35
Prescriptivism and Knowledge of Language September 21, 2012

Prescriptivism and Knowledge of Language

  • Upload
    keona

  • View
    36

  • Download
    1

Embed Size (px)

DESCRIPTION

Prescriptivism and Knowledge of Language. September 21, 2012. Current Work with Bonobos. After Nim Chimpsky, funding for primate language studies mostly dried up. …although a few experiments went on. One project involves bonobos, a sub-species of chimpanzees. - PowerPoint PPT Presentation

Citation preview

Page 1: Prescriptivism and  Knowledge of Language

Prescriptivism and Knowledge of Language

September 21, 2012

Page 2: Prescriptivism and  Knowledge of Language

Current Work with Bonobos• After Nim Chimpsky, funding for primate language studies mostly dried up.

• …although a few experiments went on.

• One project involves bonobos, a sub-species of chimpanzees.

• Bonobos Sherman and Austin have also been trained to use lexigrams.

• Kanzi learned just by watching Sherman and Austin’s training!• But the Bonobo project is now in trouble—check out:http://news.iowapublicradio.org/post/bonobo-hope-great-ape-trust-sanctuary

Page 3: Prescriptivism and  Knowledge of Language

Mission Objectives1. Wrap up Prescriptivism ~ Descriptivism

2. Try to figure out how language can be creative.

• The previous problems with prescriptivism:

1. Confusion about application of prescriptive rules

• (they’re not natural)

• Hypercorrection

2. Standards can shift over time

3. Prescriptive rules form a poor understanding of natural language.

Page 4: Prescriptivism and  Knowledge of Language

Problem #3: Missing Patterns• Prescriptivist rules do a poor job of accounting for many of the patterns we find in natural language.

• Here’s one prescriptive rule which misses a consistent pattern:

• “Incorrect”: I feel bad (about the accident).

• “Correct”: I feel badly (about the accident).

• Why? The verb “feel” should be modified by an adverb (“badly”), not an adjective (“bad”).

• But is bad/badly modifying the verb or the subject of the sentence?

Page 5: Prescriptivism and  Knowledge of Language

Linking Verbs• How about these examples?

• Bob is happy. (*Bob is happily.)

• Susie looks hot. (*Susie looks hotly.)

• The water seems fine. (*The water seems finely.)

• I feel sleepy. (*I feel sleepily.)

• James Brown feels good. (*James Brown feels well.)

• The verbs in these sentences are known as linking verbs.

• They connect the subject to some property describing the subject.

• (They do not modify the verb itself.)

Page 6: Prescriptivism and  Knowledge of Language

Different Standards• Rules for a standard form of a language…

• Normally describe the variety of language used by the group in power.

• Other forms of the language are non-standard.

• And are often identified with social, regional or ethnic groups.

• Linguists have discovered that all forms of language (standard or not) are rule-based and orderly.

• Non-standard forms of the language are not simply mistake-ridden versions of the standard form.

• There is no linguistic reason to consider one variety of language superior to another.

Page 7: Prescriptivism and  Knowledge of Language

Quick Write:Appalachian English

• Appalachian English is a variety of English traditionally spoken in the Appalachian mountains.

• Developed (and maintained) unique features due to isolation from outside communities.

• One interesting feature:

• a-prefixing…

Page 8: Prescriptivism and  Knowledge of Language

“a” prefixing, part 11. a. The man likes sailing.

b. The man went sailing.

Correct answer: (b) (83-5)

5. a. William thinks fishing is silly.

b. William goes fishing every Sunday.

Correct answer: (b) (82-6)

Rule: [a-] form cannot be a noun.

Page 9: Prescriptivism and  Knowledge of Language

“a” prefixing, part 22. a. The woman was coming down the stairs.

b. The movie was shocking.

Correct answer: (a) (76-12)

6. a. The movie was fascinating.

b. The movie kept jumping up and down.

Correct answer: (b) (68-18)

Rule: [a-] form cannot be an adjective.

Page 10: Prescriptivism and  Knowledge of Language

“a” prefixing, part 33. a. He makes money by building houses.

b. He makes money building houses.

Correct answer: (b) (71-16)

7. a. Sally got sick cooking chicken.

b. Sally got sick from cooking chicken.

Correct answer: (a) (71-16)

Rule: [a-] form cannot be preceded by a preposition.

Page 11: Prescriptivism and  Knowledge of Language

“a” prefixing, part 44. a. Sam was following the trail.

b. Sam was discovering the cave.

Correct answer: (a) (80-7)

8. a. The man was hollering at the hunters.

b. The man was recalling what happened that night.

Correct answer: (a) (79-8)

Rule: first syllable of [a-] form must be stressed.

