79
Retrospective eses and Dissertations Iowa State University Capstones, eses and Dissertations 1980 Morphine treatment at different infant ages: influence on later morphine effects in rats Michael omas Bardo Iowa State University Follow this and additional works at: hps://lib.dr.iastate.edu/rtd Part of the Biological Psychology Commons , and the Psychiatry and Psychology Commons is Dissertation is brought to you for free and open access by the Iowa State University Capstones, eses and Dissertations at Iowa State University Digital Repository. It has been accepted for inclusion in Retrospective eses and Dissertations by an authorized administrator of Iowa State University Digital Repository. For more information, please contact [email protected]. Recommended Citation Bardo, Michael omas, "Morphine treatment at different infant ages: influence on later morphine effects in rats " (1980). Retrospective eses and Dissertations. 7364. hps://lib.dr.iastate.edu/rtd/7364

Morphine treatment at different infant ages: influence on

  • Upload
    others

  • View
    3

  • Download
    0

Embed Size (px)

Citation preview

Page 1: Morphine treatment at different infant ages: influence on

Retrospective Theses and Dissertations Iowa State University Capstones, Theses andDissertations

1980

Morphine treatment at different infant ages:influence on later morphine effects in ratsMichael Thomas BardoIowa State University

Follow this and additional works at: https://lib.dr.iastate.edu/rtd

Part of the Biological Psychology Commons, and the Psychiatry and Psychology Commons

This Dissertation is brought to you for free and open access by the Iowa State University Capstones, Theses and Dissertations at Iowa State UniversityDigital Repository. It has been accepted for inclusion in Retrospective Theses and Dissertations by an authorized administrator of Iowa State UniversityDigital Repository. For more information, please contact [email protected].

Recommended CitationBardo, Michael Thomas, "Morphine treatment at different infant ages: influence on later morphine effects in rats " (1980). RetrospectiveTheses and Dissertations. 7364.https://lib.dr.iastate.edu/rtd/7364

Page 2: Morphine treatment at different infant ages: influence on

INFORMATION TO USERS

This was produced from a copy of a document sent to us for microfilming. While the most advanced technological means to photograph and reproduce this document have been used, the quality is heavily dependent upon the quality of the material submitted.

The following explanation of techniques is provided to help you understand markings or notations which may appear on this reproduction.

1. The sign or "target" for pages apparently lacking from the document photographed is "Missing Page(s)". If it was possible to obtain the missing page(s) or section, they are spliced into the film along with adjacent pages. This may have necessitated cutting through an image and duplicating adjacent pages to assure you of complete continuity.

2. When an image on the film is obliterated with a round black mark it is an indication that the film inspector noticed either blurred copy because of movement during exposure, or duplicate copy. Unless we meant to delete copyrighted materials that should not have been filmed, you will find a good image of the page in the adjacent frame.

3. When a map, drawing or chart, etc., is part of the material being photo­graphed the photographer has followed a definite method in "sectioning" the material. It is customary to begin filming at the upper left hand comer of a large sheet and to continue from left to right in equal sections with small overlaps. If necessary, sectioning is continued again—beginning below the first row and continuing on until complete.

4. For any illustrations that cannot be reproduced satisfactorily by xerography, photographic prints can be purchased at additional cost and tipped into your xerographic copy. Requests can be made to our Dissertations Customer Services Department.

5. Some pages in any document may have indistinct print. In all cases we have filmed the best available copy.

University Microfilms

International 300 N. ZEEB ROAD, ANN ARBOR, Ml 48106 18 BEDFORD ROW, LONDON WCIR 4EJ, ENGLAND

Page 3: Morphine treatment at different infant ages: influence on

8019622

BARDO, MICHAEL THOMAS

MORPHINE TREATMENT AT DIFFERENT INFANT AGES: INFLUENCE ON LATER MORPHINE EFFECTS IN RATS

Iowa State University PH.D. 1980

University Microfilms

I n 10 r n ât i O n â I 300 N. Zeeb Road, Ann Arbor, MI 48106 18 Bedford Row, London WCIR 4EJ, England

Page 4: Morphine treatment at different infant ages: influence on

Morphine treatment at different infant ages: Influence

on later morphine effects in rats

by

Michael Thomas Bardo

A Dissertation Submitted to the

Graduate Faculty in Partial Fulfillment of the

Requirements for the Degree of

DOCTOR OF PHILOSOPHY

Major: Psychology

Approved:

Charge of Major Wo

For the Major Department

For the Graduate Oollege

Iowa State University

Ames, Iowa

1980

Signature was redacted for privacy.

Signature was redacted for privacy.

Signature was redacted for privacy.

Page 5: Morphine treatment at different infant ages: influence on

ii

ABSTRACT

INTRODUCTION

EXPERIMENT 1

Method

Results

Discussion

EXPERIMENT 2

Method

Results

Discussion

GENERAL DISCUSSION

REFERENCES

ACKNOWLEDGMENTS

TABLE OF CONTENTS

Page

1

3

8

9.

13

38

43

43

44

57

59

66

74

Page 6: Morphine treatment at different infant ages: influence on

1

ABSTRACT

Evidence suggests that opiate receptor development plays a role in mor­

phine-induced analgesia. From birth to about 15 days of age in rats, there

is a concomitant increase in number of opiate receptors and morphine-in­

duced analgesic effect with virtually no change in blood-brain barrier

maturation. The present two experiments examined possible age-related

changes in tolerance to morphine-induced analgesic, hypoactive, and hyper­

thermic effects during this period.

In Experiment 1, rats were administered either morphine (1 mg/kg), sal-

line, or no treatment at one day of age. At 26 days of age, one half of

the animals from each infant treatment group received morphine (5 mg/kg)

and the other half received saline. All animals were then assessed for

pain responsivity, activity and body temperature. This injection-test pro­

cedure was repeated at 27 and 28 days of age. At 29 days of age, all an­

imals received morphine and were similarly tested. Rats treated with mor­

phine one day after birth subsequently displayed tolerance to morphine-in-

duced analgesic and hypoactive effects, but not to hyperthermic effects.

Infant slaine-treated animals displayed less activity and higher body tem­

peratures than infant untreated animals.

In Experiment 2, rats were administered either morphine (1 mg/kg) or

saline at 1, 5, 9, or 13 days of age. At 26 days of age, one half of each

infant treatment group received morphine (5 mg/kg) and the other half re­

ceived saline. Injections and tests for pain responsivity, activity, and

body temperature were similar to those of Experiment 1. Animals treated

with morphine at 1 but not at 5, 9, or 13 days of age displayed tolerance

Page 7: Morphine treatment at different infant ages: influence on

2

to morphine-induced analgesic and hypoactive effects. Animals treated with

saline early in infancy displayed less pain responsivity and activity than

animals treated with saline late in infancy.

These results indicate that single-dose tolerance to 1 mg/kg morphine

in infants is age-dependent. Age-dependent changes in susceptibility to

single-dose morphine tolerance may reflect a change in either blood-brain

barrier or opiate receptor development. Furthermore, these results indi­

cate that saline injection in infancy may produce stress-related changes

in later pain responsivity, activity, and body temperature.

Page 8: Morphine treatment at different infant ages: influence on

3

INTRODUCTION

A single systemic injection of morphine (5-15 mg/kg) can produce

various transient behavioral and physiological effects in rats. These

effects include: analgesia (Bonnet and Peterson, 1975; D'Amour and Smith,

1941; Evans, 1961; Johannesson and Woods, 1964; Saunders, Paolino, Bousquet,

and Miya, 1974), hypoactivity (Collins, 1965; Kuschinsky and Hornykiewicz,

1974) and hyperthermia (Cox, Ary, Chesarek and Lomax, 1976; Martin,

Pryzbylik, and Spector, 1977; Siegel, 1978). Each of these effects dimin­

ishes with repeated morphine injections at a constant dose and this dimin­

ished effect is termed tolerance. Although tolerance to the analgesic

effect can be obtained following a single morphine injection (Cochin and

Kornetsky, 1964; Kometsky and Bain, 1968) , tolerance to the hyperthermic

effect requires a substantially greater number of injections (Fernandas,

Kluwe and Coper, 1977). These results demonstrate that the rate of toler­

ance development depends on which morphine effect is assessed.

Recent evidence indicates that some morphine effects depend on the

developmental age of the animal. In particular, two major developmental

events are important determinants of morphine effects across the life-span.

First, the blood-brain barrier (BBB) to morphine in rats is virtually non­

existent before about 16-20 days of age. Although fetal rats are partially

protected from morphine by a placental barrier (Johannesson, Steele, and

Becker, 1972), neonates are extremely susceptible to morphine effects.

This morphine susceptibility diminishes at about 16-20 days of age, as the

BBB to morphine begins to develop, and susceptibility rapidly becomes adult­

like by about 26-32 days of age as BBB development stabilizes (Johannesson

Page 9: Morphine treatment at different infant ages: influence on

4

and Becker, 1973; Johannesson, Steele, and Becker, 1972; Kupferberg and

Way, 1963).

Development of brain opiate receptors is a second major determinant of

some morphine effects- Studies investigating stereospecific binding of the

opiate antagonists naloxone and naltrexone in rats indicate that opiate

receptors are first evident about 15 days into gestation, and that the

number of receptors progressively increases throughout the remainder of

gestation and postpartum maturation (Clendeninn, Petraitis, and Simon, 1976;

Coyle and Pert, 1976). These experiments indicate an approximately 15-

fold increase in the number of opiate receptors from birth to 60 days of

age. Although increased opiate receptor development might be expected to

produce a concomitant increase in morphine effects, this is not the case

for all morphine effects. The lethal dose (LD^Q) of morphine, for example,

is more directly related to BBB development than to opiate receptor devel­

opment. Kupferberg and Way (1963) injected rats intraperitoneally with

morphine at either 1, 2, 4, 8, 12, 16, 24, or 32 days of age and found that

LD^Q was 45 mg/kg in 1-16 day-olds, 125 mg/kg in 24 day-olds and 220 mg/

kg in 32 day-olds. This ontogenetic change in was inversely corre­

lated with brain permeability to morphine in blood. Since remained

essentially constant from 1 to 16 days of age despite an increasing number

of opiate receptors during this period, it is unlikely that morphine le­

thality is directly related to opiate receptor development.

