65
Pervasive Developmental Disorders September 5, 2006 PSYC 4930

Pervasive Developmental Disorders September 5, 2006 PSYC 4930

Embed Size (px)

Citation preview

Page 1: Pervasive Developmental Disorders September 5, 2006 PSYC 4930

Pervasive Developmental

Disorders

September 5, 2006PSYC 4930

Page 2: Pervasive Developmental Disorders September 5, 2006 PSYC 4930

Pervasive Developmental Disorders

Several disorders are included under the DSM-IV heading of Pervasive Developmental Disorders:1. Autistic Disorder

2. Asperger's Disorder

3. Rett's Disorder

4. Childhood Disintegrative Disorder

5. Pervasive developmental disorder-not otherwise specified (PDD-NOS)

Autism Spectrum Disorders (ASD) Autistic Disorder Asperger's Disorder

Rett's Disorder

Most is known about autism Relatively early stages of understanding

Page 3: Pervasive Developmental Disorders September 5, 2006 PSYC 4930

Pervasive Developmental Disorders

PDDs are characterized by severe and pervasive impairments in several areas of development (DSM-IV)

1. Reciprocal social interactions/skills

2. Communication Speech, language, & communication

3. Stereotyped or repeated behaviors, interests, activities, or routines

Page 4: Pervasive Developmental Disorders September 5, 2006 PSYC 4930

Pervasive vs. Specific Disorders

PDD are to be distinguished from "Specific Developmental Disorders" (e.g., reading, articulation, arithmetic, and language disorders).

This is because they are characterized by severe disturbances in many basic areas of development.

They may also be reflected in behaviors having no counterpart in normal development.

Thus, children with these conditions often display distorted rather than simply delayed development. E.G., Echolalia is never developmental typical

Page 5: Pervasive Developmental Disorders September 5, 2006 PSYC 4930

Why this is important to you?

Prevalence rates for Autism Spectrum Disorders Between 2 and 6 per 1,000

individuals Between 1 in 500 to

1 in 166 children have an ASD!

Center for Disease Control and Prevention (CDC)

Page 6: Pervasive Developmental Disorders September 5, 2006 PSYC 4930

The History of Pervasive Developmental Disorders Historically disorders of this type have been referred to by a variety of labels such asatypical psychosischild psychosissymbiotic psychosischildhood schizophrenia infantile autism

Page 7: Pervasive Developmental Disorders September 5, 2006 PSYC 4930

Pervasive Developmental Disorders

At one time, conditions referred to as Pervasive Developmental Disorders were often thought to be reflective of Child Psychosis

However, PDDs are not related to the psychotic conditions of adulthood (e.g. Schizophrenia), and are now referred to as "developmental" rather than "psychotic" disorders.

Page 8: Pervasive Developmental Disorders September 5, 2006 PSYC 4930

Case Example 1 7 years-old female Developmental History

Motor Sat up without propping at 9 months, walked without assistance at age 3, and showed an early right hand preference. By age 3 ½, she was reportedly able to step over objects, feed herself, and showed a pincer and palmer grip.

Communication By age 4, attained about a 25-word vocabulary and used 2-word phrases such as “go home”. Her early vocabulary included words such as “go” and “dada”.

Social At age 3, she reportedly showed appropriate interest in toys and books.

Parent estimated that peak development was at about 4 years of age (Delayed: estimated19-month-old level).

Page 9: Pervasive Developmental Disorders September 5, 2006 PSYC 4930

Case Example 1By age 7, regressed to level below 12 months old (Loss of previously acquired skills) Communication

The only word used at home was “go”. At school she would repeat “good girl” and “good job”, but this was not self-initiated.

Expressive language ~ 5 month old level Receptive language ~ 6 month old (not readily follow commands)

Motor Skills Loss of previously acquired purposeful hand skills (i.e., not independently feed herself with fork or spoon, or hold a pencil)

Development of stereotyped hand movements (i.e., hand-wringing)

Loss of locomotion and motor coordination (i.e., no longer walk up stairs).