Page 12: Prescriptivism and  Knowledge of Language

“a” Prefixing Summary• [a-] form cannot be a noun (#1 and #5)

• [a-] form cannot be an adjective (#2 and #6)

• [a-] form cannot be preceded by a preposition

(#3 and #7)

• first syllable of [a-] form must be stressed (#4 and #8)

• Note: people often consider speakers of Appalachian English to be unsophisticated

• …but the proper use of the [a-] prefix involves a relatively complex set of conditions.

Page 13: Prescriptivism and  Knowledge of Language

AAVE• Another variety of English that has (traditionally) been low on the prestige scale is African-American Vernacular English (AAVE).

• a.k.a. Black Vernacular English (BVE), Ebonics

• Predominantly spoken by African-Americans

• but not all African-Americans…

• and some others, as well.

• AAVE has a variety of interesting features...

• some familiar: multiple negation, ain’t as an auxiliary

• others are less familiar…

Page 14: Prescriptivism and  Knowledge of Language

AAVE Verbs• Verb conjugation: third personal singular verbs lack an [-s] marker.

• Ex: He look, it do, she have

• “Paradigm leveling”

• = making a set of related forms more uniform

• (similar to “he don’t”/”she don’t”)

• Under certain conditions, the verb “to be” can be deleted.

• Ex: you so crazy, she workin’, he lucky

• In the same conditions, “to be” can be contracted in standard English:

• You’re so crazy, she’s working, he’s lucky…

Page 15: Prescriptivism and  Knowledge of Language

To Be Deletion• What are the right conditions for deletion/contraction?

AAVE Standard English

You so crazy. You’re so crazy.

*He as nice as he say he. *He’s as nice as he says he’s.

*Here I. *Here I’m.

They mine. They’re mine.

*How beautiful you. *How beautiful you’re.

• The verb needs to link the subject to something after it.

Page 16: Prescriptivism and  Knowledge of Language

AAVE: Habitual Be• AAVE also has a form of “to be” that standard English does not.

• “habitual” be

• Habitual be expresses something that the subject does on a regular basis.

• Examples:

• He be working at Tim Horton’s.

• She be late. (= She is usually late.)

• She late. (= She’s late (right now).)

• Do you be tired? (=Are you often tired?)

Page 17: Prescriptivism and  Knowledge of Language

Descriptive Benefits• Language tends to operate in patterns, even if they are non-standard.

• Important: Appalachian English and AAVE speakers are not just speaking English with mistakes.

• Descriptive linguistics enables us to understand how those patterns work.

• Even if you want to change the world, you’re better off understanding how it works to begin with.

• History of economics analogy.

Page 18: Prescriptivism and  Knowledge of Language

To Be Fair• Standards are useful because they provide a single form of the language to teach to non-native speakers.

• They help establish uniformity in the written language.

• They can help clear up confusions.

• for instance: supposably

• They also help to distinguish those who have mastered the arbitrary rules from those who haven’t.

• (for better or worse)

• Otherwise:

• They are not useful for (scientific) linguistic analysis.

Page 19: Prescriptivism and  Knowledge of Language

Linguistic Creativity (again)• One of the crucial design features of language was creativity (or productivity).

• Charles Hockett:

“Language users can create and understand completely novel messages.”

“In a language, new messages are freely coined by blending, analogizing from, or transforming old ones. This says that every language has grammatical patterning.”

“In a language, either new or old elements are freely assigned new semantic loads by circumstances and context. This says that in every language new idioms constantly come into existence.”

• How is it possible for human beings to do this?

Page 20: Prescriptivism and  Knowledge of Language

To Infinity and Beyond• Last week, we found out that honeybees can produce a variety of different “dance messages”.

= “Food source beyond 65 feet, fly at 0

degree angle with the sun.”

= “Food source beyond 65 feet, fly at 45

degree angle with the sun.”

Page 21: Prescriptivism and  Knowledge of Language

To Infinity and Beyond• The number of different messages the bees can produce is limited only by the number of angles they can differentiate:

• “Food source beyond 65 feet, fly at 1 degree angle with the sun.”

• “Food source beyond 65 feet, fly at 2 degree angle with the sun.”

……………

• “Food source beyond 65 feet, fly at 359 degree angle with the sun.”

• Q: Can the bees dance at angles they haven’t seen before?

• If so, how?

Page 22: Prescriptivism and  Knowledge of Language

To Infinity and Beyond• A: yes, if they dance according to rule.

Their dances have to fit into a meaningful pattern.

• The rule = “Food source beyond 65 feet, fly at X degree angle with the sun.”

The bees have to know the rule.

• Knowing the rule enables them to exhibit “creativity”, in a sense.

