Negative Psychological Effects of Selection Methods Construct Formulation and an Empirical Investigation Into an AC

  • Upload
    tomor

  • View
    223

  • Download
    0

Embed Size (px)

Citation preview

  • 7/29/2019 Negative Psychological Effects of Selection Methods Construct Formulation and an Empirical Investigation Into an AC

    1/21

    Negative Psychological Effects of SelectionMethods: Construct Formulation and an

    Empirical Investigation into an Assessment Center

    Neil Anderson*University of Amsterdam

    Vicki GoltsiMediterranean College, Athens

    This paper defines and formulates the construct of negative psychological effects (NPEs)of selection and assessment methods upon applicants. The results of an empirical studyinto NPEs in an operational assessment center are also reported. Applicants (n5107)completed measures at three timepoints: Time 1 (just before the AC), Time 2 (immediatelyafter the AC but before outcome decisions were known), and Time 3 (6 months after theAC). Both accepted and rejected candidates completed all three measures, which included

    self-esteem, mental health, positive and negative affect, and career exploration behavior.No evidence of NPEs was found for rejected candidates despite significant between-groupdifferences on feedback reactions level items. Interestingly, well-being and positive affectdeclined slightly for successful candidates at subsequent measurement compared againstbaseline Time 1 norms. Reactions level outcomes did, however, differ significantlybetween accepted and rejected candidates, with the latter rating feedback dimensions farless favorably. These results are discussed in relation to future research into thepsychological impact of selection procedures upon candidates and the need for appliedpsychologists and HR practitioners to demonstrate that assessment methods do notexhibit NPEs upon applicants.

    E xamination of applicant reactions to selectionmethods has been a rapidly burgeoning area ofresearch interest over recent years, whose findings have

    implications for the practical design of assessment and

    selection procedures by organizations. Several comprehen-

    sive reviews of this research conducted in different

    countries internationally have been published and are

    now available (e.g., Anderson, Born, & Cunningham-

    Snell, 2001; Chan & Schmitt, 2004; Imus & Ryan, 2005;

    Ryan & Ployhart, 2000; Polyhart & Harold, 2004;

    Schmitt & Chan, 1999). However, much of this researchhas concerned immediate-level reactions and preference

    perceptions of applicants to different predictors, leaving

    open to question the longer-term impacts and outcomes of

    applicant exposure to specific selection methods (Bauer,

    Maertz, Dolen, & Campion, 1998; Gilliland & Steiner,

    2001). The unequivocal consensus across this body of

    findings is that applicants express clear preferences for

    job-relevant, transparent, and equitably administered

    methods, but that this proliferation of studies has

    failed to address centrally important questions of the

    longer-term impact of selection methods upon candidate

    decision making, attitudes to the organization, or indeed,

    important issues of applicant psychological health and

    well-being.

    Reactions level research, while important and having

    obvious implications for the design and conduct of

    organizational selection procedure, can clearly only shedlight upon perceptual responses and preference evaluations

    by applicants. As noted by Iles and Robertson (1997)

    and Anderson (2005), this level of outcome research

    can only present a picture of applicant responses that

    relates to immediate-level reactive responses. The trigger

    effects of these reactions to candidate decision making

    in selection remain a pointedly under-researched area,

    as do important questions over the impacts, both pragmatic

    and psychological, of selection methods upon applicants.

    The focus of the current paper is on the latter the negative

    *Address for correspondence: Neil Anderson, University of Amster-

    dam Business School, Roeterstraat 11, 1018 WB Amsterdam, The

    Netherlands. E-mail: [email protected]

    INTERNATIONAL JOURNAL OF SELECTION AND ASSESSMENT VOLUME 14 NUMBER 3 SEPTEMBER 2006

    236r 2006 The AuthorsJournal compilation r 2006 Blackwell Publishing Ltd, 9600 Garsington Road,

    Oxford, OX4 2DQ, UK and 350 Main St, Malden, MA 02148, USA

  • 7/29/2019 Negative Psychological Effects of Selection Methods Construct Formulation and an Empirical Investigation Into an AC

    2/21

    (and positive) psychological effects upon applicants of

    undergoing a managerial assessment center.

    This paper has two aims: (i) to provide an operational

    definition and conceptual formulation for the construct of

    negative psychological effects (NPEs) caused by expo-

    sure to selection methods, and (ii) to conduct an

    exploratory, longitudinal study into the potential NPEs of

    a managerial entry-level assessment center. We also aim to

    clarify the diffuse and widening conceptual space coveredby studies into applicant reactions by arguing that our

    understanding of these outcomes will only be improved by

    definitional clarity of the outcome variables under scrutiny.

    Finally, from a methodological point of view, the results

    from the empirical study reported here present novel and

    disconfirmatory findings that exposure to a multi-day

    assessment center caused no discernible NPEs among

    rejected candidates. Effect size statistics are reported,

    showing both a notable lack of NPEs over time upon

    unsuccessful candidates, but also an intriguing lack of

    improvement in self-esteem, psychological well-being, and

    positive affect among successful applicants.

    NPEs and Positive Psychological Effects(PPEs) of Selection Methods

    Toward a Working Definition of NPEs

    The concept of NPEs was first proposed by Anderson

    (2004). Although emphasizing the importance of research

    into the possibility that selection methods may have an

    unintentionally negative impact upon rejected candidates,

    the construct was not originally formulated in detail or a

    working definition proposed. Here, we rectify this by

    proposing that NPEs are most appropriately defined asfollows:

    Declines in applicant psychological well-being, general

    mental health, or core self-esteem that are inveterate,

    measureable, and statistically demonstrable, and that occur

    as a result of exposure to rejection decisions, violations of

    applicant rights, or unprofessional feedback given to

    applicants by recruiters, at any stage during organizational

    selection or promotion assessment procedures.

    There are four main aspects of this definition. First, in

    accordance with the wider literatures in IWO psychology

    on mental health at work, stress, and job-related burnout,NPEs relate to longer-term declines in important aspects of

    applicant mental health, psychological well-being, or core

    self-esteem. Why these referent constructs? All are centrally

    important aspects of the overall well-being of the indivi-

    dual and can be argued to be fundamental concepts

    underlying mental health (e.g., Judge & Bono, 2001; Hart

    & Cooper, 2002). All are longstanding constructs of

    research interest in applied psychology, with literally

    thousands of primary studies and many meta-analyses

    having been published into their antecedent causes and

    consequent effects. In accordance with Judge and Bono

    (2001), we therefore view NPEs as being at the interface of

    applicant mental health and core self-esteem, with NPE

    being a composite construct that encapsulates mental

    health and self-esteem outcomes relevant to applicant

    psychological well-being in its widest sense.

    Second, NPEs relate to causal effects that are inveterate,

    measureable, and statistically demonstrable. The Shorter

    Oxford English Dictionary defines the term inveterate as . . .firmly established by long continuance; deep-rooted;

    obstinate. Thus, we define NPEs as relating to longer-term

    effects that are robust and deep-rooted, but of course not

    necessarily either permanent in their duration or debilitating

    in their effects sufficient to satisfy Diagnostic and statistical

    manual of mental disorders-Fourth Edition (DSM-IV)

    criteria for on-going personality disorders (American Psy-

    chiatric Association, 1994). This acuity level is of course

    rather debateable. That is, the level of impact needed to

    satisfy the definition could be variously argued to be much

    lower (e.g., a short term and minor drop in mood states or

    positiveness), or much higher (e.g., acutely negative anddispositional changes that are relatively permanent). Rather,

    in common with Fletcher (1991), Schuler and Fruhner

    (1993), and Iles and Robertson (1997), we envisage NPEs

    as being present for several weeks and months after receipt of

    a negative selection decision. Thus, such declines in well-

    being and/or core self-esteem need to be measureable, that is,

    to register on widely accepted and psychometrically devel-

    oped instruments, and to be statistically demonstrable. Both

    of these requirements need to be fulfilled for NPEs against an

    organizations selection procedure to be demonstrated prima

    facie, even before other definitional criteria that we propose

    can be evaluated (specifically, causality). It can also be stated

    that NPEs need to be distinguishable from immediate levelreactions, preference ratings, and applicant reactions to

    feedback (see Iles & Robertson, 1997; Robertson, Iles,

    Gratton, & Sharpley, 1991; Ryan & Ployhart, 2000). The

    latter are short-term applicant perceptual reactions to

    selection methods; NPEs refer to longer-term psychological

    impacts that result from being exposed to such methods.

    Indeed, distinguishingthese two levelsof analysis in applicant

    reactions research immediate-level reactions and longer-

    term NPEs is a key contribution of the present paper.

    Third, we propose that although NPEs are more likely,

    as a result, of receiving reject outcome decisions at any

    stage during recruitment and selection procedures, they canalso occur where the candidate is subsequently accepted

    but has been treated in demonstrably unprofessional or

    unambiguously abusive ways by recruiters (Derous, Born,

    & De Witte, 2004). Here, it is possible that even accepted

    candidates could suffer NPEs if the procedural treatment of

    the organization has been so extreme, so biased, or so

    personally abusive as to trigger such perceptions of

    procedural or distributive injustice or deeper-rooted

    psychological impacts (Gilliland & Steiner, 2001). Anec-

    dotal evidence of poor treatment in selection situations

    NPES OF ASSESSMENT CENTERS 237

    r 2006 The AuthorsJournal compilationr Blackwell Publishing Ltd. 2006 Volume 14 Number 3 September 2006

  • 7/29/2019 Negative Psychological Effects of Selection Methods Construct Formulation and an Empirical Investigation Into an AC

    3/21

    abounds, so we purposely include the possibility in our

    operational definition that NPEs will not necessarily be

    restricted to rejected applicants alone.

