11
MARKETING RESEARCH ON THE SENSUALITY OF PRODUCTS edited by Aradhna Krishna University of Michigan Ann Arbor, Michigan New York london :2_010

RESEARCH Ann ON...self-report), but rather to the value placed on olfactory sensations: food aromas, perfumes, the smells of the natural world, and so forth. Gestalt psychology was

  • Upload
    others

  • View
    1

  • Download
    0

Embed Size (px)

Citation preview

Page 1: RESEARCH Ann ON...self-report), but rather to the value placed on olfactory sensations: food aromas, perfumes, the smells of the natural world, and so forth. Gestalt psychology was

MARKETING R

ES

EA

RC

H O

N THE SEN

SUALITY O

F PR

OD

UC

TS

edited by Aradhna Krishna

University o

f Michigan

Ann A

rbor, Michigan

~~ ~~o~;~~n~~~up N

ew Y

ork lon

don

:2_010

Page 2: RESEARCH Ann ON...self-report), but rather to the value placed on olfactory sensations: food aromas, perfumes, the smells of the natural world, and so forth. Gestalt psychology was

19

Psych

olo

gy an

d S

enso

ry Ma

rke

ting

, W

ith a F

ocu

s on

Food

Paul R

ozin an

d ju

lia M

. Ha

rme

s

The Pow

er of S

ensation and the Perception o

f "Mo

uth

Ob

jects"

One of the hardest reservations to get in the w

orld•is for dinner at El B

ulli, in northeast S

pain, the world's m

ost creative restaurant and by man

y con­

sidered to be the best. The chef, F

erran Adria, essentially the founder of

molecular gastronom

y, has added new dim

ensions to the experience of food. L

argely by changing the physical form of food w

ith high technology, using foam

s, gels, and the like, coupled with an exquisite aesthetic sense of

what w

orks for the human palate, A

dria has created a panoply of new

food experiences. T

here were 34 of them

in a 6-hour m

eal one of us (PR

) had the pleasure o

f consuming a few

years ago. This extraordinary sensory experi­

ence does not need to be marketed, since reservations are essentially unat­

tainable. (It is no

t true that El Bulli is so popular that nobody goes there

anymore!) T

his new w

ave in cooking, spreading around the Western w

orld, gives us an integrated m

ultisensory experience, with a special focus o

n the

texture of foods an

d textural contrasts. T

he smells, tastes, flavors, feels,

sights, and sounds of food provide an

enveloping experience. On

e example

from E

l Bulli: a glass containing at the bottom

a steaming hot, arom

atic extract of pine nuts. T

he hot liquid sits at the bottom of a glass, the top of

which is coated in

a layer of ice. When you sip it, you get all the aro

ma that

comes from

a ho

t aromatic substance paired w

ith the cold temperature that

normally suppresses arom

a: a new an

d unexpected experience. M

olecular gastronom

y sets the stage for a discussion of sensory m

arketing. W

hen

it comes to senses, m

uch of psychology has been focused on

exploring vision an

d hearing.

Vision an

d hearing can legitim

ately be thought to be o

ur m

ost important senses, as indicated by the devastation

303

Page 3: RESEARCH Ann ON...self-report), but rather to the value placed on olfactory sensations: food aromas, perfumes, the smells of the natural world, and so forth. Gestalt psychology was

304 P

aul Rozin and Julia M

. Ha

rme

s

TAB

LE 19.1 T

he H

um

an S

enses

Distance/

Sense

Surface/Internal

Valence

Vision

Distance

Neutral

Hearing

Distance

Neutral

Haptic*

Surface

Negative/neutral!positive

Sm

ell D

istance/internal N

egative/positive T

aste Internal

Negative/positive

Visceral

Internal N

egative M

uscle Internal

Negative/ neutral

Equilibrium

Internal

Neutral

*Including touch, irritation, tem

perature sense, and

pain.

Com

ment

Incorporative

Du

al! incorporative

Incorporative

caused by blindness and deafness. The eyes and e

ars-b

oth

distance recep­to

rs-con

stitute our principal w

ay of finding out about the w

orld arou

nd

us. It is quite easy to generate stim

uli in vision and

hearing, and the very fast response tim

e of the system

allows for exquisite tem

poral control of

stimuli, such as flashes o

f light. In their reasonably "raw" form

s, excluding that very sm

all subset of all visual and auditory experiences that w

e might

call art and music, light and sound are affectively neutral. T

hey serve prin

­cipally to inform

about what is going on in th

e outside world

. The o

ther

senses are often characterized as the "minor" senses (T

able 19.1). Th

ree­

taste, smell, an

d contact/haptic-sensations constitute the core of the food

experience. Since eating is a m

ajor activity of hum

ans, the third most tim

e consum

ing (including preparation of food), an

d the single m

ost economi­

cally important activity o

f humans, the senses that contribute m

ost to the

appeal of food are perhaps not so min

or after all, and certainly w

orthy of

study. Unlike sights and sounds, m

ost tastes, smells, and feels (skin sen­

sations) are positive or negative in valence. Although there is im

portant participation from

sight and

sound, the major aspect o

f eating is mo

uth

sensations, w

hich are a combination of taste, sm

ell, and a number o

f hap­tic m

odalities, including irritation or pain, contact, an

d hot or cold. T

he m

outh is a highly innervated organ, and along with the hands, the only

sense organ that actually m

anipulates the stimulus. W

hat w

e perceive is food objects, a blend o

f taste, smell, and haptic inputs. Indeed, flavor is a

seamless com

bination of taste and sm

ell. The distinctive qualities o

f most

foods are conveyed by odor carried from the m

ou

th to the nose, via the ret­

ronasal route, bu

t the sensation is experienced as coming from

the mo

uth

. H

ence, the surprise when people discover that w

hen they have a head cold that blocks the sense o

f smell, food loses m

uch of its taste. W

hen we eat,

Psychology and S

ensory Ma

rketin

g, W

ith a Fo

cus on Food

305

we do not experience m

odalities, we experience "m

outh objects." These

objects change their pro

perties-tex

tures, tem

peratures, flavo

rs-as we

chew them

, and

they produce a dynamic range o

f sensations in the period

of a few to m

any

seconds that we m

ight describe as a bite, a unit of eating.

The experience of a bite of chocolate m

ay last for m

inutes: In the first

stages, the sight of the w

rapped chocolate, the smell as it is unw

rapped, the feel of the chocolate in the hand; then, the bite itself. T

he initial firm

impact, the grow

ing aroma as the chocolate w

arms in the m

outh and

coats the inner surfaces, the change in texture from

firm to a thick, silky liquid,

the slide down the throat, and the en

du

ring

after-flavor. A bite o

f choco­late is a m

inisymphony o

f experiences. Individuals differ in their acuity w

ith respect to

the various senses involved in eating, as they do in audition an

d vision. In particular, there are

many different bitter receptors in the m

ou

th, and at least a few

are known

to be absent in some individuals based o

n th

e presence or absence of spe­

cific genes. Olfactory acuity varies w

idely, and

the,olfactory sense deterio­rates m

ore with age than m

any other systems, such as the taste system

. A

lthough it is sort of absurd to ask individuals how

important vision o

r hearing is for them

, it is quite reasonable to ask about the imp

ortan

ce of

smell, and w

e have done this (Wrzesniew

ski, McC

auley, & R

ozin, 1999). For exam

ple, inquiring of both B

elgian and

Am

erican college students, we

asked what w

as the worst thing to lose: th

e sense of sm

ell, hearing in one

ear, or the big toe on one foot. About h

alf of respondents th

ou

gh

t losing the sense o

f smell w

as most threatening. O

f course, many did n

ot realize

that food would lose m

ost of its "taste" with

ou

t a sense of smell. W

e devel­oped a m

easure of the im

portance of o

do

r to individuals and fou

nd

wide

variation. Most o

f it was not attributable to olfactory acuity (also assessed

by self-report), but rather to the value placed on olfactory sensations: food arom

as, perfumes, the sm

ells of the n

atural w

orld, and so forth. G

estalt psychology was a m

ajor movem

ent in psychology in the m

iddle o

f the 20th century. Originating in studies o

f perception, it emphasized

the importance o

f context. The role o

f context cannot be exaggerated, but it is still often ignored in research in psychology, perhaps because context m

akes things complicated

. It requires exp

and

ing

the universe of concern

beyond what laboratory experim

enters wan

t to do. It means co

nsidering

a sensory experience in terms o

f its imm

ediate precursors and successors.