Lost progress on toilet training previously acquired (i.e., required diapers since 4 years, 9 months old.

Page 10: Pervasive Developmental Disorders September 5, 2006 PSYC 4930

Case Example 1

Social Functioning and other symptoms Diminished interest in previously pleasurable play activities (i.e., playing on a swing, showed no interest when presented with a toy )

Loss of appropriate social engagement (e.g.,limited eye contact).

Decreased sensitivity to pain, becomes very hyper in new situations, and recently started playing with her feces prior to wringing her hands.

Page 11: Pervasive Developmental Disorders September 5, 2006 PSYC 4930

Our Diagnosis Developmental consistent with the IRSA’s

description of the “Atypical or Borderline” variant phenotype of Rett Syndrome.

At least 3 of 6 of the main criteria (i.e., absence or reduction of hand skills, monotonous pattern of hand stereotypies, reduction or loss of communication skills) and a number of the supportive criteria (i.e., teeth grinding, harsh sounding type, abnormal locomotion, diminished response to pain).

Page 12: Pervasive Developmental Disorders September 5, 2006 PSYC 4930

Rett Syndrome (Rett’s Disorder)

First described by Dr. Andreas Rett in 1964, but did not receive worldwide recognition until the first English language publication by Dr. Bengt Hagberg in 1983

Dr. Andreas Rett in Vienna,

Austria

Neurodevelopmental disorder Child appears to be born healthy and have a period of “normal” early development

Onset of symptoms typically occurs as early as 5 months or as late as 48 months.

Occurs in 1:10,000 to 1:23,000 female births Much rarer than Autism

Often misdiagnosed as autism, cerebral palsy or non-specified developmental delay

Page 13: Pervasive Developmental Disorders September 5, 2006 PSYC 4930

DSM-IV: Pervasive Developmental Disorder: 299.80 Rett’s Disorder

A. All of the following:1. Apparently normal prenatal and perinatal

development2. Apparently normal psychomotor development through

the first 5 months after birth3. Normal head circumference at birth

B. Onset of all of the following after the period of normal development:1. Deceleration of head growth between ages 5 and 48

months2. Loss of previously acquired purposeful hand skills

between ages 5 and 30 months with the subsequent development of stereotyped hand movements (e.g., hand-wringing or hand washing)

3. Loss of social engagement early in the course (although often social interaction develops later)

4. Appearance of poorly coordinated gait or trunk movements

5. Severely impaired expressive and receptive language development with severe psychomotor retardation

Page 14: Pervasive Developmental Disorders September 5, 2006 PSYC 4930

2002 Revised Diagnostic criteria for Rett Syndrome

Hagberg B, Hanefeld F, Percy A, Skjeldal O. An update on clinically applicable diagnostic criteria in Rett syndrome. European J of Paediatric Neurology 2002; 6: 293-297

The International Rett Syndrome Association convened a panel of international experts as part of the European Pediatric Neurology Society.

Based on the correlation between genotypes (gene mutations) and phenotypes (symptoms): Typical Atypical or Borderline Variants (15% of of diagnosed cases)

Page 15: Pervasive Developmental Disorders September 5, 2006 PSYC 4930

Course and Stages of Rett Syndrome

STAGE I: Early Onset Age: 6 months to 1½ years Duration: months Stage usually overlooked Symptoms just emerging and somewhat vague

May be reduced interest in toys, decreased eye contact, delays in gross motor milestones, decelerating head growth, non-specific hand wringing

Often described as a "good" baby, calm and placid

Page 16: Pervasive Developmental Disorders September 5, 2006 PSYC 4930

STAGE II: Rapid Destructive Age: 1 to 4 years Duration: weeks to months Rapid onset or more gradual Purposeful hand skills and spoken language are lost

