure reflects the breadth of the original dimension. Ac-cordingly, the interitem correlation is low (Gosling et al. 2003).Moreover, it is difficult to capture all aspects of a high-bandwidth trait using only two items per trait. Necessarily,some aspects of a trait will be underrepresented in a shortmeasure, which limits the content validity of the trait (Credéet al. 2012; Smith et al. 2000).

    The burgeoning literature on the association betweenpersonality and political ideology (e.g., Gerber et al. 2010;Mondak and Halperin 2008) could be particularly proneto the detrimental consequences of using brief measures.Gerber, Huber, Doherty, and Dowling (2011) indicated thatcorrelations between political ideology and the traits neu-roticism, agreeableness, and extraversion were consistentlylarger when measured with the TIPI compared to the 44-itemBFI, while the results for openness and conscientiousness

    This content downloaded from 145.018All use subject to University of Chicago Press Terms

    varied to a lesser degree based on the measure.7 AlthoughGerber et al. ultimately contend that differences between theTIPI and BFI wereminor, they also conclude that “researchersshould be sensitive to the consequences of using differentpersonality batteries for predicting political outcomes” (280).8

    In order to generate a more complete comparison of BigFive measurements and outcomes, we surveyed the pub-lished literature (overview in app. C.1). These studies haveyielded fairly consistent results when it comes to directionand statistical significance (although not strength) for thenegative association between openness and conservatism aswell as the positive association between conscientiousnessand conservatism (app. C.1, table C1). Yet our literaturereview shows that—with the exception of the consistentnegative association between openness and cultural con-servatism (app. C.1, table C2)—there is a heterogeneous pat-tern of associations between the Big Five traits and cultural

    Figure 4. Need for cognition (NfC) and policy information. Coefficient of the interaction effect between NfC and the policy information and each possible

    combination of the NfC. Distribution of the point estimates of these measures sorted by the number of items used to generate the NfC.

    .170.169 on February 25, 2019 05:11:20 AMand Conditions (http://www.journals.uchicago.edu/t-and-c).

  • Volume 80 Number 4 October 2018 / 1319

    conservatism whereby some studies find an association be-tween the Big Five traits and others do not. Likewise, foreconomic ideology, more than half the studies for each traitpoint to an association with economic ideology, while be-tween 30% and 50% of the studies failed to find an association(app. C.1, table C3). With the exception of openness, all traitshave, to different degrees, been regarded as irrelevant forpolitical ideology. Importantly, in the published literature,studies that relied on brief measures—as well as conveniencesamples—are overrepresented in the studies that report noassociations between traits and ideology.

    Study 3: Big Five traits and political ideologyLike in study 2, we rely on the Dutch LISS panel. In December2012, a random subset of the LISS panelists (N p 2; 479) wasinvited to participate in the Dutch module of the 2012 WorldValues Survey (WVS).9 The response rate of the WVS was76.6% (N p 1; 901), and the completion rate was 76.0%(N p 1; 884).

    We combine the WVS with the Personality and Valuesmodule of the LISS panel that is fielded to LISS panel mem-bers inMay of each year and contains ameasure of the Big Fivepersonality traits. To increase our sample size, we include theBig Five data from May 2011 (76.4% response rate) and May2012 (79.6% response rate).10 Our sample was restricted tothose participants who completed the WVS and had at leastonce completed a personality inventory in 2011 or 2012; thisresults in a data set with 1,419 respondents.

    Personality was assessed using two different batteries—the 10-item BFI (Rammstedt and John 2007) and the 50-itemIPIP (Goldberg et al. 2006). When measuring Big Five traits,there are roughly two traditions: inventories that consist ofa series of questions on which respondents rate themselvesand inventories that consist of a set of adjectives on whichrespondents rate themselves. The IPIP is part of the question-based approach, while BFI is an adjective-based approach.However, the IPIP and BFI show good convergent validity(Donnellan et al. 2006). A unique aspect of the 50-item IPIPis that it possible to derive a validated and reliable 20-iteminstrument, the Mini-IPIP (Donnellan et al. 2006).

    The BFI and IPIP were assessed among panel membersin different waves. The BFI was administered as part of theWVS 2012. Participants completed the 50-item IPIP in the Po-litics and Values waves in May 2011 and May 2012. Theitems of both inventories were translated into Dutch by pro-fessional translators, using the translation-back-translation

    9. Data collection ran from December 3 to 31, 2012.10. Data collection ran from May 2 to June 29, 2011, and from May 7

    to June 26, 2012.

    This content downloaded from 145.018All use subject to University of Chicago Press Terms

    method, while the principle investigators of the panel resolvedinconsistencies in the translations. The BFI was measured atthe same time as the criterion measures, while the IPIP wasmeasured before the criterion measures. Since personality traitsare relatively stable over shorter time periods (Gerber et al.2013)—and are stably associated with political attitudes overtime (Bloeser et al. 2015)—the lag between the waves shouldnot affect the nature of the associations reported here. More-over, the strength of the associations between personalityand the criterion measures should be biased in favor of theBFI compared to the measures collected earlier. We createdadditive scales of each of the Big Five traits.11

    We compared the relationships between the personalitytraits, on the one hand, and different dimensions of polit-ical ideology, on the other, as these interrelationships areamong the primary focus of the personality-politics research.Specifically, we focus on a unidimensional operationalizationof ideology (Mondak and Halperin 2008) as well as culturaland economic ideology (Bakker 2016; Feldman and Huddy2014; Gerber et al. 2010). Unidimensional ideology was part ofthe WVS 2012 and measured by asking panelists to ratethemselves on a scale from left (0) to right (10). We recodedthe ideology dimension to range from the most liberal (0; left)to most conservative (1; right) observation (M p 0:51, SD p0:23).

