214
UNIVERSIDADE DE LISBOA FACULDADE DE CIÊNCIAS DEPARTAMENTO DE BIOLOGIA ANIMAL PREDATOR-PREY INTERACTIONS BETWEEN AN INVASIVE CRAYFISH PREDATOR AND NATIVE ANURANS: ECOLOGICAL AND EVOLUTIONARY OUTCOMES ANA LUÍSA SUMARES DA CRUZ NUNES DOUTORAMENTO EM BIOLOGIA ESPECIALIDADE DE ECOLOGIA 2011

PREDATOR-PREY INTERACTIONS BETWEEN AN INVASIVE …

  • Upload
    others

  • View
    1

  • Download
    0

Embed Size (px)

Citation preview

Page 1: PREDATOR-PREY INTERACTIONS BETWEEN AN INVASIVE …

UNIVERSIDADE DE LISBOA

FACULDADE DE CIÊNCIAS

DEPARTAMENTO DE BIOLOGIA ANIMAL

PREDATOR-PREY INTERACTIONS BETWEEN AN INVASIVE

CRAYFISH PREDATOR AND NATIVE ANURANS:

ECOLOGICAL AND EVOLUTIONARY OUTCOMES

ANA LUÍSA SUMARES DA CRUZ NUNES

DOUTORAMENTO EM BIOLOGIA

ESPECIALIDADE DE ECOLOGIA

2011

Page 2: PREDATOR-PREY INTERACTIONS BETWEEN AN INVASIVE …
Page 3: PREDATOR-PREY INTERACTIONS BETWEEN AN INVASIVE …

UNIVERSIDADE DE LISBOA

FACULDADE DE CIÊNCIAS

DEPARTAMENTO DE BIOLOGIA ANIMAL

PREDATOR-PREY INTERACTIONS BETWEEN AN INVASIVE

CRAYFISH PREDATOR AND NATIVE ANURANS:

ECOLOGICAL AND EVOLUTIONARY OUTCOMES

ANA LUÍSA SUMARES DA CRUZ NUNES

TESE ORIENTADA POR:

DR. RUI MIGUEL REBELO

DR. ANSSI LAURILA

DOUTORAMENTO EM BIOLOGIA

ESPECIALIDADE DE ECOLOGIA

2011

Page 4: PREDATOR-PREY INTERACTIONS BETWEEN AN INVASIVE …

This study was funded by Fundação para a Ciência e a Tecnologia (FCT) through a PhD grant

(SFRH/BD/29068/2006) and by the research project POCI/BIA-BDE/56100/2004.

This dissertation should be cited as:

Nunes AL (2011) Predator-prey interactions between an invasive crayfish predator and native

anurans: ecological and evolutionary outcomes. PhD Thesis, Universidade de Lisboa, Portugal.

Page 5: PREDATOR-PREY INTERACTIONS BETWEEN AN INVASIVE …

Nota Prévia

A presente tese apresenta resultados de trabalhos já publicados ou em preparação para

publicação (capítulos 2 a 5), de acordo com o previsto no nº 1 do artigo 41º do Regulamento

de Estudos Pós-Graduados da Universidade de Lisboa, publicado no Diário da República II série

nº 209 de 30 de Outubro de 2006. Tendo os trabalhos sido realizados em colaboração, a

candidata esclarece que participou integralmente na concepção dos trabalhos, obtenção dos

dados, análise e discussão dos resultados, bem como na redacção dos manuscritos.

Lisboa, Novembro de 2011

Ana Luísa Sumares da Cruz Nunes

Page 6: PREDATOR-PREY INTERACTIONS BETWEEN AN INVASIVE …
Page 7: PREDATOR-PREY INTERACTIONS BETWEEN AN INVASIVE …

Aos papás e à mana

Page 8: PREDATOR-PREY INTERACTIONS BETWEEN AN INVASIVE …

ACKNOWLEDGMENTS

After almost five years of hard work, finally this thesis comes to an end. I guess one of the

things that it made me realise is my unbelievable capacity of never being able to achieve

deadlines. However, it also allowed me to become a mini-scientist and to get to experience

things that will forever be in my mind and that make me who I am today.

I start by thanking my supervisors, Rui Rebelo and Anssi Laurila. To Rui, for convincing me to

apply for a PhD grant, even though I was very reluctant to start a life as a researcher at the

time. I turned out to be right, science is way too scientific for me, but I cannot be happier for

having done this PhD. Thank you for teaching me so many things about the natural world, for

going with me to the field every time I needed, for your constant good mood but especially for,

other than a supervisor, being a true good friend. To Anssi, the nicest Finnish person I have

ever met, thank you for promptly accepting me as your half PhD student and for allowing me

to work in Uppsala for two years. Thank you for all the chats in your office about work and

many other things, from wolves and bears to the horrible Swedish weather. And for always

being so patient about me handing in work late.

Thank you both so much for helping me make this thesis a nice scientific work, but especially

for being always present, available and extremely funny supervisors. Throughout the

discussion of the thesis I use the personal pronoun “I”, but this work is really “ours”.

To Fundação para a Ciência e Tecnologia I thank for my PhD grant (SFRH/BD/29068/2006) and

for funding through the Project POCI/BIA-BDE/56100/2004. I also thank Stiftelsen för Zoologisk

Forskning for funding.

To Dr. Miguel Tejedo for having received us so well in Seville and having allowed us to perform

fieldwork in Doñana.

To my master students, Erika, Susana and Cátia, for making the huge experiment in the

Herdade a doable thing and for going to Grândola, even at weekends, to change waters and

feed the predators. Thank you for all your help in the field and in the lab.

To Pedro Andrade, the best field assistant ever, for keeping me company in Grândola so many

times, for being so good in fixing problems and in building weird necessary things and for

talking about crazy nonsense sometimes. You made my fieldwork extremely pleasant and easy.

To Sandra Amaral for making me go to Rui’s office many years ago and help trace a way into

studying amphibians. For going with me through the hard process of learning about amphibian

larvae (“Is this an Alytes or a Pelobates?”). To Maria João, for teaching me almost everything I

know about amphibians and for calling me to Sweden when I needed help with statistics. Both

you and Mário have been really nice friends.

To Helder, an amphibian companion, for the help with the phylogenetic trees, but mostly for

sharing the love for amphibians and always being ready to talk about them. Thank you for

always being ready to help and for hosting me in Seville during the congress.

Page 9: PREDATOR-PREY INTERACTIONS BETWEEN AN INVASIVE …

To all the friends I made in Sweden, thank you so much for having made life in the north a, not

only bearable, but extremely nice thing. You have allowed me to experience many incredibly

nice and different situations and to live the real grad student life.

To everyone in the department, thank you for making work a very nice place, filled with

friends. Thank you for the thursday pancakes, sushi lunches, friday fikas and Christmas dinners.

To Marianne, for all the help in solving ‘money issues’, you were great!

To Jan L, for always asking me about the thesis and for giving me nice hints on how a paper

should be written.

To the frog group, Emma, Björn, Magnus, Rike, Sandra H, Katja, German and Alex, thanks for

sharing your knowledge with me and for being nice friends. To Rike, thanks for being such a

lovely office mate and for letting me feed your snails.

To the people from the Iberian (not Spanish) office, Violeta, Maria, Germán and Alex, for

always speaking in english with me and for all the healthy gossip. To Germán and Alex, thank

you for all the help in my papers and for showing me the wonders of doing fieldwork with

frogs in Sweden. To Alex, for all the chocolates in the fika room (no more oranges for you!), for

the company in the office when no one else was there and, especially, for having become a

very good friend.

To the Costa Rica crew, Biao, Jonathan, Jenny and Caro, for an amazing trip to the other side of

the world. Thank you, Biao, for being such an easy going person and for always being in a good

mood. Thank you, Jonathan, for being my friend, despite I though you didn’t like me in the

beginning and for showing me so many birds in Costa Rica. You are lovely! Jenny, the atypical

Swedish girl, thank you for teaching me how to do kannelbulle and for all the shared lunches

outside. To Caro, thank you for all the time spent together, during and after work, for always

liking the things I cooked, for letting me get to see a black grouse (or many) and, especially, for

the very nice friendship that we have.

To other people from EBC, Jürg, Maria, Omar, Yoshi, Christina, Kasia, Anna, Richard and

Andreas for all the dinners, parties and beers in the Nations. To Kasia, the fortunately more

PopBio than AnimEcol girl, thanks for being so nice and caring and for making me understand

how much a real scientist can love her job.

To my Portuguese family in Uppsala, Tiago and Vanda, for being very close and good friends

and for allowing me to not forget my mother tongue while I was away.

To my ‘Colombian’ family, Daniel C, Daniel O, Tobias, Angie, Caro, Jessica, Ricardo, Manda,

Tanai, Sahar, Tobi, Anna, Ola, Claire and Sara for all the friendship and for making my stay in

Uppsala much better than I could wish for. Thanks for all the shared moments, especially in

our amazing trips to Sälen and Colombia. Muitossshhh obrigadossshhh, from radiosssshh

portuguessshhhh. To Caro, Angie and Tobias, for always being there for me.

Page 10: PREDATOR-PREY INTERACTIONS BETWEEN AN INVASIVE …

To my dearest friends from the “Sala dos terminais”, Ana Leal, Nuno, Tatá, Mafalda, Joana,

Miguel and Luís, thank you for making it really nice to go to work, even at the final stages of

this thesis (especially when it was a cake day). Thank you for giving me all the strength I

needed to finish this, you were the ones who made it possible for me to hand this in now. I

especially thank you for constantly asking if I needed help and for the last minute reviewing of

the thesis.

To all my korfball people, for often making me think of things other than work, for all the

holidays spent together and for, after all these years, still not understanding what my job was.

Ralha and David, now you can finally see what ‘your’ money was used for. To my twins, Sara

and Ana, thank you for coming to Grândola to help me in the field. Xiquinho, thank you for,

even when I was far, always being close through the internet.

To Joana Vinagre, Henrique, Natércia and Sónia for coming all the way from Portugal to

experience the ‘warm’ Swedish summer, making my first year in Uppsala much nicer, at a time

I wanted to come back to Portugal. To Natércia, for always asking me about the thesis and for

being there, even if through a phone call, every time I needed. To Sónia, thank you for your

help changing waters and feeding tadpoles and for all the times you stayed at home keeping

me company while I had to work.

To Joaninha and Ricardo, the ever present university friends, for the solid friendship and for

always encouraging me to finish this fast. To Lena for, even far, still asking how the thesis was

going and for chatting with me about phenotypic plasticity.

To Joana Vinagre, our long time friendship, with its highs and lows, says it all. Thank you for

your constant presence in my life.

Aos meus padrinhos, Augusta e Rufino, por todos os fantásticos almoços e por sempre estarem

presentes na minha vida.

Aos meus pais e à minha irmã, por serem uma fonte de inspiração de vida e por me terem

feito quem sou hoje. Por me terem deixado invadir a cave deles com milhares de girinos e

lagostins e ainda me terem ajudado no trabalho. Por sempre me terem apoiado em todas as

fases deste doutoramento, e por nunca me terem pressionado para o terminar. Mamã,

obrigada por tudo teres feito para estar aqui hoje e não te preocupes que vou poder repetir a

mesma palavra duas vezes quando defender a tese.

To all the people I did not mention, but that I met through this journey, be it in congresses, in

the field, in trips or in the university, thank you for being a part of this work.

Page 11: PREDATOR-PREY INTERACTIONS BETWEEN AN INVASIVE …

TABLE OF CONTENTS

Abstract i

Resumo ii

CHAPTER 1. General Introduction 1

1.1. Predator-prey interactions 3

Defensive antipredator strategies 3

- Cues used to assess predation risk 8

1.1.2. Unsuccessful predation attempts and nonlethal prey injury 10

1.2. Invasive alien species 12

1.3. Inducible responses in the presence of invasive alien predators 16

1.4. Species in focus 20

1.4.1. The invasive predator: red swamp crayfish (Procambarus clarkii) 21

1.4.2. The native prey: anurans from the Southwest of the Iberian Peninsula 24

1.5. P. clarkii and the anurans from Southwestern Iberian Peninsula 27

1.6. Goals and outline of the thesis 28

1.7. Papers presented 31

1.8. References 32

CHAPTER 2. 53

Nunes AL, Cruz MJ, Tejedo M, Laurila A, Rebelo R

Nonlethal injury caused by an invasive alien predator and its consequences for

an anuran tadpole.

Page 12: PREDATOR-PREY INTERACTIONS BETWEEN AN INVASIVE …

CHAPTER 3. 77

Nunes AL, Richter-Boix A, Laurila A, Rebelo R

Do anuran larvae respond behaviourally to chemical cues from an invasive

crayfish predator? A community-wide study.

CHAPTER 4. 109

Nunes AL, Orizaola G, Laurila A, Rebelo R

Morphological and life-history responses and integration of defensive traits

in an anuran community invaded by an exotic predator.

CHAPTER 5. 145

Nunes AL, Orizaola G, Laurila A, Rebelo R

Population divergence in antipredator defences against an invasive predator:

does coexistence time promote the evolution of adaptive responses?

CHAPTER 6. General Discussion 173

6.1. Crayfish as an agent of tail injury in anurans 175

6.2. Phenotypic variation in Iberian tadpoles 176

6.3. Plastic responses of Iberian anurans to a native and an exotic predator 178

6.4. Implications of P. clarkii invasion for anuran communities 184

6.5. Control and management of P. clarkii and conservation of native amphibians 187

6.6. Final considerations 189

6.7. References 191

Page 13: PREDATOR-PREY INTERACTIONS BETWEEN AN INVASIVE …

i

ABSTRACT

The introduction of exotic species, particularly invasive aquatic predators, is a pervasive

phenomenon worldwide, largely implicated in amphibian declines. As native prey do not share

a co-evolutionary history with exotic predators, they often lack effective antipredator

defences, which may endanger their populations. Alternatively, due to their strong predation

pressure, invaders may induce rapid evolutionary changes in invaded communities.

The red swamp crayfish Procambarus clarkii is a widespread invasive predator abundant in

freshwater habitats in Southwestern Iberian Peninsula. It is an efficient predator of Iberian

amphibian larvae and it can exert strong impact upon them. The main goal of this thesis was to

assess the impacts of P. clarkii in fitness components of larval Iberian anurans in order to

better understand variation in vulnerability of native prey to novel predators. This was done by

performing a series of laboratory experiments.

P. clarkii incurred tail injury in tadpoles at very high rates, which affected tadpole morphology

and survival, and may have delayed fitness costs. Seven out of nine anuran species showed

predator-induced behavioural plasticity in the presence of P. clarkii, but only two showed

morphological plasticity. Life-history responses, elicited by four species, were very variable and

species-specific, contrarily to behavioural defences that were similar across species. More

studies are needed to settle the adaptive value of these responses. Populations of Pelophylax

perezi differed in antipredator defences according to coexistence time with crayfish and it is

suggested that P. clarkii may have selected for rapid evolution of defences in a long-term

invaded population.

Since there are no easy solutions to cope with this invasion, understanding interactions

between P. clarkii and native species is critical for predicting how this invasion may alter the

dynamics of natural communities and for directing conservation efforts towards the most

vulnerable species, as is the case of Pelodytes ibericus, an Iberian endemic.

Keywords: Biological invasions, aquatic invasive predator, inducible defences, nonlethal injury,

amphibians, Iberian Peninsula

Page 14: PREDATOR-PREY INTERACTIONS BETWEEN AN INVASIVE …

ii

RESUMO

A introdução de espécies exóticas, fenómeno que actualmente ocorre a um ritmo muito

elevado, é considerada uma das principais ameaças para a biodiversidade global. Quando

novas espécies de predadores são introduzidas fora da sua área de distribuição original podem

causar impactos negativos na estrutura e funcionamento dos ecossistemas invadidos. Em

particular, a invasão de ecossistemas aquáticos por grandes predadores tem sido apontada

como uma das causas que mais tem contribuído para o declínio de populações de anfíbios,

fenómeno que está a acontecer a nível global. Muitas vezes isto pode dever-se ao facto de as

espécies de anfíbios não reconhecerem o predador introduzido e, como tal, não terem a

capacidade de responder com os seus mecanismos antipredatórios naturais, que podem

consistir em alterações comportamentais, morfológicas ou de parâmetros da história vital.

Estas defesas naturais aumentam a probabilidade de sobrevivência das espécies-presa perante

predadores nativos e o facto de muitas vezes não serem utilizadas na presença de predadores

exóticos aumenta grandemente a sua vulnerabilidade.

Grande parte dos estudos que documentam impactos negativos para espécies de anfíbios

decorrentes da introdução de predadores exóticos diz respeito à introdução de peixes não

nativos. No entanto, já foram também documentados os impactos negativos da introdução de

lagostins exóticos. O lagostim-vermelho-americano, Procambarus clarkii, é uma espécie nativa

do Centro-Sul dos Estados Unidos da América e do Nordeste do México que, com fins

comerciais, foi introduzida em vários países de todo o mundo, estando actualmente

estabelecida em todos os continentes, excepto na Austrália e na Antártida. Esta espécie foi

introduzida pela primeira vez em território europeu em 1973, na área de Badajoz (Espanha), e

no ano seguinte na zona de Sevilha, nas marismas do rio Guadalquivir. Através das bacias

hidrográficas do Guadiana e do Tejo a espécie rapidamente se expandiu para Portugal, sendo

actualmente muito comum e abundante em praticamente toda a Península Ibérica, sobretudo

no Sul. No Sudoeste da Península Ibérica, uma região sem espécies de lagostim de água doce

Page 15: PREDATOR-PREY INTERACTIONS BETWEEN AN INVASIVE …

iii

nativas, o P. clarkii pode ser encontrado em habitats permanentes, temporários ou mesmo

efémeros, muitas vezes coexistindo com várias espécies de anfíbios. Nesta área existem

catorze espécies de anfíbios, dez das quais são anuros. São estes a rã verde (Pelophylax perezi),

a rela meridional (Hyla meridionalis), a rela comum (Hyla arborea), o sapo comum (Bufo bufo),

o sapo corredor (Bufo calamita), o sapinho de verrugas verdes ibérico (Pelodytes ibericus), o

sapinho de verrugas verdes (Pelodytes punctatus; não estudado aqui), o sapo de unha negra

(Pelobates cultripes), a rã de focinho pontiagudo (Discoglossus galganoi) e o sapo parteiro

ibérico (Alytes cisternasii). Sabe-se que o P. clarkii é um predador eficaz de ovos e larvas de

todas as espécies de anfíbios existentes nesta área, podendo também infligir-lhes danos sub-

letais, como cortes nas caudas. A sua presença parece ter fortes consequências negativas para

estas comunidades de anfíbios, tendo sido registada uma diminuição da riqueza específica de

anfíbios em diversas áreas invadidas.

Embora existam estudos anteriores que já demonstraram alguns dos efeitos negativos que a

introdução de P. clarkii acarreta para as comunidades de anfíbios do Sudoeste da Península,

nenhum deles examinou os possíveis impactos indirectos que a presença de P. clarkii pode ter

sobre larvas de anfíbios desta área. Assim, esta tese teve como principal objectivo avaliar que

tipo de efeitos não letais pode o lagostim exercer nas formas larvares de nove das dez espécies

de anuros existentes no Sudoeste da Península Ibérica e que consequências podem os mesmos

ter para a fitness dos indivíduos. Os resultados permitirão entender melhor que espécies de

anuros são mais vulneráveis a invasões deste predador.

Foi avaliado o papel do P. clarkii como agente causador de elevadas frequências de cortes na

cauda em girinos de Pelobates cultripes e investigado em laboratório que tipo de

consequências podem esses danos ter para um girino nas suas taxas de mortalidade,

crescimento e desenvolvimento. Foi também realizada uma experiência em laboratório na

qual larvas de todas as espécies de anuros do Sudoeste da Península foram sujeitas à presença

de pistas químicas de P. clarkii, associadas ou não a cheiros de conspecíficos predados, com o

Page 16: PREDATOR-PREY INTERACTIONS BETWEEN AN INVASIVE …

iv

intuito de investigar se os girinos apresentam ou não respostas antipredatórias

(comportamentais, morfológicas ou na história vital) a este predador exótico. Estas respostas

foram comparadas com as apresentadas face a um predador nativo, larvas de libélula (Género

Aeshna). Foi ainda efectuada uma outra experiência, na qual girinos de cinco populações de

Pelophylax perezi com diferentes histórias de coexistência com o P. clarkii foram expostos à

presença deste predador, de modo a testar se em populações com uma mais longa história de

contacto com o P. clarkii são induzidas respostas antipredatórias diferentes das induzidas em

populações com menor ou nenhum tempo de coexistência com este predador.

No Parque Natural de Doñana, a frequência média de girinos de P. cultripes capturados com

cortes na cauda foi significativamente maior em locais onde o P. clarkii estava presente. A

perda de porções de cauda causou uma ligeira diminuição na sobrevivência dos girinos, mas

não afectou as suas taxas de crescimento e desenvolvimento, embora possa haver custos

tardios que não foram aqui detectados. As lesões na cauda causaram uma alteração na

morfologia caudal dos girinos após a regeneração, com os girinos lesionados a desenvolverem

músculos caudais mais altos e barbatanas caudais mais baixas. Estes efeitos variaram com a

frequência de corte e disponibilidade de alimento, tendo tido maior expressão nos indivíduos

mais alimentados.

A presença de lagostim, sobretudo quando associada a pistas químicas de conspecíficos

predados, induziu alterações no nível de actividade e na distância ao predador em girinos de

sete das nove espécies estudadas. Quatro espécies alteraram parâmetros da sua história vital,

enquanto apenas duas apresentaram alterações morfológicas em resposta ao predador

exótico. Enquanto as respostas comportamentais apresentadas pelas várias espécies foram

muito semelhantes, as alterações observadas na história vital variaram muito de espécie para

espécie, indicando respostas específicas a este predador consoante a espécie-presa em causa.

Por outro lado, quando expostas ao predador nativo, oito das nove espécies mostraram

plasticidade fenotípica em vários dos parâmetros estudados. Foi também encontrada uma

Page 17: PREDATOR-PREY INTERACTIONS BETWEEN AN INVASIVE …

v

maior integração nas respostas apresentadas quando as espécies se encontravam na presença

do predador nativo do que do exótico.

As populações de P. perezi com diferentes histórias de contacto com o P. clarkii diferiram nas

respostas antipredatórias apresentadas. Enquanto os girinos de populações que nunca

coexistiram com este predador diminuíram os níveis de actividade na sua presença, os girinos

de populações com mais de trinta anos de coexistência não o fizeram. Em vez disso, alteraram

a sua morfologia para uma forma que melhora a sua capacidade de fuga à predação por P.

clarkii, o que parece indicar que houve, neste caso, evolução rápida de respostas ao lagostim e

adaptação local à sua presença.

Este trabalho permitiu concluir que em sete das nove espécies de anuros do Sudoeste da

Península Ibérica aqui estudadas foram induzidas respostas antipredatórias na presença não

letal de um lagostim exótico. Assumindo que as respostas são efectivas na redução da

vulnerabilidade ao lagostim, estas sete espécies parecem estar mais aptas a sobreviver e

resistir à invasão dos habitats aquáticos por P. clarkii. De qualquer forma, é essencial a

realização de estudos futuros que permitam entender se as respostas aqui apresentadas

conferem, de facto, vantagens para a sobrevivência a este predador. Por outro lado, as

espécies que não responderam a P. clarkii serão potencialmente mais vulneráveis à predação

por este lagostim e estarão, a longo prazo, sujeitas a declínios populacionais ou mesmo

extinções locais. Pelodytes ibericus é um destes casos, o que a torna numa espécie cujas

populações deveriam ser monitorizadas, tendo em conta o seu estatuto endémico no

Sudoeste da Península Ibérica e a sua distribuição relativamente limitada.

Uma vez que P. clarkii se encontra actualmente largamente distribuído pela Península Ibérica e

que a sua erradicação é impossível com os meios actuais, a implementação de medidas que

permitam a coexistência entre este predador exótico e as comunidades nativas de anfíbios é

uma das poucas opções que resta. Neste sentido, estudar a dinâmica das interacções entre

Page 18: PREDATOR-PREY INTERACTIONS BETWEEN AN INVASIVE …

vi

predadores exóticos e presas nativas, assim como identificar as espécies mais vulneráveis a

invasões e com diferentes potenciais para evolução rápida de defesas antipredatórias podem

ser ferramentas fundamentais para que as populações-presa persistam em áreas invadidas.

Palavras-chave: Invasões biológicas, predador aquático introduzido, plasticidade fenotípica,

predação subletal, anfíbios, Península Ibérica

Page 19: PREDATOR-PREY INTERACTIONS BETWEEN AN INVASIVE …

1

CHAPTER 1

General Introduction

Page 20: PREDATOR-PREY INTERACTIONS BETWEEN AN INVASIVE …

2

Page 21: PREDATOR-PREY INTERACTIONS BETWEEN AN INVASIVE …

Chapter 1

3

1. GENERAL INTRODUCTION

1.1. Predator-prey interactions

Predation is a major selective force influencing the dynamics, structure and evolutionary

processes of prey communities (Lima and Dill 1990, Johnson and Agrawal 2003). Predators

affect dynamics of prey populations not only by direct predation, but also by imposing direct

or indirect nonlethal effects upon them (Lima 1998, Preisser et al. 2005, Sih et al. 2010). Direct

nonlethal effects result from prey employing costly defensive antipredator strategies (Lima

1998, Preisser et al. 2005) or from consequences of injury caused by incomplete or

unsuccessful predation attempts (Morin 1985, Harris 1989). Predators may also impact prey

communities via indirect effects propagated through the trophic web that end up impacting

the focal prey species (Wilbur 1997, Peacor and Werner 2001, Werner and Peacor 2003). In

fact, nonlethal predatory effects have been suggested to exert as strong or stronger effects

than direct consumption in the dynamics of prey populations, and this seems to be particularly

evident in aquatic ecosystems (Preisser et al. 2005, Werner and Peacor 2006).

Defensive antipredator strategies

In order to evade predation risk, prey have developed a variety of predator avoidance

defences. These defences can be constitutive and expressed at all times, or inducible and only

expressed when predators are present. Constitutive defences, which are permanently

expressed, are elicited when predation risk is stable over time (Clark and Harvell 1992, Edgell

et al. 2009). On the contrary, inducible defences are expressed in response to spatially or

temporally variable predation risk and depend on the existence of cues that allow prey to

accurately detect the risk of predation. Further, inducible defences often incur fitness costs in

Page 22: PREDATOR-PREY INTERACTIONS BETWEEN AN INVASIVE …

Chapter 1

4

the absence of predators, which prevents them from becoming constitutive (Harvell 1990,

Clark and Harvell 1992, Tollrian and Harvell 1998).

Inducible defences are a widespread form of phenotypic plasticity, the capacity of an individual

to alter its phenotype in response to alterations in environmental biotic or abiotic factors

(Pigliucci 2001, DeWitt and Scheiner 2004, Whitman and Agrawal 2009). Phenotypic plasticity

is ubiquitous and has been documented in many organisms (reviewed in Tollrian and Harvell

1998, Sultan 2000, Pigliucci 2001, DeWitt and Scheiner 2004, Whitman and Agrawal 2009).

Through predator-induced plasticity prey express phenotypic alterations in behaviour,

morphology, life-history or physiology which, by reducing vulnerability to predators, often act

as adaptive antipredator defences (Tollrian and Harvell 1998, Agrawal 2001). Inducible

antipredator defences are very variable and occur in a diversity of taxa. Some of the best

known examples include the production of defensive chemicals in plants (Berenbaum and

Zangerl 1998), diel vertical migration in cladocerans (e.g. Bollens et al. 1992, Boersma et al.

1998), formation of defensive structures such as helmets and neck teeth in cladocerans (e.g.

Spitze 1992, Boersma et al. 1998), changes in shell shape and thickness in molluscs (e.g.

Trussell and Smith 2000, Hoverman and Relyea 2007), alterations in body shape in fish (e.g.

Brönmark and Pettersson 1994) and alterations in behaviour and morphology in anuran

tadpoles (e.g. McCollum and Van Buskirk 1996).

Many experimental studies show that predator-induced phenotypic plasticity is widespread in

larval amphibians (reviewed in Lima and Dill 1990, Kats and Dill 1998, Benard 2004). In

response to strong predation pressure, many amphibian larvae alter their behaviour,

morphology, physiology or life-history. While little attention has been devoted to the study of

physiological plasticity (but see Steiner and Van Buskirk 2008), plasticity in behaviour,

morphology and life-history in response to predator cues has been thoroughly explored in this

group.

Page 23: PREDATOR-PREY INTERACTIONS BETWEEN AN INVASIVE …

Chapter 1

5

Behavioural defensive strategies in tadpoles may consist in aggregation and formation of

schools (e.g. Watt et al. 1997, DeVito 2003, Spieler 2003), a shift to safer microhabitats (e.g.

Cruz and Rebelo 2005), spatial avoidance of the predator (e.g. Laurila et al. 1997, Relyea 2001,

Nicieza et al. 2006) or a reduction in activity level (e.g. Skelly and Werner 1990, Skelly 1992,

Griffiths et al. 1998, Anholt et al. 2000, Relyea 2001, 2004, Richardson 2001, Van Buskirk 2002,

Richter-Boix et al. 2007, Laurila et al. 2006, 2008). All these behavioural alterations benefit

tadpoles, as they reduce the frequency of predator-prey encounter rates or of predator

attacks, which increases their odds of survival (Lima and Dill 1990, Kats and Dill 1998, McPeek

2004). Specifically, a decrease in activity level is one of the most commonly reported

behavioural defences against predators and seems to be an important mechanism decreasing

predation rates (Lima and Dill 1990, Van Buskirk and McCollum 2000a). This defence may be so

widespread because movement is generally used by predators to detect prey, usually causing a

positive correlation between activity level and mortality risk (Werner and Anholt 1993, Skelly

1994). As such, there should be strong selection against tadpoles having high activity levels in

the presence of predators, in order to minimize the chances of prey detectability and of

predators incurring fatal attacks.

Plastic alterations in morphology are also pervasive responses to predation risk in larval

amphibians (e.g. McCollum and Van Buskirk 1996, McCollum and Leimberger 1997, Van

Buskirk et al. 1997, Van Buskirk and McCollum 2000a, Lardner 2000, Relyea and Werner 2000,

Relyea 2001, 2004, Van Buskirk 2002, LaFiandra and Babbitt 2004, Laurila et al. 2006, 2008,

Richter-Boix et al. 2007, Van Buskirk 2009, Hettyey et al. 2010). These usually consist in an

increase in tadpole’s tail depth, an enhancement of distal tail coloration, and the development

of smaller headbodies. Although the functionalities of these traits are not completely

understood, smaller headbodies and deeper tails may enhance swimming ability, enable

tadpoles to generate faster swimming bursts and improve manoeuvrability (McCollum and

Leimberger 1997, Dayton et al. 2005; but see Van Buskirk and McCollum 2000b, Johnson et al.

Page 24: PREDATOR-PREY INTERACTIONS BETWEEN AN INVASIVE …

Chapter 1

6

2008). Deeper and more brightly coloured tails may lure predator strikes away from the

headbody into the more regenerable tail and may also facilitate membranous tail ripping when

a predator attack takes place (Doherty et al. 1998, Van Buskirk et al. 2003). These predator-

induced alterations in morphology increase the ability to escape from attacking predators and

enhance the probability of survival under predation risk (McCollum and Van Buskirk 1996, Van

Buskirk et al. 1997, Van Buskirk and McCollum 2000a, Dayton et al. 2005, Álvarez and Nicieza

2006, Johnson et al. 2008, Hettyey et al. 2010).

Frequently, the expression of behavioural and morphological defences entail growth and

development costs for prey, due to either low foraging rates or a high investment in

production and maintenance of defensive structures (e.g. Skelly 1992, McCollum and Van

Buskirk 1996, Van Buskirk 2000, Relyea 2002, LaFiandra and Babbitt 2004, Steiner 2007a). This

leads to the growth/predation risk trade-off hypothesis, which states that increasing defences

in order to avoid predation risk often comes at the expense of reduced growth (e.g. Werner

and Anholt 1993, Werner 1986, Lima and Dill 1990, Lima 1998, McPeek 2004, Stoks et al.

2005). Further, this often results in reduced body size and/or delayed metamorphosis which

can decrease future fitness by reducing viability during harsh conditions, especially in seasonal

environments, delaying maturity and reducing size at first reproduction and reproductive

success (Smith 1987, Semlitsch et al. 1988, Berven 1990).

In the presence of predators, phenotypic plasticity in prey life-history may either derive from

predator-induced alterations in behaviour or morphology or may result from direct responses

to predation risk (Beckerman et al. 2007, Relyea 2007, Steiner 2007b, Higginson and Ruxton

2009). In organisms with complex life histories, such as amphibians, predation risk can alter

the timing of major life-history transitions, by shaping both hatching and metamorphic

decisions. Amphibians have been shown to alter the timing of hatching in order to reduce the

risk of predation, either by accelerating or delaying it (e.g. Johnson et al. 2003, Warkentin

2011). Both time to and size at metamorphosis can also experience plastic adjustments in

Page 25: PREDATOR-PREY INTERACTIONS BETWEEN AN INVASIVE …

Chapter 1

7

response to predation pressure (reviewed in Benard 2004, Relyea 2007, Tejedo et al. 2010).

These traits are critical life-history attributes that greatly determine post-metamorphic fitness

in amphibians (Smith 1987, Semlitsch et al. 1988, Berven 1990). In response to predatory cues,

amphibians can shorten their time to metamorphosis and, by leaving the risky predator

environment earlier, increase their chances of survival (Babbitt and Tanner 1998, Benard 2004,

Moore et al. 2004, Higginson and Ruxton 2009). Nevertheless, a reduced time to

metamorphosis seems to be a quite rare response, maybe because shorter larval periods may

induce smaller metamorphic sizes which, in turn, can lead to reduced post-metamorphic

fitness (Relyea 2007). Increasing larval growth rate and size at metamorphosis can also be a

direct and effective response to predation, since large size can provide a safe refuge from gape

or size-limited predators (Urban 2007). This can also be advantageous because larger sizes at

metamorphosis are associated with larger sizes at maturity, increased fecundity and/or

reproductive success (Smith 1987, Semlitsch et al. 1988, Berven 1990).

A number of theoretical studies have predicted how predation risk should alter the time and

size at which prey metamorphose (e.g. Wilbur and Collins 1973, Werner 1986, Rowe and

Ludwig 1991, Abrams and Rowe 1996, Higginson and Ruxton 2010) but, except for the most

recent study, their predictions (metamorphosis earlier and at a smaller size) rarely match with

empirical results (Benard 2004, Relyea 2007). This indicates that predator-induced alterations

in life-history traits are complex and highly variable, which probably reflects the fact that these

alterations are often costs of plastic behavioural or morphological defences (Relyea 2007,

Steiner 2007b).

Often, behavioural and morphological defences, as well as life-history trait alterations, are not

just direct and independent responses to predation, but occur simultaneously and largely

influence each other (Lima 1998, Van Buskirk and McCollum 2000a, Steiner 2007b). These

traits are often functionally correlated and express complex patterns of association, indicating

phenotypic trait integration (Relyea 2001, Steiner 2007b). The degree of phenotypic

Page 26: PREDATOR-PREY INTERACTIONS BETWEEN AN INVASIVE …

Chapter 1

8

integration will then depend on the magnitude of the functional relationships among traits

(Relyea 2001). Many different and complex associations between activity, tail depth, growth

rate, body size and larval period are possible and these relationships are frequently not linear

(Relyea 2001, Benard 2004, Steiner 2007b, Laurila et al. 2008, Higginson and Ruxton 2009). For

example, behavioural and morphological defences may complement each other, but they may

also augment each other, since a reduction in activity may save energy, which can then be

used to develop morphological defensive structures (Johansson and Andersson 2009). Further,

although inducible defences may incur fitness costs, other studies have, in contrast, shown

that either reducing activity or inducing morphological defences in the presence of predators

may also result in increased growth rates (Spitze 1992, Peacor 2002, Johansson and Andersson

2009). As a result, relationships between different traits are very variable and several different

outcomes may result from predator-induced plastic trait alterations.

In amphibians, antipredator defences induced by prey can change through ontogeny (e.g. Kats

and Dill 1998, Eklöv and Werner 2000, Relyea 2003, Laurila et al. 2004, Hettyey et al. 2010).

During development, larvae experience periods of different vulnerability to predation - due,

for instance, to ontogenetic changes in body size - which may require different types and

magnitudes of antipredator defenses (Werner and Anholt 1993, Relyea 2003). Generally, prey

vulnerability and intensity of responses to predators decrease as prey size increases, the

strongest responses being elicited early in tadpole development (Eklöv and Werner 2000,

Relyea 2003, Laurila et al. 2004).

Cues used to assess predation risk

In order for prey to accurately assess predation risk and to elicit adequate antipredator

defences, reliable cues providing information on the potential predator have to be available. In

aquatic systems, prey use a variety of visual, tactile or chemical cues separately or in different

Page 27: PREDATOR-PREY INTERACTIONS BETWEEN AN INVASIVE …

Chapter 1

9

combinations to evaluate and adjust to predation risk (Lima and Dill 1990, Smith 1992, Kats

and Dill 1998). In these environments, chemical cues acquire special relevance as a reliable

source of information, because they allow prey to detect predators from a safe distance and

because they are effective in low-visibility conditions, such as at night, in turbid waters, or in

densely vegetated habitats (Kats and Dill 1998, Petranka and Hayes 1998, Ferrari et al. 2010).

Chemically-mediated predator detection is widespread among aquatic animals, such as

invertebrates, fish and amphibians (reviewed in Chivers and Smith 1998, Kats and Dill 1998,

Wisenden 2003, Ferrari et al. 2010). The chemical cues to which prey respond may arise from

predator-specific odours (predator kairomones), may be cues actively or passively released by

disturbed or injured prey (disturbance or alarm cues) or may be digestive by-products released

after predators consume prey (diet cues) (Chivers and Smith 1998, Wisenden 2003,

Schoeppner and Relyea 2005, 2009, Fraker et al 2009, Ferrari et al. 2010).

Predator kairomones are chemical signals resulting from the predator’s scent itself, which are

received by and favour prey, but do not benefit the predator (Kats and Dill 1998, Ferrari et al.

2010). They are important for prey to obtain information about predation risk and often allow

for antipredator responses to be elicited in order to evade predators (e.g. Petranka and Hayes

1998, Pettersson et al. 2000, Bryer et al. 2001, Gallie et al. 2001, Van Buskirk and Arioli 2002,

Vilhunen and Hirvonen 2003). Disturbance cues consist in chemical signals released by

disturbed or distressed prey prior to being captured, and seem to be quite widespread among

aquatic taxa (reviewed in Ferrari et al. 2010). When these cues are detected by other prey,

they usually induce weaker antipredator responses than alarm cues do, because they indicate

less risk (Chivers and Smith 1998, Gonzalo et al. 2010). Alarm cues are chemicals released by

prey upon mechanical damage caused by a predator during a predation event (Chivers and

Smith 1998, Ferrari et al. 2010). They are produced by specialised skin cells and/or released

through a regulated secretory process, inform other (conspecific or not) prey about the

presence of an active and dangerous predator in the environment and invoke antipredator

Page 28: PREDATOR-PREY INTERACTIONS BETWEEN AN INVASIVE …

Chapter 1

10

responses (Chivers and Smith 1998, Fraker et al. 2009, Ferrari et al. 2010). Several aquatic

organisms have been shown to respond to damage-released alarm cues, probably because

they provide an early warning of a predator’s presence and facilitate responses to a wide array

of predators (reviewed in Chivers and Smith 1998, Wisenden 2003, Ferrari et al. 2010). Diet

cues are post-digestion chemicals released by the predator’s digestive system during

defecation, most likely resultant from prey tissues or prey alarm cues modified by the

predator’s digestive enzymes (Wisenden 2003, Schoeppner and Relyea 2009, Ferrari et al.

2010). Diet cues also induce plastic alterations in prey and several studies have shown that

many prey species only elicit antipredator responses if the predator has been fed conspecifics

of the focal prey species (e.g. Stirling 1995, McCollum and Leimberger 1997, Slusarczk 1999,

Schoeppner and Relyea 2005, 2009). Nevertheless, many of these studies cannot distinguish if

prey are responding to either damage-released alarm cues, to a combination of alarm cues and

predator-specific odours or to diet-released cues; they can only assure that cues from

predation events mediate the responses (but see e.g. LaFiandra and Babbitt 2004, Schoeppner

and Relyea 2009). Still, cues resulting from digestion seem to be very important for some prey

species to induce complete and effective antipredator defences (Schoeppner and Relyea

2009). Further, by using cues that provide information about both the identity of the predator

and of the consumed prey species, prey will more finely assess predation risk, which can be

beneficial, since overresponding to non dangerous threats would divert energy away from

other fitness-related activities.

Unsuccessful predation attempts and nonlethal prey injury

Predators are often unsuccessful in their attempts to capture prey (Morin 1985, Abrams 1989).

Most often these failed or incomplete predation attempts result in sublethal damage being

inflicted to the attacked prey. Nonlethal injury is conspicuous in nature and can, in the long

Page 29: PREDATOR-PREY INTERACTIONS BETWEEN AN INVASIVE …

Chapter 1

11

term, depress prey survival (Harris 1989). A particular and common form of predator-inflicted

injury consists in autotomy, the self-induced release of a body part as an escape strategy to

predator attacks, a phenomenon observed in animals such as damselflies, salamanders and

lizards (Maginnis 2006).

In freshwater habitats, it is common to find amphibian prey injured due to ineffective

predation attempts (Wilbur and Semlitsch 1990, Bowerman et al. 2010). Specifically, anuran

larvae are often found with heavily damaged tails and the pattern and severity of damage

seems to vary from species to species (Wilbur and Semlitsch 1990, Tejedo 1993, Axelsson et al.

1997, McCollum and Leimberger 1997, Blair and Wassersug 2000, Nyström et al. 2001). In fact,

tail loss in larval anurans may be an important mechanism in reducing the effects of predation,

because the high fragility of their tail fin allows it to be easily torn by predators, helping

tadpoles survive otherwise lethal attacks (Wilbur and Semlitsch 1990, Doherty et al. 1998, Van

Buskirk et al. 2003).

Still, caudal injury may incur substantial short and long term costs to amphibians. Short term

costs consist in a decreased tail length and consequent reduced swimming ability, since tail

length affects tadpole swimming speed (Wassersug and Sperry 1977, Figiel and Semlitsch

1991, Teplitsky et al. 2005; but see Van Buskirk and McCollum 2000b). The diminished

swimming capacity may affect escape abilities and increase vulnerability to predation and the

odds of tadpoles suffering further damage (Semlitsch 1990, Figiel and Semlitsch 1991, Teplitsky

et al. 2005). Tail loss may also significantly reduce fitness in the short term, because the

process of tail regeneration that follows increases energetic expenditure and diverts energy

away from growth and development into the repair of the damaged tissues (Wassersug and

Sperry 1977, Semlitsch and Reichling 1989, Van Buskirk and McCollum 2000b). Nonlethal

caudal injury may also potentially incur long term costs in terms of reduced larval growth,

development and survival (Morin 1985, Semlitsch and Reichling 1989, Semlitsch 1990, Wilbur

and Semlitsch 1990, Parichy and Kaplan 1992). As the tadpole tail is resorbed and its materials

Page 30: PREDATOR-PREY INTERACTIONS BETWEEN AN INVASIVE …

Chapter 1

12

reinvested during metamorphosis, delayed costs of the loss of tail tissue may only arise at

metamorphosis. Tadpoles with injured tails may emerge later and at a smaller size, which can

have severe consequences for post-metamorphic fitness (Smith 1987, Semlitsch et al. 1988,

Berven 1990). Still, consequences of tail injury seem to be quite variable and depend on both

resource availability and injury frequency and severity (Parichy and Kaplan 1992).

1.2. Invasive alien species

Invasive alien species are species introduced outside their natural range, which populations

grow successfully and get established in the new habitat and that often become a nuisance

and a threat to native biota (Parker et al. 1999, Mack et al. 2000, Richardson et al. 2000,

Lockwood et al. 2007). The rate at which novel species are introduced into new environments

has reached an unprecedented rate and is continuously increasing (Vitousek et al. 1997,

Thompson 1998, Strauss et al. 2006). While spread of invasive species may be due to natural

processes, most dispersion across biogeographical barriers is a direct consequence of human

activities such as agriculture, aquaculture, recreation and global commerce (Lodge 1993,

Vitousek et al. 1997, Mack et al. 2000, Sakai et al. 2001, Lockwood et al. 2007).

The introduction and spread of non-native species into new regions is of global concern,

because it poses a threat to natural ecological communities and is nowadays one of the major

challenges facing global biodiversity (Lodge 1993, Vitousek et al. 1997, Mack et al. 2000, Sala

et al. 2000). Invasive species may alter native communities in many ways, acting as novel

predators, vectors of pathogens, parasites and competitors of natives or by promoting

hybridization with indigenous species (Mack et al. 2000, Sakai et al. 2001, Mitchell et al. 2006).

By creating novel ecological contexts, introduced species alter the structure and functioning of

invaded ecosystems, often causing irreversible environmental damage which results in the

displacement or extirpation of native species (Vitousek et al. 1997, Parker et al. 1999, Mack et

Page 31: PREDATOR-PREY INTERACTIONS BETWEEN AN INVASIVE …

Chapter 1

13

al. 2000, Sakai et al. 2001, Clavero and García-Berthou 2005, Schlaepfer et al. 2005, Mitchell et

al. 2006, Salo et al. 2007). Examples of invasions causing decline or extinction of local native

species have been documented worldwide. Among the most ecologically destructive are the

introduction of the Asian chestnut blight fungus (Cryphonectria parasitica) in eastern United

States, water hyacinth (Eichornia crassipes) in tropical lakes and rivers all around the world,

crayfish plague (Aphanomyces astaci) in Europe and cane toad (Rhinella marina) in Australia,

all considered by the IUCN Invasive Species Specialist Group to be among the 100 worst

invaders in the world (http://www.issg.org/database/welcome).

The introduction of alien predators, in particular, can have significant negative impacts on

native prey, not only at an individual or population levels, but often at community and

ecosystem levels (e.g. Townsend 1996, Mack et al. 2000, Kats and Ferrer 2003, Salo et al.

2007). Predator introductions are most dramatic on island ecosystems, where prey often lack

historical coexistence with certain predator archetypes (sensu Cox and Lima 2006). The major

declines and extinctions of native mammals in Australia caused by the introduction of red

foxes (Vulpes vulpes) and feral cats (Felis catus) illustrate this well (Dickman 1996). Also, the

introduction of the exotic brown tree snake (Boiga irregularis) on the island of Guam has had

an enormous negative impact on native reptiles, birds and mammals (Rodda et al. 1997).