In contrast to morphine lethality, morphine-induced analgesic effects

are thought to be more directly related to opiate receptor development, in

part because morphine-induced analgesia is antagonized by naloxone, the

Page 10: Morphine treatment at different infant ages: influence on

5

"pure" opiate antagonist which has greater affinity for the opiate receptor

than morphine (Terenius, 1974) and also because morphine produces analgesia

when microinjected into high density but not low density opiate receptor

brain sites (Mayer and Murphin, 1976; Yaksh, Yeung and Rudy, 1975), Thus,

morphine-induced analgesic effects might be expected to change as a function

of the developmental increase in number of opiate receptors that occurs

prior to BBB development. Consistent with this possibility, Auguy-Valette

and co-workers (1978) tested rats at either 5, 10, 15, 20, 25, 30, 45, 60,

or 120 days of age and observed an age-related biphasic change in morphine-

induced analgesic effect. The first phase (5-15 days) was characterized by

an incremental sensitivity to morphine-induced analgesia that was paralleled

by an increase in number of opiate receptors. The second phase (15-30 days)

was characterized by a decremental sensitivity to morphine-induced analgesia

that was paralleled by a decrease in brain permeability to the drug. These

observations indicate that both opiate receptor and BBB development influr

ence morphine-induced analgesic effects, although their influence is evident

at different stages of infancy.

Since acute morphine-induced analgesic effects are directly related to

the number of opiate receptors prior to BBB development, chronic morphine-

induced analgesic effects involved in tolerance may also be related to

opiate receptor development during this same period of infancy. Several

studies have demonstrated that morphine administration in infancy produces

reliable and long-lasting tolerance (Sonderegger, Bromley and Zimmermann,

1977; Sonderegger and Zimmermann, 1976; Sonderegger and Zimmermann, 1978;

Zimmermann, Branch, Newman Taylor, Young and Pang, 1974). In these in-

Page 11: Morphine treatment at different infant ages: influence on

6

stances, however, treatment consisted of relatively long-term morphine

exposure (either pellet implantation or daily injections for one week or

longer) and thus the potential relationship between opiate receptor devel­

opment and tolerance was not evaluated. Moreover, such long-term morphine

exposure in infancy may produce increased mortality, decreased body weight,

and delayed eye-opening onset (Zimmermann, Sonderegger, and Bromley, 1977).

These profound physiological effects, which reflect retarded development,

may be minimized by short-term rather than long-term morphine exposure

at low dosage. Short-term, low-dose morphine exposure should also permit

evaluation of the potential relationship between opiate receptor develop­

ment and tolerance.

Some evidence suggests that early short-term exposure to a low dose

of morphine may not lead to tolerance. Rats exposed to morphine on either

the day before birth (Johannesson, Steele, and Becker, 1972) or the day

after birth (Huidobro and Huidobro, 1973) display increased morphine-in-

duced analgesic effects (hyperanalgesia) when tested at a later age. These

observations contrast with reports of tolerance following single-dose mor­

phine in adults (Cochin and Kornetsky, 1964; Kornetsky and Bain, 1968) and

with reports of tolerance following long-term morphine exposure in infants

(Sonderegger, Bromley, and Zimmermann, 1977; Sonderegger and Zimmermann,

1976; Sonderegger and Zimmermann, 1978; Zimmermann, Branch, Newman Taylor,

Young, and Pang, 1974). These contrasting observations suggest the inter­

esting possibility that short-term morphine exposure during early periods

of opiate receptor development may induce increased sensitivity to sub­

sequent morphine treatment, and that short-term morphine exposure during

Page 12: Morphine treatment at different infant ages: influence on

7

later periods of opiate receptor development may induce decreased sensi­

tivity to subsequent morphine treatment. The research reported here ex­

amined that possibility.

Page 13: Morphine treatment at different infant ages: influence on

8

EXPERIMENT 1

The purpose of this experiment was to determine if the observation

of single-dose morphine hyperanalgesia reported by Huidobro and Huidobro

(1973) could be replicated. Huidobro and Huidobro (1973) used two main

treatment groups to demonstrate this effect. One group was subcutaneously

injected with 1 mg/kg morphine at one day of age and the second group was

untreated. At 16 days of age, each animal from both groups was subcuta-

neously injected with 1 mg/kg morphine and subsequently assessed for pain

responsivity with a standard hot plate procedure, i.e., latency to perform

a paw-lick response to a noxious thermal stimulus was measured. Animals

given morphine at one day of age and tested at 16 days of age displayed

greater morphine analgesia (hyperanalgesia) than untreated animals. How­

ever, since these two groups were not given similar handling-injection

treatment at one day of age, it is not possible to conclude that the hyper-

analgesic effect reflects morphine exposure per se. Treatment of neonates

which involves separation from the lactating mother may produce systematic

behavioral and physiological changes in maternal care (Barnett and Burn,

1967; Bell, Nitschke, Bell, and Zachman, 1974; Smotherman, Wiener, Mendoza,

and Levine, 1977). These stress-related effects can influence pain res-

sponsivity independently of morphine by affecting pituitary-adrenal activ­

ity which can influence pain responsivity (Bodnar, Glusman, Spiaggia, Brutus,

and Kelly, 1978).

The present experiment examined the effects of early morphine and

saline injection relative to no injection on subsequent morphine-induced

changes in pain responsivity, activity, and body temperature. Tests of

Page 14: Morphine treatment at different infant ages: influence on

9

these early treatments were given at 26 days of age rather than at the 16

day age used by Huidobro and Huidobro (1973) since preliminary data from

this laboratory indicated that paw-lick responses on a hot plate surface

were extremely variable in 16-day-old animals.

Method

Animals. Nine multiparous pregnant Long-Evans hooded rats (Blue

Spruce Farms, New York) arrived in our laboratory before parturition.

They were individually caged in standard stainless steel maternity cages

(43 X 26 X 18 cm) and maintained in a temperature and humidity controlled

room under a 12 hour light-dark cycle with water and Teklad pellets con­

tinuously available. Maternity cages were inspected twice daily during

the light hours, and the day on which a litter was first observed was

designated as the day of parturition (Day 0).

Apparatus. The hot plate apparatus consisted of a slide warming tray

(Chicago Surgical & Electrical Co., 26020) with its temperature control

dial set to remain constantly on at the highest temperature. Temperature

was controlled and maintained at a relatively constant 52° C by a Variac

(Standard Electrical Co., 300 BU). A 30 X 15 X 35 cm clear plastic chamber

with a hinged top and open bottom was placed on the hot plate surface. The

chamber was covered with brown adhesive paper except for the top and a

30 X 7 cm window along the bottom of the front wall through which animals

could be observed. The hot plate apparatus was placed in a 100 X 30 X90 cm

wooden wall cabinet with the doors and shelves removed. A 15 W white light

was mounted on the back wall of the cabinet interior and illuminated the

Page 15: Morphine treatment at different infant ages: influence on

10

hot plate apparatus. Response latencies were recorded to the nearest 0.1

sec by a hand-operated electronic timer (Hunter, 120A). The activity

apparatus consisted of a 30 X 15 X 35 cm brown adhesive-paper covered

chamber with a hinged top and hardware cloth floor. Four photocell sensors

and light sources were arranged to divide this chamber interior into nine

approximately equal rectangular sections 3 cm above the floor. Solid state

equipment (Massey Dickinson) transduced and recorded photobearn interruptions.

A reinforced thermistor bead (Fenwal Electronics, FA41J1) and ohmmeter

(Hewlett-Packard, 3476A) were used to assess rectal temperature. All appa­

ratus was located in a room adjacent to where the animals were housed.

Procedure. On the day after parturition (Day 1), pups in each litter

were sexed and the litters culled to 10 pups (5 male and 5 female) whenever

possible. After sexing and culling, which took about five minutes, all

pups from six randomly chosen litters were weighed to the nearest 0.1 g and

injected sc at the nape of the neck with either morphine or saline; three

litters received 1.0 mg/ml/kg morphine sulfate and three litters received

an equivalent volume of 0.9% saline. These pups were separated from their

mothers for about 15 minutes and were kept in huddles before and after in­

jection. Pups from the remaining three litters were neither weighed nor

injected but were returned to their mothers immediately after culling.

Following Day 1 treatment, all animals were left undisturbed until

weaning. Animals were removed from their mothers and placed with litter-

mates in a stainless steel group-cage on Day 21 and were individually caged

in standard stainless steel cages on Day 25. In both group and individual

housing conditions, Teklad pellets and water were continuously available.

Page 16: Morphine treatment at different infant ages: influence on

11

On Day 26, each animal was injected with either morphine or saline

and tested for pain responsivity, activity and body temperature. One half

of the animals from each Day 1 treatment litter were injected sc with 5.0

mg/ml/kg morphine sulfate and the remaining animals were injected with 0.9%

saline. Assignment of animals to either morphine or saline treatment was

random with the stipulation that each group consist of an approximately

equal number of males and females. Animals were tested for pain respon­

sivity 30 minutes after injection. The animal was placed on the hot plate

surface (52° C) and latency to perform a paw-lick response to a front or

hind paw was recorded as paw-lick latency (PLL). If a paw-lick response

was not observed within 120 sec, the test was terminated and PLL recorded

as 120 sec. Activity was tested 45 minutes after injection. The animal

was placed in the activity apparatus for 90 sec and each photobeam inter­

ruption was recorded as an activity count. Body temperature was. recorded

60, 120, and 180 minutes after injection. The animal was hand-held and the

thermistor bead inserted 1 cm into the rectum. After approximately 15 sec,

resistance was recorded to the nearest 0.01 k 0, and k data were later

transformed to C° data. In all cases, animals were returned to their home

cages between postinjection tests.

Injections and postinjection tests on Day 26 (Test Day 1) were repeated

on Days 27 and 28 (Test Days 2 and 3). On Day 29 (Test Day 4), all animals

were injected with 5.0 mg/ml/kg morphine and tested for pain responsivity,

activity and body temperature as described for Test Days 1-3. Summaries of

the different treatment groups and postinjection tests are provided in Ta­

bles 1 and 2 respectively.

Page 17: Morphine treatment at different infant ages: influence on

12

Table 1. Summary of treatment groups in Experiment 1.

Infant Injection Test Day Injection

Group # Litters Day 1 Sub-group N Days 26-28 Day 29

A 3 None A^ 11 Morphine Morphine

4 11 Saline Morphine

B 3 Saline 12 Morphine Morphine

^2 12 Saline Morphine

C 3 Morphine 9 Mo rphine Morphine

S 9 Saline Morphine

Table 2. Nummary of postinjection tests on each test day.

Postinjection

Interval (min) Test

30 Pain Responsivity

45 Activity

60 Body Temperature

120 Body Temperature

180 Body Temperature

Page 18: Morphine treatment at different infant ages: influence on

13

Data analysis. Potential sex and litter effects on pain responsivity,

activity and body temperature were considered nuisance variables. Although

potential sex differences were controlled experimentally by assigning

approximately one half male and one half female to each treatment group,

potential litter differences were not controlled. These potential litter

effects were therefore assessed by separate hierarchial ANOVAs for each

dependent measure in which animals were nested in litters and litters were

nested in Day 1 treatment. In these preliminary analyses, data were col­

lapsed across within-group factors.