    Cultural and economic ideology were measured using atotal of nine items that were part of the WVS 2012.12 Cul-tural ideology was measured using six items: “I find itshocking if two men kiss in public,” “Gay men and lesbianwomen should be free to live their life as they wish,” and “Ifind it shocking when a man and a woman kiss in public,”all scored on a five-point Likert scale ranging from “stronglydisagree” (1) through “strongly agree” (5); abortion “can al-ways be justified” (1) through “never be justified” (10); “Wherewould you place yourself on a scale from 1 to 5, where 1meansthat euthanasia should be forbidden and 5 means that eutha-nasia should be permitted”; “There are too many people offoreign descent in the Netherlands,” scored on a scale rangingfrom “fully disagree” (1) to “fully agree” (5).

    Economic ideology was measured using three items: “In-comes should be made more equal” (1) through “Individualeffort should be rewarded” (10); “Government should takemore responsibility to ensure that everyone is provided for”(1) through “People should take more responsibility to pro-

    11. See app. C.2 for item wording and app. C.3 for the psychometriccharacteristics of the different personality batteries.

    12. Three items were included from the Politics and Values wave,which was completed by LISS panelists at the same time as the WVS (i.e.,December 2012).

    .170.169 on February 25, 2019 05:11:20 AMand Conditions (http://www.journals.uchicago.edu/t-and-c).

  • 1320 / Selling Ourselves Short? Bert N. Bakker and Yphtach Lelkes

    vide for themselves” (10); “Where would you place yourselfon a scale from 1 to 5, where 1 means that differences inincome should increase and 5 means that these should de-crease?”

    A confirmatory factor analysis demonstrated that thenine items fit within a two-dimensional factor structurewhereby the items load high on the hypothesized ideologydimension (app. C.4). The internal consistency of the cul-tural ideology dimension (a p 0:70) and economic ideol-ogy (a p 0:75) were acceptable. We created a scale rangingfrom the most liberal (0) through the most conservative(1) cultural ideology (M p 0:33, SD p 0:16) and a scaleranging from the most liberal (0) through the most con-servative (1) economic ideology (M p 0:45, SD p 0:20).

    The three ideology dimensions are conceptually distinct.The correlation between unidimensional ideology and cul-tural ideology was fairly weak (r p 0:29), while the corre-lation between unidimensional ideology and economic ide-ology was modestly strong (r p 0:54). Cultural and economicideology were weakly associated with each other (r p 0:10;see also Bakker 2016; Feldman and Johnston 2014).

    For each personality battery, we regressed—using OLSregression models—the criterion measures on each trait aswell as sex, age, education, and income (see app. C.5 for thedescriptive statistics). To make the results easily compara-ble, we plot the unstandardized regression coefficients and95% confidence intervals in one figure with a row for eachtrait and a column for the associations between each ide-ology dimension and the BFI, Mini-IPIP, and IPIP. We dis-cuss the results for the three ideology measures on a trait-by-trait basis.13 The results discussed here do not change if we donot control for education and income (app. C.8) or if we runstructural equation models (app. C.9).

    Higher levels of openness were negatively correlated withconservatism (fig. 5, col. 1) and cultural conservatism (col. 2).However, a study using the BFI would conclude that there isa small negative association between economic ideology andopenness, while a study using the IPIP would conclude thatthere is no association between economic ideology and open-ness (fig. 5, row 1, col. 3).

    There is a consistent positive association between con-scientiousness (fig. 5, row 2) and the unidimensional mea-sure of conservatism (row 2, col. 1) and cultural conser-vatism (row 2, col. 2). However, the association betweenconscientiousness and these ideology dimensions was 1.5–2 times larger when measured using the IPIP compared tothe BFI, which suggests an underestimation of the effect of

    13. Standardized regression coefficients are reported in app. C.7.

    This content downloaded from 145.018All use subject to University of Chicago Press Terms

    conscientiousness. Finally, measurement of conscientious-ness conditions the conclusion we reach about the rela-tionship between conscientiousness and economic conser-vatism (fig. 5, row 2, cols. 2 and 3). We would likely concludethat there was no relationship between conscientiousnessand the economic ideology dimensions because the 95% con-fidence interval of the BFI contains zero. Using the more elab-orate IPIP and Mini-IPIP, conscientiousness and economicconservatism are positive and significantly correlated. Thus,scholars using the BFI underestimate the size of the associationbetween conscientiousness and ideology and might even con-clude that there is no association between conscientiousnessand economic ideology.

    The results for neuroticism show that the trait is con-sistently correlated with the ideology dimensions (fig. 5,row 3). There is no association between neuroticism and aunidimensional measure of ideology, while neuroticism ispositively correlated with cultural conservatism and nega-tively correlated with economic conservatism. Our resultssuggest that the measurement of neuroticism does not con-dition the conclusions we reach about the association be-tween this trait and various ideology dimensions.

    Measurement conditions the substantive conclusions wedraw about the association between agreeableness and thedimensions of ideology (fig. 5, row 4). The negative asso-ciation between agreeableness and conservatism (row 4,col. 1) is two times as large using the IPIP compared to theBFI, while the Mini-IPIP estimate is roughly 50% largercompared to the BFI although not statistical significant dif-ferent.