Stereotyped hand movements emerge Breathing irregularities may emerge Some girls appear autistic-like

Loss of social interaction and communication General irritability and sleep irregularity may be seen

Periods of tremulousness, especially when excited

Unsteady gait patterns Difficulty initiating motor movements Slowing of head growth is usually noticed from 3 months - 4 years

Page 17: Pervasive Developmental Disorders September 5, 2006 PSYC 4930

STAGE III: Plateau Age: Pre-school to school years (2-10 years) Duration: years Worsening motor problems

Interfering with every body movement, including eye gaze and speech

Perhaps the most severely disabling feature

Severe seizures Mental Retardation Bruxism (teeth grinding) Improvements in behavior

Less irritability and crying Less autistic features Show greater interest in surroundings Alertness, attention span and communication skills improve

Many remain in Stage III for most of their lifetime

Page 18: Pervasive Developmental Disorders September 5, 2006 PSYC 4930

Late Motor Deterioration STAGE IV A (Previously ambulant) or STAGE IV B (Never ambulant)

Age: When stage III ceases, 5-15-25-? years Duration: up to decades Usually begins after age 10 Characterized by reduced mobility

Some stop walking, others have never walked. Scoliosis is a prominent feature Muscle Wasting Rigidity (stiffness) Dystonia (increased muscle tone with abnormal extremity or trunk positions)

No decline in cognition, communication or hand skills

Repetitive hand movements may decrease. Eye gaze/contact usually improves Puberty begins at the expected age in most girls

Page 19: Pervasive Developmental Disorders September 5, 2006 PSYC 4930

RS: Pathogenesis - EtiologyGenetic Disorder Usually caused by a sporadic mutation (not inherited) or

a defective regulatory MECP2 gene on the X chromosome (often father’s sperm)

MECP2 Tell other genes when to turn off and stop producing their own proteins, failure results in excessive amounts of other proteins

Why is Rett’s seen almost exclusively in females? Since males have an X and a Y chromosome, they lack a "backup" copy of the X chromosome that can compensate for a defective 1

Mutations typically lethal to the male fetus The diagnosis remains a clinical one

Not made solely on the basis of MECP2 mutations. RS can occur with or without mutations in MECP2, and MECP2 mutations can occur without the diagnosis of RS.

MECP2 gene 70-90% with “classical” RS MECP2 gene 0-30% with atypical RS

Page 20: Pervasive Developmental Disorders September 5, 2006 PSYC 4930

Rett Syndrome Treatment: Multidisciplinary

No cure Treatment is symptomatic — focusing on the management of symptoms

Medication may be needed for breathing irregularities and motor difficulties, and antiepileptic drugs may be used to control seizures

Monitoring for scoliosis and possible heart abnormalities

Psychology: Behavioral therapy approaches designed to deal with the behavioral deficits and excesses

Occupational therapy Physiotherapy Hydrotherapy may prolong mobility Nutritionists to help them maintain adequate Special academic, social, vocational, and support services

Page 21: Pervasive Developmental Disorders September 5, 2006 PSYC 4930

Rett Syndrome

Long-Term Prognosis Little is known Females have a 95% chance of surviving to 25 years old

Morbidity often related to seizure disorder or swallowing difficulties

Future: Stem Cell and Gene Therapy

Page 22: Pervasive Developmental Disorders September 5, 2006 PSYC 4930

Case Example 2: From the Yale Developmental Disabilities Clinic

“John’s early history was within normal limits. By age 2 he was speaking in sentences, and his development appeared to be proceeding appropriately. At age 30 months he was noted to abruptly exhibit a period of marked behavioral regression shortly after the birth of a sibling. He lost previously acquired skills in communication and was no longer toilet trained. He became uninterested in social interaction, and various unusual self-stimulatory behaviors became evident. Comprehensive medical examination failed to reveal any conditions that might account for this development regression. Behaviorally he exhibited features of autism. At follow-up at age 12 he still was not speaking, apart from an occasional single word, and had been placed in a school for the severely disabled.”