Infant and test day treatment differences were analyzed as separate

split-plot ANOVAs for each dependent measure. Comparisons between infant

morphine treated and untreated animals were not of interest. Thus, data

obtained from infant untreated and saline treated animals were analyzed

separately from those obtained from infant saline treated and morphine

treated animals . Significant interactions were subsequently analyzed for

simple main effects.

Results

Table 3 summarizes the mortality data for each infant treatment group.

Although there was no significant difference in number of mortalities be­

tween infant saline treated and untreated animals, morphine treatment

produced significantly more mortalities than both saline and no treatments

(x = 4.97; = 1; 2 <.05). All mortalities occurred prior to weaning at

21 days of age.

Hierarchal analyses of variance revealed no significant differences

Page 19: Morphine treatment at different infant ages: influence on

14

Table 3. Number of mortalities from infant untreated, saline treated and morphine treated groups in Experiment 1.

Injection # Alive # Alive

Group # Litters on Day 1 on Day 1 on Day 26 Mortality

A 3 None 29 22 24%

B 3 Saline 27 24 11%

C 3 Morphine 30 18 40%

Page 20: Morphine treatment at different infant ages: influence on

15

among litters in either pain responsivity, activity, or body temperature

on test days 1-3. In these analyses, animals tested following test in­

jections of morphine displayed longer PLLs (F^ = 22,49; df^ = 1, 6; 2 <

.01), less activity (F = 71.59; d^ =1, 6; 2. .001), and higher body

temperature = 71.59; = 1, 6; & .001) than animals tested following

saline. Since no litter effects were obtained on these measures, all sub­

sequent statistical analyses were performed without regard to litter dif­

ferences .

Pain responsivity. Figure 1 summarizes the pain responsivity data

obtained from infant untreated (Panel A) and saline treated (Panel B)

animals following either morphine or saline injections on test days 1-3.

In these infant treatment groups, morphine on test day 1 produced signi­

ficantly longer PLLs than saline (% = 72.85; = 1, 70; ^ .001).

There was a significant decline in PLLs across test days 1-3 following

either morphine (F = 92.33; = 2, 84; 2. .001) or saline (F^ = 7.10;

= 2, 84; 2 .01). On test day 3, there was no significant difference in

PLLs between morphine and saline injected animals. The magnitude of mor­

phine-induced analgesic effect and rate of tolerance development were not

influenced by saline injection in infancy, as there were no significant

differences in PLLs between infant saline treated and untreated animals.

Figure 2 summarizes the data obtained from these same treatment groups

when each animal was administered morphine on test day 4. For groups la­

belled Morph this was their fourth test injection of morphine and for

groups labelled Sal this was their first test injection of morphine. Tol­

erance is evident in that Morph groups displayed significantly shorter PLLs

Page 21: Morphine treatment at different infant ages: influence on

Figure 1. Mean paw-lick latencies obtained following either morphine or saline on test days 1-3 from animals untreated in infancy (Panel A) and animals treated with saline in infancy (Panel B) in Experiment 1.

Page 22: Morphine treatment at different infant ages: influence on

o UJ CO

o z UJ

5

70

60

50

40

30

O _ J 20

< 10 û.

MORPHINE

SALINE ^ a

A

-O

_L

_ s,

1 3 I

TEST DAY

>>

BUSCsjlw

2

Page 23: Morphine treatment at different infant ages: influence on

Figure 2. Mean paw-lick latencies obtained following morphine on test day 4 from animals untreated in infancy (Panel A) and animals treated with saline in infancy (Panel B) in Experiment 1. Morph groups previously received morphine injections on test days 1-3 and Sal groups previously received saline injections on test days 1-3.

Page 24: Morphine treatment at different infant ages: influence on

70 r-

O

S "

>- 50 0

1 40 5

30

y 20

É " 0

B

ALL INJECTIONS MORPHINE

MORPH SAL MORPH SAL

DRUG TREATMENT ON TEST DAYS 1-3

Page 25: Morphine treatment at different infant ages: influence on

than Sal groups (F = 18.20; = 1, 42; £ < .001). There was no signif­

icant difference in PLLs between infant untreated (Panel A) and saline

treated (Panel B) animals. Moreover, regardless of infant treatment,

animals given morphine for the first time on test day 4 (Fig 2, Sal groups)

displayed significantly shorter PLLs than animals given morphine for the

first time on test day 1 (Fig 1, Morphine groups on test day 1; % = 10.44;

df = 1, 44; 2. <'01). This latter attenuation in PLL was produced by re­

peated hot plate tests independent of morphine and presumably reflects

behavioral tolerance (e.g., Kayan, Ferguson, and Mitchell, 1973).

Although there were no significant differences in pain responsivity

between infant saline treated and untreated animals, there were signifi­

cant differences between infant morphine and saline treated animals. Fig­

ure 3 summarizes the PLL data obtained from infant saline treated (Panel

A) and morphine treated (Panel B) animals following either morphine or

saline injections on test days 1-3. In these infant treatment groups,

morphine on test day 1 produced significantly longer PLLs than saline (2

= 30.14; f = 1, 63; 2 .001). There was a significant decline in PLLs

across test days 1-3 following either morphine (^ = 44.49; = 2, 76; 2.

< .001) or saline (^ = 4.69; df = 2, 76; 2. < .05). On test day 3, there

was no significant difference in PLLs between morphine and saline injected

animals. Tolerance was potentiated by morphine treatment in infancy, as

infant morphine treated animals displayed significantly shorter PLLs on

test day 1 than infant saline treated animals (£ = 14.16; = 1, 63; 2 <

•05). This decrease in PLLs produced by infant morphine treatment was

obtained regardless of whether the injection on test day 1 was morphine

Page 26: Morphine treatment at different infant ages: influence on

Figure 3. Mean paw-lick latencies obtained following either morphine or saline on test days 1-3 from animals treated with saline in infancy (Panel A) and animals treated with morphine in infancy (Panel B) in Experiment 1.

Page 27: Morphine treatment at different infant ages: influence on

O

bJ V)

O

z W

55

O

70

60

SO

40

30

MORPHINE

SALINE CI

3 20

§ 10 GU

TEST DAY

Page 28: Morphine treatment at different infant ages: influence on

Figure 4. Mean paw-lick latencies obtained following morphine on test day 4 from animals treated with saline in infancy (Panel A) and animals treated with morphine in infancy (Panel B) in Experiment 1. Morph groups previously received morphine injections on test days 1-3 and Sal groups previously received saline injections on test days 1-3.

Page 29: Morphine treatment at different infant ages: influence on

70

O N «0

O z Ul

50

40

30

H 20

i .0 Q.

B

ALL INJECTIONS MORPHINE

to

MORPH SAL MORPH SAL

DRUG TREATMENT ON TEST DAYS 1-3

Page 30: Morphine treatment at different infant ages: influence on

25

or saline. Figure 4 summarizes the data obtained from these same treat­

ment groups when each animal was administered morphine on test day 4. Tol­

erance resulting from morphine injections on test days 1-3 is evident in

that Morph groups displayed shorter PLLs than Sal groups. This difference

was significant for infant saline treated animals (Panel A; 2 = 21.42;

= 1, 38; 2. .05) but not for infant morphine treated animals (Panel B) .

Moreover, tolerance resulting from infant morphine treatment was evidenu

in that infant morphine treated animals displayed shorter PLLs than infant

saline treated animals. This difference was significant for Sal groups

(F = 12.11; jif = 1, 38; _£ < .05) but not for Morph groups. These data

indicate that infant morphine treatment potentiated the development of

morphine analgesic tolerance.

Activity. Figure 5 summarizes the activity data obtained from infant

untreated (Panel A) and saline treated (Panel B) animals following either

morphine or saline injections on test days 1-3. In these infant treatment

groups, morphine on test day 1 produced significantly less activity than

saline (^ = 34.84; = 1, 45; < .001). There was a significant in­

crease in activity across test days 1-3 following morphine (_F = 7.61;

=2, 68; _£ < .01) and a significant decrease in activity across test days

1-3 following saline (^ = 5.63; df = 2, 68; 2. < .01). On test day 3, there

was no significant difference in activity between morphine and saline in­

jected animals. Although the magnitude of morphine-induced hypoactive

effect and rate of tolerance development were similar in both of these

infant treatment groups, infant saline treated animals displayed signif­

icantly less activity than infant untreated animals (F^ = 11.77; d^ = 1,

Page 31: Morphine treatment at different infant ages: influence on

Figure 5. Mean activity obtained following either morphine or saline on test days 1-3 from animals untreated in infancy (Panel A) and animals treated with saline in infancy (Panel B) in Experiment 1.

Page 32: Morphine treatment at different infant ages: influence on

100

80

60

O < 40

20

SALINE — O»*" *

MORPHINE

A

J I

TEST DAY

Page 33: Morphine treatment at different infant ages: influence on

Figure 6. Mean activity obtained following morphine on test day 4 from animals untreated in infancy (Panel A) and animals treated with saline in infancy (Panel B) in Experiment 1. Morph groups previously received morphine injections on test days 1-3 and Sal groups previously re­ceived saline injections on test days 1-3.

Page 34: Morphine treatment at different infant ages: influence on

100

80

60

« 40

20

A r ALL INJECTIONS MORPHINE

B

N) If)

MORPH SAL MORPH SAL

DRUG TREATMENT ON TEST DAYS 1-3

Page 35: Morphine treatment at different infant ages: influence on

30

34; 2 .01). This depression in activity was evident on each test day

1-3 regardless of whether morphine or saline was administered. Figure 6

summarizes the data obtained from these same treatment groups when each

animal was administered morphine on test day 4. For groups labelled Morph

this was their fourth test injection of morphine and for groups labelled

Sal this was their first test injection of morphine. Tolerance is evident

in that Morph groups displayed significantly more activity than Sal groups

(F = 8.30; = 1, 34; £ < .01). Moreover, infant saline treated animals

(Panel B) displayed significantly less activity than infant untreated

animals (Panel A; = 8.57; = 1, 34; £ < .01). This depression in

activity occurred in both Morph and Sal groups, indicating that the infant

injection procedure per se depressed later activity independent of mor­

phine .

Figure 7 summarizes the activity data obtained from infant saline

treated (Panel A) and morphine treated (Panel B) animals following either

morphine or saline injections on test days 1-3. In these infant treatment

groups, morphine on test day 1 produced significantly less activity than

saline (% = 40.02; = 1, 63; £ < .001). There was a significant in­

crease in activity across test days 1-3 following morphine (£ = 11.26;

df = 2, 76; £ < .001). On test day 3, there was no significant difference

in activity between morphine and saline injected animals. Infant mor­

phine treated animals displayed significantly more activity than infant

saline treated animals on test day 2 (2 = 14.16; = 1, 63; £ < .05) but

not on test day 1. These data indicate that while tolerance was poten­

tiated by infant morphine treatment, this potentiation was not evident

Page 36: Morphine treatment at different infant ages: influence on

Figure 7. Mean activity obtained following either morphine or saline on test days 1-3 from animals treated with saline in infancy (Panel A) and animals treated with morphine in infancy (Panel B) in Experiment 1.