    The BFI estimate of the relationship between agreeable-ness and cultural conservatism was not significant. Yet thisseems to be a type M error; the relationship between agree-ableness and cultural conservatism is three times larger com-pared to the IPIP. Finally, if we employ the BFI in the study ofeconomic ideology, we would likely conclude that there is aweak negative association with economic conservatism. Thenegative association between agreeableness and economicconservatism is roughly three times as large and significantwhen we use the IPIP. The agreeableness results indicate thatthere is a severe risk of a type M error when using a briefmeasure of the trait.

    Extraversion yielded striking results with both type Mand type S errors occurring. Type M errors are found forthe relationship between extraversion and an unidimen-sional measure of ideology as well as cultural ideology. Usingthe BFI, the association between extraversion and unidimen-sional ideology was negative and not different from zero. TheIPIP—as well as the Mini-IPIP—estimate was positive andmuch larger and statistically significantly stronger compared

    .170.169 on February 25, 2019 05:11:20 AMand Conditions (http://www.journals.uchicago.edu/t-and-c).

  • Volume 80 Number 4 October 2018 / 1321

    to the BFI estimate. Similarly, those using the BFI wouldconclude that the relationship between cultural ideology andextraversion was negative and not significant, and those usingthe IPIP would probably argue that there is no relationshipbetween the two constructs. Finally, a type S error is found forthe relationship between extraversion and cultural ideology.Those using the BFI would find a negative but not differentfrom zero relationship between extraversion and economicideology. Those using either the Mini-IPIP or the IPIP wouldfind a much larger significantly positive one.

    This content downloaded from 145.018All use subject to University of Chicago Press Terms

    Would another brief personality inventory, perhaps, re-sult in estimates more consistent with larger personalitybatteries? Unfortunately, our study does not contain alter-native brief measures. We can, however, following the samelogic employed in studies 1 and 2, generate 10 one-itemmeasures, 45 different two-item measures, 120 three-itemmeasures, 210 four-item measures, 252 five-item measures,210 six-item measures, 120 seven-item measures, 45 eight-item measures, and 10 nine-item measures. We calculatedthe associations between our measures of ideology and each

    Figure 5. Personality and politics: BFI, Mini-IPIP, and IPIP results. Unstandardized ordinary least squares estimates with 95% confidence intervals (see app. C.6

    for tables with results).

    .170.169 on February 25, 2019 05:11:20 AMand Conditions (http://www.journals.uchicago.edu/t-and-c).

  • 1322 / Selling Ourselves Short? Bert N. Bakker and Yphtach Lelkes

    of these 1,022 possible combinations of the trait, controllingfor the 10-item measures of the other four traits.14 Figure 6

    14. We control for the covariates as well as 10-item measures of theother four traits because this yields the most conservative test given thatwe reduce measurement error. It becomes logistically difficult to estimateall possible covariate combinations with all possible criterion combina-tions, as it produces over 1:1# 1015 parameters.

    This content downloaded from 145.018All use subject to University of Chicago Press Terms

    plots the distribution of the point estimates of these mea-sures sorted by the number of items used to generate thetrait. These results clearly illustrate that decreasing the num-ber of items—regardless of the items chosen—generally atten-uates the relationship between a trait and the ideology di-mensions.

    Finally, in line with studies 1 and 2, we show that select-ing items of a scale using Cronbach’s alpha (app. C.10) orfactor loadings (app. C.11) does not lead to better estimates.

    Figure 6. Study 3: relationship between personality and political ideology based on the number of items used to create each Big Five trait. Associations

    between our measures of ideology and each possible combination of each trait. Distribution of the point estimates of these measures sorted by the number

    of items used to generate the trait.

    .170.169 on February 25, 2019 05:11:20 AMand Conditions (http://www.journals.uchicago.edu/t-and-c).

  • Volume 80 Number 4 October 2018 / 1323

    DISCUSSIONThe current “replication crisis” has raised serious questionsabout the validity of social science findings. Political sci-entistsmay feel shielded because sample sizes tend to bemuchlarger and more representative than those in psychology,particularly when we use large-N surveys like the ANES, Co-operative Congressional Election Studies, or the WVS. How-ever, as a trade-off for large sample sizes, political scientistsrely on very short measures of various constructs. As we havedemonstrated, this trade-off often leads to different conclu-sions than if our constructs of interest were longer (see alsoAchen 1975; Ansolabehere et al. 2008).

    The general consensus in political science is that NfCdoes not moderate a person’s reliance on cues—a consen-sus that runs counter to the Elaboration Likelihood Model(Petty and Cacioppo 1986). This work overwhelmingly relieson the two-itemANESmeasure, but when longermeasures areused, we have shown in study 2 that those with higher levels ofNfC are more likely to rely on policy information. Addition-ally, our first study indicates that those who exhibit higherlevels ofNfC are alsomore likely to rely on party cues, a findingthat runs counter to the Elaboration Likelihood Model andKam’s (2005) expectations but that is consistent with theoriesof motivated reasoning (Kahan 2013; Slothuus and De Vreese2010).

    Turning to the Big Five and ideology literature, we haveshown that—with the exception of neuroticism—the associ-ation between personality and political dimensions is highlyconditional on the measurement of personality. We foundthat the 50-item IPIP yields associations with ideology thatare twice as strong as the associations produced by the BFI.In a few instances, the BFI yields estimates of the opposite signto those of the 50-itemmeasure, which suggests the possibilityof a type S error. Traits that have largely been dismissed asirrelevant for the study of politics and personality—such asextraversion and agreeableness—are as strongly correlatedwith our outcomemeasures as those that are focal to the field.Our study thus shows that relying on a larger Big Five batterywould yield different conclusions about which traits arecorrelated with political ideology.