The chocolate bite is not captured in a m

om

entary

flash of sensation. T

he m

elt-in-the-mouth process is critical. P

art of the experience of a bite o

f an

egg roll is the change in sensation as on

e bites into different components,

of different crispness an

d different flavors, each producing a m

om

entary

Page 4: RESEARCH Ann ON...self-report), but rather to the value placed on olfactory sensations: food aromas, perfumes, the smells of the natural world, and so forth. Gestalt psychology was

306 P

aul R

ozin and Julia M. H

arm

es

burst of mouth experience. C

lassic taste research using water solutions of

glucose or sodium chloride bypasses these critical contexts, the contexts

that make eating food such a pleasant, often aesthetic experience. S

tudies on the sensory side of eating rarely consider the dynam

ic unfolding of

mouth experiences over sh

ort periods of tim

e (see Hyde and W

itherly [1993]

for an extended discussion of this perspective an

d the papers o

f P

angborn et al. [Larson-P

owers &

Pangborn, 2006; P

angborn, 1980]). F

or the case of eating, an

d by the w

ay also the appreciation of m

ost other products, such as

clothing, the important context extends w

ell beyond the seconds o

f actual exposure. This m

akes it all the more im

por­tant to expand the universe o

f concern, and all the more difficult to do

rigorous experimentation.

A piece of chocolate m

ay taste delicious until one discovers that it was

harvested with child labor o

r contained detectable insect residues. A sub­

tly flavored fish pate may be perceived as exquisite at a fine restaurant but

tasteless at a local diner. Wansink, Payne, an

d N

orth

(2007) report that the sam

e wine is judged to be low

er in sensory quality if it has a label that indicates that it com

es from N

orth Dakota as opposed to C

alifornia. S

ome people w

ho claim to love C

oke and hate Pepsi can

no

t tell them apart

in taste tests. Chicken in

a sauce flavored with chocolate (one variety o

f M

exican mole) m

ay be found distasteful because of the know

n mixture o

f chocolate w

ith a savory food, rather than because of a detached judgm

ent o

f the orosensory experience. Many people th

ink

bottled water tastes

superior to tap water, w

hen they in fact cannot tell them apart. B

ut it is im

portant to realize that to the Coke-lover, P

epsi-hater, who cannot tell

them apart, C

oca-Cola d

run

k from

a properly labeled bottle does taste better than P

epsi (if Coca-C

ola had realized this, they would not have pro­

duced the new C

oke). The taste experience o

f a food, the liking for the

food, includes the broad context in which it is consum

ed. It includes the im

mediate social context; the reactions of those one is eating w

ith influ­ence the experience a person has.

Just as it is unreasonable to thin

k that P

icasso's great artistic output is a result of extraordinary visual acuity he appreciation o

f food is only weakly

based on matters o

f acuity. Like all perception, evaluating food (or cloth­

ing, movies, cars o

r any other product, for that m

atter) involves a blend o

f bo

ttom

-up

and

top

-do

wn

processes. So far as we know

, people who

like the burn of chili pepper an

d people w

ho do no

t are getting the same

irritation signal from th

eir mouth. It is their interpretation that changes.

Furtherm

ore, most chili likers do not enjoy the disem

bodied burn of chili,

Psycholo

gy and Sensory M

arke

ting

, With

a Focus on Food

307

but want that b

urn

in association with th

e flavor of the peppers an

d the

other associated foods. T

he context expands further (see Kass [1994] an

d R

ozin [2007a] for dis­cussions of eating in a cultural context). F

ood is a basic source of nutrition.

That is its fundam

ental function. B

ut in hu

man

cultural history, by a pro­cess w

e describe as cultural preadaptation, the initial purpose of food has

been expanded so that it serves many o

ther functions. T

he aesthetic func­tion is obvious. B

ut it also serves a variety of social functions, as w

ith choco­late gifts in a rom

antic context, as with m

eeting a new person over a m

eal, as in discussing fam

ily issues over the din

ner table. M

eals are occasions, som

etimes the principal occasions, for social interaction. The m

uch longer th

an 1-hour duration of a French dinner is significantly m

ore than

the time

it takes to consume the food. It involves savoring the food, discussing it, and

general conviviality. For som

e minority in

Western developed cultures eat­

ing is like refueling, but for most it is an anticipated pleasure. O

f course, for m

any wom

en in developed Western cultures, it is iln am

bivalent experience: enjoying the sensations but feeling bad about taking in calories.

Food also enters into the m

oral domain, as h

as clearly happened with

alcohol and tobacco in A

merican culture, an

d m

ore subtly now w

ith stig­m

atization of obesity an

d high-fat foods. In o

ther cultural contexts, food

in general has moral im

plications. With

in the H

ind

u caste system

, par­ticular foods, and the social status o

f the preparers of the food, have strong

moral im

plications. Appadurai (1981) describes food as a "biom

oral" sub­stance in H

indu India. T

he specific powerful influence of culture on the appreciation o

f food and the evaluation o

f its sensory properties can be described u

nd

er the generic term

"cuisine." Elisabeth R

ozin (1982, 1983) analyzes cuisine, focus­ing on the actual dishes, in three com

ponents: staple foods, preparation

techniques, and flavor principles. Thus, C

hinese cuisine focuses on rice as a principal staple, the stir-fry technique, and a flavor principle m

ade up of

soy sauce, ginger root, and rice wine. T

he flavor principles, a quintessentially sensory com

ponent, more than any o

ther feature o

f the food, bestow the

ethnic quality on the food. Potatoes m

ade w

ith Chinese flavor principles

taste Chinese (even though potatoes are rarely used in C

hinese cuisine), and potatoes m

ade with M

exican flavor principles (e.g., chili and tomato) taste

Mexican. In addition to the characteristic sensory com

binations, derived from

staples, techniques, and flavor principles that characterize a cuisine,

there are a whole set o

f additional contexts that are p

art of the cultural fram

e of food consumption. T

hese include table manners, the utensils used,

the social organization of eating, and th

e order of courses. H

oward S

chutz

Page 5: RESEARCH Ann ON...self-report), but rather to the value placed on olfactory sensations: food aromas, perfumes, the smells of the natural world, and so forth. Gestalt psychology was

308 Paul R

ozin and Julia M. H

armes

(1989) describes many of these traditions w

ith the term "appropriateness."

Thus, in the U

nited States and many other countries, there are foods par­

ticularly appropriate for breakfast and others for special holidays. There are

combinations of food that are discouraged, such as m

any mixtures of sw

eet and savory substances in m

ost Euro-A

merican cuisines. W

hipped cream

and meat are each typically desired foods, but not appropriate (or liked) in

combination. F

or similar appropriateness reasons, carbonated m

ilk was a

failure on the Am

erican market, as w

as carbonated coffee (coffee soda). So far as w

e can tell, many of these rules are the arbitrary results o

f culinary his­tory, although som

e can be argued to be nutritionally adaptive or enhancing of certain generally appealing aspects of food flavors.