Page 37: Morphine treatment at different infant ages: influence on

100

80

>-I-> I— o <

60

40

20

0

r-SALINE

MORPHINE

L_

A B

N)

3 I

TEST DAY

2 J 3

Page 38: Morphine treatment at different infant ages: influence on

Figure 8. Mean activity obtained following morphine on test day 4 from animals treated with saline in infancy (Panel A) and animals treated with morphine in infancy (Panel B) in Experiment 1. Morph groups previously received morphine injections on test days 1-3 and Sal groups previously received saline injections on test days 1-3.

Page 39: Morphine treatment at different infant ages: influence on

100

80

t 60

< 40

20

^ r ALL INJECTIONS MORPHINE

8

LO

n

MORPH SAL MORPH SAL

DRUG TREATMENT ON TEST DAYS 1-3

Page 40: Morphine treatment at different infant ages: influence on

35

until the second test injection of morphine. Figure 8 summarizes the data

from these same treatment groups when each animal was administered mor­

phine on test day 4. Tolerance resulting from morphine injections on

test days 1-3 is evident in that Morph groups displayed significantly more

activity than Sal groups (2 = 24.25; f = 1, 38; < .001). However, tol­

erance resulting from infant morphine treatment is not evident, as there

was no significant difference in activity between infant saline treated

(Panel A) and morphine treated (Panel B) animals. These data, together

with those summarized in Figure 7, suggest that more than one test injec­

tion of morphine may be necessary to observe hypoactive tolerance result­

ing from the infant morphine treatment.

Body temperature. Table 4 summarizes the body temperature data ob­

tained from all treatment groups across all test days and all postinjection

intervals. In both infant untreated and saline treated animals, morphine on

test day 1 produced significantly higher body temperatures than saline 60

min after injection (2 = 123.00; = 1, 70; < .001). On this test day,

there was a significant decline in body temperature measured 60, 120 and

180 min after either morphine (2 = 252,58; f = 2, 84; _£ < .001) or saline

(F = 21.23; = 2, 84; < .001). There was no significant difference in

body temperature between morphine and saline injected animals 180 min after

injection. Tolerance to the morphine-induced hyperthermic effect was not

obtained, as there was no significant decline in body temperature across

test days 1-3. Independent of morphine, infant saline treated animals

displayed higher body temperatures than infant untreated animals. This

difference was significant 60 min after injection on test day 2 (F = 4.13;

Page 41: Morphine treatment at different infant ages: influence on

36

Table 4. Mean body temperatures obtained from animals untreated, saline treated and morphine treated in infancy and tested following either morphine or saline injection in Experiment 1. The three values for each subgroup on each test day represent mean tem­peratures recorded 60, 120 and 180 min after injection respec­tively, On test day 4, all injections were morphine.

Infant Test Day Group Injection Subgroup Injection

Mean Body Temperature (C°) Day 1 Day 2 Day 3 Day 4

(morphine)

None

Saline B,

Morphine C

Morphine 37.9 37.7 37.5 37.7 37.2 37.0 37.1 37.3 36.3 36.5 36.3 36.2

Saline 36.6 36.7 :#.8 37.8 36.4 216.6 36.5 37.5 36.2 :#.4 36.3 36.3

Morphine 37.7 3#.3 38.0 38.2 37.1 37.2 37.1 37.5 36.2 36.4 36.4 36.6

Saline 36.5 36.8 37.0 37.9 36.4 36.4 36.6 37.4 36.3 36.4 36.2 36.7

Morphine 37.9 38.2 38.1 38.2 36.7 37.1 37.2 37.4 36.6 36.4 36.4 36.5

Saline 36.8 37.1 37.2 38.1 36.6 36.7 36.7 37.4 36.5 36.4 36.3 36.4

Page 42: Morphine treatment at different infant ages: influence on

37

df = 1, 126; 2. ^ -05) and on test day 3 (P = 4.58; = 1, 126; 2 .05).

When each animal was administered morphine on test day 4, a similar pat­

tern of results was obtained. Morphine hyperthermic tolerance was not

evident, as there was no significant temperature difference between

animals previously given either morphine or saline on test days 1-3.

Moreover, infant saline treated animals displayed significantly higher

body temperatures than infant untreated animals 60 min after injection

(2 = 8.45; = 1, 70; < .01). These data indicate that the infant

injection procedure per se increased later body temperature independently

of morphine.

Although the infant injection procedure influenced later body tem­

perature, infant morphine exposure did not. In both infant saline and

morphine treated animals, morphine on test day 1 produced significantly

higher body temperatures than saline 60 min after injection (F^ = 136.49;

df = 1, 51; 2 .001). There was a significant decline in body temperature

measured 60, 120 and 180 min after either morphine (2 = 277.53; = 2, 76;

_£ < .001) or saline (^ = 27.05; = 2, 76; < .001). Tolerance was not

obtained, as there was no significant decline in body temperatures across

test days 1-3. When each animal was administered morphine on test day 4,

there was no significant temperature difference between animals previously

given either morphine or saline on test days 1-3. Moreover, there was no

significant difference between infant saline and morphine treated animals,

indicating that infant morphine exposure did not influence later body tem­

perature.

Page 43: Morphine treatment at different infant ages: influence on

38

Discussion

The results of Experiment 1 demonstrate that both morphine and the in­

jection procedure per se, administered one day after birth in rats, can

produce long-lasting changes in pain responsivity, activity, and body

temperature. Comparison of data obtained from infant saline treated and

untreated animals indicated that the injection procedure at one day of age

decreased activity and increased body temperature measured at 26-29 dayc

of age. Comparisons between infant morphine treated and saline treated

animals indicated that morphine exposure at one day of age decreased PLLs

and increased activity measured at 26-29 days of age. These long-lasting

changes related to the injection procedure and to morphine exposure were

observed regardless of whether test injections were morphine or saline.

These observations demonstrate the importance of using saline injected

control animals when investigating the influence of acute morphine expo­

sure during early infancy on later morphine effects.

One purpose of the present experiment was to replicate the observation

of single-dose hyperanalgesia in one-day-old rats reported by Huidobro and

Huidobro (1973). These investigators reported that animals given morphine

one day after birth later displayed greater morphine analgesia (hyper­

analgesia) than animals untreated one day after birth. In contrast, the

results of the present experiment demonstrated that animals given morphine

one day after birth later displayed less morphine analgesia (tolerance)

than animals given saline one day after birth. Although the reasons for

these conflicting results are unclear, it is unlikely that the difference

in infant control groups (untreated vs. saline treated) is involved, as

Page 44: Morphine treatment at different infant ages: influence on

39

there was no difference in pain responsivity between infant untreated and

saline treated animals in the present experiment (see Figure 1). However,

differences in test age, pre-injection training on the hot plate, and

animal strain may be relevant considerations. Huidobro and Huidobro

tested animals on the hot plate at 16 days of age. In the present exper­

iment, animals were tested at 26-29 days of age. Preliminary data from

this laboratory indicated that paw-lick responses following saline were

extremely variable in 16-day-old rats and, with some animals, were not

observed for up to 180 sec after contact with a 52° C hot plate. Further,

Huidobro and Huidobro (1973) used albino rats and pretrained them on the

hot plate prior to assessment of pain responsivity, while the present exper­

iment involved hooded rats and no pretraining.

Regardless of the source of difference between the present results and

those reported by Huidobro and Huidobro (1973), hyperanalgesia is not a con­

sistent result of early morphine exposure. The data indicate that a single

dose of morphine at one day of age produced analgesic tolerance and are

consistent with reports of single-dose tolerance in adults (Cochin and

Kornetsky, 1964; Huidobro, Huidobro-Toro, and Way, 1976; Kometsky and

Bain, 1968). Previous reports have demonstrated long-lasting tolerance

following morphine exposure in infants (Sonderegger, Bromley, and Zimmermann,

1977; Sonderegger and Zimmermann, 1976; Sonderegger and Zimmermann, 1978;

Zimmermann, Branch, Newman Taylor, Young, and Pang, 1974). In these in­

stances, however, morphine exposure was relatively long-term, consisting of

either pellet implantation or daily injections for one week or longer. The

present report clearly demonstrates long-lasting single-dose morphine anal­

Page 45: Morphine treatment at different infant ages: influence on

40

gesic tolerance in infants. This tolerance was obtained with a morphine

dose of 1 mg/kg, which contrasts with the morphine dose of 5-20 mg/kg

required to produce single-dose tolerance in adults. This difference

between infants and adults in dosages required to produce single-dose

tolerance may reflect differences in brain permeability to morphine in

blood, as the blood-brain barrier to morphine develops between about 16-32

days of age (Johannesson and Becker, 1973; Johannesson, Steele, and Becker,

1972; Kupferberg and Way, 1963) and also may involve differences in opiate

receptor development since there is an approximately 15-fold increase in

the number of opiate receptors from birth to 60 days of age (Clendeninn,

Petraitis, and Simon, 1976; Coyle and Pert, 1976).

Morphine analgesic tolerance obtained in the present experiment may

involve both pharmacological and behavioral (nonpharmacological) factors

(Adams, Yeh. Woods, and Mitchell, 1969; Gebhart and Mitchell, 1971; Kayan,

Ferguson, and Mitchell, 1973). Pharmacological tolerance has been de­

scribed as a diminished analgesic response brought about by morphine

exposure per se and may reflect alterations in neurotransmitter-receptor

or immunoreactive mechanisms (Clouet and Iwatsubo, 1975; Cochin, 1970;

Takemori, 1975). In contrast, behavioral tolerance has been described as

a diminished analgesic response brought about by experience with either a

functional or nonfunctional analgesiometric apparatus and may reflect clas­

sical conditioning of an hyperalgesic response (Siegel, 1975), instrumental

conditioning of a pain indicant response (Gebhart, Sherman, and Mitchell,

1971), or habituation to novel stimuli associated with the analgesiometric

apparatus (Bardo and Hughes, 1979). In the present experiment, tolerance

Page 46: Morphine treatment at different infant ages: influence on

41

evident in infant morphine treated animals may best be described as phar­

macological, because tolerance was obtained on the first test with the not

plate apparatus. In contrast, tolerance evident in all infant treatment

groups across repeated morphine injections and tests may be described, at

least in part, as behavioral. This behavioral contribution to tolerance

was evident in the diminished analgesic response to morphine resulting

from prior hot plate tests without morphine (cf. Figure 2, Sal groups and

Figure 1, morphine groups on test day 1).