    This study is not without its limitations. One could ar-gue that differences in criterion validity between the BFIand the IPIP in study 3 are actually not explained by thelength of the battery but by the measurement tradition (i.e.,adjectives vs. sentences). However, our randomly generatedtwo-item IPIP measures in figure 6 show the same poorcriterion validity as the adjective-based BFI. Yet, in orderto rule out this alternative explanation completely, futurestudies should collect data that contain brief and elaborate

    This content downloaded from 145.018All use subject to University of Chicago Press Terms

    measures that are based on the questionnaire and adjectiveapproach.

    Additionally, when asking questions about the crite-rion validity, studies should ideally use some gold standard,wherein they compare self-reported behavior with an actualbehavior such as the study of electoral participation (e.g.,Gerber, Huber, Doherty, Dowling, et al. 2011). We do nothave an analogous criterion measure here. This impliesthat we have to be careful in drawing conclusions that theresults of the larger batteries results in better estimates. Wehave reason to believe that the results of the larger batterieslead to estimates closer to the true estimate because of thesuperior measurement properties. Yet, we have no way ofproving this point. More research, using independent sam-ples but equivalent measures, should help us to get one stepcloser to understanding the size and direction of the asso-ciation between personality and political ideology. Finally,many studies of personality and politics have been con-ducted among American subjects, and it is possible that ourresults may have been influenced by cultural differencesbecause studies 2 and 3 were conducted in the Netherlands.Future research is well advised to replicate and extend thesefindings in other political contexts such as the United States.

    The renewed interest in personality has also sparkedinterest in a host of other personality inventories that wedid not investigate, such as other popular Big Five inven-tories (i.e., the TIPI; Gosling et al. 2003) or other popularpersonality inventories like the Need to Evaluate and theNeed for Certainty. There is no a priori reason to assumethat other brief measures of personality are less prone to thetype M—and to some extent type S—errors documented inthis study. Yet the conclusions in this study are necessarilylimited to the brief measures of personality we employed.We would welcome future research that assesses the con-sequences of the use of these brief personality measuresbecause it is not possible to generalize our findings to otherbrief measures of personality without a direct empirical test.

    Identifying the particular combination of items that bal-ances validity and scale length is beyond the scope of this ar-ticle. But we can offer a few suggestions for future research.As discussed by Cronbach and Meehl (1955, 300), “manytypes of evidence are relevant to construct validity, includ-ing content validity, inter-item correlations, inter-test corre-lations, test-criterion correlations, studies of stability overtime, and stability under experimental intervention.” Schol-ars tend to follow Cronbach and Meehl (1955) and assess theinteritem correlations (see, e.g., Gerber et al. 2010) and over-time stability of the construct (Gerber et al. 2013). However,more attention should be paid to the criterion validity of a

    .170.169 on February 25, 2019 05:11:20 AMand Conditions (http://www.journals.uchicago.edu/t-and-c).

  • 1324 / Selling Ourselves Short? Bert N. Bakker and Yphtach Lelkes

    construct (for notable exceptions, see Gerber, Huber, Do-herty, and Dowling 2011; Kam and Estes 2016).

    What is the way forward for the personality and politicsliterature in political science? Adaptive tests of personality(Montgomery and Cutler 2013)—such as the implemen-tation of the NfC in the 2016 ANES pilot—save consider-able space in surveys. However, some respondents will stillanswer long measures, which is costly, time consuming,and tiring. Another possibility is to randomly assign k itemsfrom the larger pool of N items. Given that this fulfills the“missing completely at random” assumption, we can thenimpute missing values so that a scale of length N is gen-erated for each respondent (Little and Rhemtulla 2013).

    Finally, we believe there is still value in using the ab-breviated measures that have been implemented on omni-bus surveys. The longitudinal and cross-national nature ofmany of these surveys makes them particularly invaluable.As evidenced by the NfC studies, short measures of low-bandwidth traits seem to accurately gauge the direction ofthe effect if not the magnitude. Hence, we should be moreskeptical of null relationships than non-null relationshipsfrom analyses that use these types of measures. One wayforward would be to replicate results found using high-quality probability samples and brief measures on (lessexpensive) nonprobability samples with longer measures,thereby ensuring both external validity and internal validity.

    Moving forward, we strongly advise against employingthe two-item ANES NfC measure to study the reliance onparty cues or policy information. Regression dilution seemsto seriously affect our results and the conclusions we draw.However, the 18-item NfC measure seems to be overkill. Arandomly drawn six-to-eight-item battery performs moreor less in line with the 18-item measures. While we have to becareful to generalize on the basis of this study, we stronglyadvise researchers to devote some more space in their surveyfor a NfC battery (see, e.g., Arceneaux and Vander Wielen2017; Bullock 2011). Our results suggest that conventions suchas high factor loadings or high Cronbach’s alpha should notper se be the yardstick to select the items. Instead, randomlygenerating a battery of six to eight items would suffice. Yet, wedo urge scholars to pretest their NfC battery while paying closeattention to the criterion validity of their scale.

    Like the NfC, we advise against the use of brief measuresof the Big Five personality traits such as the 10-item BFI.Type M and even type S errors seem to dominate these find-ings. Accordingly, scholars run the risk of disregarding traitsas relevant to politics, when they are in fact relevant. Again, arandomly drawn six-to-eight-item battery per trait—or to someextent relying on the four-item Mini-IPIP—seems to func-tion in line with the larger 10-item battery.

