Motivatie Chestionar

  • Upload
    119568

  • View
    308

  • Download
    3

Embed Size (px)

Citation preview

  • 8/12/2019 Motivatie Chestionar

    1/20

    Educational Psychology, Vol. 22, No. 3, 2002

    A Cognitive-behavioural Measure of StudentGoal Setting in a Tertiary Educational Context

    FIONA WHITE, School of Psychology, University of Western Sydney, Australia

    ABSTRACT This study investigated the psychometric properties of a newly developed

    measure, the Student Goals and Behaviour Questionnaire (SGBQ). The SGBQ is a unique

    measure in that it assesses both students goal setting attitudes and behaviour within a tertiary

    education context. In other words, the SGBQ measures students actual, rather than preferred

    academic goals. To date, no such instrument exists in the psychological and educational

    literature. The SGBQ, a modied version of Locke & Lathams (1990) Goal Setting

    Questionnaire (MGSQ), and Wood & Lockes (1987) measure of academic self-efcacy

    (ASEQ) were administered to 100 rst-year Psychology students. With regard to construct

    validity, predicted moderate levels of convergence were found between the SGBQ factors, and

    MGSQ and ASEQ. It was also found that the SGBQ had predictive validity with respect to

    subsequent academic performance. Furthermore, a number of demographic characteristics, suchas course of study and age were found to be associated with goal setting attitudes and

    behaviour. The SGBQ would seem to have some promise as an instrument for the assessment

    and monitoring of student goal setting.

    There is a voluminous literature concerning goal setting as a motivational technique

    for enhancing task performance in organisational and work-related contexts. One

    consistent result obtained in the literature is that setting specic and challenging goals

    result in superior performance to that obtained when participants have less well-articu-lated goals (such as do your best; Mento et al., 1987; Locke & Latham, 1990; Locke,

    1996). However, although there have been some attempts to apply goal setting

    strategies in education (e.g. Wentzel, 1991; Gillat & Sulzer-Azaroff, 1994; Berry,

    1996), no work has investigated the potential benecial effects of goal setting in a

    university context. Related to this gap in the literature, very few researchers have

    attempted to dene and measure goal setting. This study addresses these limitations

    and proposes the following denition, goal setting consists of the cognitive and

    behavioural processes involved in an individuals attempts to plan and regulate their

    ISSN 0144-3410 print; ISSN 1469-046X online/02/030285-20 2002 Taylor & Francis Ltd

    DOI: 10.1080/01443410220138520

  • 8/12/2019 Motivatie Chestionar

    2/20

    286 F. White

    FIG. 1. High Performance Cycle Model (Locke & Latham, 1990).

    activities in order to achieve some longer-term outcome. This denition will form the

    basis of a new measure, the Student Goals and Behaviour Questionnaire (SGBQ), the

    psychometric properties of which will be explicated here. It is anticipated that the

    SGBQ will identify salient factors that perhaps may aid educators and researchers in

    modifying, designing and implementing effective goal setting programmes in tertiaryeducation.

    Locke and Lathams (1990) HPC Model

    One of the leading approaches to understanding motivation is goal-setting theory

    (Locke & Latham, 1990). Goal setting refers to conscious behavioural intention that

    channel our energies or motivation to help us attain future objectives (Muchinsky,

    2000, p. 350). Goal setting theory provides the conceptual framework for Locke &Lathams (1990) High Performance Cycle (HPC) model, one of the most empirically

    tested models in the work motivation literature over the past 25 years (see Fig. 1).

    Locke (1991, p. 25) states that the HPC model is data-based, conceptually well-devel-

    oped, and integrated with other theories that have some insights to offer to researchers.

    The HPC model adopts conceptual tenets from Goal Setting theory (Locke, 1968),

    Expectancy theory (Vroom, 1964) and Social Cognitive theory (Bandura, 1986). In

    particular, the HPC model highlights Banduras (1982, p. 122) concept of self-efcacy,

    that is, ones judgement of how well one can execute courses of action required to deal

  • 8/12/2019 Motivatie Chestionar

    3/20

    Student Goal Setting 287

    with prospective situations. According to Locke & Latham (1990) self-efcacy is said

    to interact with goal setting and mediating mechanisms to predict performance.

    Self-efcacy includes not only ones estimate of the degree to which effort will pay

    off, but also ones ability, adaptability, creativity and capacity to perform in a situational

    context. Self-efcacy is thought to reect both an individuals self-perceived ability, and

    a motivational component dened by Kanfer (1987, p. 260) as intentions for effortallocation. Thus, the more capable people judge themselves to be, the higher the goals

    they will set for upcoming performance (Bandura & Cervone, 1986; Bandura & Wood,

    1989; Thomas & Mathieu, 1994). Although previous research suggests that high goal

    setting and high self-efcacy are positively related, this relationship is yet to be

    investigated within a tertiary educational context.