Page 23: Pervasive Developmental Disorders September 5, 2006 PSYC 4930

Pervasive Developmental Disorders:

Childhood Disintegrative Disorder

Originally labeled as "dementia infantilis" by Heller (1930), Childhood Disintegrative Disorder is another severe and progressive disorder occurring after a period of normality

Usually develops after age 2, but before age 10

Involves a rapid regression in behavior where there may be a loss of social, language, motor, and other skills (e.g., play, bladder and bowel control)

Regression or disintegration usually takes place over a period of six to nine months

Page 24: Pervasive Developmental Disorders September 5, 2006 PSYC 4930

DSM-IV: 299.10 CDD

Clinically significant loss of previously acquired skills in two or more of the following areas:

1. Language: Dramatic regression in expressive and/or receptive language & communication

2. Deterioration of social skills and/or adaptive skills

3. Loss of active and or imaginative play / change in method of play becomes ritualistic

4. Delays in developmental motor skills

5. Failure to control bowel & bladder functions

Page 25: Pervasive Developmental Disorders September 5, 2006 PSYC 4930

Abnormal Functioning in 2 or more of the following areas: Limited social interactions

E.g., Lack of reciprocal emotion, avoiding eye contact, inability to develop social relationships with same-age peers, and/or inappropriate distress.

Poor communication / language skills Loss of attained language, failure to initiate or maintain a conversation, involuntary repetition of words or phrases just spoken by others (i.e., Echolalia)

Restricted, repetitive, and stereotyped patterns of behavior

E.g., Lining up toys, no imaginative play Motor stereotypes and/or mannerisms such as self-stimulation, hand flapping, and tip toe walking.

DSM-IV: 299.10 CDD

Page 26: Pervasive Developmental Disorders September 5, 2006 PSYC 4930

Childhood Disintegrative Disorder

Childhood Disintegrative Disorder is very rare More common in males than females May develop slowly or suddenly (between ages of 2 and 10) and its course is variable

Sometimes, after the initial loss of skills, the condition will remain static, with some limited improvement in the area of social behavior

In other cases there is progressive deterioriation

Usually, however, problems in social interaction, communication, and behavior remain relatively constant over time

Page 27: Pervasive Developmental Disorders September 5, 2006 PSYC 4930

CDD vs. Autism The obsessional and stereotypic behaviors and the impairment in social interactions that often accompany this disorder can resemble certain aspects of autistic disorder.

10X less common than Autism Later age of onset than Autism Research suggests a generally worse

prognosis than Autism

Page 28: Pervasive Developmental Disorders September 5, 2006 PSYC 4930

Childhood Disintegrative Disorder: Etiology

Little information is available regarding the etiology of this disorder

Central nervous system involvement plays a role

While these findings are suggestive, more definitive investigations into the etiology of this disorder remain to be done.

Page 29: Pervasive Developmental Disorders September 5, 2006 PSYC 4930

Childhood Disintegrative Disorder:Treatment

Multi-component approach similar to that suggested for the treatment of Rett's disorder might be useful

Use of behavioral approaches to modify problematic behavioral excesses and deficits, along with appropriate special educational approaches

Page 30: Pervasive Developmental Disorders September 5, 2006 PSYC 4930

Pervasive Developmental Disorders:

Asperger’s Disorder First published account of this disorder by Austrian psychiatrist Hans Asperger in 1944 who initially referred to the condition as "autistic psychopathy“

However, more recent authors have commented on the similarities between Autism and Asperger’s

Indeed, there is some debate as to whether this disorder is actually distinct from autistic disorder

Page 31: Pervasive Developmental Disorders September 5, 2006 PSYC 4930

Asperger’s Disorder Essential features of the disorder:

Severe impairments in social interactions Restricted and repetitive patterns of interests, activities and/or behaviors

No clinically significant delays in cognitive development, language development, or impairments in adaptive behavior, apart from those related to impairments in social interaction

While not considered a specific symptom of Asperger's Disorder, children with this condition are often delayed in meeting major motor milestones (e.g., crawling, walking) and are frequently characterized as clumsy

Page 32: Pervasive Developmental Disorders September 5, 2006 PSYC 4930

Asperger’s Disorder Unlike autism, where social impairments seem to result from an intense desire to withdraw from or avoid social interactions, the social impairment in Asperger’s seems to result from a lack of social skills.