    This content downloaded from 145.018All use subject to University of Chicago Press Terms

    We believe the literature on personality and politics hasarrived at a turning point and should move beyond the useof extremely abbreviated scales of personality. We thereforewelcome the next of generation of personality and politicsresearch.

    ACKNOWLEDGMENTSBoth authors contributed equally to this article. Earlier ver-sions of this manuscript were presented at Tilburg Univer-sity, the University of Amsterdam, the Midwest PoliticalScience Association Meeting (2015), and the InternationalSociety of Political Psychology Meeting (2016). We thankMark Brandt, John Bullock, Scott Clifford, Donald Green,Cindy Kam, Gerard Saucier, Martijn Schoonvelde, KevinSmith,Marco Steenbergen, Claes de Vreese,Mariken van derVelden, the five anonymous reviewers, and editor JenniferMerolla for their comments and suggestions that helped usimprove the article.

    REFERENCESAchen, Christopher H. 1975. “Mass Political Attitudes and the Survey

    Response.” American Political Science Review 69 (4): 1218–31.Alvarez, R. Michael, and John Brehm. 1995. “American Ambivalence to-

    wards Abortion Policy: Development of a Heteroskedastic Probit Modelof Competing Values.” American Journal of Political Science 30 (4):1055–82.

    Ansolabehere, Stephen, Jonathan Rodden, and James M. Snyder. 2008.“The Strength of Issues: Using Multiple Measures to Gauge PreferenceStability, Ideological Constraint, and Issue Voting.” American PoliticalScience Review 102 (2): 215–32.

    Arceneaux, Kevin, and Ryan J. Vander Wielen. 2017. Taming Intuition:How Reflection Minimizes Partisan Reasoning and Promotes Demo-cratic Accountability. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Bakker, Bert N. 2016. “Personality Traits, Income and Economic Ideology.”Political Psychology 38 (6): 1025–41.

    Bakker, Bert N., Matthijs Rooduijn, and Gijs Schumacher. 2016. “ThePsychological Roots of Populist Voting: Evidence from the UnitedStates, the Netherlands and Germany.” European Journal of PoliticalResearch 55 (2): 302–20.

    Berinsky, Adam J., and Donald R. Kinder. 2006. “Making Sense of Issues

    through Media Frames: Understanding the Kosovo Crisis.” Journal ofPolitics 68 (3): 640–56.

    Bizer, George Y., Jon A. Krosnick, Richard E. Petty, Derek D. Rucker, andS. Christian Wheeler. 2000. “Need for Cognition and Need to Evaluatein the 1998 National Election Survey Pilot Study.” Report, AmericanNational Election Studies.

    Bloeser, Andrew J., Damarys Canache, Dona-Gene Mitchell, Jeffrey J.Mondak, and Emily Rowan Poore. 2015. “The Temporal Consistencyof Personality Effects: Evidence from the British Household PanelSurvey.” Political Psychology 36 (3): 331–40.

    Bullock, John G. 2011. “Elite Influence on Public Opinion in an InformedElectorate.” American Political Science Review 105 (3): 496–515.

    Burisch, Matthias. 1984. “Approaches to Personality Inventory Construc-tion: A Comparison of Merits.” American Psychologist 39 (3): 214–27.

    Cacioppo, John T., and Richard E. Petty. 1982. “The Need for Cognition.”Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 42 (1): 116–31.

    .170.169 on February 25, 2019 05:11:20 AMand Conditions (http://www.journals.uchicago.edu/t-and-c).

    https://www.journals.uchicago.edu/action/showLinks?doi=10.1086%2F698928&crossref=10.1017%2FS0003055408080210&citationId=p_15https://www.journals.uchicago.edu/action/showLinks?doi=10.1086%2F698928&crossref=10.1017%2FS0003055408080210&citationId=p_15https://www.journals.uchicago.edu/action/showLinks?doi=10.1086%2F698928&system=10.1111%2Fj.1468-2508.2006.00451.x&citationId=p_19https://www.journals.uchicago.edu/action/showLinks?doi=10.1086%2F698928&system=10.1111%2Fj.1468-2508.2006.00451.x&citationId=p_19https://www.journals.uchicago.edu/action/showLinks?doi=10.1086%2F698928&crossref=10.1037%2F0003-066X.39.3.214&citationId=p_23https://www.journals.uchicago.edu/action/showLinks?doi=10.1086%2F698928&crossref=10.1037%2F0022-3514.42.1.116&citationId=p_24https://www.journals.uchicago.edu/action/showLinks?doi=10.1086%2F698928&crossref=10.2307%2F1955282&citationId=p_13https://www.journals.uchicago.edu/action/showLinks?doi=10.1086%2F698928&crossref=10.1111%2Fpops.12349&citationId=p_17https://www.journals.uchicago.edu/action/showLinks?doi=10.1086%2F698928&crossref=10.1111%2Fpops.12067&citationId=p_21https://www.journals.uchicago.edu/action/showLinks?doi=10.1086%2F698928&crossref=10.2307%2F2111669&citationId=p_14https://www.journals.uchicago.edu/action/showLinks?doi=10.1086%2F698928&crossref=10.1111%2F1475-6765.12121&citationId=p_18https://www.journals.uchicago.edu/action/showLinks?doi=10.1086%2F698928&crossref=10.1111%2F1475-6765.12121&citationId=p_18https://www.journals.uchicago.edu/action/showLinks?doi=10.1086%2F698928&crossref=10.1017%2FS0003055411000165&citationId=p_22

  • Volume 80 Number 4 October 2018 / 1325

    Cacioppo, John T., Richard E. Petty, and Chuan F. Kao. 1984. “The Ef-ficient Assessment of Need for Cognition.” Journal of PersonalityAssessment 48 (3): 306–7.