    Finally, for NPEs to hold up as identifiable outcomes

    from exposure to selection predictors, there is an onus of

    proof of causality of exposure to the selection method itself

    as the primary cause. Threats to causal inference need to be

    examined and measured in that rejected applicants could

    also have been exposed to other extraneous life events overthe time period of interest. Although this definition

    acknowledges that NPEs can occur at any stage in

    recruitment and selection, it is most likely that they will

    emerge from methods that are of a longer duration, that

    involve applicant preparation, that are job-relevant and

    face valid, and that the results from which cannot easily be

    externally attributed by applicants (Anderson, 2004; Chan

    & Schmitt, 2004). Such effects are entirely plausible in

    external selection procedures, but perhaps even more so in

    internal assessment procedures for promotion where both

    accepted and rejected applicants remain in the employ of

    the organization (Fletcher, 1991). This raises the prospectthat NPEs can have knock-on effects to job performance,

    both in the case of unsuccessful internal candidates who

    remain in their existing job function and for successful

    applicants who are given inappropriately negative feed-

    back or who feel that their expectations of procedural

    justice have in some way been violated. Thus, NPEs may

    ultimately have considerable and quantifiable costs for an

    organization in terms of poorer levels of job performance,

    that is, NPEs will have a substantial and negative utility

    value, whose exact costs to an organization may remain

    unknown and unrecognized.

    Toward a Working Definition of PositivePsychological Effects (PPEs)

    Counter to these arguments, it is feasible to suggest that far

    more positive effects might be observed where applicants

    obtain successful outcome decisions or where candidates

    receive positive feedback after taking part in a particular

    selection method or procedure. Here, the opposite of NPEs

    that can be hypothesized may occur, that is, PPEs. As a

    direct, polemic opposite of NPEs, PPEs can be defined as

    follows:

    Increases in applicant psychological well-being, general

    mental health, or core self-esteem that are inveterate,

    measureable, and statistically demonstrable, and that occur

    as a result of exposure to acceptance decisions, perceived

    respecting of applicant rights, or complementary feedback

    given to applicants by recruiters, at any stage during

    organizational selection or promotion assessment proce-

    dures.

    Again, this definition supposes that such changes need

    to be relatively enduring and measurable through typical

    pre- and post-study designs. Moreover, it is likely that

    perhaps such changes in psychological well-being and

    mental health will be more commonly determined than

    changes in core self-esteem as a dispositional, relatively

    stable personality characteristic (e.g., Judge & Bono,

    2001). Nevertheless, it is possible that where an applicant

    is going through several selection procedures concurrently

    (as is often the case), where applicants know others who

    have been unsuccessful, and where they receive multiple

    positive decisions and/or feedback comments on theircapabilities, that this results in enhanced core self-esteem at

    least temporarily. The prospect for PPEs for successful

    candidates is therefore noted and this tentative definition

    advanced as an opposite set of outcome conditions to those

    proposed for NPEs.

    To summarize, these working definitions of NPEs and

    PPEs are proposed to guide and provoke future research

    into this neglected topic area. Both are intended to build

    upon and extend earlier work by Anderson (2004) and to

    provide a basic conceptual formulation for psychological

    effect research and, importantly, to distinguish this level of

    analysis from that of earlier research into applicantreactions and positiveness ratings of selection method

    and feedback perceptions favorability.

    Past Research into NPEs in Selection

    Appendix A provides a summary of published studies into

    applicant NPEs conducted since 1989 onwards. Studies

    were identified from a comprehensive search and review of

    the literature undertaken by the present authors. For the

    sake of brevity, we used several rules of thumb to decide

    whether or not a study should be included in this appendix.

    Initially, the study must meet key aspects of our definitionof NPEs proposed above, that is, it must have included

    outcome variables that relate to applicant mental health,

    well-being, or core self-esteem. Studies that for instance

    examined applicant reactions to selection methods alone

    (e.g., Horvath, Ryan, & Stierwalt, 2000; Lievens, De

    Corte, & Brysse, 2003) were not included in this table (for

    such a comprehensive table covering reactions-level

    studies, see Ryan & Ployhart, 2000). Furthermore, we

    only included studies in this appendix if they examined

    effects longitudinally during selection procedures, not just

    immediate pre- and post-test effects of reactions to

    cognitive ability tests and other measures (e.g., Macan,Avedon, Paese, & Smith, 1994). In this regard, past studies

    have often measured key variables using such pre- and

    post-method designs, but with notably few studies in-

    corporating a longer-term outcome measurement time-

    period some weeks or months following selection

    decisions. Finally, we include studies relating to different

    selection methods (not just assessment centers) in Appen-

    dix A on the grounds that relatively little research has been

    conducted across all predictors to date, and so a

    comprehensive summary is warranted.

    238 NEIL ANDERSON AND VICKI GOLTSI

    International Journal of Selection and Assessmentr 2006 The Authors

    Journal compilationr Blackwell Publishing Ltd. 2006

  • 7/29/2019 Negative Psychological Effects of Selection Methods Construct Formulation and an Empirical Investigation Into an AC

    4/21

    Overviewing this existing body of research and counter

    to what might be expected, there are relatively few studies

    that have so far examined aspects of psychological impact

    caused by selection procedures. This appendix summarizes

    the findings of the fourteen published studies we located

    over this 16-year period (19892004, inclusive), and covers

    all predictor methods, not just assessment centers. Far less

    research has examined NPEs then than might have been

    anticipated, even in comparison with the growth of studiesinto applicant reactions over recent years (Salgado, 1999;

    Salgado, Viswesvaran, & Ones, 2002). Despite the relative

    paucity of empirical studies, more research has been

    conducted very recently (five studies were published in

    2004 alone: Derous, Born, & De Witte; Bauer, Truxillo,

    Paronto, Campion, & Weekley; van Vianen, Taris,

    Scholten, & Schinkel; Schmitt, Oswald, Kim, Gillespie,

    & Ramsay; and, Schinkel, van Dierendonck, & Anderson),

    and important findings emerge from this emergent stream

    of research. A long-established focus of interest has been

    the impact of cognitive ability and GMA testing upon

    applicants (Gilliland, 1994; Lounsbury, Bobrow, & Jensen,1989; Ployhart & Ryan, 1997; Ployhart & Ryan, 1998;

    Ployhart, Ryan, & Bennett, 1999; Schinkel et al., 2004;

    Schmit & Ryan, 1992; Smither, Reilly, Millsap, Pearlman,

    & Stoffey, 1993; van Vianen et al., 2004). Consistent race

    differences in attitudes to GMA testing, links between

    cognitive tests and favorable attitudes toward the organi-

    zations selection procedure, recommendation and offer

    acceptance intentions by applicants, and impacts upon self-

    assessed performance, fairness, anxiety, and candidate

    affective well-being have all been reported across these

    studies. Notably fewer studies could be located that have

    examined the psychological impacts of several other

    popular selection methods, however, including structuredinterviews, personality inventories, work sample tests, or

    situational judgment tests.

    Assessment centers, despite their increasing popularity as

    final-stage predictors for selection in several countries (e.g.,

    Lievens & Klimoski, 2001), have similarly received scant

    research attention into their potential for NPEs. Only two

    peer-reviewed studies could be located that have examined

    such longer-term psychological impacts upon actual job

    applicants, each with directly contradictory findings (see also

    Schuler & Fruhner, 1993). Robertson etal. (1991) conducted

    a cross-sectional study into the impacts of three methods

    biodata, situational interviews, and assessment centers atdiffering stages in the selection procedure. They found no

    significant effects for assessment center experience or

    outcome decision upon subsequent measures of applicant

    self-esteem or psychological well-being. Fletcher (1991),

    conversely, undertook theonly truly longitudinal study of AC

    psychological impact, measuring applicant responses im-

    mediately before, immediately after, and 6 months following

    the AC itself. Significant declines in rejected applicants job

    involvement and mood at work were reported, while

    significant increases in successful applicants work ethic,

    acquisitiveness, status aspiration, and job mastery were

    found. Fletcher alsoreports significant maineffectdifferences

    at Time 3 measures between successful and unsuccessful

    applicants on measures of organizational commitment, need

    for achievement, and psychological well-being. Note that this

    AC was for an internal promotion procedure within a single

    financial services organization where the unsuccessful

    candidates continued to work alongside those who were

    successful, and therefore, subsequently promoted. Despitethe laudible longitudinal element of this study design, the

    sample was only 57 respondents,40 of whom were successful

    and only 17 of whom were rejected for an accelerated

    promotion program, an unusual ratio in comparison with

    those normally found in assessment centers for selection.

    Given that so few studies have been conducted into the

    longer-term impact of ACs upon candidates, our under-

    standing of these potential effects is at best inadequate. As

    Fletcher (1991) correctly argues, exposure to a multi-day,

    job-relevant, assessment procedure that is evaluated by

    trained assessors who themselves are perceived as success-

    ful organizational members, and where often quite detailedfeedback is given to candidates all serve as factors to

    heighten the potential for NPEs against rejected applicants.

    Likewise, we may hypothesize that successful completion

    of such an interventive and psychologically demanding

    method may well result in improvements in candidate

    psychological well-being and self-esteem in having com-

    peted successfully against many more candidates who

    were subsequently rejected: such effects might be contrarily

    termed Postitive Psychological Effects, or PPEs. Whether

    this is the case or not, operational ACs for selection and

    internal promotion present a critical case opportunity in

    which to conduct research into such effects.

    Method

    Sample

    A sample of 107 candidates at final stage assessment center

    participated in the study. The candidates applied for a

    highly prestigeous managerial-entry and graduate training

    scheme within a leading multinational organization head-

    quartered in the United Kingdom. The training scheme

    lasted 3 years in total and included fixed-term postings to

    various sectors of the business, all of which involvedperiodic attendance at trainee managerial courses and

    leadership development events. Of this total of 107

    participants, 53 (49.5%) were female and 54 (50.5%)

    were male. This proportion of virtually equal males and

    females at AC reflects the initial proportions of applicants

    to this organizations selection procedure. Candidate age

    ranged from 21 to 37, with a mean of 23.4 (SD52.6).