These children seem to have a marked inability to understand and use rules which typically guide social behavior.

The child may show significant problems with: Nonverbal behaviors such as maintaining appropriate physical proximity to others while interacting

Making and sustaining eye contact Appropriately using gestures, facial expression and other nonverbal behaviors to regulate social interactions

Page 33: Pervasive Developmental Disorders September 5, 2006 PSYC 4930

Asperger’s Disorder Social behavior often appear quite egocentric and self-centered

Children with Asperger’s may frequently pursue their own highly personalized interests in social encounters without apparent awareness that the other person does not share similar interests

Behavior occurring within the context of two way social interactions often appears as inept, naive and peculiar

Page 34: Pervasive Developmental Disorders September 5, 2006 PSYC 4930

Asperger’s Disorder Restricted and repetitive patterns of behaviors, interests, or activities are often striking and may be manifest in a variety of ways: Some may be preoccupied with specific activities (e.g., spinning objects) or become overly attached to certain objects or familiar places and become intensely upset when separated from them

Others show an inflexible adherence to daily routines

Page 35: Pervasive Developmental Disorders September 5, 2006 PSYC 4930

Asperger’s Disorder Commonly, children with this disorder show an extreme all-consuming involvement in some specific area of interest

The child may spend most of his/her time learning facts related to the area

They may collect things having to do with the area, and spend an enormous about of time talking to others about this area whether or not they are interested

While investing a great deal of time learning about their area of interest, the child may have little understanding of the facts that they learn

Page 36: Pervasive Developmental Disorders September 5, 2006 PSYC 4930

Asperger’s Disorder Unlike other PDD’s, in Asperger's Disorder No clinically significant delay in cognitive development or in the area of language

Child may learn to speak at a normal age however, they may show marked peculiarities in language: May invent words Use pronouns incorrectly Repeat words or phrases over and over in a stereotyped manner

Speech content is often overly pedantic, consisting of long one-sided discussions about the child's favorite topic

Page 37: Pervasive Developmental Disorders September 5, 2006 PSYC 4930

Asperger’s Disorder:Prevalence

Little data on prevalence, but children with Asperger's Disorder are quite rare

Probably more rare than autism More common in males than in females with sex ratios ranging from 4:1 to 9:1 reported

Page 38: Pervasive Developmental Disorders September 5, 2006 PSYC 4930

Asperger’s Disorder:Etiology

Originally considered to have a genetic basis (Asperger, 1944).

While no formal studies to assess the possibility of genetic transmission have been published, Wing (1981) has provided relevant case study findings: Of the 34 cases with this disorder that she studied, 5 of the 16 fathers and 2 of the 24 mothers had, "to a marked degree" behavior resembling that observed in their children

Although not definitive, some support for the role of biological factors in this disorder comes from the fact that these children sometimes show evidence of nonspecific neurological symptoms

Page 39: Pervasive Developmental Disorders September 5, 2006 PSYC 4930

Asperger’s Disorder:Course and Prognosis

Given higher level of functioning (due to a lack of basic cognitive and language deficits) prognosis appears much better than other PDD’s

Some able to engage in gainful employment and function in a semi-independent manner.