    Coffé, Hilde. 2013. “It’s All about the Party: Gender, Party Characteristics,and Radical Right Voting.” Single wave study, LISS Data Archive.

    Costa, Paul T., Robert R. McCrae, and David A. Dye. 1991. “Facets Scales forAgreeableness and Conscientiousness: A Revision of the NEO PersonalityInventory.” Personality and Individual Differences 12 (9): 887–98.

    Credé, Marcus, Peter Harms, Sarah Niehorster, and Andrea Gaye-Valentine.2012. “An Evaluation of the Consequences of Using Short Measures of theBig Five Personality Traits.” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology102 (4): 874–88.

    Cronbach, Lee Joseph. 1949. Essentials of Psychological Testing. New York:Harper.

    Cronbach, Lee Joseph, and Goldine G. Gleser. 1965. Psychological Testsand Personnel Decisions. Urbana: University of Illinois Press.

    Cronbach, Lee Joseph, and Paul E. Meehl. 1955. “Construct Validity inPsychological Tests.” Psychological Bulletin 52 (4): 281–302.

    Donnellan, M. Brent, Frederick L. Oswald, Brendan M. Baird, and RichardE. Lucas. 2006. “The Mini-IPIP Scales: Tiny-yet-Effective Measures ofthe Big Five Factors of Personality.” Psychological Assessment 18 (2):192–203.

    Edwards, Phil, Ian Roberts, Peter Sandercock, and Chris Frost. 2004.“Follow-Up by Mail in Clinical Trials: Does Questionnaire LengthMatter?” Controlled Clinical Trials 25 (1): 31–52.

    Feldman, Stanley, and Leonie Huddy. 2014. “Not So Simple: The Multi-dimensional Nature and Diverse Origins of Political Ideology.” Be-havioral and Brain Sciences 37 (3): 312–13.

    Feldman, Stanley, and Christopher Johnston. 2014. “Understanding theDeterminants of Political Ideology: Implications of Structural Com-plexity.” Political Psychology 35 (3): 337–58.

    Gelman, Andrew, and John Carlin. 2014. “Beyond Power Calculations:Assessing Type S (Sign) and Type M (Magnitude) Errors.” Perspectiveson Psychological Science 9 (6): 641–51.

    Gerber, Alan S., Gregory A. Huber, David Doherty, and Conor M. Dowling.2011. “The Big Five Personality Traits in the Political Arena.” AnnualReview of Political Science 14 (1): 265–87.

    Gerber, Alan S., Gregory A. Huber, David Doherty, and Conor M. Dowling.2013. “Assessing the Stability of Psychological and Political Survey Mea-sures.” American Politics Research 41 (1): 54–75.

    Gerber, Alan S., Gregory A. Huber, David Doherty, Conor M. Dowling, andShang E. Ha. 2010. “Personality and Political Attitudes: Relationshipsacross Issue Domains and Political Contexts.” American Political ScienceReview 104 (1): 111–33.

    Gerber, Alan S., Gregory A. Huber, David Doherty, Conor M. Dowling,Connor Raso, and Shang E. Ha. 2011. “Personality Traits and Par-ticipation in Political Processes.” Journal of Politics 73 (3): 692–706.

    Goldberg, Lewis R., John A. Johnson, Herbert W. Eber, Robert Hogan,Michael C. Ashton, C. Robert Cloninger, and Harrison G. Gough. 2006.“The International Personality ItemPool and the Future of Public-DomainPersonality Measures.” Journal of Research in Personality 40 (1): 84–96.

    Gosling, Samuel D., Peter J. Rentfrow, and William B. Swann Jr. 2003. “AVery Brief Measure of the Big-Five Personality Domains.” Journal ofResearch in Personality 37 (6): 504–28.

    Hatemi, Peter K., and Rose McDermott. 2016. “Give Me Attitudes.” An-nual Review of Political Science 19:331–50.

    Holbrook, Thomas M. 2006. “Cognitive Style and Political Learning in the2000U.S. Presidential Campaign.”Political ResearchQuarterly 59 (3): 343–52.

    Johnson, Timothy R., and Andrew D. Martin. 1998. “The Public’s Con-ditional Response to Supreme Court Decisions.” American PoliticalScience Review 92 (2): 299–309.

    This content downloaded from 145.018All use subject to University of Chicago Press Terms

    Kahan, Dan M. 2013. “Ideology, Motivated Reasoning, and CognitiveReflection: An Experimental Study.” Judgment and Decision Making8 (4): 407–24.

    Kam, Cindy D. 2005. “Who Toes the Party Line? Cues, Values, and In-dividual Differences.” Political Behavior 27 (2): 163–82.

    Kam, Cindy D., and Beth A. Estes. 2016. “Disgust Sensitivity and PublicDemand for Protection.” Journal of Politics 78 (2): 481–96.

    Little, Todd D., and Mijke Rhemtulla. 2013. “Planned Missing Data Designsfor Developmental Researchers.” Child Development Perspectives 7 (4):199–204.

    Lord, Frederic M., and Melvin R. Novick. 1968. Statistical Theories of MentalTest Scores. Reading, MA: Addison-Welsley.