    Fifty-seven percent of participants reported having past

    experience of attending an assessment center, while the

    modal figure for numbers of past ACs experienced was 2.

    NPES OF ASSESSMENT CENTERS 239

    r 2006 The AuthorsJournal compilationr Blackwell Publishing Ltd. 2006 Volume 14 Number 3 September 2006

  • 7/29/2019 Negative Psychological Effects of Selection Methods Construct Formulation and an Empirical Investigation Into an AC

    5/21

    Of the 107 applicants, 28 were successful (26.2%) whereas

    79 (73.8%) were rejected following completion of this AC.

    Recruitment and Prescreening Procedure

    Following advertisements in the national press, applicants

    responded by standardized application form. Applicants

    were pre-screened on the basis of university qualifications,

    expected gradings, outside interests and positions ofresponsibility, and responses to open-ended questions as

    to their reasons for applying. Short-listed applicants

    subsequently underwent a 30-min structured interview by

    telephone. Successful applicants following the structured

    interview were invited to the 2-day assessment center.

    Assessment Center

    The assessment center took place over 2 days and involved

    an intensive process of multi-rater observations of three

    exercises targeting nine dimensions. Table 1 sets out the AC

    exercise-dimension matrix in overview. In addition to thethree exercises (Analysis Report, Group Task, and Pre-

    sentation), candidates gave verbal and numerical ability

    tests and also completed an unstructured interview with

    three assessors toward the end of the entire AC. The nine

    dimensions evaluated were Analysis, Communication,

    Planning and Organizing, Influencing Others, Team Work,

    Leadership, Innovationand Change, Decision Making, and

    Personal Effectiveness.

    Procedure

    A three-measurement timepoint, longitudinal design wasused. Time 1 questionnaires were administered immedi-

    ately before the start of the AC while candidates were being

    welcomed and briefed. Time 2 questionnaires were

    completed immediately after the AC had finished. Note,

    importantly, that outcome decisions from the AC were not

    yet known by participants, this timing being essential to

    remove any potential biasing effects of outcome decisions

    upon their immediate reactions. Time 3 measures were

    taken for both successful and unsuccessful candidates 6

    months after the completion of the AC and once outcome

    decisions were known by all participants. Successful

    candidates were followed up at this stage within the

    organization as all had commenced employment by thisstage. All 28 candidates to whom offers were made

    accepted employment as this was a highly attractive

    managerial training scheme. For rejected candidates, the

    second author followed up responses using a postal

    questionnaire to their home addresses.

    Measures

    In addition to biographical variables measured at Time 1

    only (age, qualifications, ethnic minority status, and so

    forth), measures were completed at all three timepoints

    by participants. The questionnaire contained scales evalu-ating self-esteem, psychological well-being (the General

    Health Questionnaire (GHQ) short-form version), and

    positive and negative affect. Additionally, feedback

    perceptions were evaluated only at Time 3 after the

    organization had been able to give applicants formal

    feedback on their performance, and career exploration

    behavior was measured for unsuccessful and successful

    applicants at Time 1 and Time 3 only. The details of

    measures are as follows:

    Self-Esteem. Rosenbergs (1989) standarized scale ofself-esteem was used. For the present sample, internal

    consistencies (Cronbachs a) ranged from .77.88, and

    testretest reliability was .82 (Time 1 to Time 2) and .85(Time 2 to Time 3).

    Psychological Well-Being. The short-form versionGHQ (GHQ-12) was used to evaluate psyhological health

    (Goldberg, 1992). Designed to detect non-psychotic

    disorders in normal adult populations, this measure has

    been widely used as a valid and reliable scale for measuring

    key aspects of psychological health (Goldberg, 1992). The

    present sample as ranged from .82 to .90; retest reliabilities

    were above .70 between both measurement timepoints.

    Note that as a measure of clinical disorder, higher scores on

    the GHQ were originally scaled to indicate a higher

    probability of disorder and thus indicate lower levels ofpsychological well-being. Scores were reverse coded in the

    present study in order to simplify interpretation of

    subsequent analyses such that higher scores indicate more

    positive psychological well-being.

    Positive and Negative Affect. The Positive and Nega-tive Affect Schedule (PANAS) was used (Watson, Clark, &

    Tellegen, 1988). Consisting of 20 adjectives, this brief

    schedule measures the two dimensions of positive and

    negative affect. Ten adjectives describe negative moods

    (distressed, upset, guilty, scared, hostile, irritable, ashamed,

    Table 1. The assessment center matrix

    Dimensions

    Exercises

    Analysis

    report

    Group

    task Presentation

    Analysis * *Communication * *

    Planning and organizing * *

    Influencing others * *

    Team work *

    Leadership *

    Innovation and change * *

    Decision making * *

    Personal effectiveness * * *

    Note: *Rated dimension by exercise.

    240 NEIL ANDERSON AND VICKI GOLTSI

    International Journal of Selection and Assessmentr 2006 The Authors

    Journal compilationr Blackwell Publishing Ltd. 2006

  • 7/29/2019 Negative Psychological Effects of Selection Methods Construct Formulation and an Empirical Investigation Into an AC

    6/21

    nervous, jittery, afraid), while 10 adjectives describe

    positive moods (interested, excited, strong, enthusiastic,

    proud, alert, inspired, determined, attentive, active).

    Reliabilities (as) for positive affect (PA) and negative

    affect (NA) were a5 .89 and .85, respectively. The measure

    was sensitive to changes over time in the current

    study: testretest for PA was only .68 (Time 1 to Time 2)

    and .54 (Time 2 to Time 3), and for NA this was only .71

    (Time 1 to Time 2) and .45 (Time 2 to Time 3), indicating

    that there were changes in respondents self-rated affect

    over time.

    Feedback Perceptions. Feedback perceptions were

    measured with an eight-item scale derived from Dodd(1977) at Time 3 only. Cronbachs a was .88.

    Career Exploration Behavior. The Career ExplorationSurvey (CES) by Stumpf, Colarelli, and Hartman (1983)

    was used. Five of the CES scales were used: (1) environ-

    mental exploration (a5 .83, e.g., I went to various career

    orientation programmes), (2) self-exploration (a5 .88,

    e.g., I have been retrospective in thinking about my

    career), (3) intended/systematic exploration (a5 .74, e.g.,

    I tried specific work roles to see if I like them), (4)

    amount of information obtained (a5 .79, e.g., I currently

    have a moderate amount of information on how Ill fit into

    various career paths), and (5) Focus (a5

    .86, e.g., Howsure are you that you know the type of organization you

    want to work for?).

    Results

    Pre-Analysis Checks

    Initial comparisons of biographical variables between the

    accepted and rejected candidate groups were carried out.

    Independent sample t-tests at Time 1 revealed no sig-

    nificant differences on either gender (t51.34, p5NS), or

    marital status (t5 .36, p5NS), but a significant

    difference favoring younger applicants in age (t5 3.46,

    po.001). Comparisons on other biographical variables

    (number of assessment centers previously attended, educa-

    tion level, and degree type) revealed non-significant

    differences between the accepted and rejected applicants,

    indicating that these sub-groups were essentially equivalent

    and were not prone to underlying differences in composi-

    tion, which could have confounded differences found on

    study measures of mental health and career exploration

    behavior.

    Descriptive Statistics and Intercorrelations

    Table 2 reports descriptive statistics, intercorrelations, and

    reliability coefficients for all study variables at Time 1.

    Cronbachs as are all acceptable at above .80. Overviewing

    this correlation matrix, note that self-esteem correlates

    positively and significantly with psychological well-being

    (r5.20, po.05) and with positive affect (r5.48, po.01),

    but negatively and significantly with negative affect

    (r5 .22, po.05). The highest correlations were found

    between positive and negative affect (r5 .66, po.01),

    and interestingly between positive affect and careerexploration behavior (r5.54, po.01).

    Longitudinal Differences in Accepted Vs. RejectedCandidates

    Table 3 reports means, standard deviations, t-test values,

    and effect size statistics for the key outcome variables of

    mental health and career exploration behavior. Cohens d

    (Cohen, 1988) was used to examine sub-group differences

    on these variables between the rejected (i.e., majority) and

    Table 2. Descriptive statistics and intercorrelations for Time 1 study variables

    Mean SD

    Intercorrelations

    1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9

    1. Age 23.02 2.46

    2. Gender 1.50 .50 .00

    3. Marital status 1.05 .22 .40** .04

    4. Self-esteem 16.94 3.64 .04 .10

    .13 .885. Psychological well-being: GHQ-12 10.71 4.64 .10 .16 .10 .20* .84

    6. Positive affect 36.15 5.10 .09 .04 .14 .48** .32** .89

    7. Negative affect 17.12 4.00 .02 .11 .02 .22* .32** .66** .85

    8. Career exploration behavior 77.13 12.99 .13 .03 .10 .16 .02 .54** .02 .82

    9. AC outcome decision 1.74 .44 .23* .13 .03 .11 .18 .10 .09 .18

    Notes: Values on the diagonal are Cronbachs a coefficients.

    GHQ, General Health Questionnaire.*po.05,**po.01, two-tailed tests.