    The HPC model predicts that individuals with specic, challenging and clear goals

    perform better than those with no goals, easy goals or vague goals (Locke, 1991). Locke

    & Latham (1990) state that nearly 400 (mostly experimental) studies have shown this

    relationship to be the case. These specic and challenging goals then intervene with

    moderating factors (such as task complexity and ability) to predict high performance.

    High performance, in turn, produces rewards, satisfaction and commitment to future

    goals. Button et al. (1996, p. 1085) dene performance episodes as distinguishable

    periods of time over which performance accrues and is reviewed. Organisational

    practice such as yearly performance appraisals, monthly progress reports and quarterly

    sales are examples of how performance has been operationalised in work settings

    (Button et al., 1996). In fact, the HPC model has been applied almost exclusively

    within work-related settings and relatively little work has adopted this model to develop

    a comprehensive theory of academic performance. However, as Locke & Latham

    (1990, p. 245) state the usefulness of the HPC model goes beyond the connes of the

    work organisations another application of this model is to the eld of education.

    The important point to note here is that despite the general similarities that exist

    between work settings and educational contexts, each is quite specialised with regard to

    complexity of tasks, rewards and performance measures. For example, in an educa-

    tional setting, performance may be operationalised via class work and exam assess-

    ments, where students receive feedback. By adopting these specic performance

    indicators the HPC model will be applied to an educational context.

    Application of the HPC Model in an Educational Context

    The academic performance literature has previously identied a range of factors likely

    to have a positive impact upon student academic performance, and interestingly these

    factors appear similar to those highlighted in the HPC model. For example, some

    researchers have examined internal student attributes, such as cognitive styles and

    approaches to study (Biggs, 1987; Marton & Saljo, 1984) as possible determinants ofacademic performance. Other factors examined have included external determinants,

    such as teaching effectiveness. For example, Marsh (1987) and Ramsden (1990) have

    developed instruments for measuring teaching quality, nding that effective teaching,

    involving quality feedback and understandable explanations, is related to improved

    student performance. As previously mentioned, these factors appear similar to the HPC

    models moderating factors of situational constraints and feedback. Therefore in order

    to understand how student academic performance is moderated, it may be valuable to

    measure variables considered to be important within the HPC framework.

  • 8/12/2019 Motivatie Chestionar

    4/20

    288 F. White

    Attempts to Measure Individual Motives and Performance Outcomes

    As discussed, previous attempts have been made to measure individual motives and

    approaches to study. For example, Biggs (1987) Study Processes Questionnaire (SPQ)

    examines motives and strategies, surface versus deep approaches, used in learning.

    Related to this, Marton & Saljo (1984) found that students who adopted a deepapproach, which involved seeking the meaning of material, appeared to understand

    material better than students who adopted a surface approach by memorising infor-

    mation. Moreover, Pintich & DeGroot (1990) developed an attitudinal measure titled

    the Motivated Strategies for Learning Questionnaire, developed for improving second-

    ary teaching and learning. Despite the intuitive appeal of this research, results have

    shown that students approach to studying have not been found to correlate well with

    academic performance (Provost & Bond, 1997). Provost & Bond (1997) suggested that

    one of the problems with these instruments is that they tend to focus upon what

    students intend to do, rather than what they actually do in order to learn material.Attempts have also been made to specically target attitudes towards goal setting.

    Locke & Latham (1990) developed a questionnaire called the Goal Setting Question-

    naire (GSQ) in order to evaluate attitudes to goal setting within work settings. It

    consists of the items relating to goal clarity (I have specic, clear goals to aim for on

    my job) and goal conict (I have too many goals on this job) and other goal setting

    attitudes. However, none of the items measure goal setting behaviour. When specic

    behaviours (such as conscientiousness and goal orientation) have been examined,

    greater effects upon academic performance have been recorded (Colquitt & Simmering,

    1998). Therefore, in order to provide an adequate model of successful student aca-demic performance it is necessary to shift the emphasis from intention to actual

    behaviour.

    Measuring cognitive and behavioural goal setting in a tertiary educational

    context: the construction of the SGBQ

    This brief review of the literature would suggest that there is a genuine need for a

    cognitive and behavioural measure of goal setting in a tertiary educational context. TheStudent Goals and Behaviour Questionnaire (SGBQ) was constructed to address this

    need. The attitudinal and behavioural variables highlighted in the HPC model provided

    the foundation for the construction of some of the SGBQ items. For example, items

    were formulated to measure specic student goals (such as What are your short term

    goals for studying psychology?), general student goals (such as What are your long

    term goals for studying at university?). Some attitudinal items relate to difculty of

    student goals (such as How difcult are the goals you have set?); feedback (Do you

    receive adequate feedback about your academic performance?). Other items related to

    students goal setting behaviour, for example, student effort (such as How much timedid you spend writing your last class report ?) and goal setting (such as What form do

    these goals take?).