    Malka, Ariel, Christopher J. Soto, Michael Inzlicht, and Yphtach Lelkes. 2014.“Do Needs for Security and Certainty Predict Cultural and EconomicConservatism? A Cross-National Analysis.” Journal of Personality andSocial Psychology 106 (6): 1031–51.

    Mérola, Vittorio, and Matthew P. Hitt. 2016. “Numeracy and the Per-suasive Effect of Policy Information and Party Cues.” Public OpinionQuarterly 80 (2): 554–62.

    Mondak, Jeffery J., and Karen D. Halperin. 2008. “A Framework for theStudy of Personality and Political Behaviour.” British Journal of Po-litical Science 38 (2): 335–62.

    Mondak, Jeffery J., Matthew V. Hibbing, Damarys Canache, Mitchell A.Seligson, and Mary R. Anderson. 2010. “Personality and Civic En-gagement: An Integrative Framework for the Study of Trait Effects onPolitical Behavior.” American Political Science Review 104 (1): 85–110.

    Montgomery, Jacob M., and Josh Cutler. 2013. “Computerized AdaptiveTesting for Public Opinion Surveys.” Political Analysis 21 (2): 172–92.

    Petersen, Michael Bang, Martin Skov, Søren Serritzlew, and ThomasRamsøy. 2013. “Motivated Reasoning and Political Parties: Evidencefor Increased Processing in the Face of Party Cues.” Political Behavior35 (4): 831–54.

    Petty, Richard E., and John T. Cacioppo. 1986. “The Elaboration Likeli-hood Model of Persuasion.” In Richard E. Petty and John T. Cacioppo,eds., Communication and Persuasion. New York: Springer, 1–24.

    Popkin, Samuel L. 1994. The Reasoning Voter: Communication and Persuasionin Presidential Campaigns. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

    Rammstedt, Beatrice, and Oliver P. John. 2007. “Measuring Personality inOne Minute or Less: A 10-Item Short Version of the Big Five Inven-tory in English and German.” Journal of Research in Personality 41 (1):203–12.

    Rudolph, Thomas J. 2011. “The Dynamics of Ambivalence.” AmericanJournal of Political Science 55 (3): 561–73.

    Scherpenzeel, Annette, and Marcel Das. 2010. “‘True’ Longitudinal andProbability-Based Internet Panels: Evidence from the Netherlands.” InMarcel Das, Peter Ester, and Lars Kaczmirek, eds., Social and Behav-ioral Research and the Internet: Advances in Applied Methods andResearch Strategies. London: Routledge, 77–103.

    Schmitt, Neal. 1996. “Uses and Abuses of Coefficient Alpha.” PsychologicalAssessment 8 (4): 350–53.

    Slothuus, Rune, and Claes H. De Vreese. 2010. “Political Parties, Moti-vated Reasoning, and Issue Framing Effects.” Journal of Politics 72 (3):630–45.

    Smith, Gregory T., Denis M. McCarthy, and Kristen G. Anderson. 2000.“On the Sins of Short-Form Development.” Psychological Assessment12 (1): 102–11.

    Sokhey, Anand Edward, and Scott D. McClurg. 2012. “Social Networksand Correct Voting.” Journal of Politics 74 (3): 751–64.

    Tidwell, Pamela S., Cyril J. Sadowski, and Lia M. Pate. 2000. “Relation-ships between Need for Cognition, Knowledge, and Verbal Ability.”Journal of Psychology 134 (6): 634–44.

    .170.169 on February 25, 2019 05:11:20 AMand Conditions (http://www.journals.uchicago.edu/t-and-c).