    NPES OF ASSESSMENT CENTERS 241

    r 2006 The AuthorsJournal compilationr Blackwell Publishing Ltd. 2006 Volume 14 Number 3 September 2006

  • 7/29/2019 Negative Psychological Effects of Selection Methods Construct Formulation and an Empirical Investigation Into an AC

    7/21

    Ta

    ble3

    .Su

    b-g

    roup

    differenc

    es

    inmen

    talhea

    lthover

    time:

    accep

    tedvs

    .re

    jec

    tedapp

    lican

    ts

    Time1

    Time2

    Time3

    Time1

    Time3

    Accepted

    Rejected

    Accepted

    Rejected

    A

    ccepted

    Rejected

    t

    SignificanceSEd

    SD

    ratio

    d

    t

    SignificanceSEd

    SD

    ratio

    d

    Mean

    SD

    Mean

    SD

    Mean

    SD

    Mean

    SD

    Mean

    SD

    mean

    SD

    Self-esteem

    16.2

    8

    3.2

    5

    17.18

    3.7

    6

    16.3

    2

    3.8

    7

    16.4

    1

    3.9

    1

    16

    .07

    3.7

    5

    16.7

    3

    3.9

    9

    1.1

    9

    NS

    .22

    .86

    .2

    5

    .7

    8

    NS

    .22

    .94

    .1

    7

    Psychological

    well-being

    12.0

    7

    5.1

    2

    10.19

    4.3

    7

    11.8

    1

    4.8

    7

    10.4

    2

    3.8

    8

    10

    .86

    6.7

    8

    10.4

    5

    5.6

    3

    1.7

    2

    NS

    .22

    1.1

    7

    1.4

    1

    .28

    NS

    .22

    1.2

    0

    .0

    7

    Positive

    affect

    37.1

    8

    3.8

    5

    35.88

    6.2

    0

    35.4

    3

    5.8

    5

    34.3

    6

    7.9

    1

    33

    .39

    7.2

    0

    32.3

    0

    7.9

    5

    1.2

    8

    NS

    .22

    .62

    1.2

    5

    .67

    NS

    .22

    .91

    .1

    4

    Negative

    affect

    16.8

    6

    4.0

    0

    17.77

    4.6

    0

    12.8

    2

    3.5

    4

    13.0

    0

    3.5

    9

    13

    .89

    5.1

    7

    14.8

    8

    6.0

    7

    .9

    9

    NS

    .22

    .87

    .2

    3

    .8

    3

    NS

    .22

    .85

    .1

    7

    Career

    exploration

    behavior

    81.0

    0

    11.4

    3

    75.73

    13.3

    0

    80.9

    0

    12.2

    6

    77.8

    4

    13.2

    1

    62

    .17

    12.8

    6

    68.2

    4

    15.6

    0

    2.0

    0

    N

    S1

    (p5

    .051)

    .22

    .86

    1.4

    1

    1.8

    7

    NS

    .22

    .82

    .4

    3

    Notes:d5

    differencebetween

    rejectedandacceptedmeansinstandarddeviationunits(effectssize).d

    valuescomputedbyexpressingthedifferencesbetweenthe

    meansoftherejectedandacceptedgroupsinpooledstandarddeviationunits.d5

    (meanfortherejecte

    dgroupmeanfortheacceptedgro

    up)/SDpooled.

    Positived

    valuesindicateacceptedapplicantsscorehigher,negativethatrejectedapplicantsscorehigher.SEd5

    standarderrorofd(unequalN

    valuesforgroups).

    SDratio

    of

    rejectedgrouptoacceptedgroupstandarddeviations,valuesgreaterthan1.0

    0indicatealargerrejectedgroupstandarddeviation.

    p5

    .051forthist-valueoft5

    2.0

    0,

    indicatingthattheTime1d

    ifferencebetweenacceptedandrejectedgroupswasmarginallynon-significant.

    242 NEIL ANDERSON AND VICKI GOLTSI

    International Journal of Selection and Assessmentr 2006 The Authors

    Journal compilationr Blackwell Publishing Ltd. 2006

  • 7/29/2019 Negative Psychological Effects of Selection Methods Construct Formulation and an Empirical Investigation Into an AC

    8/21

    accepted (i.e. minority) groups. According to Cohen, effect

    sizes can theoretically range between positive and negative

    infinity, although in personnel selection contexts it is rare

    to find such extreme values (e.g., Anderson, Lievens, van

    Dam, & Born, 2006; Ones & Anderson, 2002). As d in

    effect indexes the standardized mean difference between

    any two groups, it is particularly valuable when examining

    differnces between sub-groups of varying sample sizes. Past

    research into ethnic minority and gender differences inselection has made extensive use of such effect size

    computations; here, we use Cohens d to compare

    immediate and longer-term differences in psychological

    well-being and career exploration behavior. Note that

    according to Cohen (1988), effect sizesof zero to .20 can be

    considered small, those around .50 are medium, and above

    .80 are large in magnitude.

    First, we consider differences in key variables at Time 1.

    Two effect sizes approaching .50 were found. Accepted

    candidates reported a higher base-line mean on psycholo-

    gical well-being than that of subsequently rejected

    candidates (means were 12.07 and 10.19, respectively,d5 .41). Subsequently accepted candidates also self-rated

    more highly on career exploration behavior than

    rejected applicants, suggesting that they were already more

    actively involved in their job search (the means were 81.00

    and 75.73, respectively, d5 .41). No other effect size

    differences or t-test differences are evident in Table 3

    between these two groups on base-line measures taken at

    Time 1.

    Second, differences between accepted and rejected

    candidates are examined at Time 3, that is, 6 months

    following this AC, and several interesting findings are

    evident. Most importantly, no statistically significant

    differences (according to t-test values) were found betweenaccepted and rejected candidates on self-esteem, psycholo-

    gical well-being, positive or negative effect, or on the CES.

    However, an effect size difference of d5 .43 was found

    between accepted and rejected candidates on career

    exploration behavior. Not surprisingly, this difference

    was substantially higher for rejected applicants, perhaps

    reflecting on-going job search behaviors for those still

    active in applying for job vacancies elsewhere. A finding of

    note here is that rejected applicants actually report a higher

    mean (although also marginally higher SD value) on self-

    esteem at Time 3 than accepted candidates (16.73 and

    16.07, respectively), although this difference is non-significant (t5 .78, p5NS; d5 .17).

    Third, receipt of a positive outcome decision did not

    appear to improve values on self-esteem, positive affect, or

    psychological well-being for accepted candidates. Time 1

    to Time 3 mean ratings on self-esteem were 16.28

    (SD53.25) and 16.07 (SD53.99), respectively; on

    positive, affect values were 37.18 (SD53.85) and 33.39

    (SD57.20); and on psychological well-being, 12.07

    (SD55.12) and 10.86 (SD56.78), respectively. None of

    these differences were significant, suggesting that there was

    no longer-term PPEs from receipt of a successful select

    decision at this AC.

    Feedback Perception Differences

    A final set of analyses were computed in order to examine

    differences in feedback perceptions between the accepted

    and rejected candidate groups. These are summarized in

    Table 4. Large effect size differences in feedback percep-tions between the two groups are evident. For the eight-

    item scale combined, d5 .86 (t54.26,po.001) on overall

    feedback perceptions. Yet, the largest effect size difference

    was found on the item Feedback information was

    accurate: d51.08 (t56.52, po.001), strongly suggest-

    ing that rejected candidates attributed unfavorable out-

    come decisions to an inaccurate selection methodology.

    Two other large effect size differences were found in

    accordance with Cohens (1988) recommendations:

    d5 .82 on Reference to good performance was made,

    and d5 .93 on Feedback had an encouraging effect.

    Note that feedback perceptions were also measured atTime 3 along with all other mental health scales and the

    CES; for the former, we found several large effect size

    differences between accepted and rejected candidates

    (Table 4), but for the latter we found no sizeable or

    statistically significant differences (Table 3).

    Discussion

    We believe that this paper makes two novel contributions

    to the existing scant research into, and thus our under-

    standing of, the psychological impact of assessment centers

    upon applicants. First, we develop and expound anoperational definition of NPEs for all selection methods,

    noting several facets of this definition and linking it to

    recent research that has adopted the applicants perspective

    to selection predictors. Second, although a relatively small-

    scale primary study, we present the findings of a three-

    timepoint longitudinal study into the NPEs and potential

    PPEs of an operational assessment center in the United

    Kingdom. Contrary to the findings of the Fletcher (1991)

    study, which used identical measurement intervals to the

    present investigation, we failed to find NPEs resulting from

    this AC, most notably even though the effect size

    differences between accepted and rejected candidates werelarge in reactions-level measures of applicant feedback

    perceptions following this assessment center.

    Non-Significant NPE Findings

    Despite the fact that 6 months after the AC, and indeed in

    most cases some months after receiving feedback on their

    performance, rejected candidates reacted far more nega-

    tively to the feedback reactions measure than did accepted

    candidates (see Table 4), the overall feedback perception

    NPES OF ASSESSMENT CENTERS 243

    r 2006 The AuthorsJournal compilationr Blackwell Publishing Ltd. 2006 Volume 14 Number 3 September 2006

  • 7/29/2019 Negative Psychological Effects of Selection Methods Construct Formulation and an Empirical Investigation Into an AC

    9/21

    effect size difference across all eight items combined was

    large (d5

    .86). It appears that rejected applicants attrib-uted this negative decision to a lack of accuracy in the

    assessment process (d51.08, t56.52, po.001) in spite of

    this being a 2-day, job-relevant assessment center. Detailed

    feedback was made available to all candidates, regardless

    of the outcome of the AC final decision. Again, it seems

    that the wisdom of providing all applicants, especially

    those who have been unsuccessful, with highly detailed

    feedback is more questionable than was previously thought

    by selection psychologists (Schinkel et al., 2004).