Prognosis is intimately related to treatment and management approaches designed to deal with the child's difficulties

Page 40: Pervasive Developmental Disorders September 5, 2006 PSYC 4930

Asperger’s Disorder:Treatment

No treatment has been shown to modify the basic underlying impairment

treatment should include behavioral approaches focusing on enhancing the child's ability to function in social situations along with an educational program

Psychotherapy may be useful as the person becomes aware of the degree to which social skills difficulties make it difficult to function

Page 41: Pervasive Developmental Disorders September 5, 2006 PSYC 4930

Pervasive Developmental Disorders:

Autistic Disorder Infantile autism was first described by Leo Kanner (l943) in his classic paper " Autistic Disturbances of Affective Contact"

In this article, Kanner highlighted the defining characteristics of 11 children seen in his child psychiatry practice at Johns Hopkins University

His views regarding this disorder have heavily influenced present day views of the disorder, emphasized a number of features

Page 42: Pervasive Developmental Disorders September 5, 2006 PSYC 4930

Autistic Disorder Unlike certain other severe disorders of childhood, Kanner assumed autism to have an early onset.

He believed the disorder to be present from the beginning of life, or at least to become obvious during the first year

He referred to it as an "inborn disturbance"

He felt that this early onset served to differentiate the disorder from other problems, which at that time, were judged to be manifestations of childhood psychosis

Page 43: Pervasive Developmental Disorders September 5, 2006 PSYC 4930

Autism: Social Deficits

Autistic children have a primary disturbance in social relationships and an apparent inability to relate to others.

They seem aloof, often oblivious to the presence of others, and are often described as being in a world of their own – “Like in a shell”, “Happiest when left alone”, “Acting as though people aren’t there”, etc.

Early life, infants may fail to show anticipatory posturing when the parent attempts to pick them up from the crib, and the failure of the infant to mold him or herself to the body of the parent

Page 44: Pervasive Developmental Disorders September 5, 2006 PSYC 4930

Autistic Disorder Kanner suggested that the outstanding fundamental disorder is “the children’s [in]ability to relate themselves in the ordinary way to people and situations from the beginning of life”

He goes on to note that “this is not as in schizophrenic children or adults, a departure from an initially present relationships - it is not a “withdrawal” from formerly existing participation.”

There is from the start, an “extreme aloneness that whenever possible disregards, ignores, shuts out anything that comes to the child from the outside”.

Page 45: Pervasive Developmental Disorders September 5, 2006 PSYC 4930

Autistic Disorder All autistic children show evidence of a severe language disorder

Speech is absent in about 50 percent of cases

Those that develop speech typically show unusual features such as echolalia (i.e., the repetition of what someone else has said), pronoun reversal (e.g., referring to oneself as "you" and to others as "I"), or atypical tone (e.g., monotone, inappropriate to content)

Even though some children with autism develop fairly large vocabularies, they usually cannot use speech to communicate with others

Page 46: Pervasive Developmental Disorders September 5, 2006 PSYC 4930

Autistic Disorder Kanner noted that although some of his 11

cases developed language, they were no better able to communicate than were those who remained mute

Children with autism who speak often have no difficulty naming objects and may have an aptitude for learning previously constructed verbal materials such as poems, songs, and lists of things

Such learning, however, seems to be without any appreciation of the meaning of these words

There is usually minimal evidence of spontaneous speech that serves a communicative function

Page 47: Pervasive Developmental Disorders September 5, 2006 PSYC 4930

Autistic Disorder Kanner and others have noted that autistic children seem to display an desire for the maintenance of sameness

children with autism often get upset when things in their environment are changed ‑ when routines are changed or when toys the child has left in a particular position are moved

This may result in a catastrophic reaction (e.g., tantrum, screaming) lasting until things are returned to their former state

This desire for sameness may lead some children to display a wide range of ritualistic behaviors

Page 48: Pervasive Developmental Disorders September 5, 2006 PSYC 4930

Autistic Disorder Other behaviors frequently observed in children with autism: Stereotyped behaviors (e.g., mouthing objects, flapping arms and hands, rocking, toe-walking).