    The purpose of this study was to provide a preliminary examination of the utility of

    the SGBQ in an educational context. Several psychometric properties of the SQBQ will

    be investigated. First, internal reliability and factorial validity of the SGBQ will be

    measured. The predictive validity of the SGBQ in relation to academic performance

    will also be reported. The construct validity of the SGBQ will be examined in relation

    to the Modied Goal Setting Questionnaire (MGSQ) and the Academic Self-Efcacy

  • 8/12/2019 Motivatie Chestionar

    5/20

    Student Goal Setting 289

    Questionnaire (ASEQ). Finally, the relationship between the SGBQ and a variety of

    demographic variables (e.g. age, sex and course of study) will be investigated in order

    to provide further information about the construct validity of the scale.

    MethodsParticipants

    The participants were 27 male and 73 female rst year undergraduate Psychology

    students from the University of Western Sydney, Macarthur. Participants were aged

    between 18 and 49 years (M522.41 years). The Tertiary Entrance Rank (TER) for the

    sample ranged from 25 to 95 (M564.35). Of the sample 60% spoke only English at

    home, 16% also spoke a European language, 16% also spoke a Middle Eastern

    language and 8% also spoke an Asian language. The majority of students were enrolled

    in Arts and Social Science degrees. All participants in the study obtained course creditfor their involvement in the study.

    Materials

    There were three questionnaires used in this study: the Student Goals and Behaviour

    Questionnaire (SGBQ), the Modied Goal Setting Questionnaire (MGSQ) and the

    Academic Self-Efcacy Questionnaire (ASEQ).

    (1) The Student Goals and Behaviour Questionnaire (SGBQ) was developed in

    order to test the salient factors identied in the HPC model and with a focus

    open actual behaviour, rather than attitudes towards goal setting. The SGBQ

    consists of 26 items. Items 1, 2, 3, 4, and 9 are categorical in nature. Items 5,

    6, 7, 8, 10, 11, 12, 13, 14, 15, 16, 17, 18, 20, 21, 22, 23a, 23b, 24, and 25 are

    responded to on a Likert rating scale. Item 19 is an open-ended item. The

    psychometric properties and subsequent scoring of the SGBQ will be outlined in

    the results section. A complete version of the SGBQ can be found in Appendix

    A.

    (2) The 53 items of Locke & Lathams (1990) Goal Setting Questionnaire (GSQ)specically refer to work settings and thus in its current form is not appropriate

    fore measuring student goal setting in a tertiary educational context. To address

    this limitation, 38 items were extracted and modied for an educational context

    resulting in the Modied Goal Setting Questionnaire (MGSQ). For example, I

    have unclear goals on this job has been changed to I have unclear academic

    goals. The MGSQ consists of a seven-point Likert scale (where 65Almost

    Always to 05Never). A full list of these items is contained in Appendix B.

    (3) Wood & Lockes (1987) Academic Self-Efcacy Questionnaire (ASEQ), consists

    of 35 items relating to Banduras conceptualisation of magnitude of self-efcacy, that is whether one can achieve that level of attainment and strength

    of self-efcacy, that is ones degree of condence in their ability to perform at

    that level (Wood & Locke, 1987). For example, the item memorise and recall

    60% of facts and concepts, measures magnitude with a yes/no response set and

    strength is measured by a degree of condence rating between 0 and 100%.

    Reliability coefcients for the seven subscales ranged from 0.56 to 0.83 (Wood

    & Locke, 1987).

    (4) Finally, students academic performance was measured via their marks achieved

  • 8/12/2019 Motivatie Chestionar

    6/20

    290 F. White

    on assessment items in a rst year Psychology course. These assessment items

    included: (a) a research report of 1250 words in length that is based on a class

    experiment; and (b) a total mark which is made up of two research report marks,

    and two sets of multiple choice exam marks that assessed lecture, tutorial and

    textbook material for each semester across the year.

    Procedure

    All participants completed the SGBQ, the MGSQ and ASEQ after receiving their

    results from their rst assessment item, a research report. Six months lapsed before

    participants received their nal total mark in rst year Psychology. The administration

    of questionnaires was counterbalanced to avoid order effects. On completion, students

    returned all questionnaires to the researcher at the end of each testing session. When

    the academic year was complete, the project investigators were provided with a list of

    student identication numbers and marks that were matched with the questionnaire

    data before identication numbers were removed. Participants remained anonymous at

    all times to the project investigators. The research reported here was approved by the

    University of Western Sydney Ethics Review Committee (project number 99/067).

    Results

    The validation of the SGBQ began with the formulation of a construct denition

    derived from Goal Setting theory, and prior research and systematic observationinvolving the HPC model (Locke & Latham, 1990). The reliability and validity of the

    SGBQ framework and empirical analyses of items now follow.