Page 23: Prescriptivism and  Knowledge of Language

Different Infinities• What kind of infinities exist in human language?

• Note that we can say (translations of) everything the bees can say:

Fly at a 1 degree angle with the sun.

Fly at a 2 degree angle with the sun.

……………

Fly at a 359 degree angle with the sun.

• We can get as detailed as we want to about it, too:

Fly at a 45 degree, 13 minute, 27.6685 second angle with the sun.

Page 24: Prescriptivism and  Knowledge of Language

Infinity + 1• In addition to the infinity of things the bees can say, we can say other things, too.

• Examples (borrowed from Ray Jackendoff):

A numeral is not a numbskull.

A numeral is not a nun.

A numeral is not a nunnery.

……………

A nun is not a nursery.

……………

An oboe is not an octopus.

Page 25: Prescriptivism and  Knowledge of Language

Linguistic Infinities• These are uninteresting, but novel sentences.

• In order to understand them, you must know the rule by which they are constructed.

• Rule:

[Sentence] = A X is not a Y.

• Point:

• Knowledge of rules is more abstract than just knowledge of sentences.

Page 26: Prescriptivism and  Knowledge of Language

Language Model #1

• In this model, all we “know” are the individual sentences we can use in language.

• (no rules)

• This is a good enough model to describe the vervets’ (or prairie dogs’) “language”.

A nun is not a nursery.

Fly at a 45 degree angle with the sun.

I like linguistics.

Page 27: Prescriptivism and  Knowledge of Language

Language Model #2

• In this model, we “know” all the rules we can use to combine words to form sentences in a language.

• This is a good enough model to describe the bees’ “language”.

• Is it good enough for human language?

A X is not a Y.

X at a Y degree angle with the Z.

X likes Y.

Page 28: Prescriptivism and  Knowledge of Language

What do you think?• No. There are even bigger infinities.

• Check out these sentences:

Bill thinks that Beth is a genius.

Sue suspects that Bill thinks that Beth is a genius.

Charlie said that Sue suspects that Bill thinks that Beth is a genius.

Jean knows that Charlie said that Sue suspects that Bill thinks that Beth is a genius.

ad infinitum...

• Some “real” examples:

Page 29: Prescriptivism and  Knowledge of Language

How many rules do we need?1. X verbs that Y is a Z.

2. W verbs that X verbs that Y is a Z.

3. V verbs that W verbs that X verbs that Y is a Z.

• and so on…

• Q: Can we store all these patterns in our heads?

• A: No, because no matter how many we store, there is always a longer one…

• So how do we know all of these sentences?

Page 30: Prescriptivism and  Knowledge of Language

Language is Recursive

• Recursive = involving a procedure that can refer to itself.

• In language, rules for producing sentences can be used in rules for producing sentences.

• Humans have to know rules of the following kind:

[Sentence] = X verbs that [Sentence]

Page 31: Prescriptivism and  Knowledge of Language

Language Model #3

• Jackendoff: “We know not just patterns of words, but patterns of patterns.”

• This is how we can be infinitely creative with a finite set of rules.

S = X likes Y.

S = A X is not a Y.

S = X verbs that S.

Page 32: Prescriptivism and  Knowledge of Language

Check it out• Included among the infinite number of things we can say is a lot of complete nonsense.

• Examples (from Chomsky and Lewis Carroll):

• Colorless green ideas sleep furiously.

• I’m memorizing the score of the sonata I hope to compose someday.

• ‘Twas brillig, and the slithy toves

Did gyre and gimble in the wabe…

• Check out the postmodernism generator:

• http://www.elsewhere.org/pomo/

Page 33: Prescriptivism and  Knowledge of Language

What’s the difference?• Nonsense sentences work because they fit in with the patterns formed by the sentences that actually do make sense.

• (and that we use every day)

• Compare with the following:

• Large green lizards sleep soundly.

• I’m memorizing the score of the sonata I hope to perform someday.

• ‘Twas evening, and the slimy toads

Did squirm and wiggle in the cage…

Page 34: Prescriptivism and  Knowledge of Language

What’s the difference? (part 2)• But the following sentences don’t work at all:

• Green sleep ideas furiously colorless.

• I’m memorizing the perform of the score I sonata to hope someday.

• Brillig and, slithy and the toves

Wabe gimble in the gyre and did…

• Note: just because we can say an infinite number of things, we can’t just say anything…

Page 35: Prescriptivism and  Knowledge of Language

Technical Terminology• The set of rules that we know for creating sentences in a language is the grammar of that language.

• The rules of grammar that we know are very abstract. (patterns of patterns)

• Strings of words which do not adhere to these rules are ungrammatical.

• Q: If these rules are so abstract, how did we figure out what they are?

• How do we learn language?