Over-responsive to environmental stimuli (e.g., light, sound, foods, clothing)

Under-responsiveness and insensitivity to pain and in associated self‑injurious behaviors (e.g,. head banging, scratching, skin picking)

In play, interest in parts of objects and little symbolic play

Often associated with mental retardation http://www.up-to-date.com/autism.tv/ARI/i

ndex.html

Page 49: Pervasive Developmental Disorders September 5, 2006 PSYC 4930

Autistic Disorder:Prevalence

Although autism is a relatively rare disorder it has been difficult to determine its exact frequency of occurrence.

Varying rates of prevalence are due to: different diagnostic criteria frequently confused with other severe disorders of childhood

not all children with autism come to the attention of researchers

Prevalence data from early studies suggested very low rates of occurrence, typically 4 or 5 cases per 10,000 children, and as low as 2 per 10,000 for "classic" cases

Page 50: Pervasive Developmental Disorders September 5, 2006 PSYC 4930

Autistic Disorder:Prevalence

Gillberg (1990) cites studies, employing differing diagnostic criteria that report figures ranging from 6.6 to 13.6 per 10,000.

The few studies using DSM criteria have reported rates on the order of 10 per 10,000.

The higher rates found in more recent studies has had to do, at least in part, with better detection of autistics among those with severe mental retardation

The disorder is more frequent in boys than in girls, with sex ratios ranging from 2.0 to 1 to 5.7 to 1

Page 51: Pervasive Developmental Disorders September 5, 2006 PSYC 4930

Autistic Disorder:Prognosis

In reviewing follow‑up studies of children with autism DeMyer, et al. (1981) note that as many as 60 to 70 percent live a life of complete or semi‑dependence, at home or in an institution

Only about 1-2% achieve normal levels of independence, while others show a borderline level of functioning

A better prognosis seemed to be associated with higher IQ (> 60)

Page 52: Pervasive Developmental Disorders September 5, 2006 PSYC 4930

Autistic Disorder:Prognosis

In a paper entitled "How far can autistic children go in matters of social adaptation?" Kanner (l973) reported on a follow‑up of 96 children with autism seen prior to l953

Although most did not fare as well, 11 of the 96 achieved a “favorable outcome” 3 obtained college degrees 3 went to junior college. 1 other was doing well in college 4 did not go beyond high school or special education

Page 53: Pervasive Developmental Disorders September 5, 2006 PSYC 4930

Autistic Disorder:Prognosis

Their occupations included accountant, duplicating machine operator, lab technician, bank teller, and several other types of unskilled work

Kanner noted that although these 11 children did show a “favorable outcome”, none seemed to show any interest in the opposite sex or marriage, suggesting continued problems in close relationships

Outcome was unrelated to having received psychiatric treatment

Best predictor seemed to be having useful speech by age 5

Page 54: Pervasive Developmental Disorders September 5, 2006 PSYC 4930

Etiology of Autism Views regarding the causes of autism can generally be classified as psychogenic or biogenic in nature

Psychogenic theories characterized the parents of autistic children as cold, aloof, obsessional, refrigerator-like, and in other less than positive terms (see Kanner, l943)

Bettelheim (l967) suggested that negative maternal attitudes are of major importance in the development of this disorder (“refrigerator mother”)

Page 55: Pervasive Developmental Disorders September 5, 2006 PSYC 4930

Etiology of Autism Research designed to link family variables to autism has provided little support for psychogenic theories

DeMyer, et al. (1981) have noted that, ". . . Parents of autistic children have been found to display no more signs of mental or emotional illness than parents of children with organic disorders (with or without psychosis).”