In any particular cuisine, certain foods and flavors find very restricted

uses, and others are widely em

ployed. In Italy, garlic is appropriate on

almost any savory food, as is soy sauce on alm

ost any savory food in C

hina. Coffee, on the other hand, in alm

ost all cultures that consume it, is

narrowly restricted to a hot beverage context; it is rarely used as a flavoring

and virtually never in savory foods.

Sensory P

leasure as a Particular T

ype of P

leasure

Most of w

hat we do, and buy, is m

otivated by either necessity or increas­ing pleasure. A

s wealth increases, the im

portance of maxim

izing pleasure grow

s with respect to m

eeting basic biological needs. Thus, w

hile food constitutes about 50%

of total expenses in developing world countries, it

falls to below 20%

in the developed world. O

f course, there is no com­

plementarity betw

een pleasure and necessity. Food is perhaps the m

ajor dom

ain in which the tw

o motivations interact. A

t least in the developed W

estern world, w

here it has been assessed, flavor (read pleasure) is the m

ajor determinant of food

choice (assum

ing availability and afford­ability). D

epending on the individual and the culture, other prominent

reasons are tradition, convenience, and

perceived healthiness. Given the

central role of m

aximizing pleasure in choice, in food and elsew

here, it is very appropriate to discuss pleasure in the context of sensory m

arketing. M

any of the pleasures of food, that is, of eating food, are rather elem

ental and raw

, and hence can be called sensory pleasures. A

ccording to some fram

eworks (R

ozin, 1999), one can partition plea­sures into three types: sensory, aesthetic, and m

astery. Sensory pleasures

are relatively unadorned, such as the taste of sw

eet, the aroma o

f choco­late, the feel o

f massage, the sensations associated w

ith sexual arousal and

Psychology and Sensory Marketing, W

ith a Focus on Food 309

orgasm. T

hese pleasures are context sensitive; but in the usual experience, w

here the context is positive and appropriate, they produce a rather simple

enjoyment. A

lthough they show adaptation over short periods, they can

be experienced hundreds or thousands of tim

es over a period of m

onths to years w

ithout declining. A good piece o

f chocolate is a sensory pleasure today, tom

orrow, and every day o

f the week for a year. A

esthetic pleasures typically have a sensory root, but are m

ore cognitively elaborated, and more

likely to be acquired over a period of tim

e (note that Krishna an

d E

lder's chapter in this volum

e suggests that sensory pleasures are also cognitively elaborated on; thus the distinction betw

een sensory and aesthetic is a mat­

ter of degree and type of elaboration). They are often m

odality specific, as w

ith the enjoyment of Picasso or M

ozart, but the representations in the

mind/brain that give rise to these pleasures m

ust be many synapses aw

ay from

primary sensory cortical representations. In the dom

ain of food,

the appreciation of fine wines and other elaborated foods constitute sen­

sory derived but yet aesthetic pleasures. A third source o

f pleasure comes

from the sense of m

astery, for example, the accom

plishment o

f being able to perform

something challenging (from

walking, to riding a bicycle, to

playing the piano). But just as aesthetic an

d sensory pleasures are linked,

so too are mastery and aesthetic pleasures. Som

e types of m

astery are not instantiated by skills, but rather by appreciation. A

s one learns to identify different grapes and vintages in the process o

f becoming a w

ine connois­seur, there is a sense of aesthetic m

astery. S

ensory marketing relates m

ost directly to sensory pleasure. How

ever, since sensations are at the root o

f most aesthetic and m

any mastery plea­

sures, all three types of pleasure have a place in sensory marketing. A

sweet

taste may be quite sim

ple and sensory; the experience of chocolate has strong

basic sensory roots, but it can move into the aesthetic dom

ain as one become

sensitive to the subtleties of chocolate aroma and the qualities o

f the mouth­

melting experience and inform

ed about the sources and nature of process­

ing of particular chocolates. The pleasures o

f Mozart are alm

ost incidentally auditory; it is in large part the internal structure, cognitively appreciated, that provides the pleasure. W

e will focus principally on sensory pleasures,

primarily in the dom

ain of food, as we adopt a tem

poral perspective.

Th

e T

em

po

ral D

om

ain

s of S

ensory Pleasure

An

experience can last for a mom

ent, a few m

oments, or an

ho

ur o

r m

ore, for the case of a meal o

r an opera. A m

eal is a natural un

it of eating

Page 6: RESEARCH Ann ON...self-report), but rather to the value placed on olfactory sensations: food aromas, perfumes, the smells of the natural world, and so forth. Gestalt psychology was

310 P

aul Rozin and Julia M

. Ha

rme

s

(Meiselm

an, 2000; Pliner &

Rozin, 2000). In the fo

od

domain, it is p

rob

­ably at the "basic" u

nit level, so th

at we say, for exam

ple, "that was a great

meal." O

n th

e oth

er han

d, w

e are unlikely to lum

p yesterday's lu

nch

and

dinner into a u

nit, an

d hence w

ill rarely assign an affective value to such

a combination. O

f course, there are imp

ortan

t smaller units in th

e food dom

ain, most p

articularly

the bite, the dish, an

d th

e course. Any o

f these sm

aller units can an

d often are given evaluative labels, especially dishes

("the omelette w

as delicious"). D

aniel Kah

nem

an, often in collaboration w

ith B

arbara Fredrickson

(Fredrickson,

2000; F

redrickson &

K

ahn

eman

, 1993;

Kah

nem

an,

Fredrickson, S

chreiber, & R

edelmeier, 1993) has p

rov

ided

a powerful fram

e­w

ork for un

derstan

din

g pleasure in its tem

poral do

main

s (Kah

nem

an,

Wakker, &

Sarin, 1997). T

hey refer to experienced (E), rem

emb

ered (R

), and anticipated (A

) pleasure. The m

eanin

gs of th

ese terms are obvious,

once this classification is expressed. Of course, th

is ER

A fram

ework is

contingent on

the selection o

f the reference unit, that is, the definition o

f the present. F

or example, for eating, is it the bite o

r is it the meal?

Kah

nem

an et al. (1997) have m

ade imp

ortan

t claims about th

e rela­tions betw

een present and

prospect and

between present an

d past. T

he m

ost critical claim ab

ou

t present and

prospect is that people are quite

poor at anticipating future experienced pleasures. That is, o

n the basis o

f the present, they often m

ake p

oo

r predictions abo

ut how

they will enjoy a

particular experience. In the initial stu

dy

by Kah

nem

an an

d S

nell (1992), individuals sam

pled

a flavored yogurt and

rated their liking for it an

d

agreed to eat the sam

e yo

gu

rt every day for a week. A

t the onset, they

were

asked to estimate how

mu

ch they w

ould like the yo

gu

rt after 1 week. T

hen after 1 w

eek of experience, th

ey rated it again. T

he predicted and

actual ratings w

ere essentially uncorrelated. A subsequent stu

dy

(Rozin, H

ank

o,

& D

urlach, 2006) exposed individuals to four new

products (two unfa­

miliar E

ast Asian food snacks and an

un

familiar toothpaste an

d show

er gel). A

gain, accord

ing

to the procedure of K

ahn

eman

and

Snell (1992),

individuals tried an

d rated their lik

ing

for each product, agreed to use each daily, an

d estim

ated w

hat their ratings w

ould be after a w

eek of use.