    The Factor Structure and Scoring of the SGBQ

    Participant responses to 25 SGBQ items were subjected to an exploratory principal

    components factor analysis with varimax rotation (NB. Item 19 was omitted from the

    analysis) with no missing data points. This procedure was conducted in order to

    investigate the SGBQs factor structure prior to reliability and validity analyses. Ac-cording to Kaisers criteria, factors are retained on the basis of having an eigenvalue

    greater than one (Bryman & Cramer, 1990). The results of the factor analysis revealed

    that there were 10 factors with eigenvalues greater than one.

    The 10 factors reported in Table I accounted for 65.8% of the total variance. These

    factors include:

    Factor 1. Effort contained items representing the amount of time students put towards

    their class work. Factor 1 accounted for the greatest percentage of total variance. The

    alpha coefcient for this ve-item scale was 0.74.Factor 2. Feedback contained items relating to the perceived effect of educators

    feedback on students goals in psychology. The alpha coefcient for this two-item scale

    was 0.96.

    Factor 3. Goal Setting contained items relating to goal setting strategies in psychology.

    The alpha coefcient for this two-item scale was 0.64.

    Factor 4. Goal Inuence contained one item relating to students perception of the

    degree to which others have inuenced their goal setting.

    Factor 5. Teaching Support contained items that measure the degree to which educa-

  • 8/12/2019 Motivatie Chestionar

    7/20

  • 8/12/2019 Motivatie Chestionar

    8/20

    292 F. White

    TABLE I. Principle components factor analysis of the SGBQ

    Factor

    loading Eigenvalue

    Factor 1: Effort

    13. How much time did you spend in the library for your last lab report? 0.605 3.7714. How much time did you spend reading your textbook for your last lab report? 0.836

    15. How much time did you spend reading your lecture notes for your

    last lab report? 0.742

    18. How much time did you s pend writing your last lab report ? 0.572

    16. How much time did you spend in discussion with your tutor for 0.440

    your last lab report?

    Factor 2: Feedback

    23 a. To what extent has feedback (grades, comments etc) affected your 0.973 2.30

    goals in psychology?

    23 b. Specically, how has feedback affected your goals ? 0.969

    Factor 3: Goal Setting

    What are your long term goals for study in psychology? 0.648 1.85

    How did you decide on these goals in psychology? 0.801

    Factor 4: Goal Inuence

    5. To what degree have the formulation of these goals been inuenced

    by others? 0.841 1.65

    Factor 5: Teaching Support

    12. Do you receive adequate feedback about your academic performance 0.811 1.5625. How important is it for your lecturers and tutors to support your need

    to set goals? 0.689

    Factor 6: Goal Efcacy

    7. With regard to Psychology, how difcult are the goals you have set? 0.822 1.46

    8. How likely do you think it is that you will achieve all of these goals? 0.435

    11. Studying is more enjoyable if I have clear goals. To what extent do

    you agree with this statement? 0.474

    Factor 7: Meeting Deadlines

    24. Do you have any trouble dealing with getting work completed on a

    deadline? 0.861 1.386. To what degree have you set deadlines for these goals? 0.479

    Factor 8: Goal Type

    9. What form do these goals take (a contract, diary entry, thoughts in my

    head etc)? 0.759 1.29

    10.To what extent do these goals place stress upon you? 0.488

    Factor 9: Student Discussion

    17. How much time did you s pend in discussi on with other st udents 0.853 1.19

    for your last lab report?

    Factor 10: Short-term Goals

    3. What are your short term goals for study in psychology? 0.818 1.10

    Factor 11: Long-term Goals

    What are your long term goals for study at university? 0.787 0.872

    Factor 12: Class work

    20. Where was this mark relative to the mark you expected to get in this lab report? 0.537 0.822

    21. Were the written comments on your last lab report (positive/negative) ? 0.735

    22. How useful were these comments? 0.590

  • 8/12/2019 Motivatie Chestionar

    9/20

    Student Goal Setting 293

    TABLE II. Course of study and means and standard deviations of SGBQ factors

    Goal Setting Goal Efcacy Meeting Deadlines

    M SD M SD M SD

    BA (n5 33) 8.88 1.93 11.15 1.18 4.64 1.22BSS (n5 15) 9.20 2.31 11.72 1.54 5.01 1.31

    BPsych (n5 9) 10.22 2.05 12.22 1.30 4.67 1.23

    BSW (n511) 7.72 1.32 10.53 1.12 1.78 0.67

    BA/Law (n5 9) 6.67 1.99 11.22 0.97 6.72 1.79

    Misc (n5 6) 9.17 1.72 12.33 0.82 4.67 2.07

    BA5Bachelor of Arts; BSS5Bachelor of Social Science; BPSych5Bachelor of

    Psychology; BSW5Bachelor of Social Work; BA/Law5Bachelor of Arts/Law; and

    Misc5miscellaneous degree students).

    signicant positive relationship was found between age and several of the SGBQ

    factors, namely:

    Factor 1: Effort (r50.509, P,0.001);

    Factor 3: Goal Setting (r50.362, P,0.001);

    Factor 6: Goal Efcacy (r50.392, P,0.001).