Freshwater ecosystems seem to be especially vulnerable to introduced predators, which may

reflect multiscale widespread prey naiveté towards invasive predators in these habitats (Sala

et al. 2000, Cox and Lima 2006). These systems have been particularly affected by predator

invasions, one reason for that being the stocking of freshwater habitats throughout the world

with non-native fish predators for recreational purposes or elimination of pests and which

often result in serious negative impacts for native biota (Mooney and Cleland 2001, Cox and

Lima 2006, Gherardi 2007a).

Page 32: PREDATOR-PREY INTERACTIONS BETWEEN AN INVASIVE …

Chapter 1

14

Crayfish have also been reported as high-impact freshwater invaders which, due to their

omnivorous diet (on detritus, algae, plants, invertebrates, amphibians and fish), act as

keystone species, often having severe negative effects at multiple trophic levels in freshwater

food webs (Lodge et al. 1994, Nyström et al. 1996, Nyström et al. 2001, Dorn and Wojdak

2004, Rodríguez et al. 2005, Gherardi and Acquistapace 2007). In Europe, there are only five

native crayfish species, all from the family Astacidae; however, several other crayfish species,

mostly indigenous from North America (mainly family Cambaridae), have been introduced into

European inland waters (Lodge et al. 2000, Holdich and Pöckl 2007, Holdich et al. 2009). These

introductions were often deliberate and due to commercial reasons (stocking or aquaculture)

or pet trade (Lodge et al. 2000, Gherardi 2006, Holdich et al. 2009). Invasive crayfish have

greatly contributed to marked declines in European crayfish species because they have

facilitated the spread of the crayfish plague in Europe, a disease caused by an oomycete

(Aphanomyces astaci), which does not affect North American species, but causes severe kill

offs in native species and also because of their superior competitive abilities (Lodge et al. 2000,

Holdich et al. 2009). Nowadays the invasive crayfish species with the widest distribution and of

most concern in Europe are Orconectes limosus (spiny-cheek crayfish), Pacifastacus leniusculus

(signal crayfish) and Procambarus clarkii (red swamp crayfish) (Holdich et al. 2009).

Amphibians are very vulnerable to aquatic invasions and the introduction of exotic predators

has been implicated in many amphibian declines (reviewed by Kats and Ferrer 2003). In fact,

alongside habitat destruction, pollution and the emergence of infectious diseases, this is one

of the main factors contributing to the worldwide amphibian declines and extinctions

(reviewed in Alford and Richards 1999, Blaustein and Kiesecker 2002, Collins and Storfer 2003).

The introduction of non-native amphibians and reptiles outside their native range, even

though often overlooked, has been shown to have negative consequences for native

amphibian populations (Scalera 2007, Polo-Cavia et al. 2010, Shine 2010). American bullfrogs

(Lithobates catesbeianus) are of special interest because, despite originating from the United

Page 33: PREDATOR-PREY INTERACTIONS BETWEEN AN INVASIVE …

Chapter 1

15

States, they are now widespread all over the world and, where introduced, have been

implicated in declines or displacement of indigenous amphibian species (Kiesecker and

Blaustein 1997, 1998, Kats and Ferrer 2003, Pearl et al. 2004, Adams and Pearl 2007).

In most cases, however, impacts have been caused by fish predators, recognized as major

contributors to drastic reductions and, at times, extirpation of native amphibian populations

(reviewed by Kats and Ferrer 2003; e.g. Finlay and Vredenburg 2007). For example, the

introduction of mosquitofish has contributed to the strong reduction of several amphibian

species around the world: Taricha torosa in Southern California (Gamradt and Kats 1996),

Lissotriton helveticus and Mesotriton alpestris in Europe (Denoël et al. 2005) and Litoria aurea

in Australia (White and Pyke 1996, Hamer et al. 2002). Amphibians from mountainous areas

seem to be particularly affected by fish invasions, since many studies have shown that the

introduction of exotic fish into montane lakes for recreational purposes has caused a decrease

in amphibian abundance and diversity in several regions (e.g. Spain: Braña et al. 1996; North

America: Pilliod and Peterson 2001, Knapp 2005, Pearson and Goater 2008, Pope 2008, Pilliod

et al. 2010).

Crayfish have also been reported as important contributors to declines of native amphibians

(Kats and Ferrer 2003). The introduced signal crayfish (Pacifastacus leniusculus) predates on

embryonic and larval amphibians and its presence seems to have caused negative impacts in

invaded amphibian populations in southern Sweden (Axelsson et al. 1997, Nyström and

Abjörnsson 2000, Nyström et al. 2001, Nyström et al. 2002). The introduction and spread of

the exotic red swamp crayfish (Procambarus clarkii) in mountain streams in California has been

indicated as the most likely cause for the declines in populations of the California newt Taricha

torosa. These declines were probably due to efficient predation by crayfish on newt eggs and

larvae, increased rates of tail injury and decreased amphibian breeding activity in sites

colonized by the crayfish (Gamradt and Kats 1996, Gamradt et al. 1997). Laboratory

experiments have shown P. clarkii to consume eggs and larvae of several native European

Page 34: PREDATOR-PREY INTERACTIONS BETWEEN AN INVASIVE …

Chapter 1

16

amphibians (Gherardi et al. 2001, Renai and Gherardi 2004, Cruz and Rebelo 2005, Cruz et al.

2006b). Further, many field studies have documented a negative association between the

presence of some amphibian species and this exotic crayfish in Europe (Beja and Alcazar 2003,

Rodríguez et al. 2005, Cruz et al. 2006a,b, 2008, Ficetola et al. 2011).

1.3. Inducible responses in the presence of invasive alien predators

For prey to detect and recognise predators and elicit adequate antipredator mechanisms there

needs to be a shared evolutionary history between predator and prey (Strauss et al. 2006, Sih

et al. 2010). When invasive predators colonise areas outside their historical geographic range,

new predator-prey interactions are established and the lack of evolutionary experience with

the invader may render native prey unable to recognise the novel predator as a dangerous

threat. Prey evolutionary naïveté may then either cause a failure to respond to novel predators

or result in inappropriate antipredator defences, compromising their ability to avoid predation

by exotic predators (Cox and Lima 2006, Strauss et al. 2006, Sih et al. 2010). The degree of such

naïveté can be higher if the phylogenetic and ecological similarity between the invader and

native predators is low, which may increase the impact that the introduction has in the

invaded ecosystem (Cox and Lima 2006, Strauss et al. 2006, Sih et al. 2010). Hence, a novel

predator very distantly or not at all related to native predators is very likely to cause a high

impact in native prey communities (Ricciardi and Atkinson 2004, Strauss et al. 2006, Sih et al.

2010).

Several studies have reported an inability of native prey to elicit antipredator responses when

exposed to cues from introduced predators (e.g. Gamradt and Kats 1996, Hutson et al. 2005,

McLean et al. 2007, McEvoy et al. 2008, Polo-Cavia et al. 2010, Gomez-Mestre and Diaz-

Paniagua 2011). This lack of responses is especially likely to occur when exotic predators

invade systems that have historically lacked predators, such as some oceanic islands or alpine

Page 35: PREDATOR-PREY INTERACTIONS BETWEEN AN INVASIVE …

Chapter 1

17

lakes (Braña et al. 1996, Rodda et al. 1997, Blackburn et al. 2004, Knapp 2005). At times,

animals also show ineffective or inappropriate responses towards exotic predators (Sih et al.

2010). This is the case of the native European water vole Arvicola terrestris, which responds to

the introduced American mink Neovison vison by taking refuge in a burrow, which results as an

ineffective response against the exotic predator (Macdonald and Harrington 2003). Surplus

killing events of native mammals and birds by foxes (Vulpes vulpes) in Australia have been also

suggested to be largely the result of ineffective antipredator defences (Short et al. 2002). This

lack of or maladaptive responses to invaders may be a major contributor to heavy native prey

mortality in the face of non-native predators and to the dramatic impacts often observed

following invasions (Cox and Lima 2006, Sih et al. 2010).

In contrast, some prey species are able to identify alien predators as potential threats and

elicit effective antipredator avoidance responses when exposed to their cues. This ability of

responding with defences to unnatural predators probably plays a key role in enabling native

prey and invasive predators to successfully coexist in the same environment. Alterations in

prey behaviour, morphology and life-history in response to exotic predators have been

documented in a wide variety of organisms, such as cladocerans (e.g. Cousyn et al. 2001, Fisk

et al. 2007), bivalves (e.g. Freeman and Byers 2006, Whitlow et al. 2003, Whitlow 2010), snails

(e.g. Trussel and Smith 2000, Edgell et al. 2009), fish (e.g. Nannini and Belk 2006, McLean et al.

2007, Stuart-Smith et al. 2008, Rehage et al. 2009), amphibians (e.g. Kiesecker and Blaustein

1997, 1998, Griffiths et al. 1998, Pearl et al. 2003, Marquis et al. 2004, Moore et al. 2004,

Bosch et al. 2006, Epp and Gabor 2008, Gall and Mathis 2010), birds (e.g. Peluc et al. 2008) and

mammals (e.g. Russel and Banks 2007, Sündermann et al. 2008). Although many of these

studies do not clearly address which process allows for prey to induce phenotypic alterations

in the presence of novel predators, this can be achieved through innate, learned or evolved

mechanisms.

Page 36: PREDATOR-PREY INTERACTIONS BETWEEN AN INVASIVE …

Chapter 1

18

Prey may innately detect unnatural threats and elicit suitable avoidance mechanisms against

introduced predators in the absence of a common evolutionary history when the exotic

predator is sufficiently similar to a native predator, if prey show generalized antipredator

responses or if they rely on general predation cues to evaluate and respond to predation risk

(Rehage et al. 2009). An invasive alien predator which is closely phylogenetically or

phenotypically similar to native predators, may also be ecologically similar (e.g. in terms of

cues or predation technique), which promotes prey responses to the novel predator (Ferrari et

al. 2007, Epp and Gabor 2008, Rehage et al. 2009, Sih et al. 2010, 2011). Innate responses to

non-native predators may also occur if prey exhibit generalized antipredator responses,

whereby exposure to different types of predation risk induce the development of the same

defences. Examples of this include avoidance responses to any large moving animal or to a

general fish odour (Magurran 1990, Gherardi et al. 2011). Probably many times this is verified

because some prey rely on general predation cues to evaluate and respond to predation risk.

In aquatic systems, general cues usually consist in disturbance or damage alarm cues released

by stressed or captured prey or in chemicals resulting from the predator’s digestion (reviewed

in Chivers and Smith 1998, Ferrari et al. 2010). If, in the face of predation events, the presence

of an invasive predator is associated with these general cues, predators may become

chemically ‘labelled’ by their diet, facilitating detection of and responses to the novel potential

threat (Marquis et al. 2004, Sündermann et al. 2008, Sih et al. 2010, 2011).

Even if animals do not innately respond to a novel predator they may, through time and due to

experience, learn the identity of the invader. Learning may play a key role in allowing native

prey species to evoke appropriate antipredator defences against unknown predators

(reviewed in Ferrari et al. 2010). In aquatic systems learning occurs through the coupling of

conspecific alarm cues with specific-predator odours and often only requires a single

conditioning event (Mirza et al. 2006). After this conditioning period, prey learn to elicit

antipredator responses to the predator odour alone, allowing for subsequent recognition of

Page 37: PREDATOR-PREY INTERACTIONS BETWEEN AN INVASIVE …

Chapter 1

19

the novel predator stimuli. Learned recognition of predator odours has been widely reported

among freshwater organisms, especially in fish (e.g. Korpi and Wisenden 2001, Kelley and

Magurran 2003, Kristensen and Closs 2004, Ferrari et al. 2005, Ferrari and Chivers 2006) and

amphibians (e.g.Mandrillon and Saglio 2005, Mirza et al. 2006, Gonzalo et al. 2007, Ferrari et

al. 2008, Shah et al. 2010), but also in other taxa (e.g. crustaceans: Bool et al. 2011).

Nevertheless, it is to notice that, even though learning can be highly beneficial for recognising

novel predators, it does not necessarily mean that the responses elicited will be effective in

evading that specific predator (Sih et al. 2010, 2011).

Since invasive predators are agents of novel selection, native species may evolve in response

to biological invasions. Given enough time, the strong selection pressure imposed by invaders

may induce evolutionary changes in native prey, promoting the evolution of predator

recognition and adaptive antipredator defences (Schlaepfer et al. 2005, Strauss et al. 2006, Sih

et al. 2010). This evolutionary process frequently occurs in contemporary timescales, often

within decades, being called as rapid (Reznick and Ghalambor 2001). For rapid evolutionary

changes to occur a particularly strong and consistent selection pressure has to be imposed by

the novel predator and there has to be a differential genetic susceptibility of individuals to the

predator. Further, native prey populations have to be large enough to withstand the negative

ecological effects incurred by the invader in the short term (Reznick and Ghalambor 2001,

Strauss et al. 2006). Several studies have documented rapid directional adaptive evolution in

native organisms following invasions (reviewed in Thompson 1998, Reznick and Ghalambor

2001, Strauss et al. 2006; e.g. Shine 2011), and introduced predators are often the selective

agent involved. Rapid evolution of behavioural, morphological or life-history adaptations in

response to exotic predators is pervasive and has been reported in prey such as bivalves (e.g.

Freeman and Byers 2006), snails (e.g. Trussel and Smith 2000, Edgell et al. 2009), crustaceans

(e.g. Cousyn et al. 2001, Fisk et al. 2007), fish (e.g. O’Steen et al. 2002) and amphibians (e.g.

Kiesecker and Blaustein 1997). Specifically in amphibians, animals with a generation time much

Page 38: PREDATOR-PREY INTERACTIONS BETWEEN AN INVASIVE …

Chapter 1

20

longer than the other abovementioned taxa, tadpoles of the native red-legged frog (Rana

aurora) have evolved the ability to detect and respond with adaptive avoidance behaviours to

chemical cues of the introduced bullfrog (Rana catesbeiana) in a period of time as short as 70

years (Kiesecker and Blaustein 1997). The evolution of adaptive responses in native prey

populations is the most effective mechanism in enhancing the probability of long term prey

population persistence.

Finally, within the same community, species may greatly vary in their ability to perceive and

elicit responses to invasive predators. For instance, Pearl et al. (2003) found different

responses in two native anurans to cues from two introduced predators, the bluegill sunfish

(Lepomis macrochirus) and the red swamp crayfish (P. clarkii): no evidence of predator

recognition was found in the Pacific treefrog (Pseudacris regilla), while the northern red-legged

frog (Rana aurora aurora) responded behaviourally to both introduced predators. As such, it is

not easy to predict how a certain prey species will react to the presence of an exotic predator.

Some features that may help explain variation in the ability of prey to respond to novel

predators may be the type of cues that prey use to gauge risk (use of broad, general cues as

opposed to specific predator cues), the existence of natural traits that facilitate escape to

predators and the extent of phenotypic plasticity that species present (Sih et al. 2011). Many

more factors will surely affect this and, in the end, all of them will influence a species ability to

survive and, in the long term, thrive in invasion altered environments.

1.4. Species in focus

The Iberian Peninsula currently maintains over 70 non-indigenous freshwater species. These

species include a wide variety of invertebrate and vertebrate taxa, flatworms, molluscs,

crustaceans and fish being the most common ones (García-Berthou et al. 2007, Cobo et al.

2010). In this area, the most abundantly spread invasive decapod crustacean is the red swamp

Page 39: PREDATOR-PREY INTERACTIONS BETWEEN AN INVASIVE …

Chapter 1

21

crayfish, Procambarus clarkii (Gherardi 2006, García-Berthou et al. 2007). At the same time,

there is only one native species of crayfish (Austropotamobius pallipes), which is currently

thought extinct in Portugal, and which has recently been proposed as being non-native to the

Iberian Peninsula (Holdich et al. 2009). The introduction of invasive aquatic species, especially

large predators, into the Iberian Peninsula acquires special relevance, since this area is a

hotspot of freshwater biodiversity comprising, among others, several endemic and nearly-

endemic amphibian species (Loureiro et al. 2008).

The invasive predator: red swamp crayfish (Procambarus clarkii Girard, 1852)

P. clarkii is a crayfish species originally endemic to northeastern Mexico and southcentral USA

that, due to its commercial value, has been largely introduced outside its natural range. It has

proven a successful invader, being now present in all continents except Australia and

Antarctica, making it the most cosmopolitan crayfish species in the world (Hobbs et al. 1989,

Gherardi 2006, Holdich et al. 2009). P. clarkii was first introduced into Spain (and Europe) in

1973 near Badajoz and one year later near Seville, for aquaculture purposes and with the aim

of supplementing crayfish stocks, after many native crayfish populations had been devastated

by the crayfish plague (Habsburgo-Lorena 1983, Gherardi 2006, Holdich et al. 2009). P. clarkii

easily managed to escape the sites of release, soon colonising and expanding into new areas,

which was aided by the spread of live specimens by fishermen (Geiger et al. 2005, Gherardi

2006). Both this, and the fact that environmental characteristics in the Iberian Peninsula seem

to be highly suitable for the establishment of this invader, allowed for its subsequent

expansion all through the Peninsula, where it is currently extremely widespread and abundant

(Correia 2002, Gil-Sánchez and Alba-Tercedor 2002, Gherardi 2006, Holdich et al. 2009,

Capinha and Anastácio 2011). Probably through common hydrographic basins and with the

help of fisherman it invaded Portugal, where its presence was first registered in 1979, in the

Page 40: PREDATOR-PREY INTERACTIONS BETWEEN AN INVASIVE …

Chapter 1

22

Caia River, located quite close to Badajoz (Ramos and Pereira 1981). By 1984 it had arrived to

the Mondego region and, by 1990, it was already abundant in the Sado River basin (Southwest

Portugal) (Anastácio et al. 1995, R. Rebelo, pers. comm.).

Numerous biological and ecological characteristics of P. clarkii allow this crayfish to be such a

successful invader (reviewed in Geiger et al. 2005). P. clarkii exhibits rapid growth, high

fecundity, high reproductive potential, early maturity and it may, sometimes, have multiple

generations in a single year (Hobbs et al. 1989, Anastácio and Marques 1995, Gutiérrez-Yurrita

and Montes 1999). It is capable of plastically adjusting its reproductive life cycle to the specific

habitat conditions, allowing it to adapt to a diversity of environments (Gutiérrez-Yurrita and

Montes 1999). Further, it has a generalist and opportunistic diet, feeding on detritus, as well as

on diverse plant and animal resources, such as algae, macrophytes, macroinvertebrates,

amphibians and fish, according to their availability (e.g. Gherardi et al. 2001, Correia 2002,

2003, Renai and Gherardi 2004, Correia et al. 2005, Gherardi and Acquistapace 2007, Correia

and Anastácio 2008). P. clarkii is also capable of learning the identity of new unknown

predators, which may contribute to its adaptation to novel environments (Gherardi et al.

2011). This crayfish inhabits a wide variety of freshwater habitats including rivers, lakes, ponds,

canals and marshes and it has a high tolerance to adverse and extreme environmental

conditions, namely drought, high temperatures, low oxygen and pollution (Gutiérrez-Yurrita

and Montes 1999, Ilhéu et al. 2007, Capinha and Anastácio 2011). Finally, it has a high

dispersal ability, since overland dispersal over long distances seems to be a frequent

phenomenon (Gherardi and Barbaresi 2000, Barbaresi et al. 2004, Aquiloni et al. 2005, Kerby

et al. 2005, Cruz and Rebelo 2007). All these traits allow crayfish to quickly build extremely

large populations and promote its high invasive potential.

P. clarkii exerts wide environmental impacts in invaded communities, and often affects the

structure and functioning of aquatic ecosystems, being considered an ‘ecosystem engineer’

(Gherardi 2007a). By consuming and destroying aquatic macrophytes and decreasing

Page 41: PREDATOR-PREY INTERACTIONS BETWEEN AN INVASIVE …

Chapter 1

23

macroinvertebrate abundance, it causes a reduction in food resources and in habitat

complexity, such as refuges and spawning sites for fish and amphibians (Gutiérrez-Yurrita and

Montes 1999, Correia 2002, Rodríguez et al. 2003, 2005, Gherardi and Acquistapace 2007).

This, together with its bioturbation activity (promoted by burrowing, ingestion of sediment

particles and alterations in dissolved inorganic nutrients), may lead to an increase in water

turbidity due to blooms of surface microalgae and to particle resuspension (Rodríguez et al.

2003, 2005, Geiger et al. 2005, Gherardi 2007b). In its turn, this turbid state will reduce light

penetration and plant primary production, which can be followed by losses in biodiversity

(Rodríguez et al. 2003, 2005, Geiger et al. 2005). All these effects, alongside with crayfish

consuming rice seedlings, are known to negatively impact agricultural areas in several regions,

including the Iberian Peninsula (reviewed in Geiger et al. 2005). Both this and the fact that, due

to burrowing, P. clarkii may cause the collapse of banks in dams or dykes, indicate that this

crayfish may have serious economic impacts in areas where it has been introduced (Geiger et

al. 2005).

As a predator, P. clarkii relies more on chemical rather than visual cues to detect prey, being

particularly active at night (even though activity may also be high during the day) (Gherardi

and Barbaresi 2000, Aquiloni et al. 2005). At times, it adopts a sit-and-wait strategy to capture

prey, but it often acts as a moderately active predator, especially during the juvenile stage

(Gherardi et al. 2001, Renai and Gherardi 2004, Correia et al. 2005, Gomez-Mestre and Diaz-

Panigua 2011, A.L. Nunes, pers. obs.). Through ontogeny, its diet shifts from being more

carnivorous as juveniles to more herbivorous in adult stages (Correia 2003, Correia et al. 2005).

It seems to be a more efficient predator than native European crayfishes and to be immune to

toxic chemical compounds that protect some prey species from other predators (Gamradt and

Kats 1996, Gherardi et al. 2001, Renai and Gherardi 2004, Cruz and Rebelo 2005).

P. clarkii is an efficient predator of a wide array of aquatic organisms. It has been reported to

feed on a variety of insects, crustaceans, snails and both eggs and larvae of fish and

Page 42: PREDATOR-PREY INTERACTIONS BETWEEN AN INVASIVE …

Chapter 1

24

amphibians (e.g. Gamradt and Kats 1996, Gherardi et al. 2001, Correia 2002, 2003, Renai and

Gherardi 2004, Correia et al. 2005, Cruz and Rebelo 2005, Ilhéu et al. 2007). Other than

predating them, P. clarkii may affect amphibians through different processes. It is capable of

inflicting serious injury to amphibian prey (Gamradt et al. 1997, Leite 2003) and, similarly to

other invasive crayfishes (e.g. Nyström and Abjörnsson 2000, Nyström et al. 2001), it induces

alterations in behaviour, morphology and life-history of larval amphibians (Pearl et al. 2003,

Cruz and Rebelo 2005, Ortiz-Santaliestra et al. 2010, this study). Finally, it can cause the

displacement of amphibian populations from their natural breeding habitats and affect their

reproductive success (Gamradt and Kats 1996, Gamradt et al. 1997, Cruz et al. 2006a,b, 2008,

Ficetola et al. 2011).

The native prey: anurans from the Southwest of the Iberian Peninsula

In the Southwest of the Iberian Peninsula there are 14 amphibian species, ten anurans and

four urodeles. In this study only the anuran community is in focus, which is comprised by the

Iberian water frog (Pelophylax perezi Seoane 1885), the Mediterranean tree frog (Hyla

meridionalis Boettger 1874), the European tree frog (Hyla arborea Linnaeus 1758), the

common toad (Bufo bufo Linnaeus 1758)1, the natterjack toad (Bufo calamita Laurenti 1768),

the Iberian parsley frog (Pelodytes ibericus Sánchez-Herráiz, Barbadillo, Machordom and

Sanchiz 2000)2, the common parsley frog (Pelodytes punctatus Daudin 1802; not in focus here),

the Western spadefoot toad (Pelobates cultripes Cuvier 1829), the Iberian painted frog

(Discoglossus galganoi Capula, Nascetti, Lanza, Bullini and Crespo 1985) and the Iberian

midwife toad (Alytes cisternasii Boscá 1879) (Fig. 1). This area holds a remarkable endemic

1 A very recent study suggests that Iberian populations of B. bufo belong to a clade including North

African and French populations, denominated as Bufo spinosus (Recuero et al. 2011). 2 Pelodytes ibericus and Pelodytes punctatus were until recently considered the same species and their

distribution limits in the Iberian Peninsula are still not well established (Sánchez-Herráiz et al. 2000). Recent genetic data suggests that our study populations likely belong to P. ibericus (J. Díaz-Rodríguez, pers. com.).

Page 43: PREDATOR-PREY INTERACTIONS BETWEEN AN INVASIVE …

Chapter 1

25

anuran community: out of ten species, two are endemic for the Southwest region (P. ibericus

and A. cisternasii), one is endemic from the Iberian Peninsula (D. galganoi) (Loureiro et al.

2008) and five others have a fairly restricted distribution outside Portugal and Spain

(Pelophylax perezi, Hyla meridionalis, Pelodytes punctatus and Pelobates cultripes). Moreover,

three of these species have the status of ‘near threatened’ either internationally (H. arborea

and A. cisternasii) or nationally (D. galganoi) (Cabral et al. 2005).

Fig. 1. Photographs of the larval and adult stages of the nine Iberian anurans studied: a) Alytes

cisternasii, b) Pelodytes ibericus, c) Pelobates cultripes, d) Hyla arborea, e) Hyla meridionalis, f)

Pelophylax perezi, g) Bufo bufo, h) Bufo calamita and i) Discoglossus galganoi. Photos of adult animals of

pictures a), b), d) and g) taken by Erika Almeida.

a

)

b c

d e f

g h i

Page 44: PREDATOR-PREY INTERACTIONS BETWEEN AN INVASIVE …

Chapter 1

26

The nine studied anuran species are distributed along different breeding habitats, largely

defined by an hydroperiod gradient. A. cisternasii, B. bufo, H. arborea and P. perezi mainly

breed in permanent and extensive water bodies, such as streams, rivers and dams, although

they may also occur in temporary ponds. P. cultripes, P. ibericus and H. meridionalis generally

occur in temporary ponds, while B. calamita is typically found in small ephemeral ponds. D.

galganoi usually inhabits these latter habitats, but at times can be found in temporary water

bodies (Diaz-Paniagua 1990, Van Buskirk 2002, Richter-Boix et al. 2007, Loureiro et al. 2008).

Larvae of these species also have a different use of space in the water bodies, occupying

different microhabitats. Species like B. calamita, D. galganoi, P. perezi and A. cisternasii are

typical bottom-dwellers, spending most of the time at the floor of the ponds or streams (Diaz-

Paniagua 1985, Cruz and Rebelo 2005). On the contrary, P. cultripes, P. ibericus and the two

hylids are mainly found in the water column. P. ibericus also uses the shallow margins and Hyla

spp. are highly associated with vegetated areas of the ponds (Diaz-Paniagua 1985, Cruz and

Rebelo 2005). Tadpoles of B. bufo are continually active, but most of their time is spent in open

water areas (Cruz and Rebelo 2005).

Previous studies have tested the existence of predator-induced behavioural, morphological or

life-history plasticity in most of these species. In the presence of predators (usually Dytiscid

(Insecta: Coleoptera) or Aeshnid (Insecta: Odonata) larvae), most of these anuran species alter

their phenotypes in order to avoid or be defended from predation. Although results often vary

from study to study, all the nine anuran species seem to be able to develop behavioural

antipredator responses (B. bufo: Semlitsch and Gavasso 1992, Laurila et al. 1998; B. calamita:

Tejedo 1993; A. cisternasii: Cruz and Rebelo 2005; D. galganoi: Nicieza et al. 2006; P. perezi:

Gonzalo et al. 2007, Richter-Boix et al. 2007, Polo-Cavia et al. 2010, Gomez-Mestre and Díaz-

Paniagua 2011; P. ibericus: Cruz and Rebelo 2005; P. cultripes: Polo-Cavia et al. 2010; H.

meridionalis: Richter-Boix et al. 2007; H. arborea: Van Buskirk 2002). Morphological and life-

history plasticity in the presence of predators has also been reported in B. bufo (Álvarez and

Page 45: PREDATOR-PREY INTERACTIONS BETWEEN AN INVASIVE …

Chapter 1

27

Nicieza 2006, Richter-Boix et al. 2007, Van Buskirk 2009), D. galganoi (Nicieza et al. 2006), P.

perezi (Richter-Boix et al. 2007, Gomez-Mestre and Díaz-Paniagua 2011) and the two Hyla

species (Lardner 2000, Van Buskirk 2002, 2009, Richter-Boix et al. 2007, Polo-Cavia et al. 2010).

B. calamita consistently shows no alterations in morphology, while it has the capacity of

altering life-history traits (Lardner 2000, Richter-Boix et al. 2007). In A. cisternasii and P.

ibericus, predator-induced alterations in morphology and life-history have not been studied

before the present study, but A. obstetricans and P. punctatus, species closely related to these,

have been shown to alter these traits. P. fuscus, a species close to P. cultripes, did not show

alterations in growth or morphology when exposed to an invertebrate predator (Lardner

2000).

1.5. P. clarkii and the anurans from Southwestern Iberian Peninsula

In the Southwest of the Iberian Peninsula, the area of introduction of P. clarkii, this crayfish is

capable of successfully colonising all types of aquatic habitats where amphibians are found,

including small and shallow ponds (Cruz and Rebelo 2007). In this region, no native crayfish

species ever existed. Eggs and larvae of all the anuran species present in this area are readily

preyed by the crayfish (Cruz and Rebelo 2005, Ortiz-Santaliestra et al. 2010). Even bufonids,

known to rely on chemical toxic deterrents as defences against predators are promptly

consumed by P. clarkii (Gherardi et al. 2001, Renai and Gherardi 2004, Cruz and Rebelo 2005).

There have also been reports of this crayfish inflicting nonlethal tail injury in species such as P.

cultripes, H. meridionalis and P. perezi (Leite 2003).

Cruz and Rebelo (2005) found that all but two (B. bufo and P. cultripes) of these nine anuran

species reduced activity level and/or shifted microhabitat use in the presence of P. clarkii.

However, another study has shown that crayfish cues induce alterations in larval swimming

activity in tadpoles of P. cultripes (Ortiz-Santaliestra et al. 2010). On the other hand, Gomez-

Page 46: PREDATOR-PREY INTERACTIONS BETWEEN AN INVASIVE …

Chapter 1

28

Mestre and Díaz-Paniagua (2011) reported a failure of P. perezi larvae to express inducible

defences against this crayfish.

Several field studies have documented strong negative impacts of this crayfish on Iberian

amphibians. In the Sado River basin, Cruz et al. (2006a) found that the presence of P. clarkii in

a water body was a negative predictor of the use of that water body by P. cultripes, B. bufo and

possibly P. ibericus for reproduction. In addition, amphibian species richness was lower in

places colonised by the crayfish. The presence of P. clarkii also seems to greatly affect the

breeding habitat use and reproductive success of B. calamita in Doñana Natural Park (Cruz et

al. 2006b). Further, Beja and Alcazar (2003) have documented a negative correlation between

the distribution of this exotic crayfish and of some amphibian species in a Nature reserve in

Southwestern Portugal. A study performed in Italy observed that larval abundance of all

studied amphibian species, including B. bufo, was negatively associated with the presence of P.

clarkii (Ficetola et al. 2011). Finally, invasion by P. clarkii has caused the disappearance of

certain amphibian species from specific areas. In a lagoon in Spain, five of the six amphibian

species present became extremely rare and two of them (B. calamita and P. cultripes)

disappeared after crayfish arrival (Rodríguez et al. 2005). P. clarkii also seems to have caused

strong reductions or the local extinction of H. arborea, P. perezi and P. punctatus in a Nature

reserve in Portugal (Cruz et al. 2008).

1.6. Goals and outline of the thesis

One of the ways to evaluate the impacts of exotic predator introductions on prey communities

is by experimentally assessing the effects of the predator on different components of prey

fitness and survival. This may help clarifying some of the mechanisms that make prey species

more or less affected by invasions.

Page 47: PREDATOR-PREY INTERACTIONS BETWEEN AN INVASIVE …

Chapter 1

29

Larval anurans provide excellent study models for investigating inducible defences in the

presence of exotic predators, since they most often show predator-induced phenotypic

plasticity. Since predation during aquatic stages is one of the dominant forces regulating

amphibian populations (Smith 1983, Azevedo-Ramos et al. 1999), lacking appropriate defences

towards an invasive predator may render the invaded communities at serious risk. Therefore,

the ongoing invasion of P. clarkii in the Iberian Peninsula presents a very good opportunity to

study early responses of native prey to the presence of novel predators.

The main goal of this thesis was to study the outcomes of recently-established predator-prey

interactions following invasion by the aquatic predator P. clarkii on amphibian communities of

the Southwest of the Iberian Peninsula. In particular, more specific objectives were:

1. To evaluate the importance of P. clarkii as an agent of tail injury on free-living tadpoles and

to assess fitness and survival costs of tail damage for tadpoles;

2. To assess if larvae of the nine anuran species from the Southwest of the Iberian Peninsula

are able to induce behavioural, morphological or life-history responses in the presence of P.

clarkii and how integrated these responses are;

3. To investigate if tadpoles from populations differing in historical exposure to P. clarkii

showed dissimilar antipredator responses to this crayfish.

This thesis is structured as a compilation of four scientific papers (one published, one in review

and two manuscripts), each corresponding to one chapter, that focus on these specific goals.

This is preceded and followed by two more general sections (Chapters 1 and 6) which present,

respectively, a general introduction to the themes focused in the following chapters and a

general discussion in which the most relevant results of the preceding chapters are discussed

and integrated.

Page 48: PREDATOR-PREY INTERACTIONS BETWEEN AN INVASIVE …

Chapter 1

30

The first goal of this thesis was addressed in Chapter 2, where results from a field study and a

laboratory experiment were integrated. First, the role of P. clarkii as an important inducer of

tail damage in tadpoles of the Western spadefoot toad, P. cultripes, was tested by comparing

frequencies of tadpole caudal injury in temporary ponds, with and without crayfish, in

Southwestern Spain. Secondly, to investigate if tail damage can have important consequences

for tadpole morphology, life-history and survival, and how these consequences may differ

according to available energetic resources, frequency of tail injury and food availability were

manipulated in the laboratory in a factorial experiment.

The second objective was examined in Chapters 3 and 4, in which a laboratory experiment was

performed to evaluate if the nine anurans studied here were able to elicit behavioural

(alterations in activity level and spatial avoidance), morphological (alterations in tadpole

headbody and tail morphology) and life-history (alterations in growth rate, time to and mass at

metamorphosis) responses when exposed to P. clarkii chemical cues. These antipredator

responses were compared with those elicited towards a common native predator, the larval

dragonfly Aeshna sp. For this, tadpoles were raised in the laboratory and exposed to scents of

a caged crayfish, a dragonfly larva or no predator scents. The role of tadpole ontogeny and of

chemical cues from predation events in shaping behavioural responses was also investigated,

as well as the existence of integration among all phenotypic responses.

Lastly, to fulfil the third goal, P. perezi tadpoles from populations with longer (ca. 30 yr),

shorter (ca. 20 yr) or no coexistence time with P. clarkii were reared in the laboratory in the

nonlethal presence of this predator and their behavioural and morphological responses were

analysed (Chapter 5). This also allowed testing for the existence of evolved adaptive responses

of a native amphibian to this invasive predator.

Page 49: PREDATOR-PREY INTERACTIONS BETWEEN AN INVASIVE …

Chapter 1

31

1.7. Papers presented

Chapter 2

Nunes AL, Cruz MJ, Tejedo M, Laurila A, Rebelo R (2010) Nonlethal injury caused by an invasive

alien predator and its consequences for an anuran tadpole. Basic and Applied Ecology 11: 645-

654.

Chapter 3

Nunes AL, Richter-Boix A, Laurila A, Rebelo R (in review) Do anuran larvae respond

behaviourally to chemical cues from an invasive crayfish predator? A community-wide study.

Oecologia.

Chapter 4

Nunes AL, Orizaola G, Laurila A, Rebelo R. Morphological and life-history responses and

integration of defensive traits in an anuran community invaded by an exotic predator

(manuscript).

Chapter 5

Nunes AL, Orizaola G, Laurila A, Rebelo R. Population divergence in antipredator defences

against an invasive predator: does coexistence time promote the evolution of adaptive

responses? (manuscript).

Page 50: PREDATOR-PREY INTERACTIONS BETWEEN AN INVASIVE …

Chapter 1

32

1.8. References

Abrams PA (1989) The evolution of rates of successful and unsuccessful predation.

Evolutionary Ecology 3: 157-171.

Abrams PA, Rowe L (1996) The effects of predation on the age and size of maturity of prey.

Evolution 50: 1052-1061.

Adams MJ, Pearl CA (2007) Problems and opportunities managing invasive bullfrogs: Is there

any hope? Pp. 679-693 in: Gherardi F. (Ed.), Biological invaders in inland waters: Profiles,

distribution, and threats. Springer, Dordrecht, The Netherlands.

Agrawal AA (2001) Phenotypic plasticity in the interactions and evolution of species. Science

294: 321-326.

Alford RA, Richards SJ (1999) Global amphibian declines: a problem in applied ecology. Annual

Review of Ecology and Systematics 30: 133-165.

Álvarez D, Nicieza AG (2006) Factors determining tadpole vulnerability to predators: can prior

experience compensate for a suboptimal shape? Evolutionary Ecology 20: 523-534.

Anastácio PM, Marques JC (1995) Population biology and production of the red swamp

crayfish Procambarus clarkii (Girard) in the lower Mondego valley, Portugal. Journal of

Crustacean Biology 15: 156-168.

Anastácio PM, Nielsen SN, Marques JC, Jorgensen SE (1995) Integrated production of crayfish

and rice: a management model. Ecological Engineering 4: 199-210.

Anholt BR, Werner EE, Skelly DK (2000) Effect of food and predators on the activity of four

larval ranid frogs. Ecology 81: 3509-3521.

Aquiloni L, Ilhéu M, Gherardi F (2005) Habitat use and dispersal of the invasive crayfish

Procambarus clarkii in ephemeral water bodies of Portugal. Marine and Freshwater

Behaviour and Physiology 38: 225-236.

Axelsson E, Nyström P, Brönmark C (1997) Crayfish predation on amphibian eggs and larvae.

Amphibia-Reptilia 18: 217-228.

Azevedo-Ramos C, Magnusson WE, Bayliss P (1999) Predation as the key factor structuring

tadpole assemblages in a savanna area in central Amazonia. Copeia 1999: 22-33.

Page 51: PREDATOR-PREY INTERACTIONS BETWEEN AN INVASIVE …

Chapter 1

33

Babbitt KJ, Tanner GW (1998) Effects of cover and predator size on survival and development

of Rana utricularia tadpoles. Oecologia 114: 258-262.

Barbaresi S, Santini G, Tricarico E, Gherardi F (2004) Ranging behaviour of the invasive crayfish,

Procambarus clarkii (Girard). Journal of Natural History 38: 2821-2832.

Beckerman AP, Wieski K, Baird DJ (2007) Behavioural versus physiological mediation of life

history under predation risk. Oecologia 152: 335-343.

Beja P, Alcazar R (2003) Conservation of Mediterranean temporary ponds under agricultural

intensification: an evaluation using amphibians. Biological Conservation 114: 317-326.

Benard MF (2004) Predator-induced phenotypic plasticity in organisms with complex life

cycles. Annual Review of Ecology, Evolution and Systematics 35: 651-673.

Berenbaum MR, Zangerl AR (1998) Coping with life as a menu option: Inducible defenses of the

wild parsnip. Pp 10-32 in: Tollrian R, Harvell CD (Eds.) The Ecology and Evolution of

Inducible Defenses. Princeton University Press, Princeton.

Berven, KA (1990) Factors affecting population fluctuations in larval and adult stages of the

wood frog (Rana sylvatica). Ecology 71: 1599-1608.

Blackburn TM, Cassey P, Duncan RP, Evans KL, Gaston KJ (2004) Avian extinction and

mammalian introductions on oceanic islands. Science 305: 1955-1958.

Blair J, Wassersug R (2000) Variation in the pattern of predator-induced damage to tadpole

tails. Copeia 2000: 390-401.

Blaustein AR, Kiesecker JM (2002) Complexity in conservation: lessons from the global decline

of amphibian populations. Ecology Letters 5: 597-608.

Boersma M, Spaak P, De Meester L (1998) Predator-mediated plasticity in morphology, life

history, and behavior of Daphnia: The uncoupling of responses. The American Naturalist

152: 237-248.

Bollens SM, Frost BW, Thoreson DS, Watts SJ (1992) Diel vertical migration in zooplankton:

field evidence in support of the predator avoidance hypothesis Hydrobiologia 234: 33-

39.

Bool JD, Witcomb K, Kydd E, Brown C (2011) Learned recognition and avoidance of invasive

mosquitofish by the shrimp, Paratya australiensis. Marine and Freshwater Research 62:

1230-1236.

Page 52: PREDATOR-PREY INTERACTIONS BETWEEN AN INVASIVE …

Chapter 1

34

Bosch J, Rincón PA, Boyero L, Martínez-Solano I (2006) Effects of introduced salmonids on a

montane population of Iberian frogs. Conservation Biology 20:180-189.

Bowerman J, Johnson PT J, Bowerman T (2010) Sublethal predators and their injured prey:

linking aquatic predators and severe limb abnormalities in amphibians. Ecology 91: 242-

251.

Braña F, Frechilla L, Orizaola G (1996) Effect of introduced fish on amphibian assemblages in

mountain lakes of northern Spain. Herpetological Journal 6: 145-148.

Brönmark C, Pettersson LB (1994) Chemical cues from piscivores induce a change in

morphology in crucian carp. Oikos 70: 396-402.

Bryer PJ, Mirza RS, Chivers DP (2001) Chemosensory assessment of predation risk by slimy

sculpins, Cottus cognatus: responses to alarm, disturbance and predator cues. Journal of

Chemical Ecology 27: 535-546.

Cabral MJ (coordinator), Almeida J, Almeida PR, Delllinger T, Ferrand de Almeida N, Oliveira

ME, Palmeirim JM, Queiroz AI, Rogado L, Santos-Reis M (eds) (2005) “Livro Vermelho

dos Vertebrados de Portugal”. Instituto da Conservação da Natureza, Lisbon.

Capinha C, Anastácio P (2011) Assessing the environmental requirements of invaders using

ensembles of distribution models. Diversity and Distributions 17: 13-24.

Chivers DP, Smith RJF (1998) Chemical alarm signalling in aquatic predator-prey systems: A

review and prospectus. Ecoscience 5: 338-352.

Clark CW, Harvell CD (1992) Inducible defences and the allocation of resources: a minimalist

model. The American Naturalist 139: 521-539.

Clavero M, García-Berthou E (2005) Invasive species are a leading cause of animal extinctions.

Trends in Ecology and Evolution 20: 110.

Cobo F, Vieira-Lanero R, Rego E, Servia MJ (2010) Temporal trends in non-indigenous

freshwater species records during the 20th century: a case study in the Iberian

Peninsula. Biodiversity and Conservation 19: 3471-3487.

Collins JP, Storfer A (2003) Global amphibian declines: sorting the hypotheses. Diversity and

Distributions 9: 89-98.

Correia AM (2002) Niche breadth and trophic diversity: feeding behavior of the red swamp

crayfish (Procambarus clarkii) towards environmental availability of aquatic

macroinvertebrates in a rice field (Portugal). Acta Oecologica 23: 421-429.

Page 53: PREDATOR-PREY INTERACTIONS BETWEEN AN INVASIVE …

Chapter 1

35

Correia AM (2003) Food choice by the introduced crayfish Procambarus clarkii. Annales

Zoologici Fennici 40: 517-528.

Correia AM, Anastácio PM (2008) Shifts in aquatic macroinvertebrate biodiversity associated

with the presence and size of an alien crayfish. Ecological Research 23: 729-734.

Correia AM, Bandeira N, Anastácio PM (2005) Predator-prey interactions of Procambarus

clarkii with aquatic macroinvertebrates in a single and multiple prey systems. Acta

Oecologica 28: 337-43.

Cousyn C, De Meester L, Colbourne JK, Brendonck L, Verschuren D, Volckaert F (2001) Rapid,

local adaptation of zooplankton behavior to changes in predation pressure in the

absence of neutral genetic changes. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences

98: 6256-6260.

Cox JG, Lima SL (2006) Naiveté and an aquatic-terrestrial dichotomy in the effects of

introduced predators. Trends in Ecology and Evolution 21: 674-680.

Cruz MJ, Rebelo R (2005) Vulnerability of southwest Iberian amphibians to an introduced

crayfish, Procambarus clarkii. Amphibia-Reptilia 26: 293-303.

Cruz MJ, Rebelo R (2007) Colonization of freshwater habitats by an introduced crayfish,

Procambarus clarkii, in Southwest Iberian Peninsula. Hydrobiologia 575: 191-201.

Cruz MJ, Rebelo R, Crespo EG (2006a) Effects of an introduced crayfish, Procambarus clarkii, on

the distribution of south-western Iberian amphibians in their breeding habitats.

Ecography 29: 329-338.

Cruz MJ, Pascoal S, Tejedo M, Rebelo R (2006b) Predation by an exotic crayfish, Procambarus

clarkii, on natterjack toad, Bufo calamita, embryos: its role on the exclusion of this

amphibian from its breeding ponds. Copeia 2006: 274-280.

Cruz MJ, Segurado P, Sousa M, Rebelo R (2008) Collapse of the amphibian community of the

Paúl do Boquilobo Natural Reserve (central Portugal) after the arrival of the exotic

American crayfish Procambarus clarkii. Herpetological Journal 18: 197-204.

Dayton GH, Saenz D, Baum KA, Langerhans RB, DeWitt TJ (2005) Body shape, burst speed and

escape behavior of larval anurans. Oikos 111: 582-591.

Denoël M, Dzukic G, Kalezic ML (2005) Effects of widespread fish introductions on

paedomorphic newts in Europe. Conservation Biology 19: 162-170.

Page 54: PREDATOR-PREY INTERACTIONS BETWEEN AN INVASIVE …

Chapter 1

36

DeVito J (2003) Metamorphic synchrony and aggregation as antipredator responses in

American toads. Oikos 103: 75-80.

DeWitt TJ, Scheiner SM (eds) (2004) Phenotypic variation from single genotypes: A primer. Pp

1-9 in: Phenotypic Plasticity: Functional and Conceptual Approaches, Oxford University

Press, New York.

Diaz-Paniagua C (1985) Larval diets related to morphological characters of five anuran species

in the Biological Reserve of Doñana (Huelva, Spain). Amphibia-Reptilia 6: 307-322.

Diaz-Paniagua C (1990) Temporary ponds as breeding sites of amphibians at a locality in

southwestern Spain. Herpetological Journal 1: 447-453.

Dickman CR (1996) Impact of non-native generalist predators on the native fauna of Australia.

Wildlife Biology 2: 185-195.