    Undoubtedly, the key findings of this study are that in

    contrast to these highly significant findings of feedback

    effect differences, rejected applicants suffered no serious

    declines in self-esteem, psychological well-being, positiveaffect, or indeed expounded career exploration behavior as

    a result of being unsuccessful at this assessment center. This

    is noteworthy in that this AC was for a highly prestigeous

    organization that is perceived by applicants as being a

    highly attractive employer. One explanation here is that the

    selection ratios involved in being successful for this

    organization were so competitive for applicants that their

    perceptions of being unsuccessful could have been moder-

    ated by this selection ratio. Various perceptual, psycholo-

    gical, and attributional biases may therefore have allowed

    unsuccessful candidates in this particular situation to

    persuade themselves that in effect so many otherapplicants were also unsuccessful that I knew all along

    that this was a long-shot. Counter to this explanation is

    the fact that this was the final stage of the selection process

    for the organization, that is, that applicants had success-

    fully survived several stages of shortlisting and assessment

    previously and this AC represented the final barrier to entry

    to employment. Despite the job relevance of exercises at

    this AC, trained assessors, and detailed feedback including

    behavioral exemplars being given to rejected candidates,

    they still construed to attribute this to inherent inaccuracies

    and unjust elements in the method itself (see also Fletcher

    & Kerslake, 1992).That unsuccessful applicants suffered no longer-term (or

    at least after a 6-month period) NPEs or deleterious

    impacts upon the career exploration behavior suggests

    robust psychological barriers and perceptual screening

    heuristics between their immediate-level reactions and

    longer-term psychological impacts. This is an important

    finding, although of course this is only a single study of a

    single selection predictor method. It could also have been

    the case that many of these rejected applicants had found

    employment elsewhere during this period; unfortunately,

    our follow-up measure did not include items to explore the

    present employment status of those individuals. Further

    research is called for to examine this issue. Yet, as arguedearlier in this paper, the AC is precisely the method that

    might be expected to cause NPEs in rejected candidates. It

    is noteworthy therefore that this study failed to find any

    evidence of longer-term negative effects, and this strongly

    suggests that future research in this area needs to

    distinguish clearly between differences that are reactions-

    level preference responses and longitudinal effects upon

    applicant decision-making, psychological well-being, and

    even career exploration behavior (Noe & Steffy, 1987).

    Non-Significant PPE Findings

    One further finding of relevance here is that we likewise

    found no strong PPEs from being successful at this AC.

    Note that self-esteem, psychological well-being, and

    positive affect did not improve markedly either at Time 2

    measures, that is, before the actual decision was known but

    immediately following the AC procedure, or 6 months later

    at Time 3. Rather, we found some evidence of declines in

    psychological well-being for successful applicants 6

    months after the AC and in most cases some months into

    their job tenure. During the intervening period, most of

    Table 4. Differences in feedback perceptions: accepted vs. rejected sub-groups

    Feedback perception

    Accepted Rejected

    t-value SEd SDratio dMean (SD) Mean (SD)

    Feedback provided 2.89 (.79) 2.28 (1.04) 3.18** .22 .76 .60

    Reference to good performance 2.96 (.94) 2.11 (.99) 3.99*** .23 .95 .82

    Reference to poor performance 2.57 (1.03) 2.24 (1.04) 1.43* .22 .99 .31

    Feedback had an encouraging effect 3.03 (1.26) 1.88 (1.09) 4.25*** .23 1.16 .93Understanding of feedback information 3.29 (.90) 2.68 (1.15) 2.81** .22 .78 .54

    Feedback information was accurate 3.59 (.75) 2.34 (1.10) 6.52*** .23 .68 1.08

    Valuable information for personal development 3.21 (1.17) 2.16 (1.37) 3.87*** .23 .85 .76

    Provision of developmental recommendations 2.07 (1.33) 2.07 (1.13) .01* .22 1.18 .00

    Overall feedback perceptions scale 23.69 (5.81) 17.79 (6.58) 4.62*** .23 .88 .86

    Notes: SEd5 standard error of d.*po.05,**po.01,***po.001.

    244 NEIL ANDERSON AND VICKI GOLTSI

    International Journal of Selection and Assessmentr 2006 The Authors

    Journal compilationr Blackwell Publishing Ltd. 2006

  • 7/29/2019 Negative Psychological Effects of Selection Methods Construct Formulation and an Empirical Investigation Into an AC

    10/21

    these applicants had already joined the organization and it

    could have been that widely documented socialization

    effects had eliminated any short-term increases in well-

    being and core self-esteem (e.g., Saks, 1995; Saks &

    Ashforth, 1997). Again, caution is warranted over the

    interpretation of these findings as the sample size in the

    present study is relatively small, so these findings should be

    tempered with some caution. Future research is certainly

    called for to examine both NPE and PPE effects inassessment centers and other selection methods; here, it is

    appropriate to highlight that different time periods of

    measurement also need to be included in future studies in

    order to test the possibility that effects may be present but

    might only remain manifest for some weeks rather than

    months after receipt of the outcome decision.

    Brief Comments over Future Directions for NPE andPPE Research

    The present investigation contributes to the rather limitednumber of studies into the possible psychological effects,

    either negative or positive, of selection methods upon

    applicants. As noted earlier in this paper, it is surprising

    that this area has attracted so little attention by psychol-

    ogists active in employee selection research. As a con-

    sequence, several major avenues for future research,

    theoretical model-building, and empirical study are im-

    mediately apparent. In the opening section of this paper,

    two working definitions of NPEs and PPEs were postu-

    lated; it is likely that other researchers will have rather

    different opinions of these constructs and so may wish to

    put forward somewhat differing formulations of these in

    due course. Additional research would, it would at leastseem, most valuable to commence the mammoth task of

    quantifying the direct effects and costs of NPEs upon

    reduced job performance; that is, to investigate the utility-

    related costs of NPEs to organizations who may well be

    unaware of this possibility. Investigation of how such costs

    can be moderated by the provision of accurate but tactful

    feedback to unsuccessful internal candidates would be a

    follow-up line for initial research. In addition to both of

    these performance- and utility-related directions for future

    studies, however, we would suggest that research into both

    NPEs and PPEs as a psychologically related strategy is fully

    warranted without the need to justify this theme of researchon what are essentially economic grounds. It is beholden

    upon selection psychologists to investigate and quantify the

    psychological impacts of the assessment methods used in

    employee recruitment and selection settings, especially

    where these may result in NPEs and outcomes. Further

    research is clearly called for in this regard not just into the

    NPEs of assessment centers but on other popular predictors

    such as structured interviews, tests of general mental ability

    and personality, job sample tests, and situational judgment

    inventories, among others.

    Conclusion

    In comparison even with the growing body of research into

    immediate-level applicant reactions, the potential for

    predictor methods to provoke NPEs upon applicants has

    remained a critically underresearched topic. Curiously, this

    dark side of the moon, as Anderson (2004) refers to it,

    has received notably little research attention by selection

    psychologists despite health psychology and research intonegative psychological impacts of workload being vibrant

    areas of study within IWO psychology more generally. This

    paper represents an initial investigation into a sub-field that

    first requires definitional clarity and conceptual distinction

    from existing research efforts into immediate-level reac-

    tions outcomes. To conclude, this study, and its principal

    finding of a lack of longer-term NPEs upon applicants

    caused by a final-stage assessment center, can only be cited

    as one of the initial steps toward establishing this important

    field of enquiry for further empirical investigation.

    Acknowledgement

    We wish to express our thanks to Sonja Schinkel for her

    comments and suggestions on an earlier version of this

    paper.

    References

    American Psychiatric Association (1994) Diagnostic and statisticalmanual of mental disorders, 4th Edn (DSM-IV). Washington,DC: APA.

    Anderson, N. (2004) Editorial the dark side of the moon:

    Applicant perspectives, negative psychological effects (NPEs),and candidate decision making in selection. International

    Journal of Selection and Assessment, 12, 18.Anderson,N. (2005) Relationships between practice and research in

    personnel selection: Does the left hand know what the right isdoing? In A. Evers, N. Anderson and O. Smit-Voskuyl (Eds),Handbook of selection (pp. 124). Oxford: Blackwell.

    Anderson, N., Born, M. and Cunningham-Snell, N. (2001)Recruitment and selection: Applicant perspectives and out-comes. In N. Anderson, D.S. Ones, H. Sinangil and C.Viswesvaran (Eds), Handbook of industrial, work and organiza-tional psychology, Vol. 1: Personnel psychology (pp. 200218).London: Sage Publications Ltd.

    Anderson, N., Lievens, F., van Dam, K. and Born, M. (2006) Aconstruct-driven investigation of gender differences in a leader-

    ship-role assessment center. Journal of Applied Psychology, 9,219240.

    Bauer, T.N., Maertz, C.P., Dolen, M.R. and Campion, M.A. (1998)Longitudinal assessment of applicant reactions to employmenttesting and test outcome feedback. Journal of Applied Psychol-ogy, 83, 892903.

    Bauer, T.N., Truxillo, D.M, Paronto, M.E., Campion, M.A. andWeekley, J.A. (2004) Applicant reactions to different selectiontechnology: Face-to-face, interactive voice response, and com-puter-assisted telephone screening interviews. International

    Journal of Selection and Assessment, 12(2), 135148.Chan, D. and Schmitt, N. (2004) An agenda for future research on

    applicant reactions to selection procedures: A construct-oriented

    NPES OF ASSESSMENT CENTERS 245

    r 2006 The AuthorsJournal compilationr Blackwell Publishing Ltd. 2006 Volume 14 Number 3 September 2006

  • 7/29/2019 Negative Psychological Effects of Selection Methods Construct Formulation and an Empirical Investigation Into an AC

    11/21

    approach. International Journal of Selection and Assessment,12, 923.

    Cohen, J. (1988) Statistical power analysis for the behavioralsciences, 2nd Edn. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Derous, E., Born, M. and De Witte, K. (2004) How applicants wantand expect to be treated: Applicants selection treatment beliefs

    and the development of the social process questionnaire onselection. International Journal of Selection and Assessment, 12,99119.

    Dodd, W.E. (1977) Attitudes toward assessment centre programs.

    In J.L. Moses and W.C. Byham (Eds), Applying the assess-ment centre method (pp. 132146). Elmsford, NY: PergamonPress.

    Fletcher, C. (1991) Candidates reactions to assessment centres andtheir outcomes: A longitudinal study. Journal of OccupationalPsychology, 64, 117127.

    Fletcher, C. and Kerslake, C. (1992) The impact of assessment

    centres and their outcomes on participants self-assessments.Human Relations, 45, 281289.