    https://www.journals.uchicago.edu/action/showLinks?doi=10.1086%2F698928&crossref=10.1093%2Fpoq%2Fnfv051&citationId=p_56https://www.journals.uchicago.edu/action/showLinks?doi=10.1086%2F698928&crossref=10.1093%2Fpoq%2Fnfv051&citationId=p_56https://www.journals.uchicago.edu/action/showLinks?doi=10.1086%2F698928&crossref=10.1007%2Fs11109-012-9213-1&citationId=p_60https://www.journals.uchicago.edu/action/showLinks?doi=10.1086%2F698928&crossref=10.1016%2FS0092-6566%2803%2900046-1&citationId=p_45https://www.journals.uchicago.edu/action/showLinks?doi=10.1086%2F698928&crossref=10.1016%2FS0092-6566%2803%2900046-1&citationId=p_45https://www.journals.uchicago.edu/action/showLinks?doi=10.1086%2F698928&crossref=10.1016%2Fj.cct.2003.08.013&citationId=p_34https://www.journals.uchicago.edu/action/showLinks?doi=10.1086%2F698928&crossref=10.1111%2Fj.1540-5907.2010.00505.x&citationId=p_64https://www.journals.uchicago.edu/action/showLinks?doi=10.1086%2F698928&crossref=10.1111%2Fj.1540-5907.2010.00505.x&citationId=p_64https://www.journals.uchicago.edu/action/showLinks?doi=10.1086%2F698928&crossref=10.2307%2F2585665&citationId=p_49https://www.journals.uchicago.edu/action/showLinks?doi=10.1086%2F698928&crossref=10.2307%2F2585665&citationId=p_49https://www.journals.uchicago.edu/action/showLinks?doi=10.1086%2F698928&crossref=10.1111%2Fcdep.12043&citationId=p_53https://www.journals.uchicago.edu/action/showLinks?doi=10.1086%2F698928&crossref=10.1017%2FS0003055410000031&citationId=p_42https://www.journals.uchicago.edu/action/showLinks?doi=10.1086%2F698928&crossref=10.1017%2FS0003055410000031&citationId=p_42https://www.journals.uchicago.edu/action/showLinks?doi=10.1086%2F698928&crossref=10.1016%2F0191-8869%2891%2990177-D&citationId=p_27https://www.journals.uchicago.edu/action/showLinks?doi=10.1086%2F698928&crossref=10.1037%2F1040-3590.12.1.102&citationId=p_68https://www.journals.uchicago.edu/action/showLinks?doi=10.1086%2F698928&crossref=10.1017%2FS0007123408000173&citationId=p_57https://www.journals.uchicago.edu/action/showLinks?doi=10.1086%2F698928&crossref=10.1017%2FS0007123408000173&citationId=p_57https://www.journals.uchicago.edu/action/showLinks?doi=10.1086%2F698928&crossref=10.1146%2Fannurev-polisci-103113-034929&citationId=p_46https://www.journals.uchicago.edu/action/showLinks?doi=10.1086%2F698928&crossref=10.1146%2Fannurev-polisci-103113-034929&citationId=p_46https://www.journals.uchicago.edu/action/showLinks?doi=10.1086%2F698928&crossref=10.1017%2FS0140525X13002562&citationId=p_35https://www.journals.uchicago.edu/action/showLinks?doi=10.1086%2F698928&crossref=10.1017%2FS0140525X13002562&citationId=p_35https://www.journals.uchicago.edu/action/showLinks?doi=10.1086%2F698928&crossref=10.1146%2Fannurev-polisci-051010-111659&citationId=p_39https://www.journals.uchicago.edu/action/showLinks?doi=10.1086%2F698928&crossref=10.1146%2Fannurev-polisci-051010-111659&citationId=p_39https://www.journals.uchicago.edu/action/showLinks?doi=10.1086%2F698928&system=10.1017%2FS0022381611000399&citationId=p_43https://www.journals.uchicago.edu/action/showLinks?doi=10.1086%2F698928&system=10.1017%2FS0022381612000461&citationId=p_69https://www.journals.uchicago.edu/action/showLinks?doi=10.1086%2F698928&crossref=10.1037%2Fh0040957&citationId=p_32https://www.journals.uchicago.edu/action/showLinks?doi=10.1086%2F698928&crossref=10.1017%2FS0003055409990359&citationId=p_58https://www.journals.uchicago.edu/action/showLinks?doi=10.1086%2F698928&crossref=10.1007%2Fs11109-005-1764-y&citationId=p_51https://www.journals.uchicago.edu/action/showLinks?doi=10.1086%2F698928&crossref=10.1111%2Fpops.12055&citationId=p_36https://www.journals.uchicago.edu/action/showLinks?doi=10.1086%2F698928&crossref=10.1207%2Fs15327752jpa4803_13&citationId=p_25https://www.journals.uchicago.edu/action/showLinks?doi=10.1086%2F698928&crossref=10.1207%2Fs15327752jpa4803_13&citationId=p_25https://www.journals.uchicago.edu/action/showLinks?doi=10.1086%2F698928&crossref=10.1037%2F1040-3590.8.4.350&citationId=p_66https://www.journals.uchicago.edu/action/showLinks?doi=10.1086%2F698928&crossref=10.1037%2F1040-3590.8.4.350&citationId=p_66https://www.journals.uchicago.edu/action/showLinks?doi=10.1086%2F698928&crossref=10.1080%2F00223980009598242&citationId=p_70https://www.journals.uchicago.edu/action/showLinks?doi=10.1086%2F698928&crossref=10.1037%2Fa0036170&citationId=p_55https://www.journals.uchicago.edu/action/showLinks?doi=10.1086%2F698928&crossref=10.1037%2Fa0036170&citationId=p_55https://www.journals.uchicago.edu/action/showLinks?doi=10.1086%2F698928&crossref=10.1016%2Fj.jrp.2005.08.007&citationId=p_44https://www.journals.uchicago.edu/action/showLinks?doi=10.1086%2F698928&crossref=10.1037%2Fa0027403&citationId=p_29https://www.journals.uchicago.edu/action/showLinks?doi=10.1086%2F698928&crossref=10.1037%2F1040-3590.18.2.192&citationId=p_33https://www.journals.uchicago.edu/action/showLinks?doi=10.1086%2F698928&crossref=10.1093%2Fpan%2Fmps060&citationId=p_59https://www.journals.uchicago.edu/action/showLinks?doi=10.1086%2F698928&crossref=10.1016%2Fj.jrp.2006.02.001&citationId=p_63https://www.journals.uchicago.edu/action/showLinks?doi=10.1086%2F698928&crossref=10.1177%2F106591290605900302&citationId=p_48https://www.journals.uchicago.edu/action/showLinks?doi=10.1086%2F698928&system=10.1086%2F684611&citationId=p_52https://www.journals.uchicago.edu/action/showLinks?doi=10.1086%2F698928&crossref=10.1177%2F1745691614551642&citationId=p_37https://www.journals.uchicago.edu/action/showLinks?doi=10.1086%2F698928&crossref=10.1177%2F1745691614551642&citationId=p_37https://www.journals.uchicago.edu/action/showLinks?doi=10.1086%2F698928&crossref=10.1177%2F1532673X12446215&citationId=p_41https://www.journals.uchicago.edu/action/showLinks?doi=10.1086%2F698928&system=10.1017%2FS002238161000006X&citationId=p_67