Doherty PA, Wassersug RJ, Lee JM (1998) Mechanical properties of the tadpole tail fin. Journal

of Experimental Biology 201: 2691-2699.

Dorn NJ, Wojdak JM (2004) The role of omnivorous crayfish in littoral communities. Oecologia

140: 150-159.

Edgell TC, Lynch BR, Trussell GC, Palmer AR (2009) Experimental evidence for the rapid

evolution of behavioral canalization in natural populations. The American Naturalist 174:

434-440.

Eklöv P, Werner EE (2000) Multiple predator effects on size-dependent behavior and mortality

of two species of anuran larvae. Oikos 88: 250-258.

Epp KJ, Gabor CR (2008) Innate and learned predator recognition mediated by chemical signals

in Eurycea nana. Ethology 114: 607-615.

Ferrari MCO, Chivers DP (2006) Learning threat-sensitive predator avoidance: how do fathead

minnows incorporate conflicting information? Animal Behaviour 71: 19-26.

Ferrari MCO, Messier F, Chivers DP (2008) Larval amphibians learn to match antipredator

response intensity to temporal patterns of risk. Behavioral Ecology 19: 980-983.

Ferrari MCO, Wisenden BD, Chivers DP (2010) Chemical ecology of predator-prey interactions

in aquatic ecosystems: a review and prospectus. Canadian Journal of Zoology 88: 698-

724.

Page 55: PREDATOR-PREY INTERACTIONS BETWEEN AN INVASIVE …

Chapter 1

37

Ferrari MCO, Gonzalo A, Messier F, Chivers DP (2007) Generalization of learned predator

recognition: an experimental test and framework for future studies. Proceedings of the

Royal Society of London B 274: 1853-1859.

Ferrari MCO, Trowell JJ, Brown GE, Chivers DP (2005) The role of leaning in the development of

threat-sensitive predator avoidance in fathead minnows. Animal Behaviour 70: 777-784.

Ficetola FG, Siesa ME, Manenti R, Bottoni L, De Bernardi F, Padoa-Schioppa E (2011) Early

assessment of the impact of alien species: differential consequences of an invasive

crayfish on adult and larval amphibians. Diversity and Distributions, in press.

Figiel CR, Semlitsch RD (1991) Effects of nonlethal injury and habitat complexity on predation

in tadpole populations. Canadian Journal of Zoology 69: 830-834.

Finlay JC, Vredenburg VT (2007) Introduced trout sever trophic connections between lakes and

watersheds: consequences for a declining amphibian. Ecology 88: 2187-2198.

Fisk DF, Latta LC, Knapp RA, Pfrender ME (2007) Rapid evolution in response to introduced

predators I: rates and patterns of morphological and life-history trait divergence. BMC

Evolutionary Biology 7: 22.

Fraker M, Hu E, Cuddapah V, McCollum S, Relyea R, Hempel H, Denver R (2009)

Characterization of an alarm pheromone secreted by amphibian tadpoles that induces

behavioral inhibition and suppression of the neuroendocrine stress axis. Hormones and

Behavior 55: 520-529.

Freeman AS, Byers JE (2006) Divergent induced responses to an invasive predator in marine

mussel populations. Science 313: 831-833.

Gall BG, Mathis A (2010) Innate predator recognition and the problem of introduced trout.

Ethology 116: 47-58.

Gallie JA, Mumme RL, Wissinger SA (2001) Experience has no effect on the development of

chemosensory recognition of predators by tadpoles of the American toad, Bufo

americanus. Herpetologica 57: 376-383.

Gamradt SC, Kats LB (1996) Effect of introduced crayfish and mosquitofish on California newts.

Conservation Biology 10: 1155-1162.

Gamradt SC, Kats LB, Anzalone CB (1997) Aggression by non-native crayfish deters breeding in

California newts. Conservation Biology 11: 793-796.

Page 56: PREDATOR-PREY INTERACTIONS BETWEEN AN INVASIVE …

Chapter 1

38

García-Berthou E, Boix D, Clavero M (2007) Non-indigenous animal species naturalized in

Iberian inland waters. Pp. 123-140 in: Gherardi F (Ed.), Biological invaders in inland

waters: Profiles, distribution, and threats. Springer, Dordrecht, The Netherlands.

Geiger W, Alcorlo P, Baltanás A, Montes C (2005) Impact of an introduced crustacean on the

trophic webs of Mediterranean wetlands. Biological Invasions 7: 49-73.

Gherardi F (2006) Crayfish invading Europe: the case study of Procambarus clarkii. Marine and

Freshwater Behaviour and Physiology 39: 175-191

Gherardi F (2007a) Biological invasions in inland waters: an overview. Pp. 3-25 in: Gherardi F.

(Ed.), Biological invaders in inland waters: Profiles, distribution, and threats. Springer,

Dordrecht, The Netherlands.

Gherardi (2007b). Understanding the impact of invasive crayfish. Pp 507-542 in: Gherardi F.

(Ed.), Biological invaders in inland waters: Profiles, distribution, and threats. Springer,

Dordrecht, The Netherlands.

Gherardi F, Acquistapace P (2007) Invasive crayfish in Europe: the impact of Procambarus

clarkii on the littoral community of a Mediterranean lake. Freshwater Biology 52: 1249-

1259.

Gherardi F, Barbaresi S (2000) Invasive crayfish: activity patterns of Procambarus clarkii in the

rice fields of the Lower Guadalquivir (Spain). Archiv für Hydrobiologie 150: 153-168.

Gherardi F, Renai B, Corti C (2001) Crayfish predation on tadpoles: a comparison between a

native (Austropotamobius pallipes) and an alien species (Procambarus clarkii). Bullettin

Français de la Peche et de la Pisciculture 361: 659-668.

Gherardi F, Mavuti KM, Pacini N, Tricarico E, Harper DM (2011) The smell of danger: chemical

recognition of fish predators by the invasive crayfish Procambarus clarkii. Freshwater

Biology 56: 1567-1578.

Gil-Sánchez JM, Alba-Tercedor J (2002) Ecology of the native and introduced crayfishes

Austropotamobius pallipes and Procambarus clarkii in southern Spain and implications

for conservation of the native species. Biological Conservation 105: 75-80.

Gonzalo A, Lopez P, Martin J (2007) Iberian green frog tadpoles may learn to recognize novel

predators from chemical alarm cues of conspecifics. Animal Behaviour 74: 447-453.

Page 57: PREDATOR-PREY INTERACTIONS BETWEEN AN INVASIVE …

Chapter 1

39

Gonzalo A, Lopez P, Martin J (2010) Risk level of chemical cues determines retention of

recognition of new predators in Iberian green frog tadpoles. Behavioral Ecology and

Sociobiology 64: 1117-1123.

Gomez-Mestre I, Díaz-Paniagua C (2011) Invasive predatory crayfish do not trigger inducible

defences in tadpoles. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B, in press.

Griffiths RA, Schley L, Sharp PE, Dennis JL, Román A (1998) Behavioural responses of Mallorcan

midwife toad tadpoles to natural and unnatural snake predators. Animal Behaviour 55:

207-214.

Gutiérrez-Yurrita PJ, Montes C (1999) Bioenergetics and phenology of reproduction of the

introduced red swamp crayfish, Procambarus clarkii, in Doñana National Park, Spain, and

implications for species management. Freshwater Biology 42: 561-574.

Habsburgo-Lorena AS (1983) Socioeconomic aspects of the crawfish industry in Spain.

Freshwater Crayfish 5: 552-554.

Hamer A, Lane S, Mahony M (2002) The role of introduced mosquitofish (Gambusia holbrooki)

in excluding the native green and golden bell frog (Litoria aurea) from original habitats in

south-eastern Australia. Oecologia 132: 445-452.

Harris RN (1989) Nonlethal injury to organisms as a mechanism of population regulation. The

American Naturalist 134: 835-847.

Harvell CD (1990) The ecology and evolution of inducible defenses. The Quarterly Review of

Biology 65: 323-340.

Hettyey A, Zsarnóczai S, Vincze K, Hoi H, Laurila A (2010) Interactions between the information

content of different chemical cues affect induced defences in tadpoles. Oikos 119: 1814-

1822.

Higginson AD, Ruxton GD (2009) Dynamic models allowing for flexibility in complex life

histories accurately predict timing of metamorphosis and antipredator strategies of

prey. Functional Ecology 23: 1103-1113.

Higginson AD, Ruxton GD (2010) Adaptive changes in size and age at metamorphosis can

qualitatively vary with predator type and available defenses. Ecology 91: 2756-2768.

Hobbs HH, Jass JP, Huner JV (1989) A review of global crayfish introductions with particular

emphasis on two North American species. Crustaceana 56: 299-316.

Page 58: PREDATOR-PREY INTERACTIONS BETWEEN AN INVASIVE …

Chapter 1

40

Holdich DM , Pöckl M (2007) Invasive crustaceans in European inland waters. Pp. 29-75 in:

Gherardi F. (Ed.), Biological invaders in inland waters: Profiles, distribution, and threats.

Springer, Dordrecht, The Netherlands.

Holdich DM, Reynolds JD, Souty-Grosset C, Sibley PJ (2009) A review of the ever increasing

threat to European crayfish from non-indigenous crayfish species. Knowledge and

Management of Aquatic Ecosystems 11: 394-395.

Hoverman JT, Relyea RA (2007) The rules of engagement: How to defend against combinations

of predators. Oecologia 154: 551-560.

Hutson KS, Ross DJ, Day RW, Ahern JJ (2005) Australian scallops do not recognise the

introduced predatory seastar Asterias amurensis. Marine Ecology Progress Series 298:

305-309.

Ilhéu M, Bernardo JM, Fernandes S (2007) Predation of invasive crayfish on aquatic

vertebrates: the effect of Procambarus clarkii on fish assemblages in Mediterranean

temporary streams. Pp. 543-558 in: Gherardi F. (Ed.), Biological invaders in inland

waters: Profiles, distribution, and threats. Springer, Dordrecht, The Netherlands.

Johansson F, Andersson J (2009) Scared fish get lazy, and lazy fish get fat. Journal of Animal

Ecology 78: 772-777.

Johnson JB, Burt DB, DeWitt TJ (2008) Form, function, and fitness: Pathways to survival.

Evolution 62: 1243-1251.

Johnson JB, Saenz D, Adams CK, Conner RN (2003) The influence of predator threat on the

timing of a life-history switch point: predator-induced hatching in the southern leopard

frog (Rana sphenocephala). Canadian Journal of Zoology 81: 1608-1613.

Johnson MTJ, Agrawal AA (2003) The ecological play of predator-prey dynamics in an

evolutionary theatre. Trends in Ecology and Evolution 18: 549-551.

Kats LB, Dill LM (1998) The scent of death: chemosensory assessment of predation risk by prey

animals. Ecoscience 5: 361-394.

Kats LB, Ferrer RP (2003) Alien predators and amphibian declines: review of two decades of

science and the transition to conservation. Diversity and Distributions 9: 99-110.

Kelley JL, Magurran AE (2003) Learned predator recognition and antipredator responses in

fishes. Fish and Fisheries 4: 216-226.

Page 59: PREDATOR-PREY INTERACTIONS BETWEEN AN INVASIVE …

Chapter 1

41

Kerby JL, Riley SPD, Kats LB, Wilson P (2005) Barriers and flow as limiting factors in the spread

of an invasive crayfish (Procambarus clarkii) in southern California streams. Biological

Conservation 126: 402-409.

Kiesecker JM, Blaustein AR (1998) Effects of introduced bullfrogs and smallmouth bass on

microhabitat use, growth, and survival of native red-legged frogs (Rana aurora).

Conservation Biology 12: 776-787.

Kiesecker JM, Blaustein AR (1997) Population differences in responses of red-legged frogs

(Rana aurora) to introduced bullfrogs. Ecology 78: 1752-1760.

Knapp RA (2005) Effects of nonnative fish and habitat characteristics on lentic herpetofauna in

Yosemite National Park, USA. Biological Conservation 121: 265-279.

Korpi NL, Wisenden BD (2001) Learned recognition of novel predator odour by zebra danios,

Danio rerio, following time-shifted presentation of alarm cue and predator odour.

Environmental Biology of Fishes 61: 205 -211.

Kristensen EA, Closs GP (2004) Anti-predator response of naïve and experienced common bully

to chemical alarm cues. Journal of Fish Biology 64: 643-652.

LaFiandra EM, Babbitt KJ (2004) Predator induced phenotypic plasticity in the pinewoods tree

frog, Hyla femoralis: necessary cues and the cost of development. Oecologia 138: 350-

359.

Lardner B (2000) Morphological and life history responses to predators in larvae of seven

anurans. Oikos 88: 169-180.

Laurila A, Kujasalo J, Ranta E (1997) Different antipredator behaviour in two anuran tadpoles:

effects of predator diet. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 40: 329-336.

Laurila A, Kujasalo J, Ranta E (1998) Predator-induced changes in life-history in two anuran

tadpoles: effects of predator diet. Oikos 83: 307-317.

Laurila A, Lindgren B, Laugen AT (2008) Antipredator defenses along a latitudinal gradient in

Rana temporaria. Ecology 89: 1399-1413.

Laurila A, Pakkasmaa S, Merila J (2006) Population divergence in growth rate and antipredator

defences in Rana arvalis. Oecologia 147: 585-595.

Laurila A, Järvi-Laturi M, Pakkasmaa S, Merilä J (2004) Temporal variation in predation risk:

stage-dependency, graded responses and fitness costs in tadpole antipredator defences.

Oikos 107: 90-99.

Page 60: PREDATOR-PREY INTERACTIONS BETWEEN AN INVASIVE …

Chapter 1

42

Leite ME (2003) Interacções tróficas entre Procambarus clarkii e vertebrados aquáticos que

integram as biocenoses do arrozal. Estágio Profissionalizante da Licenciatura em Biologia

Aplicada aos Recursos Animais, variante Marinhos. DBA, FCUL.

Lima SL, Dill LM (1990) Behavioral decisions made under the risk of predation - a review and

prospectus. Canadian Journal of Zoology 68: 619-640.

Lima SL (1998) Nonlethal effects in the ecology of predator-prey interactions: what are the

ecological effects of anti-predator decision-making? BioScience 48: 25-34.

Lockwood J, Hoopes M, Marchetti M (2007) Invasion Ecology. Blackwell Publishing Ltd.,

Malden, Massachusetts, 312 pp.

Lodge DM (1993) Biological invasions: lessons for ecology. Trends in Ecology and Evolution 8:

133-137.

Lodge DM, Kershner MW, Aloi JE, Covich AP (1994) Effects of an omnivorous crayfish

(Orconectes rusticus) on a freshwater littoral food web. Ecology 75: 1265-1281.

Lodge DM, Taylor CA, Holdich DM, Skurdal J (2000) Nonindigenous crayfishes threaten North

American freshwater biodiversity: lessons from Europe. Fisheries 25: 7-20.

Loureiro A, Ferrand de Almeida N, Carretero MA, Paulo OS (eds.) (2008) Atlas dos Anfíbios e

Répteis de Portugal. Instituto da Conservação da Natureza e da Biodiversidade, Lisboa.

257 pp.

Macdonald DW, Harrington LA (2003) The American mink: the triumph and tragedy of

adaptation out of context. New Zealand Journal of Zoology 30: 421-441.

Mack RN, Simberloff D, Lonsdale WM, Evans H, Clout M, Bazzaz FA (2000) Biotic invasions:

causes, epidemiology, global consequences, and control. Ecological Applications 10: 689-

710.

Maginnis TL (2006) The costs of autotomy and regeneration in animals: A review and

framework for future research. Behavioral Ecology 17: 857-872.

Magurran AE (1990) The adaptive significance of schooling as an anti-predator defence in fish.

Annales Zoologici Fennici 27: 3-18.

Mandrillon AL, Saglio P (2005) Prior exposure to conspecific chemical cues affects predator

recognition in larval common toad (Bufo bufo). Archiv für Hydrobiologie 164: 1-12.

Page 61: PREDATOR-PREY INTERACTIONS BETWEEN AN INVASIVE …

Chapter 1

43

Marquis O, Saglio P, Neveu A (2004) Effects of predators and conspecific chemical cues on the

swimming activity of Rana temporaria and Bufo bufo tadpoles. Archiv für Hydrobiologie

160: 153-170.

McCollum SA, Van Buskirk J (1996) Costs and benefits of a predator-induced polyphenism in

the gray treefrog Hyla chrysoscelis. Evolution 50: 583-593.

McCollum SA, Leimberger JD (1997) Predator-induced morphological changes in an amphibian:

predation by dragonflies affects tadpole shape and color. Oecologia 109: 615-621.

McEvoy J, Sinn DL, Wapstra E (2008) Know thy enemy: Behavioural response of a native

mammal (Rattus lutreolus velutinus) to predators of different coexistence histories.

Austral Ecology 33, 922-931.

McLean F, Barbee NC, Swearer SE (2007) Avoidance of native versus non‐native predator

odours by migrating whitebait and juveniles of the common galaxiid, Galaxias

maculatus. New Zealand Journal of Marine and Freshwater Research 41: 175-184.

McPeek MA (2004) The growth/predation risk trade-off: so what is the mechanism? The

American Naturalist 163: 88-111.

Mirza RS, Ferrari MCO, Kiesecker JM, Chivers DP (2006) Responses of American toad tadpoles

to predation cues: behavioural response thresholds, threat-sensitivity and acquired

predation recognition. Behaviour 143:877-889.

Mitchell CE, Agrawal AA, Bever JD, Gilbert GS, Hufbauer RA, Klironomos JN, Maron JL, Morris

WF, Parker IM, Power AG, Seabloom EW, Torchin ME, Vázquez DP (2006) Biotic

interactions and plant invasions. Ecology Letters 9: 726-740.

Moore RD, Griffiths RA, O’Brien CM, Murphy A, Jay D (2004) Induced defences in an

endangered amphibian in response to an introduced snake predator. Oecologia 141:

139-147.

Morin PJ (1985) Predation intensity, prey survival and injury frequency in an amphibian

predator-prey interaction. Copeia 1985: 638-644.

Nannini MA, Belk MC (2006) Antipredator responses of two native stream fishes to an

introduced predator: does similarity in morphology predict similarity in behavioural

response? Ecology of Freshwater Fish 15: 453-463.

Nicieza AG, Álvarez DA, Atienza EMS (2006) Delayed effects of larval predation risk and food

quality on anuran juvenile performance. Journal of Evolutionary Biology 19: 1092-1103.

Page 62: PREDATOR-PREY INTERACTIONS BETWEEN AN INVASIVE …

Chapter 1

44

Nyström P, Abjörnsson K (2000) Effects of fish chemical cues on the interactions between

tadpoles and crayfish. Oikos 88: 181-190.

Nyström P, Brönmark C, Granéli W (1996) Patterns in benthic food webs: a role for omnivorous

crayfish? Freshwater Biology 36: 631-646.

Nyström P, Birkedal L, Dahlberg C, Brönmark C (2002) The declining spadefoot toad Pelobates

fuscus: calling site choice and conservation. Ecography 25: 488-498.

Nyström P, Svensson O, Lardner B, Brönmark C, Granéli W (2001) The influence of multiple

introduced predators on a littoral pond community. Ecology 82: 1023-1039.

Ortiz-Santaliestra ME, Fernández-Benéiteza MJ, Marco A, Lizana M (2010) Influence of

ammonium nitrate on larval anti-predatory responses of two amphibian species. Aquatic

Toxicology 99: 198-204.

O'Steen S, Cullum AJ, Bennet AF (2002) Rapid evolution of escape ability in Trinidadian guppies

(Poecilia reticulata). Evolution 56: 776-784.

Parichy DM, Kaplan RH (1992) Developmental consequences of tail injury on larvae of the

Oriental Fire-Bellied Toad, Bombina orientalis. Copeia 1992: 129-137.

Parker IM, Simberloff D, Lonsdale WM, Goodell K, Wonham M, Kareiva PM, Williamson MH,

Von Holle B, Moyle PB, Byers JE, Goldwasser L (1999) Impact: toward a framework for

understanding the ecological effects of invaders. Biological Invasions 1: 3-19.

Peacor SD (2002) Positive effect of predators on prey growth rate through induced

modifications of prey behaviour. Ecology Letters 5: 77-85.

Peacor SD, Werner EE (2001) The contribution of trait-mediated indirect effects to the net

effects of a predator. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 98: 3904-3908.

Pearl CA, Adams MJ, Bury RB, McCreary B (2004) Asymmetrical effects of introduced bullfrogs

(Rana catesbeiana) on native ranid frogs in Oregon. Copeia 2004(1): 11-20.

Pearl CA, Adams MJ, Schuytema GS, Nebeker AV (2003) Behavioral responses of anuran larvae

to chemical cues of native and introduced predators in the Pacific Northwestern United

States. Journal of Herpetology 37: 572-576.

Pearson KJ, Goater CP (2008) Distribution of long-toed salamanders and introduced trout in

high- and low-elevation wetlands in southwestern Alberta, Canada. Ecoscience 15: 453-

459.

Page 63: PREDATOR-PREY INTERACTIONS BETWEEN AN INVASIVE …

Chapter 1

45

Peluc SI, Sillet TS, Rotenberry JT, Ghalambor CK (2008) Adaptive phenotypic plasticity in an

island songbird exposed to a novel predation risk. Behavioral Ecology 19: 830-835.

Petranka JW, Hayes LJ (1998) Chemically mediated avoidance of a predatory odonate (Anax

junius) by American toad (Bufo americanus) and wood frog (Rana sylvatica) tadpoles.

Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 42: 263-271.

Pettersson LB, Nilsson PA, Brönmark C (2000) Predator recognition and defence strategies in

crucian carp, Carassius carassius. Oikos 88: 200-212.

Pigliucci M (2001) Phenotypic Plasticity: Beyond Nature and Nurture. Johns Hopkins University

Press, Baltimore.

Pilliod DS, Peterson CR (2001) Local and landscape effects of introduced trout on amphibians in

historically fishless watersheds. Ecosystems 4: 322-333.

Pilliod DS, Hossack BR, Bahls PF, Bull EL, Corn PS, Hokit G, Maxell BA, Munger JC, Wyrick A

(2010) Nonnative salmonids affect amphibian occupancy at multiple spatial scales.

Diversity and Distributions 16: 959-974.

Polo-Cavia N, Gonzalo A, Lopez P, Martin J (2010) Predator recognition of native but not

invasive turtle predators by naive anuran tadpoles. Animal Behaviour 80: 461-466.

Pope KL (2008) Assessing changes in amphibian population dynamics following experimental

manipulations of introduced fish. Conservation Biology 22: 1572-1581.

Preisser EL, Bolnick DI, Benard MF (2005) Scared to death? The effects of intimidation and

consumption in predator-prey interactions. Ecology 86: 501-509.

Ramos MA, Pereira TM (1981) Um novo Astacidae para a fauna portuguesa: Procambarus

clarkii (Girard, 1852). Boletim do Instituto Nacional de Investigação e Pescas, Lisboa 6:

37-47.

Recuero E, Canestrelli D, Vörös J, Szaboó K, Poyarkov NA, Arntzen JW, Crnobrnja-Isailovic J,

Kidov AA, Cogalniceanu D, Caputo FP, Nascetti G, Martinez-Solano I (2011) Multilocus

species tree analyses resolve the radiation of the widespread Bufo bufo species group

(Anura, Bufonidae). Molecular Phylogenetics and Evolution, in press.

Rehage JS, Dunlop KL, Loftu WF (2009) Antipredator responses by native mosquitofish to non-

native cichlids: an examination of the role of prey naiveté. Ethology 115: 1-11.

Relyea RA (2001) Morphological and behavioral plasticity of larval anurans in response to

different predators. Ecology 82: 523-540.

Page 64: PREDATOR-PREY INTERACTIONS BETWEEN AN INVASIVE …

Chapter 1

46

Relyea RA (2002) Costs of phenotypic plasticity. The American Naturalist 159: 272-282.

Relyea RA (2003) Predators come and predators go: The reversibility of predator-induced

traits. Ecology 84: 1840-1848.

Relyea RA (2004) Fine-tuned phenotypes: Tadpole plasticity under 16 combinations of

predators and competitors. Ecology 85: 172-179.

Relyea RA (2007) Getting out alive: How predators affect the decision to metamorphose.

Oecologia 152: 389-400.

Relyea RA, Werner EE (2000) Morphological plasticity in four larval anurans distributed along

an environmental gradient. Copeia 2000: 178-190.

Renai B, Gherardi F (2004) Predatory efficiency of crayfish: comparison between indigenous

and non-indigenous species. Biological Invasions 6: 89-99.

Reznick DN, Ghalambor CK (2001) The population ecology of contemporary adaptation: What

empirical studies reveal about the conditions that promote adaptive evolution. Genetica

112-113: 183-198.

Ricciardi A, Atkinson SK (2004) Distinctiveness magnifies the impact of biological invaders in

aquatic ecosystems. Ecology Letters 7: 781-784.

Richardson JML (2001) A comparative study of activity levels in larval anurans and response to

the presence of different predators. Behavioral Ecology 12: 51-58.

Richardson DM, Pysek P, Rejmánek M, Barbour MG, Panetta FD, West CJ (2000) Naturalization

and invasion of alien plants: concepts and definitions. Diversity and Distributions 6: 93-

107.

Richter-Boix A, Llorente GA, Montori A (2007) A comparative study of predator-induced

phenotype in tadpoles across a pond permanency gradient. Hydrobiologia 583: 43-56.

Rodda GH, Fritts TH, Chiszar D (1997) The disappearance of Guam’s wildlife: new insights for

herpetology, evolutionary ecology, and conservation. Bioscience 47: 565-574.

Rodríguez CL, Bécares E, Fernández-Aláez M (2003) Shift from clear to turbid phase in Lake

Chozas (NW Spain) due to the introduction of American red swamp crayfish

(Procambarus clarkii). Hydrobiologia 506-509: 421-426.

Rodríguez CF, Bécares E, Fernández-Aláez M, Fernández-Aláez C (2005) Loss of diversity and

degradation of wetlands as a result of introducing exotic crayfish. Biological Invasions 7:

75-85.

Page 65: PREDATOR-PREY INTERACTIONS BETWEEN AN INVASIVE …

Chapter 1

47

Rowe L, Ludwig D (1991) Size and timing of metamorphosis in complex life cycles: time

constraints and variation. Ecology 72: 413-427.

Russell BG, Banks PB (2007) Do Australian small mammals respond to native and introduced

predator odours? Austral Ecology 32: 277-286.

Sakai AK, Allendorf FW, Holt JS, Lodge DM, Molofsky J, With KA, Baughman S, Cabin RJ, Cohen

JE, Ellstrand N, McCauley DE, O’Neil P, Parker IM, Thompson JN, Weller SG (2001) The

population biology of invasive species. Annual Review of Ecology and Systematics 32:

305-332.

Sala OE, Chapin III FS, Armesto JJ, Berlow E, Bloomfield J, Dirzo R, Huber-Sanwald E, Huenneke

LF, Jackson RB, Kinzig A, Leemans R, Lodge DM, Mooney HA, Oesterheld M, LeRoy Poff

N, Sykes MT, Walker BH, Walker M, Wall DH (2000) Global Biodiversity Scenarios for the

Year 2100. Science 287: 1770-1774.

Salo P, Korpimäki E, Banks P, Nordström M, Dickman CR (2007) Alien predators are more

dangerous than native predators to prey populations. Proceedings of the Royal Society

of London B 274: 1237-1243.

Sánchez-Herráiz MJ, Barbadillo LJ, Machordom A, Sanchiz B (2000) A new species of pelodytid

from the Iberian Peninsula. Herpetologica 56: 105-118.

Scalera R (2007) An overview of the natural history of non-indigenous amphibians and reptiles.

Pp. 141-160 in: Gherardi F. (Ed.), Biological invaders in inland waters: Profiles,

distribution, and threats. Springer, Dordrecht, The Netherlands.

Schlaepfer MA, Sherman PW, Blossey B, Runge MC (2005) Introduced species as evolutionary

traps. Ecology Letters 8: 241-246.

Schoeppner NM, Relyea RA (2005) Damage, digestion, and defence: the roles of alarm cues

and kairomones for inducing prey defences. Ecology Letters 8: 505-512.

Schoeppner NM, Relyea RA (2009) Interpreting the smells of predation: how alarm cues and

kairomones induce different prey defences. Functional Ecology 23: 1114-1121.

Semlitsch RD (1990) Effects of body size, sibship, and tail injury on the susceptibility of

tadpoles to dragonfly predation. Canadian Journal of Zoology 68: 1027-1030.

Semlitsch RD, Gavasso S (1992) Behavioural responses of Bufo bufo and Bufo calamita tadpoles

to chemical cues of vertebrate and invertebrate predators. Ethology, Ecology and

Evolution 4: 165-173.

Page 66: PREDATOR-PREY INTERACTIONS BETWEEN AN INVASIVE …

Chapter 1

48

Semlitsch RD, Reichling SB (1989) Density-dependent injury in larval salamanders. Oecologia

81: 100-103.

Semlitsch RD, Scott DE, Pechmann JHK (1988) Time and size at metamorphosis related to adult

fitness in Ambystoma talpoideum. Ecology 69: 184-192.

Shah AA, Ryan MJ, Bevilacqua E, Schlaepfer MA (2010) Prior experience alters the behavioral

response of prey to a nonnative predator. Journal of Herpetology 44: 185-192.

Shine R (2010) The ecological impact of invasive cane toads (Bufo marinus) in Australia.

Quarterly Review of Biology 85: 253-291.

Shine R (2011) Invasive species as drivers of evolutionary change: cane toads in tropical

Australia. Evolutionary Applications, in press.

Short J, Kinnear JE, Robley A (2002) Surplus killing by introduced predators in Australia -

evidence for ineffective anti-predator adaptations in native prey species? Biological

Conservation 103: 283-301.

Sih A, Ferrari MCO, Harris DJ (2011) Evolution and behavioural responses to human-induced

rapid environmental change. Evolutionary Applications 4: 367-387.

Sih A, Bolnick DI, Luttbeg B, Orrock JL, Peacor SD, Pintor LM, Preisser E, Rehage JS, Vonesh JR

(2010) Predator-prey naiveté, antipredator behavior, and the ecology of predator

invasions. Oikos 119: 610-621.

Skelly DK (1992) Field evidence for a cost of behavioral antipredator response in a larval

amphibian. Ecology 73: 704-708.

Skelly DK (1994) Activity level and the susceptibility of anuran larvae to predation. Animal

Behaviour 47: 465-468.

Skelly DK, Werner EE (1990) Behavioral and life historical responses of larval American toads to

an odonate predator. Ecology 71: 2313-2322.

Slusarczk M (1999) Predator-induced diapause in Daphnia magna may require two chemical

cues. Oecologia 119: 159-165.

Smith DC (1983) Factors controlling populations of the chorus frog (Pseudacris triseriata) on

Isle Royale, Michigan. Ecology 64: 501-510.

Smith DC (1987) Adult recruitment in chorus frogs: Effects of size and date at metamorphosis.

Ecology 68: 344-350.

Page 67: PREDATOR-PREY INTERACTIONS BETWEEN AN INVASIVE …

Chapter 1

49

Smith RJF (1992) Alarm signals in fishes. Reviews in Fish Biology and Fisheries 2: 33-63.

Spieler M (2003) Risk of predation affects aggregation size: a study with tadpoles of

Phrynomantis microps (Anura: Microhylidae). Animal Behaviour 65: 179-184.

Spitze K (1992) Predator-mediated plasticity of prey life history and morphology: Chaoborus

americanus predation on Daphnia pulex. The American Naturalist 139: 229-247.

Steiner UK (2007a) Investment in defense and cost of predator-induced defense along a

resource gradient. Oecologia 152: 201-210.

Steiner UK (2007b) Linking antipredator behaviour, ingestion, gut evacuation and costs of

predator-induced responses in tadpoles. Animal Behaviour 74: 1473-1479.

Steiner UK, Van Buskirk J (2008) Environmental stress and the costs of whole-organism

phenotypic plasticity in tadpoles. Journal of Evolutionary Biology 21: 97-103.

Stirling G (1995) Daphnia behavior as a bioassay of fish presence or predation. Functional

Ecology 9: 778-784.

Stoks R, Block MD, Van de Meutter F, Johansson F (2005) Predation cost of rapid growth:

behavioural coupling and physiological decoupling. Journal of Animal Ecology 74: 708-

715.

Strauss SY, Lau JA, Carroll SP (2006) Evolutionary responses of natives to introduced species:

what do introductions tell us about natural communities? Ecology Letters 9: 354-371.

Stuart-Smith RD, White RWG, Barmuta LA (2008) A shift in the habitat use pattern of a lentic

galaxiid fish: an acute behavioural response to an introduced predator. Environmental

Biology of Fishes 82: 93-100.

Sultan SE (2000) Phenotypic plasticity for plant development, function and life history. Trends

in Plant Science 5: 537-542.

Sündermann D, Scheumann M, Zimmermann E (2008) Olfactory predator recognition in

predator-naïve gray mouse lemurs (Microcebus murinus). Journal of Comparative

Psychology 122: 146-155.

Tejedo M (1993) Size-dependent vulnerability and behavioral responses of tadpoles of two

anuran species to beetle larvae predators. Herpetologica 49: 287-294.

Tejedo M, Marangoni F, Pertoldi C, Richter-Boix A, Laurila A, Orizaola G, Nicieza AG, Álvarez D,

Gomez-Mestre I (2010) Contrasting effects of environmental factors during larval stage

on morphological plasticity in post-metamorphic frogs. Climate Research 43: 31-39.

Page 68: PREDATOR-PREY INTERACTIONS BETWEEN AN INVASIVE …

Chapter 1

50

Teplitsky C, Plenet S, Léna J-P, Mermet N, Malet E, Joly P (2005). Escape behaviour and

ultimate causes of specific induced defences in an anuran tadpole. Journal of

Evolutionary Biology 18: 180-190.

Tollrian R, Harvell CD (eds.) (1998) The ecology and evolution of inducible defenses. Princeton

University Press, Princeton.

Thompson JN (1998) Rapid evolution as an ecological process. Trends in Ecology and Evolution

13: 329-332.

Townsend CR (1996) Invasion biology and ecological impacts of brown trout Salmo trutta in

New Zealand. Biological Conservation 78: 13-22.

Trussell GC, Smith LD (2000) Induced defenses in response to an invading crab predator: an

explanation of historical and geographic phenotypic change. Proceedings of the National

Academy of Sciences USA 97: 2123-2127.

Urban MC (2007) The growth-predation risk trade-off under a growing gape-limited predation

threat. Ecology 88: 2587-2597.

Van Buskirk J (2000) The costs of an inducible defense in anuran larvae. Ecology 81: 2813-2821.

Van Buskirk J (2002) A comparative test of the adaptive plasticity hypothesis: Relationships

between habitat and phenotype in anuran larvae. The American Naturalist 160: 87-102.

Van Buskirk J (2009) Natural variation in morphology of larval amphibians: Phenotypic

plasticity in nature? Ecological Monographs 79: 681-705.

Van Buskirk J, Arioli M (2002) Dosage response of an induced defense: How sensitive are

tadpoles to predation risk? Ecology 83: 1580-1585.

Van Buskirk J, McCollum SA (2000a) Functional mechanisms of an inducible defence in

tadpoles: morphology and behaviour influence mortality risk from predation. Journal of

Evolutionary Biology 13: 336-347.

Van Buskirk J, McCollum SA (2000b) Influence of tail shape on tadpole swimming performance.

Journal of Experimental Biology 203: 2149-2158.

Van Buskirk J, McCollum SA, Werner EE (1997) Natural selection for environmentally induced

phenotypes in tadpoles. Evolution 51: 1983-1992.

Van Buskirk J, Anderwald P, Lüpold S, Reinhardt L, Schuler H (2003) The lure effect, tadpole tail

shape, and the target of dragonfly strikes. Journal of Herpetology 37: 420-424.

Page 69: PREDATOR-PREY INTERACTIONS BETWEEN AN INVASIVE …

Chapter 1

51

Vilhunen S, Hirvonen H (2003) Innate antipredator responses of Arctic charr (Salvelinus

alpinus) depend on predator species and their diet. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology

55: 1-10.

Vitousek PM, D’Antonio CM, Loope LL, Rejmánek M, Westbrooks R (1997) Introduced species:

a significant component of human-caused global change. New Zealand Journal of

Ecology 21: 1-16.

Warkentin KM (2011) Plasticity of hatching in amphibians: Evolution, trade-offs, cues and

mechanisms. Integrative and Comparative Biology 51: 111-127.

Wassersug RJ, Sperry DG (1977) The relationships of locomotion to differential predation on

Pseudacris triseriata (Anura: Hylidae). Ecology 58: 830-839.

Watt PJ, Nottingham SF, Young S (1997) Toad tadpole aggregation behaviour: evidence for a

predator avoidance function. Animal Behaviour 54: 865-872.

Werner EE (1986) Amphibian metamorphosis: growth rate, predation risk, and optimal size at

transformation. The American Naturalist 128: 319-341.

Werner EE, Anholt BR (1993) Ecological consequences of the tradeoff between growth and

mortality rates mediated by foraging activity. The American Naturalist 142: 242-272.

Werner EE, Peacor SD (2003) A review of trait-mediated indirect interactions. Ecology 84:

1083-1100.

Werner EE, Peacor SD (2006) Lethal and nonlethal predator effects on an herbivore guild

mediated by system productivity. Ecology 87: 347-361.

White AW, Pyke GH (1996) Distribution and conservation status of the green and golden bell

frog Litoria aurea in New South Wales. Australian Zoologist 30: 177-189.

Whitlow WL (2010) Changes in survivorship, behavior, and morphology in native soft-shell

clams induced by invasive green crab predators. Marine Ecology 31: 418-430.

Whitlow WL, Rice NA, Sweeney C (2003) Native species vulnerability to introduced predators:

testing an inducible defense and a refuge from predation. Biological Invasions 5: 23-31.

Whitman DW, Agrawal A (2009) What is phenotypic plasticity and why is it important? in:

Whitman DW, Ananthakrishnan TN (eds.), Phenotypic plasticity of insects: Mechanisms

and consequences. Science Publishers, Plymouth.

Wilbur, HM (1997) Experimental ecology of food webs: Complex systems in temporary ponds.

Ecology 78: 2279-2302.

Page 70: PREDATOR-PREY INTERACTIONS BETWEEN AN INVASIVE …

Chapter 1

52

Wilbur HM, Collins JP (1973) Ecological aspects of amphibian metamorphosis. Science 182:

1305-1314.

Wilbur HM, Semlitsch RD (1990) Ecological consequences of tail injury in Rana tadpoles. Copeia

1990: 18-24.

Wisenden BD (2003) Chemically mediated strategies to counter predation. Pp. 236-251, in:

Collin SP, Marshall NJ (eds.), Sensory Processing in Aquatic Environments. Springer-

Verlag, New York.

Page 71: PREDATOR-PREY INTERACTIONS BETWEEN AN INVASIVE …

53

CHAPTER 2

Nonlethal injury caused by an invasive alien

predator and its consequences for an anuran tadpole

Nunes AL, Cruz MJ, Tejedo M, Laurila A, Rebelo R

Basic and Applied Ecology 11: 645-654

Page 72: PREDATOR-PREY INTERACTIONS BETWEEN AN INVASIVE …

54

Page 73: PREDATOR-PREY INTERACTIONS BETWEEN AN INVASIVE …

Chapter 2

55

Nonlethal injury caused by an invasive alien predator and its

consequences for an anuran tadpole

Abstract

Nonlethal tail injury resulting from unsuccessful predation attempts is common in anuran

larvae and can potentially induce significant fitness costs in terms of survival and growth. We

tested the hypotheses that the alien red swamp crayfish, Procambarus clarkii, is an important

inducer of tail injury in tadpoles of the Iberian spadefoot toad, Pelobates cultripes, and that tail

damage can have important consequences for the tadpoles’ life history and morphology. This

was investigated by first estimating frequencies of caudal injury in P. cultripes tadpoles in

temporary ponds, with and without crayfish. Secondly, we performed a laboratory experiment

in which four levels of tail injury frequency were combined with two levels of food availability.

The frequency of tadpoles with damaged tails was higher in ponds with crayfish and the

presence of this predator was the strongest predictor of tail injury frequency in a pond.

Induced tail loss decreased larval survivorship and affected tail morphology, with injured

tadpoles developing deeper tail muscles and shallower tail fins. The magnitude of these effects

depended on injury frequency, as well as on food availability. The results suggest that P. clarkii

is inflicting tail injuries at much higher levels than those occurring before its introduction;

these injuries affect tadpole morphology and may induce delayed fitness costs.

Keywords: Unsuccessful predation, alien crayfish, tail injury, tadpoles, costs

Page 74: PREDATOR-PREY INTERACTIONS BETWEEN AN INVASIVE …

Chapter 2

56

Introduction

Invasions of exotic species are one of the major factors contributing to worldwide amphibian

decline (Kats & Ferrer 2003). Freshwater ecosystems are particularly vulnerable to

nonindigenous predators, such as crayfish (Hobbs, Jass & Huner 1989). Crayfish, often

successful invaders, can impact native communities across multiple trophic levels and readily

prey upon amphibians (Nyström, Svensson, Lardner, Brönmark & Granéli 2001). Crayfish

introductions can then have important ecological consequences for amphibians, since

predation is one of the major biotic factors structuring amphibian larval communities (Wilbur

1980).

Amphibian larvae are often found with damaged tails resulting from unsuccessful predation

attempts, which can be caused by a wide variety of vertebrate (turtles, salamanders, newts)

and invertebrate (dragonfly and beetle larvae, crayfish) predators (e.g. Morin 1985; Wilbur &

Semlitsch 1990; Tejedo 1993; Nyström et al. 2001). Tadpoles continuously exposed to

predators are likely to accumulate several injuries. Injury frequencies in natural populations

are influenced by predator densities and often also by factors such as the diversity and

complexity of microhabitats (Morin 1985; Figiel & Semlitsch 1991).

The highly fragile tail fin which is easily torn by predators, can help tadpoles escape and

survive otherwise lethal attacks (Doherty, Wassersug & Lee 1998; Van Buskirk, Anderwald,

Lupold, Reinhardt & Schuler 2003). In this aspect, predator-induced tail loss in amphibian

larvae resembles autotomy, a widespread antipredator strategy involving self-induced release

of a body part, which helps animals escape predator attacks (see Maginnis 2006 for a review).

Similarly to autotomy, tail loss has an obvious immediate survival benefit, but its potential

costs are substantial and include reduced swimming performance and increased energetic

expenditure for regenerating damaged tissue (Wassersug & Sperry 1977; Van Buskirk &

McCollum 2000). Swimming performance is not only affected by tail length, but also by

Page 75: PREDATOR-PREY INTERACTIONS BETWEEN AN INVASIVE …

Chapter 2

57

tadpole morphology, implying an important role of tadpole shape in predator escape abilities.

In the presence of predators, many species develop deeper tails, which increase their survival

under predation risk, either due to enhanced swimming performance caused by larger tail

muscles (McCollum & Leimberger 1997; Teplitsky, Plenet, Léna, Mermet, Malet et al.2005;

Wilson, Kraft & Van Damme 2005) or by enabling nonlethal ripping of the deep tail fin

(Doherty et al. 1998; Van Buskirk et al. 2003). Regeneration following tail loss may also impose

a cost because energy previously used for growth and development is then diverted to repair

the damaged tissue (Semlitsch & Reichling 1989). However, the consequences of tail loss seem

to depend on injury frequency and severity, as well as on the amount of food available to

tadpoles. Injury effects become more pronounced when energy is limited (Parichy & Kaplan

1992).

In the Southwest of the Iberian Peninsula, the Iberian spadefoot toad (Pelobates cultripes)

often coexists with the invasive red swamp crayfish (Procambarus clarkii). This invasive

crayfish was introduced in this area, previously devoid of freshwater crayfish, in the early

1970’s (Gutiérrez-Yurrita & Montes 1999). It is an active predator of P. cultripes tadpoles (Cruz

& Rebelo 2005), which can inflict tail injury in mesocosms (A. L. Nunes, pers. obs.).

Furthermore, it typically consumes and damages aquatic macrophytes, which might reduce

periphyton biomass, an important food source for tadpoles (Gutiérrez-Yurrita & Montes 1999;

Nyström et al. 2001). P. cultripes breeds mostly in temporary ponds that lack fish and other

large aquatic predators, but which P. clarkii is able to colonize (Cruz & Rebelo 2007). Cruz,

Rebelo & Crespo (2006) showed that, nowadays, the probability of a water body being used as

a breeding site by P. cultripes is significantly reduced by P. clarkii’s presence.

The aim of this study was two-fold. First, we assessed the importance of P. clarkii as a tail

injury agent on free-living tadpoles, by comparing frequencies of tadpoles with tail damage in

ponds with and without P. clarkii in southwestern Spain. Secondly, we experimentally

manipulated frequency of tail injury and food availability in the laboratory, in order to assess

Page 76: PREDATOR-PREY INTERACTIONS BETWEEN AN INVASIVE …

Chapter 2

58

survival, growth and development costs of tail damage for P. cultripes tadpoles. This integrated

approach allowed us to consider a large number of factors that should be addressed in a field

study and, at the same time, look into interactions that are often difficult to establish in

complex systems but that can easily arise in a controlled laboratory setting.

Methods

Field study

The field work was conducted in Parque Natural del Entorno de Doñana, southwestern Spain.

P. clarkii was introduced in the Doñana marshes in 1974 and probably colonized this area

through La Rocina Creek, the main permanent tributary to the Guadalquivir marshlands and

the most important source of P. clarkii colonizers in the park (Gutiérrez-Yurrita & Montes

1999). The park contains a series of ecologically similar temporary ponds, free of fish due to

their hydrological cycle, but where crayfish can attain very high densities. Only ponds far from

a crayfish source remain uncolonized (Cruz & Rebelo 2007).

Sampling methods

In February-April 2004, we sampled 54 temporary ponds in the study area but, for this study,

we selected the 31 ponds in which P. cultripes tadpoles were found (Fig. 1). To investigate the

presence and abundance of P. clarkii individuals and P. cultripes tadpoles we used 5-6 baited

funnel traps (1.5 and 5 L) in each pond. These traps were left for one night and checked the

following morning. Five dip-net sweeps (30 cm diameter, 2 mm mesh size) were also

performed in each pond. Ponds having no crayfish were repeatedly trap-sampled - up to 3

times -, in order to confirm crayfish absence. Cruz et al. (2006) showed these methods

Page 77: PREDATOR-PREY INTERACTIONS BETWEEN AN INVASIVE …

Chapter 2

59

adequate to assess the presence of P. clarkii. All animals were released back in the ponds after

sampling.

Fig. 1. Map of the study area (surroundings of Doñana Natural Park) and its location in the Iberian

Peninsula, showing the 31 temporary ponds selected for this study. The star indicates the location

where the clutches for the laboratory experiment were collected.