    Gilliland, S.W. (1994) Effects of procedural and distributive justiceon reactions to a selection system. Journal of AppliedPsychology, 79, 117127.

    Gilliland, S.W. and Steiner, D.D. (2001) Causes and consequencesof applicant perceptions of unfairness. In R. Cropanzano

    (Ed.), Justice in the workplace (pp. 175195). Hillsdale, NJ:Erlbaum.

    Goldberg, D. (1992) General health questionnaire (GHQ-12).Windsor, UK: NFER NELSON.

    Hart, P.M. and Cooper, C.L. (2002) Occupational stress: Toward amore integrated framework. In N. Anderson, D.S. Ones, H.

    Sinangil and C. Viswesvaran (Eds), Handbook of industrial andorganizational psychology, Vol. 2 (pp. 93114). Thousand Oaks,CA: Sage Publications.

    Hausknecht, J.P., Day, D.V. and Thomas, S.C. (2004) Applicant

    reactions to selection procedures: An updated model and meta-analysis. Personnel Psychology, 57, 639683.

    Horvath, M., Ryan, A.M. and Stierwalt, S.L. (2000) The influenceof explanations for selection test use, outcome favorability, and

    self-efficacy on test-taker perceptions. Organizational Behavior

    and Human Decision Processes, 83, 310330.Iles, P. and Robertson, I. (1997) The impact of personnel selection

    procedures on candidates. In N. Anderson and P. Herriot(Eds),International handbook of selection and assessment (pp. 543566). Chichester, UK: Wiley.

    Imus, A.L. and Ryan, A.M. (2005) Relevance and rigor in research

    on the applicants perspective: In persuit of pragmatic science. InA. Evers, N. Anderson and O. Voskuijl (Eds), Handbook ofSelection (pp. 280296). Oxford, UK: Blackwell.

    Judge, T.A. and Bono, J.E. (2001) Relationship of core self-

    evaluation traits self-esteem, generalized self-efficacy, locusof control, andemotional stability with jobsatisfaction andjobperformance: A meta-analysis. Journal of Applied Psychology,86, 8092.

    Lievens, F. and Klimoski, R.J. (2001) Understanding the assessmentcenter process. Where are we now? In C.L. Cooper and I.T.Robertson (Eds), International review of industrial and organi-sational psychology, Vol. 16 (pp.245286). Chicester, UK: JohnWiley & Sons.

    Lievens, F., De Corte, W. and Brysse, K. (2003) Applicantperceptions of selection procedures: The role of selection

    information, belief in tests, and comparative anxiety. Interna-tional Journal of Selection and Assessment, 11, 6777.

    Lounsbury, J.W., Bobrow, W. and Jensen, J.B. (1989) Attitudestoward employment testing: Scale development, correlates and

    known-group validation. Professional Psychology: Researchand Practice, 20, 340349.

    Macan, T.H., Avedon, M.J., Paese, M. and Smith, D.E. (1994) The

    effects of applicants reactions to cognitive ability tests and an

    assessment centre. Personnel Psychology, 47, 715738.Noe, R.A. and Steffy, B.D. (1987) The influence of individual

    characteristics and assessment centre evaluation on career

    exploration behavior and job involvement. Journal of Voca-

    tional Behavior, 30, 187202.Ones, D.S. and Anderson, N. (2002) Gender and ethnic group

    differences on personality scales in selection: Some British data.

    Journal of Occupational and Organizational Psychology, 75,255276.

    Ployhart, R.E. and Harold, C.M. (2004) The applicant attribution-

    reaction theory (AART): An integrative theory of applicant

    attributional processing. International Journal of Selection and

    Assessment, 12, 8498.Ployhart, R.E. and Ryan, A.M. (1997) Toward an explanation of

    applicant reactions: An examination of organizational justice

    and attribution frameworks. Organizational Behavior and

    Human Decision Processes, 72, 308335.Ployhart, R.E. and Ryan, A.M. (1998) Applicants reactions to the

    fairness of selection procedures: The effects of positive rule

    violations and time of measurement. Journal of Applied

    Psychology, 83, 316.Ployhart, R.E., Ryan, A.M. and Bennett, M. (1999) Explanations

    for selection decisions: Applicants reactions to informational

    and sensitivity features of explanations. Journal of Applied

    Psychology, 84, 87106.Robertson, I.T., Iles, P.A., Gratton, L. and Sharpley, D. (1991) The

    impact of personnel selection and assessment methods on

    candidates. Human Relations, 44, 963982.Rosenberg, M. (1989) The self-esteem scale. Windsor, UK: NFER

    NELSON.Ryan, A.M. and Ployhart, R.E. (2000) Applicants perceptions of

    selection procedures and decisions: A critical review and agenda

    for the future. Journal of Management, 26, 565606.Ryan, A.M., Greguraas, G.J. and Ployhart, R.E. (1996) Perceived

    job relatedness of physical ability testing for firefighters:

    Exploring variations in reactions. Human Performance, 9,

    219240.Saks, A.M. (1995) Longitudinal field investigations of the moderat-

    ing and mediating effects of self-efficacy on the relationship

    between training and newcomer adjustment. Journal of AppliedPsychology, 80, 211225.

    Saks, A.M. and Ashforth, B.E. (1997) Organization socialization:

    Makingsense of thepast andpresent as a prologue forthe future.

    Journal of Vocational Behavior, 51, 234279.Salgado, J.F. (1999) Personnel selection methods. In C.L. Cooper

    and I.T. Robertson (Eds), International review of industrial andorganisational psychology (pp. 154). Chichester, UK: JohnWiley & Sons.

    Salgado, J.F., Viswesvaran, C. and Ones,D.S. (2002) Predictors used

    for personnel selection: An overview of constructs, methods

    and techniques. In N. Anderson, D.S. Ones, H. Sinangil and

    C. Viswesvaran (Eds), Handbook of industrial, work and

    organizational psychology, Vol. 1. London: Sage.Schinkel, S., van Dierendonck, D. and Anderson, N. (2004)

    The impact of selection encounters on applicants: An experi-

    mental study into feedback effects after a negative selection

    decision. International Journal of Selection and Assessment, 12,197205.

    Schmit, M.J. and Ryan, A.M. (1992) Test-taking dispositions:

    A missing link? Journal of Applied Psychology, 77, 629637.Schmit, M.J. and Ryan, A.M. (1997) Applicant withdrawal: The

    role of test-taking attitudes and racial differences. Personnel

    Psychology, 50, 855876.

    246 NEIL ANDERSON AND VICKI GOLTSI

    International Journal of Selection and Assessmentr 2006 The Authors

    Journal compilationr Blackwell Publishing Ltd. 2006

  • 7/29/2019 Negative Psychological Effects of Selection Methods Construct Formulation and an Empirical Investigation Into an AC

    12/21

    Schmitt, N. and Chan, D. (1999)The statusof research on applicant

    reactions to selection tests and its implications for managers.

    International Journal of Management Research, 4561.Schmitt, N., Oswald, F.L., Kim, B.H., Gillespie, M.A. and Ramsay,

    L.J. (2004) The impact of justice and self-serving bias explana-

    tion of the perceived fairness of different types of selection tests.

    International Journal of Selection and Assessment, 12, 160171.Schuler, H. and Fruhner, R. (1993) Effects of assessment center

    participation on self-esteem and on evaluation of the selection

    situation. In H. Schuler, C.J.L. Farr and M. Smith (Eds),Personnel selection and assessment: Individual and organiza-

    tional perspectives (pp. 109124). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence

    Erlbaum.

    Smither, J.W., Reilly, R.R., Millsap, R.E., Pearlman, K. and Stoffey,R.W. (1993) Applicant reactions to selection procedures.Personnel Psychology, 46, 4976.

    Stumpf, S.A., Colarelli, S.M. and Hartman, K. (1983) Developmentof the career exploration survey (CES). Journal of VocationalBehaviour, 22, 191226.

    van Vianen, A., Taris, R., Scholten, E. and Schinkel, S. (2004)Perceived fairness in personnel selection: Determinants andoutcomes in different stages of the assessment procedure.

    International Journal of Selection and Assessment, 12, 149159.Watson, D., Clark, L.A. and Tellegen, A. (1988) Development andvalidation of brief measures of positive and negative affect: ThePANAS scales. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 5,10631070.

    NPES OF ASSESSMENT CENTERS 247

    r 2006 The AuthorsJournal compilationr Blackwell Publishing Ltd. 2006 Volume 14 Number 3 September 2006

  • 7/29/2019 Negative Psychological Effects of Selection Methods Construct Formulation and an Empirical Investigation Into an AC

    13/21

    Ta

    bleA1

    .Chrono

    log

    ica

    lsum

    maryo

    fresearc

    hintopsyc

    ho

    log

    ica

    le

    ffec

    tso

    fse

    lec

    tionme

    tho

    dsupon

    app

    lican

    ts

    Author(s)

    Construc

    ts

    measure

    d

    Sample

    Predicto

    r

    method

    Measures

    Measurem

    ent

    timing

    Independent

    variable(s)

    Depende

    nt

    variable(

    s)

    Key

    findings

    Lounsburyetal.

    (1989)

    Attitudestoward

    employmentte

    sting

    n5

    546for

    telephoneinterview

    n5

    112applicants

    formanualwork

    position

    Testingingene

    ral

    Opinionsof

    employmenttesting

    Study1:

    Study2:post-testor

    post-feedbacktest

    Sex,age,race,

    occupation,

    education

    attainment,

    experiencewith

    testing

    Attitudestotes

    t

    takingperceive

    d

    fairnessoftests

    Instudy2Hispanics

    andyounger

    applicantshadmore

    positiveattitudes

    Significantlymore

    favorableattitudes

    wereobserved

    towardemployment

    testingwhenjob-

    relationhadbeen

    explainedand

    feedbackbeengiven

    thanifithadnot

    Rejectedapplicants

    hadmorenegative

    attitudesthan

    selectedorhold-

    fileapplicants

    Robertsonetal.