    Therefore, it would appear from these correlational results that Effort, Goal Setting and

    Goal Efcacy increase signicantly with age.

    Predictive validity of the SGBQ and academic performance. Prior to testing the predictive

    validity of the SGBQ, a correlational analysis of students TER scores and academic

    performance was conducted. Amongst a sample of 65 reported TER scores, no

    relationship was found between students TER and their research report mark

    (r50.166, P.0.05), however, a small but signicant positive relationship was found

    between students TER score and their total end of year mark in Psychology (r50.267,

    P, 0.05). Thus, it is important to note that irrespective of goal setting (SGBQ factors),

    students TER scores were related to their end of year Psychology mark.A correlational analysis between SGBQ factors and academic performance was

    conducted to investigate predictive validity. Table III reveals that signicant associa-

    tions were found between Factor 1: Effort, and Factor 6: Goal Efcacyand the research

    report. Whereas signicant associations were found between Factor 4: Goal Inuence,

    Factor 6: Goal Efcacy and Factor 10: Short Term Goals and their overall performance

    in Psychology.

    A more detailed analysis of these ndings revealed that students who set clear short

    term goals in psychology (Factor 10), such as to obtain a credit or distinction, perform

    signicantly better in their overall performance in Psychology than those studentswhose short term goals are unsure (F(5, 70)52.49, P, 0.05). See Fig. 2 for a graphical

    depiction of these ndings.

    Convergent validity of the SGBQ with the MGSQ and ASEQ. Table IV reveals support

    for the predicted moderate degree of convergence between the SGBQ factors and

    appropriate subscales of the MGSQ and ASEQ. For example, Factor 1: Effort has a

    signicant positive correlation with MGSQ subscales of Goal Efcacy and Rationale

    and ASEQ subscales of Self-efcacy Magnitude and Strength and signicant negative

  • 8/12/2019 Motivatie Chestionar

    10/20

    294 F. White

    TABLE III. A correlational analysis between SGBQ factors and academic performance

    Academic performance

    Report mark (n5 100) Total mark (n5 100)

    Factor 1: Effort 0.271** 0.132Factor 2: Feedback 20.142 2 0.056

    Factor 3: Goal Setting 0.160 0.028

    Factor 4: Goal Inuence 0.119 2 0.225*

    Factor 5: Teaching Support 20.066 2 0.152

    Factor 6: Goal Efcacy 0.362*** 0.362***

    Factor 7: Deadlines 0.191 0.165

    Factor 8: Goal Type 0.014 0.069

    Factor 9: Student Discussion 0.069 0.006

    Factor 10: Short-term Goals 0.127 0.309**

    *P,0.05; **P, 0.01 and ***P, 0.0001.

    association with MGSQ subscales of Goal Stress and Goal Conict. Factor 3: Goal

    Settingwas found to have a signicant positive correlation with the MGSQ subscale of

    Goal Efcacy and Goal Clarity and the ASEQ subscale of Self-efcacy Strength.

    Similarly, Factor 6: Goal Efcacy had a signicant positive correlation with GSG

    subscales of Goal Efcacy and Goal Clarity and ASEQ subscales of Self-efcacy

    Magnitude and Strength, and a signicant negative association with the MGSQ

    subscales of Goal Conict and Dysfunctional Effect. Interestingly, SGBQ Factors 2:

    FIG. 2. Short-term Goals (Factor 10) and academic performance.

  • 8/12/2019 Motivatie Chestionar

    11/20

  • 8/12/2019 Motivatie Chestionar

    12/20

    296 F. White

    TABLE

    IV.

    CorrelationsbetweentheSGBQfactorsandtheMGSQandASEQ

    scores

    MGSQsubscales

    ASEQ

    Subscales

    SGBQ

    Goal

    Goal

    Tang.

    Goal

    Dysfunt.