Infant morphine treatment produced tolerance to subsequent morphine-

induced analgesia and potentiated analgesic tolerance development to suc­

cessive morphine injections. The present results demonstrate similar ef­

fects of infant morphine treatment on subsequent morphine-induced hypo-

activity. Tolerance to morphine-induced hypoactivity developed more rapid­

ly across test days 1-3 in infant morphine treated animals than in infant

saline treated animals (see Figure 7). This tolerance, however, was not

evident on test day 1 but was evident on test day 2. These data contrast

with the observation of morphine analgesic tolerance obtained on test day

1 and demonstrate that the presence or absence of measurable tolerance on

a single test depends on which particular morphine effect is assessed.

Unlike analgesic and hypoactive tolerance, infant morphine treatment

failed to produce tolerance to morphine-induced hyperthermia. Regardless

of infant treatment, morphine produced a robust hyperthermic effect that

did not decline across repeated injections and tests. These data are not

consistent with at least one report of morphine hyperthermic tolerance

following repeated injections and tests in adult rats (Siegel, 1978). How­

Page 47: Morphine treatment at different infant ages: influence on

42

ever, the conditions necessary for obtaining morphine hyperthermic tol­

erance are not clear, as several other reports indicate that morphine-

induced hyperthermia may actually increase following repeated injections

(Gunne, 1960; Martin, Pryzbylik, and Spector, 1977; Thornhill, Hirst, and

Gowdey, 1978). Despite this discrepancy in the literature, the present

failure to obtain hyperthermic tolerance is consistent with the observation

that, relative to analgesic and hypoactive tolerance, the rate at which

morphine tolerance develops to hyperthermia is extremely slow (Fernandes,

Kluwe, and Coper, 1977).

Finally, acute morphine-induced analgesia has been reported to be

positively correlated with the number of opiate receptors prior to BBS

development (Auguy-Valette. Gros, Gouarderes, Gout, and Pontonnier, 1978).

Morphine-induced analgesic tolerance may also be related to opiate receptor

development during this same period of infancy. If morphine tolerance is

directly related to number of opiate receptors prior to BBB development,

then the degree of tolerance produced by a 1 mg/kg morphine injection one

day after birth, as obtained in experiment 1, ought to decrease as a func­

tion of increased injection age. Experiment 2 examined this possibility.

Page 48: Morphine treatment at different infant ages: influence on

43

EXPERIMENT 2

This experiment was designed to assess the potential influence of

single-dose morphine treatment at either 1, 5,9, or 13 days of age on

later morphine-induced analgesia, hypoactivity and hyperthermia. This

particular age range was chosen because it is characterized by rapid

opiate receptor development with minimal BBB influence.

Method

Animals. Sixteen multiparous female Long-Evans hooded rats (Blue

Spruce Farms, New York) were mated in this laboratory with male rats of

the same strain. Mating consisted of randomly housing one female and one

male together for 10 consecutive days. Following this, females were indi­

vidually caged and maintained as described in Experiment 1.

Apparatus. Apparatus was that described in Experiment 1.

Procedure. Females were examined twice daily for parturition, once

in the morning and once in the afternoon. The day on which a litter was

first observed was designated as parturition for that litter (Day 0). On

Day 1, each litter was sexed and culled to 10 pups (5 male and 5 female)

whenever possible.

Treatment was generally similar to that described in Experiment 1 ex-r.

cept that an untreated group was not used and an additional variable, age

at which infants were initially administered morphine or saline, was inves­

tigated. On Day 1, animals from two randomly chosen litters were injected

with 1 mg/ml/kg morphine sulfate and animals from two other randomly chosen

Page 49: Morphine treatment at different infant ages: influence on

44

litters were injected with 0.9% saline. Twelve more litters were similarly

treated except that treatment occurred on either Day 5, 9, or 13, with four

litters (2 morphine and 2 saline) randomly treated at each age. Animals

were weaned and housed as described in Experiment 1.

On Day 26, one half of the animals from each treatment litter were in­

jected with 5 mg/ml/kg morphine and the other half were injected with sa­

line. Assignment of animals to either the morphine or saline treatment

group was random with the stipulation that each group consist of an approx­

imately equal number of males and females. Each animal was subsequently

tested for pain responsivity, activity, and body temperature after either

morphine or saline injection as described in Experiment 1. These injections

and tests were repeated on Days 27 and 28. On Day 29, all animals were in­

jected with morphine and tested. The treatment groups are summarized in

Table 5.

Data analysis. Statistical analyses of pain responsivity, activity

and body temperature data generally paralleled those described in Experiment

1, except that day of infant morphine or saline treatment was included as an

additional between-groups factor in the split-plot ANOVAs. Trend analyses

were also used to assess differences between the ages at which morphine or

saline treatments were administered.

Results

Table 6 summarizes the mortality data for each infant treatment group.

There were no significant differences in number of mortalities between in­

fant treatment groups. Although animals given saline at 9 days of age tend?

Page 50: Morphine treatment at different infant ages: influence on

45

Table 5. Summary of treatment groups in Experiment 2,

Infant Treatment Test Day Injection

Sub-Group # Litters Age (days) Injection Group N Days 26-28 Day 29

A 2 1 Morphine 4

10 Morphine Morphine

10 Saline Morphine

B 2 1 Saline "l

10 Morphine Morphine

9 Saline Morphine

C 2 5 Morphine ^1

11 Morphine Morphine

^2 9 Saline Morphine

D 2 5 Saline 9 Morphine Morphine

^2 8 Saline Morphine

E 2 9 Morphine 9 Morphine Morphine

^2 8 Saline Morphine

F 2 9 Saline ^1

7 Morphine Morphine

^2 6 Saline Morphine

G 2 13 Morphine ^1

10 Morphine Morphine

S 8 Saline Morphine

H 2 13 Saline H 1

10 Morphine Morphine

«9 9 Saline Morphine

Page 51: Morphine treatment at different infant ages: influence on

Table 6. Mortality of infant treatment groups in Experiment 2.

# Alive Infant Treatment # Alive Mortality // Alive on Following

Group # Litters on Day 1 Age Injection Treatment Day on Day 26 Treatment

A 2 20 1 Morphine 20 20 0 %

B 2 20 1 Saline 20 19 5 %

C 2 20 5 Morphine 20 20 0 %

D 2 18 5 Saline 18 17 6 %

E 2 18 9 Morphine 17 17 0 %

F 2 20 9 Saline 16 13 19 %

G 2 19 13 Morphine 18 18 0 %

H 2 19 13 Saline 19 19 0 %

Page 52: Morphine treatment at different infant ages: influence on

47

ed to displayed the highest mortality (19%), this tendency did not appear

to reflect a treatment effect. In this infant treatment group, four an­

imals died before and three animals died after saline injection at 9 days

of age.

fain responsivity. Figure 9 summarizes the pain responsivity data ob­

tained from each infant treatment group following either morphine or saline

on test days 1-4. Treatment groups are abbreviated as M-M, M-S, S-M, and

S-S, where the first letter refers to treatment in infancy (M = morphine,

S = saline) and the second letter (M or S) refers to injections on test

days 1-3. Both infant treatment groups displayed morphine-induced anal­

gesia on test Day 1. Regardless of infant treatment, morphine on this

test day produced significantly longer PLLs than saline (F = 87.45; =

1, 212; 2. ^ .001). There was a significant decline in PLLs across test

days 1-3 following either morphine (2 = 153.43; = 2, 254; 2. .001) or

saline (F^ = 14.35; = 2, 254; 2 .001). On test day 3, there was no

significant difference in PLLs between morphine and saline injected ani­

mals. On test day 4, when each animal received morphine, tolerance is

evident in animals given morphine on test days 1-3, as they displayed

significantly shorter PLLs than animals given saline on test days 1-3

(F = 56.06; df = 1, 127; < .001).

Although test injections of morphine produced an initial analgesic

effect and subsequent tolerance in both infant treatment groups, the mag­

nitude of these effects was influenced by infant treatment. On test day

1, animals treated at one day of age with morphine displayed significantly

shorter PLLs than animals treated at one day of age with saline (F = 4.42;

Page 53: Morphine treatment at different infant ages: influence on

Figure 9. Mean paw-lick latencies obtained following morphine or saline on test days 1-4 from animals treated with either morphine or saline at either 1, 5, 9, or 13 days of age in Experiment 2. M-M groups received morphine in infancy and morphine on test days 1-3; S-M groups received saline in infancy and morphine on test days 1-3; M-S groups received morphine in infancy and saline on test days 1-3; and S-S groups received saline in infancy and saline on test days 1-3. On test day 4, all injec­tions were morphine.

Page 54: Morphine treatment at different infant ages: influence on

49

TEST DAY I

I 5 9 13

TEST DAY 3

TEST DAY 2

M-M • @ S-MB B M-S O— — — O S-S • O

D- O

I 5 9 13

TEST DAY 4

ALL INJECTIONS MORPHINE

INFANT TREATMENT AGE (DAYS)

Page 55: Morphine treatment at different infant ages: influence on

50

df = 1, 212; 2, < .05). This effect was evident in animals tested following

morphine (cf M-M and S-M groups) but not following saline (cf M-S and S-S

groups) . These data demonstrate that morphine treatment at one day of age

attenuated later morphine-induced analgesia. This effect on test day Iwas

not obtained with morphine treatment at either 5,9, or 13 days of age

however, as there was no significant difference in PLLs between animals

treated at these ages with either morphine (M-M and M-S groups) or saline

(S-M and S-S groups). Trend analyses of test day 1 data revealed that in­

fant saline treatment produced a significant linear decrease in PLLs as

a function of increased treatment age ( = 7.91; = 1, 212; 2_ < .05).

This effect demonstrates that the infant injection procedure influenced

later pain responsivity. There was also a significant quadratic trend

in PLLs as a function of infant morphine treatment age (| = 12.55; df = 1,

212; 2. < .05). On test day 4, there was a significant interaction effect

(2 = 5.37; = 3, 127; < .01) involving PLLs of animals treated with

either morphine or saline at either 9 or 13 days of age and given saline

of test days 1-3. The basis for interpreting this effect is not readily

apparent.

Activity. Figure 10 summarizes the activity data obtained from each

infant treatment group following either morphine or saline on test days

1-4. Animals given morphine on test day 1 displayed less activity than

animals given saline. This effect, however, was statistically significant

only for groups treated with saline at 1 or 5 days of age, and for groups

treated with morphine at 5 or 13 days of age (2 - 6.10; = 1, 212; 2 <

.05 in each case). Morphine on test day 2 produced less activity than

Page 56: Morphine treatment at different infant ages: influence on

Figure 10. Mean activity obtained following morphine or saline on test days 1-4 from animals treated with either morphine or saline at either 1, 5, 9, or 13 days of age in Experiment 2. M-M groups received morphine in infancy and morphine on test days 1-3; S-M groups received saline in infancy and morphine on test days 1-3; M-S groups received morphine in infancy and saline on test days 1-3; and S-S groups received saline in infancy and saline on test days 1-3. On test day 4, all injections were morphine.