The frequency of injured tadpoles in each pond was estimated only from dip-net sweeps,

because tadpoles captured in the traps might have become injured while being confined with

predators. Tadpoles were considered as having tail injury if a part of their tail had been ripped

off and not yet regenerated. In order to assess if the frequency of injured tadpoles in a pond

was influenced by the presence of crayfish, we had to account for variables such as the

Page 78: PREDATOR-PREY INTERACTIONS BETWEEN AN INVASIVE …

Chapter 2

60

presence of other potential aquatic predators, vegetation cover and pond dimensions. Aquatic

insects from the families Dytiscidae (adults and larvae), Aeshnidae, Cordulegasteridae,

Notonectidae, Corixidae, Coenagrionidae and vertebrate urodeles (Pleurodeles waltl, Triturus

marmoratus, Lissotriton boscai), were sampled using dip-net sweeps and funnel traps as

described above. Emergent, floating and submerged vegetation cover were estimated using six

1-m transects, in which the vegetation composition was determined every 10 cm (ten points

per transect). Vegetation cover was the percentage of points covered by each type of

vegetation. Pond area (m2) and maximum depth (cm) were also measured.

Statistical analyses

As ponds with and without crayfish may differ in their predator communities, we estimated

and compared relative abundances of invertebrate and vertebrate predators present in these

two types of ponds. Abundances were calculated considering the most effective sampling

method for each predator and based on catch per sampling effort. Trap captures were thus

used to estimate abundances of P. clarkii and Dytiscidae; for all other predators dip-net

sweeps proved more efficient. We used t-tests to compare the abundances of different

predators in ponds with and without crayfish. We compared frequencies of tail injury in ponds

with and without crayfish using a Mann-Whitney U test. We also performed Spearman

correlations between frequency of tail injury and abundances of all predators sampled. Effect

size estimates were determined by Cohen’s d.

We used Generalized Linear Models to investigate which factors influenced the frequency of

injured tadpoles. We used a binomial function in which the number of tadpoles having tail

damage in each pond was considered the dependent variable and the total number of

tadpoles caught in that pond treated as a trials variable. As we did not find differences in the

abundance of predators in ponds with and without crayfish (see results), we used predator

Page 79: PREDATOR-PREY INTERACTIONS BETWEEN AN INVASIVE …

Chapter 2

61

presence/absence data for the biotic variables, whereas the abiotic variables were treated as

continuous (Table 1). We performed a series of univariate analyses and the variables with P-

values < 0.25 in the Wald test were selected for the multivariate analysis. We also checked for

collinearity between abiotic variables using a Pearson correlation coefficient. Variables were

excluded from the multivariate model using a backward stepwise elimination procedure, until

all the remaining variables were significant predictors (P< 0.05). GLMs were performed using

the software package SPSS (version 15.0).

Table 1. Abiotic and biotic variables (and correspondent measurement units) recorded in each pond and

used to perform the Generalized Linear Model. For the biotic variables 0 means absence and 1 means

presence of a predator group in a pond

Variables Measurement units

Abiotic

Aquatic vegetation

- Emergent - Floating - Submerged

% cover % cover % cover

Pond area m2

Pond maximum depth cm

Biotic

Crayfish 0/1

Dytiscidae (adults) 0/1

Dytiscidae (larvae) 0/1

Aeshnidae 0/1

Cordulegasteridae 0/1

Notonectidae 0/1

Corixidae 0/1

Coenagrionidae 0/1

Vertebrate predators 0/1

Page 80: PREDATOR-PREY INTERACTIONS BETWEEN AN INVASIVE …

Chapter 2

62

Laboratory experiment

Experimental procedure

On November 30th 2005 we collected four clutches of P. cultripes in a pond in Serra de

Grândola, southwestern Portugal (38º04’N, 8º34’W) (Fig. 1), colonized by P. clarkii. The eggs

were taken to the Faculty of Sciences, University of Lisbon, where the experiment took place.

Tadpoles were kept in clutch-specific 20-L aquaria filled with aged tap water changed regularly

and fed commercial fish food ad libitum. When they reached Gosner developmental stage 25

(operculum closure over gills; Gosner 1960) we randomly selected 120 tadpoles from each

clutch and kept them individually in 0.4-L plastic cups filled with aged water, changed every

two days. Throughout the experiment, water temperature was kept between 19.5 and 22.5 ºC

and the photoperiod was 12L: 12D.

We used a factorial experimental design with frequency of injury (four levels), food availability

(high/low) and different clutches (four levels) as factors, with each treatment combination

replicated 15 times, resulting in a total of 480 tadpoles as experimental units. Frequency of

caudal injury ranged from no injury (control) to three injuries per tadpole, inflicted in events

separated by 20 days. The first injury event, in which tadpoles from injury levels 1, 2 and 3

suffered tail damage, took place January 16th (day 0 of the experiment) and the last one (injury

only at level 3) March 13th 2006. Tail damage was inflicted so that the final tail length equalled

0.5 × headbody length, the amount of tail left being always proportional to the tadpole size.

Immediately after the damage, inflicted with a disinfected scalpel, tadpoles were placed

individually in aged water and none showed signs of infection. Tadpoles regenerated their tails

in 10-15 days.

The tadpoles were fed 0.05 g of commercial fish food (Tetra Min) and boiled lettuce, according

to two different treatments of food availability. Tadpoles from the high-food treatment were

fed every two days, while tadpoles from the low-food treatment were only fed every fifth day.

Page 81: PREDATOR-PREY INTERACTIONS BETWEEN AN INVASIVE …

Chapter 2

63

The lower food treatment was known to limit growth and developmental rates of P. cultripes

tadpoles in the laboratory (A. L. Nunes, unpublished data).

At days 20, 40 and 60 (20 days after each injury event and immediately before the next one),

all the individuals were photographed in side view against a grid background, in order to

obtain data on their morphology. Gosner development stage was also determined and survival

was registered at day 60. All the photographs were analysed using software Image J 1.38x

(Image Processing and Analysis in Java) and body length, maximum body depth, maximum tail

fin depth and maximum tail muscle depth were recorded to the nearest 0.01 mm. Tail traits

were always measured in the undamaged part of the tail.

Statistical analyses

All analyses on morphology were conducted on log transformed data in order to fulfil the

assumptions of analysis of variance. Prior to analyzing morphological data, trait values were

corrected for variation in body size because morphological measurements are usually highly

correlated. In order to do this, body size was defined as the first component in a principal

component analysis performed using a correlation matrix. All the traits loaded highly positively

on the first axis, and it always explained more than 80% of the total variation. Body size (PC1)

was then used as a covariate in two-way fixed effect ANCOVAs (factors: food availability and

injury treatment) to compare tadpole morphology in the different treatments (García-Berthou

2001). Clutch identity was included in the initial analyses as a random factor, but as it did not

affect the results qualitatively in any of the analyses (in all cases, P> 0.1), it was not included in

the final models.

In the analyses of morphology at days 20, 40 and 60, we compared treatments according to

the injury events that had already been inflicted. For example, in day 20 we compared the

control tadpoles with tadpoles that tail damage induced once, whereas in day 60, after

Page 82: PREDATOR-PREY INTERACTIONS BETWEEN AN INVASIVE …

Chapter 2

64

tadpoles had been injured three times, all injury treatments were compared. The initial models

included all possible interactions with the covariate PC1, but all non-significant interactions

were removed from the final model. Post-hoc pairwise comparisons were performed using

Bonferroni corrections. Effect sizes for the analysis of variance were calculated using partial

eta squared. Developmental rates were calculated as the difference between final and initial

Gosner development stage divided by the number of days of the experiment and analyzed

with two way ANOVAs. Tadpole survival was analyzed as a binomial variable, using a factorial

logistic regression. In this case, odds ratios were used to compute effect sizes. All values in the

results are expressed as mean ± 1 S.E.

Results

Field study

P. clarkii was found in 16 of the 31 ponds with P. cultripes. In all ponds with P. clarkii, we found

evidence of crayfish reproduction (juveniles or brooding females). All the other predators were

found both in ponds with and without crayfish. The abundance of P. cultripes tadpoles did not

differ between ponds with and without crayfish (11.83 ± 4.18 individuals/trap and 12.51 ± 9.51

individuals/trap, respectively; t29 = 0.07, P = 0.947, ES = 0.025).

Abundance of invertebrate and vertebrate predators was also similar in ponds with and

without P. clarkii (t29 = -0.06, P = 0.952, ES = 0.022 and t29= 1.16, P = 0.254, ES = 0.031,

respectively). Likewise, there were no significant differences in relative abundances of each

group of invertebrate and vertebrate predators between ponds with and without crayfish (t-

tests, P > 0.05; Fig. 2). In ponds with crayfish, P. clarkii was usually the most abundant predator

(Fig. 2).

Page 83: PREDATOR-PREY INTERACTIONS BETWEEN AN INVASIVE …

Chapter 2

65

Fig. 2. Relative abundance (mean and standard error) of the invertebrate and vertebrate predators

present in temporary ponds, with and without crayfish. See methods for abbreviations of predator

group names.

The frequency of tail injury in P. cultripes tadpoles was higher in ponds with than without

crayfish (Mann-Whitney U test, Z = -3.59, P < 0.001, ES = 0.901; Fig. 3). Frequency of tail injury

was significantly correlated with the abundance of crayfish (rs = 0.63, N = 31, P < 0.001), but

not with abundance of any other predator (all P > 0.05). When testing for which factors

predicted the frequency of tail damage found in different ponds, the final model retained only

four factors, two biotic and two abiotic. The frequency of injured tadpoles was higher in ponds

with adult Dytiscidae and P. clarkii than in the ponds where these predators were absent

(Table 2). Crayfish proved to be the strongest positive predictor of the frequency of injured

tadpoles in a water body (β = 3.62 ± 0.45). In ponds with P. clarkii the probability of finding a

tadpole with tail damage was 37.4 times higher (95% C.I.: 15.49 – 90.28) than finding it in a

pond without P. clarkii. An increase in pond area increased the frequency of tail injury,

whereas tail injuries decreased with increasing vegetation cover (Table 2).

Page 84: PREDATOR-PREY INTERACTIONS BETWEEN AN INVASIVE …

Chapter 2

66

Fig. 3. Mean frequency of tadpoles (+ S.E.) having tail injury in ponds with and without crayfish.

Laboratory experiment

In the beginning of the experiment (day 0) there were no differences in the morphology of

tadpoles assigned to different treatments (for all morphological traits, P > 0.05). At days 20, 40

and 60, only food availability had an influence on body size (PC1) and developmental stage of

the tadpoles, with animals from the high-food treatment being larger and more developed

(F1,161 = 479.46, P < 0.001, ES = 0.749, F1,306 = 1770.21, P < 0.001, ES = 0.853 and F1,418 = 2311.66,

P < 0.001, ES = 0.847 respectively, for body size; F1,212 = 285.69, P< 0.001, ES = 0.574, F1,324 =

710.13 P < 0.001, ES = 0.687 and F1,418 = 550.19, P < 0.001, ES = 0.568 respectively, for

developmental stage).

Table 2. Estimated coefficients (β) with standard errors (S.E.), estimated odds ratio (exp(β)) and P-values

obtained via the Wald χ2 test for the variables retained in the final Generalized Linear Model as being

significant predictors of the frequency of tail injury in P. cultripes

Variables Coefficient (β) S.E. Wald d.f. P Exp (β)

Constant -7.871 1.428 30.396 1 <0.001 0

Dytiscidae presence (adults) 3.274 0.546 36.014 1 <0.001 26.418

Crayfish presence 3.622 0.450 64.858 1 <0.001 37.396

Emergent vegetation -1.571 0.461 11.621 1 0.001 0.208

Pond area 0.928 0.275 11.381 1 0.001 2.529

For the categorical variables the reference category is 0 (= absence of predator).

Page 85: PREDATOR-PREY INTERACTIONS BETWEEN AN INVASIVE …

Chapter 2

67

Twenty days after the first injury event and after complete tail regeneration, injured tadpoles

had shallower tail fins and deeper tail muscles than uninjured tadpoles (Table 3; Fig. 4).

Individuals from the high-food treatment had deeper tail muscles than the ones from the

low-food treatment, but there were no differences between food treatment groups for the tail

fin depth (Table 3; Fig. 4). At day 40, animals injured a second time had again shallower tail fins

than uninjured animals and animals injured only once; food availability did not have a

significant effect (Table 3; Fig. 4). For tail muscle depth, a significant interaction between food

availability and frequency of injury showed that injured tadpoles from the high-food treatment

had deeper tail muscles than uninjured tadpoles, whereas there were no differences at the

low-food level (Table 3; Fig. 4). After all the injury levels were induced (day 60), tadpoles at

high-food had deeper tail muscles and shallower tail fins than tadpoles at low-food (Table 3;

Fig. 4). Variation in tail fin depth due to tail damage depended on food availability, with injured

tadpoles having slightly deeper tail fins in the low-food treatment and shallower tail fins in the

high-food treatment (Table 3; Fig. 4).

Fig. 4. Tail morphology (tail fin and tail muscle depth) of tadpoles subjected to high (black squares) and

low food availability (open circles) at days 20, 40 and 60 (20 days after each injury event). Frequency of

injury varied in accordance with the injury events that were successively inflicted. Data are least-square

means ± S.E.

Page 86: PREDATOR-PREY INTERACTIONS BETWEEN AN INVASIVE …

Chapter 2

68

The PC1 x Food/Injury interaction was significant for tail muscle depth at day 40 and for tail fin

depth at day 60. However, one-way ANCOVAs performed separately for high and low-food

treatments showed exactly the same trends as the two-way ANCOVAs.

Table 3. Analysis of covariance (ANCOVA) on the effects of food availability and frequency of injury on

tadpoles tail morphology (tail fin and tail muscle depth) 20, 40 and 60 days after the beginning of the

experiment. PC1 is the first component from a principal component analysis and represents body size

(used as a covariate). ES stands for effect size

Day 20

Tail Fin Depth Tail Muscle Depth

Source d.f. F P ES d.f. F P ES

Food(F) 1 0.000 0.985 0 1 6.752 0.010 0.040

Injury(I) 1 7.710 0.006 0.046 1 36.294 <0.001 0.185

FxI 1 0.050 0.823 0 1 0.290 0.591 0.002

PC1 1 211.183 <0.001 0.569 1 261.834 <0.001 0.621

PC1xF

PC1xI

Error 160 160

Day 40

Tail Fin Depth Tail Muscle Depth

Source d.f. F P ES d.f. F P ES

Food(F) 1 3.025 0.083 0.010 1 45.916 <0.001 0.132

Injury(I) 2 15.116 <0.001 0.090 2 25.119 <0.001 0.143

FxI 2 2.766 0.065 0.018 2 5.515 0.004 0.035

PC1 1 346.266 <0.001 0.532 1 242.842 <0.001 0.446

PC1xF 1 25.118 <0.001 0.077

PC1xI 2 4.678 0.010 0.030

Error 305 302

Day 60

Tail Fin Depth Tail Muscle Depth

Source d.f. F P ES d.f. F P ES

Food(F) 1 20.133 <0.001 0.046 1 221.709 <0.001 0.348

Injury(I) 3 1.939 0.123 0.014 3 2.532 0.057 0.018

FxI 3 3.027 0.029 0.022 3 0.116 0.951 0.001

PC1 1 547.693 <0.001 0.570 1 162.022 <0.001 0.280

PC1xF 1 23.186 <0.001 0.053 1 15.483 <0.001 0.036

PC1xI 3 2.897 0.035 0.021

Error 413 416

Page 87: PREDATOR-PREY INTERACTIONS BETWEEN AN INVASIVE …

Chapter 2

69

Survival after 60 days was marginally significantly affected by injury treatments (logistic

regression, Wald χ2 = 7.20, d.f. = 3, P = 0.066), with animals injured twice (0.842 ± 0.033 (mean

± S.E.); Wald χ2 = 5.76, d.f. = 1, P = 0.016, ES = 0.329) or thrice (0.858 ± 0.032; Wald χ2 = 4.36,

d.f. = 1, P = 0.037, ES = 0.375) having lower survival than animals with no tail injuries (0.942 ±

0.021). Neither food availability, nor the interaction between the two factors influenced

tadpole survival (Wald χ2 = 0.15, d.f. = 1, P = 0.698, ES = 1.357 and Wald χ2 = 4.74, d.f. = 3, P =

0.192, respectively).

Discussion

We found that the presence of the invasive species P. clarkii was associated with a greatly

increased rate of tail injury in P. cultripes tadpoles. The positive correlation found between the

frequency of tail injury and crayfish abundance emphasizes the serious consequences that the

high P. clarkii densities can have for P. cultripes tadpoles, especially if we consider injury

frequency as an indicator of predator pressure (Morin 1985). Furthermore, P. cultripes

tadpoles seem to lack behavioural antipredator defenses against P. clarkii, such as decreased

activity under predation risk (Cruz & Rebelo 2005), making them more prone to both direct

predation and nonlethal attacks. In addition, high fecundity, rapid growth and ability to adjust

its reproductive cycle to the hydrological conditions allow P. clarkii to quickly build extremely

large populations in these fish-free habitats, which can become a serious risk to amphibian

populations (Hobbs et al. 1989; Gutiérrez-Yurrita & Montes 1999).

Dytiscid beetles also proved important agents of injury for P. cultripes tadpoles, reflecting the

high predation pressure they can impose (Tejedo 1993). Tejedo (1993) suggested that the large

body size of P. cultripes tadpoles could be an important antipredator mechanism against

Dytiscus pisanus larvae. This can make them hard to capture not only because of their large

size, but also because larger tadpoles are faster swimmers (Wassersug & Sperry 1977; Van

Page 88: PREDATOR-PREY INTERACTIONS BETWEEN AN INVASIVE …

Chapter 2

70

Buskirk & McCollum 2000). This may allow tadpoles to frequently escape only at the cost of a

ripped tail.

Tadpole tail injuries decreased with increasing emergent vegetation cover, probably because

vegetation provides shelter for prey by decreasing predator encounter rates (Babbitt & Tanner

1998). On the other hand, frequency of injured tadpoles increased with pond area. Pond size

can influence important biotic and abiotic factors in a community. For instance, Pearman

(1995) found that increasing pond area led to a greater incidence of Dytiscid beetles. In fact, in

the present study there was a positive correlation between pond area and adult Dytiscid

abundance, which may explain the increase in tail injuries with increased pond size.

In our experimental study, we found a tendency for larval survivorship being affected by

increased frequency of caudal injury. In the laboratory, even under high-food conditions,

repeated injuries seem to impose a cost in terms of reduced survival to P. cultripes tadpoles.

Parichy and Kaplan (1992) also observed reduced survivorship of injured tadpoles of Bombina

orientalis, but only under low-food conditions. Similarly, tail loss in the salamander

Ambystoma talpoideum was negatively correlated with survival (Semlitsch & Reichling 1989).

This mortality effect can be caused by physiological stress responses or infection due to

wounds (Langkilde & Shine 2006). However, in our field study, although tail injury frequency

was much higher in ponds with than without crayfish, tadpole abundances were similar in the

two types of ponds. Assuming there are no confounding factors, this suggests that, in our

study area, there is no apparent survival cost caused by tail damage. Alternatively, if there is a

survival cost, tadpole abundances in ponds with and without crayfish might not have differed

due to crayfish indirect effects in other potential tadpole predators, such as

macroinvertebrates or urodeles. If predation risk by these predators is reduced by crayfish-

induced antipredator behaviours (Gamradt, Kats & Anzalone 1997; Nyström et al. 2001;

Werner & Peacor 2003), predation on tadpoles will decrease and population abundances may

become similar to those in crayfish-free ponds.

Page 89: PREDATOR-PREY INTERACTIONS BETWEEN AN INVASIVE …

Chapter 2

71

Also, contrary to our expectations, we did not find a perceptible cost of tail injury on the

development and growth of P. cultripes tadpoles, even under relatively poor food conditions.

Other studies have also found weak or no effects of tail damage on developmental and growth

rates of tadpoles, suggesting that rapid regeneration could reduce the ecological costs of tail

damage (Wilbur & Semlitsch 1990; McCollum & Leimberger 1997). Following injury, all our

tadpoles regenerated their tails in less than 15 days. However, if tadpoles compensate for tail

injury by adopting antipredator behaviours like hiding, decreasing activity or moving to areas

less suitable for predators, this may reduce their foraging success and ultimately growth under

more natural conditions (Semlitsch 1990; Figiel & Semlitsch 1991).

Parichy and Kaplan (1992) found that B. orientalis tadpoles with injured tails had decreased

growth and development at metamorphosis, but only under limited food conditions. This led

to a later emergence and a smaller size at metamorphosis. Hence, even though in the

laboratory there were no evident growth costs twenty days after the injury events, it is quite

likely that there are delayed costs of tail damage for tadpoles. If tadpoles suffer repeated

injuries during the larval stage, several regeneration episodes will take place, implying major

shifts in resource allocation to support the rebuilt structures. The potential costs of

transferring energy away from processes supporting growth into the reconstruction of the tail

might only arise at metamorphosis, a period of profound physiological and morphological

changes (Werner 1986). Injured animals may metamorphose at smaller sizes, which often

leads to reduced postmetamorphic survival, delayed maturity and decreased size at first

reproduction (Smith 1987; Semlitsch, Scott & Pechmann 1988). Indeed, in our study area, a

sample of newly-metamorphosed individuals from ponds with crayfish had significantly smaller

weights and body sizes than those from ponds without crayfish (M. Tejedo, A.L. Nunes, M. J.

Cruz, R. Rebelo, unpublished data). However, we can only speculate that this might arise from

differences in frequencies of tail damage, because many other factors may influence it.

Page 90: PREDATOR-PREY INTERACTIONS BETWEEN AN INVASIVE …

Chapter 2

72

The nonlethal injuries inflicted in our experiment induced changes in tail morphology:

damaged animals had, in general, shallower tail fins, with deeper tail muscles. As

measurements of tail traits were performed in the undamaged part of the tail, this indicates

plastic changes in the original tail muscle and fin. This suggests that regeneration is not only

producing new structures, but also influencing growth of the existing ones. To our knowledge,

this is the first record of morphological plasticity in P. cultripes tadpoles. The increase in tail

muscle depth as a response to caudal injury is in accordance with morphological responses of

several tadpole species exposed to predators (e.g. Relyea 2001; Teplitsky et al. 2005; Wilson et

al. 2005). In Scaphiopus holbrookii, a species from the same family of P. cultripes, tadpoles

having predator-induced deeper tails exhibited faster burst swimming speeds, which probably

promote a higher promptness to escape an attack (Dayton et al. 2005; Teplitsky et al. 2005).

The larger muscle, apart from allowing tadpoles to outswim predators may also enable them

to survive attacks by facilitating tail ripping via enhanced thrust (Dayton et al. 2005). We can

only speculate how well this morphological response would suit P.clarkii’s predation attempts,

but the ability of a tadpole to generate bursts of speed or to facilitate membranous tail ripping

is probably a useful feature for avoiding unpredictable attacks.

Food availability proved to be an important factor in determining changes in tail depth. In the

beginning of the experiment all injured tadpoles increased their muscle depth but, after the

second injury, low-food tadpoles did not increase it anymore. This was probably due to energy

limitation, suggesting that food levels may shape the consequences of tail injury. The response

of altering muscle depth was mainly found at high resource availability and is probably absent

in macrophyte impoverished wetlands invaded by P. clarkii. If habitat productivity is dampened

by crayfish presence, the effects of tail injury might be more severe for tadpoles, as they lack

the necessary energy to recover from them.

To summarize, in areas where P. clarkii is established, it is probably imposing a high predation

pressure on P. cultripes, as reflected by the high frequencies of tadpoles with tail injuries.

Page 91: PREDATOR-PREY INTERACTIONS BETWEEN AN INVASIVE …

Chapter 2

73

However, tail damage is probably not critical for tadpole survival, because animals often

survive attacks to the tail. In fact, in our experiment, tail injury only imposed a marginal cost in

tadpole survival. It also stimulated the growth of the tail muscles, which did not exact an

immediate cost on growth and development, probably reflecting an adaptation to repeated

tail injuries. Nevertheless, costs may arise from shifts in resource allocation that have little

immediate impact, but that alternatively compromise future fitness components. Moreover, as

true fitness costs may be underestimated in laboratory conditions, studying these mechanisms

under more natural conditions is essential to understand potential impacts for amphibians in

systems invaded by this crayfish.

Acknowledgments

We thank Sandra Amaral for her help in the field and Nídia Fernandes, Ricardo Rocha, Sofia

Viegas, Pedro Andrade and Vasco Furtado for their help in the experiment. We also thank

Pedro Segurado for drawing the map. We thank the Portuguese Instituto da Conservação da

Natureza e da Biodiversidade (ICNB) for the permits. This research was supported by the FCT

Project POCI/BIA-BDE/56100/2004 and by the European Community-Access to Research

Infrastructure Action of the Improving Human Potential Programme in Doñana Biological

Station (ECODOCA).

References

Babbitt, K.J., & Tanner, G.W. (1998). Effects of cover and predator size on survival and

development of Rana utricularia tadpoles. Oecologia, 114, 258-262.

Cruz, M.J., & Rebelo, R. (2005). Vulnerability of southwest Iberian amphibians to an introduced

crayfish, Procambarus clarkii. Amphibia-Reptilia, 26, 293-303.

Page 92: PREDATOR-PREY INTERACTIONS BETWEEN AN INVASIVE …

Chapter 2

74

Cruz, M.J., & Rebelo, R. (2007). Colonization of freshwater habitats by an introduced crayfish,

Procambarus clarkii, in Southwest Iberian Peninsula. Hydrobiologia, 575, 191-201.

Cruz, M.J., Rebelo, R., & Crespo, E.G. (2006). Effects of an introduced crayfish, Procambarus

clarkii, on the distribution of south-western Iberian amphibians in their breeding

habitats. Ecography, 29, 329-338.

Dayton, G. H., Saenz, D., Baum, K. A., Langerhans, R. B., & DeWitt, T. J. (2005). Body shape,

burst speed and escape behavior of larval anurans. Oikos, 111, 582-591.

Doherty, P.A., Wassersug, R.J., & Lee, J.M. (1998). Mechanical properties of the tadpole tail fin.

Journal of Experimental Biology, 201, 2691-2699.

Figiel, C.R. & Semlitsch, R.D. (1991). Effects of nonlethal injury and habitat complexity on

predation in tadpole populations. Canadian Journal of Zoology, 69, 830-834.

Gamradt, S.C., Kats, L.B., & Anzalone, C.B. (1997). Aggression by non-native crayfish deters

breeding in California newts. Conservation Biology, 11, 793-796.

García-Berthou, E. (2001). On the misuse of residuals in ecology: testing regression residuals

vs. the analysis of covariance. Journal of Animal Ecology, 70, 708-711.

Gosner, K.L. (1960). A simplified table for staging anuran embryos and larvae with notes on

identification. Herpetologica, 16, 183-190.

Gutiérrez-Yurrita, P.J., & Montes, C. (1999). Bioenergetics and phenology of reproduction of

the introduced red swamp crayfish, Procambarus clarkii, in Doñana National Park, Spain,

and implications for species management. Freshwater Biology, 42, 561-574.

Hobbs, H.H., Jass, J.P., & Huner, J.V. (1989). A review of global crayfish introductions with

particular emphasis on two North-American species (Decapoda, Cambaridae).

Crustaceana, 56, 299-316.

Page 93: PREDATOR-PREY INTERACTIONS BETWEEN AN INVASIVE …

Chapter 2

75

Kats, L.B., & Ferrer, R.P. (2003). Alien predators and amphibian declines: review of two

decades of science and the transition to conservation. Diversity and Distributions, 9, 99-

110.

Langkilde, T., & Shine, R. (2006). How much stress do researchers inflict on their study

animals? A case study using a scincid lizard, Eulamprus heatwolei. Journal of

Experimental Biology, 209, 1035-1043.

Maginnis, T.L. (2006). The costs of autotomy and regeneration in animals: a review and

framework for future research. Behavioral Ecology, 17, 857-872.

McCollum, S.A., & Leimberger, J.D. (1997). Predator-induced morphological changes in an

amphibian: predation by dragonflies affects tadpole shape and color. Oecologia, 109,

615-621.

Morin, P.J. (1985). Predation intensity, prey survival and injury frequency in an amphibian

predator-prey interaction. Copeia, 1985, 638-644.

Nyström, P., Svensson, O., Lardner, B., Brönmark, C., & Granéli, W. (2001). The influence of

multiple introduced predators on a littoral pond community. Ecology, 82, 1023-1039.

Parichy, D.M., & Kaplan, R.H. (1992). Developmental consequences of tail injury on larvae of

the Oriental Fire-Bellied Toad, Bombina orientalis. Copeia, 1992, 129-137.

Pearman, P.B. (1995). Effects of pond size and consequent predator density on two species of

tadpoles. Oecologia, 102, 1-8.

Relyea, R.A. (2001). Morphological and behavioral plasticity of larval anurans in response to

different predators. Ecology, 82, 523-540.

Semlitsch, R.D. (1990). Effects of body size, sibship, and tail injury on the susceptibility of

tadpoles to dragonfly predation. Canadian Journal of Zoology, 68, 1027-1030.

Semlitsch, R.D., & Reichling, S.B. (1989). Density-dependent injury in larval salamanders.

Oecologia, 81, 100-103.

Page 94: PREDATOR-PREY INTERACTIONS BETWEEN AN INVASIVE …

Chapter 2

76

Semlitsch, R.D., Scott, D.E., & Pechmann, J.H.K. (1988). Time and size at metamorphosis

related to adult fitness in Ambystoma talpoideum. Ecology, 69, 184-192.

Smith, D.C. (1987). Adult recruitment in chorus frogs: effects of size and date at

metamorphosis. Ecology, 68, 344-350.

Tejedo, M. (1993). Size-dependent vulnerability and behavioral responses of tadpoles of two

anuran species to beetle larvae predators. Herpetologica, 49, 287-294.

Teplitsky, C., Plenet, S., Léna, J.-P., Mermet, N., Malet, E., & Joly, P. (2005). Escape behaviour

and ultimate causes of specific induced defences in an anuran tadpole. Journal of

Evolutionary Biology, 18, 180-190.

Van Buskirk, J., Anderwald, P., Lupold, S., Reinhardt, L., & Schuler, H. (2003). The lure effect,

tadpole tail shape, and the target of dragonfly strikes. Journal of Herpetology, 37, 420-

424.

Van Buskirk, J., & McCollum, S.A. (2000). Influence of tail shape on tadpole swimming

performance. Journal of Experimental Biology, 203, 2149-2158.

Wassersug, R.J., & Sperry, D.G. (1977). The relationships of locomotion to differential

predation on Pseudacris Triseriata (Anura: Hylidae). Ecology, 58, 830-839.

Werner, E.E. (1986). Amphibian metamorphosis: growth rate, predation risk and the optimal

size at transformation. American Naturalist, 128, 319-341.

Werner, E.E., & Peacor, S.D. (2003). A review of trait-mediated indirect interactions in

ecological communities. Ecology, 84, 1083-1100.

Wilbur, H.M. (1980). Complex life cycles. Annual Review of Ecology and Systematics, 11, 67-93.

Wilbur, H.M., & Semlitsch, R.D. (1990). Ecological consequences of tail injury in Rana tadpoles.

Copeia, 1990, 18-24.

Wilson, R.S., Kraft, P.G., & Van Damme, R. (2005). Predator-specific changes in the morphology

and swimming performance of larval Rana lessonae. Functional Ecology, 19, 238-244.

Page 95: PREDATOR-PREY INTERACTIONS BETWEEN AN INVASIVE …

77

CHAPTER 3

Do anuran larvae respond behaviourally to chemical

cues from an invasive crayfish predator?

A community-wide study

Nunes AL, Richter-Boix A, Laurila A, Rebelo R

Oecologia (in review)

Page 96: PREDATOR-PREY INTERACTIONS BETWEEN AN INVASIVE …

78

Page 97: PREDATOR-PREY INTERACTIONS BETWEEN AN INVASIVE …

Chapter 3

79

Do anuran larvae respond behaviourally to chemical cues from an

invasive crayfish predator? A community-wide study

Abstract

Antipredator behaviour is an important fitness component in most animals. A co-evolutionary

history between predator and prey is important for prey to respond adaptively to predation

threats. When non-native predator species invade new areas, native prey may not recognise

them and lack effective antipredator defences. However, responses to novel predators can be

facilitated by chemical cues from the predators’ diet. The red swamp crayfish Procambarus

clarkii is a widespread invasive predator in the Southwest of the Iberian Peninsula, where it

preys upon native anuran tadpoles. In a laboratory experiment we studied behavioural

antipredator defences (alterations in activity level and spatial avoidance of predator) of nine

anurans in response to P. clarkii chemical cues and compared them with the defences towards

a native predator, the larval dragonfly Aeshna sp. To investigate how chemical cues from

consumed conspecifics shape the responses, we raised tadpoles with either a tadpole-fed or

starved crayfish or dragonfly larva, or in the absence of a predator. Six species significantly

altered behaviour in the presence of crayfish, and this was largely mediated by chemical cues

from consumed conspecifics. In the presence of dragonfly most species exhibited behavioural

defences and often these did not require the presence of cues from predation events.

Responding to cues from consumed conspecifics seems to be a critical factor in facilitating

certain behavioural responses to novel exotic predators; this finding can be useful for

predicting antipredator responses to invasive predators and help directing conservation efforts

to the species at highest risk.

Keywords: Tadpole, activity level, spatial avoidance, behavioural plasticity, exotic predator

Page 98: PREDATOR-PREY INTERACTIONS BETWEEN AN INVASIVE …

Chapter 3

80

Introduction

Behaviour plays a decisive role in shaping the outcome of predator-prey interactions (Lima and

Dill 1990, Lima 1998, Ferrari et al. 2010). Predation is an important selective force acting on

the behaviour of prey species and, in order to minimise predation risk, many organisms have

evolved a variety of predator-avoidance behavioural defences (reviews in Lima and Dill 1990,

Kats and Dill 1998). A decrease in activity level is one of the most common and effective

behavioural antipredator responses and decreases vulnerability to predation (Lima and Dill

1990, Kats and Dill 1998). Spatial avoidance of predators also acts as an antipredator defence

by reducing the rate of predator-prey encounters and, consequently, predation risk (Laurila et

al. 1997, Relyea 2001, Nicieza et al. 2006). However, these behavioural shifts often incur costs

to animals due to reduced resource acquisition, which can alter growth, development and life-

history patterns (Werner and Anholt 1996, Lima 1998, but see Steiner 2007). Therefore, there

should be strong selection on prey to recognise dangerous predators, to accurately determine

predation risk and to adjust antipredator responses accordingly.

Invasive non-native predators are a worldwide threat to biodiversity (Blackburn et al. 2010).

Freshwater ecosystems are amongst the most invaded and particularly vulnerable to

introduced predators (Lodge et al. 1998, Cox and Lima 2006). For instance, many amphibian

population declines have been associated with the introduction of exotic aquatic predators

(review in Kats and Ferrer 2003). When predators invade areas outside their historical

geographic range, the lack of a common evolutionary history with native prey species often

renders the latter more prone to suffering heavy predation (Cox and Lima 2006, Gall and

Mathis 2010). This is because the evolutionary naïveté of prey may either cause a failure to

recognise and respond to novel predation threats, or result in inappropriate or ineffective

antipredator defences (Cox and Lima 2006, Strauss et al. 2006, Sih et al. 2010). The degree of

naïveté can depend on the similarity of the invader to native predators, since phylogenetic

Page 99: PREDATOR-PREY INTERACTIONS BETWEEN AN INVASIVE …

Chapter 3

81

relatedness is often associated with ecological similarity (Cox and Lima 2006, Sih et al. 2010).

For instance, more distantly related aquatic predator species are likely to have more dissimilar

chemical signatures. Hence, a novel predator with no related species in the native community

may pose a higher threat, as it is more unlikely to be recognised as dangerous by native prey

(Strauss et al. 2006, Gall and Mathis 2010, Sih et al. 2010). Several studies have reported a lack

of native prey responses to aquatic invasive predators, which may cause profound changes in

the invaded ecosystems. A classic example is the introduction of the Nile perch (Lates niloticus)

in Lake Victoria, which caused a severe decline in native cichlids, probably due to lack of

predator recognition (Witte et al. 1992).

In aquatic ecosystems, predator chemical cues are particularly important for prey in assessing

predation risk (Kats and Dill 1998). The chemicals to which prey respond may be predator-

specific odours, cues that are actively or passively released by injured or consumed

conspecifics or, more frequently, a combination of both (Chivers and Smith 1998, Schoeppner

and Relyea 2009, Fraker et al. 2009, Hettyey et al. 2010). Several studies have shown that fed

predators commonly elicit strong antipredator defences while starved predators often do not

(Stirling 1995, Slusarczk 1999, Schoeppner and Relyea 2005, 2009, but see Petranka and Hayes

1998, Van Buskirk and Arioli 2002). Furthermore, prey responses often depend on the

phylogenetic distance between consumed and responding prey, consumed conspecifics and

individuals of closely related species eliciting a stronger response (e.g. Kats and Dill 1998,

Laurila et al. 1998, Schoeppner and Relyea 2005, 2009). As a predator may become chemically

‘labelled’ by its diet, recognition of a novel predator can be facilitated if diet chemical cues are

associated with it (reviewed in Ferrari et al. 2010). However, studies on prey responses to

invasive predators generally do not take into account the role of chemical cues originating

from consumed conspecifics and their potential importance on enabling predator recognition

(but see Marquis et al. 2004).

Page 100: PREDATOR-PREY INTERACTIONS BETWEEN AN INVASIVE …

Chapter 3

82

Freshwater crayfishes, which have been widely introduced outside their native range, can have

major impacts in native prey species and cause severe population declines (Hobbs et al. 1989,

Lodge et al. 1998, Cox and Lima 2006, Larson and Olden 2010). In the southwest of the Iberian

Peninsula, most freshwater habitats have been invaded by Procambarus clarkii (red swamp

crayfish), an exotic crayfish endemic to northeastern Mexico and southcentral USA (Hobbs et

al. 1989, Gherardi 2006). This crayfish was introduced in Spain in 1973 and by the 1990s it was

already abundant in southwestern Portugal, an area previously devoid of freshwater crayfishes

or functionally similar species (Habsburgo-Lorena 1983, Almaça 1991). This area holds a

remarkable endemic anuran community: out of nine species three are endemic to the Iberian

Peninsula and three others have a restricted distribution outside Portugal and Spain (Gasc et

al. 1997). P. clarkii readily preys upon eggs and larvae of all these anurans (Cruz and Rebelo

2005). Due to its high fecundity and rapid growth, it can quickly build extremely large

populations, especially in fish-free habitats, and become a serious threat to amphibian

populations (Gherardi 2006, Larson and Olden 2010).

The ability of anuran tadpoles to detect and respond to P. clarkii is largely unknown, despite

the importance of this information for understanding the extent to which native amphibians

are able to cope with this invasive predator. In this study, our main goal was to understand if

anuran tadpoles from a community in southwest Iberian Peninsula invaded by P. clarkii ca. 25

years ago are able to exhibit behavioural defences in the presence of chemical cues from this

crayfish. For this, we performed a laboratory experiment in which we assessed changes in

tadpole activity level and spatial avoidance of the predator in the presence of chemical cues

from the invasive crayfish and compared them with those elicited in the presence of a

common native predator, the larval dragonfly Aeshna sp. We also investigated the role of

chemical cues from consumed conspecifics by comparing behavioural responses in the

presence of starved and conspecific-fed predators. A previous study has shown that many of

our study species may change their behaviour under direct predation risk by P. clarkii (Cruz

Page 101: PREDATOR-PREY INTERACTIONS BETWEEN AN INVASIVE …

Chapter 3

83

and Rebelo 2005), but no study has compared these responses to those elicited in the

presence of a native predator, nor investigated the role of predator chemical cues or cues from

predated conspecifics in it. Finally, we evaluated how these behavioural responses changed

during tadpoles’ ontogeny.

The anuran community in Southwestern Portugal consists of nine species: Iberian water frog

(Pelophylax perezi), Mediterranean tree frog (Hyla meridionalis), European tree frog (Hyla

arborea), common toad (Bufo bufo), natterjack toad (Bufo calamita), Iberian parsley frog

(Pelodytes ibericus), Western spadefoot toad (Pelobates cultripes), Iberian painted frog

(Discoglossus galganoi) and Iberian midwife toad (Alytes cisternasii). Except the two Bufo

species, these species (or very closely related species) show behavioural plasticity in presence

of caged Aeshnid dragonflies (Van Buskirk 2002, Nicieza et al. 2006, Richter-Boix et al. 2007,

Gomez-Mestre and Díaz-Paniagua 2011), and we expect them to show behavioural responses

in the presence of this native predator. In P. cultripes behavioural responses to Aeshnids have

not been studied before; we predict that behavioural defences may be unnecessary, because

the large body size of these tadpoles acts as an important antipredator mechanism (Tejedo

1993). Concerning responses to the exotic crayfish, we predict most of the species to not

detect and respond to this novel predator; however, chemical cues from consumed

conspecifics may ease these responses. Cruz and Rebelo (2005) have shown that these anuran

species differ in vulnerability to predation by P. clarkii, the two Bufo species, P. cultripes and D.

galganoi being the most susceptible species. This may indicate that these species lack

appropriate defences towards this crayfish. Finally, we predict antipredator defences to be

strongest in the beginning of tadpole development because prey vulnerability and responses

to predators decrease as prey size increases (e.g. Eklöv and Werner 2000; Hettyey et al. 2010).

Page 102: PREDATOR-PREY INTERACTIONS BETWEEN AN INVASIVE …

Chapter 3

84

Materials and methods

Study species and their maintenance

This study was conducted in the surroundings of the Sado River basin (SW Portugal), an area

that was colonised by P. clarkii in the 1980s (R. Rebelo pers. obs.). In this area, P. clarkii can be

found in all types of water bodies (e.g. streams, temporary ponds), including small and shallow

ones (Cruz and Rebelo 2007). Aeshna larvae are voracious native predators of tadpoles, widely

abundant in water bodies throughout this area. All anuran species included in this study co-

occur with both these predators.

The experiment took place in a laboratory of the field station of Centro de Biologia Ambiental

(Grândola, southwest Portugal, 38º06.482’N, 8º34.140’W), from 15th December 2007 to 15th

November 2008. This extended experimental period was due to differences in breeding

phenology of the study species. Several egg masses of each species were collected from ponds

located in Alentejo region, southwest Portugal (Table 1). In A. cisternasii, males show parental

care until tadpoles become free-swimming, and the tadpoles used in the experiment had been

very recently released in the ponds. All the ponds had established crayfish populations, with

evidence of crayfish reproduction (juveniles or brooding females). Clutches were kept in

several species specific 5L aquaria filled with spring water from the field station, until tadpoles

reached Gosner stage 25 (operculum closure over gills; Gosner 1960). Larvae were fed

commercial fish food and boiled lettuce ad libitum every two days. Throughout the experiment

water was changed every 5 days. Water temperature was 18.0 ± 0.17 ºC (mean ± SE) and the

photoperiod was 12L: 12D.

Predators were collected from local streams or ponds. Adult crayfish were captured using

baited funnel traps and late instar dragonfly larvae using dip-nets. Predators were transferred

to the laboratory and kept either in 40L aquaria (crayfish) or individually in 1.2L plastic boxes

Page 103: PREDATOR-PREY INTERACTIONS BETWEEN AN INVASIVE …

Chapter 3

85

(dragonflies). We fed crayfish with commercial fish food and small invertebrates, and

dragonflies with tadpoles and Ephemeroptera larvae.

Table 1. Number of clutches collected, collection site (type of water body and coordinates), date of their

collection and dates of start and end of the experiment for each of the nine studied species

Species # Clutches collected

Collection site Coordinates Date of

collection

Start of experiment

t

End of experiment

Pelophylax perezi

11 Grândola (stream) 38º07’N, 8º34’W 16-Jun 02-Jul 09-Nov

Hyla meridionalis

9 Grândola (temporary

ponds) 38º06’N, 8º34’W 13-Mar 08-Apr 15-Aug

Hyla arborea 10 Melides (rice field) 38º08’N, 8º44’W 05-Jun 17-Jun 25-Sep

Bufo bufo 6 Grândola (stream) 38º09’N, 8º39’W 07-Feb 19-Feb 04-Jun

Bufo calamita 9 Grândola (temporary/

ephemeral ponds) 38º05’N, 8º35’W 22-Apr 19-May 09-Aug

Pelodytes ibericus

10 Grândola (temporary

ponds) 38º06’N, 8º33’W 26-Dec 23-Jan 22-Jun

Pelobates cultripes

8 Grândola (temporary

ponds) 38º09’N, 8º31’W 28-Nov 15-Dec 05-Sep

Discoglossus galganoi

9 Grândola (ephemeral

ponds) 38º06’N, 8º34’W 08-Jan 24-Jan 13-May

Alytes cisternasii

- Grândola (stream) 38º05’N, 8º33’W 04-Dec 15-Dec 28-Jul

Experimental setup

We performed a factorial experiment using the nine anuran species and five predator

treatments, each combination being replicated five times. This resulted in a total of 225

experimental units, each consisting of a plastic tank (39 × 28 × 28cm) filled with 10L of water.

At the start of the experiment (day 0), ten tadpoles at developmental stage 25 and a predator

cage were added to each tank. The five predator treatments were: fed crayfish, fed dragonfly,

starved crayfish, starved dragonfly and control (no predator). The predators were placed in the

cages one day after the tadpoles. A single predator was placed in a cylindrical opaque floating

Page 104: PREDATOR-PREY INTERACTIONS BETWEEN AN INVASIVE …

Chapter 3

86

cage, which sides were covered with fine mesh netting allowing chemical cues to flow out of

the cage. Only chemical predator cues (no visual or tactile cues) were available for the

tadpoles. Crayfish and dragonfly cages had a diameter of 85 and 62 mm, respectively. In the

control treatments, we randomly used empty predator cages from one of the two sizes. While

inside the predator cage in each experimental container, predators from the ‘fed’ treatments

were fed three focal species tadpoles every other day. Starved predators were not fed during

the experiment. Prior to entering the experiment all predators were starved for, at least, 48

hours. All the predators were replaced every two weeks so that the starved predators could be

fed.

Response variables and statistical analyses

We recorded tadpole behaviour (activity levels and spatial avoidance) in the beginning (time

period 1), middle (time period 2) and close to the end (time period 3) of larval development.

As the length of larval period differs among different species, the dates of behavioural

observations - as well as headbody size and Gosner developmental stage (Gosner 1960) at

each date - also varied (Table 2). On each observation day, tadpole activity and spatial

avoidance were estimated by counting the number of active tadpoles and the number of

tadpoles close to the predator cage in each tank at five repeated occasions between 8 and 12

a.m., separated by at least 30 minutes. A tadpole was considered active when it was actively

swimming (either slowly or with fast bursts of speed), feeding (even if not substantially altering

position) or simply undulating its tail (without actively swimming). Tadpoles were considered

to be close to the predator cage if they were either in physical contact with the cage or not

more than 1cm away from it.