    Goal

    SE

    SE

    factors

    Support

    Stress

    Efficacy

    Rationale

    Rewards

    Conct

    Effect

    Clarity

    mag

    strength

    Factor1

    0.0

    65

    2

    0.1

    90*

    0.3

    96***

    0.2

    60*

    2

    0.0

    54

    2

    0.2

    09*

    2

    0.0

    37

    0.2

    57*

    0.2

    37*

    0.2

    40*

    Factor2

    0.1

    59

    0.0

    71

    0.0

    94

    0.0

    08

    0.0

    72

    2

    0.0

    59

    2

    0.1

    35

    0.1

    24

    2

    0.0

    50

    0.0

    74

    Factor3

    2

    0.1

    00

    2

    0.0

    55

    0.3

    88***

    0.0

    56

    0.0

    31

    2

    0.1

    44

    2

    0.2

    21*

    0.3

    03**

    0.1

    14

    0.2

    70**

    Factor4

    2

    0.0

    99

    0.1

    31

    0.0

    07

    0.0

    07

    0.0

    84

    2

    0.2

    10*

    2

    0.1

    46

    0.0

    55

    2

    0.1

    87

    2

    0.1

    71

    Factor5

    0.0

    24

    2

    0.0

    03

    0.1

    70

    0.2

    20*

    0.2

    11*

    2

    0.0

    58

    2

    0.0

    23

    0.2

    92**

    2

    0.3

    6

    0.0

    41

    Factor6

    2

    0.0

    92

    2

    0.0

    93

    0.4

    62***

    0.0

    87

    0.0

    45

    2

    0.2

    11*

    2

    0.2

    03*

    0.2

    23*

    0.2

    04*

    0.2

    78**

    Factor7

    0.0

    67

    2

    0.2

    46*

    0.1

    43

    0.1

    07

    0.0

    62

    2

    0.1

    77

    2

    0.0

    18

    0.0

    70

    0.0

    74

    0.1

    59

    Factor8

    2

    0.1

    85

    0.0

    58

    0.0

    84

    0.0

    40

    0.0

    41

    0.0

    28

    2

    0.1

    84

    0.1

    02

    0.7

    30

    0.0

    08

    Factor9

    0.0

    02

    2

    0.0

    29

    0.2

    14*

    0.1

    41

    2

    0.0

    04

    2

    0.2

    00*

    2

    0.0

    45

    0.2

    83**

    2

    0.0

    50

    2

    1.1

    05

    Factor10

    0.0

    45

    2

    0.1

    23

    0.3

    13**

    2

    0.0

    12

    0.0

    93

    2

    0.3

    10**

    2

    0.0

    54

    0.2

    29*

    2

    0.1

    67

    0.1

    95*

    r5

    100;*P,

    0.0

    5;**P,

    0.0

    1and***P,

    0.0

    01.

  • 8/12/2019 Motivatie Chestionar

    13/20

    Student Goal Setting 297

    found. Specically, older students placed more Effort, were more likely to set Specic

    Goals and had higher Goal Efcacy than younger students. This nding may be

    explained by the fact that older students are highly motivated because they have

    specically chosen tertiary study at this point in their life, whereas younger students

    simply see tertiary study as a transition from secondary school and are less motivated.

    Predictive Validity of the SGBQ

    Students academic performance was measured via:

    a research report of 1250 words in length that is based on a class experiment;

    a total mark that is made up of two research report marks and two sets of

    multiple choice exam marks that assessed lecture, tutorial and textbook material

    for each semester across the year study.

    Thus, the two performance measures were quite distinct. Analyses of the predictive

    validity of the SGBQ revealed that Effort and Goal Efcacy are signicantly associated

    with students research report mark. This nding may be due to the fact that most of

    the items on the Effort factor relate to the lab report. SGBQ factors of Goal Inuence,

    Goal Efcacy and Short Term Goals were found to be signicantly associated with

    students overall performance in Psychology (see Table III).

    Moreover, Figure 2 reveals that students who set clear short-term goals, such as

    seeking to obtain a credit or distinction on their next lab report, actually predicts their

    overall performance. These results support the prediction of the High Performance

    Cycle model and other research ndings (Mento et al., 1987; Locke & Latham, 1990;

    Locke, 1996) that suggest that specic goals results in superior performance. The

    signicance of these results is enhanced by the fact that students completed their goal

    setting questionnaires 6 months prior to receiving their nal mark in rst year Psy-

    chology. Because of this time lag, the results can be interpreted as genuinely predictive

    of future academic performance.

    It must be noted here that six factors of the SGBQ were not found to signicantly

    predict academic performance. Theoretically, these six factors are important (see the

    HPC model), however, the items as they currently stand may not accurately reect thetheoretical concepts that they purport to measure. For example, Goal Setting (Factor

    3) items, as they currently stand, refer to long-term goal setting and do not refer to the

    end of rst year performance. Therefore, the items that make up this factor may be

    more useful and predictive of third year psychology students performance, rather than

    rst year students. Furthermore, all of the items of the Effort factor refer to the lab

    report, thus new items that refer to exam-goal-setting-behaviour may also need to be

    included in a revised version of the SGBQ.

    Convergent Validity of the SGBQ

    Further analysis of validity revealed the SGBQ Factors 1, 3 5, 6, 9 and 10, which

    incorporates variables from the High Performance Cycle Model, showed moderate

    levels of convergence with the MGSQ, which measures attitudes to goal setting and

    Wood & Lockes (1987) ASEQ which measures students magnitude and strength of

    self-efcacy (see Table IV). This degree of convergence is supported by previous

    ndings (Bandura & Cervone, 1986; Bandura & Wood, 1989; Thomas & Mathieu,

    1994) in the literature that suggest that high goal setting and high self-efcacy are

  • 8/12/2019 Motivatie Chestionar

    14/20

    298 F. White

    positively related. However, SGBQ Factors 2 and 8 did not converge signicantly with

    any MGSQ or ASEQ factors revealing that the uniqueness of each measure was also

    conrmed.