Page 57: Morphine treatment at different infant ages: influence on

50

40

30

20

10

0

90

80

70

60

50

40

30

20

10

0

52

ALL INJECTIONS MORPHINE

I 5 9 13 i 5 9 13

INFANT TREATMENT AGE (DAYS)

Page 58: Morphine treatment at different infant ages: influence on

saline in animals treated at one day of age with saline (_F = 13.17; =

1, 212; 2. < .001). On test day 3, morphine produced significantly greater

activity than saline in animals treated with saline at 13 days of age and

in animals treated with morphine at 9 or 13 days of age (J - 4.48; =

1, 212; £ < .05 in each case). Tolerance to morphine-induced hypoactivity

was evident when all animals received morphine on test day 4, as animals

given morphine on test days 1-3 displayed greater activity th.an animals

given saline on test days l'-3 (P = 73.30; = 1, 127; 2 < .0011,

Although test injections of morphine produced an initial hypoactive

effect to which tolerance developed, the magnitude of effect was influenced

by infant treatment. Animals treated at one day of age with morphine

displayed significantly more activity than animals treated at one day of

age with saline when morphine was given on test day 1 (2 = 23.80; = 1,

212; 2 < .001), but not when saline was given on test day 1. These data

demonstrate that morphine treatment at one day of age attenuated later

morphine-induced hypoactivity. This effect on test day 1 was not obtained

with morphine treatment at either 5,9, or 13 days of age however, as there

was no significant difference in activity between animals treated at these

ages with either morphine (M-M and M-S groups) or saline (S-M and S-S groups).

Trend analyses of test day 1 data as a function of increased treatment age

revealed a significant linear trend in M-M animals (F = 4.81; = 1, 212;

2 < .05) and S-M animals (2 = 10.93; = 1, 212; 2 .01), and a significant

quadratic trend in M-S animals (F = 7.74; = 1, 212; 2 .01). These data

indicate that the age at which either morphine or saline was given in infancy

Page 59: Morphine treatment at different infant ages: influence on

54

influenced later activity. On test day 4, there were no significant dif­

ferences in activity between animals treated at either 1, 5, 9, or 13 days

of age with either morphine or saline.

Body temperature. Body temperatures of several animals treated at

9 days of age were not recorded due to an apparatus problem. To avoid ex­

tremely unequal cell sizes, ANOVA was performed only on body temperatures

from animals treated at 1, 5, or 13 days of age. These data are sum­

marized in Table 7. Morphine on test day 1 produced significantly higher

body temperatures than saline 60 min after injection (% = 171.40; = 1,

168; 2 .001) and 120 min after injection (% = 47.45; = 1, 168; 2 <

.001), but not 180 min after injection. There was a significant decline

in body temperatures measured 60, 120, and 180 min after either morphine

(F = 555.72; df = 2, 202; £ < .001) or saline (F = 56.88; df = 2, 202; £

< .001). There was no significant change in morphine-induced hyperthermia

across test days 1-3. On test day 4, when all injections were morphine,

there was no significant difference in body temperature between animals

given morphine on test days 1-3 and animals given saline on test days 1-3.

These data indicate that tolerance to morphine-induced hyperthermia did

not develop.

Infant morphine treatment also failed to produce tolerance to morphine-

induced hyperthermia. Animals treated at either 1, 5, or 13 days of age

with morphine displayed significantly higher body temperatures than animals

treated at either 1, 5, or 13 days of age with saline = 18.35; df = 1,

101; 2. < .001). This effect was obtained on test days 1-3 following either

morphine or saline, but not at each postinjection interval. On test day

Page 60: Morphine treatment at different infant ages: influence on

Table 7. Mean body temperatures obtained from animals morphine or saline treated at 1, 5, or 13 days old and tested following either morphine or saline injection in Experiment 2. The three values for each subgroup on each test day represent mean temperatures recorded 60, 120 and 180 min after injection respectively. On test day 4, all Injections were morphine.

Infant Treatment Test Day

Group Age (days) Injection Subgroup Injection Mean Body Temperature (C°)

Day 1 Day 2 Day 3 Day 4 (morphine)

Morphine 37.6 37.6 37.7 37.9 37.0 36.8 37.1 37.2 36.2 36.3 36.5 36.4

Saline 36.8 36.9 37.0 37.8 36.6 36.7 36.9 37.1 36.5 36.5 36.8 36.2

Morhpine 37.2 37.6 37.6 37.7 36.8 37.1 37.0 37.1 36.2 35.9 35.8 36.0

Saline 36.5 36.6 36.7 37.7 36.4 36.4 36.4 37.2 36.3 36.3 36.3 36.1

Morphine 37.8 37.9 37.9 37.8 37.1 37.0 36.7 36.8 36.4 36.0 36.1 36.0

Saline 36.9 37.2 37.2 37.7 36.4 36.6 36.5 36.8 36.2 36.0 36.3 36.0

Morphine

Saline

Morphine

Page 61: Morphine treatment at different infant ages: influence on

Table 7 (Continued)

Infant Treatment Test Day Mean Body Temperature (C°)

Group Age (days) Inj ect ion Subgroup Injection Day 1 Day 2 Day 3 Day 4 (morphine)

D 5 Saline Morphine 37.8 37.1 36.2

37.5 36.9 35.8

37.3 36.9 36.1

37.4 36.9 36.0

2 Saline 36.6

36.4 36.1

36.6 36.2 36.0

36.7 36.4 36.1

37.5 36.9 36.0

G 13 Morphine 1

Morphine 37.9 36.9 36.0

37.9 37.1 36.0

37.6 37.2 36.6

37.6 37.1 36.4

«2 Saline 36.8 36.4 36.2

37.1 36.9 36.5

37.3 36.9 36.8

37.6 37.4 36.5

H 13 Saline »1 Morphine 37.7 36.9 36.1

37.5 36.9 36.1

37.7 36.9 36.4

37.5 37.0 36.0

«2 Saline 3 6 . 2 36.1 35.9

3 6 . 6 36.6 36.0

3 6 . 9 36.7 36.6

37.4 37.2 36.1

Page 62: Morphine treatment at different infant ages: influence on

57

4, infant morphine treated animals displayed significantly higher body

temperatures than infant saline treated animals 60 min after injection

(2 = 6.82; = 1, 168; £ < .05) and 180 min after injection (F = 7.75;

df = 1, 168; 2 < .05). These data indicate that morphine treatment at

either 1, 5, or 13 days of age increased later body temperature.

Discussion

The results of Experiment 2 demonstrate that a single morphine dose

administered at different periods of infancy can differentially influence

later morphine-induced analgesic and hypoactive effects. Morphine exposure

at one day of age decreased PLLs and increased activity measured after

morphine injection at 26 days of age. In contrast, morphine exposure at

either 5, 9, or 13 days of age did not influence PLLs or activity measured

after morphine injection at 26 days of age. These results indicate that

the efficacy of 1.0 mg/kg morphine in producing long-lasting single-dose

tolerance in infants declines soon after one day of age.

Although early morphine treatment produced tolerance to later morphine

induced analgesic and hypoactive effects, this treatment did not produce

tolerance to later morphine-induced hyperthermia. Single-dose morphine

administered at either 1, 5, or 13 days of age increased rather than de­

creased body temperatures measured at 26-29 days of age. Moreover, this

effect was obtained regardless of whether body temperatures were measured

after morphine or saline injections. These results indicate that morphine

treatment in infancy can produce long-lasting alterations in body temper­

ature regulation.

Page 63: Morphine treatment at different infant ages: influence on

58

Infant morphine treatment produced long-lasting age-dependent changes

in pain responsivity, activity, and body temperature. Long-lasting age- de­

pendent changes in pain responsivity and activity were also produced by

saline treatment. Animals given saline early in infancy displayed greater

morphine-induced analgesic and hypoactive effects than animals given saline

later in infancy (cf. S-M and S-S groups on test day 1 in Figures 9 and 10).

Thus, the injection procedure per se administered at different infancy

periods influenced later morphine effects. However, these age-related

effects of infant saline treatment were not observed for morphine-induced

hyperthermia.

Page 64: Morphine treatment at different infant ages: influence on

59

GENERAL DISCUSSION

A single injection of 1 mg/kg morphine in one-day-old rats produced

long-lasting tolerance to subsequent morphine-induced analgesic and hypo-

active effects but not to hyperthermic effects. In Experiment 1, animals

given morphine at one day of age displayed shorter PLLs and greater activ­

ity at 26-28 days of age than animals given saline at one day of age.

These effects were obtained regardless of whether animals were tested

following morphine or saline. The PLL results contrast with the observa­

tion by Huidobrc and Huidobro (1973) that morphine treatment at one day

of age produced relatively long PLLs indicative of morphine-induced hyper-

analgesia. However, the present results are consistent with observations

of single-dose tolerance in adults (Cochin and Kometsky, 1964; Kornetsky

and Bain, 1968) and with observations of tolerance following multiple doses

or pellet implantation in infants CSonderegger, Bromley, and Zimmermann,

1977; Sonderegger and Zimmermann, 1976; Sonderegger and Zimmermann, 1978;

Zimmermann, Branch, Newman Taylor, Young, and Pang, 1974). Furthermore,

these results are also consistent with the observation that, relative to

analgesic and hypoactive effects, the development of tolerance to hyper­

thermia is extremely slow (Femandes, Kluwe, and Coper, 1977).

Single-dose tolerance to 1 mg/kg morphine in infants was age-depen-

dent. In Experiment 2, morphine treatment at 1 but not at 5, 9, or 13 days

of age attenuated morphine-induced analgesic and hypoactive effects as as—,

sessed at 26-28 days of age. This age-dependent change in susceptibility to

single-dose tolerance with 1 mg/kg morphine may reflect either a difference

in the interval between infant morphine treatment and tolerance assessment.

Page 65: Morphine treatment at different infant ages: influence on

60

a change in BBB development, or a change in opiate receptor development.