Page 105: PREDATOR-PREY INTERACTIONS BETWEEN AN INVASIVE …

Chapter 3

87

Table 2. Days of the experiment, Gosner developmental stage (according to Gosner 1960) and headbody

size when behaviour was sampled in the three time periods (early, middle and late in development) for

each species

Species Day of the experiment Median Gosner stage Average headbody size

Time 1 Time 2 Time 3 Time 1 Time 2 Time 3 Time 1 Time 2 Time 3

Pelophylax perezi 11 41 63 25 33 40 5.26 12.56 15.90

Hyla meridionalis 8 42 57 25 32 40 5.53 10.14 11.35

Hyla arborea 8 36 50 25 30 41 5.41 8.06 11.11

Bufo bufo 8 31 51 25 34 41 5.82 9.11 10.71

Bufo calamita 9 29 37 26 34 42 4.03 6.99 8.73

Pelodytes ibericus 14 27 49 25 32 39 5.61 11.11 13.01

Pelobates cultripes 13 123 199 25 30 41 5.63 14.77 23.66

Discoglossus galganoi

14 34 40 27 35 42 4.40 9.35 10.01

Alytes cisternasii 13 59 89 25 30 39 6.83 12.49 13.91

When making comparisons across taxa, there is a need to examine whether the phenotypic

traits in focus are correlated with the phylogenetic history of the focal species. Since nine

different anuran species were used in this study, we followed Abouheif’s (1999)

recommendation of testing for phylogenetic trait independence prior to analysing our data. If

no significant phylogenetic autocorrelation is detected among species (failure to reject the null

hypothesis of phylogenetic independence), phylogenetically comparative methods do not have

to be used and conventional statistical analyses can be applied (Abouheif 1999). To test for the

assumption of phylogenetic independence within our data set we first calculated, for each

species and predator treatment separately, the extent of predator-induced behavioural

plasticity. This was estimated as the proportional change in either activity level or spatial

avoidance in each of the four predator environments relative to the no predator environment

([behaviour in predator presence – behaviour in predator absence]/ behaviour in predator

absence; Richardson 2001, Van Buskirk 2002). Using these values, a Test For Serial

Independence (TFSI) was performed for each treatment, using the program “Phylogenetic

Independence, Version 2.0” (Abouheif 1999, Reeve and Abouheif 2003). The phylogenetic tree

used was constructed based on Duarte et al. (2011) (Fig. 1). Phylogenetic autocorrelation was

Page 106: PREDATOR-PREY INTERACTIONS BETWEEN AN INVASIVE …

Chapter 3

88

calculated in the form of a C‐statistics and topology of the original data was randomised

10.000 times to estimate the null hypothesis.

Fig. 1 Assumed phylogenetic relationships (based on Duarte et al. 2011) for the nine anuran species

used in this experiment: Pelophylax perezi, Hyla meridionalis, Hyla arborea, Bufo calamita, Bufo bufo,

Pelodytes ibericus, Pelobates cultripes, Discoglossus galganoi and Alytes cisternasii

Since for all predator treatments we accepted the null hypothesis of phylogenetic

independence in behavioural plasticity in both traits (see results), subsequent statistical

analyses did not need to be phylogenetically corrected. To examine differences in species

behavioural responses to the exotic and the native predators over their development, we used

Generalized Linear Models with a Binomial error distribution and a logit link function. As

behaviour was recorded in three different time periods, a repeated measures approach was

used to assess the effect of time on species responses. The total number of active tadpoles or

tadpoles close to the predator cage per container pooled over the five observations in each

time period was used as the dependent variable and the total number of tadpoles inside a

container pooled over the five observations in each time period was treated as a trials

Page 107: PREDATOR-PREY INTERACTIONS BETWEEN AN INVASIVE …

Chapter 3

89

variable. We started by performing an analysis at the community level, comparing responses

of all the studied species in the different predator treatments. Subsequently we looked at a

species-specific level to help identify more precisely antipredator responses of each species.

Since most individual species showed a significant interaction between time period and

predator treatment (see results), we also performed separate analyses for species in each time

period in order to investigate responses over time in more detail. Multiple planned

comparisons between predator treatments were performed using Bonferroni corrections.

These comparisons were chosen in order to answer the questions which species are

responding to the native or the invasive predators (control treatment vs. all others), do cues

from consumed conspecifics play a role in this (fed vs. starved treatments) and how similar is

the response to the two predators (fed dragonfly vs. fed crayfish and starved dragonfly vs.

starved crayfish). Generalized Linear Mixed models using block as a random factor were

initially performed, but as this factor was never significant for the models, it was discarded

from the definitive analyses.

In order to understand if species responding more to the native predator also respond more to

the exotic predator and if these responses are mediated by cues from consumed conspecifics,

Pearson correlations between predator-induced behavioural plasticity in the two starved and

in the two fed predator treatments were calculated. We used data from time period 1 as this

was the period when the strongest responses were observed. If species antipredator

responses are mainly mediated by consumed conspecifics cues, we expect to find a positive

correlation between the two fed predator treatments and no correlation between the two

starved treatments. If, alternatively, species require the presence of predator-specific cues to

respond, we expect a strong response to the dragonfly and a weak or no response to the

crayfish (due to the lack of recognition of crayfish kairomones), resulting in either a negative or

no correlation between the two fed and the two starved treatments. To investigate which

amount of predator-induced behavioural plasticity in the fed treatments was due to the

Page 108: PREDATOR-PREY INTERACTIONS BETWEEN AN INVASIVE …

Chapter 3

90

presence of cues from consumed conspecifics (hereafter plasticity due to CC cues) we

estimated, for each predator and species, the proportional change in activity level or spatial

avoidance in the fed predator treatment relative to the starved predator treatment

([behaviour in presence of fed predator – behaviour in presence of starved predator]/

behaviour in presence of starved predator). We then performed a correlation between the

plasticity due to the CC cues in the presence of dragonfly and crayfish. If all plasticity is

attributed to the CC cues, plasticity should be the same in presence of the two predators and a

close to perfect correlation should be observed, since the number of tadpoles fed to the two

different predators was equal.

Results

We did not find a significant phylogenetic autocorrelation for behavioural plasticity in activity

level (C = 0.177, P = 0.22 for the fed dragonfly treatment, C = 0.125, P = 0.27 for the fed

crayfish treatment, C = 0.040, P = 0.41 for the starved dragonfly treatment, C = 0.077, P = 0.32

for the starved crayfish treatment) and spatial avoidance (C = 0.114, P = 0.27 for the fed

dragonfly treatment, C = 0.353, P = 0.09 for the fed crayfish treatment, C = 0.141, P = 0.24 for

the starved dragonfly treatment, C = 0.015, P = 0.52 for the starved crayfish treatment) of the

nine species in any of the four different predator treatments, suggesting no association

between behavioural plasticity and species phylogenetic history.

When comparing behaviour among the nine anuran species, both activity level and spatial

avoidance varied across time periods (Wald χ2 = 106.28, df = 2, 538, P < 0.001 for activity level;

Wald χ2 = 51.05, df = 2, 538, P < 0.001 for spatial avoidance). Further, a significant interaction

between time period and predator treatment indicates complex behavioural responses of

tadpoles to different predator regimes throughout their development (Wald χ2 = 28.91, df = 8,

538, P < 0.001 for activity level; Wald χ2 = 143.73, df = 8, 538, P < 0.001 for spatial avoidance).

Page 109: PREDATOR-PREY INTERACTIONS BETWEEN AN INVASIVE …

Chapter 3

91

When analysing behavioural responses to predators, we always refer to comparisons between

each predator and the no-predator (control) treatments, unless otherwise noted.

When looking at species separately, time period was an important factor in determining

activity level in most of the species. B. bufo, H. meridionalis, P. cultripes, H. arborea, B.

calamita and P. perezi increased their overall activity with time, whereas D. galganoi

decreased activity (in all analyses, P < 0.001 for the factor ‘time period’; Fig. 2). Only A.

cisternasii and P. ibericus did not show an alteration of activity with time (P > 0.06; Fig. 2). A

significant interaction between time period and predator treatment was found for all species

except B. calamita and P. ibericus, indicating that alterations in behavioural responses to

predators throughout development were common (for all analyses, P < 0.05; Fig. 2). For B.

calamita no differences were detected between predator treatments (Wald χ2 = 8.924, df = 4,

60, P = 0.063), while P. ibericus reduced activity in the presence of dragonfly, with a stronger

response to the fed than to the starved predator (Wald χ2 = 51.179, df = 4, 60, P < 0.001; Fig.

2).

In species which behavioural responses varied over time, dragonfly presence (together with

predated conspecifics) strongly affected tadpole activity level, with reduced activity in all time

periods in all species except P. cultripes and P. perezi (Tables 3, 4, Fig. 2). B. bufo was the only

species showing a continuous and similar in magnitude response to both dragonfly (fed and

unfed) and crayfish (fed) predators, this response becoming stronger with developmental time

(Tables 3, 4, Fig. 2). An initial significant response to the fed crayfish and a later response to

the starved dragonfly arose in A. cisternasii, while in D. galganoi these responses appeared

later in development and were stronger in the fed treatments. Both hylid species were less

active in the presence of the crayfish predator late in their development, either when the

predator was fed (H. meridionalis) or unfed (H. arborea) (Tables 3, 4, Fig. 2). For P. cultripes we

found an initial response of reduced activity to both fed predators, the response to the crayfish

being weaker than to the dragonfly, which later disappeared. For P. perezi, early in ontogeny,

Page 110: PREDATOR-PREY INTERACTIONS BETWEEN AN INVASIVE …

Chapter 3

92

tadpoles from the fed dragonfly treatment reduced activity, while the ones from the starved

dragonfly treatment showed a significantly higher proportion of active tadpoles (Tables 3, 4,

Fig. 2).

Fig. 2 Proportion of active tadpoles (mean ± SE) of the nine anuran species, in presence of five predator

treatments, in three different time periods

Page 111: PREDATOR-PREY INTERACTIONS BETWEEN AN INVASIVE …

Chapter 3

93

Table 3. Generalized Linear Model for the activity level and spatial avoidance of the nine anuran species when subjected to different predator treatments, in

each of the three time periods. The analyses were not performed for P. ibericus because it did not show a significant interaction between predator treatment

and time period for any of the traits. P-values < 0.05 are marked in boldface.

Activity level Spatial avoidance

Time 1 Time 2 Time 3 Time 1 Time 2 Time 3

Wald χ2 P Wald χ

2 P Wald χ

2 P Wald χ

2 P Wald χ

2 P Wald χ

2 P

P. perezi 32.086 <0.001 11.515 0.021 14.014 0.007 7.096 0.131 1.854 0.763 10.647 0.031

H. meridionalis 31.937 <0.001 18.023 0.001 38.538 <0.001 42.314 <0.001 35.606 <0.001 8.021 0.091

H. arborea 19.614 0.001 21.778 <0.001 13.538 0.009 - - - - - -

B. bufo 51.938 <0.001 75.177 <0.001 134.372 <0.001 45.998 <0.001 11.381 0.023 27.548 <0.001

B. calamita - - - - - - 0.001 1.000 7.082 0.132 8.601 0.072

P. cultripes 31.154 <0.001 22.096 <0.001 2.213 0.697 8.367 0.079 13.724 0.008 13.678 0.008

D. galganoi 59.337 <0.001 65.994 <0.001 50.534 <0.001 19.707 0.001 21.666 <0.001 14.307 0.006

A. cisternasii 34.349 <0.001 33.652 <0.001 23.999 <0.001 24.084 <0.001 46.338 <0.001 51.320 <0.001

Page 112: PREDATOR-PREY INTERACTIONS BETWEEN AN INVASIVE …

Chapter 3

94

Table 4. Results of planned comparisons between predator treatments for tadpole activity level and spatial avoidance after analysis shown in Table 3.

Results are not shown for P. ibericus because an analysis per period was not performed (see Results) and for B. calamita because no significant differences

were found. Significant values after Bonferroni correction (alpha level set to 0.006) are marked with an asterisk. ‘Drag’ stands for dragonfly and ‘Crayf’ for

crayfish.

P. perezi H. meridionalis H. arborea B. bufo P. cultripes D. galganoi A. cisternasii

Time period 1 2 3 1 2 3 1 2 3 1 2 3 1 2 3 1 2 3 1 2 3

Activity level

Control-Fed Drag * * * * * * * * * * * * * * * *

Control-Starved Drag * * * * * * * *

Control-Fed Crayf * * * * * * * *

Control-Starved Crayf * *

Fed Drag-Fed Crayfish * * * * * * * * * * *

Starved Drag-Starved Crayf * * * * * * * *

Fed Drag-Starved Drag * * * * * * * * * * *

Fed Crayf-Starved Crayf * * * * * *

Spatial avoidance

Control-Fed Drag * * * * * * * * * * *

Control-Starved Drag * * * * * * * *

Control-Fed Crayf * * * * * * *

Control-Starved Crayf * *

Fed Drag-Fed Crayfish * * * * * *

Starved Drag-Starved Crayf * * * * *

Fed Drag-Starved Drag * *

Fed Crayf-Starved Crayf * * *

Page 113: PREDATOR-PREY INTERACTIONS BETWEEN AN INVASIVE …

Chapter 3

95

When looking at species-specific spatial avoidance, A. cisternasii, P. ibericus, B. bufo, H.

arborea and P. perezi did not alter the degree of predator spatial avoidance with time (in all

analyses, P > 0.2 for the factor ‘time period’; Fig. 3). Once again, a significant interaction

between time period and predator treatment was found for all species except H. arborea and

P. ibericus, indicating alterations in antipredator responses through ontogeny (for all analyses,

P < 0.05; Fig. 3). Both H. arborea (Wald χ2 = 12.916, df = 4, 60, P = 0.012; Fig. 3) and P. ibericus

(Wald χ2 = 31.907, df = 4, 60, P < 0.001; Fig. 3) avoided the predator cage in the presence of fed

dragonfly.

Fig. 3 Proportion of tadpoles near the predator cage (mean ± SE) of the nine anuran species, in presence

of five predator treatments, in three different time periods

Page 114: PREDATOR-PREY INTERACTIONS BETWEEN AN INVASIVE …

Chapter 3

96

In each time period, all significant responses of spatial avoidance consisted of moving away

from the predator cage. Tadpoles of A. cisternasii showed continuous and strong avoidance

behaviour throughout development to both predators (both fed and starved). This was the

only species avoiding the predator cage in the starved crayfish treatment (Tables 3, 4, Fig. 3).

D. galganoi, B. bufo and H. meridionalis strongly avoided the cage with the dragonfly predator

(fed and unfed), and both D. galganoi (time periods 1 and 3) and B. bufo (period 1) also

avoided the fed crayfish predator as strongly as they avoided the native predator. Tadpoles of

P. cultripes avoided the cage with fed crayfish in time period 2 (Tables 3, 4, Fig. 3).

No correlation was detected between species-level plasticity in activity level in the starved

dragonfly and starved crayfish treatments (r = 0.269, N = 9, P = 0.484; Fig. 4a). However, there

was a significant positive correlation between species plasticity in the fed dragonfly and fed

crayfish treatments (r = 0.717, N = 9, P = 0.03; Fig. 4b). A positive correlation was also found

between the two predator treatments for plasticity in activity attributed to the CC cues (r =

0.699, N = 9, P = 0.036; Fig. 4c), suggesting that the correlation between plasticity in the two

fed treatments was due to among-species variation in plasticity to CC cues. Concerning

behavioural plasticity in spatial avoidance of the predator, no significant correlations were

found between fed (r = 0.317, N = 9, P = 0.406) or starved treatments (r = 0.09, N = 9, P =

0.889), or for plasticity to the CC cues (r = 0.039, N = 9, P = 0.920).

Discussion

This study provides the first community-wide assessment of antipredator behaviour of anuran

larvae in response to chemical cues of the invasive crayfish P. clarkii, the most cosmopolitan

crayfish species in the world (Gherardi 2006). Even though P. clarkii was relatively recently

introduced in our study area, we found that six out of nine prey species responded

Page 115: PREDATOR-PREY INTERACTIONS BETWEEN AN INVASIVE …

Chapter 3

97

behaviourally to this predator. As expected, these responses were greatly mediated by

chemical cues resulting from predation events.

Fig. 4 Relationship between behavioural plasticity in activity level of the nine anuran species in the

presence of dragonfly and crayfish, in time period 1: a) plasticity in presence of the starved predators:

for each predator axis, ([activity with starved predator present – activity with predator absent]/activity

with predator absent); b) plasticity in presence of the fed predators: for each predator axis, ([activity

with fed predator present – activity with predator absent]/activity with predator absent); c) plasticity

attributed to the consumed conspecifics chemical cues: for each predator axis, ([activity with fed

predator present – activity with starved predator present]/activity with starved predator present). The

grey line in graph c) represents the expected relationship if species respond to diet cues exactly in the

same way in the dragonfly and crayfish treatments. See figure 1 for species names abbreviations.

Plasticity values below zero indicate a decrease in activity in the presence of predators and positive

plasticity values reflect an increase in activity with predators present

Behavioural responses to the native and introduced predators

In the presence of dragonfly, six species had a strong behavioural response (A. cisternasii, D.

galganoi, B. bufo, H. meridionalis, H. arborea and P. ibericus), two had a moderate response (P.

cultripes and P. perezi), while one species had no significant response (B. calamita). In general,

activity levels were greatly reduced and strong spatial avoidance of the predator was observed

in the presence of the native predator. These are common and usually effective antipredator

responses (reviews in Lima and Dill 1990, Kats and Dill 1998), often reported for larval

Page 116: PREDATOR-PREY INTERACTIONS BETWEEN AN INVASIVE …

Chapter 3

98

amphibians exposed to odonate predators (e.g. Werner and Anholt 1996, Relyea 2001,

Richardson 2001, Nicieza et al. 2006). The strong responses observed here indicate that, as

expected, most of these anuran species do have the potential of exhibiting behavioural

strategies to avoid predation. The significant behavioural response of B. bufo to the dragonfly

was somewhat unexpected, since most studies report a lack of behavioural antipredator

responses in this species (e.g. Laurila et al. 1997, Richter-Boix et al. 2007). However, although

bufonids are known to rely on chemical toxic deterrents as defences against predators, several

invertebrate predators (including P. clarkii) are resistant to them, probably inducing tadpoles

to use alternative defences (Semlitsch and Gavasso 1992, Cruz and Rebelo 2005). Laurila et al.

(1998) also reported a weak decrease in B. bufo activity in the presence of a dragonfly.

We found that six tadpole species (A. cisternasii, B. bufo, D. galganoi, H. meridionalis, H.

arborea and P. cultripes) significantly altered activity level and/or showed spatial avoidance in

the presence of P. clarkii. Although these responses were generally weaker than the ones

elicited towards the native predator, this indicates that the ability to respond to the invasive

crayfish is quite pervasive in this amphibian community. These results are in line with previous

studies that have shown amphibians to alter behaviour as a response to chemical cues of non-

native predators, such as P. clarkii (e.g. Pearl et al. 2003, Marquis et al. 2004, Gall and Mathis

2010). Pearl et al. (2003) found different responses in two native anurans to cues from an

introduced predator which, together with our study, show that species from the same

community may commonly respond differently to exotic predators.

Differences in the ability of prey species to respond behaviourally to the crayfish could have

arisen due to several aspects of species ecology, such as their habitat use or body size (and

associated swimming abilities). Anuran species face different selective environments along the

pond permanency gradient, ranging from permanent lakes and ponds to ephemeral pools

(Wellborn et al. 1996). Since permanent water bodies have a higher predator abundance and

diversity than ephemeral ones, species using the former habitats (P. perezi, H. arborea, B. bufo

Page 117: PREDATOR-PREY INTERACTIONS BETWEEN AN INVASIVE …

Chapter 3

99

and A. cisternasii in our case) are likely to have more generalized (as opposed to predator-

specific) antipredator defences that allow responses to a wide array of predators, likely

facilitating responses to novel invasive predators (Richter-Boix et al. 2007, Smith and Awan

2009, Gómez and Kehr 2011). Indeed, in this study, three of the four species typically

inhabiting permanent water bodies responded to the exotic predator (A. cisternasii, B. bufo

and H. arborea). Nonetheless, P. perezi, also a permanent pond species, did not respond to the

crayfish, indicating that the hydroperiod gradient does not explain all the differences in species

antipredator responses. Alternatively, prey that have small body size and relatively weak

swimming ability, such as the bufonids and D. galganoi, should elicit antipredator defences in

the presence of many different predators. Two of these species (the exception being B.

calamita) indeed responded to the crayfish predator. However, species with larger body size

and good swimming ability, such as the hylids, also elicited behavioural responses to the exotic

crayfish. The evolutionary history of a species can also be important in defining differences in

species antipredator responses (Richardson 2001); however, we did not find any phylogenetic

autocorrelations in the behavioural responses of these species, indicating that evolutionary

history does not seem to play an important role here (see also Richter-Boix et al. 2007). On the

whole, since none of the previously mentioned factors entirely explains differences in

behavioural responses to the exotic predator, it is possible that all these factors interact to

define the final species-specific responses.

The display of behavioural responses by B. bufo, D. galganoi and P. cultripes towards P. clarkii

is surprising, since an earlier study found that these species were the most vulnerable to P.

clarkii predation (Cruz and Rebelo 2005). This might indicate that in Cruz and Rebelo’s study

tadpoles did not have enough time to develop behavioural defences against the crayfish (it

only lasted 48 hours) or, alternatively, that the behavioural responses shown here may be a

non-effective response towards this exotic predator. For example, because crayfish rely much

more on chemical rather than on visual cues to detect prey, decreased activity in response to

Page 118: PREDATOR-PREY INTERACTIONS BETWEEN AN INVASIVE …

Chapter 3

100

crayfish may render inactive tadpoles to an equal risk to that experienced by active ones

(Aquiloni et al. 2005).

The importance of chemical cues from consumed conspecifics

In this study, most species significantly changed behaviour in the presence of fed predators,

suggesting that cues from consumed conspecifics play a very important role in inducing anuran

behavioural responses. However, six out of nine species also altered behaviour when exposed

to chemical cues from the starved dragonfly indicating that predator-specific odours from a

native predator are, in many cases, sufficient to launch behavioural defences.

In the three species that responded strongly to the exotic predator (A. cisternasii, B. bufo and

D. galganoi), behavioural plasticity (considering activity level) to the fed crayfish was mostly

explained by plasticity to the CC cues (cues from consumed conspecifics), indicating that the

response to the exotic predator was mediated by cues resulting from predation events. These

results are in accordance with those by Marquis et al. (2004), who suggested the presence of

conspecific alarm cues to be crucial in chemical detection of predators by B. bufo. Injured

tadpoles of this species are known to release alarm cues - chemicals released by specialised

cells in the skin - that induce antipredator responses, such as a reduction in activity, in

conspecifics (Chivers and Smith 1998, Hagman 2008, Fraker et al. 2009). Responding to cues

from injured conspecifics is frequent among anuran species (Chivers and Smith 1998, Hagman

2008, Ferrari et al. 2010) and may also be the case for A. cisternasii and D. galganoi (S. Amaral,

M.J. Cruz and R. Rebelo, unpublished data). Responding to broad and general alarm cues

seems to be a critical factor in allowing tadpoles to elicit behavioural defences to novel

invasive predators and makes common evolutionary history between predator and prey

unnecessary for responses to be elicited (Sih et al. 2010).

Page 119: PREDATOR-PREY INTERACTIONS BETWEEN AN INVASIVE …

Chapter 3

101

Behavioural responses across ontogeny

As predicted, tadpoles of all species (except P. ibericus) showed different antipredator

responses to predator chemical cues over ontogeny. All the species responding to the native

predator elicited behavioural defences in the first developmental period, indicating an early

development of antipredator behaviour, which is in accordance with other studies (e.g.

Petranka and Hayes 1998, Laurila et al. 2004). However, not all these species elicited defences

in time period three, at times the antipredator response disappearing as the tadpoles grew

larger. This indicates that, as tadpole size increased, vulnerability to predation probably

decreased, reducing the importance of behavioural defences (e.g. Eklöv and Werner 2000,

Hettyey et al. 2010). In the case of P. cultripes, overall activity level greatly increased at the

same time as the antipredator response disappeared, suggesting that in this case large body

size and high activity levels are linked with decreased behavioural responses towards

predators. On the other hand, since predation risk did not increase with time in this

experiment, we cannot exclude the possibility that, in some of the species, tadpoles might

have stopped responding due to habituation (Magurran 1990).

Behavioural plasticity correlations

We found a positive correlation between predator-induced behavioural plasticity in activity

level in the presence of the fed native and exotic predators. Since there was no relationship

between plasticity in the two starved treatments, the former correlation was probably a result

of the correlation between plasticity to the CC cues (cues from consumed conspecifics) in the

two predator treatments. This reinforces the idea that the antipredator responses were greatly

mediated by chemical signals resulting from predation events upon conspecifics. Further, it

suggests that the magnitude of antipredator responses to native predators, when greatly

Page 120: PREDATOR-PREY INTERACTIONS BETWEEN AN INVASIVE …

Chapter 3

102

mediated by CC cues, can be a good proxy for predicting the potential antipredator responses

to novel predators in amphibians and possibly even in other species. Still, the correlation

between plasticity to the CC cues in the two predator treatments was not close to perfect, as

expected if all the observed plasticity was due to the CC cues. Instead, species plasticity to the

CC cues was stronger in the presence of dragonfly than in the presence of crayfish, indicating

that additional information from the predator contributed to an increased plastic response

when the dragonfly was present. Once again, there seems to be a difference in species

responses depending on whether they co-evolved for a long time with the predator or not.

The fact that we found no significant correlations between predator treatments for plasticity in

predator spatial avoidance was probably due to many species not altering this trait in predator

presence and to the wide variation in responses often observed (large standard errors). This

suggests that plasticity in this trait is less widespread among species from this community than

plasticity in activity level. Alternatively, it may also indicate that, for some species, chemical

cues are not enough to induce alterations in this behavioural trait, maybe because it implies

more risk or is more costly.

Conclusions

Since P. clarkii arrived to the southwest of Portugal in the 1980s, larval anurans have been

exposed to high predation pressure imposed by this exotic predator (Cruz and Rebelo 2005).

Our experiment showed that six out of nine anuran species present in this area showed

behavioural responses to this novel predator. We suggest that these species, provided the

behavioural responses are effective, will be better able to persist with the continued

expansion and establishment of this introduced predator. However, since failure to recognise

an exotic species as a predator is probably one of the most damaging forms of prey naiveté

(Cox and Lima 2006), the remaining three species - unless eliciting alternative defences - will

Page 121: PREDATOR-PREY INTERACTIONS BETWEEN AN INVASIVE …

Chapter 3

103

be highly vulnerable, which may lead to population declines or even local extinctions. Since

one of these species, P. ibericus, is an Iberian endemic with a very limited distribution area

(Loureiro et al. 2008), serious conservation problems may arise in the near future. Evaluating

which species rely on chemical cues from predation events in their responses to predators may

be a useful tool for understanding the potential of native prey species to respond to invasive

alien predators and help direct conservation efforts to the most susceptible species.

Acknowledgments

We thank Pedro Andrade, Erika Almeida, Susana Alves and Cátia Guerreiro for their invaluable

help in the field and experimental work, Hélder Duarte for providing data on species phylogeny

and Jesús Díaz-Rodríguez for providing an insight on the Pelodytes taxonomy. Permits were

provided by the Portuguese Instituto da Conservação da Natureza e da Biodiversidade (ICNB).

This research was funded by the FCT Project POCI/BIA-BDE/56100/2004, FCT grant

SFRH/BD/29068/2006 and by Stiftelsen för Zoologisk Forskning.

References

Abouheif E (1999) A method to test the assumption of phylogenetic independence in

comparative data. Evol Ecol Res 1: 895-909

Almaça C (1991) L’ecrevisse a pieds blancs, Astacus pallipes Lereboullet 1858, au Portugal.

L’Astaciculteur de France 28: 11-16

Aquiloni L, Ilhéu M, Gherardi F (2005) Habitat use and dispersal of the invasive crayfish

Procambarus clarkii in ephemeral water bodies of Portugal. Mar Freshw Behav Physiol

38: 225-236

Page 122: PREDATOR-PREY INTERACTIONS BETWEEN AN INVASIVE …

Chapter 3

104

Blackburn TM, Pettorelli N, Katzner T, Gompper ME, Mock K, Garner TWJ, Altwegg R, Redpath

S, Gordon IJ (2010) Dying for conservation: eradicating invasive alien species in the face

of opposition. Anim Conserv 13: 227-228

Chivers DP, Smith RJF (1998) Chemical alarm signaling in aquatic predator-prey systems: A

review and prospectus. Ecoscience 5: 338-352

Cox JG, Lima SL (2006) Naiveté and an aquatic-terrestrial dichotomy in the effects of

introduced predators. Trends Ecol Evol 21: 674-680

Cruz MJ, Rebelo R (2005) Vulnerability of southwest Iberian amphibians to an introduced

crayfish, Procambarus clarkii. Amphibia-Reptilia 26: 293-303

Cruz MJ, Rebelo R (2007) Colonization of freshwater habitats by an introduced crayfish,

Procambarus clarkii, in Southwest Iberian Peninsula. Hydrobiologia 575: 191-201

Duarte H, Tejedo M, Katzenberger M, Marangoni F, Baldo D, Beltrán JF, Martí DA, Richter-Boix

A, Gonzalez-Voyer A (2011) Can amphibians take the heat? Vulnerability to climate

warming in subtropical and temperate larval amphibian communities. Global Change

Biology, in press

Eklöv P, Werner EE (2000) Multiple predator effects on size-dependent behavior and mortality

of two species of anuran larvae. Oikos 88: 250-258

Ferrari MCO, Wisenden BD, Chivers DP (2010) Chemical ecology of predator-prey interactions

in aquatic ecosystems: a review and prospectus. Can J Zool 88: 698-724

Fraker M, Hu E, Cuddapah V, McCollum S, Relyea R, Hempel H, Denver R (2009)

Characterization of an alarm pheromone secreted by amphibian tadpoles that induces

behavioral inhibition and suppression of the neuroendocrine stress axis. Horm Behav 55:

520-529

Gall B G, Mathis A (2010) Innate predator recognition and the problem of introduced trout.

Ethology 116: 47-58

Page 123: PREDATOR-PREY INTERACTIONS BETWEEN AN INVASIVE …

Chapter 3

105

Gasc JP, Cabela A, Crnobrnja-Isailovic J, Dolmen D, Grossenbacher K, Haffner P, Lescure J,

Martens H, Martínez Rica JP, Maurin H, Oliveira ME, Sofianidou TS, Veith M, Zuiderwijk

A (1997) Atlas of amphibians and reptiles in Europe, Collection Patrimoines Naturels, 29,

SPN/IEGB/MNHN, Paris, 496 pp.

Gherardi F (2006) Crayfish invading Europe: the case study of Procambarus clarkii. Mar Freshw

Behav Physiol 39: 175-191

Gómez VI, Kehr AI (2011) Morphological and developmental responses of anuran larvae

(Physalaemus albonotatus) to chemical cues from the predators Moenkhausia

dichoroura (Characiformes: Characidae) and Belostoma elongatum (Hemiptera:

Belostomatidae). Zoolog Stud 50: 203-210

Gomez-Mestre I, Díaz-Paniagua C (2011) Invasive predatory crayfish do not trigger inducible

defences in tadpoles. Proc Royal Soc London B, in press

Gosner KL (1960) A simplified table for staging anuran embryos and larvae with notes on

identification. Herpetologica 16: 183-190

Habsburgo-Lorena AS (1983) Socioeconomic aspects of the crawfish industry in Spain. Freshw

Crayfish 5: 552-554

Hagman M (2008) Behavioral responses by tadpoles of six Australian species to chemical cues

from other tadpoles. Herpetol Conserv Biol 3: 239-246

Hettyey A, Zsarnóczai S, Vincze K, Hoi H, Laurila A (2010) Interactions between the information

content of different chemical cues affect induced defences in tadpoles. Oikos 119: 1814-

1822

Hobbs HH, Jass JP, Huner JV (1989) A review of global crayfish introductions with particular

emphasis on two North-American species (Decapoda, Cambaridae). Crustaceana 56:

299-316

Kats LB, Dill LM (1998) The scent of death: chemosensory assessment of predation risk by prey

animals. Ecoscience 5: 361-394

Page 124: PREDATOR-PREY INTERACTIONS BETWEEN AN INVASIVE …

Chapter 3

106

Kats LB, Ferrer RP (2003) Alien predators and amphibian declines: review of two decades of

science and the transition to conservation. Divers Distrib 9: 99-110

Larson ER, Olden JD (2010) Latent extinction and invasion risk of crayfishes in the southeastern

United States. Conserv Biol 24: 1099-1110

Laurila A, Kujasalo J, Ranta E (1997) Different antipredator behaviour in two anuran tadpoles:

effects of predator diet. Behav Ecol and Sociobiol 40: 329-336

Laurila A, Kujasalo J, Ranta E (1998) Predator-induced changes in life-history in two anuran

tadpoles: effects of predator diet. Oikos 83: 307-317

Laurila A, Järvi-Laturi M, Pakkasmaa S, Merilä J (2004) Temporal variation in predation risk:

stage-dependency, graded responses and fitness costs in tadpole antipredator defences.

Oikos 107: 90-99

Lima SL, Dill LM (1990) Behavioral decisions made under the risk of predation: A review and

prospectus. Can J Zool 68: 619-640

Lima SL (1998) Nonlethal effects in the ecology of predator-prey interactions: what are the

ecological effects of anti-predator decision-making? BioScience 48: 25-34

Lodge DM, Stein RA, Brown KM, Covich AP, Bronmark C, Garvey JE, Klosiewski SP (1998)

Predicting impact of freshwater exotic species on native biodiversity: Challenges in

spatial scaling. Aust J Ecol 23: 53-67

Loureiro A, Ferrand de Almeida N, Carretero MA, Paulo OS (eds.) (2008) Atlas dos Anfíbios e

Répteis de Portugal. Instituto da Conservação da Natureza e da Biodiversidade, Lisboa.

257 pp.

Magurran AE (1990) The adaptive significance of schooling as an anti-predator defence in fish.

Ann. Zool. Fennici 27: 3-18

Marquis O, Saglio P, Neveu A (2004) Effects of predators and conspecific chemical cues on the

swimming activity of Rana temporaria and Bufo bufo tadpoles. Arch Hydrobiol 160: 153-

170

Page 125: PREDATOR-PREY INTERACTIONS BETWEEN AN INVASIVE …

Chapter 3

107

Nicieza AG, Álvarez DA, Atienza EMS (2006) Delayed effects of larval predation risk and food

quality on anuran juvenile performance. J Evol Biol 19: 1092-1103

Pearl CA, Adams MJ, Schuytema GS, Nebeker AV (2003) Behavioral responses of anuran larvae

to chemical cues of native and introduced predators in the Pacific Northwestern United

States. J Herpetol 37: 572-576

Petranka JW, Hayes LJ (1998) Chemically mediated avoidance of a predatory odonate (Anax

junius) by American toad (Bufo americanus) and wood frog (Rana sylvatica) tadpoles.

Behav Ecol Sociobiol 42: 263-271

Reeve J, Abouheif E (2003) Phylogenetic Independence. Version 2.0. Computer Program

Relyea RA (2001) Morphological and behavioral plasticity of larval anurans in response to

different predators. Ecology 82: 523-540

Richardson JML (2001) A comparative study of activity levels in larval anurans and response to

the presence of different predators. Behav Ecol 12: 51-58

Richter-Boix A, Llorente GA, Montori A (2007) A comparative study of predator-induced

phenotype in tadpoles across a pond permanency gradient. Hydrobiologia 583: 43-56

Sánchez-Herráiz MJ, Barbadillo LJ, Machordom A, Sanchiz B (2000) A new species of pelodytid

from the Iberian Peninsula. Herpetologica 56: 105-118

Schoeppner NM, Relyea RA (2005) Damage, digestion, and defence: the roles of alarm cues

and kairomones for inducing prey defences. Ecol Lett 8: 505-512

Schoeppner NM, Relyea RA (2009) Interpreting the smells of predation: how alarm cues and

kairomones induce different prey defences. Funct Ecol 23: 1114-1121

Semlitsch RD, Gavasso S (1992) Behavioural responses of Bufo bufo and Bufo calamita tadpoles

to chemical cues of vertebrate and invertebrate predators. Ethol Ecol Evol 4: 165-173.

Sih A, Bolnick DI, Luttbeg B, Orrock JL, Peacor SD, Pintor LM, Preisser E, Rehage JS, Vonesh JR

(2010) Predator-prey naiveté, antipredator behavior, and the ecology of predator

invasions. Oikos 119: 610-621

Page 126: PREDATOR-PREY INTERACTIONS BETWEEN AN INVASIVE …

Chapter 3

108

Slusarczk M (1999) Predator-induced diapause in Daphnia magna may require two chemical

cues. Oecologia 119: 159-165

Smith GR, Awan AR (2009) The roles of predator identity and group size in the antipredator

responses of American toad (Bufo americanus) and bullfrog (Rana catesbeiana)

tadpoles. Behaviour 146: 225-243

Steiner UK (2007) Investment in defense and cost of predator-induced defense along a

resource gradient. Oecologia 152: 201-210

Stirling G (1995) Daphnia behavior as a bioassay of fish presence or predation. Funct Ecol 9:

778-784

Strauss SY, Lau JA, Carroll SP (2006) Evolutionary responses of natives to introduced species:

what do introductions tell us about natural communities? Ecol Lett 9: 354-371

Tejedo M (1993) Size-dependent vulnerability and behavioral responses of tadpoles of two

anuran species to beetle larvae predators. Herpetologica 49: 287-294

Van Buskirk J (2002) A comparative test of the adaptive plasticity hypothesis: Relationships

between habitat and phenotype in anuran larvae. Am Nat 160: 87-102

Van Buskirk J, Arioli M (2002) Dosage response of an induced defense: How sensitive are

tadpoles to predation risk? Ecology 83: 1580-1585

Wellborn GA, Skelly, DK, Werner, EE (1996) Mechanisms creating community structure across

a freshwater habitat gradient. Annu Rev Ecol Syst 27: 337-363

Werner EE, Anholt BR (1993) Ecological consequences of the tradeoff between growth and

mortality rates mediated by foraging activity. Am Nat 142: 242-272

Werner EE, Anholt BR (1996) Predator-induced behavioral indirect effects: Consequences to

competitive interactions in anuran larvae. Ecology 77: 157-169

Witte F, Goldschmidt T, Wanink J, Van Oijen M, Goudswaard K, Witte-Maas E, Bouton N (1992)

The destruction of an endemic species flock: quantitative data on the decline of the

haplochromine cichlids of Lake Victoria. Environ Biol Fishes 34: 1-28

Page 127: PREDATOR-PREY INTERACTIONS BETWEEN AN INVASIVE …

109

CHAPTER 4

Morphological and life-history responses and

integration of defensive traits in an anuran community

invaded by an exotic predator

Nunes AL, Orizaola G, Laurila A, Rebelo R

Manuscript

Page 128: PREDATOR-PREY INTERACTIONS BETWEEN AN INVASIVE …

110

Page 129: PREDATOR-PREY INTERACTIONS BETWEEN AN INVASIVE …

Chapter 4

111

Morphological and life-history responses and integration of defensive

traits in an anuran community invaded by an exotic predator

Abstract

Predator-induced phenotypic plasticity has been widely documented in response to native

predators, but studies examining the extent to which prey can respond to exotic predators are

scarce. As native prey do not share a long evolutionary history with exotic predators, they

often lack or develop ineffective defences against them. This may lead to population declines

and even extinctions, making exotic predators a serious threat to biodiversity. Here we studied

if tadpoles of an anuran community in the Southwest of the Iberian Peninsula developed

morphological or life-history responses when exposed to chemical cues of the exotic red

swamp crayfish, Procambarus clarkii, a predator recently introduced in this area. We reared

tadpoles of nine species until metamorphosis and examined predator-induced responses in

terms of larval morphology, growth and development. These responses were compared with

the ones developed in the presence of a native predator, the larval dragonfly Aeshna sp.. Eight

of the nine species altered their morphology or life-history in response to the native predator,

but only four responded to the exotic predator, indicating among-species variation in the

ability to respond to novel predators. While morphological defences were generally similar

across species (deeper tail fins) and almost exclusively elicited in the presence of the native

predator, life-history responses were very variable and commonly elicited in the presence of

the exotic predator. Phenotypic trait correlations were more common and the defences more

integrated in the presence of dragonfly than crayfish. Five species, of which Pelodytes ibericus

is an Iberian endemic, did not respond to crayfish. The lack of response may increase the risk

of local extinctions and ultimately reduce diversity of the invaded amphibian communities.

Understanding how native prey species vary in their responses to invasive predators is

Page 130: PREDATOR-PREY INTERACTIONS BETWEEN AN INVASIVE …

Chapter 4

112

important in predicting the negative impacts caused by altered predator-prey interactions

following biological invasions.

Keywords: Tadpole, exotic crayfish, morphology, life-history, phenotypic integration

Introduction

Invasive species are currently one of the most serious ecological threats to biodiversity. They

can modify ecosystems through habitat alteration, introduction of new diseases, genetic

introgression, competition and predation (Lockwood et al. 2007). By creating new interactions

with native species, invasives may drive native populations to extinction and profoundly alter

ecosystems (Lockwood et al. 2007). Invasive predators have drastically reduced or extirpated

populations of native species, largely due to a lack of co-evolutionary history between

predator and prey (reviews in Kats and Ferrer 2003, Cox and Lima 2006). When a predator

invades a new area, native prey may not be able to detect or identify the novel predator as a

dangerous threat, resulting in no or inappropriate antipredator responses (Cox and Lima 2006,

Sih et al. 2010). This lack of native prey responses to invasive predators is likely to increase

prey mortality, and severely impact invaded populations and communities (e.g. Witte et al.

1992, Polo-Cavia et al. 2010).

On the other hand, naturally co-occurring predators and prey share a common evolutionary

history, and prey are usually able to detect and evaluate predation risk and use adequate

defensive mechanisms that enhance their survival (Lima and Dill 1990, Sih et al. 2010).

Predator-induced defences are a widespread form of phenotypic plasticity and involve

alterations in behaviour, morphology, physiology and life-history (e.g. Lima and Dill 1990,

Benard 2004). Morphological defences typically consist of developing defensive structures,

such as spines and helmets in crustaceans, alterations of body shape in fish and amphibian

Page 131: PREDATOR-PREY INTERACTIONS BETWEEN AN INVASIVE …

Chapter 4

113

larvae (e.g. deeper body and tail) and increased shell thickness in snails, all of which improve

survival under predation risk (e.g. Spitze 1992, McCollum and Van Buskirk 1996, Hoverman and

Relyea 2007, Johansson and Andersson 2009). For example, in amphibians, a deeper tail lures

predator strikes away from the vulnerable headbody into the regenerable tail (Van Buskirk et

al. 2003) and may enable tadpoles to generate faster swimming bursts and improve

manoeuvrability (McCollum and Leimberger 1997, Dayton et al. 2005).

Predators can also induce alterations in the life-history of their prey (Benard 2004, Beckerman

et al. 2007, Relyea 2007). Predation risk can affect key life-history decisions such as time to

and size at metamorphosis, which can strongly influence individual fitness (reviews in Benard

2004, Relyea 2007). For instance, a reduction in time to metamorphosis can be a direct

response to predation risk which allows prey to leave earlier the risky predacious

environments, thereby reducing mortality risk (Benard 2004, Nicieza et al. 2006, Higginson and

Ruxton 2009). As large size can provide a refuge from predation, increasing growth rate and

size at metamorphosis can also be a direct and adaptive response to predation by gape or size-

limited predators (Urban 2007b). Importantly, however, the growth/predation risk trade-off is

a common constraint documented for many organisms: higher growth rate often comes at the

expense of increased vulnerability to predators (e.g. Werner 1986, Lima and Dill 1990, McPeek

2004).

An integrated view of inducible defences states that selection acts in favour of expressing

simultaneously multiple integrated defensive traits (Steiner and Pfeiffer 2007). Defensive traits

are often correlated and the degree of phenotypic trait integration depends on the magnitude

of the functional relationships among traits (Relyea 2001). Behavioural and morphological

defences, as well as life-history trait alterations, are not just direct and independent responses

to predation, but occur together and often depend on each other, promoting phenotypic

integration (Steiner 2007). Some of these relations indicate trade-offs among traits and reflect

either fitness costs or benefits associated with the development of inducible defences (e.g.

Page 132: PREDATOR-PREY INTERACTIONS BETWEEN AN INVASIVE …

Chapter 4

114

Van Buskirk 2000, Relyea 2001, Higginson and Ruxton 2009). For example, behaviourally and

morphologically defended prey may allocate fewer resources into growth and development,

often resulting in reduced body size or delayed metamorphosis (Spitze 1992, Van Buskirk 2000,

Higginson and Ruxton 2010; but see Steiner 2007). However, these defences may also result in

increased growth rates, indicating that the link between behavioural and morphological

defences and growth rate is not necessarily straightforward (Spitze 1992, Peacor 2002,

McPeek 2004, Johansson and Andersson 2009). Relationships between different traits, as well

as trait integration, vary across species and populations and can be altered by environmental

conditions (Relyea 2001, Benard 2004, Laurila et al. 2008, Higginson and Ruxton 2009,

Johansson and Andersson 2009). For instance, if exposed to an exotic predator, prey may show

a more weakly integrated defensive response than in the presence of a native predator, due to

the lack of co-evolutionary history selecting for an integrated response.

Many amphibians are negatively impacted by the introduction of invasive species (Kats and

Ferrer 2003), and their larval stages provide a very good study model for examining inducible

defences in the presence of exotic predators. In amphibians, larval predation is one of the

major sources of mortality (Werner 1991) and lacking appropriate defences towards an exotic

predator may render the invaded communities at serious risk. Even so, within a community,

species may greatly vary in their ability to detect and respond to novel predators. For instance,

prey species that use more general - as opposed to predator-specific - chemical cues to assess

predation risk, are more likely to respond to novel exotic predators (Sih et al. 2011, Chapter 3).

In amphibians, examples of these cues are chemical alarm cues released by injured or

consumed conspecifics (Sih et al. 2011). To date, several studies have examined behavioural

responses to exotic predators in amphibians (e.g. Pearl et al. 2003, Polo-Cavia et al. 2010,

Chapter 3). However, only few have investigated morphological and life-history responses (e.g.

Moore et al. 2004, Gomez-Mestre and Díaz-Paniagua 2011), and none of these have used a

community-wide approach.

Page 133: PREDATOR-PREY INTERACTIONS BETWEEN AN INVASIVE …

Chapter 4

115

The red swamp crayfish Procambarus clarkii, endemic to northeastern Mexico and southern

USA, is a successful invader worldwide (Holdich et al. 2009). It was introduced in Spain in 1973

and has now established populations all over the Iberian Peninsula (Holdich et al. 2009).