Again, as w

ith th

e Kah

nem

an results, people w

ere po

or at anticipating th

e changes in th

eir preferences. They often w

ere inco

rrect in even predict­ing the direction o

f change. Tw

o oth

er findings of interest em

erged from

this study. First, relative accuracy at p

redictin

g th

e change for any

on

e product did not p

redict accuracy for p

redictin

g changes for o

ther p

rod

­ucts. S

econd, the p

articipan

ts in this stu

dy

were 20 college stu

den

ts and

20 parents (one p

arent from

each of th

e students). We hypothesized th

at

Psychology and Sensory M

arke

ting

, With

a Focus o

n Food

311

with

age people would have a great deal o

f experience with th

eir hedonic trajectories an

d becom

e better at anticip

ating

hedonic changes. In fact, th

ere was no im

provement at all co

nseq

uen

t on mo

re than

20 additional years o

f experience with oneself.

Th

e inability to predict the effect of ex

po

sure o

n one's fu

ture sensory

and

oth

er preferences is imp

ortan

t for marketing, especially since people

are typically unaware of how

po

orly

they

perform in

this do

main

. Much

of the inaccuracy com

es from overconfidence that th

e present reaction to a n

ew en

tity w

ill be like the future reaction

once it has beco

me fam

iliar. P

eople typically both underestimate ad

aptatio

n (L

oewenstein &

Frederick,

1997) and

underestimate the positive effects o

f mere exposure o

n increas­

ing likin

g. TI1is causes them

, for exam

ple

, to mak

e lon

g-term

comm

it­m

ents to p

rod

ucts (such as an

nu

al sub

scriptio

ns to an

initially engaging m

agazine) on the assum

ption that p

resent responses w

ill be sustained,

or to fail to

give a new p

rod

uct a seco

nd

chance if the initial response is

mildly negative o

r neutral. T

he stud

y o

f the relations betw

een experienced ~nd remem

bered

pleasure by K

ahn

eman

and

others (Frederickson, 2000; F

redrickson & K

ahn

eman

, 1993; K

ahn

eman

et a!., 1993) has b

een a particularly fertile

area. This

research, based almost entirely on h

edo

nically

negative experiences, has led to

three principles th

at represent majo

r distortions of experience th

at o

ccur w

hen the experience is

remem

bered

. The h

edo

nic p

eak (i.e., th

e m

ost h

igh

ly valenced point in th

e experience) and

the h

edo

nic state at th

e en

d p

oin

t of the experience have a p

redo

min

ant influence on th

e mem

ory

for th

e experience (the "peak-end" rule). The p

eak ru

le is often apparent, as w

hen

a few seconds o

f discomfort in

the dentist ch

air completely d

om

i­nate th

e hedonic mem

ory of a h

alf ho

ur o

f more o

r less painless experi­ence. T

he third rule is described as du

ration

neglect: ou

r mem

ory

does n

ot seem

to track duration w

ell and

tend

s to remem

ber events an

d n

ot

their d

uratio

n. A

s a result, a con

tinu

ou

s or repetitive experience is typi­

cally remem

bered

as a single event. Tw

o or eight sips o

f an excellent w

ine

tend

to be rem

embered as the sam

e sipp

ing

of the win

e and

the associated

flavor experience. We have ex

tend

ed th

is work on rem

emb

ered pleasure

into

the positive dom

ains of en

joy

men

t of m

eals (Rode, R

ozin, & D

urlach, 2007), m

usic (Rozin, G

uillot, & R

ozin, in preparation; R

ozin, Rozin, &

G

oldberg, 2004), and

art exhibits (Ro

zin &

Taylor, in preparation). W

e find pow

erful support for du

ration

neglect; for example, d

ou

blin

g th

e size o

f the p

ortio

n o

f the favorite food in a m

eal, wh

ich clearly increases

exp

erienced

pleasure, has no effect on

remem

bered

pleasure. On

the oth

er h

and

, we have not found reliable ev

iden

ce for a peak

effect (a particu

larly

Page 7: RESEARCH Ann ON...self-report), but rather to the value placed on olfactory sensations: food aromas, perfumes, the smells of the natural world, and so forth. Gestalt psychology was

312 P

aul Rozin and Julia M

. Ha

rme

s

strong effect in an

overall evaluation from

the favorite paintings in

an art

exhibit, a favorite musical selection in a "concert," o

r a favorite dish in a

meal). O

ur ow

n experience suggests a strong peak

effect, bu

t we have b

een

unable to find it in controlled laboratory situations. In

these studies, we

have found abo

ut as m

uch

evidence for a prim

acy as an en

d effect. W

e still do not k

no

w th

e conditions under wh

ich peak, end, an

d onset co

me

to dominate an experience. W

e also do no

t kn

ow

the conditions that b

lun

t duration neglect, alth

ou

gh

it is likely that division o

f an event into d

istinct

segments m

ay be on

e factor that reduces it (A

riely & Z

auberman, 2000).

The disparity betw

een experienced and

remem

bered

pleasure has majo

r im

plications for mark

eting

. At the point o

f purchase, say in a food store or

restaurant, we consult o

ur m

em

ory of th

e food or dish in question, since

we d

o n

ot have d

irect access to ou

r past experience with

it. Hence, the rep

­resentation o

f past experiences in

mem

ory

is the critical base for u

nd

er­stan

din

g m

ost curren

t choices. (Obviously, th

is is no

t true in cases w

here

a person is actually samp

ling

the choice o

f foods available or directly co

parin

g item

s of clothing o

r pictures.)

Ind

ividu

al and C

ultural Differences in E

RA

Profiles

Individuals vary in the amo

un

t of tim

e and

imp

ortan

ce they devote to experiencing in th

e mo

men

t, rehearsing mem

ories, and

anticipating the

future (Rozin &

Han

ko

, in preparation). A

great m

eal may last 2 hours, b

ut

its mem

ory

may b

e activated for dozens of h

ou

rs over the following years.

An

d th

e second visit to the source o

f the great m

eal may engage m

any

h

ou

rs of anticipation. W

hen

a person chooses to schedule an anticip

ated

positive event in th

e near o

r distan

t future, he o

r she is mak

ing

a choice about w

hether to increase anticipation, at the cost o

f having less time to

"consum

e" the mem

ory, or reducing anticipation in

order to have mo

re tim

e to remem

ber. People differ in the decisions they m

ake in choices

of th

is sort. So far as ou

r still unpublished data indicate, th

eir pattern

of

favoring anticipation or m

emo

ry tends n

ot to

be general bu

t rather spe­

cific to particu

lar do

main

s (Rozin, H

ank

o, &

Gohar, 2009; R

ozin, Rem

ick

& F

ischler, 2008). Som

e people, faced with

a platter of three foods (say the stan

dard

meat, potatoes, an

d vegetable), consistently eat their favor­

ite food first, oth

ers eat their favorite food last, an

d m

any

do neither. Bu

t the people w

ho eat their favorite food first are no

t more likely to listen to

th

eir favorite music first th

an those w

ho eat their favorite food last (R

ozin, H

ank

o, &

Gohar, in

preparation).

Psychology and S

ensory Ma

rketin

g, With

a Focus on F

ood 313

It is pro

bab

ly tru

e that cultures differ in

the im

po

rtance they b

estow

on

m

emories versus anticipation (perhaps translatable into th

e past vs. th

e future), an

d it is also

likely that the rem

emb

ered or anticipated b

alance

shifts with

age. Old

er people have the sam

e potential experience anticipat­ing a positive event th

at will occur in

the n

ear future, bu

t will h

ave less

op

po

rtun

ity to co

nsu

me the m

emo

ry because o

f a shorter lifespan ahead

. T

he utility

of b

uild

ing

mem

ories declines with

age, even assum

ing

the

acuity of m

emo

ry rem

ains intact! The psychology o

f savoring and

remi­

niscing, and

their tradeoff's, and th

e parallel psychology of d

readin

g an

d

remem

berin

g negative events have m

any

implications for m

arketin

g an

d

for op

timizin

g th

e pleasure of life.