Page 56: Pervasive Developmental Disorders September 5, 2006 PSYC 4930

Etiology of Autism Growing evidence suggests that autism is a biologically based disorder

This point of view is supported by a wide range of studies that have in one way or another implicated the role of biological factors

The specific biological factors that cause this disorder have not been identified, although several candidates have been identified

Page 57: Pervasive Developmental Disorders September 5, 2006 PSYC 4930

Etiology of Autism Autism has been shown to be related to biological problems such as: the development of seizure disorders abnormal EEG's congenital infections (e.g,.rubella) chromosomal abnormalities pregnancy and birth complications neurotransmitter abnormalities abnormal CT scans structural abnormalities of the left hemisphere

genetic factors

Page 58: Pervasive Developmental Disorders September 5, 2006 PSYC 4930

Etiology of Autism Taken together, research findings provide strong support for a biogenic perspective

Indeed, after reviewing much of the literature related to neurobiological factors in autism, Gillberg (1990) has concluded that "autism is now regarded as a behaviorally defined syndrome of neurological impairment with a wide variety of underlying medical etiologies (p. 106)"

Again, the specific biological factors most relevant to the etiology of autism and their specific role remain to be uncovered

Page 59: Pervasive Developmental Disorders September 5, 2006 PSYC 4930

Autistic Disorder:Treatment

http://viewers.multicastmedia.com/viewer/viewerbroadcast_odc.asp?type=1&podc=1002381&networkid=3000091&setspeed=go&oid=1007032&ver=7&gid=0

Due to cognitive and social impairments, most children with autism are unlikely to benefit from "talk therapies", although some authors have suggested that individual psychotherapy can be of some value in working with a small number of higher functioning children

While there is presently no "cure" for autism, behavioral approaches have achieved the most obvious success

Page 60: Pervasive Developmental Disorders September 5, 2006 PSYC 4930

Autistic Disorder:Treatment

Applied Behavior Analysis Operant procedures (e.g., rewards, shaping) combined with modeling, have been found useful in teaching language, as well as other socially adaptive behaviors, and in decreasing many inappropriate behaviors

Bringing about such behavioral changes requires skills which very few clinicians possess and an enormous amount of time

Page 61: Pervasive Developmental Disorders September 5, 2006 PSYC 4930

Autistic Disorder:Treatment

Lovaas (1987) conducted intensive, long-term, treatment program that focused on imitation, language development, the expression of appropriate emotions, and appropriate play behaviors

Of the 19 children participating in the program, it was possible to mainstream 8, and 7 were described as being indistinguishable from typical children

While the degree to which these children did in fact approach normality has been questioned, the results clearly highlight the potential contribution of behavioral treatments

Page 62: Pervasive Developmental Disorders September 5, 2006 PSYC 4930

Autistic Disorder:Treatment

Treatment and Education of Autistic and Related Communication Handicapped Children (TEACCH)

N.C. program for individuals with autism Includes education, communication, and behavior strategies for working with children with autism

Focuses on the building upon the strengths of children rather than just improving deficits

Page 63: Pervasive Developmental Disorders September 5, 2006 PSYC 4930

Autistic Disorder:Treatment

TEACCH structured teaching: Use of schedules Classroom structure (e.g., setting up

clear areas for work/play, minimizing distractions)

Use of visual cues (e.g., polaroids, color-coded tasks)

Alternate enjoyable and less enjoyable activities

Use minimal language or gestures www.teacch.com

Page 64: Pervasive Developmental Disorders September 5, 2006 PSYC 4930

Autistic Disorder:Treatment

Some drugs may have beneficial effects for children with autism: Haloperidol

Associated with improved learning and behaviors

Fenfluramine Reduces levels of serotonin in the blood, some promise in improving social and intellectual functioning

Stimulants: manage hyperactivity, impulsivity Vitamin B6 with magnesium supplements

While behavior therapy and drug treatments have both shown some promise in treating autism, there is research evidence to suggest that treatment should not be an either/or proposition

Page 65: Pervasive Developmental Disorders September 5, 2006 PSYC 4930

Living with Autism:Temple Grandin, Ph.D.

http://www.npr.org/templates/story/story.php?storyId=4278538