Several studies have documented strong negative impacts of this crayfish on Iberian

amphibians (e.g. Cruz et al. 2006, 2008). A recent community-wide study in southwestern

Portugal showed that six out of nine anuran species elicited behavioural defences when

exposed to P. clarkii, while eight species responded to a common native predator (dragonfly

larva (Aeshna sp.); Chapter 3). Species not responding behaviourally to the crayfish may be

highly vulnerable to this predator, unless they are able to use alternative defences against it. In

this paper, we aimed to understand if anuran species from this invaded community show

predator-induced plasticity in morphology and life-history characters in the presence of this

exotic crayfish and compared these defences to the responses to a native dragonfly larva.

Further, we explored if integration of defences differed in the presence of the native and

exotic predators. We predict that most anuran species will respond to the native predator, and

that species using more general chemical cues to respond to predators (such as Alytes

cisternasii, Bufo bufo and D. galganoi in this community; Chapter 3) are more likely to show

morphological or life-history alterations in the presence of the novel predator. Finally, we

predict patterns of trait integration to vary across species and expect to find more trait

correlations and a stronger pattern of integration in the presence of the native than in the

presence of the exotic predator.

Methods

Study species and experimental procedure

This study was conducted in an area located in the Sado River basin (SW Portugal), where a

variety of freshwater habitats ranging from streams to rice fields can be found (Cruz et al.

Page 134: PREDATOR-PREY INTERACTIONS BETWEEN AN INVASIVE …

Chapter 4

116

2006). This area, which naturally lacks any other freshwater crayfishes, was invaded by P.

clarkii ca. 25 years ago (R. Rebelo, pers. obs.). Nine anuran species occur in this area: Iberian

midwife toad (Alytes cisternasii), Iberian painted frog (Discoglossus galganoi), Western

spadefoot toad (Pelobates cultripes), Iberian parsley frog (Pelodytes ibericus), common toad

(Bufo bufo), natterjack toad (Bufo calamita), European tree frog (Hyla arborea), Mediterranean

tree frog (Hyla meridionalis) and Iberian water frog (Pelophylax perezi). Three of these species

are Iberian endemics (A. cisternasii, D. galganoi and P. ibericus) and three others (P. cultripes,

H. meridionalis and P. perezi) have a restricted distribution outside Iberia (Loureiro et al. 2008).

P. clarkii is an efficient predator of larvae of all these species and is now abundant throughout

southwestern Portugal (Cruz and Rebelo 2005). The native predator used in this study, late-

instar dragonfly larvae (Aeshna sp.), are voracious tadpole predators, widespread in the study

area. All the anuran species included in this study presently co-occur with both these

predators.

Because of differences in breeding phenology among the species, experiments were

conducted in different times throughout the year. From November 2007 to June 2008,

according to species phenology, several clutches of eight of the nine anuran species were

collected in streams or ponds in the study area (Table 1). For A. cisternasii, a species in which

males carry the eggs until hatching, larvae were collected very shortly after being released in

the water. A reproducing P. clarkii population was present in all the water bodies from where

egg masses were collected. The egg clutches were transported to the field station of the

Centre for Environmental Biology in Grândola (38º06’N, 8º34’W), where the experiment took

place. Embryos and larvae were kept in several species-specific 5L containers filled with spring

water that was changed every three days, and were fed commercial fish food and boiled

lettuce ad libitum. After reaching Gosner stage 25 (operculum closure over gills; Gosner 1960)

they were randomly assigned to different treatments and the experiment was initiated. Adult

crayfish and late-instar larval dragonflies were caught in ponds close to the field station (< 30

Page 135: PREDATOR-PREY INTERACTIONS BETWEEN AN INVASIVE …

Chapter 4

117

km). While not in the experiment, crayfishes were kept in several 40L plastic tanks and fed

commercial fish food and small invertebrates, whereas dragonfly larvae were kept individually

in 1.2 L plastic boxes and fed Ephemeroptera larvae. Prior to entering the experiment

predators were starved for 48-72 hours.

Table 1. Number of clutches collected, dates of collection, of the start of the experiment, of

photography (median Gosner stage at this date) and of the end of the experiment for each of the

nine studied species

Species Nr. clutches

collected Date of

collection Start of

experiment Morphology registered

Gosner stage

End of experiment

Alytes cisternasii - 04-Dec 15-Dec 12-Fev 30 28-Jul

Discoglossus galganoi 9 08-Jan 24-Jan 26-Fev 35 13-May

Pelobates cultripes 8 28-Nov 15-Dec 16-Apr 30 05-Sep

Pelodytes ibericus 10 26-Dec 23-Jan 20-Fev 32 22-Jun

Bufo bufo 6 07-Feb 19-Feb 20-Mar 34 04-Jun

Bufo calamita 9 22-Apr 19-May 17-Jun 34 09-Aug

Hyla arborea 10 05-Jun 17-Jun 23-Jul 30 25-Sep

Hyla meridionalis 9 13-Mar 08-Apr 20-Mai 32 15-Aug

Pelophylax perezi 11 16-Jun 02-Jul 03-Sep 33 09-Nov

We performed a factorial experiment using nine anuran species and three predator

treatments. At the beginning of the experiment, groups of ten tadpoles were allocated to

plastic containers (39 × 28 × 28 cm) filled with 10 L of water. A cylindrical predator cage, either

containing one crayfish (cage diameter 85 mm), one dragonfly larva (cage diameter 62 mm) or

no predator (randomly one of the two diameters) was added to each container. Cages were

opaque and covered with fine mesh netting on both sides, so that chemical cues could flow

out, but visual or tactile cues were not available. Predators were fed three tadpoles

conspecifics of the experimental individuals every second day. The three different predator

treatments were “crayfish”, “dragonfly”, and “control” (empty predator cage). For each of the

nine species all treatments were replicated once in each of five randomized blocks, resulting in

a total of 135 experimental units. Once tadpoles approached metamorphosis (Gosner stage 42:

Page 136: PREDATOR-PREY INTERACTIONS BETWEEN AN INVASIVE …

Chapter 4

118

emergence of at least one forelimb), we checked the containers daily for metamorphosed

individuals, which were then removed. Throughout the experiment, the tadpoles were fed fish

flakes and boiled lettuce ad libitum three times per week and water was changed every five

days. Water temperature was 18.0 ± 0.17 ºC (mean ± SE) and the photoperiod 12L:12D.

Response variables

In order to examine tadpole morphology each larva was photographed with a digital camera in

side view against a grid background. Photographs were taken, depending on species, when

tadpoles were between Gosner developmental stages 30 and 35 (see Table 1). Photographs of

five random tadpoles per container were loaded into MakeFan7 (Sheets 2009) to create a

standardized template for digitizing the different landmarks. The body shape of tadpoles was

captured by digitizing 20 landmarks on each individual (Appendix 1), using tpsDig2 software

(Rohlf 2008). Some of the landmarks were chosen according to specific and easily identifiable

anatomical points in the tadpole body (e.g. the centre of the eye, the tip of the tail muscle and

fin), while others were defined by using proportional distances within a structure (Appendix 1).

We conducted landmark-based geometric morphometrics analyses on these digitized

landmarks; this method is a powerful tool for analyzing and visualizing morphological variation

between individuals (Rohlf and Marcus 1993, Zelditch et al. 2004). Landmark coordinates were

imported into tpsRelw (Rohlf 2007) to create relative warps which, in a multivariate geometric

morphometrics analysis, are the equivalent to the principal component scores from Principal

Component Analysis. Relative warp scores on tadpole morphology were estimated using the

consensus (i.e. average) morphology for each experimental container in order to avoid

pseudoreplication. Consensus conformations were estimated using tpsRelw (Rohlf 2007).

At metamorphosis, tadpoles were blotted dry and weighed with a digital balance to the

nearest 0.01 g (mass at metamorphosis). Time to metamorphosis was defined as the number

Page 137: PREDATOR-PREY INTERACTIONS BETWEEN AN INVASIVE …

Chapter 4

119

of days elapsed between the beginning of the experiment (day of Gosner stage 25) and the

onset of metamorphosis (day of Gosner stage 42). Growth rate was estimated as the quotient

between mass at and time to metamorphosis.

Statistical analyses

All morphological analyses were conducted on relative warp scores and independently for

each of the nine studied species. The body shape of tadpoles was examined using the first

relative warp yielded by the geometric morphometrics analyses (RW1) which accounted, on

average, for over 50% of the total morphological variance (Fig. 1; see Orizaola et al. (2011) for

a similar approach). The predator effects on the other RWs were negligible in all species and

are not presented here. We analyzed the effects of the different predator treatments on

tadpole morphology by using univariate analyses of variance (ANOVAs) with type III sum of

squares, where RW1 scores were the dependent variable describing tadpole morphology.

Post-hoc analyses were performed using Tukey HSD tests.

To investigate for the effects of predator treatments in larval life-history traits (time to and

mass at metamorphosis, and growth rate), we first performed a multivariate analysis of

variance (MANOVA) for each species, followed by univariate ANOVAs for each trait. Tank

means were always used as the unit of analysis, and values were log transformed in order to

fulfil the assumptions of analysis of variance. Block effects were not significant for any of the

traits and were excluded from the analyses. Post-hoc comparisons were performed using

Tukey HSD tests. An analysis comparing all the studied species was not performed, since

species responses were expected to differ, and we were mostly interested in examining if a

specific species was responding or not to the exotic predator.

Page 138: PREDATOR-PREY INTERACTIONS BETWEEN AN INVASIVE …

Chapter 4

120

To determine whether predator-induced changes in morphology were associated with induced

changes in life-history characters, we performed Pearson correlations between traits, within

each species and across predator environments. However, in order to examine if the presence

of an invasive predator induced relationships different than the ones induced by a native

predator, correlations were performed on tank means (n = 10 per species), separately for

crayfish and dragonfly treatments (plus the control treatment). In order to obtain a more

integrative approach, we also used behavioural data from a companion paper (Chapter 3) to

investigate relationships between alterations in behaviour (the average proportion of active

tadpoles across ontogeny for each species), morphology and life-history traits. Across-species

correlations were not performed because we were interested in looking at trade-offs and

integration within each species.

Results

Morphological responses

All species except B. calamita modified their morphology when exposed to the native predator

(Table 2; Fig. 1). Of these eight species, all except B. bufo responded by increased tail fin depth,

and all but B. bufo and P. perezi had a more anterior insertion of the tail fin to the headbody

(Fig. 1). Induced tadpoles of A. cisternasii, D. galganoi, P. perezi and P. cultripes also had

deeper tail muscles. The three former species together with P. ibericus and H. meridionalis also

increased their headbody depth (Fig. 1). B. bufo altered its morphology by increasing headbody

length (Fig. 1). On the contrary, when exposed to crayfish, only B. bufo changed their

morphology, the response being similar to that found in the presence of the dragonfly (Table

2; Fig. 1).

Page 139: PREDATOR-PREY INTERACTIONS BETWEEN AN INVASIVE …

Chapter 4

121

-0.04

-0.02

0.00

0.02

0.04

No predator Crayfish Dragonfly

RW

1 =

39.5

5%

Pelobates cultripes

-0.04

-0.02

0.00

0.02

0.04

No predator Crayfish Dragonfly

RW

1 =

62.5

7%

Pelodytes ibericus

-0.04

-0.02

0.00

0.02

0.04

0.06

No predator Crayfish Dragonfly

RW

1 =

80.1

1%

Alytes cisternasii

-0.08

-0.04

0.00

0.04

0.08

0.12

No predator Crayfish Dragonfly

RW

1 =

88.8

0%

Discoglossus galganoi

Fig. 1 Mean ± S.E. values of Relative Warp 1 (RW1) representing tadpoles shape of nine anuran species

(A. cisternasii, D. galganoi, P. cultripes, P. ibericus, B. bufo, B. calamita, H. arborea, H. meridionalis and

P. perezi) in the presence of the different predator treatments. Drawings placed on the right hand side

of each graph show the shape of larvae representing the extreme positive (black) and negative (grey)

scores of RW1.

Page 140: PREDATOR-PREY INTERACTIONS BETWEEN AN INVASIVE …

Chapter 4

122

-0.02

-0.01

0.00

0.01

0.02

0.03

No predator Crayfish Dragonfly

RW

1 =

42.7

6%

Hyla meridionalis

-0.04

-0.02

0.00

0.02

0.04

No predator Crayfish Dragonfly

RW

1 =

42.5

7%

Pelophylax perezi

-0.02

-0.01

0.00

0.01

0.02

0.03

No predator Crayfish Dragonfly

RW

1 =

37.9

4%

Hyla arborea

-0.03

-0.02

-0.01

0.01

0.02

No predator Crayfish Dragonfly

RW

1 =

46.1

6%

Bufo bufo

-0.02

-0.01

0.00

0.01

0.02

No predator Crayfish Dragonfly

RW

1 =

44.5

8%

Bufo calamita

Fig. 1 Extended

Page 141: PREDATOR-PREY INTERACTIONS BETWEEN AN INVASIVE …

Chapter 4

123

Table 2. Univariate ANOVAs on the morphology (first relative warp, RW1) of the nine anuran species in

presence of the different predator treatments (left hand panel). P-values for the Tukey post-hoc tests

referring to comparisons between the control and each predator treatment (right hand panel). P-values

< 0.05 are marked in boldface

Morphology Dragonfly Crayfish

df F P P P

A. cisternasii 2, 15 165.786 <0.001 <0.001 0.745

D. galganoi 2, 15 79.434 <0.001 <0.001 0.419

P. cultripes 2, 15 7.207 0.009 0.012 0.907

P. ibericus 2, 15 42.452 <0.001 <0.001 0.610

B. bufo 2, 15 28.615 <0.001 <0.001 0.005

B. calamita 2, 15 0.121 0.887 - -

H. arborea 2, 15 13.654 0.001 0.003 0.910

H. meridionalis 2, 15 8.047 0.006 0.035 0.589

P. perezi 2, 13 14.796 0.001 0.001 0.164

Life-history responses

Species differed substantially in life-history traits. B. calamita and D. galganoi had the shortest

larval periods (ca 50 days; Fig. 2), roughly 25% of the larval period of A. cisternasii and P.

cultripes, which had the slowest development. Excluding P. cultripes, a species that grew much

faster and attained a much higher mass than all the others, larvae of A. cisternasii and P. perezi

had the highest masses at metamorphosis and P. perezi the highest growth rates. In contrast,

B. calamita had the lowest growth rate and was smallest at metamorphosis (Fig. 2).

MANOVAs detected treatment effects in life-history traits for all the anuran species (P < 0.038)

except for P. perezi (P = 0.489). In B. calamita none of the univariate ANOVAs was significant

(Table 3). The univariate ANOVAs followed by Tukey tests showed that five species - B. bufo, D.

galganoi, H. arborea, H. meridionalis and P. ibericus - significantly changed life-history traits as

a response to the native predator (Table 3). Induced tadpoles of P. ibericus increased growth

rate and metamorphosed at larger mass (Table 3; Fig. 2). Tadpoles of H. meridionalis also

Page 142: PREDATOR-PREY INTERACTIONS BETWEEN AN INVASIVE …

Chapter 4

124

attained a larger mass at metamorphosis, while H. arborea tadpoles increased their growth

rates. On the contrary, B. bufo tadpoles decreased both growth rate and time to

metamorphosis, which resulted in a smaller mass at metamorphosis (Table 3; Fig. 2). Tadpoles

of D. galganoi also reduced their growth rates, but they took longer to metamorphose; this

was the only species to increase time to metamorphosis in response to predator presence

(Table 3; Fig. 2).

Four species altered their life-history traits in the presence of crayfish (A. cisternasii, B. bufo, H.

arborea and P. cultripes; Table 3). From these species, only B. bufo and H. arborea also

responded to the native predator, the responses to the two predators being very similar, but

with some small differences. In response to crayfish, B. bufo did not alter growth rate (while

reducing time to and mass at metamorphosis) and H. arborea, in addition to increasing growth

rate, also reduced time to metamorphosis (Table 3; Fig. 2). Tadpoles of both A. cisternasii and

P. cultripes also reduced time to metamorphosis when exposed to crayfish. Finally, tadpoles of

A. cisternasii grew faster and attained higher masses (Table 3; Fig. 2).

Trait correlations

None of the paired trait correlations was statistically significant for B. calamita, P. cultripes and

P. perezi (Table 4). When exposed to the dragonfly larvae six species showed significant

correlations between different traits, while only two species had significant correlations in the

presence of crayfish. Although there were generally relatively few significant trait correlations

per species, many more significant relationships were found in the presence of the native than

in the presence of the exotic predator (19 vs. 7, Table 4).

Page 143: PREDATOR-PREY INTERACTIONS BETWEEN AN INVASIVE …

Chapter 4

125

Table 3. Analyses of larval life-history traits (time to metamorphosis, mass at metamorphosis and growth rate) for each of the nine anuran species. Results (P-

values) of the univariate ANOVAs performed for each trait (df = 2,15) are shown on the left hand side of each column. Results (P-values) of the Tukey post-hoc

tests are shown on the right hand side and values refer to comparisons of each predator treatment with the control treatment. P-values < 0.05 are marked in

boldface

Time to metamorphosis Mass at metamorphosis Growth rate

P Dragonfly Crayfish P Dragonfly Crayfish P Dragonfly Crayfish

A. cisternasii <0.001 0.834 <0.001 0.022 0.177 0.018 <0.001 0.322 <0.001

D. galganoi <0.001 0.001 0.727 0.766 - - 0.004 0.014 0.886

P. cultripes 0.012 0.621 0.012 0.838 - - 0.640 - -

P. ibericus 0.429 - - <0.001 0.002 0.374 0.001 0.002 0.950

B. bufo 0.001 0.001 0.006 <0.001 <0.001 0.003 0.019 0.031 0.995

B. calamita 0.053 - - 0.110 - - 0.254 - -

H. arborea 0.044 0.737 0.043 0.067 - - 0.002 0.031 0.002

H. meridionalis 0.118 - - 0.007 0.030 0.746 0.120 - -

P. perezi 0.957 - - 0.054 - - 0.067 - -

Page 144: PREDATOR-PREY INTERACTIONS BETWEEN AN INVASIVE …

Chapter 4

126

a)

b)

c)

Fig. 2 a) Time to metamorphosis, b) Mass at metamorphosis and c) Growth rate (mean ± SE) of nine anuran species, in presence of the different predator

treatments. The three treatments are control (No pred), crayfish (Crayf) and dragonfly (Dragonf). Note different scales in the graphs in the end of each row, for

species having very high values of a specific trait.

Page 145: PREDATOR-PREY INTERACTIONS BETWEEN AN INVASIVE …

Chapter 4

127

In the presence of dragonfly larvae, D. galganoi had a negative correlation between activity

level (behaviour) and morphology - low activity was correlated with deeper headbody, tail fin

and tail muscle in this species. Further, induced morphology was correlated with longer larval

period (as was also activity) and reduced growth rate. In fact, D. galganoi was the only species

for which there was a negative correlation between morphology and growth rate (Table 4). On

the contrary, for P. ibericus there was a positive correlation between these two traits; tadpoles

that grew faster (and had larger mass at metamorphosis) also had deeper headbody and tail

fin. In both this species and H. meridionalis, induced morphology was associated with low

activity level which, in turn, was associated with higher mass (and in the case of P. ibericus

with higher growth rate) (Table 4). Activity level was also negatively correlated with growth

rate in H. arborea.

For species showing trait correlations when exposed to crayfish, A. cisternasii exhibited a

different relationship than the one shown in the presence of the dragonfly. When exposed to

dragonfly, there was a significant negative correlation between behaviour and morphology,

lower activity being correlated with deeper headbody, tail fin and tail muscle (Table 4). On the

other hand, in the presence of crayfish, A. cisternasii activity was negatively correlated with

mass. There was also a negative correlation between time to and mass at metamorphosis

(Table 4). B. bufo was the only species showing very similar correlations in the presence of the

native and the exotic predators. Induced morphology was negatively correlated with both time

to and mass at metamorphosis (the latter only in dragonfly presence), while activity was

positively correlated with these two metamorphic traits. There was also a positive correlation

between time to and mass at metamorphosis and a negative correlation between activity and

morphology. This indicates that lower activity was associated with longer headbody and a

faster metamorphosis at lower mass (Table 4).

Page 146: PREDATOR-PREY INTERACTIONS BETWEEN AN INVASIVE …

Chapter 4

128

Table 4. Phenotypic Pearson correlations (r and P values) performed between morphological, behavioural and life-history traits for the nine studied species. N =

10 for all the correlations. Correlations for which P > 0.05 are marked as non significant (N.S.).

Time met-Morphol Mass met-Morphol Growth rate-Morphol Time met-Behav Mass met-Behav Growth rate-Behav

Dragonfly Crayfish Dragonfly Crayfish Dragonfly Crayfish Dragonfly Crayfish Dragonfly Crayfish Dragonfly Crayfish

A. cisternasii N.S. N.S. N.S. N.S. N.S. N.S. N.S. N.S. N.S. r = -0.687 P = 0.028

N.S. N.S.

D. galganoi r = 0.882 P = 0.001

N.S. N.S. N.S. r = -0.787 P = 0.007

N.S. r = -0.639 P = 0.047

N.S. N.S. N.S. N.S. N.S.

P. cultripes N.S. N.S. N.S. N.S. N.S. N.S. N.S. N.S. N.S. N.S. N.S. N.S.

P. ibericus N.S. N.S. r = 0.741 P = 0.014

N.S. r = 0.853 P = 0.002

N.S. N.S. N.S. r = -0.820 P = 0.004

N.S. r = -0.889 P = 0.001

N.S.

B. bufo r = -0.907 P < 0.001

r = -0.765 P = 0.010

r = -0.861 P = 0.001

N.S. N.S. N.S. r = 0.849 P = 0.002

r = 0.708 P = 0.022

r = 0.871 P = 0.001

r = 0.728 P = 0.017

N.S. N.S.

B. calamita N.S. N.S. N.S. N.S. N.S. N.S. N.S. N.S. N.S. N.S. N.S. N.S.

H. arborea N.S. N.S. N.S. N.S. N.S. N.S. N.S. N.S. N.S. N.S. r = -0.830 P = 0.011

N.S.

H. meridionalis N.S. N.S. N.S. N.S. N.S. N.S. N.S. N.S. r = -0.700 P = 0.024

N.S. N.S. N.S.

P. perezi N.S. N.S. N.S. N.S. N.S. N.S. N.S. N.S. N.S. N.S. N.S. N.S.

Time met - Time to metamorphosis; Mass met - Mass at metamorphosis; Morphol - Morphology; Behav - Behaviour

Page 147: PREDATOR-PREY INTERACTIONS BETWEEN AN INVASIVE …

Chapter 4

129

Table 4. Extended.

Time met-Mass met Morphol-Behav

Dragonfly Crayfish Dragonfly Crayfish

N.S. r = -0.827 P = 0.003

r = -0.819 P = 0.004

N.S.

N.S. N.S. r = -0.789 P = 0.007

N.S.

N.S. N.S. N.S. N.S.

N.S. N.S. r = -0.637 P = 0.047

N.S.

r = 0.875 P = 0.001

r = 0.803 P = 0.005

r = -0.907 P < 0.001

r = -0.635 P = 0.048

N.S. N.S. N.S. N.S.

N.S. N.S. N.S. N.S.

N.S. N.S. r = -0.887 P = 0.001

N.S.

N.S. N.S. N.S. N.S.

Page 148: PREDATOR-PREY INTERACTIONS BETWEEN AN INVASIVE …

Chapter 4

130

Discussion

We found that both morphological and life-history plasticity in response to predator chemical

cues were widespread in a community of larval anurans. Plastic responses were more common

in response to the native than to the exotic predator and, responses to the exotic predator,

when present, were often qualitatively different from the responses to the native predator.

Furthermore, phenotypic integration was overall much higher in the presence of the native

than in the presence of the exotic predator.

Eight of our study species showed antipredator responses in the presence of the native

dragonfly (all but B. calamita), while only four showed plasticity in the presence of the exotic

crayfish (A. cisternasii, B. bufo, H. arborea and P. cultripes). In larval anurans, previous studies

have reported morphological and life-history responses to chemical cues from both caged

dragonfly (e.g. Laurila et al. 1998, Relyea 2001, Richter-Boix et al. 2007) and crayfish predators

(e.g. Nyström and Abjörnsson 2000, Saglio and Madrillon 2006). In this study, a higher

proportion of the species responding to a specific predator altered morphology in the

presence of dragonfly (eight in eight for dragonfly against one in four for crayfish), whereas in

the presence of crayfish a higher proportion altered life-history traits (four in four for crayfish

against five in eight for dragonfly). This may suggest that life-history characters are more

evolutionary labile and more readily altered in response to new selective pressures than

morphological characters (Blomberg et al. 2003). In addition, if life-history alterations are more

costly than morphological ones (Higginson and Ruxton 2010), they may be selected against

when co-evolutionary history between predator and prey is longer. Alternatively, responses to

the native predator may not be efficient against the invasive predator, suggesting predator-

specific defences (e.g. Relyea 2001, 2002). In fact, among the species that responded to the

two predators, only B. bufo exhibited the same type of defences, while both A. cisternasii and

P. cultripes had completely dissimilar responses, altering only morphology in the presence of

Page 149: PREDATOR-PREY INTERACTIONS BETWEEN AN INVASIVE …

Chapter 4

131

dragonfly and only life-history traits in the presence of crayfish. Further studies testing the

efficiency of these inducible responses against the two different predators would be valuable

(e.g. Gomez-Mestre and Diaz-Paniagua 2011).

Morphological responses

Dragonfly presence strongly affected tadpole morphology, with tadpoles of most species

showing quite similar morphological plastic responses. These consisted of developing a deeper

headbody, tail fin and tail muscle and a more anterior insertion of the tail fin to the headbody.

The development of deep tails when exposed to chemical cues from predatory dragonflies is a

widespread adaptive response in larval anurans (e.g. Relyea 2001, Van Buskirk 2009), which

enhances the probability of a tadpole escaping a predator attack (e.g. McCollum and Van

Buskirk 1996, Van Buskirk et al. 2003, Dayton et al. 2005, Johnson et al. 2008). A more anterior

insertion of the tail fin into the upper part of the headbody has also been previously reported

as a response to predation risk (Van Buskirk 2009). The headbody region of five species (A.

cisternasii, D. galganoi, H. meridionalis, P. ibericus and P. perezi) also changed with dragonfly

predation cues, with induced tadpoles having deeper headbodies. This may not have a direct

defensive function, but may enable tadpoles to have a greater gut surface area for food

digestion, which would facilitate accumulating energy reserves, maybe allowing for less

foraging activity (Van Buskirk 2009).

The only species for which we detected morphological plasticity in response to crayfish was B.

bufo. Previous studies have generally failed to detect predator-induced morphological

plasticity in this species (e.g. Lardner 2000, Richter-Boix et al. 2007). However, also using

geometric morphometrics tools, Van Buskirk (2009) recently reported the same morphological

alteration that we found for this species in predator presence (increased headbody size). This

may indicate that morphological alterations in B. bufo have not been previously detected due

Page 150: PREDATOR-PREY INTERACTIONS BETWEEN AN INVASIVE …

Chapter 4

132

to technical constraints. Further, when faced with crayfish, responses of B. bufo were the same

as in the presence of dragonfly indicating that the same type of defences are used against the

native and invasive predators. This reinforces the idea that, in this species, responses to the

exotic predator are mainly mediated by the presence of general alarm cues, rather than to

predator specific cues (Semlitsch and Gavasso 1992, Chapter 3). B. calamita was the only

species lacking morphological plasticity, which is in accordance with previous studies (Lardner

2000, Richter-Boix et al. 2007).

Life-history responses

Contrary to induced morphological responses, life-history alterations were extremely variable

among the anuran species and the two predators. The large variation in life-history responses

supports the view that these changes can either be a direct response to predation risk or a

consequence of the development of behavioural and/or morphological inducible defences

(Steiner 2007). The most frequent alterations were found in time to metamorphosis and the

least frequent in mass at metamorphosis. This suggests less plasticity in the latter trait,

possibly because mass at metamorphosis is a very important life-history attribute in

determining adult fitness in amphibians, and because a threshold size has to be attained

before metamorphosis can occur (Wilbur and Collins 1973, Relyea 2007, Higginson and Ruxton

2009). P. perezi and B. calamita were the only species showing no significant predator-induced

plasticity in life-history traits.

B. bufo tadpoles reduced both time to and mass at metamorphosis in the presence of

dragonfly and crayfish (as well as growth rate in dragonfly presence) and the responses to the

two predators were very similar. This again suggests a generalized response towards predators

in B. bufo (probably due to a response to alarm cues), which may facilitate responses to novel

invasive predators (Sih et al. 2011, Chapter 3). Laurila et al. (1998) found a similar response to

Page 151: PREDATOR-PREY INTERACTIONS BETWEEN AN INVASIVE …

Chapter 4

133

a dragonfly predator. Metamorphosing earlier is a rare, but potentially advantageous response

to predators which has been reported also in other bufonids (Relyea 2007). Contrary to B.

bufo, tadpoles of D. galganoi exposed to a dragonfly predator significantly prolonged time to

metamorphosis, most likely due to strong morphological and behavioural defences, as has

been previously shown for this species (Nicieza et al. 2006). A longer larval period can be

detrimental for ephemeral pond species like D. galganoi because it is likely to increase

mortality when the pond dries. Morphological defences were negatively correlated with

growth, suggesting that they were more costly than behavioural defences (Van Buskirk 2000).

Three of our study species significantly increased growth rates in the presence of either

dragonfly (P. ibericus and H. arborea) or crayfish (A. cisternasii and H. arborea). Increased

growth is not a common prey response to caged predators, but has been previously reported

in amphibians (Werner 1991, McCollum and Leimberger 1997, Van Buskirk and Relyea 1998,

Peacor 2002, Teplitsky et al. 2003, Urban 2007a) and other taxa (e.g. Crowl and Covich 1990,

Spitze 1992, Johansson and Andersson 2009). Increased growth rates may be due to

physiological changes in metabolism, such as increased food assimilation or food conversion

efficiency (McCollum and Van Buskirk 1996, McPeek 2004, Stoks et al. 2005, Steiner 2007,

Dmitriew 2011). If tadpoles reach larger size through these mechanisms, this may act as a

defence against size-limited predators (Urban 2007b, Dmitriew 2011), and Beckerman et al.

(2007) proposed that size-selective predation may induce direct physiologically mediated

alterations in life-history traits of prey. Although dragonflies and crayfish are not strictly gape-

limited predators, dragonflies are less able to capture and handle prey above a certain size

(Werner and McPeek 1994). Increased growth rates may also be achieved by increased

foraging effort and activity (McPeek 2004, Urban 2007a), which was not the case here (see

Table 4). On the contrary, in A. cisternasii and P. ibericus we found a negative correlation

between behaviour and mass at metamorphosis and/or growth rate, suggesting that less

active animals grew faster and became larger. While this contrasts with the classical

Page 152: PREDATOR-PREY INTERACTIONS BETWEEN AN INVASIVE …

Chapter 4

134

behaviourally-mediated growth/predation risk trade-off, other studies have also found an

association between reduced activity and higher growth rates in predator presence. Johansson

and Andersson (2009) proposed that the reduced activity of crucian carp in the presence of

pike allowed more energy to be saved and then allocated to growth. Our finding that exposure

to predators can have a net positive effect on growth, even when behavioural defences are

elicited, reinforces the idea that a decoupling between behaviour and growth is common (see

also McPeek 2004, Stoks et al. 2005). It should be noticed that, in spite of its benefits,

accelerated growth may compromise post-metamorphic fitness by preventing allocation to

other important traits or functions, such as immune defence and resistance to oxidative stress

(Dmitriew 2011).

Two of the species that grew faster in predator presence also took a shorter time to reach

metamorphosis (A. cisternasii and H. arborea). This was only observed in response to crayfish

and may indicate the perception of a higher mortality risk in the aquatic habitat. While these

concurrent alterations in time to and mass at metamorphosis are not predicted to occur in

nature by recent dynamic state-dependent models (Higginson and Ruxton 2010, but see

Teplitsky et al. 2003), they can be beneficial for tadpoles avoiding crayfish predation.

Trait correlations

The patterns of trait integration varied widely across species and predator environment. In

general, more species showed trait correlations when exposed to the native than to the exotic

predator, and within species traits were generally more integrated (higher number of trait

correlations) in the presence of the native predator. D. galganoi, B. bufo and P. ibericus

showed a highly integrated response in the presence of the dragonfly larvae. Interestingly, B.

bufo showed similar integration in response to both crayfish and dragonfly, again suggesting

that a response to alarm chemicals rather than to predator cues may have homogenized the

Page 153: PREDATOR-PREY INTERACTIONS BETWEEN AN INVASIVE …

Chapter 4

135

responses in this species. A. cisternasii showed a higher level of phenotypic integration in

crayfish than dragonfly presence, which might suggest an adaptive response to this novel

predator. In this case, trait correlations found in the presence of dragonfly and crayfish were

dissimilar because this species responded to the crayfish by altering different suites of traits

than when responding to the dragonfly. H. arborea and P. cultripes, although eliciting defences

to the crayfish predator showed no trait integration.

In the presence of the same predator, correlations were very variable from species to species,

suggesting species-specific defensive strategies. Further, the same traits were sometimes

related in opposite manners in the same predator environment. For instance, in A. cisternasii,

time to and mass at metamorphosis were negatively correlated, whereas in B. bufo these traits

were positively related. This shows that traits are not linearly related across species and that

outcomes are specific for each prey species. Nevertheless, the correlation between behaviour

and morphology was consistently negative across species, indicating that a decrease in activity

did not reduce the energy necessary for eliciting morphological defences. On the contrary,

alterations in morphology could have been indirect effects of reduced tadpole activity, as has

been observed before (e.g. Johansson and Andersson 2009). This suggests that these two

types of defences may frequently complement or augment each other, which is predicted to

maximize fitness (Steiner and Pfeiffer 2007).

Conclusions

In conclusion, our work highlights the importance of studying whole communities to evaluate

responses to predators, adding to the growing evidence that different species use specific

suites of defences against the same predator. Our results reinforce that extant interspecific

differences in behavioural, morphological and physiological characteristics as well as in species

ecology, select for different adaptive antipredator responses in different species (Lardner

Page 154: PREDATOR-PREY INTERACTIONS BETWEEN AN INVASIVE …

Chapter 4

136

2000). We found that, from the nine anurans present in the study area, four species reacted to

crayfish cues in the examined traits. In three of these species, crayfish presence produced a

different suite of defences than in the presence of dragonfly, indicating predator-specific

responses and the ability to detect dissimilar predator signals. This suggests detection of the

exotic crayfish as a threat, either by recognition of this novel invasive predator, or by a mere

reaction to an unknown chemical stimulus possibly resembling kairomones emitted by another

predator in the community. P. ibericus and P. perezi, while responding to the dragonfly, did not

respond to the exotic predator (a similar result was also found in behaviour; Chapter 3),

apparently not recognizing the crayfish as a threat. As such, these species seem to be

undefended against P. clarkii, which may lead to population declines and compromise their

persistence in habitats invaded by the crayfish. This acquires special relevance in the case of P.

ibericus, a species endemic to the Iberian Peninsula (Loureiro et al. 2008). Since most inland

aquatic habitats in the Iberian Peninsula are already invaded by P. clarkii, and most of the

areas not invaded seem to be suitable for its establishment (Capinha and Anastácio 2011),

special efforts are needed to determine more precisely the level of threat that this invasive

crayfish poses to these native anurans. Examining how prey species vary in their responses to

introduced predators is essential for understanding the dynamics of predator-prey interactions

following biological invasions and, ultimately, for preventing extinctions in invaded

communities.

Acknowledgements

We thank Pedro Andrade, Erika Almeida, Susana Alves and Cátia Guerreiro for their invaluable

help in the field and experimental work. We also thank Jesús Díaz-Rodríguez for helping with

the Pelodytes taxonomy. Comments from Frank Johansson greatly improved the manuscript.

Permits were provided by the Portuguese Instituto da Conservação da Natureza e da

Page 155: PREDATOR-PREY INTERACTIONS BETWEEN AN INVASIVE …

Chapter 4

137

Biodiversidade (ICNB). This research was funded by the FCT Project POCI/BIA-

BDE/56100/2004, FCT grant SFRH/BD/29068/2006 and Stiftelsen för Zoologisk Forskning.

References

Beckerman AP, Wieski K, Baird DJ (2007) Behavioural versus physiological mediation of life

history under predation risk. Oecologia 152:335-343.

Benard MF (2004) Predator-induced phenotypic plasticity in organisms with complex life

cycles. Annual Review of Ecology, Evolution and Systematics 35: 651-673.

Blomberg SP, Garland T Jr., Ives AR (2003) Testing for phylogenetic signal in comparative data:

behavioral traits are more labile. Evolution 57: 717-745.

Capinha C, Anastácio P (2011) Assessing the environmental requirements of invaders using

ensembles of distribution models. Diversity and Distributions 17: 13-24.

Cox JG, Lima SL (2006) Naiveté and an aquatic-terrestrial dichotomy in the effects of

introduced predators. Trends in Ecology and Evolution 21: 674-680.

Crowl TA, Covich AP (1990) Predator-induced life-history shifts in a freshwater snail. Science

247: 949-951.

Cruz MJ, Rebelo R (2005) Vulnerability of southwest Iberian amphibians to an introduced

crayfish, Procambarus clarkii. Amphibia-Reptilia 26: 293-303.

Cruz MJ, Rebelo R, Crespo EG (2006) Effects of an introduced crayfish, Procambarus clarkii, on

the distribution of south-western Iberian amphibians in their breeding habitats.

Ecography 29: 329-338.

Cruz MJ, Segurado P, Sousa M, Rebelo R (2008) Collapse of the amphibian community of

thePaul do Boquilobo Natural Reserve (central Portugal) after the arrival of the exotic

American crayfish Procambarus clarkii. Herpetological Journal 18: 197-204.

Page 156: PREDATOR-PREY INTERACTIONS BETWEEN AN INVASIVE …

Chapter 4

138

Dayton GH, Saenz D, Baum KA, Langerhans RB, DeWitt TJ (2005) Body shape, burst speed and

escape behavior of larval anurans. Oikos 111: 582-591.

Dmitriew CM (2011) The evolution of growth trajectories: what limits growth rate? Biological

Reviews 86: 97-116.

Gomez-Mestre I, Díaz-Paniagua C (2011) Invasive predatory crayfish do not trigger inducible

defences in tadpoles. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B, in press.

Gosner KL (1960) A simplified table for staging anuran embryos and larvae with notes on

identification. Herpetologica 16: 183-190.

Higginson AD, Ruxton GD (2009) Dynamic models allowing for flexibility in complex life

histories accurately predict timing of metamorphosis and antipredator strategies of

prey. Functional Ecology 23: 1103-1113.

Higginson AD, Ruxton GD (2010) Adaptive changes in size and age at metamorphosis can

qualitatively vary with predator type and available defenses. Ecology 91: 2756-2768.

Holdich DM, Reynolds JD, Souty-Grosset C, Sibley PJ (2009) A review of the ever increasing

threat to European crayfish from non-indigenous crayfish species. Knowledge and

Management of Aquatic Ecosystems 394-395: 11.

Hoverman JT, Relyea RA (2007) The rules of engagement: how to defend against combinations

of predators. Oecologia 154: 551-560.

Johansson F, Andersson J (2009) Scared fish get lazy, and lazy fish get fat. Journal of Animal

Ecology 78: 772-777.

Johnson JB, Burt DB, DeWitt TJ (2008) Form, function, and fitness: Pathways to survival.

Evolution 62: 1243-1251.

Kats LB, Ferrer RP (2003) Alien predators and amphibian declines: review of two decades of

science and the transition to conservation. Diversity and Distributions 9: 99-110.

Lardner B (2000) Morphological and life history responses to predators in larvae of seven

anurans. Oikos 88: 169-180.

Page 157: PREDATOR-PREY INTERACTIONS BETWEEN AN INVASIVE …

Chapter 4

139

Laurila A, Kujasalo J, Ranta E (1998) Predator-induced changes in life-history in two anuran

tadpoles: effects of predator diet. Oikos 83: 307-317.

Laurila A, Lindgren B, Laugen AT (2008) Antipredator defenses along a latitudinal gradient in

Rana temporaria. Ecology 89: 1399-1413.

Lima SL, Dill LM (1990) Behavioral decisions made under the risk of predation: A review and

prospectus. Canadian Journal of Zoology 68: 619-640.

Lockwood J, Hoopes M and Marchetti M (2007) Invasion Ecology. Blackwell Publishing Ltd.,

Malden, Massachusetts, 312 pp.

Loureiro A, Ferrand de Almeida N, Carretero MA, Paulo OS (eds.) (2008) Atlas dos Anfíbios e

Répteis de Portugal. Instituto da Conservação da Natureza e da Biodiversidade, Lisboa.

257 pp.

McCollum SA, Van Buskirk J (1996) Costs and benefits of a predator-induced polyphenism in

the gray treefrog Hyla chrysoscelis. Evolution 50: 583-593.

McCollum SA, Leimberger JD (1997) Predator-induced morphological changes in an amphibian:

predation by dragonflies affects tadpole shape and color. Oecologia 109: 615-621

McPeek MA (2004) The growth/predation risk trade-off: so what is the mechanism? American

Naturalist 163: 88-111.

Moore RD, Griffiths RA, O’Brien CM, Murphy A, Jay D (2004) Induced defences in an

endangered amphibian in response to an introduced snake predator. Oecologia 141:

139-147.

Nicieza AG, Álvarez DA, Atienza EMS (2006) Delayed effects of larval predation risk and food

quality on anuran juvenile performance. Journal of Evolutionary Biology 19: 1092-1103.

Nyström P, Åbjörnsson K (2000) Effects of fish chemical cues on the interactions between

tadpoles and crayfish. Oikos 88: 181-190.

Orizaola G, Dahl E, Laurila A (2011) Reversibility of predator-induced plasticity and its effect at

a life-history switch point. Oikos, in press.

Page 158: PREDATOR-PREY INTERACTIONS BETWEEN AN INVASIVE …

Chapter 4

140

Peacor SD (2002) Positive effect of predators on prey growth rate through induced

modifications of prey behaviour. Ecology Letters 5: 77-85.

Pearl CA, Adams MJ, Schuytema GS, Nebeker AV (2003) Behavioral responses of anuran larvae

to chemical cues of native and introduced predators in the Pacific Northwestern United

States. Journal of Herpetology 37: 572-576.

Polo-Cavia N, Gonzalo A, Lopez P, Martin J (2010) Predator recognition of native but not

invasive turtle predators by naive anuran tadpoles. Animal Behaviour 80: 461-466.

Relyea RA (2001) Morphological and behavioral plasticity of larval anurans in response to

different predators. Ecology 82: 523-540.

Relyea RA (2002) Local population differences in phenotypic plasticity: Predator-induced

changes in wood frog tadpoles. Ecological Monographs 72: 77-93.

Relyea RA (2007) Getting out alive: How predators affect the decision to metamorphose.

Oecologia 152: 389-400.

Richter-Boix A, Llorente GA, Montori A (2007) A comparative study of predator-induced

phenotype in tadpoles across a pond permanency gradient. Hydrobiologia 583: 43-56.

Rohlf FJ (2007) tpsRelw. Department of Ecology and Evolution. State Univ. New York, Stony

Brook.

Rohlf FJ (2008) tpsDig2. Department of Ecology and Evolution. State Univ. New York, Stony

Brook.

Rohlf FJ, Marcus LF (1993) A revolution in morphometrics. Trends in Ecology and Evolution 8:

129-132.

Saglio P, Mandrillon, AL (2006) Embryonic experience to predation risk affects tadpoles of the

common frog (Rana temporaria). Archiv für Hydrobiologie 166: 505-523.

Semlitsch RD, Gavasso S (1992) Behavioural responses of Bufo bufo and Bufo calamita tadpoles

to chemical cues of vertebrate and invertebrate predators. Ethology, Ecology and

Evolution 4: 165-173.

Page 159: PREDATOR-PREY INTERACTIONS BETWEEN AN INVASIVE …

Chapter 4

141

Sheets HD (2009) MakeFan7. Department of Physics, Canisius College, Buffalo.

Sih A, Bolnick DI, Luttbeg B, Orrock JL, Peacor SD, Pintor LM, Preisser E, Rehage JS, Vonesh JR

(2010) Predator-prey naiveté, antipredator behavior, and the ecology of predator

invasions. Oikos 119: 610-621.

Sih A, Ferrari MCO, Harris DJ (2011) Evolution and behavioural responses to human-induced

rapid environmental change. Evolutionary Applications 4: 367-387.

Spitze K (1992) Predator-mediated plasticity of prey life history and morphology: Chaoborus

americanus predation on Daphnia pulex. The American Naturalist 139: 229-247.

Steiner UK (2007) Linking antipredator behaviour, ingestion, gut evacuation and costs of

predator-induced responses in tadpoles. Animal Behaviour 74: 1473-1479.

Steiner UK, Pfeiffer T (2007) Optimizing time and resource allocation trade-offs for investment

in morphological and behavioral defense. The American Naturalist 169: 118-29.

Stoks R, Block MD, Van de Meutter F, Johansson F (2005) Predation cost of rapid growth:

behavioural coupling and physiological decoupling. Journal of Animal Ecology 74: 708-

715.

Teplitsky C, Plénet S, Joly P (2003) Tadpoles’ responses to risk of fish introduction. Oecologia

134: 270-277.

Urban MC (2007a) Risky prey behavior evolves in risky habitats. Proceedings of the Natural

Academy of Sciences USA 104: 14377-14382.

Urban MC (2007b) The growth-predation risk trade-off under a growing gape-limited predation

threat. Ecology 88: 2587-2597.

Van Buskirk J, Relyea RA (1998) Natural selection for phenotypic plasticity: Predator-induced

morphological responses in tadpoles. Biological Journal of the Linnean Society 65: 301-

328.

Van Buskirk J (2000) The costs of an inducible defense in anuran larvae. Ecology 81: 2813-2821.

Page 160: PREDATOR-PREY INTERACTIONS BETWEEN AN INVASIVE …

Chapter 4

142

Van Buskirk J, Anderwald P, Lupold S, Reinhardt L, Schuler H (2003) The lure effect, tadpole tail

shape, and the target of dragonfly strikes. Journal of Herpetology 37: 420-424.

Van Buskirk J (2009) Natural variation in morphology of larval amphibians: Phenotypic

plasticity in nature? Ecological Monographs 79: 681-705.

Werner EE (1986) Amphibian metamorphosis: growth rate, predation risk, and optimal size at

transformation. American Naturalist 128: 319-341.

Werner EE (1991) Nonlethal effects of a predator on competitive interactions between two

anuran larvae. Ecology 72: 1709-1720.