Co

mfo

rts and Joys

Tibor S

citovsky (1992) draws a d

istinctio

n b

etween

what he calls com

forts and pleasures. (W

e thin

k joy is a b

etter wo

rd to 'describe his im

po

rtant

contrast, because it implies a sh

orter tim

e interval, and

we use jo

y to

sub­stitute for pleasure.) In his view

, com

forts m

ake life easier, th

ey are like

good mattresses an

d air conditioning, an

d ice dispensers on refrigerators.

Joys are un

iqu

e events, such as a meal, a co

ncert, a m

eeting with

friends. H

e holds, we th

ink

with good reason, th

at comforts are subject to

majo

r ad

aptatio

n effects an

d are alm

ost invisible as part o

f remem

bered

plea­sure. Joys, b

ecause they are unique events, are w

ell remem

bered

. Wh

en

we reflect o

n w

heth

er last year was g

oo

d o

r no

t, we do n

ot cite o

ur air

conditioning, mattress, or autom

atic g~rage d

oo

r opener. We th

ink

of th

e personal fam

ily events, the trips, the plays, m

ovies, concerts, spo

rt events, and so

forth. Am

ericans spend d

ispro

po

rtion

ately o

n com

forts com

pared

to th

e French (R

ozin et a!., 2009) and

presu

mab

ly do not h

arvest as m

uch

rem

emb

ered pleasure. T

his imp

ortan

t po

int relates to d

uratio

n neglect

and adap

tation

and

the idea that w

hat w

e remem

ber is events, preferably

events woven in

to a narrative.

We have

instan

tiated the

com

fort-jo

y d

istinctio

n w

ith its relation­

ship to rem

emb

ered pleasure in a sim

ple choice paradigm. W

e ask people

whether, w

hen they

go to their favorite restauran

t, they order their favor­

ite dish

or so

meth

ing

new (R

ozin et a!., 2009). We ask the sam

e qu

estion

ab

ou

t hearin

g th

eir favorite musical g

rou

p o

r traveling. If you op

t for yo

ur

favorite, you will probably have h

igh

er anticip

ation

and

a better experi­

ence, bu

t you will add little to y

ou

r mem

ory

, since the mem

ory

is already

in place. T

he mem

ory

of eating th

e same foie g

ras recipe twice is ab

ou

t the

Page 8: RESEARCH Ann ON...self-report), but rather to the value placed on olfactory sensations: food aromas, perfumes, the smells of the natural world, and so forth. Gestalt psychology was

314 P

aul Rozin and Julia M

. Horm

es

same as th

at of eatin

g it once. B

ut if they op

t for the n

ew dish, although th

e an

ticipatio

n w

ill be less certain

ly positive an

d th

e exp

erience w

ill probably be less positive (th

ink

ing

in term

s of regression to th

e mean), a new

posi­tive m

emo

ry w

ill have been

created (Rozin, H

ank

o, &

Gohar, in prepara­

tion; Rozin et al., 2009).

Th

e Origin o

f Preferen

ces

Often, w

hen

a perso

n prefers object X

to Y (e.g., a fo

od

or m

usic), it is the

senso

ry properties th

at determ

ine the choice. T

hey like X m

ore th

an th

ey

like Y. L

ikings abo

ut food in

particu

lar are mo

stly ab

ou

t senso

ry m

atters. W

here do these sen

sory

-based

likings com

e from? W

e are remarkably

ign

oran

t in this area, abo

ut food, m

usic, spo

rts, spo

rt teams, clothing, o

r an

yth

ing

else. Psychologists have not b

een th

at interested

in this area o

f life, w

hich

is of fu

nd

amen

tal daily imp

ortan

ce and

a core issue for mar­

keters and

econ

om

ists (for general reviews o

n th

e orig

in o

f food and

oth

er preferences, see B

irch, Fisher, an

d G

rimm

-Th

om

as [1996], Booth [1994],

and

Rozin [2006a, 2007a]).

In th

e food do

main

, as omnivorous an

imals, h

um

ans have a very open­

end

ed attitu

de to

ward

foods and

principally acqu

ire mo

st of their food

likes and

dislikes un

der the heavy guidance o

f cultu

re. There appear to be

no

inn

ately negative o

r positive odors (Bartoshuk, 1990), b

ut there is an

in

nate aversion to irritan

t or extrem

e oral tem

peratu

re sensations, bitter, an

d reasonably stro

ng

sou

r or salty tastes. S

weetness is positive, for som

e, at an

y level, an

d for o

thers u

p to a high level, at w

hich

po

int it declines as

sweetness co

ntin

ues to rise (P

angborn, 1980). P

eople rarely come to dis­

like sweets th

rou

gh

experience, bu

t they frequently com

e to like innately negative o

ral properties, such

as ice cold beverages, bitter foods or bever­

ages (e.g., coffee), or irritan

t foods (e.g., foods season

ed w

ith chili pepper).

Ev

eryth

ing

we k

no

w suggests th

at this is a hed

on

ic reversal, that is, the sen­

sory

inp

ut is u

nch

ang

ed, b

ut its valence inverts fro

m negative to positive.

We d

o n

ot k

no

w h

ow

this happens, but it is v

ery co

mm

on

(Rozin, 1990).

Three processes have been identified th

at can ch

ang

e the reaction to a

senso

ry experience. O

ne is m

ere exposure, wh

ich at m

od

est frequencies, ten

ds to en

han

ce likin

g (Z

ajonc, 1968). A seco

nd

is evaluative condition­ing, th

e pairin

g o

f a relatively neutral senso

ry ex

perien

ce (say a mild odor)

with

an already positive (e.g., sw

eet) or negative (e.g., bitter) experience.

The co

mm

on

ph

eno

men

on

of acquired taste aversions, in

anim

als and

h

um

ans, is a result o

f evaluative con

ditio

nin

g an

d is easily d

emo

nstrated

Psych

olo

gy a

nd Sensory Marketing

, With a Fo

cus on Fo

od 3

15

in th

e laboratory as well as by q

uestio

nn

aire (Pelch

at & R

ozin, 1982). This

Pavlovian process h

as been

stud

ied ex

tensiv

ely in

the lab

orato

ry in an

i­m

als and

clearly pro

du

ces a change in lik

ing

for a taste or flavor stim

ulu

s. F

or hu

man

s, evaluative con

ditio

nin

g has b

een d

emo

nstrated

man

y times

in th

e laboratory, usu

ally in

the framew

ork

of in

creased liking b

y co

ntin

­u

ed p

airing

of a n

eutral taste o

r situation with

an alread

y positive situ

ation

(D

e Houw

er, Th

om

as, & B

aeyens, 2001; Ro

zin, W

rzesnieswski, &

Byrnes,

1998). Alth

ou

gh

evaluative conditioning is surely

imp

ortan

t in real wo

rld

situations, it app

ears to be a rather fragile ph

eno

men

on

in the labo

ratory

(R

ozin et al., 1998). We d

o n

ot k

no

w w

hy. Th

e third

and

pro

bably m

ost

powerful force for creatin

g likes an

d d

islikes m

asqu

erades u

nd

er the gen­eral n

ame o

f "social influence." We do n

ot fu

lly u

nd

erstand

ho

w it w

ork

s, b

ut it is clear th

at un

der so

me conditions, th

e reaction

s of respected o

thers

to a food, piece of m

usic, or clothing can ch

ang

e ou

r likin

g for it (B

irch et al., 1996). A

dv

ertisers use all three o

f these p

athw

ays to induce lik

ing

for th

eir products. Bu

t like psychologists, they

do

no

t,kn

ow

ho

w to create lik­

ings reliably. All th

ree meth

od

s can back

fire. T

here are two special m

echanisms th

at may

be involved

in the co

mo

n conversion o

f aversions into

preferen

ces by

hu

man

s. These reversals

(referred to above) include, on the sen

sory

side, co

min

g to like very co

ld

beverages, bitter o

r very

sou

r or salty tastes, a

nd

oral irritants. G

oin

g p

ast th

e sensory level, these include co

min

g to lik

e the experience o

f fear (e.g., in

roller coasters), d

isgu

st (e.g., in disg

ust h

um

or), an

d sadness (e.g., w

ith

sad m

usic or m

ovies). Since this seem

s to be a u

niq

ue h

um

an ex

perien

ce, th

e exp

lanatio

n m

igh

t be expected to inv

ok

e un

iqu

ely h

um

an processes.