    (1991)

    Beliefsaboutthe

    adequacyofse

    lection

    procedures,

    perceivedcareer

    impact

    n5

    170

    Employeesofa

    financialinstitution

    Biodata,situational

    interview,

    assessmentcenter

    Withdrawal

    cognitions

    Organizational

    commitment

    Psychologicalhealth

    Self-esteem

    Atspecificpoin

    ts

    duringthe

    assessmentprocess

    Select/reject

    Withdrawal

    cognitions

    Organizational

    commitment

    Psychological

    health

    Perceivedcareer

    impactwas

    significantlygreater

    forrejectedthanfor

    acceptedcandidates

    Acceptedand

    rejectedcandidates

    didnotdifferon

    psychologicalhealth

    orself-esteem

    Candidatesinthe

    assessmentcenter

    phaseweremore

    concernedwith

    adequacythan

    candidatesinthe

    situationalinterview

    phase

    Acceptedcandidates

    exhibitedhigher

    organizational

    commitmentand

    believedmethods

    AppendixA

    248 NEIL ANDERSON AND VICKI GOLTSI

    International Journal of Selection and Assessmentr 2006 The Authors

    Journal compilationr Blackwell Publishing Ltd. 2006

  • 7/29/2019 Negative Psychological Effects of Selection Methods Construct Formulation and an Empirical Investigation Into an AC

    14/21

    Ta

    bleA1

    .Con

    tinue

    d

    Author(s)

    Construc

    ts

    measure

    d

    Sample

    Predicto

    r

    method

    Measures

    Measurem

    ent

    timing

    Independent

    variable(s)

    Depende

    nt

    variable(

    s)

    Key

    findings

    weremoreadequate

    thanrejected

    candidates

    Fletcher(1991)

    Psychological

    well-being

    Needforachievement

    Organizational

    commitment

    Jobinvolvement

    n5

    57

    Highergradeclerical

    staff

    Assessmentce

    nter

    (groupdiscussion,

    in-tray,objective

    setting,psycho

    metric

    tests,self-repo

    rt,

    interview)

    Self-esteematwork

    anddepressedmood

    atwork

    Pre-test

    Post-test(straight

    afterassessme

    nt)

    Post-test(6months

    later)

    Select/reject,

    assessmentcenter

    participation

    Psychological

    well-being

    Successful

    candidateshad

    significantlylower

    scoresthan

    unsuccessful

    candidateson

    depressedmoodat

    work

    Self-esteemincreases

    fromT1toT2and

    dropsatT3toalevel

    lowerthanatT1

    Unsuccessful

    candidatesshoweda

    significantlylower

    levelofself-esteemat

    T3

    Smitheretal.(1993)Perceivedpred

    ictive

    validityandface

    validity,job

    relatedness,

    proceduraland

    distributivejus

    tice

    n15

    154entry-level

    andrecruiting/

    employment

    managers

    n25

    1895

    applicantsforawide

    rangeofcivilservice

    jobs

    Descriptionsofeight

    cognitivetestsandsix

    otherprocedures

    (personality

    inventory,

    in-basket,

    leaderlessgrou

    p

    discussion,

    bio

    data,

    unstructured

    interview,and

    structuredinterview)

    Jobrelatedness

    (predictivevalidity

    andfacevalidity)

    organizational

    attractiveness,

    likelihoodof

    improvement,

    perceivedknowledge

    ofresults,affect,

    cognitiveability

    Study1:post-test

    Study2:post-test

    andpost-feedb

    ack

    testafter1mo

    nth

    Study1:perceived

    predictivevalidity,

    facevalidityGPA,

    andcognitivetest

    performance

    Study2:

    organizational

    attractiveness,

    likelihoodof

    improvement,

    perceivedknowledge

    ofresults,affect

    Perceptionsofjob

    relatedness,

    proceduraljust

    ice,

    distributivejustice

    andrecommen

    dation

    intentions

    Perceptionsof

    validitywerenot

    enhancedthrough

    facevalidity

    manipulations

    Perceivedvaliditydid

    notinescapably

    correspondwiththe

    actualvalidityofthe

    selectionprocedures

    Applicantsreactions

    werepositively

    relatedto

    organizational

    attractiveness,

    justice

    perceptionsand

    willingnessto

    recommendthe

    employertoothers

    Higher-ability

    applicantswerenot

    morelikelyto

    perceiveselection

    NPES OF ASSESSMENT CENTERS 249

    r 2006 The AuthorsJournal compilationr Blackwell Publishing Ltd. 2006 Volume 14 Number 3 September 2006

  • 7/29/2019 Negative Psychological Effects of Selection Methods Construct Formulation and an Empirical Investigation Into an AC

    15/21

    Ta

    bleA1

    .Con

    tinue

    d

    Author(s)

    Construc

    ts

    measure

    d

    Sample

    Predicto

    r

    method

    Measures

    Measurem

    ent

    timing

    Independent

    variable(s)

    Depende

    nt

    variable(

    s)

    Key

    findings

    proceduresaslessjob

    related

    Proceduraljusticeas

    wellas

    recommendation

    intentionswere

    positivelyrelatedto

    affectandlikelihood

    ofimprovementand

    perceivedpredictive

    validity

    Gilliland(1994)

    Proceduralfairness,

    distributivefairness

    n5

    270

    undergraduate

    studentsparticipating

    inexchangefor

    financialrewards

    (whenselected)and/

    orcredits(when

    rejected)

    Worksampletest,

    cognitiveabilitytest,

    overtintegritytest

    Fairnessperceptions

    Recommendation

    intentions:

    Self-efficacy

    Performance

    Post-feedback

    test

    Hiringexpectation

    Perceivedfairness,

    recommendation

    intentions,self-

    efficacy,performance

    Selectedindividuals

    perceivedgreater

    fairnessinthe

    selectionprocessand

    outcomedecision

    thanrejected

    individuals,andthis

    effectwasmore

    pronouncedwhen

    individualshadhigh

    expectationsofbeing

    hired

    Rejectedapplicants

    weremorelikelyto

    recommendwhen

    givenexplanationsfor

    rejectdecision

    Fairprocedureslead

    tohigherself-efficacy

    forselected

    candidatesandlower

    forrejected

    Ryan,

    Greguras,and

    Ployhart(1996)

    Self-efficacy

    Job-relatedness,

    experience,

    consistencyof

    test

    administration

    N5

    81Fire

    Firefightersattending

    aweekofsessionsat

    astatefireschool

    3.7

    %female

    81.4

    8%male

    Sevenphysicalability

    tests(PAT)

    NA

    Self-efficacy,

    experience,current

    fitnesslevel,

    departmentfitness

    climate

    Perceived

    job-relatedness

    Self-efficacyfora

    specificPATwas

    positivelyrelatedto

    theperceivedjob-

    relatednessofthat

    PAT

    Individualswith

    higherself-efficacy

    sawPATs

    250 NEIL ANDERSON AND VICKI GOLTSI

    International Journal of Selection and Assessmentr 2006 The Authors

    Journal compilationr Blackwell Publishing Ltd. 2006

  • 7/29/2019 Negative Psychological Effects of Selection Methods Construct Formulation and an Empirical Investigation Into an AC

    16/21

    Ta

    bleA1

    .Con

    tinue

    d

    Author(s)

    Construc

    ts

    measure

    d

    Sample

    Predicto

    r

    method

    Measures

    Measurem

    ent

    timing

    Independent

    variable(s)

    Depende

    nt

    variable(

    s)

    Key

    findings

    PloyhartandRyan

    (1997)

    Processandou

    tcome

    fairness

    n5

    80297

    applicantsto

    graduateschoolin

    psychology

    GRE-scores,GPA,

    researchandw

    ork

    experiences,re

    search

    interests,lettersof

    recommendation,

    personalstatem

    ent

    Organizationaljustice

    Processfairness

    Outcomefairness

    Selectiondecision

    Perceivedcausefor

    decision

    Self-efficacy

    Self-esteem,

    Self-assessed

    performance

    Recommendation

    intention

    Applicationintention

    Acceptanceintention

    Reapplication

    intention

    Expectancyoffuture

    success

    Pre-test(before

    application),post-

    test

    (afteroffer)

    Admissionstatus,

    attributionsfor

    decision

    Recommendation

    Application

    Acceptanceand

    reapplication

    intentions

    Self-efficacy

    Self-esteem

    Self-assessed

    performance

    Morefairlyperceived

    proceduresleadto

    morefavorable

    intentionsandself-

    assessed

    performance.

    Effectincreasedas

    outcomefairness

    increased

    Applicantswho

    perceivedprocedures

    tobeunfairandwere

    selectedreported

    lowerself-efficacy

    Self-servingbiasonly

    occurredwhen

    individualsperceived

    fairprocedures

    SchmitandRyan

    (1997)

    Test-takingattitudes,

    withdrawal

    cognitions

    n5

    3290Applicants

    forthepostofpolice

    officer,ofwhom

    n5

    618were

    interviewedafter

    withdrawal

    Surveysand

    Interviews

    Test-takingattitudes

    Pre-test

    Anxiety

    Motivation

    Race

    Withdrawal

    Higherlevelsof

    anxietyand

    motivationin

    applicantsleadto

    lowerlikelihoodof

    withdrawal

    Caucasians

    possessedslightly

    highertest-taking

    motivation,wereless

    anxious,andbelieved

    moreintheefficacyof

    thetestscomparedto

    African-Americans

    Test-takingattitudes

    onlypossessasmall

    relationtowithdrawal

    PloyhartandRyan

    (1998)

    Processandou

    tcome

    fairness

    n5

    283

    undergraduate

    studentsparticipating

    inexchangefor

    credits

    CognitiveabilitytestFairness,

    Intentions,Job

    acceptance,

    Future

    experiment,

    Reapplication,

    Recommendation

    Pre-testpost-te

    st

    post-feedback

    questionnaire

    Select/reject,

    Administrative

    consistency

    Performance

    expectations,job

    acceptance

    intentions,futu

    re

    experiment

    intentions,

    Positive

    inconsistency

    generallydoesnot

    resultindifferent

    perceptionsof

    fairnesscompared

    NPES OF ASSESSMENT CENTERS 251

    r 2006 The AuthorsJournal compilationr Blackwell Publishing Ltd. 2006 Volume 14 Number 3 September 2006

  • 7/29/2019 Negative Psychological Effects of Selection Methods Construct Formulation and an Empirical Investigation Into an AC

    17/21

    Ta

    bleA1

    .Con

    tinue

    d

    Author(s)

    Construc

    ts

    measure

    d

    Sample

    Predicto

    r

    method

    Measures

    Measurem

    ent

    timing

    Independent

    variable(s)

    Depende

    nt

    variable(

    s)

    Key

    findings

    Self-perceptions,

    Withdrawal

    reapplication

    intentions,

    recommendation

    intentions,

    withdrawal

    withconsistency

    Post-hirejob

    acceptancewas

    unaffectedbyprocess

    fairness

    Unfairlyperceived

    proceduresleadto

    negativeintentions.