    Limitations and Future Applications of the SGBQ

    When interpreting these ndings, sample size limitations need to be taken into account.

    A larger more heterogeneous sample of tertiary students, involving second and third

    year undergraduates would need to be included in future research investigations using

    the SGBQ. Moreover, existing items will need to be modied and new items added as

    previously discussed. Once such changes to sample characteristics and items are made,

    the generalisability of these results, reliability and validity of the SGBQ will be

    improved.

    It is anticipated that the SGBQ will have important applications in the tertiary

    educational context. Once acceptable levels of reliability and validity are achieved, the

    SGBQ can be used to assess student attitudes and behaviour to goal setting, identify

    problem areas in student goals setting, and implement changes to the curriculum where

    students are taught to set specic high goals. Experiments can then be conducted to

    investigate whether students who set specic high goals have improved academic

    performance relative to a control group of students who do not set specic goals.

    The development of a valid scale requires multiple procedures that are employed

    sequentially at different stages of test construction (Anastasi & Urbina. 1997, p. 137).

    Thus, further psychometric validation of the SGBQ amongst a larger, more heteroge-neous sample of tertiary students will be needed before rm conclusions can be made

    about the psychometric properties of the SGBQ. Continued validation of the SGBQ

    will allow it to be used as an instrument by psychologists, educators and other

    researchers in future research concerned with the nature of processes involved in

    student academic performance. Moreover, should future research adopting the SGBQ

    support the nding that systematic goal setting improves students academic perform-

    ance, then all stakeholders in academic institutions have the potential to benet from

    this research outcome.

    Correspondence: Fiona White, School of Psychology, University of Western Sydney,

    Locked Bag 1797, South Penrith Distribution Centre, NSW, Australia, 1797.

    NOTES

    [1] Portions of this research s tudy were presented at the Teaching and Learning Conference, Northern

    Territory University, Australia, 1999. This study is supported in part by a research grant awarded

    by the Research Committee of the University of Western Sydney.

    REFERENCES

    Anastasi, A. & Urbina, S. (1997). Psychological Testing. New Jersey: Prentice Hall.

    Bandura, A. (1982). Self-efcacy mechanism in human agency. American Psychologist, 37, 122147.

    Bandura, A. (1986). Social Foundations of Thought and Action. New Jersey: Prentice Hall.

    Bandura, A. & Cervone, D. (1986). Differential engagement of self-reactive inuence in cognitive

    motivation. Organizational Behaviour and Human Decision Processes, 50, 248287.

    Bandura, A. & Wood, R. (1989). Effect of conceived controllability and performance standards on

    self-regulation of complex decision-making. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 56, 805814.

  • 8/12/2019 Motivatie Chestionar

    15/20

    Student Goal Setting 299

    Berry, G. (1996). Improving Student Achievement through Behaviour Intervention. EDRS: Microche [2

    card(s)], Paper.

    Biggs, J.B. (1987). Student Approaches to Learning and Studying. Melbourne: Australian Council for

    Educational Research.

    Biggs, J.B. (1993). What do inventories of students learning processes really measure? A theoretical

    review and clarication. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 63, 319.

    Bryman, A. & Cramer, D. (1990). Quantitative Data Analysis for the Social Scientists. London:Routledge.

    Button, S.B., Mathieu, J.E. & Aiken, K.J. (1996). An examination of the relative impact of assigned

    goals and self-efcacy on personal goals and performance over time. Journal of Applied Social

    Psychology, 26, 10841103.

    Colquitt, J.A. & Simmering, M.J. (1998). Conscientiousness, Goal Orientation, and Motivation to

    learn during the learning process: A longitudinal study. Journal of Applied Psychology, 83, 654665.

    Gillat, A. & Sulzer-Azaroff, B. (1994). Promoting principals managerial involvement in instructional

    improvement. Journal of Applied Behaviour Analysis, 27, 115129.

    Kanfer, R. (1987). Task-specic motivation: An integrative approach to issues of measurement,

    mechanisms, processes, and determinants. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 5, 237264.

    Lee, C., Bobko, P., Earley, P.C. & Locke, E.A. (1991). An empirical analysis of a goal setting

    questionnaire, Journal of Organizational Behaviour, 12, 467482.

    Locke, E.A. (1968). Toward a theory of task motivation and incentives. Organisational Behaviour and

    Human Performance, 3, 157189.

    Locke, E.A. (1991). Goal theory versus control theory: Contrasting approaches to understanding work

    motivation. Motivation and Emotion, 15, 928.

    Locke, E.A. (1996). Motivation through conscious goal setting. Applied & Preventative Psychology, 5,

    117124.

    Locke, E.A. & Latham, G.P. (1990). Work motivation and satisfaction: light at the end of the tunnel.

    Psychological Science, 1, 240246.