Mere exposure an

d evaluative co

nd

ition

ing

are clearly present in an

imals.

Social influence h

as been dem

on

strated

in an

imals, b

ut is m

uch

mo

re pow

erful in hu

man

s. So one possibility, for sen

sory

or other reversals, is

the generally p

ow

erful effects o

f elders an

d p

eers du

ring

dev

elop

men

t and

in

adulthood. Pleasure experienced by o

thers in

con

sum

ptio

n of so

me­

thin

g m

ay, perh

aps b

y a lin

k w

ith evaluative con

ditio

nin

g, induce p

refer­ences, an

d th

ere are a few d

emo

nstratio

ns o

f this (B

aeyens, Kaes, E

el en, &

S

ilverans, 1996). T

he seco

nd

m

echan

ism, w

hich w

e h

ave

called benign m

asoch

ism,

results from a u

niq

ue h

um

an en

joy

men

t of a

negative experience that

ou

r min

d k

no

ws is n

ot threatening. It is a m

atter of m

ind

ov

er body; we

enjoy irritant tastes o

r disgusting exp

eriences because they are negative,

ou

r body respo

nd

s as if they

are, bu

t we k

no

w b

etter than

ou

r body, and

this m

astery p

rod

uces pleasure. It is in

teresting

in th

is regard that w

e find

th

at for man

y ch

ili pep

per likers, th

eir favo

rite level of b

urn

is just belo

w

Page 9: RESEARCH Ann ON...self-report), but rather to the value placed on olfactory sensations: food aromas, perfumes, the smells of the natural world, and so forth. Gestalt psychology was

316 P

aul Rozin and ju

lia M

. Horm

es

the level they consider too pain

ful (R

ozin, 1990, 2007a; Rozin &

Schiller,

1980). Hu

man

s seem to enjoy p

ush

ing

the envelope o

fbearab

ility an

d get­

ting

pleasure ou

t of it.

On

e can exam

ine sensory likings, as for food, from a developm

ental per­spective. H

ere the question is wh

at are the relative roles of parents, peers, th

e m

edia, and

particular influential people in establishing likes and

dislikes? C

om

mo

n sense looks p

rimarily

to parents who contribute genes, predom

i­n

ant control over the en

viro

nm

ent for the first 5 years o

f life, and

substan­tial influence for the rest o

f child

ho

od

. It is thus sobering to realize that

with

in cultures, the correlation betw

een the food or m

usic likes of p

arents

and

those of th

eir adult children are very low, usually in th

e rang

e of .15 to

.30 (Rozin, 1991). V

alues, such as attitudes to abortion, show h

igh

er paren

t­child correlations. If the parents aren't the shaping force, w

hat is? P

eers are the m

ost likely principal source, alth

ou

gh

one study that directly tested this for fo

od

and

music preferences fo

un

d a surprisingly sm

all role for peers, either in

elementary school o

r college (Rozin, R

iklis, & M

argolis, 2004). We

can describe the current situation as th

e family and the p

eer paradox. T

here is on

e important fin

din

g in

this area that com

es ou

t of th

e mar­

ketin

g literature an

d is n

ot w

idely kn

ow

n in

psychology. Ho

lbro

ok

and

S

chin

dler (1989)

have shown, p

articularly

for music, th

at exp

osu

re to m

usic styles (presumably p

eer related) du

ring

the ages of 15 an

d 30 is m

ost

influential in creating lifetime preferences. W

e have gath

ered su

pp

ortin

g

data for this point, for m

usic and

to som

e degree for food. It is notable that 15 to

30 years of age is a p

eriod

of p

eak p

eer influence. Som

ewhere in

ou

r 20s o

r 30s, mo

st Am

ericans settle do

wn

and

have families an

d w

ithd

raw

from

the intense peer activity an

d th

ose stro

ng

social influences that ch

ar­acterized

their adolescence an

d y

ou

ng

adu

lt years. This is a v

ery p

rom

ising

h

int ab

ou

t taste formation.

Som

e Reflections on S

ensory Ma

rketin

g from

the P

sychological P

erspective

So far as we can

tell, to a considerable degree, marketing is a b

ranch

of

psychology, bu

ilt principally on

prio

r research in social psychology and

the psychology o

f judgments an

d decision m

aking. Sen

sory

mark

eting

b

ring

s in ano

ther branch o

f psychology, nam

ely the study o

f sensatio

n an

d

perception. The psychology o

f sensation and

perception is pro

bab

ly the

mo

st adv

anced

and

"scientific" part o

f psychology. Historically, it has b

een

based prim

arily on the d

escriptio

n o

f basic phenomena an

d fu

nctio

nal

Psychology and S

ensory Ma

rketin

g, With

a Focus o

n F

ood 317

relations, such as the dark adaptation curv

e and

the laws o

f color mixing,

followed by sophisticated th

eory

and

experimentation. S

ocial psychology, in

con

trast, has accomplished m

uch

less than

sensation and

perception, at least p

artly because w

hat it is stud

yin

g is m

uch

mo

re complex an

d m

ul­tid

etermin

ed th

an th

e subject of sensation an

d p

erceptio

n. B

y its nature, it involves m

ore th

an one p

erson

and

often requires the consideration o

f context, w

hich, as we have discussed already, can

be extrem

ely difficult.

Perhaps because social psychology stands at the less acco

mp

lished

edge o

f psychology, it has the mo

st potential. There is m

ore to

find out. Bu

t it is also the m

ost insecure about its n

atural scientific status an

d has resp

on

ded

to

this by

the development o

f incredible sop

histicatio

n in

the design o

f ex

perim

ents an

d th

e use of sophisticated statistics to analyze th

e results. It is focused o

n th

e hypothesis-experiment m

odel of science. T

he art of

sophisticated experiment, in

clud

ing

pro

per controls, careful exploration

and

elimin

ation

of alternative explanations, an

d m

anip

ulatio

n checks, has

reached

a new h

igh

in the field. B

ut this has come at a price. U

nlik

e phys­ics, chem

istry, biology, and

the psychology o

f se~sation and

perception, social psychology has p

aid little attention, an

d assigns little prestige, to

the first stages o

f science: accurate description of the social w

orld, the identi­

fication of fu

nd

amen

tal invariances (either with

in o

r betw

een cultures),

and

the description of th

e fun

dam

ental functional relations in

the social

world (the equivalent o

f the dark

adap

tation

function, or B

oyle's law in

physics). It has focused on

the sophisticated testing o

f hy

po

theses w

ithout first identifying the fu

nd

amen

tal thin

gs th

at are to be ex

plain

ed. E

rving G

offm

an, am

on

g others, d

id this, just as D

arwin

did

it for som

e branches o

f biology (Haig, 2005; R

ozin, 2001, 2006b, 2007b). T

his critique of m

od

ern social psychology is n

ot o

rigin

al to us: it w

as stated

clearly in 1952 by the great social psychologist o

f the 2

0th

centu

ry

So

lom

on

Asch:

Before w

e inquire into origins and functional relations, it is necessary to know

the thing w

e are trying to explain. (A

sch, 1952, p. 65)

If there must be principles o

f scientific method

, then surely the first to claim

our attention is that one should describe pheJ)om

ena faithfully and

allow th

em to

guide the choice of problems an

d procedures. If social psychology is to

mak

e a contribution to hum

an knowledge, ifit is to d

o m

ore than

add footnotes to ideas developed in other fields, it m

ust look freely at its phenom

ena and exam

ine its

foundations. (Asch, 1952, p. xv)

Th

e resuJt of this focus o

n hypothesis testing has b

een great so

ph

istication

in

stud

yin

g th

e mechanism

s of lab

orato

ry findings. T

he findings may or

Page 10: RESEARCH Ann ON...self-report), but rather to the value placed on olfactory sensations: food aromas, perfumes, the smells of the natural world, and so forth. Gestalt psychology was

318 P

aul Rozin and julia M

. Ho

rme

s

may

not have gen

erality w

ithin

the laboratory (that is, they m

ay be frag­ile and dependent o

n a lim

ited selection o

f param

eters), and

they may o

r m

ay not m

ap on

to th

e real world. T

he result is that th

e great majority o

f experim

ents are do

ne o

n A

merican college stu

den

ts. No

doubt their visual system

s work in

the sam

e basic way as that o

f adu

lts arou

nd

the w

orld. But

their social w

orld, as they

enter this peculiar p

eriod

of life th

at is a transi­tio

n betw

een ho

me an

d in

dep

end

ent life, p

articularly

in the U

nited States,

is very different from

mo

st social worlds o

f oth

er hu

man

s (Arnett, 2008;

Rozin, 2001, 2006b). T

he A

merican

college un

derg

radu

ate is no

t as good a m

odel for Hom

o sapiens as the fruit fly or Escherichia coli is for genetics.

The

result is

that

the prem

ier journal in

the

field, the Journal

of

Personality and Social P

sychology (JPSP), is difficult to read and

is about narrow

ly defined labo

ratory

phenomena an

d th

e mech

anism

s or causes o

f them

. It is not abo

ut th

e ph

eno

men

a of the social world, an

d it is not about

the dom

ains oflife (R

ozin, 2007b). Sadly, in o

ur view

, mark

eting

, at least the p

art that is built on psy­

chology, has ado

pted

the JP

SP

model, an

d th

is can b

e evidenced in its p

remier journals. W

e are hopeful that by integ

rating

the psychology o

f sensation an

d p

erceptio

n in

to m

arketing, senso

ry m

arketin

g w

ill also tu

rn o

ur atten

tion

mo

re to describing the basic ph

eno

men

a in the world

of m

arketing, as was tru

e in the history of sen

sation

and

perception. D

escription, generality, replicability, and

do

cum

entatio

n o

f functional relations should be cen

tral in th

e field. Show

ing that color m

atters in food selection is m

ore th

an d

oin

g an

experiment o

n th

e fact that color (often

represented by

two

different colors) influences foo

d choice in college stu

­dents. W

e have to b

e very

careful not to mak

e the m

istake of finding a

repeatable labo

ratory

parad

igm

, dependent on

the selection o

f a particu­lar set o

f param

eters from

a wide range o

f possibilities, and

analyzing it to death.

From

this perspective, it seems m

ost auspicious that sensory m

arket­in

g brings to b

ear a great fu

nd

of know

ledge in sensation an

d percep­

tion and

a set of m

etho

do

log

ies that are at once h

igh

ly sophisticated, often

quantitative, bu

t sou

nd

ly based o

n basic em

pirical relationships. In its his­

tory, sensation and

percep

tion

went th

rou

gh

a perio

d w

hen the Gestalt

model, w

hich privileges context, played a central role. It w

as out of that

tradition that So

lom

on

Asch w

rote what w

e con

sider the great book o

f th

e field, Social Psychology (1952), w

hich is still very

mu

ch w

orth

reading. O

ur challenge in

mark

eting

and

in social psychology is to be as rigorous

as we can be, w

hile at the sam

e time keeping an

eye on

the real ~

odd. W

e m

ust carefully co

nsid

er wh

ether w

hat we are m

od

eling

in the laboratory

Psycho

log

y and Sensory M

arketing, With

a Focus on Food

319

is som

ethin

g th

at is ou

t there. Sim

plif;zing is a po

werfu

l tool and

the heart

of experim

entation. But too sim

ple bo

rders o

n th

e meaningless. S

tudying h

um

an responses to sugar in w

ater has limited

value, and studying frozen m

oments o

f hu

man

facial expressions, wh

ile very

imp

ortan

t and

produc­tive, leaves o

ut m

uch

of w

hat goes on in th

e world.

References

Appadurai, A

. (1981). Gastro-politics in H

indu South A

sia. Am

erican Ethnologist,

8(3), 494-511. A

riely, D., &

Zauberm

an, G. {2000). O

n the making of an experience: The effects

of breaking and combining experiences in their overall evaluation. Journal o

f B

ehavioral Decision M

aking, 13(2), 21

9-2

32

. A

rnett, J. J. {2008). T

he neglected 95%

: W

hy Am

erican psychology needs to becom

e less Am

erican. Am

erican Psychologist, 63, 602-614.

Asch, S. {1952). Social psychology. N

ew Y

ork: PrenticeJ-lall.

Baeyens, F., K

aes, B., E

elen, P., & Silverans, P. {1996). O

bservational evaluative conditioning of an em

bedded stimulus elem

ent. European Journal o

f Social P

sychology, 26, 15-28. B

artoshuk, L. M. {1990). D

istinctions between taste and sm

ell relevant to the role

of experience. In E. D. C

apaldi & T. L. Pow

ley (Eds.), Taste, experience and

feeding (pp. 62-72). Washington, D

C: A

merican Psychological A

ssociation.

Birch, L. L

., Fisher, J. 0

., & G

rimm

-Thom

as, K.

(1996). The development o

f children's eating habits. In H

. L.

Meiselm

an &

H. J. H

. MacFie (E

ds.), F

ood choice, acceptance and consumption (pp.

161-206). London: B

lackie A

cademic and Professional.

Booth, D

. A. (1994). P

sychology of nutrition. L

ondon: Taylor and Francis.

De H

ouwer, J., T

homas, S., &

Baeyens, F. {200 1). A

ssociation learning of likes and dislikes: A

review of 25 years of research on hum

an evaluative conditioning. P

sychological Bulletin, 127(6), 853-869.

Fredrickson, B. {2000). Extracting m

eaning from past affective experiences: T

he im

portance of peaks, ends, and specific emotions. C

ognition an

d E

motion,

14, 577-606. Fredrickson, B. L., &

Kahnem

an, D. (1993). D

uration neglect in retrospective eval­uations of affective episodes. Journal o

f Personality and Social P

sychology, 65, 44-55.

Haig,

B. D

. {2005).

An

abductive theory of scientific m

ethod. P

sychological M

ethods, 10, 371-388. H

olbrook, M. B

., & Schindler, R

. M. {1989). Som

e exploratory findings on the developm

ent of musical tastes. Journal o

f Consum

er Research, 16, 119-124. H

yde, R. J., & W

itherly, S. A. ( 1993). D

ynamic contrast: A

sensory contribution to

palatability. Appetite, 21, 1-

16.

Page 11: RESEARCH Ann ON...self-report), but rather to the value placed on olfactory sensations: food aromas, perfumes, the smells of the natural world, and so forth. Gestalt psychology was

320 Paul R

ozin and julia M. H

ormes

Kahnem

an, D., F

redrickson, B. L., S

chreiber, C. A

., & R

edelmeier, D

. A. {1993).