Age-dependent changes in susceptibility to single-dose tolerance may

reflect a change in the interval between infant morphine treatment and tol­

erance assessment. Tolerance assessment was initiated at 26 days of age

regardless of when morphine was administered in infancy. Thus, animals

treated at 1 day of age had a longer interval between morphine treatment

and tolerance assessment than animals treated at either 5, 9, or 13 days

of age; i.e., animals treated at either 1, 5, 9, or 13 days of age had

treatment-tolerance assessment intervals of either 25, 21, 17, or 13 days

respectively. Tolerance obtained following morphine at 1 but not at 5, 9,

or 13 days of age might therefore suggest that a 25-day "incubation" period

is necessary for tolerance to be manifest. According to this interpreta­

tion, morphine administration at either 1, 5, 9, or 13 days of age would

be expected to produce similar tolerance if assessed at either 26, 30, 34,

or 38 days of age respectively. This possibility however, is not consis­

tent with reports demonstrating that tolerance decreases when the inter­

val between initial morphine treatment and tolerance assessment increases

beyond about three days CCochin and Rometsky, 1964; Huidobro, Huidobro-

Toro, and Way, 1976). This evidence suggests that animals treated late

in infancy should have displayed greater tolerance than animals treated

early in infancy. Since this effect was clearly not obtained, it is un­

likely that the age-dependent change in susceptibility to tolerance re­

ported here reflects a difference in treatment-tolerance assessment inter­

vals .

Page 66: Morphine treatment at different infant ages: influence on

61

Age-dependent changes in susceptibility to morphine tolerance may

reflect a change in BBB development. As previously described, the BBB

to morphine is virtually nonexistent up to about 15 days of age, but

rapidly develops from this time and becomes adult-like by about 32 days

of age (Johannesson and Becker, 1973; Johannesson, Steele, and Becker,

1972; Kupferberg and Way, 1963). During this period of BBB development,

there is a concomitant decrease in morphine-induced analgesic effect

CHuidobro and Huidobro, 1973), These observations indicate that the

magnitude of morphine-induced analgesic and perhaps hypoactive effects

are potentially related to BBB maturation. If this is the case, then

the attenuation in morphine-induced analgesic and hypoactive effects

produced by morphine treatment at 1 but not at 5, 9, or 13 days of age

in the present experiments may reflect a potentiation in BBB maturation

induced by morphine treatment at 1 but not at 5, 9, or 13 days of age.

This possibility could be investigated by assaying morphine levels in

brain and blood after morphine injection at 26 days of age in animals

previously treated with morphine at different periods of infancy.

Age-dependent changes in susceptibility to morphine tolerance may

reflect a change in opiate receptor development. As previously described,

opiate receptors are first evident two weeks into gestation and progres­

sively increase in number up to adulthood (Clendeninn, Petraitis, and

Simon, 1976; Coyle and Pert, 1976). These receptors appear to be in­

volved in morphine-induced analgesia, as morphine-induced analgesia is

antagonized by naloxone, an opiate antagonist which has greater affinity

Page 67: Morphine treatment at different infant ages: influence on

for the opiate receptor than morphine (Terenius, 1974). Moreover, mor­

phine produces analgesia when microinjected into high density but not

low density opiate receptor sites (Mayer and Murphin, 1976; Yaksh, Yeung,

and Rudy, 1976), and finally the analgesic efficacy of morphine is direct

ly correlated with the normal ontogenetic increase in number of opiate

receptors from birth to about 15 days of age in rats (Auguy-Valette, Gros

Gouarderes, Gout, and Pontonnier, 1978), Opiate receptors may also be in

volved in morphine-induced hypoactivity, as morphine-induced hypoactivity

like analgesia, is antagonized by naloxone CBhargava, 1978). These ob­

servations suggest that the magnitude of morphine-induced analgesic and

hypoactive effects may be related to the number of opiate receptors in

brain. If this is the case, then the attenuation in morphine-induced

analgesic and hypoactive effects produced by morphine treatment at 1 but

not at 5, 9, or 13 days of age in the present experiments may reflect an

attenuation in opiate receptor development induced by morphine treatment

at 1 but not at 5, 9, or 13 days of age. This possibility is consistent

with evidence indicating that the number of opiate receptors decreases

following morphine treatment. Although this effect has only been observed

in adult brain slices (Davis, Akera, and Brody, 1975), there is some ev­

idence that infant treatment can alter opiate receptor development. In

infants, the number of opiate receptors has been reported to increase fol­

lowing naloxone (Diaz, Paul, Frenk, and Bailey, 1978) or stress (Torda,

1977). One report, however, has failed to observe any alteration in the

number of opiate receptors in infants following morphine (Coyle and Pert,

1976) . Clearly, further research is necessary to determine whether the

Page 68: Morphine treatment at different infant ages: influence on

53

age-dependent change in susceptibility to tolerance reported here reflects

a change in opiate receptor development.

Changes in opiate receptor or BBB development may underly age-depen­

dent changes in morphine tolerance. The present experiments demonstrate

that these possibilities may be assessed following acute low'-dose morphine

treatment. Previous reports of long-lasting morphine tolerance in infants

have administered morphine for one week or Ipnger (Sonderegger, Erpmley, and

Zimmermann, 1977; Sonderegger and Zimmermann, 1976; Sonderegger and

Zimmermann, 1978; Zimmermann, Branch., Newman Taylor, Young, and Pang, 1974).

Such long-term treatment does not allow for evaluation of age dependent

changes in susceptibility to tolerance within a short period of infancy

such as from 1 to 13 days of age. Moreover, such long-term treatment in

infancy increases mortality CZinnaennann, Sonderegger and Bromley, 1977).

In the present experiments, mortality following morphine treatment was

minimized by acute injection and in fact was not observed following acute

morphine at either 1, 5, 9, or 13 days of age in Experiment 2.

Early morphine injection clearly influenced subsequent response mea

sures. The present results also demonstrate that early saline injection

can produce robust long-lasting changes in pain responsivity, activity,

and body temperature. In Experiment 1, animals treated with saline at

one day of age displayed less activity and higher body temperatures than

animals untreated at one day of age. These effects were obtained at 26-28

days of age regardless of whether animals were tested following morphine

or saline. In Experiment 2, animals treated early in infancy with saline

displayed longer PLLs and less activity than animals treated late in in­

Page 69: Morphine treatment at different infant ages: influence on

64

fancy with saline. These effects were obtained at 26-28 days of age when

animals were tested following morphine. Some evidence suggests that these

effects may be stress-related.

Stressful stimulation during infancy produces a variety of long-last­

ing physiological and behavioral changes in rats CDaly, 1973; Denenberg

and Zarrow, 1971; Levine, 1969; Russell, 1971). These changes reflect

direct effects on the infant, as well as indirect effects induced by al­

terations in maternal care (Bamett and Bum, 1967; Bell, Nitschke, Bell,

and Zachman, 1974; Smotherman, Wiener, Mendoza, and Levine, 1977). Stress

effects in infants have typically been produced by administering intense

and chronic sensory stimulation over a long period of infancy; e.g,, inter­

mittent shock applied throughout preweaning development. In the present

experiments however, changes in pain responsivity, activity, and body

temperature were produced by administering relatively moderate and acute

sensory stimulation over a brief period of infancy; i.e., saline injection

applied at either 1, 5, 9, or 13 days of age. The injection procedure in­

volved a 15-minute period of separation from the lactating mother, during

which time infants were handled and injected with saline. Changes in pain

responsivity, activity, and body temperature produced by the infant saline

treatment may therefore reflect stress induced by separation from the moth­

er, handling, and/or the injection procedure. Regardless of these factors

played a role in the long-lasting changes produced by infant saline treat­

ment reported here, the present observations illustrate the difference be­

tween saline-injected and untreated control groups for assessing long-last­

ing psychopharmacological consequences of infant drug treatment.

Page 70: Morphine treatment at different infant ages: influence on

65

Finally, saline administration at 26-28 days of age also may have in­

fluenced pain responsivity, activity, and body temperature independent of

morphine. This possibility was not directly assessed in the present exper­

iments, as no control group was uninjected at 26-28 days of age. However,

it has previously been shown that saline injection produces a transient

increase in body temperature CGunne, 1960), suggesting that it may be

stressful. In the present experiments, body temperatures were higher 60

minutes after saline injection than 180 minutes after injection. Exposure

to the hot plate may have contributed to this temperature increase, as

hot plate tests preceded body temperature measurements and hot plate ex

posure is known to be stressful CTorda, 1977), Acute stress produces a

transient elevation in endogenous morphine-like neuropeptides in brain

CGuillemin, Vargo, Bossier, Minick, Ling, Rivier, Vale, and Bloom, 1977;

Madden, Akil, Patrick, and Barchas, 1977) and produces various effects

which are functionally similar to those produced by morphine, including

analgesia (Akil, Madden, Patrick, and Barchas, 1976; Bodnar, Kelly,

Spiaggia, and Glusman, 1978), hypoactivity (Gray, Solomon, Dunphy, Carr,

and Hession, 1976; Pinel and Mucha, 1973) and hyperthermia (Delini-Stula,

1970) . These observations indicate that stressful stimulation is an im­

portant factor to consider when assessing morphine effects across the life­

span.

Page 71: Morphine treatment at different infant ages: influence on

66

REFERENCES

Adams, W. J., Yeh, S. Y., Woods, L. A. and Mitchell, C. L. Drug-test

interaction as a factor in the development of tolerance to the

analgesic effect of morphine. Journal of Pharmacology and Experi­

mental Therapeutics, 1969, 168, 251-257.

Akil, H., Madden, J., Patrick, R. L. and Barchas, J. D. Stress-induced

increase in endogenous opiate peptides : Concurrent analgesia and

its partial reversal by naloxone. In H. W. Kosterlitz (Ed.), Opiates

and Endogenous Opioid Peptides. Amsterdam; North Holland, 1976.

Auguy-Valette, A., Cros, J., Gouarderes, C,, Gout, A. and Pontonnier, G.

Morphine analgesia and cerebral opiate receptors : A developmental

study. British Journal of Pharmacology, 1978, 6 , 303-308.

Bardo, M. T. and Hughes, R. A. Exposure to a nonfunctional hot plate as

a factor in the assessment of morphine-induced analgesia and analgesic

tolerance in rats. Pharmacology, Biochemistry & Behavior, 1979, 10,

481-485.

Barnett, S. and Bum, J. Early stimulation and maternal behavior. Nature,

1967, 213, 150-152.

Bell, R. W., Nitschke, W., Bell, N. J, and Zachman, T. A. Early experience,

ultrasonic vocalizations and maternal responsiveness in rats. Develop­

mental Psychobiology, 1974, 235-242.

Bhargava, H. N. Effects of methionine-enkephalin and morphine on sponta­

neous locomotor "activity ; Antagonism, by naloxone. Phg-rmaeology,

Biochemistry & Behavior, 19.78, _9, 167-171.

Page 72: Morphine treatment at different infant ages: influence on

Bodnar, R. J., Glusman, M., Spiaggia, A., Brutus, M. and Kelly, D. D.

Attenuation of stress-induced increases in nociceptive thresholds by

hypophysectomy. Pain Abstracts, 1978, ]., 262.

Bodnar, R. J., Kelly, D. D., Spiaggia, A. and Glusman, M. Stress-induced

analgesia: Adaptation following chronic cold-water swins. Bulletin

of the Psychonomic Society, 1978, jLl, 337-340.