    Themostunfavorable

    reactionsoccurred

    withunfairly

    perceivedprocedures

    andafairoutcome

    Pre-hiringintentions

    andoutcomeare

    centraltotheforming

    ofreactionsbecause

    oftheirinfluenceon

    post-hireintentions

    andfairness

    PloyhartandRyan

    (1998)

    Processfairnes

    s,

    explanationadequacy

    n15

    156

    undergraduate

    studentsparticipated

    inexchangefor

    credits

    n25

    35entry-level

    graduatestudents

    justinformedonthe

    decision

    Study1:Scenarioof

    cognitiveabilitytest,

    andjobknowledge

    teststaken

    Study2:GRE,

    GPA,

    researchintere

    st,

    personalstatem

    ent,

    researchexperience,

    workexperienc

    e,

    letterof

    recommendation

    Processfairness

    Reportedself-

    perceptions,

    Organizational

    perceptionsletter

    Informationaland

    sensitivity

    explanation,select/

    reject

    Perceptionsof

    processfairnesswere

    enhancedby

    providingpersonalor

    procedural

    information

    Reportsofself-

    perceptionsfor

    rejectedparticipants

    wereharmedby

    providingpersonal

    informationand

    enhancedby

    providingadiversity

    justification.

    Vice

    versaforaccepted

    applicants

    Reportsonself-

    perceptionswere

    mostfavorablewhen

    researchinterest,

    workexperience,or

    researchexperience

    informationwas

    provided

    252 NEIL ANDERSON AND VICKI GOLTSI

    International Journal of Selection and Assessmentr 2006 The Authors

    Journal compilationr Blackwell Publishing Ltd. 2006

  • 7/29/2019 Negative Psychological Effects of Selection Methods Construct Formulation and an Empirical Investigation Into an AC

    18/21

    Ta

    bleA1

    .Con

    tinue

    d

    Author(s)

    Construc

    ts

    measure

    d

    Sample

    Predicto

    r

    method

    Measures

    Measurem

    ent

    timing

    Independent

    variable(s)

    Depende

    nt

    variable(s)

    Key

    findings

    Derousetal.(2004)Selectiontreatment

    beliefs

    n15

    660senior-

    yearstudentsat

    campusrecruitment

    event

    n25

    634applicants

    fordifferentbranches

    ofindustry

    SocialProcess

    Model

    ofSelection(SPS

    model)

    Socialprocess

    questionnaireon

    selectionimportance

    (SPQS-I),

    Expectedrealization

    (SPQS-E)!

    only

    theapplicantsfilled

    outthisquestionnaire

    Students:SPQS-I

    questionnairea

    fter

    screeningprocedure

    onthesameday

    Applicants:

    SPQS-IandSP

    QS-E

    afterinitialscreening

    andbeforeactual

    testing

    Gender,workstatus,

    educationallevel,

    motivationtoapply,

    priorselection

    experience

    Selectiontreatment

    importance,

    expectedselection

    treatment

    Supportforstability

    ofsix-factormodel

    (SPQS-I)

    Workstatusdidnot

    effectvaluationand

    expectationof

    treatment

    Motivationandprior

    selectionrelatedto

    SPQS-Ebutnotto

    SPQS-I

    Educationrelatedto

    SPQS-Ebutnotto

    SPQS-I

    Femalesvaluedall

    treatmentfactors

    higherthanmales(in

    SPQS-I)

    Bauer,Truxillo,

    Paronto,

    Weekley,

    andCampion(2004)

    Selectionprocedural

    justice

    n5

    153students

    Face-to-face

    interviewscree

    nings,

    interactivevoic

    e

    response(IVR)

    screenings,

    computer-assis

    ted

    telephone-screening

    interviews

    (contentofscreening

    isidenticalfor

    3

    screeningmeth

    ods)

    WonderlicPersonnel

    test,

    NEO(measures

    conscientiousness),

    selectionprocedural

    justicescale(SPJS)

    !

    11dimensions

    offairness

    Time1:inclassroom,

    questionnaires

    to

    measureindependent

    variables

    Time2:

    IVRcondition:

    studentshadtocall

    forautomatic

    screeningandtook

    surveyonwebsite

    TIcondition:

    studentswerecalled

    forscreeningandtook

    surveyonwebsite

    FTFcondition:

    livescreening

    interviewatcampus.

    Afterscreening

    taken

    tocomputerforfinal

    survey

    Cognitiveability,

    conscientiousness,

    age,gender,work

    experience

    Processandou

    tcome

    fairness

    IVRwasratedlower

    oninterpersonal

    treatment,two-way

    communicationand

    openness(IVRisnon-

    interpersonal)

    Nodifferenceson

    otherprocedural

    justicefactors

    betweenIVRand

    othermethodsof

    screening

    vanVianenetal.

    (2004)

    Fairnessinpersonnel

    selection

    n5

    282applicants

    fordifferentjobsin

    different

    organizations

    Psychometrictests,

    feedbackinterview

    Testbeliefs,

    perceivedfairnessof

    theselection,

    perceptionofthe

    Time1:pre-testing

    Time2:post-testing,

    pre-feedback

    Time3:post-

    feedback

    Opennessto

    experiences,age,

    education

    Testbeliefs,

    perceivedjob

    relatedness,

    perceived

    performance,

    Significanteffectof

    previoustest

    experiencesontest

    beliefs

    Opennessto

    NPES OF ASSESSMENT CENTERS 253

    r 2006 The AuthorsJournal compilationr Blackwell Publishing Ltd. 2006 Volume 14 Number 3 September 2006

  • 7/29/2019 Negative Psychological Effects of Selection Methods Construct Formulation and an Empirical Investigation Into an AC

    19/21

    Ta

    bleA1

    .Con

    tinue

    d

    Author(s)

    Construc

    ts

    measure

    d

    Sample

    Predicto

    r

    method

    Measures

    Measurem

    ent

    timing

    Independent

    variable(s)

    Depende

    nt

    variable(s)

    Key

    findings

    feedback-interview,

    jobattractiveness

    pre-andpost-

    feedbackfairne

    ss

    measures,perc

    eived

    feedbacktreatm

    ent,

    feedbackconte

    nt,

    jobattractivene

    ss

    experienceinfluenced

    perceptionsof

    performance,

    feedbackandfairness

    Perceived

    performanceand

    perceivedjob

    relatednesswere

    independentlyrelated

    topre-feedback

    fairnessperceptions

    Post-feedback

    fairnessperceptions

    werepositively

    affectedbypre-

    feedbackfairness

    perceptions,

    feedbackcontentand

    perceivedfeedback

    treatment.Itwasnot

    relatedtojob

    attractiveness

    Perceivedfeedback

    treatmentand

    feedbackcontent

    directlyaffectedjob

    attractiveness

    Schmittetal.(2004)

    n5

    644freshmen

    students

    Situational

    JudgementInventory

    (SJI)

    Biographicalitems,

    situationaljudgment

    items,reactionitems

    onperformance

    beliefs,

    test

    relevance,

    fairness

    Reactionitems

    directlyafterth

    e

    bio-dataand

    SJIitems

    Biographicalitems

    andsituational

    judgmentitems

    in12performance

    dimensions

    (measuresprevious

    experience,

    interest,

    motivation),ACT/SAT

    test

    Performancebeliefs

    (absoluteand

    comparative),

    perceptionsoftest

    relevance,

    fairness

    Relevanceand

    fairnessaredirectly

    influencedby

    performancebeliefs

    (consistentwithself-

    servingbias)

    Indirecteffectof

    performancebeliefs

    onfairnessmediated

    byrelevance

    perceptions

    (consistentwith

    justiceperspective)

    Relevanceand

    performanceinteract

    intheirimpacton

    fairnesswhen

    comparative

    254 NEIL ANDERSON AND VICKI GOLTSI

    International Journal of Selection and Assessmentr 2006 The Authors

    Journal compilationr Blackwell Publishing Ltd. 2006

  • 7/29/2019 Negative Psychological Effects of Selection Methods Construct Formulation and an Empirical Investigation Into an AC

    20/21

    Ta

    bleA1

    .Con

    tinue

    d

    Author(s)

    Construc

    ts

    measure

    d

    Sample

    Predicto

    r

    method

    Measures

    Measurem

    ent

    timing

    Independent

    variable(s)

    Depende

    nt

    variable(

    s)

    Key

    findings

    performanceas

    opposedtoabsolute

    performanceis

    considered

    Minoritiesreactless

    favorablyto

    standardizedtests

    thanmajorities

    Schinkeletal.

    (2004)

    Feedbackeffec

    t

    n5

    119students

    GMAtests

    Affectivewell-being

    scal