Werner EE, McPeek MA (1994) Direct and indirect effects of predators on two anuran species

along an environmental gradient. Ecology 75: 1368-1382.

Wilbur HM, Collins JP (1973) Ecological aspects of amphibian metamorphosis. Science 182:

1305-1314.

Witte F, Goldschmidt T, Wanink J, Van Oijen M, Goudswaard K, Witte-Maas E, Bouton N (1992)

The destruction of an endemic species flock: quantitative data on the decline of the

haplochromine cichlids of Lake Victoria. Environmental Biology of Fishes 34: 1-28.

Zelditch ML, Swiderski DL, Sheets HD, Fink WL (2004) Geometric morphometrics for biologists:

a primer. Academic Press.

Page 161: PREDATOR-PREY INTERACTIONS BETWEEN AN INVASIVE …

Chapter 4

143

Appendix 1. Location of the 20 landmarks digitized for estimating body shape of each tadpole

and detailed explanation of their position

12

34

5

6

7

8

9

10

11

12

13

14

15

16

17

18

1920

1 - Most anterior point of the headbody

2 - Centre of the eye

3 - Point of intersection between the headbody and the upper edge of the tail fin

4 - Point of the tail fin at two thirds of the distance between landmarks 1 and 9

5 - Point of the headbody (dorsal side) at two thirds of the distance between landmarks 1 and

9

6 - Point of the headbody (ventral side) at two thirds of the distance between landmarks 1 and

9

7 - Point of intersection between the headbody and the upper edge of the tail muscle

8 - Point of intersection between the headbody and the central line of the tail muscle

9 - Point of intersection between the headbody and the lower edge of the tail muscle

10 - Point of intersection between the anus and the lower edge of the tail fin

11 - Point of the tail fin (dorsal side) at one quarter of the distance between landmarks 8 and

20

12 - Point of the tail fin (ventral side) at one quarter of the distance between landmarks 8 and

20

13 - Point of the tail fin (dorsal side) at half distance between landmarks 8 and 20

14 - Point of the tail muscle (dorsal side) at half distance between landmarks 8 and 20

15 - Point of the tail muscle (ventral side) at half distance between landmarks 8 and 20

16 - Point of the tail fin (ventral side) at half distance between landmarks 8 and 20

17 - Point of the tail fin (dorsal side) at three quarters of the distance between landmarks 8

and 20

18 - Point of the tail fin (ventral side) at three quarters of the distance between landmarks 8

and 20

19 - Tip of the tail muscle

20 - Tip of the tail fin

Page 162: PREDATOR-PREY INTERACTIONS BETWEEN AN INVASIVE …

144

Page 163: PREDATOR-PREY INTERACTIONS BETWEEN AN INVASIVE …

145

CHAPTER 5

Population divergence in antipredator defences

against an invasive predator: does coexistence time

promote the evolution of adaptive responses?

Nunes AL, Orizaola G, Laurila A, Rebelo R

Manuscript

Page 164: PREDATOR-PREY INTERACTIONS BETWEEN AN INVASIVE …

146

Page 165: PREDATOR-PREY INTERACTIONS BETWEEN AN INVASIVE …

Chapter 5

147

Population divergence in antipredator defences against an invasive

predator: does coexistence time promote the evolution of adaptive

responses?

Abstract

Invasive alien predators can impose strong selection on native prey populations and induce

rapid evolutionary change in the invaded communities. The red swamp crayfish Procambarus

clarkii has invaded most freshwater habitats in the Iberian Peninsula, where it preys on native

amphibian larvae. We raised Iberian waterfrog (Pelophylax perezi) tadpoles from populations

differing in historical exposure to P. clarkii (30 years, 20 years or no coexistence) and examined

how coexistence time shaped antipredator responses in different P. perezi populations. We

show that tadpoles from non-invaded populations reduced activity level in the presence of P.

clarkii, whereas long-term invaded populations did not. Further, tadpoles from one of the long-

term invaded populations responded to the crayfish with inducible morphological defences

(shorter headbody and deeper tail). These results suggest that not reducing activity level and

inducing morphological defences are likely adaptive responses to the predation pressure

imposed by this crayfish. Although quite recent (ca. 32 years), it is possible that the strong

predation pressure imposed by P. clarkii has selected for rapid evolution of morphological

responses in populations of P. perezi and accounts for the divergence in antipredator defences

found between different populations. The considerable among-population variation found in

prey responses to an invasive predator stresses that integrative approaches may be especially

relevant to understand the outcomes of predator-prey interactions in biological invasions.

Keywords: Historical coexistence, invasive predator, antipredator defences, population

divergence, rapid evolution

Page 166: PREDATOR-PREY INTERACTIONS BETWEEN AN INVASIVE …

Chapter 5

148

Introduction

Predation is an important selective force acting on the fitness of prey organisms, and there is

strong selection on prey populations to evolve traits that enhance their ability to escape or

deter predation (Lima and Dill 1990). Prey develop a wide variety of inducible antipredator

strategies which involve modifications in behaviour, morphology and life-history that reduce

their vulnerability to predators (Lima and Dill 1990, Benard 2004). Antipredator behaviours can

consist of changing microhabitat use, aggregating, using refuges, spatially avoiding the

predator and, most commonly, reducing activity levels; these alterations are adaptive as they

reduce predator-prey encounter rates and prey detectability (Lima and Dill 1990, Kats and Dill

1998). Morphological defences typically consist of defensive structures, such as spines,

changes in body shape or shell thickness, which increase the probability of escaping an attack

and enhance the chances of survival (e.g. Spitze 1992, Trussel and Smith 2000, Relyea 2001).

Successful predator recognition and effective avoidance strategies typically occur when

predator and prey have a common evolutionary history (Strauss et al. 2006, Sih et al. 2010).

Nowadays, invasive alien predators are introduced into new environments at an

unprecedented rate, resulting in new predator-prey interactions between organisms sharing

no evolutionary history (Thompson 1998, Strauss et al. 2006). Invaded prey populations often

suffer dramatic declines or even extinctions due to their inability to respond with appropriate

antipredator defences (Cox and Lima 2006, Strauss et al. 2006, Sih et al. 2010). However, by

imposing strong selection pressure upon native organisms, invasive predators may induce

rapid evolutionary changes in native species, through which prey evolve the ability to

recognise and respond to them (Schlaepfer et al. 2005, Strauss et al. 2006, Sih et al. 2010). For

rapid adaptation to occur, selection pressure imposed by the exotic predator has to be

sufficiently strong and consistent, and native populations have to be genetically variable in

susceptibility to the predator (Strauss et al. 2006). Natural selection will favour prey individuals

Page 167: PREDATOR-PREY INTERACTIONS BETWEEN AN INVASIVE …

Chapter 5

149

that can detect and exhibit predator avoidance in response to the novel predator. Many

examples of rapid directional adaptive evolution in native prey species in response to

introduced predators have been reported in several taxa including molluscs, crustaceans, fish,

amphibians and reptiles (Kiesecker and Blaustein 1997, Losos et al. 2004, O’Steen et al. 2002,

Freeman and Byers 2006, Fisk et al. 2007).

However, some prey species are able to ‘innately’ detect and elicit adaptive responses to

stimuli of novel predators in the absence of a common evolutionary history (Epp and Gabor

2008, Peluc et al. 2008, Sündermann et al. 2008, Rehage et al. 2009). This ability may be due to

the latter’s phylogenetic or phenotypic similarity to native predators co-occurring with the

prey (Epp and Gabor 2008, Rehage et al. 2009). Prey may also exhibit generalized antipredator

responses, whereby exposure to predation risk induces the development of defences,

regardless of previous direct experience with a particular predator (e.g. response to a general

fish odour or to a large moving object; Magurran 1990, Gherardi et al. 2011). ‘Innate’

responses of native prey to invasive predators can also be facilitated if detection of predators

relies on general predation cues, such as cues associated with the predator’s diet or

disturbance/damage alarm cues released by startled or attacked prey (Sündermann et al.

2008, Ferrari et al. 2010). Alternatively to ‘innate’ responses, prey may learn to identify and

respond to alien predators through experience by pairing novel predator scents with cues

released by attacked or consumed conspecifics. Hence, prey may learn to ‘label’ exotic

predators as a threat respond to them (review in Ferrari et al. 2010).

Freshwater environments, which have been widely invaded by exotic species, are particularly

vulnerable to introduced predators (Cox and Lima 2006). One of the most conspicuous non-

native predator species introduced worldwide is the red swamp crayfish (Procambarus clarkii),

originally endemic from southcentral USA and northeastern Mexico (Hobbs et al. 1989,

Gherardi 2006). This invasive species, which was first recorded in the Iberian Peninsula in 1973,

has rapidly spread throughout the area and is now an abundant species in most freshwater

Page 168: PREDATOR-PREY INTERACTIONS BETWEEN AN INVASIVE …

Chapter 5

150

habitats all over the Peninsula (Holdich et al. 2009). P. clarkii is an extraordinarily successful

invader, often altering the dynamics of the ecosystems it colonises (e.g. Geiger et al. 2005). It is

an efficient predator of amphibian larvae known to have strong negative impacts on Iberian

amphibians (Cruz and Rebelo 2005, Cruz et al. 2008).

Tadpoles of the Iberian waterfrog, Pelophylax perezi, exhibit typical behavioural (reduced

activity level) and morphological (shorter and deeper headbodies and deeper tails) plastic

antipredator responses when exposed to chemical stimuli of native aquatic predators (Gonzalo

et al. 2007, Richter-Boix et al. 2007, Polo-Cavia et al. 2010, Gomez-Mestre and Diaz-Paniagua

2011, Chapters 3 and 4). These behavioural and morphological alterations are common

responses of larval anurans to native caged predators, which are known to increase the odds

of survival in predator presence (Skelly 1994, Van Buskirk and Relyea 1998, Teplitsky et al.

2005). In the presence of P. clarkii, however, three studies did not detect behavioural or

morphological responses of P. perezi to the predator (Gomez-Mestre and Diaz-Paniagua 2011,

Chapters 3 and 4). Nevertheless, Gomez-Mestre and Diaz-Paniagua (2011) found that induced

morphological defences elicited by P. perezi in the presence of a native larval dragonfly

predator (a deeper and more pigmented tail) increased tadpole survival in the presence of P.

clarkii.

In this study, using five populations of P. perezi that differ in coexistence time with P. clarkii,

we investigated if coexistence with the introduced predator affects the ability to respond to

this invasive crayfish. We compared behavioural and morphological responses of five

populations from three areas characterized by different exposure times to P. clarkii: longer (ca.

30 yr), shorter (ca. 20 yr) or no coexistence time with this predator. Assuming that P. clarkii

imposes strong selection on P. perezi populations, we expect that tadpoles from invaded

populations will show antipredator defences to the crayfish (such as adaptive alterations in

morphology), while individuals from non-invaded areas will show no responses to this

predator.

Page 169: PREDATOR-PREY INTERACTIONS BETWEEN AN INVASIVE …

Chapter 5

151

Methods

Study populations

P. perezi is the most abundant and widely distributed anuran in Portugal (Loureiro et al. 2008).

It inhabits a wide variety of aquatic habitats, but prefers permanent water bodies (Almeida et

al. 2001). It is commonly found in both P. clarkii invaded and non-invaded areas.

We selected populations of P. perezi from southern Portugal, the region where P. clarkii was

first recorded in Portugal and where no native crayfishes exist (Ramos and Pereira 1979,

Almaça 1991). P. clarkii is currently widespread over large areas throughout Portugal, and we

were not able to find a region where the three different types of populations could be found.

Hence, we collected populations with different coexistence times with crayfish from three

geographically distinct areas. We first chose P. perezi populations from the area of the Caia

River, an affluent of the Guadiana River (Table 1, Fig.1). This is the area where P. clarkii was

first recorded in Portugal in 1979 (Ramos and Pereira 1979), and populations from Caia are

probably the ones in Portugal which have been exposed to P. clarkii for the longest time

(Longer Coexistence time – LC). Two study populations 34 km apart from each other were

collected in this region (Fig.1). Secondly, we collected one population in Melides, an area

which was invaded by the crayfish around 1990 (R. Rebelo pers. obs.; Table 1, Fig.1). P. perezi

populations from this area have coexisted with crayfish for a shorter time than those from the

Caia region (Shorter Coexistence time – SC). This region is characterized by extensive areas of

rice culture, where the crayfish can attain very high densities. Finally, we collected P. perezi

populations that have never been in contact with P. clarkii (No Coexistence – NC) in the

Monchique region (Table 1, Fig.1). This is a mountain range located in the far South of

Portugal, where strong water flow and topography have probably so far prevented invasion by

P. clarkii (Gil-Sánchez and Alba-Tercedor 2002, Kerby et al. 2005, Cruz and Rebelo 2007).

Repeated surveys using baited traps during 2009 proved this area still devoid of crayfish (A.L.

Page 170: PREDATOR-PREY INTERACTIONS BETWEEN AN INVASIVE …

Chapter 5

152

Nunes and R. Rebelo, unpubl.). In this region, we collected two different study populations

situated 12 km apart and located in different hydrographic basins, separated topographically

by a marked ridge (Table 1, Fig.1).

All study populations were collected from slow-flowing sections of streams harbouring a rich

fauna of native predators (insects, native fish, and snakes). Trapping and visual observations

confirmed that dense P. clarkii populations coexisted with LC and SC populations.

Table 1. Area, coordinates, elevation above sea level of collection sites, type of water body,

approximate coexistence time between Procambarus clarkii and Pelophylax perezi and dates of

collection and of the start of the experiment for each of the five studied populations of P. perezi

Population LC1 LC2 SC NC1 NC2

Area Caia Caia Melides Monchique Monchique

Type of water body

Rice field on the bank of the

Caia river Caia river

Melides stream and rice fields

Monchique river

Affluent of the Seixe river

Coordinates 38º52’N 7º03’W

39º07’N 7º17’W

38º08’N 8º44’W

37º17’N 8º33’W

37º22’N 8º38’W

Elevation a.s.l 170 m 270 m 25 m 200 m 100 m

Approximate coexistence time

32 years 32 years 21 years 0 years 0 years

Date of collection

10-June 16-June 08-July 18-June 18-June

Number of clutches collected

8 10 10 9 12

Start of experiment

02-July 02-July 17-August 04-August 05-August

Page 171: PREDATOR-PREY INTERACTIONS BETWEEN AN INVASIVE …

Chapter 5

153

Fig. 1. Map of Portugal with the areas of origin of each study population (LC – Longer Coexistence

populations, SC – Shorter Coexistence population and NC – No Coexistence populations). The black

triangle indicates the location where P. clarkii was first introduced in Spain and the white triangle

indicates the site where crayfish presence was first registered in Portugal. Grey areas indicate regions

700 m a.s.l.

Experimental procedure

During June and July 2009, we collected several egg masses of Iberian waterfrog from each of

the abovementioned populations (Table 1). The eggs were taken to the laboratory in the field

station of Centro de Biologia Ambiental in Grândola (38º06’N, 8º34’W), where the experiments

took place. Clutches were kept in different 1L containers filled with well water until tadpoles

reached Gosner developmental stage 25 (Gosner 1960), when the experiment was initiated.

Adult crayfish were caught in streams close to the field station using baited funnel traps. While

not in the experiment, the crayfish were kept in 28 × 20 × 14cm containers and fed commercial

fish food ad libitum.

Page 172: PREDATOR-PREY INTERACTIONS BETWEEN AN INVASIVE …

Chapter 5

154

To examine the effects of crayfish chemical cues on P. perezi antipredator defences, we used a

factorial design crossing population origin with two predator treatments. Each combination

was replicated once in each of five randomized blocks. Populations represented different

coexistence times with the exotic crayfish: Longer Coexistence time (LC1 and LC2), Shorter

Coexistence time (SC) and No Coexistence (NC1 and NC2) (Table 1). The two predator

treatments were the presence or absence of a single caged crayfish predator. Each

experimental unit consisted of a 39 × 28 × 28cm container filled with 10L of well water, ten

tadpoles and a suspended predator cage. Predator cages were opaque plastic cylinders

(diameter 85 mm, length 16 cm) with top and bottom covered with fine mesh netting (1.5

mm), allowing the tadpoles to receive chemical, but not visual or tactile cues from the crayfish.

Containers in the no-predator treatment had an empty cage. Crayfish were fed three P. perezi

tadpoles every other day, and the tadpoles were fed commercial fish food and boiled lettuce

ad libitum every three days. Water was changed weekly. Photoperiod was kept 12D:12N

throughout the experiment and the water temperature was 21.0 ± 0.08 ºC (mean ± SE).

Response variables

We recorded tadpole activity at the beginning of the experiment (day 10) and in the middle of

the larval development (day 60). The number of active tadpoles in each tank was recorded

between 9 and 12 am in five sequential moments, each observation separated by at least 30

minutes. Tadpoles were considered active if they were swimming, foraging or moving their

tails.

Tadpole morphology was recorded at day 60 of the experiment by placing each tadpole in a

small Plexiglas box with a grid background and taking a side-view digital photograph. We used

geometric morphometrics tools to examine variation in tadpole morphology (Rohlf and Marcus

1993, Zelditch et al. 2004). Photographs of five random individuals per container were loaded

Page 173: PREDATOR-PREY INTERACTIONS BETWEEN AN INVASIVE …

Chapter 5

155

into MakeFan7 (Sheets 2009) to produce deformation grids describing tadpole body shape. In

each tadpole 19 landmarks representing an adequate coverage of the tadpole body shape

were marked using the image analysis software tpsDig2 (Rohlf 2008; Fig. 2). These landmarks

were chosen based on previous studies (Orizaola et al. 2011, Chapter 4). In order to extract

tadpole shape variables, photographs with landmark coordinates were imported into tpsRelw

(Rohlf 2007). This software allowed us to generate a mean (consensus) landmark configuration

for each experimental container, described by several components (relative warps), which

were later used as shape variables in the statistical analysis. The two first relative warps (RW1

and RW2) explained 54.78% of the initial morphological variance (Fig.4) and were used as

dependent variables in the analyses.

Fig. 2. The 19 landmarks digitized for estimating body-shape in each tadpole.

Statistical analysis

Variation in antipredator responses among populations was analysed for activity level and

morphology. For activity, we performed a Generalized Linear Model (GLM), using a Binomial

error distribution and a logit link function. The total number of active tadpoles per container

summed for the five observations in each time period was used as the dependent variable and

the total number of tadpoles pooled over the five observations in each container as the trials

variable. As activity was registered in two different time periods, a repeated measures

approach was used. Pairwise comparisons were performed to inspect differences among

populations. To examine differences in morphology of different populations, we used

Page 174: PREDATOR-PREY INTERACTIONS BETWEEN AN INVASIVE …

Chapter 5

156

univariate analyses of variance (ANOVAs) followed by Tukey HSD post-hoc tests. Population

was always used as a fixed factor because study populations were selected based on the time

of coexistence with P. clarkii. Since we found differences between populations and a

significant (or nearly significant) difference for the population × treatment effect (see Results),

GLMs (activity) or univariate ANOVAs (morphology) were performed for each population

separately. Initially, block was used as a random factor and mixed models were performed.

However, this factor was always non-significant (P >0.1) and it was removed from the final

models. All analyses were performed using the software package SPSS (version 19.0).

Results

Activity level increased as tadpole development progressed (Table 2, Fig.3). A significant

interaction between time period and population was detected, indicating that activity varied

differently among populations throughout tadpole development (Table 2, Fig.3). Further, we

found significant differences in activity level among different populations. Populations LC1 and

LC2 had lower activity than the other populations (pairwise comparisons, P< 0.05; Table 2,

Fig.3). Populations NC1 and NC2 had similar activity levels and population SC had higher

activity than all the other populations (pairwise comparisons, P> 0.05; Table 2, Fig.3). A

significant interaction between population and predator treatment indicated that populations

differed in their responses to the crayfish (Table 2). When looking at each population

separately, early in larval development, only population SC reduced activity in the presence of

crayfish cues. Later in ontogeny, the two NC populations strongly reduced their activity levels

in predator presence, while LC and SC populations showed no alterations in activity (Table 2,

Fig.3).

Page 175: PREDATOR-PREY INTERACTIONS BETWEEN AN INVASIVE …

Chapter 5

157

Table 2. Generalized Linear Models for the activity level of P. perezi populations when exposed to

control or crayfish treatments, in the two time periods. Left-hand panel shows analysis for all

populations and right-hand panel analysis for each population separately. df= 4,77 for all the factors

involving the ‘population’ term and df= 1,77 for all the other factors. P-values < 0.05 are marked in

boldface

Activity level Period 1 Period 2

Wald χ2 P Wald χ

2 P Wald χ

2 P

Period 29.486 < 0.001

Period × Population 20.891 < 0.001 LC1 0.150 0.698 0.419 0.517

Period × Predator Treatment 4.182 0.041 LC2 0.001 0.969 0.430 0.512

Period × Pop × Pred Treat 7.397 0.116 SC 8.673 0.003 1.739 0.187

Population 105.418 < 0.001 NC1 0.394 0.530 19.926 < 0.001

Predator Treatment 13.689 < 0.001 NC2 2.967 0.085 23.002 < 0.001

Pop × Pred Treat 9.842 0.043

Fig. 3. Proportion of active tadpoles (mean ± SE) of the five studied populations when exposed to

control (white square) or crayfish (black square) treatments, in the two time periods (LCs – Long

Coexistence populations, NCs – No Coexistence populations and SC – Short Coexistence population).

Page 176: PREDATOR-PREY INTERACTIONS BETWEEN AN INVASIVE …

Chapter 5

158

We found significant differences between populations in morphological traits, (F4,36 = 20.185,

P< 0.001 for RW1 and F4,36 = 8.240, P< 0.001 for RW2). Population LC2 did not differ from

populations LC1 and NC2 and population NC1 had a similar morphology to population NC2.

Population SC was different from all the other populations except LC1, having longer and wider

headbodies, wider tails and a more anterior insertion of the tail fin to the headbody (post-hoc

tests, P< 0.05; Fig.4).

-0.03

-0.02

-0.01

0.00

0.01

0.02

0.03

RW

1 =

39.7

4%

a)

No predator Crayfish

LC1 LC2 SC NC1 NC2

-0.02

-0.01

0.00

0.01

0.02

0.03

RW

2 =

15.0

4%

b)

LC1 LC2 SC NC1 NC2

Population

Population

Fig. 4. Mean ± SE values of a) Relative Warp 1 (RW1) and b) Relative Warp 2 (RW2) representing

tadpoles shape of the five populations in the presence of control (white square) or crayfish (black

square) treatments (LCs – Long Coexistence populations, NCs – No Coexistence populations and SC –

Short Coexistence population). Drawings on the right hand side of each graph show the shape of larvae

representing the extreme positive (upper panel) and negative (lower panel) scores of RW1 or RW2.

Page 177: PREDATOR-PREY INTERACTIONS BETWEEN AN INVASIVE …

Chapter 5

159

There was a non significant tendency towards an interaction between the factors ‘population’

and ‘predator treatment’ in RW2 (F4,36 = 2.129, P= 0.097), while this did not happen for RW1

(F4,36 = 0.813, P= 0.525). When looking at populations separately, population LC1 showed

significant alterations in RW2, with tadpoles developing smaller headbodies, deeper tail

muscles and a more anterior insertion of the tail fin into the headbody in the presence of

crayfish (F1,6 = 10.489, P= 0.018; Fig.4); no evidence for alterations in morphology was found in

other populations or in RW1.

Discussion

We found that tadpoles of P. perezi respond to chemical cues of P. clarkii. Specifically, tadpoles

from one of the populations with a long history of contact with P. clarkii (henceforth LC)

showed morphological plasticity in crayfish presence, while there was a reduction in activity in

response to the crayfish in no coexistence time populations (NC). Our results suggest that the

length of coexistence time with the invasive predator has an important role in defining the

type of responses elicited. Further, previous studies have failed to detect responses of P. perezi

to P. clarkii (Gomez-Mestre and Diaz-Paniagua 2011, Chapter 3), indicating that there is

considerable variation among local populations in the ability to induce plastic responses

towards this exotic predator.

The plastic responses to P. clarkii differed between populations coexisting with the crayfish

and populations that have never been in contact with it. NC populations greatly reduced their

activity levels (in time period 2) in the presence of crayfish; this is a common adaptive defence

against predators (Lima and Dill 1990, Kats and Dill 1998). However, coexistence populations

generally did not alter their activity level in the presence of the crayfish. The hypothesis that

baseline activity level in LC populations is so low that a reduction in its level would not be

possible is unlikely, as both this (Polo-Cavia et al. 2010, Gomez-Mestre and Diaz-Paniagua

Page 178: PREDATOR-PREY INTERACTIONS BETWEEN AN INVASIVE …

Chapter 5

160

2011, Chapter 3) and other anuran species with low baseline activity (e.g. Richardson 2001,

Laurila et al. 2006) are able to reduce their activity in response to chemical cues from other

predators. We suggest two different hypotheses to explain the lack of behavioural responses

in populations coexisting with crayfish.

First, given that the LC populations had quite low baseline activity levels, this lack of response

could suggest the existence of a constitutive behavioural defence in these populations, which

may evolve under uniform and strong predation risk (Moran 1992, Edgell et al. 2009). In this

case, strong selection by P. clarkii would have promoted the loss of behavioural plasticity in

these populations, leading to permanently well defended inactive tadpoles, irrespective of the

presence of crayfish in the environment. However, we do not think this is the most likely

hypothesis because the population with a short coexistence time with crayfish (SC) had high

baseline activity level, yet it only reduced activity in crayfish presence early in ontogeny.

Alternatively, we suggest that reducing activity is not the most effective defence against this

predator and that not altering activity may be an evolved adaptive response to the predator’s

presence. Although some previous studies have documented a reduction in prey activity in

crayfish presence (Bryan et al. 2002, Marquis et al. 2004, Mandrillon and Saglio 2005, Chapter

3), many others have reported no alterations or an increase in prey activity level in the

presence of crayfish (Rahel and Stein 1988, Lefcort 1996, Åbjörnsson et al. 2000, Nyström et al.

2001, Correia et al. 2005, Leite et al. 2005, Hoverman and Relyea 2008). In fact, this defence

may not be very effective against P. clarkii which is a moderately active predator (Gomez-

Mestre and Diaz-Panigua 2011) and relies more on chemical rather than visual cues to detect

prey (Aquiloni et al. 2005). This may put inactive tadpoles at the same risk as active ones and

make swimming ability more important than inactivity in avoiding crayfish predators (Lefcort

1996, Nyström et al. 1999). Even though in their case tadpole activity was recorded in the

presence of a free-ranging crayfish, Cruz and Rebelo (2005) did not find any relationship

between activity rates or plasticity in activity and survival to P. clarkii. Future studies

Page 179: PREDATOR-PREY INTERACTIONS BETWEEN AN INVASIVE …

Chapter 5

161

investigating more thoroughly if induced behavioural alterations are adaptive under direct

predation by the crayfish are clearly needed.

When exposed to crayfish, tadpoles from one of the longer coexistence populations (LC1)

altered morphology by developing a smaller headbody, a deeper tail muscle and a more

anterior insertion of the tail fin into the upper part of the headbody, all of which are common

adaptive morphological alterations of larval anurans to predators (McCollum and Van Buskirk

1996, Relyea 2001, Teplitsky et al. 2005, Van Buskirk 2009). Gomez-Mestre and Diaz-Paniagua

(2011) showed that similar morphological alterations - induced by dragonflies - are efficient in

decreasing P. perezi mortality imposed by P. clarkii. This suggests that the morphological

response to P. clarkii in LC1 is adaptive (as is the lack of response in behaviour) and that this

population has adapted locally by evolving morphological defences against the invasive

crayfish. This result agrees with the idea that invasive species can be a strong selective force

generating local adaptation and rapid evolution (Strauss et al. 2006, Sih et al. 2010). While our

study reports the fastest evolution of predator defences documented for an amphibian (ca. 32

years, 10-15 P. perezi generations), there are several studies reporting rapid evolution in

response to selection from invading predators (Schlaepfer et al. 2005, Freeman and Byers

2006, Strauss et al. 2006), including one case in amphibians (Kiesecker and Blaustein 1997).

Considering this, we find surprising the fact that LC2 did not respond with morphological

defences to crayfish chemical stimuli (despite a tendency for that). We hypothesize this may

be because LC1 is located at a rice field in a bank of the Caia River, very close to where it meets

the Guadiana River (see Fig.1), the river through which P. clarkii likely expanded into Portugal,

before colonizing the Caia river (Ramos and Pereira 1981). Further, rice fields are one of the

preferred habitats of the crayfish in the Iberian Peninsula, where they attain very high

densities (Anastácio et al. 1995, Fidalgo et al. 2001). This suggests that LC1 may have coexisted

for a slightly longer time with more stable populations of P. clarkii than LC2. Alternatively, this

Page 180: PREDATOR-PREY INTERACTIONS BETWEEN AN INVASIVE …

Chapter 5

162

may indicate differences between the populations in the intensity of selection imposed by the

crayfish or differences in the evolutionary potential of the populations.

Interestingly, Gomez-Mestre and Díaz-Paniagua (2011) did not find morphological responses of

P. perezi to P. clarkii in a population from Doñana National Park where P. clarkii was found

already in 1974. While we cannot exclude population differences in intensity of selection or in

P. perezi’s potential to evolve responses to P. clarkii, the discrepancy between those and the

present results may be explained by the more robust approach used to examine tadpole

morphology in the present study.

The fact that NC populations altered behaviour in crayfish presence indicates the detection of

a threat by these populations and the ability of tadpoles to elicit defences towards a predator

with which they have had no previous evolutionary contact. This suggests an ‘innate’ response

of these populations to the introduced crayfish predator. Many species innately recognise and

respond to chemical stimuli from native predators (review in Wisenden 2003), but examples of

‘innate’ recognition of exotic predators in amphibians are not common (but see Epp and Gabor

2008, Rehage et al. 2009). Naive populations responding to unknown predators usually share

an evolutionary history with native predators closely related to the novel predator (Epp and

Gabor 2008, Rehage et al. 2009). Austropotamobius pallipes, the only native crayfish species in

Portugal, is now extinct across the Portuguese territory. Its original geographical range did not

reach southern Portugal (Almaça 1991), suggesting that the responses of NC populations are

not likely due to the phylogenetic similarity to a native predator in the community (Strauss et

al. 2006). Instead, the responses may have been facilitated by the detection of cues from

predation events, resulting from injured or digested conspecifics (Ferrari et al. 2010) and/or by

an innate antipredator response towards generalized predator signals (Gherardi et al. 2011).

The fact that crayfish presence induced a similarly strong behavioural response in the two NC

populations reinforces the idea of a general response towards predators or diet cues,

independent of the specific predatory threat.

Page 181: PREDATOR-PREY INTERACTIONS BETWEEN AN INVASIVE …

Chapter 5

163

The differences between coexistence and no-coexistence populations may be related to a

differential perception of different populations in the available predation cues present in our

experimental containers. Tadpoles may require more complete information about both the

predator species and the predator diet (not only the predator diet alone) before investing in

longer-term and less reversible morphological defences (Relyea 2003, Schoeppner and Relyea

2005, Orizaola et al. 2011). By having locally adapted to P. clarkii, coexistence populations (or

at least LC1) may be able to detect and respond to specific kairomones from this predator. In

contrast, NC populations probably lack this ability, having only detected alarm or diet cues

resulting from predation events and eliciting short-term and more reversible behavioural

defences.

We found significant among-population variation according to populations’ geographical area

of origin. There was a clear geographical pattern of phenotypic divergence in larval behaviour

and morphology between populations from Caia (LC) and Monchique (NC), the former being

less active and usually having deeper headbodies and tails than the latter, suggesting either

genetic population differentiation or maternal effects. By influencing embryo size and energy,

maternal investment may greatly affect an individual’s phenotype, especially through

alterations in growth rate (Laugen et al. 2002). Genetic differences may result from adaptation

to local habitat features such as temperature, canopy cover, native predators, competitors or

food availability (Relyea 2002, Van Buskirk and Arioli 2005, Richter-Boix et al. 2010). However,

we note that the populations from the two regions experience quite similar native predator

regimes (diverse and dense populations of predators), and large population differences in

native predation pressure are not likely to be important here. Studies using multiple

populations from several different geographical areas can definitely determine the role of

rapid evolution in favouring local adaptation in populations invaded by P. clarkii.

This study highlights the importance of studying population variation in antipredator defences

towards introduced predators and suggests that invasive alien predators may drive rapid

Page 182: PREDATOR-PREY INTERACTIONS BETWEEN AN INVASIVE …

Chapter 5

164

evolutionary change in invaded communities. Taking into account its enormous invasion

potential in the Iberian Peninsula (Capinha and Anastácio 2011), P. clarkii is likely to colonize

Monchique and other crayfish-free regions in the future. This study shows that some invaded

prey populations do have the potential to locally adapt to the crayfish presence. However,

given the strong selection pressure that this predator imposes (Cruz and Rebelo 2005, Cruz et

al. 2008, Nunes et al. 2010, Ficetola et al. 2011), other invaded populations or populations not

yet invaded may not be able to evolve defensive adaptations rapidly enough to hinder strong

declines or even extinction. As amphibian species appear to differ in their ability to respond to

P. clarkii (Chapters 3 and 4), future studies should span populations of several species in order

to identify the ones at most risk.

Acknowledgments

We thank Bruno Carreira, Rosário Sumares, João Nunes and Sónia Longueira for their help in

the laboratory. We also thank Rui Braz for designing the map of P. perezi populations. Permits

were provided by the Portuguese Instituto da Conservação da Natureza e da Biodiversidade

(Licence n.º228/2009/CAPT/TRANS). This research was funded by the FCT Project POCI/BIA-

BDE/56100/2004, FCT grant SFRH/BD/29068/2006 and by Stiftelsen för Zoologisk Forskning.

References

Åbjörnsson K, Dahl J, Nyström P, Brönmark C (2000) Influence of predator and dietary chemical

cues on the behaviour and shredding efficiency of Gammarus pulex. Aquatic Ecology 34:

379-387.

Almaça C (1991) L’ecrevisse a pieds blancs, Astacus pallipes Lereboullet 1858, au Portugal.

L’Astaciculteur de France 28: 11-16.

Page 183: PREDATOR-PREY INTERACTIONS BETWEEN AN INVASIVE …

Chapter 5

165

Almeida NF, Almeida P, Gonçalves H, Sequeira F, Teixeira J and Almeida F (2001) Guias FAPAS:

Anfíbios e Répteis de Portugal. FAPAS and Câmara Municipal do Porto, Porto: 249 pp.

Anastácio PM, Nielsen SN, Marques JC, Jorgensen SE (1995) Integrated production of crayfish

and rice: a management model. Ecological Engineering 4: 199-210.

Aquiloni L, Ilhéu M, Gherardi F (2005) Habitat use and dispersal of the invasive crayfish

Procambarus clarkii in ephemeral water bodies of Portugal. Marine and Freshwater

Behaviour and Physiology 38: 225-236.

Benard MF (2004) Predator-induced phenotypic plasticity in organisms with complex life

cycles. Annual Review of Ecology, Evolution and Systematics 35: 651-673.

Bryan SD, Robinson AT, Sweetser MG (2002) Behavioral responses of a small native fish to

multiple introduced predators. Environmental Biology of Fishes 63: 49-56.

Capinha C, Anastácio P (2011) Assessing the environmental requirements of invaders using

ensembles of distribution models. Diversity and Distributions 17: 13-24.

Correia AM, Bandeira N, Anastácio PM (2005) Predator-prey interactions of Procambarus

clarkii with aquatic macroinvertebrates in single and multiple prey systems. Acta

Oecologica 28: 337-343.

Cox JG, Lima SL (2006) Naiveté and an aquatic-terrestrial dichotomy in the effects of

introduced predators. Trends in Ecology and Evolution 21: 674-680.

Cruz MJ, Rebelo R (2005) Vulnerability of southwest Iberian amphibians to an introduced

crayfish, Procambarus clarkii. Amphibia-Reptilia 26: 293-303.

Cruz MJ, Rebelo R (2007) Colonization of freshwater habitats by an introduced crayfish,

Procambarus clarkii, in Southwest Iberian Peninsula. Hydrobiologia 575: 191-201.

Cruz MJ, Segurado P, Sousa M, Rebelo R (2008) Collapse of the amphibian community of

thePaul do Boquilobo Natural Reserve (central Portugal) after the arrival of the exotic

American crayfish Procambarus clarkii. Herpetological Journal 18: 197-204.

Page 184: PREDATOR-PREY INTERACTIONS BETWEEN AN INVASIVE …

Chapter 5

166

Edgell TC, Lynch BR, Trussell GC, Palmer AR (2009) Experimental evidence for the rapid

evolution of behavioral canalization in natural populations. American Naturalist 174:

434-440.

Epp KJ, Gabor CR (2008) Innate and learned predator recognition mediated by chemical signals

in Eurycea nana. Ethology 114: 607-615.

Ferrari MCO, Wisenden BD, Chivers DP (2010) Chemical ecology of predator-prey interactions

in aquatic ecosystems: a review and prospectus. Canadian Journal of Zoology 88: 698-

724.

Ficetola FG, Siesa ME, Manenti R, Bottoni L, De Bernardi F, Padoa-Schioppa E (2011) Early

assessment of the impact of alien species: differential consequences of an invasive

crayfish on adult and larval amphibians. Diversity and Distributions, in press.

Fidalgo ML, Carvalho AP, Santos P (2001) Population dynamics of the red swamp crayfish,

Procambarus clarkii (Girard, 1852) from the Aveiro region, Portugal (Decapoda,

Cambaridae). Crustaceana 74: 369-375.

Fisk DF, Latta LC, Knapp RA, Pfrender ME (2007) Rapid evolution in response to introduced

predators I: rates and patterns of morphological and life-history trait divergence. BMC

Evolutionary Biology 7: 22.

Freeman AS, Byers JE (2006) Divergent Induced Responses to an Invasive Predator in Marine

Mussel Populations. Science 313: 831-833.

Geiger W, Alcorlo P, Baltanás A and Montes C (2005) Impact of an introduced Crustacean on

the trophic webs of Mediterranean wetlands. Biological Invasions 7: 49-73.

Gherardi F (2006) Crayfish invading Europe: the case study of Procambarus clarkii. Marine and

Freshwater Behavioral Physiology 39: 175-191.

Gherardi F, Mavuti KM, Pacini N, Tricarico E and Harper DM (2011) The smell of danger:

chemical recognition of fish predators by the invasive crayfish Procambarus clarkii.

Freshwater Biology 56: 1567-1578.

Page 185: PREDATOR-PREY INTERACTIONS BETWEEN AN INVASIVE …

Chapter 5

167

Gil-Sánchez JM, Alba-Tercedor J (2002) Ecology of the native and introduced crayfishes

Austropotamobius pallipes and Procambarus clarkii in southern Spain and implications

for conservation of the native species. Biological Conservation 105: 75-80.

Gomez-Mestre I, Díaz-Paniagua C (2011) Invasive predatory crayfish do not trigger inducible

defences in tadpoles. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B, in press.

Gonzalo A, López P and Martín J (2007) Iberian green frog tadpoles may learn to recognize

novel predators from chemical alarm cues of conspecifics. Animal Behaviour 74: 447-

453.

Gosner KL (1960) A simplified table for staging anuran embryos and larvae with notes on

identification. Herpetologica 16: 183-190.

Hobbs HH, Jass JP, Huner JV (1989) A review of global crayfish introductions with particular

emphasis on two North American species. Crustaceana 56: 299-316.

Holdich DM, Reynolds JD, Souty-Grosset C, Sibley PJ (2009) A review of the ever increasing

threat to European crayfish from non-indigenous crayfish species. Knowledge and

Management of Aquatic Ecosystems 394-395: 11.

Hoverman JT, Relyea RA (2008) Temporal environmental variation and phenotypic plasticity: a

mechanism underlying priority effects. Oikos 117:23-32.

Kats LB, Dill LM (1998) The scent of death: chemosensory assessment of predation risk by prey

animals. Ecoscience 5: 361-394.

Kerby JL, Riley SPD, Kats LB, Wilson P (2005) Barriers and flow as limiting factors in the spread

of an invasive crayfish (Procambarus clarkii) in southern California streams. Biological

Conservation 126: 402-409.

Kiesecker DG, Blaustein AR (1997) Population differences in responses of red-legged frogs

(Rana aurora) to introduced bullfrogs (Rana catesbeiana). Ecology 78: 1752-1760.

Page 186: PREDATOR-PREY INTERACTIONS BETWEEN AN INVASIVE …

Chapter 5

168

Laugen AT, Laurila A, Merilä J (2002) Maternal and genetic contributions to geographical

variation in Rana temporaria larval life-history traits. Biological Journal of the Linnean

Society 76: 61-70.

Laurila A, Pakkasmaa S, Merilä J (2006) Population divergence in growth rate and antipredator

defences in Rana arvalis. Oecologia 147: 585-595.

Laurila A, Lindgren B, Laugen AT (2008) Antipredator defenses along a latitudinal gradient in

Rana temporaria. Ecology 89: 1399-1413.

Lefcort H (1996) Adaptive, chemically mediated fright response in tadpoles of the Southern

Leopard Frog, Rana utricularia. Copeia 1996: 455-459.

Leite EP, Anastácio PM, Ferreira M, Vicente L, Correia AM (2005) Do Eastern mosquitofish

exhibit anti-predator behavior towards red swamp crayfish? Zoological Studies 44: 513-

518.

Lima SL, Dill LM (1990) Behavioral decisions made under the risk of predation: A review and

prospectus. Canadian Journal of Zoology 68: 619-640.

Losos JB, Schoener TW and Spiller DA (2004) Predator-induced behaviour shifts and natural

selection in field-experimental lizard populations. Nature 432: 505-508.

Loureiro A, Ferrand de Almeida N, Carretero MA, Paulo OS (eds.) (2008) Atlas dos Anfíbios e

Répteis de Portugal. Instituto da Conservação da Natureza e da Biodiversidade, Lisboa.

257 pp.

Magurran AE (1990) The adaptive significance of schooling as an anti-predator defence in fish.

Annales Zoologici Fennici 27: 3-18.

Mandrillon A-L, Saglio P (2005) Prior exposure to conspecific chemical cues affects predator

recognition in larval common toad (Bufo bufo). Archiv für Hydrobiologie 164: 1-12.

Marquis O, Saglio P, Neveu A (2004) Effects of predators and conspecific chemical cues on the

swimming activity of Rana temporaria and Bufo bufo tadpoles. Archiv für Hydrobiologie

160: 153-170.

Page 187: PREDATOR-PREY INTERACTIONS BETWEEN AN INVASIVE …

Chapter 5

169

McCollum SA, Van Buskirk J (1996) Costs and benefits of a predator-induced polyphenism in

the gray treefrog Hyla chrysoscelis. Evolution 50: 583-593.

Moran NA (1992) The evolutionary maintenance of alternative phenotypes. American

Naturalist 139: 971-989.

Nunes AL, Cruz MJ, Tejedo M, Laurila A, Rebelo R (2010) Nonlethal injury caused by an invasive

alien predator and its consequences for an anuran tadpole. Basic and Applied Ecology

11: 645-654.

Nyström P, Brönmark C, Granéli W (1999) Influence of an exotic and a native crayfish on a

littoral benthic community. Oikos 84: 545-553.

Nyström P, Svensson O, Lardner B, Brönmark C, Granéli W (2001) The influence of multiple

introduced predators on a littoral pond community. Ecology 82: 1023-1039.

O'Steen S, Cullum AJ, Bennet AF (2002) Rapid evolution of escape ability in Trinidadian guppies

(Poecilia reticulata). Evolution 56: 776-784.

Orizaola G, Dahl E, Laurila A (2011) Reversibility of predator-induced plasticity and its effect at

a life-history switch point. Oikos, in press.

Peluc SI, Sillet TS, Rotenberry JT and Ghalambor CK (2008) Adaptive phenotypic plasticity in an

island songbird exposed to a novel predation risk. Behavioral Ecology 19: 830-835.

Polo-Cavia N, Gonzalo A, Lopez P, Martin J (2010) Predator recognition of native but not

invasive turtle predators by naive anuran tadpoles. Animal Behaviour 80: 461-466.

Rahel FJ, Stein RA (1988) Complex predator-prey interactions and predator intimidation among

crayfish, piscivorous fish, and small benthic fishes. Oecologia 75: 94-98.

Ramos MA, Pereira TM (1981) Um novo Astacidae para a fauna portuguesa: Procambarus

clarkii (Girard, 1852). Boletim do Instituto Nacional de Investigação e Pescas, Lisboa 6:

37-47.

Rehage JS, Dunlop KL, Loftu WF (2009) Antipredator responses by native mosquitofish to non-

native cichlids: an examination of the role of prey naiveté. Ethology 115: 1-11.

Page 188: PREDATOR-PREY INTERACTIONS BETWEEN AN INVASIVE …

Chapter 5

170

Relyea RA (2001) Morphological and behavioral plasticity of larval anurans in response to

different predators. Ecology 82: 523-540.

Relyea RA (2002) Local population differences in phenotypic plasticity: Predator-induced

changes in wood frog tadpoles. Ecological Monographs 72: 77-93.

Relyea RA (2003) Predators come and predators go: the reversibility of predator-induced traits.

Ecology 84: 1840-1848.

Richardson JML (2001) A comparative study of activity levels in larval anurans and response to

the presence of different predators. Behavioral Ecology 12: 51-58.

Richter-Boix A, Llorente GA, Montori A (2007) A comparative study of predator-induced

phenotype in tadpoles across a pond permanency gradient. Hydrobiologia 583: 43-56.

Richter-Boix A, Teplitsky C, Rogell B, Laurila A (2010) Local selection modifies phenotypic

divergence among Rana temporaria populations in the presence of gene flow. Molecular

Ecology 19: 716-731.

Rohlf FJ (2007) tpsRelw. Department of Ecology and Evolution. State Univ. New York, Stony

Brook.

Rohlf FJ (2008) tpsDig2. Department of Ecology and Evolution. State Univ. New York, Stony

Brook.

Rohlf FJ, Marcus LF (1993) A revolution in morphometrics. Trends in Ecology and Evolution 8:

129-132.

Schlaepfer MA, Sherman PW, Blossey B, Runge MC (2005) Introduced species as evolutionary

traps. Ecology Letters 8: 241-246.

Schoeppner NM, Relyea RA (2005) Damage, digestion, and defence: the roles of alarm cues

and kairomones for inducing prey defences. Ecology Letters 8: 505-512.

Sheets HD (2009) MakeFan7. Department of Physics, Canisius College, Buffalo.

Page 189: PREDATOR-PREY INTERACTIONS BETWEEN AN INVASIVE …

Chapter 5

171

Sih A, Bolnick DI, Luttbeg B, Orrock JL, Peacor SD, Pintor LM, Preisser E, Rehage JS, Vonesh JR

(2010) Predator-prey naiveté, antipredator behavior, and the ecology of predator

invasions. Oikos 119: 610-621.