    Marsh, H. W. (1987). Students evaluations of university teaching: research ndings, methodologicalissues, and directions for future research. International Journal of Educational Research, 11.

    Marton, F. & Saljo, R. (1984). Approaches to learning. In, F. Marton, D. Hounsell, & N. Entwistle

    (Eds) The Experience of Learning(pp. 3655). Edinburgh, Scottish Academic Press.

    Mento, A.J., Steel, R.P. & Karren, R.J. (1987). A meta-analysis of the effects of goal setting on task

    performance: 19661984. Organisational Behaviour and Human Decision Processes, 39, 5283.

    Muchinsky, P.M. (2000). Psychology Applied to Work. USA: Wadsworth.

    Pintich, P. & Degroot, E. (1990). Motivational and self-regulated learning components of classroom

    academic performance. Journal of Educational Psychology, 82, 3340.

    Provost, S.C. & Bond, N.W. (1997). Approaches to studying and academic performance in a

    traditional psychology course. Higher Education Research and Development, 16, 309320.

    Ramsden, P. (1991). A performance indicator of teaching quality in higher education: the course

    experience questionnaire. Studies in Higher Education, 16, 129150.

    Thomas, K.M. & Mathieu, J.E. (1994). Role of causal attributions in dynamic self-regulation and goal

    processes. Journal of Applied Psychology, 79, 812818.

    Vroom, V.H. (1964). Work and Motivation. New York, NY: Wiley.

    Wentzel, K.R. (1991). Relations between social competence and academic achievement in early

    adolescents. Child Development, 62, 10661078.

    Wood, R.E. & Locke, E.A. (1987). The relations of self-efcacy and grade goals to academic

    performance. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 47, 10131025.

  • 8/12/2019 Motivatie Chestionar

    16/20

    300 F. White

    Appendix A: Student Goals and Behaviour Questionnaire (SGBQ)

  • 8/12/2019 Motivatie Chestionar

    17/20

    Student Goal Setting 301

    Appendix A: Student Goals and Behaviour Questionnaire (SGBQ)continued

  • 8/12/2019 Motivatie Chestionar

    18/20

    302 F. White

    Appendix A: Student Goals and Behaviour Questionnaire (SGBQ)continued

  • 8/12/2019 Motivatie Chestionar

    19/20

    Student Goal Setting 303

    Appendix B: Modied Goal Setting Questionnaire (MGSQ)

    1. I understand exactly what I am supposed to do as a student

    2. I have specic, clear goals to aim for as a student

    3. The academic goals I have are challenging, but reasonable (neither too hard or too easy)

    4. I understand how my academic performance is measured5. I have deadlines for meeting my study goals

    6. If I have more than one goal to accomplish, I know which ones are most important and which

    are least important

    7. Lecturers and/or tutors clearly explain to me what my goals should be

    8. Lecturers and/or tutors tell me the reasons for the goals I should have

    9. Lecturers and/or tutors are supportive with respect to encouraging me to reach my goals

    10. Lecturers and/or tutors let me participate in the setting of my goals

    11. Lecturers and/or tutors let me have a say in deciding how I will go about implementing my goals

    12. If I reach my goals I know my lecturers and/or tutors will be pleased

    13. I get credit and recognition when I attain my academic goals

    14. Trying for academic goals makes studying more fun than it would be without goals15. I feel proud when I get feedback (in the form of grades) indicating that I have achieved my

    academic goals

    16. The other students I work with encourage me to attain my academic goals

    17. I sometimes compete with other students to see who can do the best in reaching their academic

    goals

    18. If I reach my academic goals, I feel that this will increase my academic opportunities

    19. If I reach my academic goals, it increases my chances of a pay rise

    20. If I reach my academic goals, it increases my chances of getting into a higher degree

    21. I usually feel that I have a suitable or effective action plan(s) for reaching my academic goals

    22. I get regular feedback from my lecturers and/or tutors indicating how I am performing in relation

    to my academic goals

    23. I feel that my course was good enough so that I am capable of reaching my academic goals

    24. Departmental policies help, rather than hurt academic goal attainment

    25. Students work together to attain academic goals

    26. This department provides sufcient resources (equipment, etc.) to make goal setting work

    27. I nd working toward my academic goals very stressful

    28. My academic goals are much too difcult

    29. In the past I have not succeeded in attaining my academic goals

    30. My lecturers and/or tutors acts non-supportively when I fail to reach my academic goals

    31. I have too many academic goals

    32. Some of my academic goals conict with my personal values33. I am given incompatible or conicting goals by different people (or even by the same person)

    34. I have unclear academic goals

    35. My academic goals lead me to take excessive risk

    36. My academic goals serve to limit, rather than raise my academic performance

    37. The academic goals that I have lead me to ignore other important aspects of university life

    38. The academic goals that I have focus only on short-range accomplishment and ignore important

    long-range consequences

  • 8/12/2019 Motivatie Chestionar

    20/20