Wh

en m

ore pain is preferred

to less: Adding a b

etter end. Psychological

Science, 4, 401-405. K

ahneman, D

., & Snell, J. (1992). P

redicting a changing taste: Do

people know

wh

at they will like? Journal o

f Behavioral D

ecision Making, 5, 187-200.

Kahnem

an, D., W

akker, P. P., & S

arin, R. ( 1997). Back to B

enth

am? E

xplorations of

experienced utility. Quarterly Journal o

f Econom

ics, 11

2,3

75

-40

5.

Kass, L. ( 1994). The hungry soul: E

ating and the perfecting of our nature. C

hicago: U

niversity of C

hicago Press.

Larson-P

owers, N

., & P

angborn, R. M

. (2006). Paired com

parison and

time-intensity

measurem

ents of the sen

sory

properties of beverages an

d gelatins containing

sucrose or synthetic sweeteners. Journal o

f Food Science, 43(1 ), 41-46. L

oewenstein, G

., & F

rederick, S. (1997). P

redicting reactions to environmental

change. In M.

H. B

azerman

(Ed.), E

nvironment,

ethics, and behavior (pp.

52-72). Lanham

, MD

: Lexington B

ooks. M

eiselman, H

. (Ed.). (2000). D

imensions o

f the meal: The science, culture, business,

and art of eating. G

aithersburg, MD

: Aspen P

ublishers. P

angborn, R. M

. (1980). A critical analysis o

f sensory responses to sweetness. In

P. Koivistoinen &

L. Hy

vo

nen

(Eds.), C

arbohydrate sweeteners in foods and

nutrition (pp. 87-110). Lo

nd

on

: Academ

ic Press. P

elchat, M. L., &

Rozin, P. (1982). T

he special role of nausea in th

e acquisition of

food dislikes by humans. A

ppetite, 3, 341-351. P

liner, P., & R

ozin, P. (2000). Th

e psychology of the m

eal. In H

. Meiselm

an (Ed.),

Dim

ensions of the meal: The science, cultu1·e, business, and art o

f eating (pp. 19-46). G

aithersburg, MD

: Aspen P

ublishers. R

ode, E., R

ozin, P., & D

urlach, P. (2007). Experienced and rem

emb

ered pleasure for

meals: D

uration neglect bu

t minim

al peak-end effects. Appetite, 49, 18-29.

Rozin, E

. ( 1982). The stru

cture o

f cuisine. In L. M. B

arker (Ed.), The psychobiology

of hum

an food selection (pp. 189-203). Westport, C

T: A

VI.

Rozin,

E. (1983).

Ethnic

cuisine: The flavor

principle cookbook.

New

Y

ork: H

awth

orn

Books.

Rozin, P. (1990). G

etting to like the b

urn

of chili pepper: B

iological, psychological an

d cultural perspectives. In

B. G

. Green, J. R. M

ason, & M

. R. K

are (Eds.),

Chem

ical senses, Vol. 2: Irritation (pp. 231-269). N

ew Y

ork: Marcel D

ekker. R

ozin, P. (1991). Fam

ily resemblance in food an

d other d

om

ains: T

he family para­

dox and the role of p

arental congruence. A

ppetite, 16, 93

-10

2.

Rozin, P. (1999). P

readaptation and

the puzzles and pro

perties o

f pleasure. In D.

Kahnem

an, E. Diener, &

N. S

chwarz (E

ds.), Well being: The foundations of

hedonic psychology (pp. 1 09

-13

3). N

ew Y

ork: Russell S

age. R

ozin, P. (2001). Social psychology an

d science: S

ome lessons from

Solom

on Asch.

Personality and Social P

sychology Review

, 5, 2-14. R

ozin, P. (2006a). Food choice: A

n introduction. In L. J.

Frew

er & H

. van Trijp

(Eds.). U

nderstanding consumers of food products (pp. 3

-29

). Cam

bridge, UK

: W

oodhead.

Psycholog

y and Sensory Marketing, W

ith a Focus on Food

321

Rozin

, P. (2006b). Do

main

denigration and

pro

cess preference in academic psy­

chology. Perspectives on P

sychological Science, 1, 36

5-3

76

. R

ozin, P. (2007a). Food an

d eating. In S. K

itayam

a & D

. Co

hen

(Eds.), H

andbook o

f cultural psychology (pp. 391-416). New

York: G

uilford. R

ozin, P. (2007b). E

xploring the landscape of m

od

ern

academic

psychology: F

inding and filling the holes. Am

erican Psychologist, 62, 754-766.

Rozin, A

., Guillot, L., &

Rozin

, P. (2007). Mem

ory for the attributes (liking, happy, sad) for

three-piece concerts:

Distortions

and accuracies.

Manuscript

in

preparation. R

ozin, P., Hanko, K

., & G

ohar, D. (2009). Individual differences in the im

portance o

f experienced, remem

bered and anticipated pleasure across different domains

of life. M

anuscript in preparation.

Rozin, P., H

ank

o, K., &

Durlach, P. (2006). S

elf-prediction of hedonic trajecto

­ries for repeated use o

f bo

dy

pro

du

cts and

foods: Po

or perform

ance, no

t im

proved by a full generation of experience. A

ppetite, 46, 297-303. R

ozin, P ., Rem

ick, A. R

., & F

ischler, C. (2009). B

road themes o

f difference between

French and A

mericans in attitudes to food and pleasure: Individualized vs col­

lective values, abundance vs moderation, quantity vs quality, and com

forts vs joys. S

ubmitted m

anuscript. R

ozin, P., Riklis, J., &

Margolis, L. (2004). M

utu

al exposure or close peer relation­

ships do no

t seem to

foster increased similarity

in food, music or television

program preferences. A

ppetite, 42, 41

-48

. R

ozin, A., R

ozin, P., &

Goldberg, E. (2004). T

he feeling o

f music past: H

ow

listen­ers rem

ember m

usical affect. Music P

erception, 22, 15

-39

. R

ozin, P., & S

chiller, D. ( 1980). T

he nature and

acquisition of a preference for chili

pep

per by hum

ans. Motivation and E

motion, 4, 7

7-1

01

. R

ozin, P., & T

aylor, S. (2006). Determ

inants of rem

embered pleasure after experienc­

ing an art exhibit w

ith rooms devoted to three different artists. M

anuscript in

preparation. R

ozin, P., Wrzesniew

ski, A., &

Byrnes, D

. (1998). T

he elusiveness of evaluative

conditioning. Learning & M

otivation, 29, 39

7-4

15

. S

chutz, H. G

. (1989). Beyond preference: A

pp

rop

riateness as a m

easure of contex­

tual acceptance of food

. In D. M

. H. T

ho

mso

n (E

d.), Food acceptability (pp. 115-134). E

ssex, Eng.: E

lsevier Applied S

cience Publishers.

Scitovsky, T. (1992). The joyless economy. O

xfo

rd, U

K: O

xfo

rd U

niversity Press. W

ansink. B., P

ayne, C. R

., & N

orth, J. (2007). Fin

e as No

rth D

akota wine: S

ensory expectations an

d th

e intake of com

pan

ion

foods. Physiology and B

ehavior, 9

0,7

12

-71

6.

Wrzesniew

ski, A., M

cCauley, C

. R., &

Rozin, P. (1999). O

do

r and affect: Individual differences in the im

pact of o

do

r on

liking for places, things and

people.

Chem

ical Senses, 24, 713-721. Z

ajonc, R. B

. (1968). Attitudinal effects""o

f mere exposure. Journal of P

ersonality and Social P

sychology. 9, 1-27.