Skelly DK (1994) Activity level and the susceptibility of anuran larvae to predation. Animal

Behaviour 47: 465-468.

Spitze K (1992) Predator-mediated plasticity of prey life history and morphology: Chaoborus

americanus predation on Daphnia pulex. The American Naturalist 139: 229-247.

Strauss SY, Lau JA, Carroll SP (2006) Evolutionary responses of natives to introduced species:

what do introductions tell us about natural communities? Ecology Letters 9: 354-371.

Sündermann D, Scheumann M, Zimmermann E (2008) Olfactory predator recognition in

predator-naïve gray mouse lemurs (Microcebus murinus). Journal of Comparative

Psychology 122: 146-155.

Teplitsky C, Plenet S, Léna J-P, Mermet N, Malet E, Joly P (2005) Escape behaviour and ultimate

causes of specific induced defences in an anuran tadpole. Journal of Evolutionary Biology

18: 180-190.

Thompson JN (1998) Rapid evolution as an ecological process. Trends in Ecology and Evolution

13: 329-332.

Trussell GC, Smith LD (2000) Induced defenses in response to an invading crab predator: an

explanation of historical and geographic phenotypic change. Proceedings of the National

Academy of Sciences USA 97: 2123-2127.

Van Buskirk J (2009) Natural variation in morphology of larval amphibians: Phenotypic

plasticity in nature? Ecological Monographs 79: 681-705.

Van Buskirk J, Arioli M (2005) Habitat specialization and adaptive phenotypic divergence of

anuran populations. Journal of Evolutionary Biology 18: 596-608.

Page 190: PREDATOR-PREY INTERACTIONS BETWEEN AN INVASIVE …

Chapter 5

172

Van Buskirk J, Relyea RA (1998) Natural selection for phenotypic plasticity: Predator-induced

morphological responses in tadpoles. Biological Journal of the Linnean Society 65: 301-

328.

Wisenden BD (2003) Chemically mediated strategies to counter predation. In: Sensory

Processing in Aquatic Environments (Collin, S. P. & Marshall, N. J., eds). Springer-Verlag,

New York, pp. 236-251.

Zelditch ML, Swiderski DL, Sheets HD, Fink WL (2004) Geometric morphometrics for biologists:

a primer. Academic Press.

Page 191: PREDATOR-PREY INTERACTIONS BETWEEN AN INVASIVE …

173

CHAPTER 6

General Discussion

Page 192: PREDATOR-PREY INTERACTIONS BETWEEN AN INVASIVE …

174

Page 193: PREDATOR-PREY INTERACTIONS BETWEEN AN INVASIVE …

Chapter 6

175

6. GENERAL DISCUSSION

In this thesis, I conducted a thorough analysis of the antipredator strategies used by larval

anurans from the Southwestern Iberian Peninsula to respond to the exotic crayfish P. clarkii.

More specifically, I assessed the importance of P. clarkii as a tail injury agent on anuran

tadpoles and the consequences of tail injury to prey. I examined the behavioural,

morphological and life-history antipredator responses to P. clarkii in the whole community of

larval anurans and analysed how strongly these responses were phenotypically integrated.

Finally, I compared crayfish-induced phenotypic plasticity in invaded and non-invaded anuran

populations.

The work presented here helps to understand how anuran prey populations deal with invasion

by a novel introduced predator and sets the scene for future research in this area. The aim of

this chapter is to synthesise and integrate the main findings exposed in the previous chapters

in the light of current knowledge on biological invasions, to set the results in an evolutionary

framework and to make recommendations on how to promote persistence of vulnerable prey

species.

6.1. Crayfish as an agent of tail injury in anurans

This work showed that P. clarkii incurs nonlethal tail injury on P. cultripes tadpoles at a much

higher rate than native predators do, indicating that this invasive predator is a strong agent of

injury in invaded anuran populations. This may severely impact native amphibians, since I

found a tendency for decreased larval survival as the frequency of caudal injury increased,

even when resource availability was high (Chapter 2).

Nonlethal injury induced alterations in tadpole tail morphology, with injured animals having

deeper tail muscles. Since a deeper tail improves the odds of survival in the presence of P.

Page 194: PREDATOR-PREY INTERACTIONS BETWEEN AN INVASIVE …

Chapter 6

176

clarkii (Gomez-Mestre and Diaz-Paniagua 2011) and this seems to be an adaptive response

evolved by populations with a long coexistence time with crayfish (Chapter 5), the induced

morphological alterations following tail injury may increase the survival probability of the

tadpoles.

Tail regeneration and alterations in tail morphology did not incur costs in terms of tadpole

growth and development, even though these may only arise later in life. In future studies,

animals surviving injury should be followed until metamorphosis and at post-metamorphic

stages, to examine if repeated tail loss has effects on traits such as body mass, jumping ability

or resistance to desiccation. It is also important to note that P. cultripes has a very long larval

period, which may allow it to better recover from tail injury than other species with shorter

larval periods and for which consequences of tail loss may be more severe.

6.2. Phenotypic variation in Iberian tadpoles

My experiments allowed documentation of species differences in phenotypic traits. In fact, the

phenotypic variation of some larval anurans in focus in this thesis has never been studied

before, as is the case of A. cisternasii, P. cultripes and P. ibericus (but see Diaz-Paniagua 1985

for P. cultripes).

Out of the nine study species, these three species were the ones having the longest larval

periods: 112 days for P. ibericus, 200 days for A. cisternasii and 222 days for P. cultripes

(Chapter 4). Contrary to A. cisternasii, which typically inhabits permanent water bodies, P.

cultripes reproduces in temporary ponds. The long larval period of P. cultripes suggests that

there is either great larval mortality due to pond desiccation, or that tadpoles accelerate their

development when cues from a decreasing water level are present. In comparison with the

other species, P. cultripes had a very high growth rate and mass at metamorphosis (Chapter 4).

Page 195: PREDATOR-PREY INTERACTIONS BETWEEN AN INVASIVE …

Chapter 6

177

This suggests that the tadpoles have to maintain very high activity level (and high foraging

rates), which was indeed observed late in their development (Chapter 3).

In general, species inhabiting ephemeral and temporary habitats are expected to be very

active and maintain high feeding rates in order to develop fast and reach metamorphosis

before the water body dries (Wellborn et al. 1996, Laurila and Kujasalo 1999, Anholt et al.

2000, Richter-Boix et al. 2007). On the contrary, permanent ponds species do not need to cope

with periodic drying, and are expected to be less constrained in terms of behaviour and

development. However, when looking at baseline activity levels of the nine anuran species

(Chapter 3), I found that B. calamita and D. galganoi, both species from ephemeral habitats,

had quite low baseline activity levels and that B. bufo, a permanent pond species, had a

moderately high activity level. This indicates that the relationship between hydroperiod

gradient and baseline levels of activity does not hold in this community (see also Richardson

2001, Van Buskirk 2002). Species phylogeny did not explain this pattern since I did not find a

phylogenetic autocorrelation in baseline activity level of the study species and since species

closely related to these do not have similar baseline activity levels to the ones found here

(Richter-Boix et al. 2007).

Further, species with the highest activity level were not the ones with the shortest time to

metamorphosis, suggesting that, contrary to results of some other studies (e.g. Relyea 2001,

Richter-Boix et al. 2007), activity was not linearly related to developmental rate (Chapters 3

and 4). In some of these species behavioural changes appeared to have little impact on

developmental rates, suggesting a decoupling between activity level and food consumption

rate (McPeek 2004, Steiner 2007b). Hence, as behaviour seems to be uncoupled from

development, it is not surprising that activity of different species does not reflect differences in

pond hydroperiod.

Page 196: PREDATOR-PREY INTERACTIONS BETWEEN AN INVASIVE …

Chapter 6

178

Different species have different phenotypic traits and, in fact, even the same species in

different studies often differs in phenotype, which may reflect experimental or methodological

differences, but most likely reflects population genetic divergence (e.g. for P. perezi: Richter-

Boix et al. 2007, Polo-Cavia et al. 2010, Egea-Serrano et al. 2011, Gomez-Mestre and Diaz-

Paniagua 2011). As seen in Chapter 5, intraspecific phenotypic variation is often high in larval

amphibians (e.g. Relyea 2002b, Van Buskirk and Arioli 2005, Laurila et al. 2006, 2008). More

studies examining among-population phenotypic variation are essential, especially considering

that some anuran species from the Iberian Peninsula show significant patterns of population

subdivision (e.g. Rosa 1995, Gonçalves et al. 2009) and that the taxonomy of many of these

species is constantly under revision (e.g. P. ibericus, Sánchez-Herráiz et al. 2000; B. spinosus,

Recuero et al. 2011).

6.3. Plastic responses of Iberian anurans to a native and an exotic predator

Although species differed in their responses to predators, I found that, with the exception of B.

calamita, all anurans from Southwestern Iberian Peninsula showed predator-induced

phenotypic plasticity. Surprisingly, only one of these species did not show any alteration in

phenotype in the presence of the exotic predator, indicating that native prey species are not

completely defenceless against invasive predators.

All species responded to the presence of dragonfly with alterations in behaviour and

morphology. However, while all but P. ibericus responded to the crayfish behaviourally, only B.

bufo and P. perezi altered morphology in its presence (Chapters 3 and 4, Table 1). Even though

P. perezi did not show any change in behaviour, morphology or life-history in Chapters 3 and 4,

in Chapter 5 I found that some P. perezi populations do have the ability to respond to P. clarkii.

In this case, time of coexistence with the invasive predator was the factor behind different

types of responses. As discussed in Chapter 5, this suggests genetic variation in antipredator

Page 197: PREDATOR-PREY INTERACTIONS BETWEEN AN INVASIVE …

Chapter 6

179

responses, evolved through natural selection imposed by the invasive predator. I found that

only two out of nine species altered morphology in the presence of crayfish, while eight

species did so in the presence of dragonfly (Table 1). This cannot be due to morphological

defences not being effective against the crayfish predator, as Gomez-Mestre and Diaz-

Paniagua (2011) showed that tadpoles with increased tail depth are better in surviving

predation by P. clarkii (see also Chapter 5). The lack of morphological defences may then be a

consequence of these defences being less reversible than behavioural ones (Relyea 2001,

2003, Orizaola et al. 2011) and, as a result, not being as easily elicited in the presence of an

unknown chemical stimulus, even if associated with distress cues from predated conspecifics

(Schoeppner and Relyea 2009). An alternative hypothesis relates to the fact that morphological

characters are considered less plastic than behavioural traits and so less readily altered in

response to new selective pressures (Relyea 2001, Blomberg et al. 2003, Blumstein 2006). At

the same time, it suggests that alterations in behaviour are likely more prompt responses of

prey when a chemical cue is detected in the environment, being it a predator odour or simply

cues from consumed conspecifics. In the three species that were most responsive to the

crayfish behaviourally (A. cisternasii, B. bufo and D. galganoi), cues resulting from predation

events largely mediated behavioural plasticity (Chapter 3), reinforcing the idea that

behavioural antipredator responses often derive from the presence of cues from consumed

conspecifics (Chapter 5, Schoeppner and Relyea 2005). In light of these results, the fact that A.

cisternasii also reduced activity level when exposed to cues from starved crayfish is intriguing

and deserves further study.

Page 198: PREDATOR-PREY INTERACTIONS BETWEEN AN INVASIVE …

Chapter 6

180

Table 1. Antipredator responses elicited by the nine anuran species in the presence of the invasive (P. clarkii) or native (Aeshna spp.) predator.

Behaviour Morphology Life-history

Crayfish Dragonfly Crayfish Dragonfly Crayfish Dragonfly

Alytes cisternasii ↓ activity

↑ cage avoidance

↓ activity

↑ cage avoidance -

↑ headbody depth

↑ tail fin depth

↑ tail muscle depth

↑ tail fin insertion

↓ time to metamorphosis

↑ mass at metamorphosis

↑ growth rate -

Discoglossus galganoi ↓ activity

↑ cage avoidance

↓ activity

↑ cage avoidance -

↑ headbody depth

↑ tail fin depth

↑ tail muscle depth

↑ tail fin insertion

-

↑ time to metamorphosis

↓ growth rate

Pelobates cultripes ↓ activity

↑ cage avoidance

↓ activity

-

↑ tail fin depth

↑ tail muscle depth

↑ tail fin insertion

↓ time to metamorphosis

-

Pelodytes ibericus -

↓ activity

↑ cage avoidance -

↑ headbody depth

↑ tail fin depth

↑ tail fin insertion

-

↑ mass at metamorphosis

↑ growth rate

Bufo bufo ↓ activity

↑ cage avoidance

↓ activity

↑ cage avoidance

↑ headbody length

↑ headbody length

↓ time to metamorphosis

↓ mass at metamorphosis

↓ time to metamorphosis

↓ mass at metamorphosis

↓ growth rate

Bufo calamita - - - - - -

Hyla arborea ↓ activity ↓ activity

↑ cage avoidance - ↑ tail fin depth

↑ tail fin insertion

↓ time to metamorphosis

↑ growth rate

↑ growth rate

Hyla meridionalis ↓ activity

↓ activity

↑ cage avoidance -

↑ headbody depth

↑ tail fin depth

↑ tail fin insertion

-

↑ mass at metamorphosis

Pelophylax perezi ↓ activity*

↓ activity

↓ headbody length**

↑ tail muscle depth**

↑ tail fin insertion**

↑ headbody depth

↑ tail fin depth

↑ tail muscle depth

- -

* only crayfish-free populations

** only a long-term crayfish invaded population

Page 199: PREDATOR-PREY INTERACTIONS BETWEEN AN INVASIVE …

Chapter 6

181

However, behavioural responses elicited in the presence of fed crayfish may not necessarily be

the most effective defenses against this predator. This seems to be the case for P. perezi, a

species for which a reduction in activity level likely represents a poorly effective defence

towards P. clarkii (Chapter 5). This may also hold true for A. cisternasii, a species that, similarly

to P. perezi, uses the bottom of ponds or streams as a preferred microhabitat (Díaz-Paniagua

1985, Cruz and Rebelo 2005). On the contrary, for species mainly found in shallow margins of

ponds or in open water (B. bufo, Hyla spp. and P. cultripes) (Díaz-Paniagua 1985, Cruz and

Rebelo 2005), a decrease in activity, together with a safe distance to the predator, will surely

be adaptive. A. cisternasii has been reported to change microhabitat use in P. clarkii’s

presence: it avoids pond bottom and moves to the shallow margins (Cruz and Rebelo 2005),

making low activity an effective response. Predation experiments assessing the survival value

of different behavioural responses against P. clarkii would help settling their adaptive value

and should clearly follow.

The lack of antipredator responses towards both predators in B. calamita can be explained by

the fact that this species reproduces in extremely ephemeral ponds, where no large aquatic

predators usually exist (Diaz-Paniagua 1990, Richter-Boix et al. 2007), making it highly

vulnerable to P. clarkii predation (Cruz and Rebelo 2005, Cruz et al. 2006b).

While predator-induced responses to the native dragonfly were usually strong and highly

integrated, responses to the invasive crayfish were often weak and generally not integrated

(Chapters 3 and 4). The exceptions were B. bufo, which showed similar integration to both

predators, and A. cisternasii, for which a more integrated response was found in the presence

of crayfish than dragonfly (Chapter 4). These two species, together with H. arborea and P.

perezi (only one long-term coexistence population), were the most responsive to the novel

predator. Further, A. cisternasii and H. arborea altered different suites of traits in the presence

of the exotic and the native predator, and responded behaviourally when exposed to chemical

cues from the starved crayfish (Chapters 3 and 4). This suggests that these species are capable

Page 200: PREDATOR-PREY INTERACTIONS BETWEEN AN INVASIVE …

Chapter 6

182

of detecting crayfish kairomones in the environment and associate them with a signal of

danger, different to that caused by the native dragonfly predator. This may indicate that the

study populations of A. cisternasii and H. arborea have already evolved the ability to recognise

and respond to cues from the novel predator, although I cannot be as sure as in the case of

long-term coexistence populations of P. perezi (Chapter 5). Studies assessing rapid adaptive

evolution on these two species would be very interesting, for example testing antipredator

responses in the F1 generation of several populations with different histories of contact with P.

clarkii, in order to understand the genetic basis of the observed changes (Reznick and

Ghalambor 2001).

The four abovementioned most responsive species to the crayfish all breed in permanent

water bodies. Even though the type of habitat where species reproduce is not the only factor

determining responses to this invasive predator (Chapters 3 and 4), this trend may be due to

these habitats being the main crayfish sources, where more stable and long lasting crayfish

populations have likely been established.

Costs of inducible defences were quite rare in this anuran community, at least under the

experimental conditions used in this study. Although the expression of behavioural and

morphological defences commonly incurs proximate costs in terms of growth and

development (reviewed by DeWitt et al. 1998, Van Buskirk 2000, Relyea 2002a, Steiner 2007a,

b, Callahan et al. 2008, Auld et al. 2010), other studies have either found weak or no costs

(Scheiner and Berrigan 1998, Van Buskirk and Saxer 2001, Teplitsky et al. 2003, Van Buskirk

and Steiner 2009). Here, potential costs of defence were only detected in two out of nine

anuran species, D. galganoi and B. bufo (Chapter 4). This may be related to these two species

having the smallest body size of all species (excluding B. calamita; Chapter 3), making any

alteration in their energy reserves due to induced defences (more energy expenditure or less

energy gain) much more detrimental than for larger species. This reinforces the idea of the

existence of prey-specific costs (Callahan et al. 2008). Yet, the possibility that more costs were

Page 201: PREDATOR-PREY INTERACTIONS BETWEEN AN INVASIVE …

Chapter 6

183

not detected under our experimental conditions due to food conditions being unrestricted,

probably allowing tadpoles to attain high energy levels, cannot be excluded.

I also found that inducible defences were concurrent with increased growth and

developmental rates in some of the species. Both A. cisternasii and H. arborea reduced their

activity level while they increased growth rate and attained metamorphosis faster in the

presence of crayfish (and at a larger size, in the case of A. cisternasii; Chapter 4). Similarly,

Teplitsky et al. (2003) found that, when exposed to fish chemical cues, tadpoles of H. arborea

grew and developed faster (although they did not detect any behavioural alterations).

Attaining metamorphosis earlier and at an equal or larger mass has not been predicted by

recent dynamic state-dependent models (Higginson and Ruxton 2010). This may be related to

limitations in the models. For instance, the models assume that when prey are inactive there is

no food consumption which, in the light of present results, is unlikely to be true. The models

also do not allow for a negative association between activity/foraging rate and growth or mass

at metamorphosis, contrary to what was observed in this (Chapter 4) and other recent studies

(Johansson and Andersson 2009). It is clear that future models should incorporate this

information.

The general lack of costs observed in this community and the decoupling observed between

behaviour and growth/development may help explain why species from temporary habitats,

contrary to what is predicted, did not invest less in behavioural defences than species from

permanent habitats (Chapters 3 and 4). It is commonly stated that species inhabiting

temporary water bodies, due to having a limited time span to reach metamorphosis, cannot

afford to reduce activity level as a behavioural defence (Wellborn et al. 1996, Laurila and

Kujasalo 1999, Richardson 2001, Richter-Boix et al. 2007). However, if a reduction in activity

level does not cause reduced growth or development in temporary pond species (as was

observed here; see the section ‘Species phenotypic traits’ and Chapter 4), individuals with

behavioural defences will still grow fast enough and reach metamorphosis before the pond

Page 202: PREDATOR-PREY INTERACTIONS BETWEEN AN INVASIVE …

Chapter 6

184

dries. In fact, other authors have also reported as strong behavioural defences in temporary as

in permanent habitats species (Van Buskirk 2002).

Overall, the capacity to elicit antipredator responses towards an invasive crayfish predator and

the type of responses elicited varied a lot among the study species. Some of the species did

not respond to the crayfish at all (B. calamita and P. ibericus), some responded with only few

trait alterations (H. meridionalis and D. galganoi), one responded using a generalized response

promoted by alarm cues (B. bufo), three responded by changing a suite of traits in a likely

efficient manner (A. cisternasii, H. arborea and P. cultripes) and one seems to have evolved

adaptive morphological responses to this predator (P. perezi). According to our results, P.

ibericus, which responds strongly to the native but not to the invasive predator, is one of the

most vulnerable species to crayfish introduction in this community. In fact, previous studies

have shown that P. clarkii is a negative predictor of the presence of this species in a water

body (Beja and Alcazar 2003, Cruz et al. 2006a), and that the crayfish has caused the local

extinction of a closely related species (P. punctatus) in a Nature Reserve (Cruz et al. 2008).

6.4. Implications of P. clarkii invasion for anuran communities

The species showing the strongest antipredator responses to the crayfish (A. cisternasii, B.

bufo, H. arborea and P. cultripes) can be predicted to be the ones with the highest survivorship

to direct predation by P. clarkii. However, this does not seem to be the case if we compare the

results obtained here with those obtained by Cruz and Rebelo (2005) (Table 2). In their study,

B. calamita, B. bufo and P. cultripes were the most vulnerable species to P. clarkii which,

excluding B. calamita, were species defended against P. clarkii in our study (Chapters 3 and 4,

Table 2). Conversely, for other species, results of the two studies overlapped, as in the case of

A. cisternasii and H. arborea, both well defended species, apparently less susceptible to

predation (Chapters 3 and 4, Table 2) (Cruz and Rebelo 2005).

Page 203: PREDATOR-PREY INTERACTIONS BETWEEN AN INVASIVE …

Chapter 6

185

While the discrepancy between these studies may be partially caused by methodological

constraints, the results suggest that predator-induced defences may not be enough to explain

tadpole vulnerability to P. clarkii. Still, some of the antipredator responses assumed here as

defences against the crayfish, such as a reduced time to metamorphosis, although beneficial,

do not influence vulnerability to a predator before or when an attack takes place. This type of

defences should, in turn, be reflected in the susceptibility of natural anuran populations to

invasion by P. clarkii. Several aspects of tadpole ecology probably act together in explaining

vulnerability to this novel predator (e.g. antipredator defences, swimming ability, microhabitat

use), and studies aiming to understand which factors are the most important and in which way

they interact are an essential next step to understand which species are more vulnerable to

invasions.

Neither anuran antipredator defences (Chapters 3, 4 and 5, Table 2) nor survival as measured

by Cruz and Rebelo (2005) seem to provide a general mechanistic explanation for the patterns

of anuran displacement and declines observed in crayfish-invaded areas (Table 2) (Cruz et al.

2006a, 2008). For example, for B. bufo and P. cultripes, both defended species (Chapters 3 and

4, Table 2), crayfish presence was a negative predictor of their breeding probability (Cruz et al.

2006a). However, a consistent trend was found for Pelodytes spp., a species poorly defended

against P. clarkii (although with quite high survival), to suffer strongly from crayfish invasion

(Chapters 3, 4 and 5, Table 2) (Cruz et al. 2006a, 2008). Further, the distribution of some

species associated with permanent water bodies (A. cisternasii and H. arborea) was not

affected by crayfish presence in the Sado region (Cruz et al. 2006a), which may be related to

these species being efficiently defended against this predator (Chapters 3 and 4). Yet, field

data from another region in Portugal (Paúl do Boquilobo Nature Reserve) showed that

populations of H. arborea, apparently resistant to crayfish in Sado, were excluded or strongly

reduced after crayfish arrival (Cruz et al. 2008).

Page 204: PREDATOR-PREY INTERACTIONS BETWEEN AN INVASIVE …

Chapter 6

186

Table 2. Approximate ranking of species vulnerability to P. clarkii inferred by different studies and

considering anuran antipredator defences (Chapters 3, 4 and 5), survival (Cruz and Rebelo 2005),

probability of reproducing in a crayfish invaded habitat (Cruz et al. 2006a) and population resilience

(Cruz et al. 2008).

Antipredator Defences Survival

Probability of reproducing in a crayfish invaded

habitat

Population resilience

B. calamita B. calamita P. cultripes Pelodytes punctatus

P. ibericus B. bufo B. bufo H. arborea

H. meridionalis P. cultripes P. ibericus P. perezi

D. galganoi D. galganoi H. arborea

P. cultripes P. perezi A. cisternasii

P. perezi* A. cisternasii B. calamita

B. bufo P. ibericus P. perezi

H. arborea H. arborea H. meridionalis

A. cisternasii H. meridionalis D. galganoi

* only a long-term crayfish invaded population

These lines of evidence suggest that the impacts that P. clarkii exerts in amphibian populations

do not derive solely from direct predation and that the dynamics of declining amphibian

communities is not only influenced by species’ responses or survival to crayfish predation. P.

clarkii must affect amphibian populations through other indirect ways and, in fact, invaders

such as crayfish can impact organisms at different ecological and multitrophic levels (Nyström

et al. 2001, Rodríguez et al. 2005). By greatly increasing the rate at which tadpoles suffer tail

injury, crayfish may affect anuran survival and metamorphic recruitment (Chapter 2). By

grazing and nonconsumptive destruction of macrophytes, crayfish reduce the area available

for the growth of periphyton, affecting the quantity of food resources for tadpoles (Lodge et

al. 1994, Nyström et al. 2001, Rodríguez et al. 2005). Macrophyte consumption also reduces

structural complexity of the aquatic habitat, decreasing shelters and destroying egg-laying sites

of some amphibian species (Gutiérrez-Yurrita and Montes 1999, Rodríguez et al. 2005). Dense

crayfish populations can also affect water quality by increasing turbidity and nutrient release,

which is associated to its intense foraging and burrowing activity (Rodríguez et al. 2003, 2005).

The type of resources exploited by P. clarkii coupled with their action as bioturbators, may lead

-

+

Page 205: PREDATOR-PREY INTERACTIONS BETWEEN AN INVASIVE …

Chapter 6

187

to habitat degradation and dramatic direct and indirect effects on amphibian populations

(Geiger et al. 2005, Rodríguez et al. 2005).

6.5. Control and management of P. clarkii and conservation of native

amphibians

As introduced species become established and abundant in their new habitats, eradication and

management options become more limited (Sakai et al. 2001, Lockwood et al. 2007). In the

Iberian Peninsula, P. clarkii has already passed the early stages of invasion - establishment and

spread - when total eradication should be attempted, being now at the ‘ecological impact’

stage, which demands for intensive control (Kolar and Lodge 2001, Sakai et al. 2001, Simberloff

2003, Lockwood et al. 2007). Although there are examples of successful control of established

invasive species ( see examples in Simberloff 2003), for aggressive and very successful

invaders, such as P. clarkii, eradication and direct control are very unlikely or even impossible

(Barbaresi and Gherardi 2000, Lodge et al. 2000, Kerby et al. 2005, Gherardi 2007). Among the

techniques that can be used to control or eradicate invasive crayfish are mechanical (trapping

and electrofishing), physical (draining ponds or creating barriers), chemical (use of biocides) or

biological (microbial insecticides and introduction of fish predators) approaches, some of

which have been proven unfruitful in the elimination of P. clarkii (Anastácio et al. 2000, Kerby

et al. 2005, Freeman et al. 2010). Additionally, given P. clarkii’s ability to quickly expand and its

enormous invasion potential in the Iberian Peninsula there is likely to be only very few

crayfish-free aquatic habitats in the near future (Capinha and Anastácio 2011, Liu et al. 2011).

Considering this scenario, only a few options are left to sustain persistence of amphibian

populations in the Southwest of the Iberian Peninsula. First of all, given enough resources,

control or eradication of crayfish in small ephemeral or temporary ponds with the highest

suitability for amphibians could be an important measure, since anuran species from these

Page 206: PREDATOR-PREY INTERACTIONS BETWEEN AN INVASIVE …

Chapter 6

188

habitats seem to be the most undefended against the crayfish (Chapters 3, 4 and 5; see also

Cruz et al. 2006a). This is especially relevant for endemic species, such as P. ibericus and D.

galganoi (Loureiro et al. 2008).

Another sustainable solution consists of finding strategies that allow coexistence between P.

clarkii and vulnerable native prey species, for which the results of the present study may help

setting a head start (Carroll 2011). Since anuran populations seem to be able to, through time,

evolve efficient morphological defences against this invasive crayfish (Chapter 5), management

plans could promote survival of native prey populations long enough to allow adaptive

evolution to occur (Schlaepfer et al. 2005, Lankau et al. 2011). This could be promoted by, for

instance, increasing structural habitat complexity in order to provide more shelters and refuge

areas for amphibian larvae to hide from crayfish attacks (Axelsson et al. 1997, Babbitt and

Tanner 1998). Yet, caution has to be taken because some other predators, such as dragonflies,

often use vegetation cover as a preferential place to launch their attacks. In fact, the

directional selection imposed by P. clarkii should be combined with prey population growth,

which may be a key factor hindering extinction and allowing for rapid evolution to occur

(Reznick and Ghalambor 2001). Hence, population size of P. clarkii should be controlled so as

to not cause extirpation of the native prey populations and allow population growth.

Another approach consists of stocking naïve or recently invaded populations with individuals

from longer-term invaded populations, with already evolved adequate morphological defences

towards P. clarkii to, via hybridization, increase the evolutionary potential of the former prey

populations (Schlaepfer et al. 2005, Lankau et al. 2011). However, this option may be quite

risky, because it may promote loss of intraspecific adaptive phenotypic trait variation,

pervasive in several anuran species from the Iberian Peninsula (e.g. Chapter 5, Rosa 1995,

Gonçalves et al. 2009, Egea-Serrano et al. 2009). Finally, the capacity that some amphibians

have of, through association with alarm cues, learning to recognise and respond to the scent of

novel predators (Ferrari et al. 2010), may be used as a tool by conservation biologists

Page 207: PREDATOR-PREY INTERACTIONS BETWEEN AN INVASIVE …

Chapter 6

189

(Schlaepfer et al. 2005). By ‘teaching’ individuals that a certain predator is dangerous, prey will

avoid it by adopting antipredator defences, hopefully adequate to the specific predator (Sih et

al. 2011).

It is important to note that rapid evolution of morphological responses to P. clarkii was only

detected in populations of P. perezi (Chapter 5), and the evolution-based management

techniques proposed above are dependent on the assumption that a similar pattern will occur

in populations of the other Southwestern Iberian Peninsula anuran species. Species with a

similarly short generation time as P. perezi (2-3 years), high reproductive output and short

larval period, such as both Hyla spp. and B. bufo (Almeida et al. 2001), may have high potential

to locally adapt to the presence of P. clarkii. Further, this may be enhanced for H. arborea, a

species that, along with P. perezi, reproduces in late spring/early summer, when crayfish also

reproduces, making selection predation pressure stronger and maybe promoting local

adaptation (provided selection is not too strong) (Gutiérrez-Yurrita and Montes 1999, Almeida

et al. 2001).

6.6. Final considerations

Invasive species are altering the biotic structure of many ecosystems and have become a major

cause of extinction. The high rate of anthropogenic-mediated species introductions will

continue to promote faunal homogenization, making the number of biological invasions

inevitably destined to increase. Further, global climate change will most likely allow for exotic

species to expand their ranges even more (e.g. Liu et al. 2011).

This work adds to the growing evidence that the invasion of the Southwest of the Iberian

Peninsula by the invasive crayfish P. clarkii impacts native anuran prey populations. This is

reflected by the high rates of tadpole tail injury found in crayfish-invaded populations, the

Page 208: PREDATOR-PREY INTERACTIONS BETWEEN AN INVASIVE …

Chapter 6

190

alterations in tadpole behaviour, morphology and life-history caused by crayfish presence and

the evolutionary changes observed in populations with a long coexistence time with P. clarkii.

The results found here extend the taxonomic occurrence of predator-induced plasticity, here

reported for the first time for some Iberian anurans. Antipredator responses towards the

invasive predator varied greatly within the same community, which can make the impact of P.

clarkii on amphibian populations uneven and hard to predict. More studies are clearly needed

and they should focus on the most vulnerable species to this invasion, such as P. ibericus, an

endemic Iberian species (Loureiro et al. 2008). P. clarkii proved to be a strong selective agent,

promoting adaptive evolution in populations of P. perezi in a quite short period of time. As

such, and since there are no easy solutions to cope with P. clarkii’s invasions, employing

strategies that allow native communities to coexist with this predator are probably the best

option left. Still, there is no doubt that P. clarkii will continue to induce deep alterations in

amphibian communities in Southwestern Iberian Peninsula in the future.

Understanding the dynamics of the interactions between exotic predators and native prey and

what role the former may have in structuring the future evolutionary pathway of invaded

systems is now essential and requires the convergence in the fields of ecology and evolution.

Given the more ecological approach of this thesis, I hope that these results can be combined

with more evolutionary-oriented studies to improve the understanding of the essentially

irreversible outcomes resulting from the invasion of P. clarkii.

Page 209: PREDATOR-PREY INTERACTIONS BETWEEN AN INVASIVE …

Chapter 6

191

6.7. References

Almeida NF, Almeida PF, Gonçalves H, Sequeira F, Teixeira J, Almeida FF (2001) Guias FAPAS:

Anfíbios e Répteis de Portugal. FAPAS and Câmara Municipal do Porto, Porto: 249 pp.

Anastácio, PM, Frias AF, Marques JC (2000) Impact of crayfish densities on wet seeded rice and

the inefficiency of a non-ionic surfactant as an ecotechnological solution. Ecological

Engineering 15: 17-25.

Anholt BR, Werner EE, Skelly DK (2000) Effect of food and predators on the activity of four

larval ranid frogs. Ecology 81: 3509-3521.

Auld JR, Agrawal AA, Relyea RA (2010) Re-evaluating the costs and limits of adaptive

phenotypic plasticity. Proceedings of the Royal Society B of London B 277: 503-511.

Axelsson E, Nyström P, Brönmark C (1997) Crayfish predation on amphibian eggs and larvae.

Amphibia-Reptilia 18: 217-228.

Babbitt KJ, Tanner GW (1998) Effects of cover and predator size on survival and development

of Rana utricularia tadpoles. Oecologia 114: 258-262.

Barbaresi S, Gherardi F (2000) The invasion of the alien crayfish Procambarus clarkii in Europe,

with particular reference to Italy. Biological Invasions 2: 259-264.

Beja P, Alcazar R (2003) Conservation of Mediterranean temporary ponds under agricultural

intensification: an evaluation using amphibians. Biological Conservation 114: 317-326.

Blomberg SP, Garland T Jr., Ives AR (2003) Testing for phylogenetic signal in comparative data:

behavioral traits are more labile. Evolution 57: 717-745.

Blumstein DT (2006) The multipredator hypothesis and the evolutionary persistence of

antipredator behavior. Ethology 112: 209-217.

Callahan HS, Maughan H, Steiner UK (2008) Phenotypic plasticity, costs of phenotypes, and

costs of plasticity toward an integrative view. The New York Academy of Sciences 1133:

44-66.

Capinha C, Anastácio P (2011) Assessing the environmental requirements of invaders using

ensembles of distribution models. Diversity and Distributions 17: 13-24.

Carroll SP (2011) Conciliation biology: the eco-evolutionary management of permanently

invaded biotic systems. Evolutionary Applications 4: 184-199.

Page 210: PREDATOR-PREY INTERACTIONS BETWEEN AN INVASIVE …

Chapter 6

192

Cruz MJ, Rebelo R (2005) Vulnerability of southwest Iberian amphibians to an introduced

crayfish, Procambarus clarkii. Amphibia-Reptilia 26: 293-303.

Cruz MJ, Rebelo R, Crespo EG (2006a) Effects of an introduced crayfish, Procambarus clarkii, on

the distribution of south-western Iberian amphibians in their breeding habitats.

Ecography 29: 329-338.

Cruz MJ, Pascoal S, Tejedo M, Rebelo R (2006b) Predation by an exotic crayfish, Procambarus

clarkii, on natterjack toad, Bufo calamita, embryos: its role on the exclusion of this

amphibian from its breeding ponds. Copeia 2006: 274-280.

Cruz MJ, Segurado P, Sousa M, Rebelo R (2008) Collapse of the amphibian community of

thePaul do Boquilobo Natural Reserve (central Portugal) after the arrival of the exotic

American crayfish Procambarus clarkii. Herpetological Journal 18: 197-204.

DeWitt TJ, Sih A, Wilson DS (1998) Costs and limits of phenotypic plasticity. Trends in Ecology

and Evolution 13: 77-81.

Diaz-Paniagua C (1985) Larval diets related to morphological characters of five anuran species

in the Biological Reserve of Doñana (Huelva, Spain). Amphibia-Reptilia 6: 307-322.

Diaz-Paniagua C (1990) Temporary ponds as breeding sites of amphibians at a locality in

southwestern Spain. Herpetological Journal 1: 447-453.

Egea-Serrano A, Tejedo M, Torralva M (2009) Populational divergence in the impact of three

nitrogenous compounds and their combination on larvae of the frog Pelophylax perezi

(Seoane, 1885). Chemosphere 76: 869-877.

Egea-Serrano A, Tejedo M, Torralva M (2011) Behavioral responses of the Iberian waterfrog,

Pelophylax perezi (Seoane, 1885), to three nitrogenous compounds in laboratory

conditions. Ecotoxicology 20: 1246-1257.

Ferrari MCO, Wisenden BD, Chivers DP (2010) Chemical ecology of predator-prey interactions

in aquatic ecosystems: a review and prospectus. Canadian Journal of Zoology 88: 698-

724.

Freeman MA, Turnbull JF, Yeomans WE, Bean CW (2010) Prospects for management strategies

of invasive crayfish populations with an emphasis on biological control. Aquatic

Conservation: Marine and Freshwater Ecosystems 20: 211-223.

Geiger W, Alcorlo P, Baltanás A, Montes C (2005) Impact of an introduced crustacean on the

trophic webs of Mediterranean wetlands. Biological Invasions 7: 49-73.

Page 211: PREDATOR-PREY INTERACTIONS BETWEEN AN INVASIVE …

Chapter 6

193

Gherardi F (2007) Biological invasions in inland waters: an overview. Pp. 3-25 in: Gherardi F.

(Ed.), Biological invaders in inland waters: Profiles, distribution, and threats. Springer,

Dordrecht, The Netherlands.

Gomez-Mestre I, Díaz-Paniagua C (2011) Invasive predatory crayfish do not trigger inducible

defences in tadpoles. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B, in press.

Gonçalves H, Martinez-Solano I, Pereira RJ, Carvalho B, Garcia-Paris M, Ferrand N (2009) High

levels of population subdivision in a morphologically conserved Mediterranean toad

(Alytes cisternasii) result from recent, multiple refugia: evidence from mtDNA,

microsatellites and nuclear genealogies. Molecular Ecology 18: 5143-5160.

Gutiérrez-Yurrita PJ, Montes C (1999) Bioenergetics and phenology of reproduction of the

introduced red swamp crayfish, Procambarus clarkii, in Doñana National Park, Spain, and

implications for species management. Freshwater Biology 42: 561-574.

Higginson AD, Ruxton GD (2010) Adaptive changes in size and age at metamorphosis can

qualitatively vary with predator type and available defenses. Ecology 91: 2756-2768.

Johansson F, Andersson J (2009) Scared fish get lazy, and lazy fish get fat. Journal of Animal

Ecology 78: 772-777.

Kerby JL, Riley SPD, Kats LB, Wilson P (2005) Barriers and flow as limiting factors in the spread

of an invasive crayfish (Procambarus clarkii) in southern California streams. Biological

Conservation 126: 402-409.

Kolar CS, Lodge DM (2001) Progress in invasion biology: predicting invaders. Trends in Ecology

and Evolution 16: 199-204.

Lankau R, Jørgensen PS, Harris DJ, and Sih A (2011) Incorporating evolutionary principles into

environmental management and policy. Evolutionary Applications 4: 315-325.

Laurila A, Kujasalo J (1999) Habitat duration, predation risk and phenotypic plasticity in

common frog (Rana temporaria) tadpoles. The Journal of Animal Ecology 68: 1123-1132.

Laurila A, Pakkasmaa S, Merila J (2006) Population divergence in growth rate and antipredator

defences in Rana arvalis. Oecologia 147: 585-595.

Laurila A, Lindgren B, Laugen AT (2008) Antipredator defenses along a latitudinal gradient in

Rana temporaria. Ecology 89: 1399-1413.

Page 212: PREDATOR-PREY INTERACTIONS BETWEEN AN INVASIVE …

Chapter 6

194

Liu X, Guo Z, Ke Z, Wang S, Li Y (2011) Increasing potential risk of a global aquatic invader in

Europe in contrast to other continents under future climate change. PLoS One 6:

e18429.

Lockwood J, Hoopes M and Marchetti M (2007) Invasion Ecology. Blackwell Publishing Ltd.,

Malden, Massachusetts, 312 pp.

Lodge DM, Kershner MW, Aloi JE, Covich AP (1994) Effects of an omnivorous crayfish

(Orconectes rusticus) on a freshwater littoral food web. Ecology 75: 1265-1281.

Lodge DM, Taylor CA, Holdich DM, Skurdal J (2000) Nonindigenous crayfishes threaten North

American freshwater biodiversity: lessons from Europe. Fisheries 25: 7-20.

Loureiro A, Ferrand de Almeida N, Carretero MA, Paulo OS (eds.) (2008) Atlas dos Anfíbios e

Répteis de Portugal. Instituto da Conservação da Natureza e da Biodiversidade, Lisboa.

257 pp.

McPeek MA (2004) The growth/predation risk trade-off: so what is the mechanism? The

American Naturalist 163: 88-111.

Nyström P, Svensson O, Lardner B, Brönmark C, Granéli W (2001) The influence of multiple

introduced predators on a littoral pond community. Ecology 82: 1023-1039.

Orizaola G, Dahl E, Laurila A (2011) Reversibility of predator-induced plasticity and its effect at

a life-history switch point. Oikos, in press.

Polo-Cavia N, Gonzalo A, Lopez P, Martin J (2010) Predator recognition of native but not

invasive turtle predators by naive anuran tadpoles. Animal Behaviour 80: 461-466.

Recuero E, Canestrelli D, Vörös J, Szaboó K, Poyarkov NA, Arntzen JW, Crnobrnja-Isailovic J,

Kidov AA, Cogalniceanu D, Caputo FP, Nascetti G, Martinez-Solano I (2011) Multilocus

species tree analyses resolve the radiation of the widespread Bufo bufo species group

(Anura, Bufonidae). Molecular Phylogenetics and Evolution, in press.

Relyea RA (2001) Morphological and behavioral plasticity of larval anurans in response to

different predators. Ecology 82: 523-540.

Relyea RA (2002a) Costs of phenotypic plasticity. The American Naturalist 159: 272-282.

Relyea RA (2002b) Local population differences in phenotypic plasticity: Predator-induced

changes in wood frog tadpoles. Ecological Monographs 72: 77-93.

Relyea RA (2003) Predators come and predators go: The reversibility of predator-induced

traits. Ecology 84: 1840-1848.

Page 213: PREDATOR-PREY INTERACTIONS BETWEEN AN INVASIVE …

Chapter 6

195

Reznick DN, Ghalambor CK (2001) The population ecology of contemporary adaptation: What

empirical studies reveal about the conditions that promote adaptive evolution. Genetica

112-113: 183-198.

Richardson JML (2001) A comparative study of activity levels in larval anurans and response to

the presence of different predators. Behavioral Ecology 12: 51-58.

Richter-Boix A, Llorente GA, Montori A (2007) A comparative study of predator-induced

phenotype in tadpoles across a pond permanency gradient. Hydrobiologia 583: 43-56.

Rodríguez CL, Bécares E, Fernández-Aláez M (2003) Shift from clear to turbid phase in Lake

Chozas (NW Spain) due to the introduction of American red swamp crayfish

(Procambarus clarkii). Hydrobiologia 506-509: 421-426.

Rodríguez CF, Bécares E, Fernández-Aláez M, Fernández-Aláez C (2005) Loss of diversity and

degradation of wetlands as a result of introducing exotic crayfish. Biological Invasions 7:

75-85.

Rosa HD (1995) Estrutura e diferenciação genética de populações de anuros da fauna

Portuguesa. PhD Thesis, University of Lisbon, Lisbon, Portugal.

Sakai AK, Allendorf FW, Holt JS, Lodge DM, Molofsky J, With KA, Baughman S, Cabin RJ, Cohen

JE, Ellstrand N, McCauley DE, O’Neil P, Parker IM, Thompson JN, Weller SG (2001) The

population biology of invasive species. Annual Review of Ecology and Systematics 32:

305-332.

Sánchez-Herráiz MJ, Barbadillo LJ, Machordom A, Sanchiz B (2000) A new species of pelodytid

from the Iberian Peninsula. Herpetologica 56: 105-118.

Scheiner SM, Berrigan D (1998) The genetics of phenotypic plasticity. VIII. The cost of plasticity

in Daphnia pulex. Evolution 52: 368-378.

Schlaepfer MA, Sherman PW, Blossey B, Runge MC (2005) Introduced species as evolutionary

traps. Ecology Letters 8: 241-246.

Schoeppner NM, Relyea RA (2005) Damage, digestion, and defence: the roles of alarm cues

and kairomones for inducing prey defences. Ecology Letters 8: 505-512.

Schoeppner NM, Relyea RA (2009) Interpreting the smells of predation: how alarm cues and

kairomones induce different prey defences. Functional Ecology 23: 1114-1121.

Sih A, Ferrari MCO, Harris DJ (2011) Evolution and behavioural responses to human-induced

rapid environmental change. Evolutionary Applications 4: 367-387.

Page 214: PREDATOR-PREY INTERACTIONS BETWEEN AN INVASIVE …

Chapter 6

196

Simberloff D (2003) How much information on population biology is needed to manage

introduced species? Conservation Biology 17: 83-92.

Steiner UK (2007a) Investment in defense and cost of predator-induced defense along a

resource gradient. Oecologia 152: 201-210.

Steiner UK (2007b) Linking antipredator behaviour, ingestion, gut evacuation and costs of

predator-induced responses in tadpoles. Animal Behaviour 74: 1473-1479.

Teplitsky C, Plénet S, Joly P (2003) Tadpoles’ responses to risk of fish introduction. Oecologia

134: 270-277.

Van Buskirk J (2000) The costs of an inducible defense in anuran larvae. Ecology 81: 2813-2821.

Van Buskirk J (2002) A comparative test of the adaptive plasticity hypothesis: Relationships

between habitat and phenotype in anuran larvae. The American Naturalist 160: 87-102.

Van Buskirk J, Arioli M (2005) Habitat specialization and adaptive phenotypic divergence in

anuran populations. Journal of Evolutionary Biology 18:596-608.

Van Buskirk J, Saxer G (2001) Delayed costs of an induced defense in tadpoles? Morphology,

hopping, and development rate at metamorphosis. Evolution 55: 821-829.

Van Buskirk J, Steiner UK (2009) The fitness costs of developmental canalization and plasticity.

Journal of Evolutionary Biology 22: 852-860.

Wellborn GA, Skelly DK, Werner EE (1996) Mechanisms creating community structure across a

freshwater habitat gradient. Annual Review of Ecology and Systematics 27: 337-363.