Bonnet, K. A. and Peterson, K. E. A modification of the jump-flinch tech­

nique for measuring pain sensitivity in rats. Pharmacology, Bio­

chemistry & Behavior, 1975, 47-55.

Clendeninn, N, J., Petraitis, M. and Simon, E. J, Ontological development

of opiate receptor in rodent brain. Brain Research, 1976, 118, 157-160.

Clouet, D. H. and Iwatsubo, K. Mechanisms of tolerance to and dependence

on narcotic analgesic drugs. Annual Review of Pharmacology, 1975, 15,

49-71.

Cochin, J. Possible mechanisms in development of tolerance. Federation

Proceedings, 1970, _2£, 19-27.

Cochin, J. and Kornetsky, C. Development and loss of tolerance to morphine

in rat after single and multiple injections. Journal of Pharmacology

and Experimental Therapeutics, 1964, 145, 1-10,

Collins, L. G. Effect of morphine sulfate on spontaneous activity level

of the albino rat. Psychological Reports, 1965, 693-696.

Cox, B., Ary, M., Chesarek, W. and Lomax, P. Morphine hyperthermia in the

rat: An action on the central thermostats. European Journal of

Pharmacology, 1976, 33-39.

Page 73: Morphine treatment at different infant ages: influence on

68

2 Coyle, J. T. and Pert, C. B. Ontogenetic development of H3 naloxone

binding in rat brain. Neuropharmacology, 1976, 1 , 555-560.

Daly, M. Early stimulation of rodents: A critical review of present

interpretations. British Journal of Psychology, 1973, 6 , 435-460.

D'jAmour, F. E. and Smith, D. L. A method for determining loss of pain

sensation. Journal of Pharmacology and Experimental Therapeutics,

1941, 72, 74-79.

Davis, M. E., Akera, T. and Brody, T. M. Saturable binding of morphine to

rat brain-stem slices and the effect of chronic morphine treatment.

Research Communications in Chemical Pathology and Pharmacology, 1975,

409-418.

Delini-Stula, A. The development and the extinction of hyperthermia

induced by conditioned avoidance behavior in rats. Journal of the

Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 1970, 14, 213-218.

Denenberg, V. H. and Zarrow, M. X. Effects of handling in infancy upon

adult behavior and adrenocortical activity: Suggestions for a

neuroendocrine mechanism. In D. N. Walcher and D. L. Peters (Eds.),

Early Childhood; The Development of Self-Regulatory Mechanisms.

Hew York: Academic Press, 1971,

Diaz, J., Paul, L., Frank, E, and Bailey, B, Permanent alterations of

central opiate systems as a result of chronic opiate antagonism during

infancy in rats. Proceedings of the Western Pharmacological Society,

1978, 21, 377-379,

Evans, W. 0. A new technique for the investigation of some analgesic dirugs

on a reflexive behavior in the rat. Psychopharmacologia, 1961, 2,

318-325.

Page 74: Morphine treatment at different infant ages: influence on

Fernandea, M., Kluwe, S. and Coper, H. The development of tolerance to

morphine in the rat. Psychopharmacology, 1977, 19.7r>201,

Gebhart, G, F. and Mitchell, C, L. Further studies on the development of

tolerance to the analgesic effect of morphine; The role played by

the cylinder in the hot plate testing procedure. Archives Interna -

tionales de Pharmacodynamie et de Therapie, 1971, 191, 96-103,

Gebhart, G. F., Sherman, A. D. and Mitchell, C, L. The influence of

learning on morphine analgesia and tolerance development in rata

tested on the hot plate. Psychopharmacologia, 1971, 22, 295-304.

Gray, P., Solomon, J., Dunphy, M., Carr, F. and Hession, M, Effects of

lithium on open field behavior in "stressed" and "nonstressed" rats.

Psychopharmacology, 1976, 277-281.

Guillemin, R., Vargo, T., Rossier, J., Minick, S., Ling, N., Eivier, C.,

Vale, W. and Bloom, F. B-endorphin and adrenocorticotropin are

secreted concomitantly by the pituitary gland. Science, 1977, 197,

1367-1369,

Gunne, L, M. The temperature response in rats during acute and chronic

morphine administration; A study of morphine tolerance. Archives

Internationales de Pharmacodynamie et de Therapie, 1960, 79, 416-428.

Huidobro, F., Huidobro-Toro, J. P. and Way, E. L. Studies on tolerance

development to single doses of morphine in mice. Journal of Pharma­

cology and Experimental Therapeutics, 1976, 198, 318-329.

Huidobro, J. P. and Huidobro, F. Acute morphine tolerance in new born and

young rats. Psychopharmacologia, 1973, 27-34.

Page 75: Morphine treatment at different infant ages: influence on

Johannesson, T. and Becker, B. A. Morphine analgesia in rats at various

ages. Acta Pharmacologica et Toxicologica, 1973, 13, 429-441.

Johannesson, T., Steele, B. J. and Becker, B. A. Infusion of morphine in

maternal rats at near-term; Maternal and foetal distribution and

effects on analgesia, brain DNA, RNA and protein. Acta Pharmacologica

et Toxicologica, 1972, 3 , 353-368.

Johannesson, T. and Woods, L. A. Analgesic action and brain and plasma

levels of morphine and codeine in morphine tolerant, codeine tolerant

and non-tolerant rats. Acta Pharmacologica et Toxicologica, 1964,

21, 381-396.

Kay an, S., Ferguson, R. K. and Mitchell, C, L. An investigation of

pharmacologic and behavioral tolerance to morphine in rats. Journal

of Pharmacology and Experimental Therapeutics, 1973, 185, 300-306.

Kornetsky, C. and Bain, G. Morphine; Single-dose tolerance. Science,

1968, 162, 1011-1012,

Kupferberg, H. J. and Way, E. L. Pharmacologic basis for the increased

sensitivity of the newborn rat to morphine. Journal of Pharmacology

and Experimental Therapeutics, 1963, 141, 105-112.

Kuschinsky, K. and Hornykiewicz, 0, Effects of morphine on striatal dop­

amine metabolism; Possible mechanism of its opposite effect on

locomotor activity in rats and mice. European Journal of Phar­

macology. 1974, 26, 41-50.

Levine, S. An endocrine theory of infantile stimulation. In A, Ambrose

(Ed.), Stimulation in Early Infancy. London; Academic Press, 1969.

Page 76: Morphine treatment at different infant ages: influence on

71

Madden, J., Akil, H., Patrick, R. L. and Barchas, J. D. Stress-induced

parallel changes in central opioid levels and pain responsiveness in

the rat. Nature, 1977, 265, 358-360.

Martin, G. E., Pryzbylik, A. T. and Spector, N. H. Restraint alters the

effects of morphine and heroin on core temperature in the rat, Pharma­

cology, Biochemistry & Behavior, 1977, 2» 463-469.

Mayer, D. J. and Murphin, R. Stimulation-produced analgesia CSPA) and

morphine analgesia (MA): Cross-tolerance from application at the

same brain site. Federation Proceedings, 1976, 15, 385,

Pinel, J. P. J. and Mucha, R. F. Activity and reactivity in rats at

various intervals after footskock. Canadian Journal of Psychological

Review and Canadian Psychology, 1973, 112'-118.

Russell, P, A, "Infantile stimulation" in rodents; A consideration of

possible mechanisms. Psychological Bulletin, 19.71, 75, 19L2r-202,

Saunders, D. R., Paolino, R. M,, Bousquet, F. W. and Miya, T. S, Age-

related responsiveness of the rat to drugs affecting the central

nervous system. Proceedings of the Society for Experimental Biology

and Medicine, 1974 147, 593 59.5,

Siegel, S. Evidence from rats that morphine tolerance is a learned response.

Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 19.75, 89, 49.8t5Q6,

Siegel, S. Tolerance to the hyperthermic effect of morphine in the rat is

a learned response. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychol­

ogy 3 1978, , 1137-1149.

Page 77: Morphine treatment at different infant ages: influence on

Smotherman, W. P., Wiener, S. G., Mendoza, S. P. and Levine, S, Maternal

pituitary-adrenal responsiveness as a function of differential treat­

ment of rat pups. Developmental Psychobiology> 1977, 10, 113-122.

Sonderegger, T. B., Bromley, B. and Zinnnermann, E. Effects of morphine

pellet implantation in neonatal rats. Proceedings of the Society

for Experimental Biology and Medicine, 1977, 154, 435-438,

Sonderegger, T, and Zimmermann, E. Persistent effects of neonatal narcotic

addiction in the rat. In D. H. Ford and H, C. Clouet (Ed.), Tissue

Responses to Addictive Drugs. New York: Spectrum, 1976.

Sonderegger, T, and Zimmermann, E. Adult behavior and adrenocortical

function following neonatal morphine treatment in rats. Psychopharma-

cology, 1978, 56, 103-109.

Takemori, A, E, Neurochemical bases for narcotic tolerance and dependence.

Biochemical Pharmacology, 1&75, 24, 2121 2126,

Terenius, L, Contribution of "receptor" affinity to analgesic potency.

Journal of Pharmacy and Pharmacology, 1974, 26, 146-148,

Thornhill, J. A,, Hirst, M. and Gowdey, C. VÎ. Changes in the hyperthermic

response of rats to daily injections of morphine and the antagonism

of the acute response by naloxone. Candian Journal of Physiological

Pharmacology. 1978, 483-489.

Torda, C. Effects of recurrent postnatal pain-related stressful events on

opiate receptor-endogenous ligand system. Psychoneuroendocrinology,

1977, 3, 85-91.

Page 78: Morphine treatment at different infant ages: influence on

73

Yaksh, T. L., Yeung, J. C. and Rudy, T. A. Systematic examination in the

rat of brain sites sensitive to direct application of morphine:

Observation of differential effects within the periaqueductal gray.

Brain Research, 1976, 114, 83-103.

Zimmermann, E., Branch, B., Newman Taylor, A., Young, J. and Pang, C. N.

Long-lasting effects of prepuberal administration of morphine in

adult rats. In E. Zimmermann and R. George (Ed.), Narcotics and

the Hypothalamus. New York: Raven, 1974 .

Zimmermann, E., Sonderegger, T. and Bromley, B. Development and adult

pituitary-adrenal function in female rats injected with morphine

during different postnatal periods. Life Sciences, 1977, 639-646.

Page 79: Morphine treatment at different infant ages: influence on

74

ACKNOWLEDGMENTS

I would like to thank Dr. Richard A. Hughes for the generous help

and guidance he has enthusiastically given me throughout my graduate ca­

reer. I would also like to thank Dr. Ronald H. Peters, whose teaching

and research abilities I have always admired. Thanks must also go to

Dr. Robert F. Strahan for assisting me in statistics, and to Dr. Gary

Phye and Dr. Charles D. Drewes for serving on my graduate committee.